1 1 phonetic and phonological research on native...

65
1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE AMERICAN 1 LANGUAGES: PAST, PRESENT, AND FUTURE 1 2 3 MATTHEW K. GORDON 4 5 UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SANTA BARBARA 6 7

Upload: others

Post on 12-Aug-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

1

PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE AMERICAN 1

LANGUAGES: PAST, PRESENT, AND FUTURE1 2

3

MATTHEW K. GORDON 4

5

UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SANTA BARBARA 6

7

Page 2: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

2

TABLE OF CONTENTS 8

9

1. Introduction 10

2. Early research on the sound systems of Native American languages 11

3. Instrumental phonetic research on Native American Languages 12

4. Phonological research on Native American Languages 13

5. Four case studies of the phonetics and phonology of Native American Languages 14

5.1. Iambic metrical structure 15

5.1.1. Iambic lengthening in Chickasaw 16

5.1.2. High tone assignment in Creek 17

5.2. Weight-sensitive tone 18

5.3. Prosodic morphology 19

5.4. Phonetics and phonology in language endangerment and revitalization 20

6. Prospects for the future 21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

Page 3: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

3

1. Introduction 30

31

The study of Native American languages has played a prominent role in the development 32

of modern phonetic and phonological description and theory. Dating back to pioneering 33

work in the early 20th century by Franz Boas, Edward Sapir, and Pliny Earl Goddard, 34

detailed case studies of native languages of the Western Hemisphere have been integral 35

to fleshing out the typological variation in sound systems that serves as the basis for 36

theory construction and evaluation. Many of the insights of modern phonetics and 37

phonology crucially rely on the discovery in Native American language of features not 38

found in the Indo-European and Uralic languages providing the fodder for the pioneering 39

linguistic research of the 19th century. The advancements in phonetics and phonology 40

inspired by the study of Native American languages span diverse subfields, including 41

acoustic, articulatory, and auditory phonetics and phonological description and theory. 42

The breadth of methodologies attested in research on indigenous languages of the 43

Americas is matched by the wide range of phenomena that have been investigated. The 44

combination of the phonetic and phonological diversity and, in many cases, complexity 45

of Native American languages coupled with their relatively understudied status ensures 46

their continued importance in the enhancement of our understanding of phonetics and 47

phonology. On a practical level, new avenues of research on the phonetics and phonology 48

of Native American languages have also emerged as communities engage in language 49

revitalization efforts. 50

Page 4: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

4

This paper provides an overview of how research on Native American languages 51

has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics and phonology, spanning the period 52

shortly before the publication of IJAL’s inaugural issue in 1917 to the present. In order 53

to operationalize the presentation, discussion will be organized around the division 54

between instrumental phonetic studies and descriptive and theoretical research in 55

phonology, even though in practice the two threads of research are closely intertwined. 56

Work on languages spoken throughout the Americas will be discussed with emphasis on 57

languages of the Southeastern United States, which serve as the basis for three presented 58

case studies attesting to the important contributions of this linguistic area to our 59

knowledge of phonetics and phonology. A fourth case study is devoted to an increasingly 60

prominent area of work: the study of phonetic and phonological characteristics of 61

speakers, semi-speakers and learners of endangered languages. 62

63

2. Early research on the sound systems of Native American languages 64

In his introduction to the inaugural issue of IJAL in 1917, Franz Boas recognized the 65

relatively impoverished state of knowledge of Native American languages (especially 66

those of Central and South America) at that time but also expressed hope that recent 67

progress in the scope of material collected and the methodologies employed in its 68

collection augured well for future research. He also was well aware of the fragile status of 69

many languages owing to the rapid encroachment of socially and culturally dominant 70

European languages, contact with which threatened to dramatically alter the indigenous 71

Page 5: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

5

languages of the Americas, e.g. the strength of the glottalized stops in languages of the 72

Pacific coast (Boas 1917a:2). 73

At the time of IJAL’s first edition, there had already been some descriptive 74

phonetic and phonological work on Native American languages of North America that 75

was considerably more rigorous than early cursory descriptions from explorers, 76

missionaries and traders. Research at the turn of the 20th century focused on descriptions 77

of particular languages and the synthesis of material from multiple languages to establish 78

genetic relationships between languages. Boas himself had published extensively on 79

Native American languages, including chapters on Tsimshian, Chinook, and Kwakiutl in 80

his own edited volume Handbook of American Indian Languages (Boas 1911), which 81

also contained descriptions of Tlingit, Haida, Fox, Maidu, Dakota, and Inuktitut. Sapir 82

was also already a prominent figure in language documentation and historical-83

comparative linguistics by the time of IJAL’s inauguration, having published language 84

descriptions and comparative work on Chinookan, Wakashan, Uto-Aztecan, Na Dene, 85

and Takelma. 86

As portended by Boas, IJAL would play an integral role in the promotion of 87

research on Native American languages, including in the areas of phonetics and 88

phonology. Boas himself was at the vanguard of advancing IJAL’s agenda, publishing in 89

the first issue a description of the now extinct Pochutec variety of Nahuatl, including 90

several pages of phonology (Boas 1917b). 91

Boas’ paper on Pochutec would set a template for many future papers in IJAL that 92

provide language descriptions, which characteristically include a section devoted to 93

Page 6: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

6

phonology. This traditional type of grammatical overview paper has been supplemented 94

with an increasing number of publications focused on particular phonetic or phonological 95

topics. Concomitant with the narrowing in scope of individual papers, progressively more 96

research is also showcased in non-areal journals publishing research in phonetics and/or 97

phonology. Through these publications as well as through early language descriptions 98

that still retain their importance, phonetic and phonological research on Native American 99

languages has played a prominent role in shaping the fields of phonetics and phonology. 100

The remainder of the paper summarizes these contributions from an historical 101

perspective, starting in the next section with phonetics. 102

103

3. Instrumental phonetic research on Native American Languages 104

105

In his introduction to IJAL, Boas (1917a) singles out for commendation the phonetic 106

fieldwork conducted by Pliny Earle Goddard at the turn of the 20th century on the Pacific 107

Coast Athabaskan languages Hupa and Kato (Goddard 1907; 1912). Goddard’s work was 108

ambitious in its employment of an impressive array of instrumental techniques to 109

investigate the aerodynamic and articulatory characteristics of both languages, Hupa 110

more comprehensively than Kato. His methodologies included photography to assess the 111

position of the mouth during the production of vowels, palatography to examine the 112

location of contact between the tongue and the roof of the mouth, and kymography, to 113

investigate fluctuations in air pressure during speech. The kymograph could be outfitted 114

with different fittings to analyze data produced at different sources, including the mouth, 115

Page 7: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

7

the nose, and the larynx. In a later study, Goddard (1928) investigated pitch variation to 116

demonstrate that Hupa (like other Pacific Coast Athabaskan languages and unlike 117

Southern Athabaskan and many Northern Athabaskan languages) does not use tone to 118

mark lexical contrasts. Goddard’s use of multiple instrumental methodologies was far 119

ahead of its time, only reemerging again at the end of the 20th century albeit with far 120

more sophisticated tools (see below). 121

Figure 1 illustrates two representative palatograms from Goddard (1907). The one 122

on the left shows the contact pattern on the roof of the mouth for the denti-alveolar /t/ in 123

/taw/ ‘no’, while the one on the right shows contact for the palatalized velar in /kʲʰa/ 124

‘dress’. In both palatograms, the area depicted is the region in front of the velum; 125

palatography is thus not an effective methodology for examining contact patterns for 126

posterior articulations. The contact between the tongue and the roof of the mouth, 127

represented as the dark region, is fairly broad in the front-back domain for the denti-128

alveolar, extending from the alveolar ridge all the way forward to the back of the upper 129

teeth. For the palatalized velar stop, contact extends along both sides of the mouth 130

forward in a triangular pattern indicative of a raised tongue body associated with the 131

secondary palatalization gesture. The velar closure does not show up in the palatogram 132

since it is behind the depicted region. 133

134

Page 8: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

8

135

Figure 1. Palatograms of the denti-alveolar stop in /t/ in /taw/ ‘no’ (on left) and the 136

palatalized velar in /kʲʰa/ ‘dress’ (on right) (Goddard 1907). 137

138

Figure 2 shows kymography traces from the mouth (the top line) and nose (the 139

bottom line) in the word /tʰaʔnaːn/ ‘water, to drink’ (Goddard 1907). 140

141

Page 9: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

9

Nasal 142

143

144

145

Figure 2. Kymography traces for the oral (top) and nasal (bottom) air in the word 146

/tʰaʔnaːn/ ‘water, to drink’. 147

148

The general relationship between oral and nasal flow is evident in the figure: as air comes 149

out through the nose in the production of the two nasals, it ceases to vent through the 150

mouth, although there is a brief transitional period of coarticulation between the nasal and 151

adjacent vowels during which air escapes through both the nose and mouth. The glottal 152

stop is apparent from the cessation of airflow through either the nasal or oral cavities. The 153

Oral

t ʰ a ʔ n aː n

Page 10: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

10

figure suggests venting of air through the nose during the oral stop at the beginning of the 154

word, plausibly reflecting passive breathing prior to phonation. 155

Although Goddard’s work provided insight into many characteristics of the sound 156

systems of Hupa and Kato, the limitations in both the instrumentation of the time and in 157

Goddard’s phonetic expertise yielded some descriptive inaccuracies (see Gordon 1996 for 158

a more recent phonetic study of Hupa). For example, in grappling with the description of 159

the ejective /t’/, Goddard correctly characterizes both its long lag voice-onset-time and 160

(less confidently) the glottalic nature of the airstream mechanism, but also incorrectly 161

suggests that it is produced with ingressive airflow: 162

163

“Hupa has another t formed in the same tongue position, but having quite 164

a different quality. It appears to lie between d [its voiceless unaspirated 165

counterpart] and t [its aspirated counterpart], and is at first distinguished 166

from them with great difficulty. It differs from d that there is a definite 167

period of time after the breaking of the contact before sonancy begins. It 168

differs from t in that it lacks the aspiration. In fact the breath seems to be 169

drawn in rather than forced out. This does not appear to be done from the 170

lungs but from the mouth, either by the sudden withdrawing of the tongue 171

enlarging the buccal cavity, or more probably by a closure of the glottis” 172

(Goddard 1907:14; italics mine). 173

174

Page 11: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

11

The development of the sound spectrograph during World War II offered new 175

opportunities for the instrumental investigation of speech. In a series of four papers, 176

Hickerson (1958a; 1958b; 1959a; 1959b) employed spectography to conduct a 177

comprehensive study of both segmental and suprasegmental properties of Shawnee. 178

Hogan (1976) is a spectrographic study of quantitative and qualitative properties of Dëne 179

Sųłiné ejectives. He includes representative spectrograms illustrating the ejective 180

fricatives and afficates, which are characterized by a long lag voice-onset-time, similar to 181

those in Hupa. 182

Lindau’s (1984) cross-linguistic acoustic study of ejectives represents a 183

significant methodological advancement in that it includes data from multiple speakers of 184

the same language (including nine Navajo speakers), which allows for teasing apart 185

properties that are characteristic of the population of speakers as opposed to idiosyncratic 186

properties of someone’s idiolect. Dart’s (1991; 1993) phonetic research on the coronal 187

contrasts of Tohono O’odham is another relatively early multi-speaker study employing 188

spectral data, which Dart uses in conjunction with palatography and its inverse technique 189

linguography, in which contact patterns on the tongue are investigated. 190

A concentrated body of research on phonetic properties of Native American 191

languages emerged during the 1990s as part of the NSF Sounds of the World’s 192

Languages grant awarded to Peter Ladefoged and Ian Maddieson at UCLA (where 193

Lindau and Dart also conducted their research). Languages (from North, South, and 194

Central America) described in a series of UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics volumes 195

(and subsequently, in most cases, in other journals) include Navajo (de Jong and 196

Page 12: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

12

McDonough 1993, McDonough and Ladefoged 1993, McDonough et al. 1993, 197

McDonough and Austin-Garrison 1994), Tlingit (Maddieson et al. 1996), Montana Salish 198

(Flemming et al. 1994) Western Apache (Potter et al. 2000), Jicarilla Apache (Tuttle 199

2000), Hupa (Gordon 1996), Aleut (Cho et al. 1997), Chickasaw (Gordon et al. 1997), 200

Jalapa Mazatec (Silverman et al. 1994), Banawá (Ladefoged and Everett 1996) and Wari’ 201

(MacEachern et al. 1996). 202

These studies are quantitative and rely on data from several speakers of both 203

genders, though the number of speakers is small in the case of some severely threatened 204

languages. The papers include descriptions of basic phonetic properties, such as vowel 205

formant frequencies, durational characteristics of consonants and vowels, and, in the case 206

of tone languages, fundamental frequency values. They also include qualitative and 207

quantitative analyses of typologically unusual properties found in the particular language 208

under investigation. Topics of interest targeted by the studies include (among many 209

others) the ejective fricatives of Tlingit, the phonation contrasts of Jalapa Mazatec, the 210

velar vs. uvular distinction in Aleut, the consonant clusters of Montana Salish, and the 211

atypical vowel system of Wari’, which contains more front rounded vowels than back 212

rounded vowels. 213

The studies employ diverse methodologies. For example, Maddieson et al. (1996) 214

use acoustic and aerodynamic data in tandem to infer the articulatory characteristics of 215

the typologically rare Tlingit ejective fricatives, which are found in only 10 of 451 (2.2%) 216

languages in the UPSID survey (Maddieson and Precoda 1990). In their study of 217

Montana Salish, Flemming et al. (1994) complement their acoustic data with several 218

Page 13: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

13

types of aerodynamic (oral pressure and flow and nasal flow) and physiological 219

(electroglottography) data. 220

The data from the Ladefoged and Maddieson project also have been pooled and 221

employed in typological studies of phonetic properties. For example, Cho and Ladefoged 222

(1999) is a survey of voice-onset-time (VOT) in 18 languages targeted for study by the 223

grant, the majority of which (11) are Native American languages. One of the goals of 224

their study is to explore the relationship between VOT values in languages lacking a 225

contrast between unaspirated and aspirated stops and in languages possessing an 226

aspiration contrast. One working hypothesis guiding their research is that VOT values for 227

unaspirated stops are smaller in languages with an aspiration contrast due to the need to 228

maintain a perceptually salient distinction with the aspirated series. 229

Figure 3 depicts average VOT values for the velar stops (the place of articulation 230

most broadly represented in the database) in the surveyed Native American languages. 231

The languages on the left lack an aspiration contrast for voiceless stops, whereas those on 232

the right have the contrast. 233

234

Page 14: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

14

235

Figure 3. Voice-onset-time (VOT) values (in milliseconds) for velar stops in 11 Native 236

American languages (Cho and Ladefoged 1999). 237

238

As figure 3 shows, although VOT values for the unaspirated series are generally smaller 239

(as predicted) in languages with an aspiration contrast, this pattern is not universal. VOT 240

values for the unaspirated stops in Hupa and Navajo, which both contrast aspiration, are 241

thus either equivalent to or larger than values for the unaspirated stops in Chickasaw and 242

Banawá, both of which lack an aspiration contrast. It is also striking that VOT values for 243

the two Aleut varieties, which lack an aspiration contrast, are roughly equivalent to (or 244

even larger than) VOT values for the phonemic aspirated stops in three of the five 245

languages with an aspiration contrast. The considerable cross-linguistic variability 246

observed in Cho and Ladefoged’s (1999) study underscores the extent to which the 247

phonetic realization of a phonological property may differ across languages, often in 248

0  20  40  60  80  100  120  140  160  180  

Unaspirated  

Aspirated  

Page 15: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

15

ways that are not necessarily predictable from the structure of a language. Nevertheless, 249

despite the cross-linguistic variation in VOT values, the phonetic distinction between 250

unaspirated and aspirated stops is clearly maintained in all languages in Cho and 251

Ladefoged’s study with contrastive aspiration: VOT values for the aspirated series are 252

thus at least twice as long as those for unaspirated stops and, in some languages, four or 253

five times as long. Another interesting finding of the Cho and Ladefoged (1993) study is 254

that in three of the four languages surveyed with both an aspiration contrast and ejective 255

stops (Navajo, Western Apache, and Tlingit but not Hupa), VOT values for the ejectives 256

fall between those of their unaspirated and aspirated counterparts, suggesting that VOT 257

may be used as a supplemental cue to the identification of ejectives. 258

Recent work on Native American languages has built on phonetic research of the 259

20th century by employing more sophisticated techniques for directly accessing 260

articulatory data, rather than merely inferring it from acoustic and aerodynamic 261

measurements. Esling et al. (2005) use laryngoscopy in their study of Nuuchahnulth to 262

examine the production of glottal stop, glottalized sonorants, and pharyngeals. McDowell 263

(2004), Namdaran (2006), and Hudu (2008) employ ultrasound to investigate the 264

phonetic realization of vowel retraction triggered by uvulars and pharyngeal consonants 265

in Interior Salish languages. Gick et al. (2012) also use ultrasound data in their 266

examination of voiceless vowels in Oneida and Blackfoot, finding that articulatory 267

reflexes of these vowels remain even in the absence of acoustic traces. They further show 268

in a perception experiment involving Blackfoot listeners that listeners are perceptually 269

Page 16: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

16

unable to distinguish the identity of vowels that have been acoustically but not 270

articulatorily deleted. 271

The Gick et al. (2012) study belongs to a growing area of research in Native 272

American languages, and more generally the field of phonetics, employing perception 273

experiments in tandem with production data in order to assess the mapping between 274

articulation and perception. Wright et al. (2002) complement their production study of 275

Witsuwit’en ejectives with a perception study, finding that the contrast between ejective 276

and unaspirated stops is less reliably perceived than other distinctions including the 277

contrast between aspirated and unaspirated stops. This result plausibly is tied to the 278

characteristically short VOT values for ejectives in Witsuwit’en relative to those in the 279

four languages with ejectives considered in the Cho and Ladefoged (1999) study 280

discussed earlier. 281

Because of their overall more robust status relative to their neighbors to the North 282

and South, much of the work on perception has focused on languages of Central America. 283

Gerfen and Baker (2005), for example, examine the acoustic and perceptual correlates of 284

laryngealized phonation in the Otomanguean language Coatzospan Mixtec, finding that 285

drops in amplitude or fundamental frequency alone are sufficient to cue judgments of 286

laryngealization even in the absence of differences in spectral tilt, which is typically 287

regarded as the most salient acoustic reflex of non-modal phonation (Gordon and 288

Ladefoged 2001). Clopper and Tonhauser (2013) examine the perceptual correlates of 289

focal prominence in Paraguayan Guaraní. Crowhurst and Olivares (2014) is a perception 290

Page 17: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

17

study that explores rhythmic groupings and the psychological underpinnings of rhythmic 291

biases in Betaza Zapotec (see section 5.1 for more research on rhythmic groupings). 292

One area of research emphasized by Boas (1917a) to be of great importance, 293

studies based on narrative and discourse data, has recently gained greater prominence in 294

the phonetic literature. Because naturalistic data collected outside of the laboratory 295

introduce a number of confounding factors that can make results difficult to interpret, 296

most quantitative phonetic studies on Native American languages (and more generally in 297

the field of phonetics) are based on elicited data. Discourse data, however, are critical to 298

gain a comprehensive understanding of prosodic properties such as prominence, 299

intonation, and prosodic constituency. Hintz’s (2006) paper on South Conchucos 300

Quechua and Gordon and Rose’s (2006) work on Emerillón are two acoustic studies of 301

stress correlates in Native American languages of South America that compare words 302

uttered in isolation with narrative data. Both studies find differences in the phonetic 303

realization of prominence and in its location between the two contexts, confirming the 304

need for more discourse-based studies of prosody. Several recent studies have used 305

narrative data to explore higher level prosodic units in Native American languages, e.g. 306

Beck and Bennett (2007) on Lushootseed, Berez (2011) on Ahtna, and Lovick and Tuttle 307

(2012) on Dena’ina. Along similar lines, Russell (2008) is an acoustic study of vowel 308

coalescence across word boundaries in Plains Cree based on data from two narratives. 309

310

311

312

Page 18: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

18

4. Phonological research on Native American Languages 313

314

Research on languages of the Americas has also played an important role in advances in 315

phonological description and theory over the last century. Already at the turn of the 20th 316

century, Sapir was churning out remarkably detailed descriptions of phonological 317

patterns in a variety of languages. Sapir’s (1912) grammar of Takelma illustrates the 318

precision and scope of his phonological analyses, which comprised a broad range of 319

topics, including phoneme inventories, phonotactic restrictions, segmental alternations 320

(e.g. assimilation, dissimilation, deletion, degemination), and prosody. Many of his 321

phonological descriptions inform phonological theory even today, and the value of his 322

work is heightened by the fact that most of the languages he described are now either 323

moribund or extinct. Sapir’s (1930) description of the phonology of Southern Paiute, for 324

example, has sparked debate about syllable integrity in stress systems (Halle and 325

Vergnaud 1987, Hayes 1995), i.e. whether foot boundaries can split syllables, and the 326

characterization of the relationship between reduplication and segmental alternations 327

(McCarthy and Prince 1995, Gurevich 2000, Raimy 2000). Even Sapir’s textual materials 328

have been consulted in corpus-based phonological studies. Gordon and Luna (2004), for 329

example, analyze the location of stress for a subset of Sapir’s Hupa texts (Sapir and Golla 330

2001), extracting a number of statistical biases in stress placement that have analogs even 331

at later stages of the language. 332

Descriptive work on the phonology of Native American languages has continued 333

unabated over the last century, much of it represented in IJAL articles and publications. 334

Page 19: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

19

Over time, as descriptive coverage of most languages has expanded, increasingly more 335

research has employed data from Native American languages to inform topical issues in 336

phonological theory. Research on Native American languages has played a prominent 337

role in the development and elucidation of many phonological frameworks and 338

formalisms, most recently the constraint-based framework of Optimality Theory (Prince 339

and Smolensky 1993). In many cases, data from older comprehensive grammars of now 340

extinct languages continue to receive extensive treatment in the theoretical literature. The 341

importance of Sapir’s Southern Paiute data for metrical stress theory was mentioned 342

earlier. Similarly, Stanley Newman’s (1944) grammar of Yokuts provided the material 343

for Archangeli’s (1984) seminal dissertation on non-linear phonology and 344

underspecification of features. Hoijer’s (1933) description of syncope and shortening in 345

Tonkawa has likewise provided ample fodder for the study of metrical structure and 346

reduplication (e.g. Phelps 1975, Kenstowicz and Kisseberth 1979, Noske 1993, 347

Gouskova 2003; 2007). 348

The data from these classic works as well as from more recent studies of Native 349

American languages have been instrumental in broadening the typological coverage of a 350

wide range of phenomena, including phonemic inventories, phonotactics, syllable 351

structure, stress, tone, intonation, poetic metrics, prosodic morphology, and prosodic 352

constituency. I cite here just a few of the contributions of phonological research on 353

Native American languages to these topics. Pirahã, which has only seven consonants and 354

eight vowels (Everett 1986), possesses the smallest phoneme inventory in the world 355

(Maddieson 1984), while varieties of Eastern Chatino have perhaps the most complex 356

Page 20: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

20

tone inventories in the world, contrasting as many as twelve tones (Cruz and Woodbury 357

2006, Villard 2009, Campbell and Woodbury 2010). On an historical level, Northern 358

Athabaskan languages, which diverge in their synchronic tonal reflexes of glottal 359

constriction, have played a pivotal role in our understanding of tonogenesis (Kingston 360

2005). The vowelless words of certain Salishan languages, e.g. Nuxalk (Bagemihl 1991), 361

present challenges to theories that assume the universality of syllables and sonority 362

sequencing principles governing phonotactics. Sibilant harmony systems of the type 363

found in Chumashan and Athabaskan have played a prominent role in the development of 364

theories of long-distance assimilation (Gafos 1999, Hansson 2001, Poser 2004, McCarthy 365

2007). Several intonation systems reported for languages of the Americas, e.g. Wari’ 366

(Everett and Kern 1997) and Chickasaw (Gordon 2005a), violate the overwhelming 367

cross-linguistic tendency for the default intonational contour in declaratives to be 368

characterized by a terminal fall. Work by Hinton (1984; 1990) on Havasupai and 369

Fitzgerald (1998) on Tohono O’odham provide much needed breadth to typological 370

knowledge of linguistic properties of sung verse. The stress system of Pirahã (Everett 371

and Everett 1984, Everett 1986) has provided the impetus for research on onset-sensitive 372

weight (Topintzi 2004; 2010, Gordon 2005b) with further reinforcement from the 373

sensitivity of the minimal word requirement to onset complexity in Yakima Sahaptin 374

(Hargus and Beavert 2006). The prosodic system of Kashaya (Oswalt 1988, Buckley 375

1994; 2013) has necessitated expansion of the theory of extrametricality to include cases 376

at the left edge of prosodic domains. The scalar weight hierarchies of Asheninca (Payne 377

1990) and Nanti (Crowhurst and Michael 2005), which are simultaneously sensitive to 378

Page 21: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

21

vowel quality, vowel length and the presence vs. absence of a coda consonant, present 379

challenges to the most commonly adopted theory of weight, moraic theory (Hayes 1989). 380

Native American languages of the Pacific Northwest have provided virtually the entire 381

corpus of material underlying the debate about the role of phonetic factors in analyzing 382

the phasing of laryngeal and oral gestures relative to each other in glottalized sonorants 383

(e.g. Plauché et al. 1998, Steriade 1999, Howe and Pulleyblank 2001, Shaw et al. 2005, 384

Bird et al. 2008, Bird 2011). 385

386

5. Four case studies of the phonetics and phonology of Native American Languages 387

388

This section presents four case studies illustrating ways in which work on Native 389

American languages have contributed to the fields of phonetics and phonology. The first 390

three case studies focus on prosodic features, including iambic metrical structure (5.1), 391

weight-sensitive tone restrictions (5.2), and prosodic morphology (5.3). The final case 392

study (5.4) deals with an area of ever increasing importance as the number of endangered 393

languages grows: phonological and phonetic characteristics of semi-speakers and their 394

acquisition by second language learners. Focus in the case studies, particularly the first 395

three, is on languages of the Southeastern United States, which have played a prominent 396

role in furthering our understanding of phonetics and phonology over the last century. 397

398

399

400

Page 22: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

22

5.1. Iambic metrical structure 401

402

The most widely adopted metrical stress theory, that proposed in Hayes (1987; 1995) 403

assumes an asymmetric inventory of feet consisting of three types. According to this 404

theory, trochaic feet may, on a language-specific basis, either be weight-sensitive or not, 405

whereas iambic feet are universally weight-sensitive. The three types of feet in Hayes’ 406

proposed inventory are shown schematically in (1), where the metrically strong element 407

is marked with an ‘x’. 408

409

(1) Asymmetric foot inventory (Hayes 1987; 1995) 410

411

Syllabic Trochee Moraic Trochee Iamb

x

(σ σ)

x

(µ µ)

x

(µ µ)

412

As (1) shows, canonical weight-sensitive feet, including both the moraic trochee and the 413

iamb, consist of two moras (assigned to vowels and, on a language-specific basis, coda 414

consonants), whereas weight-insensitive feet contain two syllables. Hayes grounds his 415

foot inventory in data from numerous psychological experiments in which listeners prefer 416

trochaic groupings for alternating loud and soft stimuli that are durationally balanced but 417

iambic groupings for alternating long and short stimuli that are balanced in terms of 418

loudness. Results of these experiments suggest an inherent sensitivity to duration in the 419

Page 23: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

23

case of iambs but not trochees. Hayes finds linguistic support for this asymmetry between 420

the two foot types in his cross-linguistic survey of stress systems, showing that iambic 421

stress languages are overwhelmingly (if not exclusively) weight-sensitive, whereas 422

trochaic stress languages vary in their weight-sensitivity. 423

Virtually all of Hayes’ (1995) survey of iambic languages is based on languages 424

of the Americas (many of which are genetically diverse), including Hixkaryana, Creek, 425

Chickasaw, Choctaw, Delaware, Malecite-Passamaquoddy, Eastern Ojibwa, Menomini, 426

Potawatomi, Cayuga, Seneca, Onondaga, Yupik, Hopi, Sierra Miwok, and Maidu. The 427

Southeastern languages Chickasaw, Choctaw and Creek contribute some of the especially 428

key case studies illustrating iambic metrical structure. These are discussed in the next 429

two sections. 430

431

5.1.1. Iambic lengthening in Chickasaw 432

433

One of the pervasive features of iambic systems, exemplified by Chickasaw and 434

Choctaw, is iambic lengthening, a phenomenon whereby metrically strong phonemic 435

short vowels in open syllables lengthen to create a durationally unbalanced short-long 436

foot just like the preferred iambs in the psychological experiments discussed above. 437

Examples of iambic lengthening in Chickasaw (Munro and Ulrich 1984, Munro and 438

Willmond 1994; 2008, Gordon and Munro 2007) appear in (2), where lengthened vowels 439

are indicated by an IPA half-length symbol. As in other languages with weight-sensitive 440

iambs, feet in Chickasaw may consist of two light syllables, a single heavy syllable 441

Page 24: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

24

(closed syllables and those with long vowels in Chickasaw), or a light followed by a 442

heavy syllable. In Chickasaw, unlike many other iambic stress language, a final light 443

syllable immediately following a strong syllable also constitutes a foot on its own. Note 444

that primary stress in the Chickasaw examples preferentially docks on a long or 445

lengthened vowel and otherwise on the final syllables in words lacking a long vowel. 446

447

(2) Iambic lengthening in Chickasaw 448

449

(aˈbiˑ)(kaˌtok) ‘s/he was sick’

(aˌsaˑ)(biˈkaˑ)(ˌtok) ‘I was sick’

(kiˈsiˑ)(liˌtok) ‘I bit it’

(ʧiˌkiˑ)(siˈliˑ)(ˌtok) ‘I bit you’

(ʧoˌkoʃ)(koˈmoˑ)(ˌtok) ‘s/he played’

(aˈsaˑ)(biˌka) ‘I am sick’

(ʧoˌkoʃ)(koˈmo) ‘s/he plays’

(aˈbiˑ)(ˌka) ‘s/he is sick’

(kiˈsiˑ)(ˌli) ‘I bite it’

450

A curious feature of languages with iambic lengthening is the apparent absence 451

(according to phonological descriptions) of lengthening in final syllables even if they are 452

open and metrically strong, e.g. (ʧoˌkoʃ)(koˈmo) not (ʧoˌkoʃ)(koˈmoˑ), (aˈsaˑ)(biˌka) not 453

(aˌsaˑ)(biˈkaˑ). The failure of lengthening to apply word-finally is particularly puzzling 454

Page 25: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

25

given that final position is cross-linguistically a common locus of phonetic lengthening in 455

languages as diverse as Taiwanese (Peng 1997), Yoruba (Nagano-Madsen 1992), 456

Inuktitut (Nagano-Madsen 1992), English (Klatt 1975, Umeda 1975, Wightman et al. 457

1992), Hungarian (Hockey and Fagyal 1999), and Chickasaw’s Muskogean relative 458

Creek (Johnson and Martin 2001). One might thus expect final syllables to be most 459

conducive to iambic lengthening. 460

Gordon and Munro (2007) explore the puzzling absence of lengthening final 461

stressed syllables in a phonetic study of final vowel length under different metrical 462

conditions in Chickasaw. Since all final syllables in Chickasaw are metrically strong as 463

evidenced by stress patterns (Munro 1996, Gordon 2004), the relevant comparisons are 464

between final vowels that are part of a disyllabic foot consisting of two light syllables and 465

final vowels that consist of a single light (CV) syllable. Gordon and Munro (2007) also 466

compare vowels that are word-final but phrase-internal with those that are phrase-final 467

but not utterance-final and those that are utterance-final. They find that all final vowels 468

undergo lengthening regardless of the size of the foot to which they belong; thus, the final 469

vowel in (aˈbiˑ)(ˌka) ‘s/he is sick’ is equivalent in length to the final vowel in 470

(aˈsaˑ)(biˌka) ‘I am sick’ despite the fact that the rightmost foot is monosyllabic in the 471

first word but disyllabic in the second. They further discover that word-final lengthening 472

is cumulative across domains of different sizes: greatest utterance-finally, slightly smaller 473

phrase-finally but utterance-medially, and smallest in magnitude phrase-medially. Much 474

of the length (almost 40% averaged across speakers) of utterance-final vowels, however, 475

is associated with breathiness, unlike phrase-final and word-final vowels (that are not 476

Page 26: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

26

utterance-final), which have considerably shorter breathy phases: 17% for phrase-final 477

vowels and 10% for word-final vowels. The cumulative nature of final lengthening and 478

breathiness across domains of different sizes is consistent with cross-linguistic patterns, 479

leading Gordon and Munro (2007) to suggest that the natural tendency for final 480

lengthening, which can be associated with slowing down of articulatory gestures without 481

necessarily any increase in gestural magnitude (see Edwards et al. 1991, Beckman et al. 482

1992), plausibly supersedes lengthening due to metrical prominence, which unlike final 483

lengthening is attributed to an increase in gestural magnitude. 484

Another interesting finding of Gordon and Munro’s (2007) study is that word-485

medial lengthened vowels remain durationally distinct from phonemic long vowels (at 486

least for certain speakers). The phonemic long /iː/ in (tiː)(kãːʔ)(tiʔ) ‘dirt dauber (wasp)’ 487

for you’ is thus longer than the lengthened /iˑ/ in (satiˑ)(kah)(bi) ‘I am tired’. Iambic 488

lengthening in Chickasaw thus falls in the class of near-neutralizing alternations, which 489

makes it problematic to capture using discrete units of weight like the mora. Under 490

Hayes’ (1995) account, iambic lengthening creates a heavy, i.e. bimoraic, strong syllable; 491

however, assuming that the lengthened vowel is bimoraic incorrectly implies that it is 492

equivalent in length to phonemic long vowels. It is hoped that future phonetic study of 493

iambic lengthening in other languages will determine whether it triggers near or complete 494

neutralization of underlying length distinctions. Study of other languages in which final 495

short vowels are only stressed when part of a disyllabic foot (unlike in Chickasaw) will 496

also reveal whether the final lengthening patterns in Chickasaw are potentially an artifact 497

of the final stress or are also observed in final unstressed vowels. 498

Page 27: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

27

5.1.2. High tone assignment in Creek 499

500

Not all iambic languages manifest metrical strength through vowel lengthening. Another 501

Muskogean language, Creek, has a high tone that occurs in a location that is predictable 502

from iambic foot structure. As described by Haas (1977) and Martin (2011) and 503

instrumentally verified by Martin and Johnson (2002), the default high tone in Creek 504

words lacking a lexically specified accent spans from the leftmost to the rightmost 505

metrically strong syllable, either an initial heavy syllable containing a long vowel or a 506

coda consonant, or the second syllable if the first syllable is light. Metrical structure and 507

tonal patterns in Creek are illustrated in figure 4 for the words (atʃok)(ɬán)wa ‘daddy 508

longlegs’ and (apa)(taná) ‘bullfrog’. In the second form, the high tone span continues 509

from the second vowel all the way through most of the final vowel, which is metrically 510

strong. In the first form, in contrast, there is a precipitous fall in F0 starting at the 511

beginning of the final vowel, which is metrically unparsed. 512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

Page 28: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

28

Time (s)0 0.6971

Pitc

h (H

z)

100

150

521 522 523 524 525 528 529 530 531

532 a tʃ o k ɬ a n w a a p a t a n a 533 534 Figure 4. Iambic high tone assignment in the Creek words (atʃok)(ɬán)wa ‘daddy 535

longlegs’ (on left) and (apa)(taná) ‘bullfrog’ (on right) 536

In a study of vowel quality and duration, Johnson and Martin (2001) find that 537

final vowels, both phonemically short and long, are centralized relative to their initial 538

counterparts despite being longer, a result that suggests that final position triggers both a 539

slowing down of articulatory gestures and a decrease in tongue displacement in Creek. 540

This combination of results is consistent with the hypothesis advanced above that final 541

lengthening in Chickasaw has a fundamentally different articulatory source from 542

lengthening associated with metrical prominence. 543

544

5.2. Weight-sensitive tone 545

546

Another phenomenon informed by research on languages of the Southeastern United 547

States is weight-sensitive tone, the restriction against contour tones on light syllables 548

observed in many languages (Clark 1983, Hyman 1988, Gordon 2001, Zhang 2002). If 549

Time (s)0 0.9756

Pitc

h (H

z)

100

175

H H

Page 29: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

29

contour tones are viewed compositionally as combinations of low plus high tones (in 550

either order), restrictions against them can be captured in theories of weight, such as 551

moraic theory, as a one-to-one upper limit on associations between tones and units of 552

weight (Woo 1969). In languages imposing this constraint on tone-to-weight mappings, 553

the configuration in (3b) with two tones linked to a single mora is not licit, whereas the 554

same pattern is permitted in a language that does not place weight-sensitive restrictions 555

on the licensing of contour tones. One-to-one mappings between tones and moras, as in 556

(3a) and (3d), are allowed in both types of languages as are mappings of a single tone to 557

multiple moras, as in (3c). 558

559

(3) Various tone-to-mora mappings in languages with and without weight-sensitive tone 560

561

(a) σ

µ

T

(b) σ

µ

T T

(c) σ

µ µ

T

(d) σ

µ µ

T T Weight-sensitive tone √ * √ √ No weight-sensitive tone √ √ √ √ 562 Contour tone restrictions are illustrated by Koasati (Gordon et al. 2015), in which a 563

subset of nouns carry a rising tone and this rising tone is (almost exclusively) limited to 564

syllables containing a long vowel (abbreviated CVV) or syllables closed by a coda 565

sonorant (abbreviated CVR). Examples of lexical rising tone in Koasati appear in (4), 566

followed by a pitch trace of a word with rising tone in figure 5. 567

568

569

Page 30: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

30

Time (s)0 0.8256

Pitc

h (H

z)

125

200

(4) Lexical rising tone on CVV and CVR in Koasati 570

571

hopǒːni ‘a cook’

tǎlwa ‘singer’

pokko tǒːli ‘ball player’

naːɬǐːka ‘speaker’

mobǐːla ‘car’

athǒmma ‘Indian’

572 573 574 575 576 577

578 579 580 581 582 h o p ǒː n i 583 584 585

Figure 5. Lexical rising tone on the penult of the Koasati noun hopǒːni ‘a cook’ 586

587

A similar confinement of phonological contour tones to CVV and CVR syllables 588

is also found in Caddo (Chafe 1976) and in Koasati’s linguistic relative Creek (Martin 589

2011). Caddo differs from Koasati, however, in permitting only falling and not rising 590

tones. 591

H L

Page 31: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

31

Oklahoma Cherokee also has a restriction against contour tones, which differs 592

from the Koasati and Caddo pattern in permitting contours only on syllables containing a 593

long vowel (Wright 1996, Uchihara 2013). Oklahoma Cherokee also has a more complex 594

tone inventory than the Koasati and Caddo data discussed thus far in featuring both rising 595

and falling tones. Examples of rising and falling tone on long vowels in Cherokee appear 596

in (5) (from Uchihara 2013:172). 597

598

599

(5) Contour tones on CVV in Oklahoma Cherokee 600

601

602

603

Because it is realized along the same physical dimension of fundamental 604

frequency, intonation in many languages is subject to similar restrictions on pitch 605

excursions to those governing lexical tone. For example, imperatives in Koasati are 606

marked with a terminal high-low fall in pitch, which is realized in its canonical form on 607

final syllables that end in a sonorant coda. (Koasati lacks final long vowels.) If, however, 608

the final syllable is not CVR (which also licenses low-high lexical rises) the high-low fall 609

kʰijûːka ‘chipmunk’

kakʰâːneha ‘he is giving him a living thing’

kʰawǒːnu ‘duck’

kaleːjʌˇːska ‘A long object is falling from an

upright position’

Page 32: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

32

H H

L

is truncated to a simple high tone. The canonical and truncated versions of the terminal 610

high-low fall in Koasati imperatives are illustrated for the words ishol ‘you all take it!’ 611

and halatk ‘grab on!’, respectively, in figure 6. Note that both words begin with a 612

lowered baseline pitch (not attributed to the imperative), which accounts for the initial 613

rise to the high tone. 614

615

616 i s h o l h a l a t k 617 618 Figure 6. Falling tone in Koasati imperative ishol ‘you all take it!’ (on left) and simple 619

high tone in Koasati imperative halatk ‘grab on!’ (on right) 620

621

The Koasati/Caddo type (CVV, CVR heavy) and Cherokee type (CVV heavy) 622

tone restrictions are typologically very common in the world and find an explanation in 623

terms of phonetic considerations. The acoustic correlate of tone is fundamental 624

frequency, which is most effectively realized on a sonorous sound, where vowels provide 625

a slightly better backdrop than sonorant consonants. Obstruents are least conducive to 626

supporting fundamental frequency information. Because contour tones involve a 627

fundamental frequency excursion, they generally require more time to implement than 628

Time (s)0 0.5972

Pitc

h (H

z)

100

250

Time (s)0 0.5456

Pitc

h (H

z)

100

275

Page 33: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

33

level tones. For this reason, a long vowel provides the best docking site for a contour 629

tone, slightly better than a sequence of a short vowel plus sonorant consonant. Both CVV 630

and CVR are far superior in their tone-bearing capacity to either a short vowel in an open 631

syllable or one followed by an obstruent coda (see Zhang 2002 for further discussion). 632

Languages like Cherokee that restrict contour tones to CVV thus impose the most 633

stringent requirement on contour tones, whereas those like Koasati and Caddo that permit 634

contour tones on both CVV and CVR employ a slightly less strict weight criterion. 635

Predicted not to occur given phonetic grounding is a hypothetical language in which 636

syllables closed by an obstruent are able to support contour tones but those closed by a 637

sonorant are not or a hypothetical language in which CVR can carry contours but CVV 638

cannot. 639

Tonal restrictions may also be sensitive to the type of contour tone. Zhang (2002) 640

observes that rising tones are subject to more stringent restrictions than falling tones 641

cross-linguistically. Many languages thus have falling but not rising tones, while others 642

restrict rising tones more severely in terms of the contexts in which they may occur. This 643

asymmetry between the two types of contours also falls out from phonetic considerations 644

discussed in Zhang (2002): rising tones take longer to execute than falling tones. One 645

would thus predict that tolerance of rising tones in a language predicts the occurrence of 646

falling tones in that language. 647

The Caddo and Creek data discussed thus far display the predicted type of 648

asymmetry in contour tone licensing, since they possess falling but not rising tones. 649

Koasati, however, contradicts this pattern in having rising but not falling lexical tones 650

Page 34: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

34

(though it does have falling intonational contours, as shown earlier), suggesting that the 651

distribution of contour tones is not necessarily always predictable on phonetic grounds 652

synchronically. 653

As it turns out, consideration of the tonal history of the Muskogean language 654

family to which Koasati and Creek both belong sheds some light on the apparently 655

anomalous behavior of the Koasati tone system synchronically. As Martin (2013) and 656

Gordon et al. (2015) show, rising tone in Koasati corresponds to falling tone in some 657

other Muskogean languages, including Koasati’s close relative Alabama, e.g. Koasati 658

tǎlwa vs. Alabama tậlwa ‘singer’ (Gordon et al. 2015:115). Martin (2013) and Gordon et 659

al. (2015) suggest that falling tone reflects the original pattern and that the rising tone of 660

Koasati resulted historically from the typologically common phonetic phenomenon of 661

peak delay (e.g. Silverman and Pierrehumbert 1990, Prieto et al. 1995, Xu 2001), 662

whereby the fundamental frequency peak is shifted rightward phonetically. As a result of 663

peak delay, the syllable originally associated with the falling tone became phonetically 664

associated with an F0 rise, eventually being analyzed as carrying a rising tone instead. 665

The Koasati reanalysis provides an illustration of how one phonetically natural event, in 666

this case, peak delay, can trigger a typologically uncommon (and phonetically 667

unpredicted) phonological pattern, in this case, the occurrence of a phonological rising 668

but not a falling tone. 669

670

671

672

Page 35: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

35

5.3. Prosodic morphology 673

674

Prosodic morphology is a cover term for a cluster of phenomena sensitive to the 675

phonological shape of morphemes and formally united in a research program initiated by 676

McCarthy and Prince (1986/1996). One property falling under the purview of prosodic 677

morphology is the class of templatic morphological operations characterized by the 678

selection of a prosodically defined morpheme. The most pervasive type of templatic 679

morphology is reduplication, a process of word formation (often conveying meanings 680

such as intensification, repeated action, or plurality) associated with the copying of a 681

morpheme or part of a morpheme. The examples in (6) illustrate the marking of plurality 682

through reduplication in Tunica data appearing in an early IJAL article by Swanton 683

(1921). 684

685

(6) Reduplication in Tunica (Swanton 1921:5-6) 686

687

Base Gloss Reduplication Gloss

miːliː ‘red miːliːmiːliːta ‘many red things’

meːliː ‘black’ meːliːmeːliːta ‘many black things’

toːluː ‘round’ toːluːtoːluːta ‘many round things’

koːra ‘to drink’ koːkoːra ‘drink (pl.)’

Page 36: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

36

As the forms from Tunica indicate, the shape of the reduplicant can vary. In these data, 688

the first three forms thus display reduplication of the entire base, while the last form 689

undergoes reduplication of the first CV sequence. 690

A salient, though not universal feature, of reduplication is that the reduplicant 691

adheres to a defined template rather than merely copying a constituent of the root. This 692

property is evident in the Creek data in (7), which illustrate reduplication in the 693

distributive of intransitive verbs (Martin 2011). 694

695

(7) CV reduplication in Creek (Martin 2011:203-204) 696

697

Base Gloss Reduplication Gloss

(a) likátʃw-iː ‘dirty’ likatʃliw-íː (two or more)

hopánk-iː ‘broken’ hopanhok-íː (two or more)

(b) apoːk-itá ‘(three or more)

to sit, live’

apoːpok-itá (in several places)

tóːsk-iː ‘mangy’ toːstok-íː (two or more)

(c) hátk-iː ‘white’ háthak-íː (two or more)

tolk-itá ‘to fall over’ toltok-íta (two or more)

698

The reduplicant is characteristically a copy of the first CV sequence of the root and falls 699

to the left of the final consonant of the root (7a). The fixed nature of the reduplicative 700

template as CV is demonstrated by the fact that a long vowel in the first syllable of the 701

Page 37: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

37

root surfaces as short in the reduplicant (7b) and a coda consonant is not copied (7c). The 702

CV reduplicant template employed in Creek is one of many attested in languages 703

throughout the world. Others include a single heavy syllable (either CVV or CVC), a 704

long vowel (CVV), a disyllabic sequence, and an entire root. 705

A relatively rare type of reduplication involves copying of a single segment. The 706

data in (8) and (9) illustrate consonant reduplication in Jakalktek (Day 1973) and vowel 707

reduplication in Nuxalk (Nater 1984:109), respectively. 708

709

(8) Consonant reduplication in Jakaltek (Day 1973:45) 710

Base Gloss Reduplication Gloss

piʦ’-a ‘squeeze s.th.

gently’

ʧa piʦ’p-e ‘you squeeze s.th.

gently several times’

onom juk-e

‘noise and/or

motion of leaves

on shaken trees’

ʧa jukj-e ‘you shake s.th.’

711

(9) Vowel reduplication in Nuxalk (Nater 1984:109) 712

713

Base Gloss Reduplication Gloss

t’ixɬala ‘robin’ ʔit’ixɬala-j (diminutive)

k’n̩ts ‘sperm whale’ ʔn̩k’n̩ts (diminutive)

714

Page 38: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

38

Reduplication can also involve two copies or “triplication”, as in the data in (10) 715

from Stát’imcets (Shaw 2005, van Eijk 1997) combining diminutive and 716

plural/collectivity reduplication (with accompanying glottalization of the sonorant in the 717

second set of examples). 718

719

(10) Multiple reduplication in Stát’imcets (van Eijk 1997:62, Shaw 2005:177) 720

721

s-ˈqaχaʔ Nom-Root ‘dog’

s-ˈqəә-qχaʔ Nom-DIM-Root ‘puppy’

s-qəәχ-ˈqəә-qχaʔ Nom-PL-DIM -Root ‘puppies’

s-ˈjaqʧaʔ Nom-Root ‘woman’

s-ˈjʔəә-jʔqʧaʔ Nom-DIM-Root +[CG] ‘girl’

s-ˈjəәq-jʔəә-ˈjʔqʧaʔ Nom-PL-DIM-Root +[CG] ‘girls’

722

It is also possible for reduplication to involve copying in conjunction with an 723

invariant segment. Koasati displays this variety of “fixed segmentism” reduplication in 724

the construction termed “punctual reduplication” by Kimball (1991), in which the first 725

consonant of the root plus the vowel [oː] is inserted before the final syllable of the root. 726

727

728

729

Page 39: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

39

(11) Fixed segmentism in Koasati punctual reduplication (Kimball 1991:325-6) 730

731

tahaspin tahastoːpin ‘to be light in weight’

lapatkin lapatloːkin ‘to be narrow’

talasban talastoːban ‘to be thin’

ɬimihkon ɬimihɬoːkin ‘to be smooth’

ʧofoknan ʧofokʧoːnan ‘to be angled’

732

As stated earlier, a salient characteristic of reduplication is its adherence to a 733

template that is not dependent on the shape of the base. Although this feature is nearly 734

universal, there are rare instances of reduplication varying as a function of the shape of 735

the base. One such exceptional case involves syllable reduplication in the habitual aspect 736

in the Uto-Aztecan language Hiaki (Haugen 2003; 2014). In Hiaki, the shape of the 737

reduplicant varies between CV and CVC to match the shape of the first syllable of the 738

root (12). 739

740

(12) Syllable reduplication in Hiaki (Haugen 2014:511) 741

742

Template Base Reduplication Gloss

CV vu.sa vu.vu.sa ‘awaken’

ʧi.ke ʧi.ʧi.ke ‘comb one’s hair’

he.wi.te he.he.wi.te ‘agree’

Page 40: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

40

CVC vamse vam.vam.se ‘hurry’

hit.ta hit.hit.ta ‘make a fire’

ʔat.bʷa ʔat.ʔat.bʷa ‘laugh at’

bʷal.ko.te bʷal.bʷal.ko.te ‘soften, smooth’

743

Reduplication is not the only type of templatic morphological operation attested 744

cross-linguistically. Another variety of templatic morphology involves truncation of 745

material from the base to adhere to a fixed shape, as in hypocoristic formation in 746

Japanese (Poser 1990), in which the abbreviated name adheres to a bimoraic template 747

consisting of two light syllables, e.g. takatɯgɯ → taka-ʨaɴ, taɺoː → taɺo-ʨaɴ, or a 748

single heavy syllable, e.g. joːko → joː-ʨaɴ. 749

Although the template in most cases of truncation is enforced on the preserved 750

material (as in Japanese), there is another type of truncation, in which the template 751

instead holds of the deleted (or subtracted) material. Subtractive morphology is illustrated 752

by the Koasati plurals in (13), which are formed by deleting the final rime of the root 753

(either a short vowel + coda consonant or a long vowel) and adding the suffixes –ka or –li 754

followed by –n (Kimball 1983; 1991, Martin 1988). 755

756

757

758

759

Page 41: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

41

(13) Subtractive morphology in Koasati (Martin 1988:230-1) 760

761

Singular Plural Gloss

lataf-kan lat-kan ‘to kick something’

lasap-lin lap-lin ‘to lick something’

misip-lin mis-lin ‘to wink’

tipas-lin tip-lin ‘to pick something off’

atakaː-lin atak-lin ‘to hang something’

albitiː-lin albit-lin ‘to place on top of’

aʧokʧanaː-kan aʧokʧan-kan ‘to quarrel with someone’

762

In summary, Native American languages have contributed substantially to typological 763

knowledge of prosodic morphology, including the range of variation in the shape of 764

templates and the types of phenomena employing those templates. 765

766

5.4. Phonetics and phonology in language endangerment and revitalization 767

768

The decline in language usage in many Native American communities has spawned new 769

areas of engagement that transcend the traditional boundaries of scholarly research. 770

Already at the time of IJAL’s inception, Boas (1917a) had recognized the profound 771

impact that extensive contact with European languages had exerted on the phonetic 772

characteristics of Native American languages, citing as an example the reduced strength 773

Page 42: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

42

of the glottalized consonants found in languages of the Pacific Coast. The influence of 774

language contact has become increasingly more pervasive over the last century, spurring 775

linguists and community members to investigate the ways in which languages are shaped 776

by other languages, the differences between the speech of native speakers and that of 777

semi-speakers of a language, and the mechanism of language acquisition for second 778

language speakers of Native American languages. On a practical level, understanding of 779

these issues informs methodologies for effectively teaching second language learners. An 780

important thread of this work, both theoretical and applied, is the inextricable relationship 781

it assumes between the language itself and its speakers (or learners) for whom language is 782

an essential marker of cultural identity. 783

Rankin’s (1978) paper on the phonemes of Quapaw, which had at the time of his 784

work recently lost its last native speaker, made clear the complex nature of phonological 785

change associated with language desuetude in the face of language contact (see also Cook 786

1989 on Tsuut’ina and Dëne Sųłiné, the latter of which is also the subject of a 787

longitudinal acquisition study in Cook 2006). Although certain shifts he reports likely 788

reflect the influence of English, e.g. the deglottalization of glottalized fricatives and 789

stops, the change of retroflex fricatives to palato-alveolars and ultimately clusters of 790

alveolar fricative plus /r/, other developments were not plausibly attributed to English. 791

For example, for semi-speakers whose parents had been fluent and who still could 792

produce many words (between 150 and 300) and some short sentences, the contrast 793

between aspirated and unaspirated stops was neutralized to the unaspirated series even in 794

pre-tonic contexts where English has aspiration. Rankin (1978) suggests that this shift is 795

Page 43: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

43

attributed to the less marked status of unaspirated stops relative to aspirated stops, as 796

reflected in cross-linguistic frequency patterns and ultimately attributed to phonetic 797

factors such as ease of articulation and perceptual salience. He offers a similar 798

explanation for the sporadic preservation of glottalization in stops for the oldest 799

generation of non-fluent speakers (represented by a woman in her 70s in Rankin’s study), 800

even though they completely lost glottalization in fricatives, which is considerably rarer 801

cross-linguistically. As English continued to make inroads, Quapaw’s phonology became 802

increasingly English-like, even at the expense of increasing markedness. Thus, the 803

youngest generation of speakers in his study had completely adopted the allophonic 804

distribution of aspiration found in English. 805

Although Rankin’s study clearly demonstrates the profound nature of 806

phonological change among speakers not using a language regularly, it leaves open the 807

relative contribution of external factors such as language contact as opposed to internal 808

pressures such as markedness. Much of the uncertainty arising in the interpretation of 809

Rankin’s results stems from the inherent elusiveness of the notion of markedness, which 810

in the case of the Quapaw data could be attributed to frequency or phonetic naturalness. 811

For example, a statistical predominance of unaspirated stops in Quapaw relative to their 812

aspirated counterparts could have led to their adoption by speakers neutralizing the two 813

series. Alternatively, the preservation of the unaspirated stops might have been attributed 814

to their greater phonetic “naturalness”. Investigation of the source of the aspiration 815

patterns in Quapaw would also have been aided by a phonetic study to determine whether 816

voice-onset-time was in fact influenced on a low level by stress as in English. 817

Page 44: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

44

Haynes (2010) attempts to tease apart the many factors, both linguistic and 818

cultural, driving language change in a detailed production and perception study of 819

phonetic characteristics of Numu (Oregon Northern Paiute) among four populations 820

living in or near the Warm Springs, Oregon tribal community. The first group included 821

fluent speakers of Numu from Warm Springs. The second group consisted of members of 822

the Warm Springs community who were not fluent but had experienced direct exposure 823

to Numu through family members or classes. The third population included Warm Spring 824

community members with only ambient exposure to Numu, though some had exposure to 825

another (genetically unrelated) Native American language spoken in the Warm Springs 826

community. The fourth group consisted of residents of the nearby town of Madras who 827

reported no exposure to Numu. 828

Haynes (2010) examines a number of phonological and phonetic characteristics of 829

Numu as reproduced by non-fluent speakers mimicking a list of words uttered by fluent 830

speakers in order to examine the extent to which populations with differing degrees of 831

exposure to Numu are able to replicate features absent from English and to determine the 832

source of discrepancies between the three groups in their performance. Her results 833

suggest a number of linguistic and social factors at work. 834

As expected, there was considerable variation among non-fluent speakers even 835

those belonging to the same broad categories adopted by Haynes. Furthermore, intertoken 836

variation was also observed even within speakers, a result that mirrors data from 837

Rankin’s (1978) study. Also in keeping with Rankin (1978), Haynes (2010) finds that 838

many sounds and contrasts that are not found in English are either replaced or 839

Page 45: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

45

phonetically altered in a direction that is predictable from English influence. In some 840

cases, exposure to Numu confers some advantage in the ability to replicate native speaker 841

pronunciation. For example, the initial affricate /ts/ in Numu is preserved in gradient 842

fashion in proportion to a speaker’s level of exposure to Numu. Similarly, in a 843

comparison of fortis and lenis stops, which differ from each other along a number of 844

dimensions including voice-onset-time, duration, and burst intensity, Haynes (2010) finds 845

that non-fluent speakers exaggerate VOT differences but reduce durational differences 846

between the two stop series in proportion to their degree of exposure to Numu, where the 847

group with the most exposure to Numu is most accurate in its phonetic reproduction of 848

the fortis vs. lenis contrast. Only the non-fluent group with the greatest exposure to Numu 849

is able to reproduce the burst intensity distinction between the two sets of stops. 850

In other cases, there is no clear advantage associated with increased exposure to 851

Numu. For example, the uvularization of /k/ before the vowels /a/ and /ɔ/ is not reliably 852

produced by non-fluent speakers regardless of their level of experience to Numu. All 853

non-fluent groups also decrease the duration ratio between intervocalic geminate and 854

singleton nasals relative to the fluent speakers, primarily due to lengthening of the 855

singleton series, a result that Haynes (2010) speculates is due to the lack of a gemination 856

contrast in English. Similarly, influence of English likely contributes to the compacted 857

vowel space of Numu relative to that of non-fluent speakers, especially in the central and 858

back space, where Numu contrasts /ɨ/, /u/, /ɔ/, and /a/. 859

Haynes (2010) also observes evidence of hypercorrection in her data. Non-fluent 860

speakers from the two Warm Springs groups but not the Madras group thus often apply 861

Page 46: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

46

final vowel devoicing even in contexts where fluent speakers do not, which Haynes 862

suggests is attributed to devoicing being identified as a distinguishing characteristic of 863

Numu that is being overapplied by non-native speakers. Another interesting finding is the 864

spontaneous production of ejectives by participants belonging to both Warm Springs 865

groups even though Numu lacks ejectives. Haynes (2010) hypothesizes that the ejective 866

productions are due to the occurrence of ejectives in two other Native American 867

languages spoken in Warm Springs, Kiksht and Ichishkin, which has lead non-fluent 868

Numu speakers to associates ejectives with Native American languages, even those like 869

Numu that lack them. She suggests that this type of overgeneralization potentially offers 870

an explanation for the common linguistic phenomenon of areal spreading of features, 871

including ejectives, an areal feature of Northwest Native American languages. 872

In a companion perception study in Haynes (2010), tokens produced by non-873

fluent speakers and containing non-native features of Numu were played to two native 874

speaker listeners to ascertain which properties contribute most to a perceived non-native 875

accent. There was considerable variation between the two speakers in their tolerance of 876

non-native features and their sensitivity to different features as markers of a non-native 877

accent. Some of the more robust findings, however, were that deviations from the native 878

devoicing patterns triggered relatively strong non-native judgments, as did non-native 879

productions of the fortis vs. lenis contrast. Interestingly, however, differences in VOT, on 880

which non-native speakers relied in producing the contrast, did not strongly induce non-881

native judgments in the perception task. Ejectives only exerted an effect on judgments for 882

one of the two listeners. Social factors such as the individual and the gender of the 883

Page 47: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

47

speaker (males perceived as less native-like) also impacted perceptions of native 884

pronunciation. 885

Haynes’ (2010) results highlight the considerable complexity of the mapping 886

between speech characteristics of native speakers of Native American languages and 887

those with varying degrees of proficiency. Besides the expected influence of a socially 888

dominant language like English the cultural association of particular properties with a 889

language, whether actually part of that language or not, may exert an impact on the 890

production of non-native speakers. Her results also indicate that properties that appear to 891

be most divergent between native and non-native speakers in production may not 892

necessarily be those that listeners identify as critical to perceived linguistic proficiency on 893

the part of the listener. As Haynes (p. 128) points out, this result suggests that “Numu 894

[language] (and other endangered languages in similar social contexts) may continue to 895

be an authentic marker of Numu culture, even if it incorporates some of the features of 896

second language learner speech”. 897

898

6. Prospects for the future 899

900

The scope and volume of recent studies attest to the vitality of the research program 901

devoted to the phonetics and phonology of Native American languages. This is 902

demonstrated not only by work published in IJAL, but also by articles appearing in 903

diverse journals and by recent edited books focused on phonetic and phonological topics 904

(e.g. Hargus and Rice 2005, Goodwin Gómez and Van der Voort 2014, Avelino et al. 905

Page 48: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

48

2016). Concomitant with the expanded range of research, there has been a geographic 906

burgeoning in the form of vastly increased work on the phonology and phonetics of 907

Central and South American languages. This trend is amply reflected in the pages of 908

IJAL over the last decade (e.g. Hintz 2006, Malone 2006; 2010, Stenzel 2007, Elías Ulloa 909

2009, Maddieson et al. 2009, Everett 2011, Dicanio 2012, Clopper and Tonhauser 2013, 910

Michael et al. 2013, Noyer 2013, Crowhurst and Trechter 2014, Caballero and Carroll 911

2015) and reflects tremendous progress from the state of affairs at the time Boas 912

(1917a:1) wrote in his introduction to the inaugural issue of IJAL “it is not saying too 913

much if we claim that for most of the native languages of Central and South America the 914

field is practically terra incognita”. 915

916

REFERENCES 917

918

ARCHANGELI, DIANA. 1984. Underspecification in Yawelmani Phonology and 919

Morphology. Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT (Published in 1988 by Garland Publishing, 920

New York). 921

AVELINO, HERIBERTO, MATT COLER, AND W. LEO WETZELS (eds.). 2016. Laryngeal 922

Features in the Languages of the Americas (Brill’s Studies in the Indigenous 923

Languages of the Americas) Boston: Brill. 924

BAGEMIHL, BRUCE. 1991. Syllable structure in Bella Coola. Linguistic Inquiry 22:589–925

646. 926

Page 49: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

49

BECK, DAVID AND DAVID BENNETT. 2007. Extending the prosodic hierarchy: Evidence 927

from Lushootseed narrative. Northwest Journal of Linguistics 1:1–34. 928

BECKMAN, MARY, JAN EDWARDS, AND JANET FLETCHER. 1992. Prosodic structure and 929

tempo in a sonority model of articulatory dynamics, in G. Docherty and D. R. Ladd 930

(eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology II: Gesture, Segment, Prosody, pp. 68–86. 931

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 932

BEREZ, ANDREA. 2011. Prosody as a genre-distinguishing feature in Ahtna: a quantitative 933

approach. Functions of Language 18:210–236. 934

BIRD, SONYA. 2011. The nature of laryngealization in St’át’imcets laryngealized 935

resonants. International Journal of American Linguistics 77:159–184. 936

BIRD, SONYA, MARION CALDECOTT, FIONA CAMPBELL, BRYAN GICK AND PATRICIA 937

SHAW. 2008. Oral-laryngeal timing in glottalized resonants. Journal of Phonetics 938

36:492–507. 939

BOAS, FRANZ. 1911. Handbook of American Indian Languages, vol. 1. Washington: 940

Smithsonian Institution. 941

BOAS, FRANZ. 1917a. Introductory. International Journal of American Linguistics 1:1–8. 942

_____. 1917b. El Dialecto Mexicano de Pochutla, Oaxaca. International Journal of 943

American Linguistics 1:9-44. 944

BUCKLEY, EUGENE. 1994. Persistent and cumulative extrametricality in Kashaya. Natural 945

Language and Linguistic Theory 12:423–464. 946

_____. 2013. Kashaya extrametricality and formal symmetry. Supplemental 947

Page 50: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

50

Proceedings of the 2013 Meeting on Phonology. 948

CABALLERO, GABRIELA AND LUCIEN CAROLL. 2015. Tone and stress in Choguita 949

Rarámuri (Tarahumara) word prosody. International Journal of American 950

Linguistics 81:457–493. 951

CAMPBELL, ERIC AND ANTHONY WOODBURY. 2010. The comparative tonology of 952

Chatino: A prolegomenon. Paper presented at the Society for the Study of the 953

Indigenous Languages of the Americas (SSILA), winter meeting. Baltimore, 954

Maryland, January 7-10. 955

CHAFE, WALLACE. 1976. The Caddoan, Iroquoian, and Siouan Languages. The Hague: 956

Mouton. 957

CHO, TAEHONG AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1999. Variation and universals in VOT: 958

evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics 27:207–229. 959

CHO, TAEHONG, ALICE TAFF, MOSES DIRKS, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1997. Some 960

phonetic structures of Aleut. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 95:68–90. 961

CLARK, MARY. 1983. On the distribution of contour tones. West Coast Conference on 962

Formal Linguistics 2:44–55. 963

CLOPPER, CYNTHIA G. AND JUDITH TONHAUSER. 2013. The prosody of focus in 964

Paraguayan Guaraní. International Journal of American Linguistics 79:219–251. 965

COOK, EUNG-DO. 1989. Is phonology going haywire in dying languages? Phonological 966

variations in Chipewyan and Sarcee. Language in Society 18:235–55. 967

_____. 2006. The patterns of consonantal acquisition and change in Chipewyan (Dëne 968

Sųłiné). International Journal of American Linguistics 72:236–263. 969

Page 51: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

51

CROWHURST, MEGAN J., AND LEV MICHAEL. 2005. Iterative footing and prominence-970

driven stress in Nanti (Kampa). Language 81:47–95. 971

CROWHURST, MEGAN AND SARA TRECHTER. 2014. Vowel-rhotic metathesis in Guarayu. 972

International Journal of American Linguistics 80:127–173. 973

CROWHURST, MEGAN J., AND AMADOR TEODOCIO OLIVARES. 2014. Beyond the Iambic-974

Trochaic Law: the joint influence of duration and intensity on the perception of 975

rhythmic speech. Phonology 31:51–94. 976

CRUZ, HILARIA, AND ANTHONY C. WOODBURY. 2006. La fonología y tonología 977

comparativa del Chatino: un informe de campo en Zacatepec. Proceedings of the 978

Conference on Indigenous Languages of Latin America II. Austin: AILLA. 979

DART, SARAH (1991). Articulatory and Acoustic Properties of Apical and Laminal 980

Articulations (UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 79). Los Angeles: UCLA 981

Department of Linguistics. 982

_____. (1993). Phonetic properties of O'odham stop and fricative contrasts. International 983

Journal of American Linguistics 59:16–37. 984

DAY, CHRISTOPHER. 1973. The Jacaltec Language. Indiana University Publications, 985

Language Science Monographs 12). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. 986

DE JONG, KENNETH, AND JOYCE MCDONOUGH. 1993. Tone and tonogenesis in Navajo. 987

UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 84:165–182. 988

DICANIO, CHRISTIAN T. 2012. The phonetics of fortis and lenis consonants in Itunyoso 989

Trique. International Journal of American Linguistics 78:239–272. 990

EDWARDS, JAN, MARY BECKMAN, AND JANET FLETCHER. 1991. The articulatory 991

Page 52: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

52

kinematics of 992

ELÍAS-ULLOA, JOSÉ. 2009. The distribution of laryngeal segments in Capanahua. 993

International Journal of American Linguistics 75:159–206. 994

ESLING, JOHN H., KATHERINE E. FRASER, AND JIMMY G. HARRIS. 2005. Glottal stop, 995

glottalized resonants, and pharyngeal: a reinterpretation with evidence from a 996

laryngscopic study of Nuuchahnulth (Nootka). Journal of Phonetics 33:383–410. 997

EVERETT, CALEB. 2011. Variable velic movement in Karitiâna. International Journal of 998

American Linguistics 77:33–58. 999

EVERETT, DANIEL. 1986. Pirahã, in Desmond Derbyshire and Geoffrey Pullum (eds.), 1000

Handbook of Amazonian Languages 1, 200–325. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 1001

EVERETT, DANIEL, AND KEREN EVERETT. 1984. On the relevance of syllable onsets to 1002

stress placement. Linguistic Inquiry 15:705–711. 1003

FITZGERALD, COLLEEN M. 1998. The meter of Tohono O’odham songs. International 1004

Journal of American Linguistics 64:1–36. 1005

FLEMMING, EDWARD, PETER LADEFOGED, AND SARAH THOMASON. 1994. Phonetic 1006

structures of Montana Salish. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 87:1–33. 1007

GAFOS, ADAMANTIOS. 1999. The Articulatory Basis of Locality in Phonology. New York: 1008

Garland. 1009

GERFEN, CHIP, AND KIRK BAKER. 2005. The production and perception of laryngealized 1010

vowels in Coatzospan Mixtec. Journal of Phonetics 33:311–334. 1011

Page 53: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

53

GICK, BRYAN, HEATHER BLISS, KARIN MICHELSON, AND BOSKO RADANOV. 2012. 1012

Articulation with acoustics: “Soundless” vowels in Oneida and Blackfoot. Journal of 1013

Phonetics 40:46–53. 1014

GODDARD, PLINY EARLE. 1907. The Phonology of the Hupa Language, Part I, The 1015

Individual Sounds. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. 1016

_____. 1912. Elements of the Kato Language. California University of California Press. 1017

_____. 1928. Pitch Accent in Hupa. Berkeley, California: University of California 1018

Press. 1019

GÓMEZ, GALE GOODWIN AND HEIN VAN DER VOORT (eds.). 2014. Reduplication in 1020

Indigenous Languages of South America. Boston: Brill. 1021

GORDON, MATTHEW. 1996. The phonetic structures of Hupa. UCLA Working Papers in 1022

Phonetics 93:164–187. 1023

_____. 2001. A typology of contour tone restrictions. Studies in Language 25:405–444. 1024

_____. 2004. A phonetic and phonological study of word-level stress in Chickasaw, 1025

International Journal of American Linguistics 70:1–32. 1026

_____. 2005a. An autosegmental/metrical model of Chickasaw intonation, in S-A. Jun 1027

(ed.), Prosodic typology—the phonology of intonation and phrasing, pp. 301–330. 1028

Oxford University Press. 1029

_____. 2005b. A perceptually-driven account of onset-sensitive stress. Natural 1030

Language and Linguistic Theory 23:595–653. 1031

Page 54: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

54

GORDON, MATTHEW, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 2001. Phonation types: a cross-linguistic 1032

overview, Journal of Phonetics 29:383–406. 1033

GORDON, MATTHEW, AND ED LUNA. 2004. An intergenerational investigation of Hupa 1034

stress, Berkeley Linguistics Society 30:105–117. 1035

GORDON, MATTHEW, JACK MARTIN, AND LINDA LANGLEY. 2015. Some phonetic 1036

structures of Koasati. International Journal of American Linguistics 81, 83–118. 1037

GORDON, MATTHEW, AND PAMELA MUNRO. 2007. A phonetic study of final vowel 1038

lengthening in Chickasaw, International Journal of American Linguistics 73:293–1039

330. 1040

GORDON, MATTHEW, PAMELA MUNRO, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1997. The phonetic 1041

structures of Chickasaw. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 95:41–67. 1042

GORDON, MATTHEW, AND FRANÇOISE ROSE. 2006. Émérillon stress: a phonetic and 1043

phonological study. Anthropological Linguistics 48:132–168. 1044

GOUSKOVA, MARIA. 2003. Deriving Economy: Syncope in Optimality Theory. Ph.D. 1045

Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. (Available online on the 1046

Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA 610, http://roa.rutgers.edu) 1047

_____. 2007. The reduplicative template in Tonkawa. Phonology 24:367–396. 1048

GUREVICH, NAOMI. 2000. Reduplication in Southern Paiute and Correspondence Theory. 1049

West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 19:167–177. 1050

HAAS, MARY. 1977. Tonal accent in Creek, in Larry Hyman (ed.), Studies in Stress and 1051

Accent (Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4), pp. 195–208. Los 1052

Page 55: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

55

Angeles: University of Southern California. [Reprinted in Sturtevant, William C. 1053

(ed.), 1987, A Creek source book. New York: Garland Publishing.] 1054

HALLE, MORRIS, AND VERGNAUD, JEAN-ROGER. 1987. An Essay on Stress. Cambridge, 1055

Mass.: MIT Press. 1056

HANSSON, GUNNAR ÓLAFUR. 2001. The phonologization of production constraints: 1057

Evidence from consonant harmony. Chicago Linguistic Society 37:187–200. 1058

HARGUS, SHARON, AND VIRGINIA BEAVERT. 2006. Word-initial clusters and minimality in 1059

Yakima Sahaptin. Phonology 23:21–58. 1060

HARGUS, SHARON AND KEREN RICE (eds.). 2005. Athabaskan Prosody. Philadelphia: John 1061

Benjamins Publishing Company. 1062

HAUGEN, JASON. 2003. Allomorphy in Yaqui reduplication, in Studies in Uto-Aztecan, 1063

MIT Working Papers on Endangered and Less Familiar Languages 5. Cambridge, 1064

MA: MITWPL, 75-103. 1065

_____. 2014. Hiaki (Yaqui) reduplication redux: Morphological and prosodic targets in 1066

syllabic reduplication. International Journal of American Linguistics 80:507–532. 1067

HAYES, BRUCE. 1987. A revised parametric metrical theory. Northeastern Linguistics 1068

Society 17:274–289. 1069

_____. 1989. Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology. Linguistic Inquiry 1070

20:253-306. 1071

_____. 1995. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago: University 1072

of Chicago Press. 1073

Page 56: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

56

HAYNES, ERIN. 2010. Phonetic and Phonological Acquisition in Endangered Languages 1074

Learned by Adults: A Case Study of Numu (Oregon Northern Paiute). Ph.D. thesis, 1075

University of California, Berkeley. 1076

HICKERSON, NANCY. 1958a. An acoustic analysis of Shawnee: I. International Journal of 1077

American Linguistics 24: 20–29. 1078

_____. 1958b. An acoustic analysis of Shawnee: II. International Journal of American 1079

Linguistics 24: 130–141. 1080

_____. 1959a. An acoustic analysis of Shawnee: III. International Journal of American 1081

Linguistics 25: 22–31. 1082

_____. 1959b. An acoustic analysis of Shawnee: IV. International Journal of American 1083

Linguistics 25: 97–104. 1084

HINTON, LEANNE. 1984. Havasupai Songs: A Linguistic Perspective. Tübingen: Gunter 1085

Narr Verlag. 1086

_____. 1990. Song metrics. Linguistics Society 16, Parasession on Native Languages. 1087

Berkeley: Department of Linguistics, University of California. 1088

HINTZ, DIANE. 2006. Stress in South Conchucos Quechua: a phonetic and phonological 1089

study. International Journal of American Linguistics 72:477–521. 1090

HOCKEY, BETH ANN, AND ZSUZSANNA FAGYAL. 1999. Phonemic length and pre-boundary 1091

lengthening: An experimental investigation on the use of durational cues in 1092

Hungarian. Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Congress of Phonetic 1093

Sciences, pp. 313–16. 1094

Page 57: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

57

HOGAN, JOHN T. 1976. An analysis of the temporal features of ejective consonants. 1095

Phonetica 33:275–284. 1096

HOIJER, HARRY. 1933. Tonkawa: An Indian language of Texas. New York: Columbia 1097

University. 1098

HOWE, DARIN, AND DOUGLAS PULLEYBLANK. 2001. Patterns and timing of glottalisation. 1099

Phonology 18:45-80. 1100

HUDU, FUSHEINI. 2008. The low vowel and retraction in St’át’imcets: An ultrasound 1101

investigation. SKY Journal of Linguistics 21:67–81. 1102

HYMAN, LARRY. 1988. Syllable structure constraints on tonal contours. Linguistique 1103

Africaine 1:49-60. 1104

JOHNSON, KEITH, AND JACK MARTIN. 2001. Acoustic vowel reduction in Creek: Effects of 1105

distinctive length and position in the word. Phonetica 58:81–102. 1106

KENSTOWICZ, MICHAEL, AND CHARLES KISSEBERTH. 1979. Generative Phonology: 1107

Description and Theory. New York: Academic Press. 1108

KIMBALL, GEOFFREY. 1983. Verb pluralization in Koasati. 1982 Mid-America Linguistics 1109

Conference Papers: 401–411. 1110

KINGSTON, JOHN. 2005. The phonetics of Athabaskan tonogenesis, in Sharon Hargus and 1111

Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody, pp. 137–184. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 1112

KLATT, DENNIS H. 1975. Vowel lengthening is syntactically determined in a connected 1113

discourse. Journal of Phonetics 3:129–40. 1114

LADEFOGED, PETER, AND DANIEL EVERETT. 1996. Phonetic structures of Banawá, an 1115

endangered language. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 93:47–64. 1116

Page 58: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

58

LINDAU, MONA (1984). Phonetic differences in glottalic consonants. Journal of 1117

Phonetics, 12:147–155. 1118

LOVICK, OLGA, AND SIRI TUTTLE. 2012. The Prosody of Dena’ina narrative discourse. 1119

International Journal of American Linguistics 78:293–334. 1120

MACEACHERN, MARGARET, BARBARA KERN, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1996. Wari’ 1121

phonetic structures. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 93:65–93. 1122

MADDIESON, IAN. 1984. Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1123

MADDIESON, IAN, HERIBERTO AVELINO, AND LORETTA O’CONNOR. 2009. The phonetic 1124

structures of Oaxaca Chontal. International Journal of American Linguistics 75:69–1125

101. 1126

MADDIESON, IAN, NICOLA BESSEL, AND CAROLINE SMITH. 1996. A preliminary report on 1127

the sounds of Tlingit. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 93:125–148. 1128

MADDIESON, IAN, AND KRISTIN PRECODA. 1990. Updating UPSID. UCLA Working 1129

Papers in Phonetics 74:104–111. 1130

MALONE, TERRY. 2006. Tone and Syllable Structure in Chimila. International Journal of 1131

American Linguistics 72:1–58. 1132

_____. 2010. Nasality and nasal prosody in Chimila. International Journal of American 1133

Linguistics 76:1–41. 1134

MARTIN, JACK. 1988. Subtractive morphology as dissociation. West Coast Conference on 1135

Formal Linguistics 7:229–240. 1136

_____. 2011. A grammar of Creek (Muskogee). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. 1137

Page 59: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

59

_____. 2013. The geminating grade in Koasati and Muskogean. Paper presented at the 1138

annual SSILA meeting, Boston, Mass. 1139

MARTIN, JACK, AND KEITH JOHNSON. 2002. An acoustic study of "tonal accent" in Creek. 1140

International Journal of American Linguistics 68:28–50. 1141

MCCARTHY, JOHN. 2007. Consonant harmony via correspondence: evidence from 1142

Chumash. Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series. Paper 31. (available at 1143

http://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/31) 1144

MCCARTHY, JOHN, AND PRINCE, ALAN. 1986. Prosodic Morphology. Ms. University of 1145

Massachusetts, Amherst & Rutgers University. (Published 1996. Linguistics 1146

Department Faculty Publication Series. Paper 13. 1147

http://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/13) 1148

_____. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity, in J. Beckman, S. Urbanczyk, and 1149

L. W. Dickey (eds.), University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 1150

18: Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistics Students 1151

Association, 249–384. (Available online on the Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-1152

60, www.roa.rutgers.edu) 1153

MCDONOUGH, JOYCE, AND MARTHA AUSTIN-GARRISON. 1994. Vowel enhancement and 1154

dispersion in the vowel space of Western Navajo: a study of Navajo speakers. UCLA 1155

Working Papers in Phonetics 87:93–104. 1156

MCDONOUGH, JOYCE, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1993. Navajo stops. UCLA Working 1157

Papers in Phonetics 84:151–164. 1158

Page 60: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

60

MCDONOUGH, JOYCE, PETER LADEFOGED, AND HELEN GEORGE. 1993. Navajo vowels and 1159

phonetic universal tendencies. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 84:143–150. 1160

MCDOWELL, RAMONA. 2004. Retraction in Montana Salish Lateral Consonants: An 1161

Ultrasonic Study. M.A. thesis, University of British Columbia. 1162

MICHAEL, LEV, STEPHANIE FARMER, GREGORY FINLEY, CHRISTINE BEIER, AND KARIAN 1163

SULLÓN ACOSTA. 2013. A sketch of Muniche segmental and prosodic phonology. 1164

International Journal of American Linguistics 79:307–347. 1165

NAMDARAN, NAHAL. 2006. Retraction in St’a ́t’imcets: An Ultrasonic Investigation. M.A. 1166

thesis, University of British Columbia. 1167

MUNRO, PAMELA. 1996. The Chickasaw sound system. Ms., University of California, Los 1168

Angeles. 1169

MUNRO, PAMELA, AND CHARLES ULRICH. 1984. Structure-preservation and Western 1170

Muskogean rhythmic lengthening. West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 1171

3:191-202. 1172

MUNRO, PAMELA, AND CATHERINE WILLMOND. 1994. Chickasaw: An Analytical 1173

Dictionary. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. 1174

_____. 2008. Let’s Speak Chickasaw: Chikashshanompa’ Kilanompoli’. Norman, 1175

Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press. 1176

NAGANO-MADSEN, YASUKO. 1992. Mora and Prosodic Coordination: A Phonetic Study 1177

of Japanese, Eskimo, and Yoruba. Lund: Lund University Press 1178

Page 61: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

61

NATER, H. F. 1984. The Bella Coola Language, National Museum of Man Mercury 1179

Series, Canadian Ethnology Service Paper No. 92. Ottawa: National Museums of 1180

Canada. 1181

NEWMAN, STANLEY. 1944. Yokuts Language of California. New York: Viking Fund. 1182

NOSKE, ROLAND. 1993. A Theory of Syllabification and Segmental Alternation.With 1183

Studies on the Phonology of French, German, Tonkawa and Yawelmani. Tübingen: 1184

Niemeyer. 1185

NOYER, ROLF. 2013. A generative phonology of San Mateo Huave. International Journal 1186

of American Linguistics 79:1–60. 1187

OSWALT, ROBERT L. 1988. The floating accent of Kashaya, in William Shipley (ed.), In 1188

Honor of Mary Haas, pp. 611-621. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin. 1189

PAYNE, DORIS. 1990. Asheninca stress patterns. In Doris Payne (ed.), Amazonian 1190

Linguistics, pp. 185–212. Austin: University of Texas Press. 1191

PENG, SHU-HUI. 1997. Production and perception of Taiwanese tones in different tonal 1192

and phrasal contexts. Journal of Phonetics 25:371–400. 1193

PHELPS, ELAINE. 1975. Iteration and disjunctive domains in phonology. Linguistic 1194

Analysis 1:137–172. 1195

PLAUCHÉ, MADELAINE C., ROSEMARY BEAM DE AZCONA, RUNGPAT ROENGPITYA, AND 1196

WILLIAM F. WEIGEL. 1998. Glottalized sonorants: a phonetic universal? Berkeley 1197

Linguistics Society 24:381–389. 1198

POSER, WILLIAM. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66:78–105. 1199

Page 62: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

62

_____. 2004. On the status of Chumash sibilant harmony. Ms., University of 1200

Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Available online at 1201

http://www.ai.mit.edu/projects/dm/featgeom/poser-chumashnew.pdf). 1202

POTTER, BRIAN, MATTHEW GORDON, JOHN DAWSON, WILLEM DE REUSE, AND PETER 1203

LADEFOGED. 2000. Phonetic structures of Western Apache. UCLA Working Papers 1204

in Phonetics 98:10–38. 1205

PRIETO, PILAR, JAN VAN SANTEN, AND JULIA HIRSCHBERG. 1995. Tonal alignment 1206

patterns in Spanish. Journal of Phonetics 23:429–451. 1207

PRINCE, ALAN, AND SMOLENSKY, PAUL. 1993. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction 1208

in Generative Grammar. Ms. Rutgers University and University of Colorado at 1209

Boulder. (Published in 2004 Blackwell Publishers, Malden Mass.) 1210

RAIMY, ERIC. 2000. The Phonology and Morphology of Reduplication. Berlin: Mouton de 1211

Gruyter. 1212

RANKIN, ROBERT. 1978. The unmarking of Quapaw: A case study in language death. 1213

Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 3:45–52. 1214

RUSSELL, KEVIN. 2008. Sandhi in Plains Cree. Journal of Phonetics 36:450–464. 1215

SAPIR, EDWARD. 1912. The Takelma Language of Southwestern Oregon. Washington: 1216

Government Printing Office. 1217

_____. 1930. Southern Paiute: a Shoshonean Language. Proceedings of the American 1218

Society of Arts and Sciences 65. 1219

SAPIR, EDWARD, AND VICTOR GOLLA. 2001. Hupa texts, with notes and lexicon. In Victor 1220

Page 63: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

63

Golla and Sean O’Neill (eds.), Northwest California Linguistics [The Collected 1221

Works of Edward Sapir XIV], pp. 19–1011. New York: Mouton. 1222

SHAW, PATRICIA A. 2005. Non-adjacency in reduplication, in Bernhard Hurch (ed.), 1223

Studies on reduplication, pp. 161–210. New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 1224

SHAW, PATRICIA A., FIONA CAMPBELL, FLORA EHRHARDT, AND PATRICIA MCKAY. 2005. 1225

Patterns and timing of resonant glottalization in Nɬeʔkepmxcin. Proceedings of the 1226

40th International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages, UBC 1227

Working Papers in Linguistics 16:210–224. 1228

SILVERMAN, DANIEL, BARBARA BLANKENSHIP, PAUL KIRK, AND PETER LADEFOGED. 1229

1994. Phonetic stuctures in Jalapa Mazatec. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 1230

87:113–130. 1231

SILVERMAN, KIM, AND JANET PIERREHUMBERT. 1990. The timing of prenuclear high 1232

accents in English, in John Kingston and Mary Beckman (eds.), Papers in 1233

Laboratory Phonology 1: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech, pp. 72–106. 1234

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1235

STENZEL, KRISTINE. 2007. Glottalization and other suprasegmental features in Wanano. 1236

International Journal of American Linguistics 73:331–366. 1237

STERIADE, DONCA. 1999. Phonetics in phonology: the case of laryngeal neutralization, in 1238

Matthew K. Gordon (ed.), UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, vol. 2, October 1239

1999, Papers in Phonology 3:25–146. 1240

SWANTON, JOHN. 1921. The Tunica language. International Journal of American 1241

Linguistics 2:1–39. 1242

Page 64: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

64

TOPINTZI, NINA. 2004. Moraic onsets and WSP partition in Pirahã. CamLing 2004:211–1243

218. 1244

_____. 2010. Onsets: Suprasegmental and Prosodic Behaviour. Cambridge: 1245

Cambridge University Press. 1246

TUTTLE, SIRI. 2000. Unaspirated coronal stops in Jicarilla Apache. UCLA Working 1247

Papers in Phonetics 98:39–53. 1248

UCHIHARA, HIROTO. 2013. Tone and Accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. Ph.D. dissertation, 1249

The University at Buffalo, State University of New York. 1250

UMEDA, NORIKO. 1975. Vowel duration in American English. Journal of the Acoustical 1251

Society of America 58:434–45. 1252

VAN EIJK, JAN P. 1997. The Lillooet Language: Phonology, Morphology, and Syntax. 1253

Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press. 1254

VILLARD, STÉPHANIE. 2009. Grammatical Sketch of Zacatepec Chatino. M.A. thesis, 1255

University. 1256

WIGHTMAN, COLIN, STEFANIE SHATTUCK-HUFNAGEL, MARIE OSTENDORF, AND PATTI 1257

PRICE. 1992. Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. 1258

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 92:1707–17. 1259

WOO, NANCY. 1969. Prosody and Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT. 1260

WRIGHT, RICHARD. 1996. Tone and Accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. In Pamela Munro 1261

(ed.), Cherokee papers from UCLA. (University of California Occasional Papers in 1262

Linguistics 16). Los Angeles: U. of California, Los Angeles, pp. 11–22. 1263

WRIGHT, RICHARD, SHARON HARGUS, AND KATHARINE DAVIS. 2002. On the 1264

Page 65: 1 1 PHONETIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH ON NATIVE …gordon.faculty.linguistics.ucsb.edu/Gordon_NAPhonetics... · 2018-09-25 · 52 has contributed to advances in the fields of phonetics

65

categorization of ejectives: data from Witsuwit’en. Journal of the International 1265

Phonetic Association 32:43–77. 1266

XU, YI. 2001. Fundamental frequency peak delay in Mandarin. Phonetica 58:26–52. 1267

ZHANG, JIE. 2002. The Effects of Duration and Sonority on Contour Tone Distribution: 1268

Typological Survey and Formal Analysis. New York: Routledge. 1269

1 Thank you to David Beck, Donna Gerdts, and an anonymous reviewer for their

numerous thoughtful and helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper.