(1) information and political perception

173
INFORMATION AND POLITICAL PERCEPTION by JORDAN PENNEFATHER B.A., CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, DOMINGUEZ HILLS, 2003 M.A., UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO AT BOULDER, 2005 A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Colorado in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Psychology 2008

Upload: enangdudi

Post on 01-Nov-2014

10 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: (1) Information and Political Perception

INFORMATION AND POLITICAL PERCEPTION

by

JORDAN PENNEFATHER

B.A., CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, DOMINGUEZ HILLS, 2003

M.A., UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO AT BOULDER, 2005

A thesis submitted to the

Faculty of the Graduate School of the

University of Colorado in partial fulfillment

of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Psychology

2008

Page 2: (1) Information and Political Perception

3337221

3337221 2009

Page 3: (1) Information and Political Perception

This thesis entitled:

Information and Political Perception

written by Jordan Pennefather

has been approved for the

Department of Psychology

Dr. Bernadette Park (chair)

Dr. Charles Judd

Date: ____________

The final copy of this thesis has been examine by the signatories, and we find

that both the content and the form meet acceptable presentation standards of

scholarly work in the above mentioned discipline.

HRC protocol # 0707.12

Page 4: (1) Information and Political Perception

iii

ABSTRACT

Jordan Pennefather (Ph.D. Psychology)

Information and Political Perception

Directed by Dr. Bernadette Park, Professor, Department of Psychology,

University of Colorado at Boulder

Past research into the effect of information on political perception has

tended to use survey research, which while allowing strong inferences about

external validity is troubled by decreased control of the information that

respondents use when forming impressions of the candidates. Moving into the

laboratory allows us to examine the effect of the specific information on

perceptions.

The first study examined how the relevance and diagnosticity of

individuating information to judgments of political candidates’ attitude position on

various issues influences perception of the candidates. At Time 1, subjects

learned basic information about the candidates (photograph, name, and political

party). At Time 2 they learned information that was irrelevant, relevant but

nondiagnostic, or relevant and diagnostic of the candidates’ political stands.

Overall subjects projected more to the ingroup candidates. Ingroup projection

decreased directionally at Time 2. At Time 1 subjects contrasted the outgroup

Page 5: (1) Information and Political Perception

iv

candidates, at Time 2 projection was non-significant. Overall Democrat subjects

agreed more about the candidates’ positions than Republicans. Polarization of

the political parties decreased at Time 2.

In the second study subjects learned about an ingroup candidate

(Democrat) who held positions on six issues that were consistent with the

stereotypic positions of his political party, or were mixed (three consistent and

three inconsistent). Additionally, subjects learned that the candidate was either

highly endorsed by fellow Democrats (popular), or weakly endorsed (unpopular).

Subjects showed preference for the consistent candidate. Additionally, highly

affiliated subjects showed a non-significant tendency to use consistency

information less when the candidate was popular.

The final study examined data from the 1980 and 1988 American National

Election Studies (ANES). Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used to

examine both ideological and issue-specific projection, as well as the impact of

self-reported political knowledge on polarization of the two parties. In general the

two models demonstrated the tendency of the respondents to project their own

political positions towards their own political party. Additionally, we have shown

that the respondents contrasted the outgroup party’s position from their own.

Increased political knowledge led to polarization of the two parties, by increasing

perceptions of extremity for both ingroup and outgroup.

Page 6: (1) Information and Political Perception

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ..................................................................... 1

Models of Vote Choice...................................................................... 1

Styles of Voting................................................................................. 3

Party Voting................................................................................... 3

Ideological Voting .......................................................................... 4

Personality Voting ......................................................................... 5

Issue Voting................................................................................... 6

Information Types ............................................................................. 6

Own Position ................................................................................. 6

Party Stand ................................................................................... 9

Candidate Information ................................................................. 10

Endorsement and Popularity ....................................................... 12

CHAPTER 2: STUDY ONE ....................................................................... 17

Projection........................................................................................ 18

Consensus...................................................................................... 19

Polarization ..................................................................................... 20

Strength of Affiliation....................................................................... 21

Method............................................................................................ 23

Subjects ...................................................................................... 24

Page 7: (1) Information and Political Perception

vi

Materials...................................................................................... 24

Procedure.................................................................................... 27

Results............................................................................................ 30

Strength of Affiliation ................................................................... 31

Projection .................................................................................... 31

Consensus .................................................................................. 37

Polarization ................................................................................. 43

Discussion ...................................................................................... 47

CHAPTER 3: STUDY TWO....................................................................... 50

Consistency .................................................................................... 50

Popularity........................................................................................ 53

Method............................................................................................ 56

Subjects ...................................................................................... 56

Materials...................................................................................... 56

Procedure.................................................................................... 59

Results............................................................................................ 60

Strength of Affiliation ................................................................... 61

Impression Variables................................................................... 61

Projection .................................................................................... 69

Process Variables ....................................................................... 71

Discussion ...................................................................................... 82

CHAPTER 4: STUDY THREE .................................................................... 86

Method............................................................................................ 87

Page 8: (1) Information and Political Perception

vii

1980 ANES ................................................................................. 87

1988 ANES ................................................................................. 89

Results............................................................................................ 91

1980 ANES Dataset .................................................................... 91

1988 Super Tuesday ANES Dataset ........................................... 95

Discussion .................................................................................... 105

CHAPTER 5: GENERAL DISCUSSION...................................................... 108

REFERENCES ..................................................................................... 115

APPENDIX A: PHOTOGRAPHS OF POLITICAL CANDIDATES ....................... 130

APPENDIX B: STUDY ONE POLITICALLY IRRELEVENT STATEMENTS.......... 131

APPENDIX C: STUDY ONE POLITICALLY RELEVENT-UNDIAGNOSTIC

STATEMENTS ...................................................................................... 132

APPENDIX D: STUDY ONE POLITICALLY RELEVENT-DIAGNOSTIC

STATEMENTS................................................................................................. 133

APPENDIX E: STUDY ONE SELF RATING QUESTIONNAIRE ....................... 135

Section 1: Self-ratings of Personality Traits .................................. 135

Section 2: Political Attitudes, Ideology, and Knowledge................ 136

APPENDIX F: STUDY ONE RATINGS OF POLITICAL CANDIDATES

QUESTIONNAIRE ................................................................................. 138

Section 1: Ratings of Candidates' of Personality Traits ................ 138

Section 2: Political Attitudes, Ideology, and Knowledge................ 139

APPENDIX G: STUDY TWO TARGET....................................................... 141

Popular ......................................................................................... 141

Page 9: (1) Information and Political Perception

viii

Unpopular ..................................................................................... 141

APPENDIX H: STUDY TWO CONSISTENT TARGET INFORMATION .............. 142

APPENDIX I: STUDY TWO INCONSISTENT TARGET INFORMATION.............. 143

APPENDIX J: STUDY TWO SELF RATING QUESTIONNAIRE ....................... 144

APPENDIX K: STUDY ONE RATINGS OF POLITICAL CANDIDATES

QUESTIONNAIRE ................................................................................. 147

APPENDIX L: STUDY TWO RECALL MEASURE ........................................ 152

APPENDIX M: STUDY TWO RECOGNITION MEASURE .............................. 153

APPENDIX N: STUDY TWO SUBTYPING MEASURE .................................. 155

APPENDIX O: STUDY THREE 1980 ANES POLITICAL ISSUES .................. 156

APPENDIX P: STUDY THREE 1980 ANES KNOWLEDGE MEASURES…..…158

APPENDIX Q: STUDY THREE 1988 ANES POLITICAL ISSUES .................. 159

APPENDIX R: STUDY THREE 1988 ANES IDEOLOGY MEASURE .............. 160

Page 10: (1) Information and Political Perception

ix

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1. 1980 ANES: Loading and structural coefficients: Unstandardized and standardized estimates for Democrat (left) and Republican (Right) respondents................................................................................ 96

Table 2. 1980 ANES: Standardized Residual Covariances for Democrat

(Left) and Republican (Right) respondents. ........................................... 97 Table 3. 1988 ANES: Loading and structural coefficients: Unstandardized

and standardized estimates for Democrat (left) and Republican (Right) respondents.............................................................................. 103

Table 4. 1988 ANES: Standardized Residual Covariances for Democrat

(Left) and Republican (Right) respondents .......................................... 104

Page 11: (1) Information and Political Perception

x

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Affiliation. Frequency Distribution of subjects’ self-rated strength of affiliation to their political party.. ........................................... 31

Figure 2. Projection. Correlation between subjects’ self-ratings and their ratings of the targets, by ingroup/outgroup and affiliation (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation).. ............................................................................. 33

Figure 3. Projection. Correlation between subjects’ self-ratings and their

ratings of the targets, by time and ingroup/outgroup.............................. 35 Figure 4. Consensus. Average agreement between subjects’ ratings of

the targets on the 16 attitude items by time and information condition. . 41 Figure 5. Consensus. Average agreement between subjects’ ratings of

the targets on the 16 attitude items by subjects’ political party and information condition. ............................................................................. 42

Figure 6. Polarization. Average rating of Democrat targets on the 16

attitude issues minus the average rating of the Republican targets by time and information condition. .............................................................. 45

Figure 7. Polarization. Average rating of Democrat targets on the 16

attitude issues minus the average rating of the Republican targets by subject’s political party membership and strength of affiliation (+1 and -1 standard deviation). ........................................................................... 46

Figure 8. Affiliation Strength. Frequency Distribution of Subjects’ self-

rated strength of affiliation to their political party. ................................... 62 Figure 9. Evaluation. Subjects’ rated liking of the candidate by

consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation). ............................................................. 63

Figure 10. Voting Likelihood. Subjects’ rated likelihood of voting for the

candidate by consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation).................................. 65

Page 12: (1) Information and Political Perception

xi

Figure 11. Similarity. Subjects’ rated similarity to the candidate by consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation). ............................................................. 67

Figure 12. Candidate’s Affiliation Strength. Subjects’ rating of the

candidate’s strength of affiliation to the Democratic Party by consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation). ............................................................. 68

Figure 13. . Recall. Frequency Distribution of number freely recalled

items ...................................................................................................... 72 Figure 14. Recall. Number of freely recalled candidate statements by

popularity and consistency conditions.................................................... 73 Figure 15. Recognition. Weighted average of recognized candidate

statements by consistency condition and whether the statements were actually mentioned about the candidate (Old) or newly presented (New). ................................................................................... 75

Figure 16. Recognition in the Inconsistent Conditions. Weighted average

of recognized candidate statements by whether the candidate’s statement was consistent or inconsistent on the issue and whether the statement was actually mentioned about the candidate (Old) or newly presented (New). ......................................................................... 77

Figure 17. Valuation across Popularity Conditions. Average decrease in

valuation of the issues from Time 1 and Time 2 by consistency condition and whether the issue had been mentioned in the candidate statement (seen) or was not mentioned (unseen).................................. 80

Figure 18. Subtyping. Subjects’ rated typicality of the candidate for the

Democratic Party by popularity, and affiliation (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation)............................................................................................... 82

Figure 19a. 1980 ANES Structural Equation Model 1 (Democrat Voters):

Model of Democratic Party voters’ self-ratings on four political issues and their self-rated political knowledge effect on projection and polarization of the Democratic and Republican Parties.......................... 92

Figure 19b. 1980 ANES Structural Equation Model 1 (Republican Voters):

Model of Republican Party voters’ self-ratings on four political issues and their self-rated political knowledge effect on projection and polarization of the Democratic and Republican Parties.......................... 93

Page 13: (1) Information and Political Perception

xii

Figure 20a. 1988 Super Tuesday ANES Structural Equation Model 1 (Democrat Voters): Model of Democratic Party voters’ self-ratings on political ideology and their self-rated political knowledge effect on projection and polarization of the Democratic and Republican Parties. . 99

Figure 20b. 1988 Super Tuesday ANES Structural Equation Model 1

(Republican Voters): Model of Republican Party voters’ self-ratings on political ideology and their self-rated political knowledge effect on projection and polarization of the Democratic and Republican Parties.100

Page 14: (1) Information and Political Perception

1

Chapter 1:

Introduction

"A share in the sovereignty of the state, which is exercised by the citizens at

large, in voting at elections is one of the most important rights of the subject, and

in a republic ought to stand foremost in the estimation of the law." Alexander

Hamilton.

As Alexander Hamilton pointed out, voting in political elections is one of

the central rights that Americans have. Voting Americans help to elect

representatives who will have the power to shape the country. Whether it is

local, state, or national elections, voters are selecting representatives who will

have both direct and indirect impact on their lives. Since elections are so

important and are the average American’s main form of political participation, it is

important to understand how they decide which candidate to vote for.

Models of Vote Choice

A number of models have been developed over the years to explain voting

behavior. One of the earliest was the unconstrained rational voter model (Arrow,

1951; von Neuman & Morgenstern, 1947). This model, borrowed from economic

theory, described political decision makers as engaging in value-maximization,

that is they examine all the existing information and make the choice that best

meets their self-interested goals. This model views voters as “omniscient

calculators” (Lupia, McCubbins, & Popkin, 2000), or “ambulatory encyclopedias”

Page 15: (1) Information and Political Perception

2

(Lau & Redlawsk, 2006). The constrained rational model (Downs, 1957) grew

from the unconstrained perspective and, while it makes the same assumptions

about the processes involved, it does not assume that the voters have the same

cognitive capabilities to search out, encode, and recall unlimited information.

One of the most popular models of voter decision making in political science was

outlined in The American Voter (Campbell, Converse, Miller, & Stokes, 1960).

This model describes voting behavior as early learned social identification. In

this model party identification is key to vote choice, and colors later views of

candidates from both parties (Sears, 1975; Sears & Funk, 1991). A fourth model,

adapted from work on heuristics by Kahneman and Tversky (1972, 1973, 1984;

Tversky & Kahneman, 1973, 1974) argues that voting is a very complex decision,

involving large amounts of information in which mental shortcuts, or heuristics,

are often used to simplify the choice. One example of a using a heuristic in the

political domain is using the representativeness heuristic to apply stereotypes or

schemata about where the political parties stand on issues to form impressions

about a specific party member. A final model describes voters as “intuitive

decision makers” (Lau & Redlawsk, 2006) who use “low information rationality” to

make the choice with very little effort (Popkin, 1991; Sniderman, Brody, &

Tetlock, 1991). This model assumes that voting is more of a semiautomatic

response than a careful weighting of the merits of each candidate. This model

argues that since most voters do not know much about politics (Delli Carpini &

Keeter, 1996) and few have a real ideology (Zaller, 1992) they do not have the

Page 16: (1) Information and Political Perception

3

information or motivation required to make the complex calculations required by

the rational models.

Styles of Voting

What all of these models assume is that voters use some information in

their vote decision. The models differ in the amount, and in some cases type of

information used to make their choice. Each of these models makes

assumptions about the dimensions of the candidate that voters use to make their

decisions. The two rational models would argue that voters should vote based

on either the candidate’s political ideology or stance on specific issues. The

social identification, heuristic, and intuitive models all suggest that voters use

party identification more than specific issues or ideology. The heuristic and

intuitive models would also suggest voters use the candidate’s personality to

vote.

Party Voting. One way to vote is along party lines. Most Americans are

members of one of the two primary political parties, and thus could always vote

for the candidates from their own party. Research has demonstrated that this is

often the case, with political party being the most powerful predictor of electoral

behavior (Belknap & Campbell, 1952; Miller & Shanks, 1996). Party cues help to

reduce uncertainty for voters (Goodman & Murray, 2007). However, party

membership is not the whole answer. Within primaries there are multiple

candidates from the same party, so party membership will not suffice to select a

candidate. Even within general elections political party is not the entire answer.

According to a recent study, the influence of political partisanship on voting has

Page 17: (1) Information and Political Perception

4

been decreasing in strength, with the percentage of voters identifying with one

party declining from 73% in the 1950’s and 1960 to 61% by 1992 (Holbrook,

1996). This decrease in party identification has been accompanied by a shift

from party-centered voting to candidate-centered voting (Wattenburg, 1992). If

the candidate does in fact influence the voting process above and beyond the

power of the political party, what is it about the candidate that influences these

judgments?

Ideological Voting. Voters could judge the candidates using their

perceptions of the candidates’ ideology. “Ideology provides clues to how the

candidate will act in the future and what positions he will take on issues that have

yet to emerge” (Waldman & Jamieson, 2002). So voters could place the

candidates along a continuum from liberal to conservative and select the

candidate that most closely matches their own position on this continuum.

However, many researchers argue that the average American is not very

ideologically sophisticated (Kinder, 1983; Sniderman, 1993). In fact, Bennett

(1995) found that the typical American has very limited understanding about what

liberalism and conservatism actually mean. More importantly, Dalton (1979)

found that only 21% of American voters polled in 1974 and 1975 reported using

ideological concepts in their vote decision-making process. If ideology explains

only a portion of vote choice this leaves a gap in our understanding of voting

behavior. If voters are making candidate-based decisions, and are not simply

using ideology, what else can they use?

Page 18: (1) Information and Political Perception

5

Personality Voting. Voters may also be using the candidates’

personalities to inform their decisions (Caprara & Zimbardo, 2004). Sears (1969)

proposed that American voters use personalities to make their choice of

candidates because personalities are often easier to process than candidate

platform. Miller, Wattenburg, and Malanchuk (1985) state that candidate image

is “one of the most important but least understood facets of American politics.”

Researchers have found that voters look for candidates that are trustworthy (Kaid

& Hirsh, 1973), charismatic (Baxter, Young, & Bittner, 1973; see Hellweg, 1995),

and competent (Wakshlag & Edison, 1979). Voters who are frequent television

news viewer are more likely to rely on candidate personality to make their vote

choice (Keeter, 1987). Additionally, less educated individuals are more likely to

vote using personality than more educated voters (Abrajano, 2005; Nicholson,

Pantoja, & Segura, 2006). Media research has demonstrated that exposure to

televised coverage of the 1972 Presidential campaign increased interest in both

the candidates’ personalities and the issues (Hofstetter, Zukin, & Buss, 1978).

Other research has shown that the three major networks influenced the issue

agenda but had little influence on changing voters’ views of the candidates’

personalities (Hellweg, 1995). Many researchers argue that rather than voting

based on personality, voting on the issues is the more rational approach (Kinder,

1998).

Issue Voting. Downs (1957) argued that voters have specific policy

preferences on issues of interest to them and select parties that are closest to

their own. Proponents of the “rational voter” would argue that voting for a

Page 19: (1) Information and Political Perception

6

candidate with similar attitudes, especially on issues of importance to the voter is

a rational strategy (Shapiro, 1969). An abundance of research has demonstrated

that voters tend to select candidates that they view as holding similar attitudes

(Brent & Granberg, 1982; Feldman & Conover, 1983; Granberg & Brent, 1974;

Judd, Kenny, & Krosnick, 1983; Kendall & Yum, 1984; Kinder, 1978; Krosnick,

1990). So when people vote on the issues, which according to some, is the most

rational way to vote, they want to select the candidate they think shares their

attitudes (Shapiro, 1969). This raises an important question: how do voters

judge where candidates stand on the issues?

Information Types

There are a several types of information that voters use to form their

impressions about political candidates. Voters can use their own stand on the

issue. They can use their stereotypes of where the candidate’s political party

normally stands on the issue. Voters can also use information they have

received about the political candidate. Finally, voters can use endorsements

from trusted groups or individuals. Let us examine each of these sources of

information in greater detail.

Own Position. Before voters know anything about the specific candidate’s

position they know where they themselves stand on the issue. A voter’s own

positions on the issue can influence their perceptions of the candidate’s stand.

This process of using one’s own attitude to form impressions of candidates’

stands is known as projection and has been recognized by many researchers as

important to how voters form impressions of political candidates (Brent &

Page 20: (1) Information and Political Perception

7

Granberg, 1982; Feldman & Conover, 1983; Granberg & Brent, 1974; Judd,

Kenny, & Krosnick, 1983; Kinder, 1978; Krosnick, 1990). In their model of voter

choice, Conover and Feldman (1982) maintained that projection is a central

process in vote decisions, demonstrating that even when the position of the

candidate’s political party is controlled for there is still significant correlation

between the voter’s position and the ingroup candidate’s position. In fact, Quist

and Crano (2003) found that perceived similarity between the voters’ own

position and their impressions of the candidates’ position predicted over 84% of

the variance in voter choice.

The typical examination of projection in the political domain has involved

examining survey data looking for correlations between the voter’s self-reported

ideology or issue position and their rating of the candidates. For example, Brent

and Granberg (1977) examined data from the 1972 American National Election

Studies (ANES), a nationally representative sample collected during presidential

elections. They calculated the correlations between the respondent’s self-ratings

on the issues (guaranteed jobs, tax reform, Vietnam, inflation, legalizing

marijuana, bussing, health insurance, pollution, and women’s role in society) and

their perceptions of Richard Nixon (Republican) and George McGovern

(Democrat). The researchers found significant positive correlation between the

respondent’s own positions and their rating of their preferred candidate’s

positions, and significant, but weaker negative correlation for the nonpreferred

candidate. These results have been supported many times, during multiple

different elections (Brent & Granberg, 1982; Feldman & Conover, 1983; Judd,

Page 21: (1) Information and Political Perception

8

Kenny, & Krosnick, 1983; Kinder, 1978; Krosnick, 1990). Other projection

findings in the political domain suggest that projection is greater towards

candidates whose policy positions are ambiguous (Kinder, 1978; Granberg &

Brent, 1974).

Research within social psychology suggests that projection is a robust

phenomenon, often operating outside of awareness (Krueger, 2007). It seems to

operate as an anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic, where people use their own

position to rate others, based on shared group membership (DiDonato &

Krueger, 2007). However, when the person is a member of the outgroup,

projection decreases, is non-significant, or reverses, becoming a contrast effect

(Allen & Wilder, 1979; Clement & Krueger, 2002; Robbins & Krueger, 2005;

Wilder, 1984).

Projection is an important phenomenon for understanding impression

formation and vote choice. As mentioned above, the majority of research on

projection in the political domain has used survey research. The major problem

with examining projection in survey data is that the level of correlation between

the voter and candidate may be due to a number of other processes rather than

just projection, namely persuasion, actual shared issue position, and scale usage

(Conover & Feldman, 1982; Krosnick, 2002).

Since the surveys are cross-sectional measures of the respondent’s

position and their perceptions of the candidates are taken concurrently,

persuasion can influence the respondent’s position on the issue. Since the

respondent’s exposure to information about the candidate is difficult to

Page 22: (1) Information and Political Perception

9

adequately control, prior exposure to a popular candidate could have resulted in

a shift in the respondent’s positions, rather than his or her position influencing

perceptions of the candidate’s position. Additionally, the respondent may be

aware of the candidate’s actual position and actually share it, which would mean

the self-candidate correlation is actual shared issue position. Finally, the

respondent may prefer one end of the scale to the other, which would also lead

to significant self-candidate correlation. However, if this is the case, one would

then expect this bias consistently across target candidates regardless of whether

they were ingroup members or not.

Party Stand. Another source of information that voters have access to

before they know anything specific about the candidate is the stereotypic stand of

the two political parties on the issue. Once voters know which party the

candidate is a member of, they can use their stereotypes about the candidate’s

political party to inform their impressions of the candidate. Research has

demonstrated that political party has a large impact on how voters view the

candidates (Layman & Casey, 2002; Page & Jones, 1979). When people have

information about an individual’s group membership they often use stereotypic

information about that group to form impressions about specific members of the

group (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). This is also true with political

party membership, where information about where the party stands on issues

informs impressions of specific candidates (Golebiowska, 2001). Interestingly,

when partisan information about a candidate is known, peripheral information

such as candidate’s attractiveness is absent in ratings of the candidate (Riggle,

Page 23: (1) Information and Political Perception

10

Ottati, Wyer, Kuklinksi, & Schwartz, 1992). Political cue theory (Conover, 1981;

Conover & Feldman, 1982, 1989; Feldman & Conover, 1983) argues that voters

need to make sense of the world, and they develop schemas about political

parties in order to understand the political world. They then use this information

as an anchor when forming impressions of specific political candidates, which

has been demonstrated by a significant correlation between perceptions of the

issue position of the candidate and the candidate’s political party (Conover &

Feldman, 1982, 1989; Feldman & Conover, 1983). So knowing a candidate is a

Democrat should influence voters’ judgments about where that candidate stands

on issues such as abortion, affirmative action, and the environment, even absent

information about that candidate’s actual position. In fact, party membership is

consistently a significant predictor of voting in both experimental and real world

elections (for a review see Lau & Redlawsk, 2006). However, party membership

is not helpful in distinguishing between candidates during primary elections in

which the candidates are all members of the same party. Additionally, voters still

defect from party even during the general election so party cannot be the whole

story. An examination of ANES data from 1952 to 2004 has shown that on

average, 37% of Americans do not see a difference between the parties during

Presidential elections, while 46% do not see a difference during midterm election

periods (Goodman & Murray, 2007).

Candidate Information. Ideally, information about the candidates would be

the best form of information to use to inform voters’ perceptions of what the

Page 24: (1) Information and Political Perception

11

candidates believe. Unfortunately there are a number of problems inherent in

using information about candidates to form impressions of their positions.

First, information about the candidate is not always relevant to the issue at

hand. One might think that this would not be a problem because voters can

simply disregard irrelevant information. However, information about the

candidate that is not related to the issues will often be used when voters form

impressions of the candidate’s stand on that issue. For example, incidental

similarities between oneself and the candidate, such as sharing the same

birthday, could potentially influence impressions (Burger, Messian, Patel, del

Prado, & Anderson, 2004; Heider, 1958).

The second problem is that it is often difficult to gain access to information

about the candidates’ actual positions. There are a number of reasons that this

is so. Some researchers argue that it is often in the candidates’ best interest not

to make their issue positions clear (Downs, 1957; Page, 1976; Shepsle, 1972).

Additionally, the media devotes little attention to reporting candidate’ positions,

rather it tends to focus on personalities (Graber, 1984; Patterson, 1980). Adding

to the problem, many voters are not motivated or interested to seek out

information about the candidates’ positions (Keeter & Zukin, 1983). Finally,

efficient assimilation of the new information fails to occur when voters have low

motivation, are pressed for time, or have strong prior convictions, and the issues

are often complex in modern politics (Lodge & Taber, 2000). However, while

some ambiguity about candidates’ positions is usually if not always present

(Waldman & Jamieson, 2003) some researchers argue that access to the

Page 25: (1) Information and Political Perception

12

candidates’ positions has become easier to obtain (Shively, 1992). The final

problem is that even when voters do have access to information about the

candidates’ positions they may not use that information accurately, that is, they

may use the information to make inappropriate assessments of the candidates.

Research in person perception has demonstrated that in general people are not

very accurate in their assessments of other people (DePaulo, Kenny, Hoover,

Webb, & Oliver, 1987; Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, 2004; Swann & Gill, 1997).

Research in the political domain has demonstrated that when subjects who have

made a vote choice based on their self-directed perusal of candidate information

are later given all the information about the candidates, 70% of the subjects felt

they made the correct vote choice when there were two candidates and only 31%

of the subjects felt they selected correctly when there were four candidates; that

is, after learning all the candidate information, 30% of voters in two candidate

elections and 69% of voters in four candidate elections felt they had made the

incorrect choice (Lau & Redlawsk, 1997). This is similar to the findings from the

ANES from 1980-2000 (see Lau & Redlawsk, 2006). There are also a number of

biases that can influence judgments, including halo effects (Asch, 1946; Cooper,

1981), in-group favoritism (Brewer, 1979; Turner, Brown, & Tajfel, 1979), and

others.

Endorsement and Popularity

The final type of information used in voting is endorsements from trusted

groups. This type of information is often used as a shortcut to avoid additional

information search. Rather than learning more specific information about the

Page 26: (1) Information and Political Perception

13

candidate, voters can simply rely on the recommendations of close friends,

trusted experts (Carmines & Kuklinski, 1990; Mondak, 1993; Sniderman, Brody,

& Tetlock, 1991), or trusted groups (Brady & Sniderman, 1985; Lau & Redlawsk,

2001).

If the candidate is the most viable to win, especially when it is a primary

election, voters may again stop further information search or processing (Aldrich,

1980; Bartels, 1988; Lau & Redlawsk, 2001). In fact, voters often vote for a less

preferred candidate because their preferred candidate has no chance to win

(Abramson, Aldrich, Paolino, & Rhode, 1992).

So the information that voters have about the candidates is the most

important factor in their vote choice (Lau & Redlawsk, 2006). However, as

mentioned earlier, in the real world the amount, type, and access to information

differs across elections and between voters. This fluctuation in information

makes using survey research to examine the influence of information on

impressions of political candidates very difficult. ANES surveys have on

occasion taken multiple ratings of candidates across the campaign season,

however these are also cross-sectional and the information the respondents had

was not controlled (Krosnick, 2002).

The purpose of the first two studies of this dissertation was to exert

experimental control on the information that the subjects use in their impressions

of the candidates. I used this rigorous control to examine how different types of

individuating information about the candidates affect perceptions of them. The

first study examined how the relevancy and diagnosticity of the individuating

Page 27: (1) Information and Political Perception

14

candidate information to the impression dimension being judged - in this case

attitudes to specific issues - influenced perceptions of the candidate. The second

study examined how candidate popularity and consistency to the party

stereotype influenced impressions.

The final study of the dissertation returned to the ANES dataset and

examined how self-reported political knowledge influenced voters’ perceptions of

polarization between Democratic and Republican candidates, as well as

correlations between the respondent’s positions and their perceptions of the

candidates from each party.

As mentioned earlier, before voters know anything about a particular

candidate’s stand on an issue, they know their own position and have a

perception about where the two political parties stand. This information can allow

the voter to use the more heuristic based decision models; social identification,

heuristic, and intuitive. However, what happens when voters receive more

information about the candidate? Do voters use the additional individuating

information when updating their impressions or do they continue to rely only on

their own position and that of the party?

One study that examined changes in impressions due to exposure to

information about the candidates found that ambiguous political statements

decreased the impact of judges’ own attitudes on their impressions of same-party

candidates (Pennefather & Park, 2006). Subjects in this study first indicated their

own position on a number of political issues. They were then shown

photographs of middle aged white males and were told they were candidates for

Page 28: (1) Information and Political Perception

15

political office. Names and political party were also given for each candidate.

Subjects then reported where they perceived each of the candidates stood on the

political issues they had earlier reported their own stand on. Next the subjects

received short political statements from each candidate. These statements were

designed to be undiagnostic about the candidates’ specific stance on the issues,

while appearing to be informative. For example:

Our country is the economic power-house of the world. We need leaders who will

continue to carry on this tradition. Michael Larson will ensure that our country’s

economy remains strong and competitive. He understands the necessity of tightening

our country’s borders and increasing national security spending. He is a strong supporter

of our men and women serving overseas. Michael Larson is a leader for today’s world.

The statements were counterbalanced across political party. At the first rating

subjects projected significantly only towards in-group candidates. Once the

subjects received the statements the level of projection towards the in-group

candidates, while remaining significant, decreased. However, the amount of

projection towards the out-group candidates stayed steady at zero. So it appears

that when judging the ingroup party candidates, the subjects incorporated the

ambiguous individuating information, in that they relied less on their own position.

The decrease in ingroup projection raises the question of what in the

statements caused the judges to use self-referent stances on the issues less in

their impression. Was the decrease in the magnitude of projection towards the

in-group candidates due to the relevancy of the information given about the

candidates? If the information was relevant and diagnostic, rather than merely

relevant but undiagnostic, would the judges have used self-referent information

Page 29: (1) Information and Political Perception

16

even less or is any individuating information about the candidates enough to

decrease the projection effect? That is, does any information about the

candidates “dilute” the effect of party information and self-position (Nisbett,

Zukier, & Lemley, 1981)?

Page 30: (1) Information and Political Perception

17

Chapter 2:

Study One

The first study of this dissertation examined the influence of the relevancy

and diagnosticity of the information on the use of a voter’s own position to form

their impressions of the candidates (projection), the agreement between the

subjects about the candidates’ positions (consensus), and perceived differences

between Republican and Democrat candidates (polarization). I was interested in

how learning individuating information affected the use of the early heuristic

information (one’s own position and stereotypes about the parties) in forming

impressions of specific candidates.

The subjects first learned basic information about each of the candidates,

a photograph accompanied by their name and political party. After rating the

candidates’ positions on eight political issues, using this basic information, the

subjects read a short statement about each of them. They either learned

information that was irrelevant to judgments about the candidates’ positions

(hobbies, vacations they have taken, favorite song), information that was relevant

but undiagnostic of their position (identical to the general statements used in

Pennefather & Park, 2006; for example: “Michael Larson will ensure that our

country’s economy remains strong and competitive.”), or relevant and diagnostic

(the general statements plus specific issue positions; for example: “Michael

Larson will ensure that our country’s economy remains strong and competitive by

helping large businesses that are the backbone of our economy and making the

recent tax cuts permanent.”). They then re-rated the candidates’ positions on the

Page 31: (1) Information and Political Perception

18

eight political issues. The ratings made of the candidates’ positions on the issues

as well as the subject’s self-ratings were then used to examine projection,

consensus, and polarization. Possible mediation of these effects by the subject’s

strength of affiliation was also examined.

Projection

As mentioned in the introduction, voters can use a number of different

types of information to form impressions of political candidates. Voters can use

their impressions of the candidate’s political party, specific information about the

candidate, and their own position to form their impression about a political

candidate’s position. Use of one’s own attitude position when forming

impressions about other people is projection, which I examined first. As

mentioned earlier, the majority of research examining projection in the political

domain has relied on survey research, in which it is difficult to conclude that self-

candidate correlations are in fact projection (Krosnick, 2002). In the current

study self-ratings were collected first and the subjects rated previously unknown

candidates to eliminate the possibility of persuasion and actual shared position

effects.

The subjects were expected to project more towards ingroup candidates

than outgroup candidates, replicating previous studies (Allen & Wilder, 1979;

Clement & Krueger, 2002; Wilder, 1984). Additionally, our subjects were

expected to project less once they received additional information about the

candidates. In an earlier study the statements about ingroup candidates that

were relevant to political attitudes, but not diagnostic of the candidate’s position

Page 32: (1) Information and Political Perception

19

resulted in decreases in projection (Pennefather & Park, 2006). I predicted that

this decrease in projection was due to the relevancy of the information, rather

than the mere presence of individuating information. Since ambiguous candidate

positions lead to greater projection I predicted that projection towards ingroup

candidates would decrease in the two relevant conditions but remain stable in the

irrelevant condition (Kinder, 1978; Granberg & Brent, 1974).

Consensus

Consensus was of interest because it can be used as a measure of the

application of stereotypes about the candidate. Political Cue Theory (Conover,

1981; Conover & Feldman, 1982, 1989; Feldman & Conover, 1983) argues that

stereotypes about the candidate’s political party act as an anchor when judging

the candidate. Additionally, Kenny’s (2004) PERSON model argues that

information shared by judges increases consensus about what the target is like.

This information includes stereotypes about the candidate; schemas about the

candidate’s political party. The schemas about where the political parties stand

on various issues should be similar between voters regardless of their own

political party membership, with the Democratic party consistently seen as liberal

on the issues and Republicans with a more conservative position (Feldman &

Conover, 1983; Hamill, Lodge, & Blake, 1985; Lodge & Hamill, 1986), which

should lead to some degree of agreement about where the candidates stand.

I predicted that learning individuating information about the political

candidates would lead to less use of the subject’s stereotypes of the political

parties when forming impressions of the specific candidates. Since the

Page 33: (1) Information and Political Perception

20

stereotypes about the parties are highly shared between people, this decrease

should be reflected in a decrease in consensus. However, the decrease for the

subjects who learned relevant-diagnostic information should be the smallest.

This is because the information, while decreasing the use of party stereotype is

accompanied by specific information about the candidates’ actual stands, which

as shared information should lead to more consensus than the irrelevant or

relevant but undiagnostic information.

Polarization

The final process examined in the first study is the polarization of the

positions of the two parties. Polarization is the tendency for voters to see the

Democratic and Republican parties at extreme opposition to one another. For

example, during the Vietnam War hawks and doves exaggerated the extremity of

the other’s view (Dawes, Singer, & Lemons, 1972), with doves believing the

hawks were more in favor of the war than the hawks themselves claimed, and

vice versa. The process of politics itself can also increase polarization. Political

deliberation increases polarization, partially due to deferment to individuals with

extreme views and a desire to demonstrate party loyalty (Sunstein, 2002).

Political primaries lead to polarization, which accelerates during the campaign

season (Raven & Gallo, 1965). In addition to polarization of the candidates’

positions, by extension the voters’ opinions are also polarized due to political

debate between political candidates, which in turn, leads to polarization of public

opinion (Zaller, 1992). Our government’s policy of public decision making also

Page 34: (1) Information and Political Perception

21

increases polarization, through repeated expression of each parties’ often

diametrically opposed position (Stasavage, 2007).

While this measure does not directly address the use of individuating

information versus heuristics, it does have a number of interesting and

sometimes problematic outcomes. For example, party polarization increases

legislative gridlock (Jones, 2001); believing that legislators on the other side of

the aisle are more opposed to your positions than they actually are makes

compromise much more difficult. Additionally, party polarization decreases split-

ticket voting, party defection, vote swing, and electoral volatility (Brown & Wright,

1992). The prevalence of polarization within politics and the outcomes it

engenders makes it an area of study both interesting and vital.

Both increased relevancy and increased diagnosticity of the information

were predicted to lead to increased polarization of candidates from the

Democratic and Republican Parties. I expected that relevance would increase

the use of the subject’s existing schemas about the position of the candidates’

political parties when forming impressions about their position (Conover, 1981).

Diagnosticity should increase polarization since the information given was

consistent with the positions of each candidate’s party, which by nature are

polarized.

Strength of Affiliation.

One possible mediator of the processes I investigated (projection,

consensus, and polarization) was voters’ strength of affiliation towards their

political party. I predicted that highly affiliated subjects would use their own

Page 35: (1) Information and Political Perception

22

position more when judging both ingroup (increased projection) and outgroup

(increased contrast) candidates since highly affiliated subjects should be more

likely to incorporate new, stereotypically consistent information into their existing

schemas (stereotypes) of the candidates (Shanteau, 1988).

Another question that this study addressed is how the judges’ strength of

affiliation to their political party moderates consensus. The tendency for strongly

affiliated voters to vote consistently within their party suggests they have a more

social identification style of voting, which means that they should agree about

what the candidates are like more than weakly identified voters. This should

occur because more strongly affiliated judges rely more heavily on beliefs about

what each of the political parties is like when judging what the candidates are like

(Conover, 1981; Sigel, 1964). The influence of affiliation strength on consensus

has not been investigated in any domain, much less within politics, so this

examination should be informative to both social and political psychologists alike.

It is possible that the more strongly affiliated a person is to a group the more they

use stereotypic beliefs about both the in-group and out-group to form

impressions of the members of those groups. If that is so, strongly affiliated

individuals should agree more with one another than less strongly affiliated group

members. This finding, along with the effect of affiliation strength on projection,

would support the argument that more affiliated voters rely more on heuristics –

both their own position and the stereotypic position of each of the parties – when

forming impressions of political candidates.

Page 36: (1) Information and Political Perception

23

I expected that the subject’s strength of affiliation would increase

polarization. As mentioned above, more affiliated subjects should rely more on

their stereotypes about how each party stands on the issue when forming

impressions of where the candidates stand, often with the two parties opposing

each other (Conover, 1981; Sigel, 1964). This existing schema of the two parties

being opposed to one another should increase polarization of the individual

candidates. Additionally, if the more affiliated subjects use their own impression

more in judging the ingroup candidates (projection) and the subjects are more

extreme on the issues; this should increase the perceived extremity of ingroup

candidates, which in turn will increase polarization.

Method

At pretest subjects rated their own attitudes on eight political issues, as

well as their political party membership, strength of affiliation to their party, level

of political knowledge, and their personalities. At the experimental session the

subjects viewed photographs of 8 political candidates, one half of which were

identified as Republican, and the other half as Democrat. They rated where they

perceived each of the candidates to stand on each of 8 political issues (two

questions per issue). Finally, the subjects were given short statements about

each of the candidates that were either irrelevant to the political issues, were

relevant to political attitudes but undiagnostic of the candidate’s actual position,

or information that was both relevant and diagnostic. Subjects again made

ratings of their perceptions of the candidates’ attitudes.

Page 37: (1) Information and Political Perception

24

Subjects

119 subjects were recruited from the Introductory Psychology subject pool

at the University of Colorado, Boulder and the Psychology Paid subject pool.

Subjects were recruited based on their responses to the political membership

item on the pre-test questionnaire. 61 self-identified Democrats and 58 self-

identified Republicans were recruited to participate. The subjects were randomly

assigned to one of three experimental conditions (irrelevant, relevant-

undiagnostic, or relevant-diagnostic).

Materials

Questionnaires. The core ratings were composed of 10 bipolar

personality trait rating items, 16 attitude items, a bipolar measure of liberalism-

conservatism, and a number of other ratings detailed below. The personality

items consisted of two bipolar Likert scales for each of the Big-Five personality

dimensions (Norman, 1963). The scales for extraversion were extroverted at one

end and shy at the other, and reserved-outgoing; those for agreeableness were

disagreeable-agreeable and good natured-irritable, the two conscientiousness

measures were trustworthy-untrustworthy and irresponsible-responsible,

neuroticism were self-doubting-self-confident and self-assured-insecure, and for

culture they were knowledgeable-uninformed and unintelligent-intelligent (see

Appendix E).

Attitudes were measured using two statements on eight political issues.

The eight issues were: abortion, taxation, immigration, gay rights, gun laws,

national defense, health care, and energy. For each issue there was one

Page 38: (1) Information and Political Perception

25

statement expressing a conservative viewpoint and one statement expressing a

liberal viewpoint. For example, the liberal abortion statement was: “I support a

woman’s right to abortion on demand.” The conservative abortion statement

was: “I view abortion as the taking of a human life.” When indicating their own

position the subjects rated how much they agreed with each statement (Appendix

E). When rating the target candidates the subjects indicated where they perceive

the candidate to stand on the issue (Appendix F).

Overall political ideology was measured using a bipolar measure (see

Appendix E). The subjects indicated where they fell along the liberal to

conservative spectrum as well as where they perceived each of the target

candidates stood.

Targets. The targets consisted of 8 black and white photographs of

middle-aged white males in suits and ties. The photos were cropped so that only

the face and upper torso was visible (Appendix A). Each photograph was

randomly assigned a name with 4 identified as Republican and 4 as Democratic

candidates. Additionally, the political party identification and the political

statements attached to each photograph were counterbalanced across subjects,

so that the photographs that were assigned to Democrats in Set-A were assigned

to Republicans in Set-B, and those assigned to Republicans in Set-A were

assigned to Democrats in Set-B.

Experimental Conditions. In the second wave of impression formation, the

previously presented target information (including the candidate’s photograph,

name, and party affiliation) were paired with a statement about the candidate.

Page 39: (1) Information and Political Perception

26

Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions, each

of which received different statements about the political candidates (see

Appendices B, C, and D). While the target photograph was counterbalanced

across subjects the names and statements were consistently paired with a

candidate from a specific political party. For example, all the statements below

were consistently paired with the candidate William McCartell, a Democrat, while

the photograph accompanying the name and statement was counterbalanced.

The irrelevant statements consisted of four pieces of information that were

irrelevant to forming impressions of the candidates’ political views. For each

candidate the statement contained their favorite song, favorite quote, last

vacation, and a hobby (Appendix B). For example:

William McCartell is an avid golfer and also enjoys refurbishing antique furniture. His favorite song is “Thunder Road” by Bruce Springsteen. His last vacation was scuba-diving off the Great Barrier Reef in Australia. William McCartell’s favorite quote is “You can’t build a reputation on what you are going to do” by inventor Henry Ford. The relevant-undiagnostic statements contained ambiguous statements

about the candidates’ political attitudes (see Appendix C). These statements

were designed to give the impression of containing attitude information but were

actually not informative. For example:

Our nation needs to re-examine its priorities. We are at a cross-road in our history and need a leader with the vision to guide us to a better future. William McCartell is the leader to do it. He has plans to refocus our country’s priorities on job creation, education, and health care. William McCartell, defending America.

The relevant-diagnostic statements in fact gave information about where

the candidates stood on the political issues, which was always stereotypically

Page 40: (1) Information and Political Perception

27

consistent with the candidate’s stated political party membership. Therefore all

the politically relevant diagnostic information given to the subjects about the

candidates was consistent with the positions that are stereotypically associated

with the candidates’ parties. These statements were identical to the relevant-

undiagnostic statements except they included two unambiguous position

statements (Appendix D). For example:

Our nation needs to re-examine its priorities. We are at a cross-road in our history and need a leader with the vision to guide us to a better future. William McCartell is the leader to do it. He has plans to refocus our country’s priorities on job creation, education, and health care. He plans to increase Medicare coverage, and has outlined a universal health care plan. He will focus on keeping our families and our country safe, by bringing estranged nations such as Syria and Iran to the bargaining table. William McCartell, defending America.

As with the first rating period, during the second rating period, the

photographs, names, and political party affiliation were present in the upper left

corner of each page. However, the political statement was also present at the

top of each page to insure the proper target was being judged, as well as

allowing the subject to refer back to the information contained in the statements

(Appendix F).

Procedure

Pretest. Prior to recruitment, the subjects completed a pretest

questionnaire. The questionnaire included a question about which political party

the subject belonged to, which was used for recruitment. The questionnaire also

included the personality, attitude, and liberal-conservative ideology measures

mentioned above. The subjects answered these items about their own

Page 41: (1) Information and Political Perception

28

personality, political attitudes, and ideology. In addition, the subjects rated their

own affiliation to their self-identified political party, their level of political

knowledge, and their interest in politics.

Experimental Session. Upon arrival at the laboratory, subjects were

informed that they were taking part in a study on the formation of impressions of

political candidates. The experimental session took place in an open room with

individual carrels for the subjects.

During the experimental session, the subjects rated the target candidates

at two times (detailed below). These ratings consisted of the personality,

attitude, and liberal-conservative ideology measures mentioned above. For this

task the subjects indicated their perceptions of each of the candidates’

personalities, political attitudes, and ideology.

In the first wave of impression formation the subjects formed impressions

of the candidates using only photographs of the targets and their name and

political party. The targets were presented one at a time to the subjects. During

the rating period the photograph, name, and party of the target was present on

each page of ratings for that target. This insured that the subject was making the

ratings for the correct target.

Once the subjects were seated the experimenter handed out the first

booklet, which contained the photograph, name, and political party of the target

candidates. The subjects were told:

You will now be forming impressions of the eight political candidates. Inside of the booklet is a page with the photograph, name, and political party of each of the candidates. In a moment you will be told to flip to the first page and form an impression of the candidate. You will have 15

Page 42: (1) Information and Political Perception

29

seconds to study each page and form an impression of the candidate contained on that page. You will be told when the 15 seconds has elapsed and be instructed to turn to the next candidate. Once you have formed impressions of all the candidates you will rate each of them on a number of personality and political attitude items. Please turn to the first candidate now.

After 15 seconds the subjects were told to flip to the next candidate. This

continued until the subjects had viewed all eight candidates.

After completing the impression formation task the subjects completed the

first set of ratings about all of the candidates. The experimenter handed out the

second booklet which contained the first target questionnaire. The subjects were

then told:

You will now make ratings of each of the candidates’ political attitudes and personalities. On each page of the questionnaire are the photograph, name, and political party of the candidate you are currently rating. There are two pages for each candidate. You will have 15 minutes to complete this questionnaire. Once you have completed the questionnaire please put it face down in front of you. We will move on to the next phase once the 15 minutes have elapsed, so if you complete the questionnaire before the 15 minutes have elapsed please sit quietly. You may now begin.

After completing the first set of ratings the experimenter collected the

rating booklet. The subjects then completed the second impression formation

task. This task was similar to the first impression task, except that in addition to

the photographs, names, and political party of each target candidate the booklet

contained the statements about the candidates which differed depending on the

condition the subject was assigned to. The subjects had 1 minute to read

through each of the statements, with the experimenter telling them when to

advance on to the next target. One minute allowed the subjects enough time to

comfortably read through each statement. The subjects were told:

Page 43: (1) Information and Political Perception

30

You will now be reading statements about each of the candidates. On each page of this booklet are the photographs, names, and political parties of each of the candidates that you viewed earlier, as well as a short statement about each of the candidates. You will have one minute to read through each statement and view the photograph. You will be told when to advance to the next page. You will then be asked to re-rate the individuals using your new impressions of them. Please turn to the first candidate.

After the subjects completed the second impression formation task they re-rated

the candidates, based on the new information they had learned about them.

They were told:

You will now make a final rating of each of the candidates. The questionnaire is very similar to the previous one. In addition to the photograph of the candidate at the top of the page, the statement about that candidate will also be present so that you can refer back to it to make your ratings if needed. Do not worry about how you previously rated each individual when making your ratings, you will be using new information to form your impression so do not be concerned if you are rating them the same or differently than the time before. You will again have 15 minutes to complete this task. Once you have completed the questionnaire please place it face down in front of you.

Once all the subjects had completed the task they were debriefed, thanked for

their time, and excused.

Results

The purpose of Study One was to examine the effect of various types of

information on impressions of political candidates’ attitudes. I examined how the

diagnosticity and relevance of information about political candidates influenced

the extent to which the judges used their own attitudes to form impressions

(projection), their level of agreement about the candidates (consensus), and how

differently they rated Democrat and Republican candidates from each other

(polarization).

Page 44: (1) Information and Political Perception

31

Strength of Affiliation

Before examining the impression variable I will discuss the results of the

subjects’ self reported strength of affiliation to their own party. In general the

distribution of strength of party affiliation was normal (Figure 1). Additionally,

Democrat subjects reported higher levels of affiliation than did Republican

candidates. t(1,119) = 2.89, p < .01 (Means: Democrats = 4.77, Republicans

4.12).

Figure 1. Affiliation. Frequency Distribution of subjects’ self-rated

strength of affiliation to their political party.

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Subject Self-Rated Affiliation Strength

Number of Subjects

Projection

Overview. I predicted that projection would be greater to the ingroup

candidates (Allen & Wilder, 1979; Clement & Krueger, 2002; Wilder, 1984), and

that projection would decrease at Time 2, after the subjects learned information

about the candidates, however the more ambiguous the statements were the

smaller the decrease.

Page 45: (1) Information and Political Perception

32

Analyses. The projection results were calculated using a multi-level

regression. For the first level, a regression was conducted for each subject,

predicting ratings of the eight target candidates’ positions across the 16 attitude

items and across the two time periods (for a total of 256 “observations” for each

regression). The first level of the regression included all the within subject

variables with the following model:

AR = β0 + β1S + β2P + β3T + β4S*P + β5S*T + β6P*T + β7S*P*T + ε

AR represents the judge’s rating of the candidate on an attitude item, with self-

rating for that item represented by S. P is a contrast coded variable indicating

whether the candidate is a member of the judge’s political party (-1 = Outgroup

and +1 = Ingroup). The time point (T) when the rating was made is denoted by -1

as Time1 and +1 as Time 2. Finally, all the two-way and the three-way

interactions are included.

These regressions resulted in a beta for each subject for each of the

factors in the model. The second level analyses regressed the resulting slopes

onto the two contrast coded experimental condition variables (Irrelevant vs. the

average of the two relevant conditions, and Relevant-Diagnostic vs. Relevant-

Undiagnostic), a contrast code of the judge’s political party (-1 = Democrat and

+1 = Republican), the mean deviated rating of the judge’s strength of political

affiliation and all the interactions1.

Results. The betas were analyzed to examine projection effects,

specifically the betas that included the subjects’ self-ratings, specifically β1, β4,

1 Note: Target-Set (Set-A versus Set-B) did not interact with self-rating or interaction containing

self-rating and thus Target-Set was excluded from the analysis.

Page 46: (1) Information and Political Perception

33

β5, and β7. I expected both Democrat and Republican judges to show identical

projection effects specifically that β1 would not depend on Self-Party, which was

indeed the case, t(1,109) = -.66, p = .51.

Testing β1 (the effect of self-rating) against zero, the judges used their

own political attitudes when forming impressions of the candidates’ attitudes, t(1,

109) = 8.25, p < .0001. This result supports the first hypothesis that the subjects

would project their own attitudes onto the candidates.

Examining the beta of the Self-rating X Ingroup/Outgroup factor (β4)

projection depended on whether the candidate was a member of the judge’s

political party or not, t(1,109) = 6.49, p < .0001, with greater projection towards

ingroup candidates. The two-way interaction depended upon the judge’s

strength of political affiliation, t(1,109) = 2.27, p = .03, with the ingroup-outgroup

projection difference being greater for more affiliated subjects (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Projection. Unstandardized Regression Coefficients between

subjects’ self-ratings and their ratings of the targets, by ingroup/outgroup

and affiliation (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation).

-0.5

-0.4

-0.3

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

Lo Affiliation Hi Affiliation

Projection

Ingroup

Outgroup

Page 47: (1) Information and Political Perception

34

I predicted that once the subjects received additional information about the

candidates they would decrease the amount of projection. Testing the beta from

Self-rating X Time (β5) against zero, this did not appear to be the case, t(1,109) =

1.49, p = .14. However, the predicted three-way interaction of projection, time,

and ingroup/outgroup was significant, t(1,109) = -2.69, p < .01 (see Figure 3).

Examining the simple changes over time for outgroup, projection increased at

Time 2, t(1,109) = -2.68, p < .01, with significant contrast (negative projection)

towards the outgroup candidates at Time 1, t(1,109) = -2.21, p = .03, and no

projection at Time 2, p = .39. Examining the simple change over time for the

Ingroup there was no change in projection over time, t(1,109) = -1.41, p = .16.

Projection towards the ingroup candidates was significant at both Time 1,

t(1,109) = 11.21, p <.0001 and Time 2, t(1,109)= 6.94, p < .0001. While the

mean level of projection towards the ingroup decreased between Time 1 and

Time 2, this difference was not statistically significant, something that had been

predicted.

The main prediction was that projection would decrease in the two

relevant conditions but remain stable in the irrelevant condition. However,

testing the Self X Time beta (β5S*T), neither the irrelevant versus the two

relevant contrast, t(1,109) = -0.06, p = .95, nor the relevant-undiagnostic versus

relevant-diagnostic contrast, t(1,109) = -1.18, p = .24 significantly moderated

changes across time in projection.

Page 48: (1) Information and Political Perception

35

Figure 3. Projection. Unstandardized Regression Coefficients between

subjects’ self-ratings and their ratings of the targets, by time and

ingroup/outgroup.

-0.5

-0.4

-0.3

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

Time 1 Time 2

Projection

Outgroup

Ingroup

In Pennefather and Park (2006) the decrease in projection was only

significant for ingroup candidates, however testing the Self X Time beta (β5S*T)

for only the ingroup candidates neither the irrelevant versus the two relevant

contrast, t(1,109) = 0.59, p = .55, nor the relevant-undiagnostic versus relevant-

diagnostic contrast, t(1,109) = 0.13, p = .90 significantly moderated the decrease

in projection for ingroup candidates with additional information. Finally,

examining just the relevant-diagnostic condition the Self X Time interaction was

not significant, t(1,39) = -.84, p = .41. In general, neither the relevancy nor the

diagnosticity of the information moderated projection. The sole exception was

that the more strongly affiliated the subjects were to their political party, the more

they projected towards both ingroup and outgroup candidates when the

information was irrelevant, t(1, 109) = 2.43, p = .017.

Page 49: (1) Information and Political Perception

36

As part of our analyses there were a number of non-projection effects.

These effects were found examining the betas that did not include the subjects’

self-ratings, specifically β0, β2, β3, and β6. There was a significant main effect of

time (β3), t(1, 109) = 2.53, p = .01, wherein all the candidates were rated as more

liberal at Time 2. Additionally, examining the intercept beta (β0) there was a main

effect of the subject’s strength of affiliation, t(1,109) = 3.11, p < .01, with more

affiliated subjects rating the candidates as more liberal. Again examining the

intercept beta, there was a main effect of the first contrast coded condition

variable, t(1,109) = 2.20, p = .03, with the subjects in the two relevant conditions

rating all the targets as more liberal than subjects in the irrelevant condition. This

effect was due to the content of the statements, with no actual political

information contained in the irrelevant statements, all of the statements in the

relevant condition may have been written as more liberal. There was also a

significant interaction of the subject’s political party and target party, t(1,109) =

16.78, p < .0001, with Democrat subjects seeing their ingroup members as more

liberal. The target party by time beta (β6) interacted with the subject’s party,

t(1,109) = -3.16, p < .01, with the Democrats seeing their ingroup candidates as

even more liberal at Time 2. Again this effect was due to the content of the

statements. This three-way interaction was further moderated by the second

contrast coded condition, t(1,109) = -2.51, p = .01, with Democrats in the

relevant-undiagnostic condition seeing their ingroup as even more liberal at Time

2 than Time 1, than the Democrats in the relevant-diagnostic condition. This

interaction was moderated by the subject’s strength of affiliation, t(1,109) = 2.00,

Page 50: (1) Information and Political Perception

37

p = .05, with low affiliated Democrats showing the above effect more strongly

than highly affiliated Democrats.

These patterns of findings indicate that in general neither the diagnosticity

nor relevance of information influenced the extent to which the judges used their

own positions to form impressions of the political candidates in this study.

Rather, the judges used their own positions more when making judgments about

ingroup candidates than outgroup candidates and this did not depend on the

relevancy or diagnosticity of the information they learned about the candidates.

In a previous study (Pennefather & Park, 2006) the amount of projection towards

the ingroup candidates decreased after receiving relevant-undiagnostic

information, while in this study projection towards the ingroup did not change

over time. Additionally, there was significant contrast towards the outgroup at

Time 1, which became zero at Time 2.

Consensus

Overview. Next, the influence of the different levels of information on

agreement between the subjects about what the candidates were like was

examined. The first prediction was that the subjects would show significant

agreement about the candidates’ attitudes. More importantly it was predicted

that individuating information would decrease the use of heuristics, which are

shared by subjects, resulting in a decrease in the level of consensus about the

candidates’ attitudes. Thus, I predicted that the level of consensus about the

candidates would decrease at Time 2 once the subjects learned more

individuating information about the candidates and relied less on their

Page 51: (1) Information and Political Perception

38

stereotypes about the parties. The decrease in consensus should be less when

the information was diagnostic of actual position, since the shared information in

the relevant condition was more predictive of position it may have been used

more than the information in the irrelevant condition, compensating somewhat for

the decreased use of the party stereotype. Schemas that voters have about the

political parties are the most powerful in politics (Conover & Feldman, 1986,

1989; Hamill, Lodge, & Blake, 1985; Nicholson, 2005), which may result in high

agreement before the individuating information is learned.

Analysis. The consensus analyses examined the extent to which the

judges agreed about what the candidates were like. The consensus results were

calculated first using BLOCKO (Kenny, 1983). BLOCKO is a program for the

analysis of dyadic block designs. The current study was a half-block design in

which measurements were taken only from members of one group (judge-

subjects) about their impressions of the other (target-candidates). BLOCKO

allowed us to decompose the variance in ratings into three components: target,

judge, and relationship.

Target effects measure the average response for a target across judges,

that is how much of the total variance in ratings can be attributed to the targets

being rated. For example, Candidate X is seen by others as strongly liberal.

Thus target effects are a measure of the agreement between the judges about

what the targets are like, so in the current study I use absolute target variance as

the measure of consensus.

Page 52: (1) Information and Political Perception

39

The judge effects are the measure of the rater’s general response across

targets for a particular item. For example, Judge A may rate the candidates as

being strongly liberal. This consistent judgment of others as strongly liberal could

be due to a measurement artifact, that is, Judge A tends to use the high end of

the scale. More interestingly, judge effects could be a measure of a person’s

psychological processing of other people.

Relationship effects measure the interaction of judge and target effects.

The relationship effect is the idiosyncratic perception that a judge has about a

particular target. Thus, the relationship effect operates at the dyadic level

between a single judge and a single target, while judge and target effects are the

average effect across raters and those being rated.

Judges sharing party, condition, and target-set were combined into

groups, with between eight and eleven judges in each of the 12 party by

condition by target-set groups. The variance decomposition, analyzing party and

condition differences in consensus was performed. For each group the level of

absolute stable variance due to target (consensus) was calculated. The variance

decomposition was performed on eight issue constructs; each of these

constructs was composed of the two attitude items that address the same issue.

After this group level analysis the amount of consensus due to each judge was

calculated using a jack-knifing technique (see Park & Judd, 1989), whereby each

judge was left out of the BLOCKO analysis and the amount of absolute stable

target variance remaining was included in the following formula to find the level of

target variance due to each judge (for a group with 11 judges): Vi = (11 * V11) –

Page 53: (1) Information and Political Perception

40

(10 * V11-1), where Vi is the ith subject’s psuedovalue for a given variance. V11 is

the variance estimate from the full sample (one Subject Party X Condition X

Target Set group at one Time, in this case having 11 judges) and V11-1 is the

variance estimate of the sample excluding that subject’s data. I ended up with a

measure of each subject’s individual contribution to his or her group’s consensus

for each of the eight attitude constructs at both Time 1 and Time 2. The eight

scores at each time were then averaged to create a single measure of attitude

consensus at Time 1 and Time 2.

These consensus scores were regressed on the two contrast-coded

Information Condition variables (Irrelevant vs. the two relevant conditions, and

Relevant-Diagnostic vs. Relevant-Undiagnostic), Rating Period (-1 = Time 1 and

+1 = Time 2), Subject Party (+1 = Democrat and -1 = Republican), Target Set,

and all the interactions.

Results. I predicted that the overall level of consensus would differ from

zero, indicating that there was agreement overall about the candidates’ attitudes,

which was indeed the case at both Time 1, F(1,120) = 7.16, p < .0001 and Time

2, F(1,120) = 6.00, p < .0001 (see Figure 4). Next, the prediction that

individuating information would lead to a decrease in consensus was not

supported, F(1,120) = 1.70, p = .19. Neither relevancy (Condition 2 contrast),

F(1,120) = 2.29, p = .13 nor diagnosticity (Condition 1 contrast), F(1,120) = 0.79,

p = .38, were significant. Therefore the shared stereotypes about the positions of

each political party on the issues lead to agreement about the candidates’

positions when no individuating information was known, which did not change

Page 54: (1) Information and Political Perception

41

when they learned individuating information about each candidate. Increased

relevance and diagnosticity of the information did not, in general influence

agreement more than the irrelevant information did.

Figure 4. Consensus. Average agreement between subjects’ ratings of the

targets on the 16 attitude items by time and information condition.

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

0.3

0.35

Irrelevant Rel-Undiag Rel-Diag

Information Condition

Average Relative Partner Variance

Time 1

Time 2

However, there was a significant interaction between the subject’s party

and the first condition contrast-code F(1,97) = 4.29, p = .04, and a marginal

interaction between the subject’s party and the second condition contrast F(1,97)

= 3.70, p = .06. While the Democrat subjects in all the information condition had

equally higher levels of agreement than Republican subjects, this difference was

less in the relevant-diagnostic condition (see Figure 5). The interaction of the

subjects’ party and first condition contrast depended on time, F(1,97) = 3.66, p =

.06. Examining simple changes in the consensus between Time1 and Time 2 for

each condition, the change was significant for the subjects in the relevant-

Page 55: (1) Information and Political Perception

42

undiagnostic condition, F(1,37) = 4.34, p = .04, but not for the irrelevant

condition, p = .78, nor the relevant-diagnostic, p = .53.

Figure 5. Consensus. Average agreement between subjects’ ratings of the

targets on the 16 attitude items by subjects’ political party and information

condition.

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

0.3

0.35

0.4

Irrelevant Rel-Undiag Rel-Diag

Information Condition

Average Relative Partner Variance

Dem Subjects

Rep Subjects

Additionally, there was a main effect of target set, with subjects who

received the first target set agreeing more about what the candidates were like,

F(1,97) = 12.44, p < .001. The target set effect was moderated by time, F(1,97)

= 4.42, p = .04, with the difference in consensus between the target sets being

larger at Time 2.

One possibility for this difference due to target set is that in the first set the

Democrat candidates all appeared younger than the Republicans, while in the

second set this was reversed. This unintentional difference could have resulted

in the use of two different stereotypes when judging the candidates. The first

Page 56: (1) Information and Political Perception

43

stereotype is the expected one that Democrat candidates are more liberal. The

second stereotype is that older people are more conservative. In the first target

set both the young-liberal and Democrat-liberal stereotypes and the older-

conservative and Republican conservative stereotypes were going in the same

direction. In the second target set the two stereotypes were working against

each other. If the subjects who received the second target set used the age and

political party stereotypes to different degrees the amount of agreement about

the candidates in that condition would suffer, leading to a target set difference.

Polarization

Overview. As mentioned in the introduction there are a number of

processes that lead to seeing the candidates from the two parties as holding

more extreme attitudes, or polarization. I predicted that both the diagnosticity

and the relevance of the information that our subjects received about the

candidates would influence how polarized they saw the candidates. I predicted

that receiving information about the candidates that was relevant to their attitude

positions should lead to increases in perceived polarization. This should have

been especially true when the information was diagnostic of the candidate’s

actual position (as well as party consistent), which reinforced the existing

stereotypes about each of the parties’ positions. Specifically, subjects in the two

relevant conditions should see the candidates as more polarized than the

subjects in the irrelevant condition. Additionally, the subjects in the relevant-

diagnostic condition should see more polarization than those subjects in the

relevant-undiagnostic condition.

Page 57: (1) Information and Political Perception

44

Analysis. The polarization results were calculated using a General Linear

Model (GLM) analysis. First, the attitude items were reverse scored so that they

were all scored higher for the liberal end. For each subject the average of the 16

attitude items for each target candidate was calculated. The average of these

ratings across time and target party were calculated, which resulted in four

scores: Democrat Targets-Time 1, Democrat Targets-Time 2, Republican

Targets-Time 1, and Republican Targets-Time 2. These four scores were then

included as repeated measures in a GLM, with the two contrast-coded

Information Condition variables (Irrelevant vs. the two relevant conditions, and

Relevant-Diagnostic vs. Relevant-Undiagnostic), Subject Party (+1 = Democrat

and -1 = Republican), subject’s strength of affiliation, and all the interactions as

predictors in the model.

Results. The was a significant main effect for target party, F(1,108) =

285.02, p < .0001, indicating that the subjects perceived polarization between the

Democrat and Republican target candidates. This effect was not moderated by

the subject’s political party membership, F(1,108) = 2.72, p = .10, indicating that

both Democrat and Republican subjects perceived the same amount of

polarization, though directionally the Democrat subjects showed more

polarization. The target party effect was moderated by time, F(1,108) = 9.18, p <

.01, with less perceived polarization once the judges learned information about

the candidates. This two-way interaction of time and target party was further

moderated by a three-way interaction with the second condition contrast,

Page 58: (1) Information and Political Perception

45

F(1,108) = 5.44, p = .02 (see Figure 6). To better examine this effect I ran

separate analyses examining the simple change in polarization for each

Figure 6. Polarization. Average rating of Democrat targets on the 16

attitude issues minus the average rating of the Republican targets by time

and information condition.

Polarization by Time and Condition

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

Irrelevant Rel-Undiag Rel-Diag

Condition

Average Democrat Minus

Republican Rating

Time 1

Time 2

condition. The decrease in polarization at Time 2 was significant for subjects in

the relevant-undiagnostic condition, F(1,36) = 3.94, p < .001, and marginal for the

irrelevant condition, F(1,37) = 1.87, p = .07. However there was not a significant

decrease in the relevant-diagnostic condition with more information, F(1,35) = -

.01, p =.99. As predicted when subjects learned either irrelevant or relevant-

undiagnostic information they decreased their perceptions of polarization while

subjects who learned relevant-diagnostic information did not, demonstrating that

the diagnosticity of the information influences how extreme candidates from the

two parties are seen. There was a significant three-way interaction between

target party, subject’s strength of affiliation, and the subject’s political party,

Page 59: (1) Information and Political Perception

46

F(1,108) = 4.82, p = .03. While highly affiliated subjects from both parties

showed equally high degrees of polarization, the low affiliated Republicans

showed less polarization than low affiliated Democrats (see Figure 7). Finally,

there was a main effect of Time, F(1,108) = 6.53, p = .01, with subjects

perceiving all of the targets as generally more liberal at Time 2. It could be that

the statements were all written to liberally, even those about the Republican

candidates.

Figure 7. Polarization. Average rating of Democrat targets on the 16

attitude issues minus the average rating of the Republican targets by

subject’s political party membership and strength of affiliation (+1 and -1

standard deviation).

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

Lo Aff Hi Aff

Average Democrat Minus Average

Republican

Dem Subjects

Rep Subjects

While the predicted overall polarization difference between the two

relevant conditions and the irrelevant condition was not found, as predicted, the

subjects in the relevant-diagnostic condition did not decrease the extent to which

they polarized the candidates, while subjects in the other two conditions did.

Page 60: (1) Information and Political Perception

47

Discussion

Study One was designed to investigate how relevant and diagnostic

information influenced impression formation. I expected that any individuating

information, be it relevant and/or diagnostic would decrease the relationship

between the subject’s own positions and those of the candidates, specifically

ingroup candidates. This was not the case; neither type of information

significantly changed the extent to which the subjects projected. The subjects

did project more towards the ingroup than towards the outgroup. Also, when the

subjects learned more about the ingroup candidates than merely their name,

political party, and a photograph the amount the subjects projected decreased

directionally, but not significantly. On the other hand, when the subjects learned

only the basic information about the outgroup candidates the subjects contrasted

their positions from their perceptions of the outgroup candidates’ positions

(reverse projection), but after learning individuating information they no longer

projected in either direction.

In a previous study (Pennefather & Park, 2006) subjects decreased their

projection towards ingroup candidates after learning relevant-undiagnostic

information about them, however, in the current study projection towards the

ingroup did not change significantly, even when only the relevant-undiagnostic

condition was examined. The only differences between the current study’s

relevant-undiagnostic condition and the previous study was the collection of self-

ratings of the attitude measures at pre-test rather than during the first part of the

Page 61: (1) Information and Political Perception

48

experimental session and the information was not counter-balanced across party.

Why the decrease in projection did not attain significance, and differed from the

previous investigation will need further examination.

Kenny’s (2004) PERSON model argues that increases in shared

knowledge about other people increases agreement between raters.

Directionally, the findings supported our predictions that when subjects had very

little information, which included party membership of the candidates, they would

rely more on the stereotypes of each of the parties. While overall the decrease

in consensus was not significant at Time 2, it did decrease significantly in the

relevant-undiagnostic condition. In the current study Democrats agreed with one

another more than the Republicans regardless of the type of information, but less

so when the information was relevant and diagnostic. These results suggest that

Democrats have a stronger shared stereotype, about where each of the parties

stand on the issues, or use the stereotype more. However, when the information

was relevant and diagnostic it appears to have increased the use of the

stereotypes by the Republican subjects. The fact that the Republicans agreed

less about the candidates could be due to the fact they were less strongly

affiliated to their party, t(1,119) = 2.89, p < .01 (Means: Democrats = 4.77,

Republicans 4.12) and less politically knowledgeable, t(1,119) = 2.33, p = .05

(Means: Democrats = 4.05. Republicans = 3.59) than the Democrat subjects.

As expected the subjects perceived the candidates from the two parties as

less extreme once they learned more information about them. This was

expected because prior to receiving the information at Time 2, the subjects were

Page 62: (1) Information and Political Perception

49

relying more heavily on their stereotypes about each party, which by nature were

polarized. The prediction that the diagnosticity of the information would also

influence polarization was supported, with decreases in polarization at Time 2 for

the subjects who learned relevant-undiagnostic and irrelevant (marginal)

information, but not for those who learned relevant-diagnostic information.

Interestingly, Democrat subjects directionally perceived more polarization than

did Republican subjects. It is possible that this trend is due to Democrats being

more numerous on campus, and thus more powerful, resulting in more perceived

polarization by Democrat subjects (Ebenbach & Keltner, 1998).

It appears that the subjects were using the information rationally in their

perceptions of candidate extremity. The relevant-diagnostic information was

designed to express party consistent positions, which as mentioned above are

the nature of the issues. This information would reinforce existing party

stereotypes, which resulted in no change in polarization between Time 1 when

they were relying heavily on the party stereotypes and Time 2, when they learned

the diagnostic information, which was consistent with the stereotype. It is

interesting however, that polarization decreased in the other two conditions,

though only marginally in the irrelevant condition. It seems that individuating

information, when it does not specifically reinforce the political party stereotypes

decreases the use of the polarized party stereotypes. The fact that the decrease

was significant for the irrelevant-diagnostic condition could mean that the

ambiguous nature of the statements resulted in perceptions of the candidates as

more moderate. This would support previous work that suggests that ambiguity

Page 63: (1) Information and Political Perception

50

is the best way to capture moderate and independent voters, especially when the

candidate’s position is substantially different from the median public position

(Campbell, 1983; Kinder, 1978).

One question these results raise is: how does the consistency of the

information influence the impressions? In none of the conditions in Study One

did the subjects learn information that ran counter to their existing stereotypes

about the candidates. In fact, in the relevant-diagnostic condition the information

reinforced the stereotype. The next study examines the impact of consistency on

impressions of political candidates.

Page 64: (1) Information and Political Perception

51

Chapter 3:

Study Two

In Study One the influence that different levels of diagnostic information

have on political perception was examined. In that study all the politically

relevant information given to the subjects about the candidates was consistent

with the positions that are stereotypically associated with the candidates’ parties.

While many political candidates do toe the party line on most issues, it is also the

case that there are candidates that express views that are inconsistent with the

party norm (Kam, 2005). An interesting question is how voters deal with

inconsistent information about political candidates?

Consistency

A prime example of a candidate that expressed party-inconsistent

attitudes was the former Mayor of New York, Rudolph Giuliani. Mr. Giuliani,

during his political career expressed liberal views on abortion, gay marriage, and

gun control. Interestingly, once he announced his candidacy for president in

2007 he quickly became a favorite for the Republican ticket. This reaction could

be expected from fiscal Republicans who may not care as much about social

issues; however, even strong social conservatives like Pat Roberts backed Mr.

Giuliani. How do political party members deal with party-inconsistent information

about their candidates?

Social psychologists have long been interested in stereotype consistency

and its effect on various impression formation processes. Social psychologists

have examined the effects of stereotype consistent and inconsistent information

Page 65: (1) Information and Political Perception

52

on judgments of the group and individuals within the group (Krueger & Rothbart,

1988; Park & Rothbart, 1982). Additionally, researchers have examined the

influence of inconsistency on memory, in some cases finding greater recall for

inconsistent behaviors (Hastie & Kumar, 1979) and in others finding greater

recall for stereotype-consistent events (Rothbart, Evans, & Fulero, 1979). While

these findings seem at odds, Srull, Lichtenstein, and Rothbart (1985) found that

while recall for inconsistent behaviors was greater for individuals and meaningful

groups, the reverse was true for meaningless groups. Moreno and Bodenhausen

(1999) argue that in the face of inconsistent information people typically adopt a

defensive stance to protect their stereotypes about the group, but this can be

moderated if they are motivated to be accurate.

Work in the political domain supports Hastie and Kumar’s (1979), finding

that when the target person’s ideology differed from their own, people

remembered more inconsistent than consistent information, however when the

target had the same ideology as the subject recall for inconsistent information did

not differ from that for consistent information (Wänke & Wyer, 1996). In a series

of elaborate mock election studies Lau and Redlawsk (2006) showed that their

subjects were “flummoxed” when confronted by a counter-stereotypic outgroup

candidate and engaged in more effortful processing, using the rational-

calculation based forms of vote decision making. However, Lau and Redlawsk

(2006) did not examine the influence of a counter-stereotypic ingroup candidate.

Page 66: (1) Information and Political Perception

53

Popularity

During general elections, when the choice is between a candidate from

one’s own party and a candidate from the outgroup party I would argue that

voters, are motivated less by accuracy, and more by a desire to protect their

social identities as members of the party (Moreno & Bodenhausen, 1999). One

reason voters may be less accuracy motivated could be the “horse race”

coverage of the elections. Bartels (1988) states that “reporting focuses on who’s

ahead, who’s gaining ground, and who was first across the finish line. The horse

race only incidentally touches on matters of political substance”. Research has

supported this, with more media coverage of competition than any other form of

information, such as the candidates’ political positions (Robinson & Sheehan,

1983). This type of coverage influences the perceptions of the public about the

candidates, for example voters’ support for candidates is moderated by their

expectations of that candidate’s chance to win (Bartels, 1988). One prime

example of this effect was Jimmy Carter, who was a virtual unknown before the

primaries began, but with a good showing in the first caucus gained enough

momentum to gain the nomination and eventually the Presidency.

As mentioned in the introduction one type of information that voters use to

form impressions of political candidates are endorsements by trusted groups

(Brady & Sniderman, 1985; Lau & Redlawsk, 2001) or individuals (Carmines &

Kuklinski, 1990; Mondak, 1993; Sniderman, Brody, & Tetlock, 1991). How does

knowledge that the candidate is supported by a majority of voters from ones’ own

political party influence how people use inconsistent information in their

Page 67: (1) Information and Political Perception

54

impressions? What would happen with a candidate that expressed inconsistent

attitudes on some of the issues; would it matter if he was popular or not? I

would expect that an inconsistent but popular ingroup candidate would increase

the voter’s use of the party stereotype. Voter should also rely on the

endorsements of their ingroup, making it even less likely that inconsistent

information will influence impressions. The current study also examined possible

mechanisms by which the influence of inconsistent information about candidates

is minimized.

One possible way to deal with inconsistent information is simply to ignore

it. Recent examinations have found that people often do ignore inconsistent

information (Erber & Fiske, 1984). If subjects do ignore inconsistent information

about the candidates, they should exhibit decreased recall of the inconsistent

statements. Research in political science on memory and popularity has shown

that voters have greater recall for supported candidates; however this effect is

moderated by the type of information, with greater recall for positive than

negative items (Lau & Redlawsk, 2006). Thus popularity should increase

recognition in the popular condition, but more so for consistent items.

Another way that voters can deal with inconsistency is to subtype the

inconsistent individual as the exception to the party (Allport, 1954; Weber &

Crocker, 1983). If the subjects are subtyping the inconsistent individual, a task

asking the subjects how representative the target candidate is should

demonstrate this.

Page 68: (1) Information and Political Perception

55

Voters can also deal with candidates who are inconsistent on issues by

changing how much they value the issues, decreasing the importance of the

issues the ingroup candidate expressed inconsistent attitudes on, and increasing

the importance of those the candidate was consistent on. Asking subjects how

important the issues are to them, both before and after learning about the

candidates, allows changes in their valuation of the issues due to exposure to

inconsistent candidates to be examined. Holbrook, Berent, Krosnick, Visser, and

Boninger (2005) argue that the accumulation of new knowledge influences the

importance that people put on the issues. Thus it is possible that when an

ingroup candidate, particularly a popular one, expresses inconsistent views on

issues the voters may change the importance they place on that issue.

The specific predictions of the present study were that subjects would

have more positive evaluations, and be more likely to vote for consistent

candidates. The same should be true for popular candidates. However,

popularity should moderate the influence of inconsistency, with less negative

impact of inconsistency when the candidate is popular. The influence of the

subjects’ strength of political affiliation and their political knowledge on how the

candidate was perceived was also examined. As for mechanisms, I examined

whether these effects were driven by misattribution and failure to encode the

information, or subtyping and changes in valuation of the issues.

Page 69: (1) Information and Political Perception

56

Method

Subjects

75 subjects were recruited from the Introductory Psychology subject pool

at the University of Colorado, Boulder. Subjects were recruited based on their

response to a political membership item on the pretest questionnaire.

Specifically, subjects were recruited from individuals who responded that they

identified themselves with the Democratic Party. Subjects were randomly

assigned to one of the four conditions, with 19 in the Consistent-Popular

condition, 18 in the Consistent-Unpopular condition, 19 in the Inconsistent-

Popular condition, and 19 in the Inconsistent-Unpopular condition. One subject

failed to complete all of the questions and was excluded from analysis.

Materials

Target. The target was a black and white photograph of a middle aged

white male, cropped to show just his upper body and face (Appendix G). The

photograph was accompanied by a name, political party membership

(Democrat), and nine statements about the candidate. Unlike in the previous

study, only one target was used, thus all the subjects received the same

photograph, name, and political party information (Democrat).

Experimental Conditions.

The experimental manipulation crossed the stereotypical-consistency and

popularity of the target candidate, and was between subjects. One half of the

subjects learned that the candidate held stereotype-consistent attitudes on six

issues, as well as three pieces of irrelevant information (i.e. the candidates’

Page 70: (1) Information and Political Perception

57

hobbies; see Appendix H). The other half of the subjects learned that the

candidate held three consistent and three inconsistent attitudes, as well as three

irrelevant pieces of information (see Appendix I).

Popularity was manipulated by having half of the subjects from each

consistency condition learn that the majority of Democrat voters in the

candidate’s home district supported him:

Recently Democrat voters in Stephen Johnson’s home district were polled about whom they planned to vote for in the upcoming Congressional primary. 79 percent of registered Democrats stated they would vote for Stephen Johnson. The next most popular Democrat candidate received only 6 percent support.

The other half of the subjects learned that the candidate had little

Democratic support:

Recently Democrat voters in Stephen Johnson’s home district were polled about whom they planned to vote for in the upcoming Congressional primary. Only 6 percent of registered Democrats stated they would vote for Stephen Johnson. The most popular Democrat candidate received 79 percent support.

This popularity information was presented on the same sheet as the

photograph, name, and party information (Appendix G).

Questionnaires. The core ratings were composed of the 10 bipolar

personality trait rating items, 24 attitude items, a bipolar measure of liberalism-

conservatism, and a number of other ratings detailed below. The personality

items consisted of two bipolar Likert scales for each of the Big-Five personality

dimensions used in Study One (Appendix J).

Page 71: (1) Information and Political Perception

58

Attitudes were measured using two statements on 12 political issues.

Eight of the issues were the same as those used in Study One: abortion,

taxation, immigration, gay rights, gun laws, national defense, health care, and

energy. The four new issues were: welfare, death penalty, Homeland Security,

and drug laws. As with the measures from Study One for each issue there was

one statement expressing a conservative viewpoint and one statement

expressing a liberal viewpoint. When indicating their own position, the subjects

rated how much they agreed with each statement (Appendix J). When rating the

target candidate the subjects indicated where they perceived the candidate to

stand on the issue (Appendix K).

Overall political ideology was measured using a bipolar measure (see

Appendix J), with subjects indicating where they fell along the liberal to

conservative spectrum as well as where they perceived the target candidate to

stand (Appendix K).

Subjects rated how important each of the 12 issues were to themselves,

as well as self-rated strength of affiliation to party and degree of political

knowledge (see Appendix J).

Subjects completed a free recall task, in which, they were given a page of

blank paper and asked to recall as many of the statements about the candidate

as possible.

Subjects completed a cued recognition task (Appendix M), in which, they

were given a sheet containing all of the political statements previously learned,

as well as 18 filler items consisting of 9 items stereotypically consistent with the

Page 72: (1) Information and Political Perception

59

Democratic Party’s stand and 9 items stereotypically inconsistent and indicated

whether they thought the candidate had made the statement or not, and how

sure they were about their choice. This questionnaire also included a subtyping

measure, which asked how typical or atypical were the candidate’s attitudes and

beliefs compared to the attitudes and beliefs of the other members of his political

party (see Appendix N).

Procedure

At pretest subjects indicated their own political party membership, their

strength of party affiliation, the ideology measure, and their positions on the 24

attitude statements. The political party membership question was used in

recruitment (only Democrats were recruited).

Upon arrival at the laboratory, subjects were informed that they were

taking part in a study on the formation of impressions about political candidates.

The experimental session took place in an open room with individual carrels for

the subjects.

Once the subjects had been seated the experimenter handed out the first

booklet. This booklet included the measures of how important each of the issues

was to the subject. After completing the self-ratings the experimenter collected

them, and handed out the booklet containing the candidate target information.

This included the candidate’s name, political party, photograph, and the

popularity condition information and nine statements about the candidate. Once

all the subjects had received their booklet they were instructed as follows:

You will now be forming impressions of the political candidate. Inside of the booklet are the photograph, name, political party, and some

Page 73: (1) Information and Political Perception

60

information about the candidate. This candidate is running for state congress in another state, so you will most likely not be familiar with him beforehand. You will have three minute to read through the statements and view the photograph. Once you have formed impressions about the candidate you will rate him of on a number of personality and political attitude items. Please begin.

Once the subjects had viewed and formed an impression about the candidate

they were given the rating booklet. They were told:

You will now make ratings of the candidate’s political attitudes and personality. You will have 5 minutes to complete this questionnaire. Once you have completed the questionnaire please close it and flip it over and I will collect it. We will move on to the next phase once the 5 minutes have elapsed, so if you complete the questionnaire before the 5 minutes have elapsed please sit quietly. You may now begin.

Next the subjects completed a second self-rating questionnaire, which contained

the 24 own attitude measures, self-ideology, and importance of the issues to

them. After completing this task the subjects completed the recall task. Finally,

the subjects completed the cued recognition and subtyping measures.

Results

Study Two was designed to examine the influence of candidate popularity

and consistency, as well as the subject’s strength of party affiliation and political

knowledge on impressions of the candidate’s attitudes, as well as a number of

other impression variables; such as projection of the subject’s attitudes onto the

candidate, liking of the candidate, likelihood of voting for him, overall perceived

similarity, and perceptions of the candidate’s strength of party affiliation. I

predicted that both consistency and popularity would increase all of these

variables. I also expected that the popularity of the candidate would moderate the

Page 74: (1) Information and Political Perception

61

consistency effect, with consistency mattering less when the candidate was

popular.

In addition to examining these impression variables a number of potential

process variables to account for these changes were included. As mentioned in

the introduction I expected that differences in memory would be responsible for

the impression differences, therefore recall and recognition measures were

taken. While I did not believe that changes in valuation of the issues, nor

subtyping of the candidate would be responsible for the impression differences,

these measures were included as well.

Strength of Affiliation

Before examining the impression variables I will discuss the subjects’ self-

rated strength of affiliation to political party. There were no significant pre-

existing differences in strength of affiliation due to popularity condition (Means:

Popular = 4.58, Unpopular = 4.45) or consistency (Means: Consistent = 4.29,

Inconsistent = 4.73), though the distribution of self-rated affiliation was slightly

negatively skewed (Figure 8).

Impression Variables

Overview. The impression variables were examined first; how much the

subjects liked the candidate, how likely they were to vote for him, how strongly

affiliated they perceived him to be to his political party, and how similar they

perceived him to be to themselves.

Analyses. For all of the impression variables a regression was performed

with the following predictors: contrast coded popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1

Page 75: (1) Information and Political Perception

62

Popular), contrast coded consistency (-1 Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), the

subject’s self-reported mean-deviated political knowledge and strength of party

affiliation, and all the interactions. All of the impression measures were collected

after the subjects completed the attitude impression measures, but before they

re-rated their own positions (see Appendix K).

Figure 8. Affiliation Strength. Frequency Distribution of Subjects’ self-

rated strength of affiliation to their political party.

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Subject Self-Rated Affiliation Strength

Number of Subjects

Evaluation. Evaluation was measured with a single 7-point Likert scale

asking how much the subject liked the candidate, ranging from 1 - disliked very

much, to 7 – liked very much. As predicted there was a significant main effect for

consistency, t(1,74) = 4.82, p < .0001, with subjects in the consistent conditions

liking the candidates more (Means: consistent = 5.19, inconsistent = 3.82) . The

predicted main effect of popularity was not significant, t(1,74) = -0.84, p = .41, nor

was the interaction of consistency and popularity, t(1,74) = 1.32, p = .19. The

Page 76: (1) Information and Political Perception

63

three-way interaction of affiliation, consistency, and popularity was significant,

t(1,74) = -2.01, p = .05 (see Figure 9). While highly affiliated subject liked the

consistent candidate more than the inconsistent candidate regardless of

popularity, less affiliated subjects liked the inconsistent-unpopular candidate as

much as the consistent ones (both popular and unpopular). Separate analyses

testing the effects at both high and low affiliation (+1 and -1 standard deviation)

were run. The consistency by popularity interaction was significant at low

affiliation, t(1,59) = 2.32, p = .02, but not at high affiliation, t(1,59) = -.073, p =

.47.

Figure 9. Evaluation. Subjects’ rated liking of the candidate by

consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1

and +1 Standard Deviation).

Lo Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Liking

Consistent

Inconsistent

Hi Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Liking

Consistent

Inconsistent

Finally, there was a significant interaction of the subject’s strength of

affiliation and political knowledge, t(1,74) = 2.15, p = .04, with more highly

affiliated-highly knowledgeable subjects liking the candidates more overall.

I predicted that the subjects would use the consistency information more

when the candidate was unpopular. For highly affiliated subjects, directionally

Page 77: (1) Information and Political Perception

64

the pattern was as expected, though not significantly. Low affiliated subjects

showed the opposite effect, with consistency information mattering less when the

candidate was unpopular. It is possible that the less affiliated subjects liked a

candidate who also appeared to be less affiliated, which could explain why they

liked the unpopular-inconsistent candidate so much. It is also possible that they

were less motivated to pay attention to the statements about the candidate.

Voting Likelihood. Voting likelihood was measured with a single 7-point

Likert scale asking how likely the subject was to vote for the candidate, ranging

from 1 – not at all likely, to 7 – very likely. As predicted, the subjects in the

consistent conditions were more likely to vote for the candidate t(1,74) = 3.14, p

< .01 (Means: consistent = 4.92, inconsistent = 3.61). As with evaluation, the

popularity main effect was not significant, t(1,74) = 0.23, p = .82, nor was the

consistency by popularity interaction, t(1,74) = 1.02, p = .31.

There was a significant three-way interaction of affiliation, consistency,

and popularity, t(1,74) = -2.51, p = .02 (Figure 10). Separate analyses testing the

effects at both high and low affiliation (+1 and -1 standard deviation) were

conducted. The consistency by popularity interaction was significant at low

affiliation, t(1,59) = 2.50, p = .02, but not at high affiliation, t(1,59) = -1.32, p =

.19. When the subjects were highly affiliated to their political party they showed

the predicted effect, though not significantly; consistency information mattered

less when the candidate was popular. When the subject was less affiliated the

pattern was reversed; consistency information mattered more when the

candidate was popular. High affiliated subjects were directionally less likely to

Page 78: (1) Information and Political Perception

65

vote for an unpopular-inconsistent candidate than an popular-consistent one,

while the opposite was true for low affiliated subjects.

Figure 10. Voting Likelihood. Subjects’ rated likelihood of voting for the

candidate by consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated

subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation).

Lo Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Voting Likelihood

Consistent

Inconsistent

Hi Affiliation

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Voting Likelihood

Consistent

Inconsistent

Directionally, but not significantly the highly affiliated subjects again

support the prediction that the subjects would use the popularity information

more when the candidate was inconsistent. As with evaluation it appears that

the highly affiliated subjects were not using the consistency information as much

when the candidate was popular, though they were when the candidate was

unpopular. The lower affiliated subjects were using the consistency information

in both popularity conditions (though in opposing ways). In the popular

conditions the low affiliated subjects were using the popularity information as

expected, being less likely to vote if the candidate was inconsistent. In the

unpopular condition voting likelihood reversed, with the subjects more likely to

Page 79: (1) Information and Political Perception

66

vote for the inconsistent candidate. This result was similar to what was found for

evaluation.

Perceived Similarity. Perceived similarity was measured using a single 7-

point Likert scale asking how similar the subject perceived the candidate to be to

themselves, ranging from 1 – not at all similar, to 7 – very similar. The predicted

consistency main effect was significant, t(1,74) = 3.86, p < .001 (Means:

consistent = 4.73, inconsistent = 3.21). Neither the predicted popularity main

effect, t(1,74) = 0.63, p = .65, nor the interaction of consistency and popularity,

t(1,74) = 0.57, p = .57 were significant. There was a marginal affiliation main

effect, t(1,74) = -1.94, p = .06, in which the more affiliated a subject was, the less

similar they perceived the candidate to be to themselves. The more politically

knowledgeable the subject was the more similarity they perceived between

themselves and the candidate, t(1,74) = 2.68, p < .01.

There was a significant affiliation by consistency interaction, t(1,74) = 2.45,

p = .02, with low affiliated subjects showing the consistency effect less than

highly affiliated subjects. Affiliation also interacted with popularity, t(1,74) = 2.35,

p = .02, with high affiliated subjects in the popular conditions seeing the

candidate as more similar, while the low affiliated subjects showed the opposite

effect. Finally, the affiliation by popularity interaction was moderated by

consistency, t(1,74) = -2.66, p < .01 (Figure 11). Separate analyses were run

testing the effects at both high and low affiliation (+1 and -1 standard deviation).

The consistency by popularity interaction was significant at low affiliation, t(1,59)

= 2.33, p = .02, but only marginally at high affiliation, t(1,59) = -1.74, p = .08. As

Page 80: (1) Information and Political Perception

67

with liking and voting likelihood the highly affiliated subjects directionally, but not

significantly supported the hypothesis that when the candidate was popular the

consistency information would matter less. However, less affiliated subjects

showed the obverse pattern, with greater perceived similarity to the unpopular-

inconsistent candidate than the popular-inconsistent one.

Figure 11. Similarity. Subjects’ rated similarity to the candidate by

consistency, popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1

and +1 Standard Deviation).

Lo Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Perceived Similarity to Candidate

Consistent

Inconsistent

Hi Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Perceived Similarity to Candidate

Consistent

Inconsistent

Perceived Strength of Target’s Affiliation. The strength of the target’s

affiliation was measured with a single 7-point Likert scale which asked how

strongly affiliated the subject thought the candidate was, ranging from 1 – very

weakly affiliated, to 7 – very strongly affiliated. The predicted consistency effect

was significant, t(1,74) = 2.95, p < .01, with subjects in the consistent conditions

rating the candidate as more strongly affiliated (Means: consistent = 5.30,

inconsistent = 4.39). Neither the predicted popularity main effect, t(1,74) = -0.04,

Page 81: (1) Information and Political Perception

68

p = .97, nor the interaction of consistency and popularity, t(1,74) = 1.32, p = .19

were significant. There was a marginal interaction of consistency and popularity

with the subject’s strength of affiliation, t(1,74) = -1.77, p = .08 (Figure 12).

Figure 12. Candidate’s Affiliation Strength. Subjects’ rating of the

candidate’s strength of affiliation to the Democratic Party by consistency,

popularity, for low (left) and high (right) affiliated subjects (-1 and +1

Standard Deviation).

Lo Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Perceived Strength of Candidate Affiliation

Consistent

Inconsistent

Hi Affiliation

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Perceived Strength of Candidate Affiliation

Consistent

Inconsistent

Separate analyses testing the effects at both high and low affiliation (+1

and -1 standard deviation) were run. The consistency by popularity interaction

was significant at low affiliation, t(1,59) = 2.14, p = .04, but not at high affiliation,

t(1,59) = -.53, p = .60. Specifically, both high and low affiliated subjects in the

consistent-popular condition perceived the candidate as more affiliated than

those in the popular-inconsistent condition. However, the highly affiliated

subjects in the inconsistent-popular condition perceived the candidate as

affiliated as those in the inconsistent-unpopular condition, while low affiliated

subjects saw the inconsistent-unpopular as more affiliated.

Page 82: (1) Information and Political Perception

69

The pattern of results for perceived affiliation strength was not as

predicted. Highly affiliated subjects were using the popularity information in both

the consistent and inconsistent condition, rather than in just the inconsistent

ones, though not significantly. As with the liking, voting, and similarity the

inconsistent-unpopular condition had the highest mean among the lower affiliated

subjects. The possibility that the low affiliated subjects identified more with low

affiliated candidates seems less likely for this dependent variable. Since the task

was to determine how affiliated the candidate was to his party, these results are

difficult to explain. However, the three-way interaction was only marginal in this

case.

Projection

The extent to which the subject’s own attitudes on the issues influenced

their perceptions of the candidate’s position on the issues was examined next. I

predicted that both consistency and popularity would influence projection;

subjects would project both when the candidate was popular and consistent. I

also predicted that these effects would be moderated by the subject’s strength of

affiliation, with more affiliated subjects showing a larger effect. Possible

moderation by the subject’s political knowledge was also examined

Analysis. The projection results were calculated using a multi-level

regression. The first level of the regression predicted the judges’ ratings of the

target candidates’ positions on the 24 attitude items, using the judges’ mean

centered self-ratings as the predictor. This first level regression was computed

Page 83: (1) Information and Political Perception

70

for each subject, which resulted in a beta for the intercept and self-rating

(projection).

The resulting slopes were then regressed onto consistency (coded -1

Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1 Popular),

subject’s self-rated strength of affiliation (mean-deviated), subject’s self-rated

political knowledge (mean-deviated), and all the interactions.

Results. The regression using the first level regression self-rating beta

examines projection. As expected there was significant overall projection to the

candidate, t(1,74) = 6.14, p < .0001. As predicted the subjects in the consistent

condition projected more than subjects in the inconsistent condition, t(1,74) =

2.66, p < .01. However, popularity, subject’s strength of affiliation, and their

political knowledge did not influence projection.

There were also a number of non-projection effects. Subjects in the

consistent condition rated the candidate as more liberal than those subjects in

the inconsistent conditions, t(1,74) = 5.55, p < .0001, as expected. The

popularity of the candidate did not influence how liberal he was rated, t(1,74) = -

1.14, p = .26. However, the subject’s strength of affiliation interacted significantly

with popularity, t(1,74) = 2.94, p < .01, with more strongly affiliated subjects in the

popular conditions rating the candidate as more liberal. This two-way interaction

was moderated by consistency, t(1,74) = 3.17, p = .03. Specifically, more

strongly affiliated subjects rated the popular candidate as even more liberal when

the candidate was also consistent.

Page 84: (1) Information and Political Perception

71

The more politically knowledgeable subjects rated the candidate as more

liberal, t(1,74) = 2.06, p = .04. The more strongly affiliated the subjects the

greater their political knowledge influenced their rating of how liberal they

perceived the candidate, t(1,74) = 2.52, p < .01.

As I predicted, the subjects projected the most towards the popular

candidate, regardless of consistency. I also predicted that the subjects would

project more towards the inconsistent-popular candidate than the inconsistent-

unpopular candidate. The subjects did project their own positions more onto the

ingroup candidate, even more so when the candidate was consistent. However,

the expected popularity effect was not significant. So it appears that the

consistency information increased the subject’s perceptions of attitude similarity

between themselves and the candidate, while popularity did not.

Process Variables

In addition to examining how consistency and popularity influence

impressions, I was also interested in factors which could contribute to these

effects. The following analyses examined how consistency, popularity, subject’s

strength of affiliation, and their political knowledge influenced the recall and

recognition of the candidate’s statements, the subject’s valuation of the issues,

and subtyping of the candidate.

I predicted that differential memory encoding would be responsible for the

differences due to consistency and popularity, rather than changes in valuation or

subtyping of the candidate (especially in the popular-inconsistent condition).

Since the prediction that popularity would moderate consistency only occurred for

Page 85: (1) Information and Political Perception

72

the highly affiliated subjects (directionally) I predicted similar effects for the

memory measure for highly affiliated subjects, but not for the valuation and

subtyping measures.

Recall. The number of correctly recalled statement items that the subjects

recorded on the blank sheet during the recall task was calculated. This value

was then regressed with contrast coded popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1

Popular), contrast coded consistency (-1 Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), the

subject’s self-reported mean-deviated political knowledge and strength of party

affiliation, and all the interactions as predictors. The average number of recalled

items was 4.84, with a negatively skewed distribution (Figure 13).

Figure 13. Recall. Frequency Distribution of number freely recalled items.

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Number of Items Freely Recalled

Number of Subjects

The main effect of popularity was marginal, t(1,74) = -1.89, p = .06, with

subjects recalling more information about the unpopular candidates (see Figure

14). Neither the predicted main effect of consistency, t(1,74) = 1.20, p = .24, nor

Page 86: (1) Information and Political Perception

73

the interaction of consistency and popularity t(1,74) = -0.68, p = .50 was

significant.

Figure 14. Recall. Number of freely recalled candidate statements by

popularity and consistency conditions.

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Popular Unpopular

Total number of Items Freely Recalled

Consistent

Inconsistent

So while the subjects used the consistency information more when

evaluating the candidate, determining voting likelihood, and perceived similarity

they did not recall significantly more about candidate when he was consistent.

However they did recall somewhat more about the candidate in the unpopular

condition than the popular ones.

Recognition. The recognition task consisted of the six attitude statements

(6 consistent in the consistent condition, or three consistent and three

inconsistent in the inconsistent condition) that the subjects had read about the

candidates and 18 filler items (nine stereotypically consistent with the Democratic

Party and nine inconsistent). For each of these items the subject answered

whether or not they had seen it before and on a 3-point scale how sure they were

Page 87: (1) Information and Political Perception

74

about their answer. From these two ratings I calculated a new scale ranging

from 1 to 6. The scores were:

1 = very confident not seen 2 = somewhat confident not seen 3 = not at all confident not seen 4 = not at all confident seen 5 = somewhat confident seen 6 = very confident seen

For all subjects the average value for both old and new items was calculated.

For the subjects in the inconsistent condition, the average for old-consistent, old-

inconsistent, new-consistent, new inconsistent items was calculated. For the

subjects in the consistent condition, the average for old-consistent and new-

consistent items was calculated. These measures were used in three separate

models. One model examined old versus new items for subjects in both the

consistent and inconsistent conditions. Another model examined old versus new

and consistent versus inconsistent items in the inconsistent conditions. A final

model examined old-consistent versus new-consistent items in the consistent

condition.

The first model examined the difference in recognition for the actual

statements about the candidates that the subjects read (old items) versus the

filler items (new items) across the consistency conditions. The scores for each

subject on the old and new items were submitted to a GLM, with contrast coded

popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1 Popular), contrast coded consistency (-1

Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated

political knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as

predictors.

Page 88: (1) Information and Political Perception

75

The expected item main effect was significant, F(1,74) = 304.91, p <

.0001, with greater recognition of the previously seen items. There was also a

marginal item by consistency interaction, F(1,74) = 2.99, p = .09. While subjects

in both the consistent and inconsistent conditions had greater recognition of the

previously seen items than the new ones, the subjects in the inconsistent

conditions showing greater recognition for the previously seen statements.

Subjects in the consistent conditions demonstrated greater familiarity for the

statements which had not been seen previously (see Figure 15). There was not

an item by popularity interaction, F(1,74) < 1, p = .98, nor a three-way interaction

of item with popularity and consistency, F(1,74) = 0.23, p = .63.

Figure 15. Recognition. Weighted average recognition of candidate

statements by consistency condition and whether the statements were

actually mentioned about the candidate (Old) or newly presented (New).

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

Consistent Inconsistent

Average W

eighted Recognition

Old Items

New Items

There were also a number of non-recognition effects (no interaction with

item). There was a significant interaction of the subject’s political knowledge with

Page 89: (1) Information and Political Perception

76

both popularity, F(1,74)= 3.9, p = .02, and consistency, F(1,74)= 3.8, p = .02, with

more knowledgeable subjects showing greater familiarity towards all the items

overall (both new and old items) in the popular and consistent conditions, but

their recognition of the old items was not better.

The second model examined differences in recognition for subjects in the

two inconsistent conditions. The scores for the old-consistent, old-inconsistent,

new-consistent, new inconsistent items were submitted to a GLM, with contrast

coded popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1 Popular), the subject’s self-reported

mean-deviated political knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the

interactions as predictors.

The only recognition effect was the expected main effect of item, F(1,35) =

136.75, p < .0001, with subjects showing greater recognition for the actual

candidate statements (see Figure 16). This result is consistent with Srull,

Lichtenstein, and Rothbart (1985) that when inconsistent items were present

recognition was improved for both inconsistent and consistent items.

There were also a number of effects that did not interact with item, which

indicate greater familiarity, but not recognition. There was a significant main

effect for knowledge, F(1,35) = 5.88, p = .03, with more knowledgeable subjects

reporting more familiarity to the items overall (both new and old items). The

effect of knowledge was moderated by popularity, F(1,35) = 6.32, p = .03, with

the effect of knowledge larger in the inconsistent-popular condition than the

inconsistent-unpopular. There was also a main effect for the consistency of the

items, F(1,35) = 8.56, p < .01, with greater familiarity, for stereotypically

Page 90: (1) Information and Political Perception

77

consistent items, regardless of whether they were new or old items. This effect

was not moderated by item, so there was no differentiation of the effect by

whether it had been previously seen (no recognition effect). The three-way

interaction of consistency of the item, the subject’s strength of affiliation, and

popularity condition was significant, F(1,35) = 5.1, p = .03, with familiarity, but not

recognition for the consistent items in the popular condition being larger for more

affiliated subjects.

Figure 16. Recognition in the Inconsistent Conditions. Weighted average

of recognized candidate statements by whether the candidate’s statement

was consistent or inconsistent on the issue and whether the statement

was actually mentioned about the candidate (Old) or newly presented

(New).

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

Consistent

Items

Inconsistent

Items

Average W

eighted Recognition

Old Items

New Items

The third model examined differences in recognition for subjects in the two

consistent conditions. The subject’s scores for old-consistent and new-

consistent items were submitted to a GLM, with contrast coded popularity (-1

Page 91: (1) Information and Political Perception

78

Unpopular and +1 Popular), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated political

knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as predictors.

The only significant result was the expected significant main effect of item,

F(1,28) = 29.84, p < .0001, with greater recognition for the actual candidate

statements (mean = 4.77) than the newly presented items (mean = 3.21) and no

other effects.

While the recall results do indicate that the subjects encoded the

information more when the candidate was unpopular, the recognition results

show that they used both popularity and consistency information. The first

recognition model demonstrated that overall the subjects encoded the

information about the candidate more when he was inconsistent. In the third

model, when the candidate was consistent, popularity did not influence

recognition. So it appears that when the candidate was inconsistent, the

subjects encoded more than when the candidate is consistent. Popularity did not

influence the amount of recognition, but did influence recall, with greater recall for

unpopular candidates.

Valuation. The difference in valuation for each issue between the first

rating and the second rating was calculated. For each subject, an average of the

differences for the six issues they learned the candidate’s position on and the six

they did not learn anything about was calculated. For the subjects in the two

inconsistent conditions the average differences on the three issues that they

learned consistent information about the candidate, as well as an average of the

differences on the three issues they learned inconsistent information about the

Page 92: (1) Information and Political Perception

79

candidate were calculated. Using these new values three separate models were

analyzed to examine how the subjects changed how they valued the issues

before learning about the candidate, and after.

The first model examined changes in valuation for all subjects on the

issues they learned information about, and those they did not. The scores for

each subject on the learned and unlearned issues were submitted to a GLM, with

popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1 Popular), contrast coded consistency (-1

Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated

political knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as

predictors.

For the first model there were no interactions of issue type with

consistency, popularity, or the three-way. Additionally, the four-way interaction of

issue type, subject’s affiliation strength, consistency, and popularity was also

non-significant. There was a significant three-way interaction of issue type

(learned versus unlearned), consistency, and subject’s political knowledge,

F(1,59) = 6.09, p = .02, with more knowledgeable subjects valuing the seen

issues more in the consistent conditions (see Figure 17).

The second model examined differences in valuation for subjects in the

two inconsistent conditions. The scores for the unlearned, learned-consistent,

and learned-inconsistent issues were submitted to a GLM, with popularity (-1

Unpopular and +1 Popular), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated political

knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as predictors.

Page 93: (1) Information and Political Perception

80

Again there was no main effect for issue type, nor did it interact with any of

the other factors. In fact, the only effect near to significance was a marginal main

effect of popularity, F(1,30) = 3.77, p = .06, in which overall subjects in the

popular-inconsistent condition (Mean: - .15) decreased their valuation across all

issues (both previously seen and unseen) more so than subjects in the popular-

consistent condition (Mean: -.09).

Figure 17. Valuation across Popularity Conditions. Average change in

valuation of the issues from Time 1 and Time 2 by consistency condition,

whether the issue had been mentioned in the candidate statement (seen)

or was not mentioned (unseen), and for low (left) and high (right)

knowledge subjects (-1 and +1 Standard Deviation).

.

Lo Knowledge

-0.5

-0.4

-0.3

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

Consistent Inconsistent

Average Change in Valuation between Time 1 and

Time 2

Seen Item

Unseen Item

Hi Knowledge

-0.5

-0.4

-0.3

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

Consistent Inconsistent

Average Change in Valuation between Time 1 and

Time 2

Seen Item

Unseen Item

The third model examined differences in valuations for subjects in the two

consistent conditions. The subject’s scores for learned and unlearned issues

were submitted to a GLM, with contrast coded popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1

Page 94: (1) Information and Political Perception

81

Popular), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated political knowledge and

strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as predictors.

There was only one significant effect, specifically a two-way interaction of

issue type and the subject’s political knowledge, F(1,29) = 5.18, p = .03, with

more knowledgeable subjects valuing the items mentioned in the candidate

statements more than those that that were not.

These patterns of results seem to indicate that politically knowledgeable

subjects changed their valuation based on which issues were mentioned by the

candidate. Specifically, when the candidate was consistent the more

knowledgeable subjects valued the issues the candidate supported more than

issues not mentioned. This could be because the candidate’s statements made

the subject’s own attitudes on the issues more salient, which made these issues

seem more important.

Subtyping. Subtyping of the target from his political party was measured

with a single 7-point Likert scale which asked the subjects how typical they

thought the candidate was of the Democratic Party, ranging from 1 – very typical,

to 7 – very atypical. A regression was performed on this measure with contrast

coded popularity (-1 Unpopular and +1 Popular), contrast coded consistency (-1

Inconsistent and +1 Consistent), the subject’s self-reported mean-deviated

political knowledge and strength of party affiliation, and all the interactions as

predictors.

As predicted there was not a significant main effect for consistency,

popularity, or the interaction. The three-way interaction of subject’s affiliation

Page 95: (1) Information and Political Perception

82

strength, consistency, and popularity was also not significant. There was a

significant two-way interaction of the subject’s political knowledge and popularity,

t(1,74) = 2.04, p = .05, with more knowledgeable subjects subtyping the

unpopular candidate more (see Figure 18).

Figure 18. Subtyping. Subjects’ rated typicality of the candidate for the

Democratic Party by popularity, and affiliation (-1 and +1 Standard

Deviation).

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Lo Knowledge Hi Knowledge

Perceived Typicality of Candidate

Popular

Unpopular

Discussion

In Study Two I expected that the candidate’s popularity would moderate

the consistency of the candidate, overwhelming the consistency information

when the candidate was popular; only mattering when the candidate was

unpopular. I expected that when other members of their political party had

already “vouched” for the candidate, in the form of overwhelming support, the

Page 96: (1) Information and Political Perception

83

subjects would pay less attention to the other information presented about him.

Directionally, the highly motivated subjects performed as expected.

The subjects did pay close attention to the consistency information. The

memory measures showed that our subjects had marginally better recognition of

the candidate’s statements when he was inconsistent. When the subjects

learned both consistent and inconsistent statements about the candidate they

had greater familiarity of consistent items, but not better recognition for

previously seen items. There was a marginal moderating effect of popularity on

recall, with greater recall for statements about the unpopular candidate.

It appears that to some extent the subjects did rely on the endorsement of

his political party members for the popular candidate. In fact, when freely

recalling information about the candidate the subjects recalled marginally more of

the statements when the candidate was popular.

The subject’s strength of affiliation to their political party had a large

impact on how the subject perceived the political candidate. Highly affiliated

subjects showed preference for the consistent candidate. The highly affiliated

subjects were more likely to vote for the consistent candidate, saw him as more

similar, and liked him more, regardless of popularity. Directionally but not

significantly, when the candidate was inconsistent, highly affiliated subjects used

the candidate’s popularity information to a greater extent; liking the unpopular-

inconsistent candidate the least, were least likely to vote for him, and saw him as

the least similar. The less affiliated subjects showed a preference for the

outsider, they liked, wanted to vote for, and felt similar to the unpopular-

Page 97: (1) Information and Political Perception

84

inconsistent candidate. However, the less affiliated subjects did not show the

same preference for the popular-inconsistent candidate.

Regardless of the subject’s strength of affiliation they all projected their

own attitudes more onto the candidate when the candidate was consistent. The

popularity of the candidate did not influence the degree to which the subjects

projected.

The failure of the predicted pattern of findings to attain significance for the

highly affiliated subjects could be due to the nature of the task. The subjects

were only learning about a single candidate, so there was no comparison

candidate. It could be that if popularity and consistency had been within subject

rather than between subjects the addition of more candidates could have led to

greater discrimination on the evaluation, voting likelihood, and similarity

measures resulting in the predicted pattern attaining significance. However, the

results did go in the predicted direction for highly affiliated subjects and bears

further investigation.

It is also possible that the “highly affiliated” subjects were not affiliated

enough. The sample was drawn from the Introductory Psychology Subject Pool,

mostly comprised of first and second year college students. Research on

attitude strength has demonstrated that young adults, of which the sample was

drawn from, have less consistent attitudes, which are more easily changed than

later in life (Krosnick & Alwin, 1989; Visser & Krosnick, 1998). So it is possible

that with the sample drawn from a more politically established and active

Page 98: (1) Information and Political Perception

85

population the directional interaction of affiliation strength, popularity, and

consistency would attain significance.

The previous two studies have examined impression formation of political

candidates in the laboratory. The final study was designed to investigate political

perception using a representative voting population.

Page 99: (1) Information and Political Perception

86

Chapter 4:

Study Three

The purpose of the third study was to examine the influence of information

and political party membership on perceptions about political candidates and

parties in a real-world, representative sample. The previous two studies

examined the effect of different types of information on political impressions in

the laboratory. The first study examined how the diagnosticity and relevance of

information influences projection, consensus, and polarization. The second

study explored how the consistency and popularity of a candidate influences

projection and a number of other impressions. This final study examined similar

questions using the American National Election Study (ANES) data from 1980

and 1988, a nationally representative sample.

The specific question examined in this study was how voters’ political

party membership and ideological and issue-specific positions influenced

projection2 of their position to the candidates, and polarization of the parties.3

The influence of the voter’s level of political knowledge on perception of

polarization between the Democratic Party and Republican Party was also

examined.

2 In survey data the correlation between the respondent’s own position and their rating of the

candidate is not always due to projection (Krosnick, 2002). However, the model of the 1980 data uses an aggregate measure of political attitudes for the self and an aggregate measure across multiple candidates, which should decrease the likelihood that the correlation is due to persuasion effects. The 1988 data measures the correlation between self and other using a single measure of political ideology for the self rating, and an aggregate measure of ideology across target candidates for other. Ideology should be less changeable than individual attitudes. Thus, for the purpose of the current examination the term projection will be used when discussing the self-other correlation. 3 A model of within-party variability was also run on both datasets. However, no significant paths

were found, and a repeated measure ANOVA did not find significant outgroup homogeneity effects. Therefore these models were not included in this paper.

Page 100: (1) Information and Political Perception

87

I expected that the processes demonstrated in the laboratory with the

undergraduate population would also occur with a “real-world” sample of voters.

Specifically, I expected that voters would project their own positions more

towards their own political party. I also predicted that the more ideologically

extreme and politically knowledgeable the respondent, the more polarized they

would see the parties.

Method

For this study two datasets from the ANES database were used. A data

set with ratings of multiple candidates was needed to examine ingroup-outgroup

projection and polarization. The two datasets that fit this requirement best were

the 1980 two-wave Presidential election survey, and the 1988 Super Tuesday

survey. Each of these surveys will be examined in turn.

1980 ANES

Survey. The 1980 ANES was a two-wave panel study, which collected

data both before and after the 1980 Presidential election. Pre-election interviews

were conducted between September 7, 1980 and November 3, 1980, with post-

election interviews conducted between November 5, 1980 and February 7, 1981.

The primary sampling unit was the congressional district, with 108 districts

selected in a multi-stage probability sample. The sample included 2,249 eligible

persons, among whom 1,614 completed pre-election interviews and 1,408 were

subsequently re-interviewed after the election. The SEM analysis used data only

from registered Democrats (n = 558) and Republicans (n = 298) to examine

ingroup-outgroup differences in perceptions, with 758 omitted due to being

Page 101: (1) Information and Political Perception

88

unregistered (n = 148), members of a third party (n = 137), or missing party

information (n = 473). Data were collected on electoral participation, voting

behavior, public opinion, voter attitudes on numerous issues, and impressions of

candidates’ attitudes on the same issues.

The primary reason for using this particular ANES study is the inclusion of

questions about multiple targets from each political party on multiple political

issues, which allowed us to examine differential projection towards in-group

versus out-group candidates.

Targets. As mentioned above the 1980 ANES study included questions

about multiple targets from both major political parties on multiple political issues.

Specifically, there were questions about 3 Democratic targets (Jimmy Carter, Ted

Kennedy, and the Democratic Party in general) and two Republican targets

(Ronald Reagan and the Republican Party in general).

Measures. Four political issues were used in the analyses. The voter’s

own position on the issues as well as where they saw the targets on the issues

were used to examine projection and polarization. The issues were guaranteed

job security, aid for minorities, tax increases, and governmental services (see

Appendix O). Prior to being analyzed the measures were recoded so they scaled

liberal to conservative.

In addition to the issue measures voters also answered five questions

about their level of political knowledge (see Appendix P), which were used to

form the latent variable Self-Knowledge. These measures asked the voters about

their interest in the campaign, the amount of news they watch, how much

Page 102: (1) Information and Political Perception

89

attention they pay to politics, amount of attention paid to the national polls, and

an observation of the voter’s level of knowledge by the interviewer. Finally, the

voters answered whether they were registered to vote, and if so with which party.

This measure was then used as the between group measure in the SEM

analyses, with voters not registered to vote with one of the two major parties

being excluded from the analyses.

1988 Super Tuesday ANES

Survey. The 1988 Super Tuesday ANES survey was a two-wave study,

which collected data both before and after Super Tuesday, on which sixteen

states held a presidential primary. The first wave of data was collected during

the seven weeks prior to Super Tuesday, with a brief follow-up was collected

immediately after (2/3 collected in the first week). The primary sampling unit was

the congressional district, with 108 districts selected in a multi-probability sample.

A total of 2076 interviews were distributed over the seven weeks. The second

wave reinterviewed 1688 of the voters.

The SEM analysis used data only from registered Democrats (n = 812)

and Republicans (n = 452) to examine ingroup-outgroup differences in

perceptions, with 812 omitted due to being unregistered (n = 87), members of a

third party (n = 271), or missing party information (n = 454). Measures included

feeling thermometers and traits, assessment of each candidate's chances of

winning his/her party's nomination and the November general election, attitudes

on public issues, vote intention and choice, and respondent's age, race,

education, occupation, labor union membership, income, and religious affiliation.

Page 103: (1) Information and Political Perception

90

More importantly for the current analysis measures were collected about the

voter’s own political ideology (liberal to conservative) as well as their impressions

of seven politicians from each party on the same ideology measure. Additionally,

voters were asked a number of questions about their own level of political

knowledge.

The primary reason for using this particular ANES study is the inclusion of

the ideology measure about multiple targets from each political party, which

allowed examination of differential projection towards in-group versus out-group

candidates.

Targets. As mentioned above the 1988 Super Tuesday ANES study

included questions about multiple candidates from both major political parties.

Specifically, there were questions about seven Democratic candidates (Albert

Gore, Michael Dukakis, Gary Hart, Bruce Babbitt, Jesse Jackson, Richard

Gephardt, and Paul Simon) and seven Republicans (Ronald Reagan, George

Bush Sr., Alexander Haig, Pat Robertson, Robert Dole, Jack Kemp, and Pierre

Dupont).

Measures. The voter’s own position on the ideology measure as well as

where they saw the targets on the same ideology measure were used to examine

projection and polarization. The ideology measure was a 7-point Likert scale

ranging from 1 –Very Liberal to 7 – Very Conservative. In addition to the

ideology measure voters also answered four questions about their level of

political knowledge (see Appendix Q), which were used to form the latent

variable Self-Knowledge. These measures asked the voters about their interest

Page 104: (1) Information and Political Perception

91

in the Presidential campaign, the amount of news they watch, how much

attention they pay to politics in newspapers, and amount of interest in politics.

Finally, the voters answered whether they were registered to vote, and if so with

which party. This measure was then used as the between group measure in the

SEM analyses, with voters not registered to vote with one of the two major

parties being excluded from the analyses.

Results

1980 ANES Dataset

The first model used data from the 1980 ANES in which respondents were

asked for their own positions on seven different issues (see Appendix O) as well

as the positions that the perceived Democrats and Republicans to have on these

issues (Democrats in general plus two Democratic candidates, Republicans in

general plus one Republican candidate). I started by looking at the degree to

which these seven issues represented a single underlying factor, examining

through exploratory factor analysis their loadings on the first principle component

(using self ratings only). From this analysis it appeared that the following four

variables all shared considerable common variance and hence these four were

used in our model as indicators of a common underlying factor.

The model, as depicted in Figures 19a and 19b, has four latent variables,

two exogenous and two endogenous. The exogenous ones are own ideology

(with four indicators of own position on each of the four issues) and own political

knowledge, measuring this with four indicators (Appendix P). The latent

Page 105: (1) Information and Political Perception

Fig

ure

19

a. 1

980

AN

ES

Str

uctu

ral E

qu

atio

n M

od

el 1

(D

em

ocra

t V

ote

rs):

M

od

el of

Dem

ocra

tic P

art

y v

ote

rs’ se

lf-

ratin

gs o

n fo

ur

po

litic

al is

su

es a

nd

th

eir s

elf-r

ate

d p

olit

ica

l kn

ow

led

ge

eff

ect o

n p

roje

ction

an

d p

ola

riza

tio

n o

f th

e

De

mo

cra

tic a

nd

Re

pu

blic

an

Pa

rtie

s.

Se

lf

Ide

olo

gy

Se

rvS

elf

e1

.69

Aid

Se

lfe

3.4

8

Jo

bS

elf

e4

.67

De

mo

cra

t

Ide

olo

gy

Av

gJ

ob

De

me

5

Av

gA

idD

em

e6

Av

gS

erv

De

me

8

.68

.63

.45

Re

pu

blic

an

Ide

olo

gy

Av

gJ

ob

Re

pe

9

Av

gA

idR

ep

e1

0

Av

gS

erv

Re

pe

12

.72

.72

Se

lf

Kn

ow

led

ge

Info

e1

4

Ob

sIn

tere

st

e1

5

Po

lls

e1

6

.87

.75

.41

Se

lfIn

tere

st

e1

7

.39

e1

8

e1

9

-.0

1

-.2

1

-.1

7

.31

-.2

4

.41

.27

.35

.10

.14

.17

.31

.49

.25

.01

.08

Un

em

pS

elf

e2

0

.50

Av

gU

ne

mp

De

me

21

.48

Av

gU

ne

mp

Re

pe

22

.46

.08

.33

.00

92

Page 106: (1) Information and Political Perception

Fig

ure

19

b. 1

98

0 A

NE

S S

tru

ctu

ral E

qu

atio

n M

od

el 1

(R

ep

ub

lican

Vo

ters

):

Mo

de

l of

Re

pub

lica

n P

art

y v

ote

rs’ se

lf-

ratin

gs o

n fo

ur

po

litic

al is

su

es a

nd

th

eir s

elf-r

ate

d p

olit

ica

l kn

ow

led

ge

eff

ect o

n p

roje

ction

an

d p

ola

riza

tio

n o

f th

e

De

mo

cra

tic a

nd

Re

pu

blic

an

Pa

rtie

s. S

elf

Ide

olo

gy

Serv

Self

e1

.53

Aid

Self

e3

.48

JobS

elf

e4

.59

De

mo

cra

t

Ide

olo

gy

Avg

JobD

em

e5

Avg

Aid

Dem

e6

Avg

Serv

Dem

e8

.65

.68

.43

Re

pu

blic

an

Ide

olo

gy

Avg

JobR

ep

e9

Avg

Aid

Rep

e1

0

Avg

Serv

Rep

e1

2

.63

.67

Se

lf

Kn

ow

led

ge

Info

e1

4

ObsIn

tere

st

e1

5

Polls

e1

6

.88

.73

.36

SelfIn

tere

st

e1

7

.41

e1

8

e1

9

-.3

4

-.4

1

.71

.02

-.1

6

.05

.05

-.3

3

.38

.27

.36

.48

.41

-.0

4

-.0

3

-.2

5

Unem

pS

elf

e2

0

.40

Avg

Unem

pD

em

e2

1

.44

Avg

Unem

pR

ep

e2

2

.49

.47

-.3

7

-.3

0

93

Page 107: (1) Information and Political Perception

94

endogenous variable were perception of the ideology of the Democrats and

perception of the ideology of the Republicans, using each issue as separate

indicators of these (and averaging within each issue across the ratings of the

party and the candidates).

The structural part of the model estimates the partial effects of the two

latent exogenous variables (own ideology and subjective knowledge) on the two

latent endogenous variables (perceived Democratic ideology and perceived

Republican ideology), allowing the disturbances to these two endogenous latent

variables to be correlated. Additionally, to account for issue specific variance in

the judgments the disturbances to the indicators involving the same issue were

allowed to correlate (e.g., disturbances to self-unemployment, Democrat-

unemployment, and Republican unemployment). Finally, a multiple group

analysis was conducted, estimating the model simultaneously for respondents

who identified themselves as Democrats and those who self-identified as

Republicans.

I first estimated this model on both groups with no between group

constraints on the parameter estimates. Although the resulting goodness of fit

Chi square was highly significant, χ172

2 = 388.71 , other measures of goodness-of-

fit that are less influenced by the large sample size used in these analyses

suggest that the model is reasonably consistent with the data, CFI = .896,

RMSEA =.038. Given this, our next step was to impose measurement invariance

between the two groups, i.e., forcing all of the loadings of the indicators on the

latent variables for the two groups to be equal between groups. This condition of

Page 108: (1) Information and Political Perception

95

measurement invariance is a necessary one in order to compare structural

coefficients between the two groups. The fit indices of the resulting model were

very similar to the model that did not impose measurement invariance,

χ184

2 = 406.29 , CFI = .893, RMSEA = .038. A comparison of the Chi-squares for

these two models indicates that the measurement invariance model does not fit

the data significantly less well than the unconstrained model∆χ12

2 = 17.58, p > .25 .

In Table 1 are the loading coefficients, the standardized loadings, the

structural coefficients (with standardized values) and in Table 2 all standardized

residual covariances from this model with imposed measurement invariances. As

the standardized loadings indicate, all indicators loaded significantly and highly

on their respective latent variables. The structural coefficients from own latent

ideology reveal significant projection from the self to ones’ own party among the

Republicans (Self-Ideology to Republican-Ideology, standardized value .706, p <

.001) but not among the Democrats (Self-Ideology to Democrat-Ideology

standardized value -.008, ns).

In the case of judgments of the other party, there was significant reverse

projection (i.e., contrast) in the case of both Republicans (Self-Ideology to

Democrat-Ideology, standardized value -.336, p < .001) and Democrats (Self-

Ideology to Democrat-Ideology standardized value -.169, p = .02).

In addition to overall ideological projection the covariances between the

individual attitude items were examined (see Table 2). For the Democrat

respondents there was shared residual measurement error between the self and

the ingroup on all the issues; job security (covariance .509, p < .001), minority aid

Page 109: (1) Information and Political Perception

Tab

le 1

. 19

80 A

NE

S: Lo

ad

ing a

nd

str

uctu

ral co

eff

icie

nts

: U

nsta

nd

ard

ize

d a

nd

sta

nda

rdiz

ed

estim

ate

s fo

r D

em

ocra

t (le

ft)

an

d R

ep

ub

lica

n (

Rig

ht)

re

sp

on

den

ts. *

= p <

.0

5, **

= p <

.0

1, **

* = p <

.00

01,

an

d n

s = p >

.7

0.

D

em

ocra

t R

espondent

R

epublic

an

Respondent

U

nsta

ndard

ized

Estim

ate

S

tandard

ized

Estim

ate

P

U

nsta

ndard

ized

Estim

ate

S

tandard

ized

Estim

ate

P

Dem

Ideolo

gy

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

-0.0

08

-0.0

13

ns

-0.3

05

-0.3

36

**

Dem

Ideolo

gy

<--

S

elf_K

now

ledge

-0.2

02

-0.2

11

**

-0.4

51

-0.4

06

***

Rep I

deolo

gy

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

-0.1

4

-0.1

69

* 0.6

28

0.7

06

***

Rep I

deolo

gy

<--

S

elf_K

now

ledge

0.3

73

0.3

12

***

0.0

27

0.0

25

ns

Serv

Self

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

1

0.6

88

1

0.5

33

Aid

Self

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

0.6

69

0.4

85

***

0.6

69

0.4

81

***

JobS

elf

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

1.0

15

0.6

68

***

1.0

15

0.5

89

***

Unem

pS

elf

<--

S

elf_Id

eolo

gy

0.6

15

0.5

04

***

0.6

15

0.4

44

***

AvgS

erv

Dem

<

--

Dem

Ideolo

gy

0.6

2

0.4

53

***

0.6

2

0.4

27

***

AvgA

idD

em

<

--

Dem

Ideolo

gy

0.9

21

0.6

27

***

0.9

21

0.6

8

***

AvgJobD

em

<

--

Dem

Ideolo

gy

1

0.6

76

1

0.6

47

AvgU

nem

pD

em

<

--

Dem

Ideolo

gy

0.7

5

0.4

79

***

0.7

5

0.4

44

***

AvgS

erv

Rep

<--

R

ep I

deolo

gy

0.6

78

0.4

89

***

0.6

78

0.4

12

***

AvgA

idR

ep

<--

R

ep I

deolo

gy

0.8

77

0.7

17

***

0.8

77

0.6

69

***

AvgJobR

ep

<--

R

ep I

deolo

gy

1

0.7

2

1

0.6

32

AvgU

nem

pR

ep

<--

R

ep I

deolo

gy

0.6

47

0.4

57

***

0.7

5

0.4

89

***

Info

<

--

Self_K

now

ledge

1

0.8

67

1

0.8

76

ObsIn

tere

st

<--

S

elf_K

now

ledge

0.8

4

0.7

5

***

0.8

4

0.7

35

***

Polls

<

--

Self_K

now

ledge

0.7

16

0.4

11

***

0.7

16

0.3

59

***

SelfIn

tere

st

<--

S

elf_K

now

ledge

0.6

79

0.3

95

***

0.6

79

0.4

09

***

96

Page 110: (1) Information and Political Perception

97

the issues; job security (covariance .614, p < .001), minority aid (covariance .239,

p < .01), government services (covariance .673, p < .001), and unemployment

(covariance .659, p < .001). There was also negatively correlated shared error

variance for Republican respondents towards the outgroup (Democrats) on

government services (covariance -518, p < .001) and unemployment (covariance

-.605, p < .001). For Democrat respondents there was no negatively correlated

shared error variance, however Democrats did have positively correlated shared

error variance towards the outgroup (Republicans) on minority aid (covariance

.176, p < .05) and job security (covariance .432, p < .001).

Table 2. 1980 ANES: Standardized Residual Covariances for Democrat

(Left) and Republican (Right) respondents. *** = p < .0001.

Democrat Respondents Republican Respondents

Covariance P Covariance P

Self-Knowledge <--> Self_Ideology 0.179 0.006 0.248 ***

Dem Ideology <--> Rep Ideology -0.186 0.003 -0.061 0.279

ServSelf <--> AvgServDem 0.449 *** -0.518 ***

AidSelf <--> AvgAidDem 0.377 *** 0.046 0.561

JobSelf <--> AvgJobDem 0.509 *** 0.068 0.546

UnempSelf <--> AvgUnempDem 0.481 *** -0.605 ***

ServSelf <--> AvgServRep 0.165 0.111 0.673 ***

AidSelf <--> AvgAidRep 0.176 0.038 0.239 0.003

JobSelf <--> AvgJobRep 0.432 *** 0.614 ***

UnempSelf <--> AvgUnempRep 0.14 0.2 0.659 ***

AvgServDem <--> AvgServRep 0.092 0.186 -0.338 ***

AvgAidDem <--> AvgAidRep 0.008 0.886 -0.019 0.766

AvgJobDem <--> AvgJobRep 0.215 0.002 -0.035 0.698

AvgEmpDem <--> AvgUnempRep 0.004 0.97 -0.406 0.002

Turning to the impact of subjective self-knowledge, for both groups, the

more knowledgeable the respondent the more they saw Democrats as having

liberal ideologies (For Republicans, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-Ideology,

Page 111: (1) Information and Political Perception

98

standardized value -.406, p < .001; For Democrats, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-

Ideology, standardized value -.211, p = .002). On the other hand among

Democrats the more knowledgeable the more Republicans were seen to be

conservative in their ideology (Self-Knowledge to Republican-Ideology,

standardized value .312, p < .001), but for Republicans, self-knowledge had no

impact on the perceived ideology of Republicans (Self-Knowledge to Republican-

Ideology, standardized value .025, ns). Thus it appears that more knowledge on

the part of respondents is associated with seeing the outgroup party as more

extreme in its ideological beliefs.

In sum, in perceiving the outgroup, there is significant reverse projection:

As a Democratic respondent is more liberal, Republicans are seen as more

conservative; As a Republican respondent is more conservative, Democrats are

seen as more liberal. Positive Projection to the ingroup only occurred among

Republicans: As a Republican respondent is more conservative; Republicans are

seen as more conservative. Looking at subjective self-knowledge, higher values

among both Republicans and Democrats is associated with the tendency to see

the outgroup party as more polarized (Democrats as more liberal and

Republicans as more conservative).

1988 Super Tuesday ANES Dataset

The second model used data from the 1988 Super Tuesday ANES in

which respondents were asked for their own positions on political ideology, from

very liberal to very conservative (see Appendix R) as well

Page 112: (1) Information and Political Perception

Fig

ure

20

a. 1

98

8 S

upe

r T

ue

sda

y A

NE

S S

tructu

ral E

qu

ation

Mod

el 1

(D

em

ocra

t V

ote

rs):

M

od

el of

De

mo

cra

tic

Pa

rty v

ote

rs’ se

lf-r

atin

gs o

n p

olit

ica

l id

eo

logy a

nd

th

eir s

elf-r

ate

d p

olit

ica

l kn

ow

led

ge

eff

ect o

n p

roje

ctio

n a

nd

p

ola

riza

tio

n o

f th

e D

em

ocra

tic a

nd R

epu

blic

an

Pa

rtie

s (

***

= p <

.00

01

).

Self R

ate

d

Ideolo

gy

1.0

0

Se

lfe

11

.00

Self-K

now

ledg

e.5

8

Inte

restP

ap

er

e4

.76

.19

Inte

restP

res

e5

.44

.36

Inte

restG

ov

e6

.60

.20

Mean D

em

.32

Go

ree

8

.57

.46

Du

kakis

e9

.68

.22

Hart

e1

0.4

6.3

3

Bab

bitt

e1

1.5

7

.25

Jackso

ne

12

.50

.39

Ge

ph

ard

te

13

.62

.40

Sim

on

e1

4

.63

.06

Mean R

ep

.62

Bu

sh

e1

5

.79

.49

Haig

e1

6

.70

.11

Ro

be

rtso

ne

17

.33

.27

Do

lee

18

.52

.27

Ke

mp

e1

9

.52

.32

Du

po

nt

e2

0

.56

.64

Re

ag

an

e2

1

.80

-.1

3

e2

2

e2

3

.17

-.2

3.35 -.

15

.56

Inte

restT

Ve

24

.75

-.1

0

***

-.2

5 *

**

***

*n

s

***

99

Page 113: (1) Information and Political Perception

Fig

ure

20

b. 1

98

8 S

upe

r T

ue

sda

y A

NE

S S

tructu

ral E

qu

ation

Mod

el 1

(R

ep

ub

lica

n V

ote

rs):

M

od

el of

Re

pu

blic

an

Pa

rty v

ote

rs’ se

lf-r

atin

gs o

n p

olit

ica

l id

eo

logy a

nd

th

eir s

elf-r

ate

d p

olit

ica

l kn

ow

led

ge

eff

ect o

n p

roje

ctio

n a

nd

p

ola

riza

tio

n o

f th

e D

em

ocra

tic a

nd R

epu

blic

an

Pa

rtie

s (

***

= p <

.00

01

).

Self R

ate

d

Ideolo

gy

1.0

0

Se

lfe

11

.00

Self-K

now

ledg

e.5

4

Inte

restP

ap

er

e4

.73

.17

Inte

restP

res

e5

.41

.36

Inte

restG

ov

e6

.60

.13

Mean D

em

.32

Go

ree

8

.57

.43

Du

kakis

e9

.66

.31

Hart

e1

0.5

6.3

6

Bab

bitt

e1

1.6

0

.35

Jackso

ne

12

.59

.47

Ge

ph

ard

te

13

.69

.45

Sim

on

e1

4

.67

.11

Mean R

ep

.51

Bu

sh

e1

5

.71

.32

Haig

e1

6

.56

.07

Ro

be

rtso

ne

17

.26

.19

Do

lee

18

.43

.18

Ke

mp

e1

9

.42

.25

Du

po

nt

e2

0

.50

.67

Re

ag

an

e2

1

.82

.25

e2

2

e2

3

.19

-.2

5-.2

0 .23

.53

Inte

restT

Ve

24

.73

-.2

8

-.2

2 *

**

***

*

*** ***

**

100

Page 114: (1) Information and Political Perception

101

as the positions that they perceived Democrats and Republicans to have on

ideology (seven Democrats and seven Republicans).

The model, as depicted in figures 20a and 20b, has four latent variables,

two exogenous and two endogenous. The exogenous ones are own ideology

(with the single indicator and no variance on its error term) and own political

knowledge, measuring this with four indicators (Appendix Q). The latent

endogenous variables were perception of the ideology of the Democrats and

perception of the ideology of the Republicans, using the perceived ideology of

each candidate as separate indicators.

The structural part of the model estimates the partial effects of the two

latent exogenous variables (own ideology and subjective knowledge) on the two

latent endogenous variables (perceived Democratic ideology and perceived

Republican ideology), allowing the disturbances to these two endogenous latent

variables to be correlated. A multiple group analysis was conducted, estimating

the model simultaneously for respondents who identified themselves as

Democrats and those who self-identified as Republicans.

I first estimated this model on both groups with no between group

constraints on the parameter estimates. Although the resulting goodness of fit

Chi square was highly significant, 26.6262

294 =χ , other measures of goodness-of-

fit that are less influenced by the large sample size used in these analyses

suggest that the model is reasonably consistent with the data, CFI = .864,

RMSEA =.030. Given this, our next step was to impose measurement invariance

between the two groups, i.e., forcing all of the loadings of the indicators on the

Page 115: (1) Information and Political Perception

102

latent variables for the two groups to be equal between groups. This condition of

measurement invariance is a necessary one in order to compare structural

coefficients between the two groups. The fit indices of the resulting model were

very similar to the model that did not impose measurement invariance,

15.6522

309 =χ , CFI = .860, RMSEA = .030. A comparison of the Chi-squares for

these two models indicates that the measurement invariance model does not fit

the data significantly less well than the unconstrained, model 89.252

80 =∆χ , p >

.50.

Our final step was to constrain the ingroup and outgroup projection paths

to be equal to each other. Thus, the Self-Ideology to Democrat Ideology path for

Democrat subjects was constrained to be equal to the Republican subjects’ Self-

Ideology to Republican Ideology path (ingroup projection) and the Democrat

subjects’ subjects’ Self-Ideology to Republican Ideology path was constrained to

be equal to the Republican subjects’ Self-Ideology to Democrat Ideology path

(outgroup projection). Additionally, the two paths from the Self-Knowledge latent

variable to Democrat Ideology and Republican Ideology were constrained to be

equal for both Democrat and Republican subject. The fit indices of the resulting

model were very similar to the measurement invariance model, 19.6552

313 =χ , CFI

= .860, RMSEA = .030. A comparison of the Chi-squares for these two models

indicates that the measurement invariance model does not fit the data

significantly less well than the unconstrained model, 04.32

4 =∆χ , p > .50.

In Table 3 are the loading coefficients, the standardized loadings, the

structural coefficients (with standardized values) and in Table 4 all standardized

Page 116: (1) Information and Political Perception

Tab

le 3

. 19

88 A

NE

S: Lo

ad

ing a

nd

str

uctu

ral co

eff

icie

nts

: U

nsta

nd

ard

ize

d a

nd

sta

nda

rdiz

ed

estim

ate

s fo

r D

em

ocra

t (le

ft)

an

d R

ep

ub

lica

n (

Rig

ht)

re

sp

on

den

ts. *

** = p <

.0

00

1.

D

em

ocra

t R

espon

den

ts

Repu

blic

an

Respon

den

ts

U

nsta

ndard

ized

Estim

ate

S

tandard

ized

Estim

ate

P

U

nsta

ndard

ized

Estim

ate

S

tandard

ized

Estim

ate

P

Mean R

ep

<--

S

elf-K

now

ledge

0.2

23

0.1

59

***

0.2

23

0.1

99

***

Mean D

em

<

--

Self-K

now

ledge

-0.2

1

-0.2

51

***

-0.2

1

-0.2

23

***

Mean D

em

<

--

Self-R

ating

0.1

48

0.3

49

***

-0.1

1

-0.2

02

***

Mean R

ep

<--

S

elf-R

ating

-0.1

1

-0.1

55

***

0.1

48

0.2

27

***

Lib

Con1

<--

S

elf-R

ating

1

1

1

1

Inte

restT

V

<--

S

elf-K

now

ledge

1

0.7

47

1

0.7

29

Inte

restP

aper

<--

S

elf-K

now

ledge

1.0

16

0.7

65

***

1.0

16

0.7

35

***

Inte

restP

res

<--

S

elf-K

now

ledge

0.5

43

0.4

41

***

0.5

43

0.4

14

***

Inte

restG

ov

<--

S

elf-K

now

ledge

0.6

99

0.6

03

***

0.6

99

0.6

**

*

LC

Gore

1

<--

M

ean D

em

1

0.5

67

1

0.5

64

LC

Duk1

<--

M

ean D

em

1.3

49

0.6

79

***

1.3

49

0.6

56

***

LC

Hart

1

<--

M

ean D

em

1.0

65

0.4

66

***

1.0

65

0.5

53

***

LC

Bab1

<--

M

ean D

em

0.9

86

0.5

76

***

0.9

86

0.5

99

***

LC

Jack1

<--

M

ean D

em

1.3

15

0.5

03

***

1.3

15

0.5

87

***

LC

Gep1

<--

M

ean D

em

1.1

57

0.6

26

***

1.1

57

0.6

85

***

LC

Sim

on1

<--

M

ean D

em

1.3

76

0.6

35

***

1.3

76

0.6

7

***

LC

Bush1

<--

M

ean R

ep

1

0.7

89

1

0.7

13

LC

Haig

1

<--

M

ean R

ep

1.0

81

0.7

02

***

1.0

81

0.5

65

***

LC

Rob1

<--

M

ean R

ep

0.4

95

0.3

34

***

0.4

95

0.2

65

***

LC

Dole

1

<--

M

ean R

ep

0.5

96

0.5

16

***

0.5

96

0.4

35

***

LC

Kem

p1

<--

M

ean R

ep

0.6

79

0.5

14

***

0.6

79

0.4

25

***

LC

Dupont1

<

--

Mean R

ep

0.8

13

0.5

61

***

0.8

13

0.5

**

*

LC

Reag1

<--

M

ean R

ep

1.2

33

0.7

99

***

1.2

33

0.8

16

***

103

Page 117: (1) Information and Political Perception

104

residual covariances from this model with imposed measurement invariances. As

the standardized loadings indicate, all indicators loaded significantly and highly

on their respective latent variables. The structural coefficients from own latent

ideology reveals significant projection from the self to ones’ own party among the

Republicans (Self-Ideology to Republican-Ideology, standardized value .227, p <

.001) and among the Democrats (Self-Ideology to Democrat-Ideology

standardized value .349, p < .001).

In the case of judgments of the other party, there is significant reverse

projection (i.e., contrast) in the case of both Republicans (Self-Ideology to

Democrat-Ideology, standardized value -.202, p < .001) and Democrats (Self-

Ideology to Democrat-Ideology standardized value -.155, p = .001).

Table 4. 1988 ANES: Standardized Residual Covariances for Democrat (Left) and Republican (Right) respondents. *** = p < .0001.

Democrat Respondents Republican

Respondents

Covariance P Covariance P

Self-Knowledge <--> Self-Rating -0.167 0.012 0.246 ***

Dem-Ideology <--> Rep Ideology -0.067 0.142 -0.154 0.001

Turning to the impact of subjective self-knowledge, for both groups, the

more knowledgeable the respondent the more they saw Democrats as having

liberal ideologies (For Republicans, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-Ideology,

standardized value -.223, p < .001; For Democrats, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-

Ideology, standardized value -.251, p = .001). Additionally, for both groups, the

more knowledgeable the respondent the more they saw Republicans as having

Page 118: (1) Information and Political Perception

105

conservative ideologies (For Republicans, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-Ideology,

standardized value .199, p < .001; For Democrats, Self-Knowledge to Democrat-

Ideology, standardized value .159, p = .001). Thus it appears that more

knowledge on the part of respondents is associated with seeing both the ingroup

and the outgroup party as more extreme in its ideological beliefs.

In sum, in perceiving the outgroup, there is significant reverse projection:

As a Democratic respondent is more liberal, Republicans are seen as more

conservative; As a Republican respondent is more conservative, Democrats are

seen as more liberal. Positive Projection to the ingroup occurred among both

Democrats and Republicans: as a Republican respondent is more conservative,

Republicans are seen as more conservative and as a Democrat respondent is

more liberal, Democrats are seen as more liberal. Looking at subjective self-

knowledge, higher values among both Republicans and Democrats are

associated with the tendency to see both parties as more polarized (Democrats

as more liberal and Republicans as more conservative).

Discussion

The two models present a consistent view about projection and

polarization. In general the two models demonstrated the tendency of the

respondents to project their own political positions - both ideological and issue-

specific - towards their own political party. Additionally, the respondents

contrasted the outgroup party’s ideological position from their own ideological

position.

Page 119: (1) Information and Political Perception

106

In the 1988 model the respondents from both political parties projected

their own ideology onto their ingroup, and contrasted their ideological position to

the outgroup. The 1980 model had slightly different results, in that the Democrat

subjects did not show ideological projection towards their ingroup. I believe

these differences between the models can be explained by the different

measures that made up the Democrat and Republican Ideology latent variables

in each. In the 1980 model the latent variables were composed of multiple

attitude issues across a few target candidates, while the 1988 model the latent

variables were made up of a single indicator of ideology across seven target

candidates. The covariances between the issues were controlled for in the 1980

ANES, which led to the lack of projection towards the ingroup for Democrat

respondents. Additionally, it is possible that Democrats had a much more varied

set of personal issue stands, which were not always liberal (Hayes, Murphy,

Johnson, & Bowler, 2008).

In general increased political knowledge led to polarization of the two

parties ideological position, by increasing perceptions of ideological extremity of

both the ingroup and the outgroup. This is interesting because it demonstrates

that more knowledgeable voters increased their perception of political

polarization by changing their impressions of the ideological extremity of both

parties, not merely the outgroup. In the 1988 model knowledgeable respondents

from both parties saw both parties as more extreme. In the 1980 ANES dataset

there were slightly different results. Specifically the Republican respondents’

self-knowledge did not increase perceived extremity of the Republican targets.

Page 120: (1) Information and Political Perception

107

Again this difference between the models could be how the ideology was

measured in each. It seems likely that overall ideology, as measured in the 1988

model was more susceptible to perceptions of extremity, while the 1980 ideology

measure, made up of multiple attitudes regressed more towards the mean.

Page 121: (1) Information and Political Perception

108

Chapter 5:

General Discussion

The purpose of this dissertation was to examine the influence of different

types of information on perception of political candidates. The type and amount

of information that individual voters have access to is large and complex. How

much information voters think they have is also very flexible; what one voter

thinks of as being well informed, another may not. The first two studies were

designed to control the amount and type of information that was available, and

examine how it impacted impressions. In the final study I did not have this

control; I had to use the voter’s opinion of their political knowledge. Both the

laboratory and survey studies gave me an interesting picture of information’s role

in political perceptions.

Before voters know anything about a specific political candidate’s position

on an issue they know their own position. Both the laboratory and survey studies

supported previous work that the voter’s own political position, both ideologically

and issue-specific, impact how they perceive the candidates (Brent & Granberg,

1982; Feldman & Conover, 1983; Judd, Kenny, & Krosnick, 1983; Kinder, 1978;

Krosnick, 1990). Specifically, people project their own positions onto candidates

from their ingroup, and occasionally contrast the outgroup candidates. In Study

One neither relevancy nor diagnosticity influenced the amount the subjects

projected their own attitudes. Prior to learning individuating information about the

candidates the subjects contrasted the outgroup candidates’ position from the

subject’s own. There was also contrast in the ANES data, which suggests that

Page 122: (1) Information and Political Perception

109

when individuating information about the candidates is low voters rely more on

their heuristics, one of which is to use their own position as an anchor, projecting

towards the ingroup and contrasting the outgroup. Study Two demonstrated that

candidate consistency, but not popularity increases projection to an ingroup

candidate. These data support Conover and Feldman’s (1982) argument that

projection is a central process in political impression formation, often used as a

heuristic when information about the candidate is low or ambiguous.

These results inform an important debate within political science about the

type and amount of information that a candidate should present to the public

during a campaign. Early political scientists have pointed out the tendency of

politicians to adopt ambiguous issue positions (Downs, 1957; Key, 1958), while

other work has demonstrated that politicians pander to their audience, modifying

their position to suit public opinion (Barone, 1997;Lippmann, 1955; Page, 1978).

As discussed earlier, many voters want to select a candidate that shares their

position on important issues, with theorists viewing this type of voting as rational

(Downs, 1957). The projection results from Study One would suggest that issue

ambiguity is the best strategy for candidates to adopt; the voter’s stereotypic

views of where the parties stand on the issues overwhelms individuating

information about the candidates (Conover, 1981), especially when the

information is irrelevant or ambiguous. This is especially true when the audience

are not members of the candidate’s own political party. When subjects in Study

One learned information about the outgroup candidate that was not diagnostic of

the candidate’s position, it was either ambiguous or irrelevant, the voters’

Page 123: (1) Information and Political Perception

110

contrast of the outgroup candidates’ positions from the subject’s own position

disappeared. In the present study, ambiguity did not significantly impact the

subjects’ level of projection towards ingroup candidates. Since there is a strong

and reliable relationship between perceptions of attitude similarity and voting

behavior (Quist & Crano, 2003) and ambiguity increases or maintains projection

of attitude position, ambiguity seems the best strategy for candidates to adopt.

One question not addressed in the present set of studies is how ambiguity

would impact projection within a primary election, when the goal of the

candidates is to individuate oneself from other within-party candidates. It is

possible that ambiguity would increase projection in primary elections as well as

when comparing both ingroup and outgroup candidates. It is also possible that

ambiguity would result in the voter regressing the candidate’s position towards

the middle, which may or may not increase projection depending on the voter’s

own position on the issue. This is an important question and worthy of further

exploration.

The survey study demonstrated how the voter’s own political ideology

influences how differently they see political candidates from each party. The

more ideologically extreme the voters were the more polarized they perceived

the two political parties to be. The survey study also revealed that voters who

considered themselves to be more knowledgeable about politics also perceived

greater political polarization between the parties. In Study One there was less

polarization after the subjects received relevant-undiagnostic and irrelevant

(marginal) information about the candidates. Much of the candidate information

Page 124: (1) Information and Political Perception

111

presented in the media is ambiguous (Waldman & Jamieson, 2003), is focused

on personality rather than position (Graber, 1984; Patterson, 1980), and reports

more on the “horserace” aspect of the campaign than candidate positions

(Bartels, 1988). Thus, voters who feel they are more informed about politics may

not necessarily be better informed about the candidates’ positions or even about

the candidates in general. In the laboratory the information was all individuating,

that is, it gave the subject a better picture of the candidate. Even in Study One,

when the information was irrelevant to the candidates’ attitude positions it still

individuated the candidates, which in turn “diluted” the effect of party position

(Nisbett, Zukier, & Lemley, 1981). This dilution led to decreased polarization of

the two parties, and a non-significant decrease in projection towards ingroup

candidates. However, when the information about the candidate was diagnostic

of their political positions and in line with stereotypes about their party’s position,

the subjects maintained the same level of polarization as they did when all they

had was the candidates’ names, pictures, and political party. So while

knowledge about a candidate’s actual position may maintain or increase

perceptions of party extremity, individuating information about political candidates

that is irrelevant or undiagnostic seems to decrease polarization.

As mentioned in the introduction both political deliberation (Sunstein,

2002) and political primaries (Raven & Gallo, 1965) increase polarization. The

decrease in polarization of candidates from the opposing parties after learning

either irrelevant or ambiguous information about the candidates in Study One

that this increase in polarization during deliberation and primaries is due more to

Page 125: (1) Information and Political Perception

112

stereotypic views of the political parties and less to do with individuating

information about the candidates. However, when the information about the

candidates’ positions were clear, polarization remained high which supports

findings that repeated expression of each parties’ often diametrically opposed

position increases polarization (Stasavage, 2007).

Directionally, the subjects relied less on their stereotypes about the groups

when they learned diagnostic information about the candidates, resulting in less

consensus. Overall, the Democrats had stronger stereotypes about the parties,

agreeing with each other more than the Republicans did, however this was less

true for subjects who learned relevant and diagnostic information. It seems that

Democrats had a more coherent schema for the political positions of each party

on political issues, which they applied when the information was ambiguous. It

could be that Democrats used general party ideology to form impressions of

specific candidates on specific issues more than the Republicans did (Waldman

& Jamieson, 2002).

How affiliated voters were to their political parties influenced how they

used the candidate’s popularity and attitude consistency information. Strongly

affiliated voters (in the study only Democrat subjects were examined) preferred a

candidate that held attitudes that were consistent with the party ideology; that is,

liberal Democrats. Directionally when the voter was strongly affiliated and the

ingroup candidate was consistent, popularity mattered less, however, when the

candidate was inconsistent the more popular candidate was preferred. Less

affiliated voters preferred the outsider, the unpopular-inconsistent candidate. It

Page 126: (1) Information and Political Perception

113

seems they identified with the candidate that crossed the party-line on some

issues and that wasn’t supported by the majority of their fellow party members.

Less affiliated voters may have held inconsistent views on some issues and may

have preferred a candidate that demonstrated ideological flexibility. Why they did

not also support the inconsistent candidate when he was popular is puzzling. It

could be that the less affiliated subjects were less motivated to pay attention to

the statements about the candidates and the task in general. Regardless of how

affiliated voters were to their political party, they project more towards consistent

political candidates than inconsistent ones. So while in explicit measure of liking,

similarity, and voting likelihood the lower affiliated subjects preferred an

inconsistent-unpopular candidate, they still tended to project their own attitudes

more on the consistent candidates.

How people use information to form impressions about other people is an

important question on its own. In the political domain it has even more apparent

importance; voters’ impressions of political candidates can influence how they

vote. Selecting a political candidate who shares our political attitudes could be

seen as the most rational way to vote (Downs, 1957; Shapiro, 1969). If that is

true, then how people use information to form, maintain, and change their

impressions of political candidates can in turn inform researchers about how

people vote. In the end voting is the average person’s main form of political

expression. Since I began this dissertation with a quote by one of our nation’s

greatest political thinkers about the importance of voting, it is appropriate to end

with another.

Page 127: (1) Information and Political Perception

114

“The rational and peacable instrument of reform, the suffrage of the people.”

Thomas Jefferson.

Page 128: (1) Information and Political Perception

115

References

Abrajano, M. (2005). “Who Evaluates a Presidential Candidate by Using

Non-Policy Campaign Messages?”. Political Research Quarterly, 58, 55-

67.

Abramson, P. R., Aldrich, J. H., Paolino, P., & Rhode, D. W. (1992).

Sophisticated voting in the 1988 Presidential primaries. American Political

Science Review, 86, 55-69.

Aldrich, J. H. (1980). Before the convention: Strategies and choices in

presidential nomination campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago

Press.

Allen, V. L., & Wilder, D. A. (1979). Social support in absentia: The effect of an

absentee partner on conformity. Human Relations, 32, 103-111.

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Garden City, NY: Doubleday

Anchor.

Arrow, K. J. (1951). Social choice and individual values. New York: John Wiley

and Sons.

Ashe, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal

and Social Psychology, 41, 258-290.

Barone, M. (1997). Polls are part of the air politicians breathe. The Public

Perspective, 8, 1-2.

Bartels, L. M. (1988). Presidential primaries and the dynamics of public choice.

Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Page 129: (1) Information and Political Perception

116

Baxter, L. A., Young, T. J., & Bittner, J. R. (1973). Identifying the charisma

factor in political communication. . Paper presented at the International

Communication Association Convention, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

Belknap, G., & Campbell, A. (1952). Political party identification and attitudes

toward foreign policy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 601-623.

Bennett, S. E. (1995). American’s knowledge of ideology. American Politics

Quarterly, 23, 259-278.

Brady, H. E., & Sniderman, P. M. (1985). Attitude attribution: A group basis for

political reasoning. American Political Science Review, 79, 1061-1078.

Brent, E. & Granberg, D. (1982). Subjective agreement with the presidential

candidates of 1976 and 1980. . Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 42, 393-403.

Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal group situation. A cognitive –

motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307-324.

Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. S.

Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition: A dual process

model of impression formation, 1-36 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brown, R. D., & Wright, G. C. (1992). Elections and state party polarization.

American Politics Research, 20, 411-426.

Burger, J. M., Messian, N., Patel, S., del Prado, A. & Anderson, C. (2004). What

a coincidence! The effects of incidental similarity on compliance.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 35-43.

Campbell, J. (1983). The electoral consequences of issue ambiguity: An

Page 130: (1) Information and Political Perception

117

examination of presidential candidates’ issue positions from 1968 to 1980.

Political Behavior, 5, 277-291.

Campbell, A., Converse, P.E., Miller, W.E., & Stokes, D.E. (1960). The

American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Caprara, G. V. & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Personalizing politics: A congruency

model of political preference. American Psychologist, 59, 581-594.

Carmines, E. G., & Kuklinski, J. H. (1990). Incentives, opportunities, and the

logic of public opinion in American political representation. In J.A.

Ferejohn and J.H. Kuklinski (Eds.), Information and democratic processes.

Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Clement, R. W., & Krueger, J. (2002). Social categorization moderates social

projection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 219-231.

Conover, P. J. (1981). Political cues and perception of candidates. American

Politics Research, 9, 427-448.

Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1982). Projection and the perception of

candidates issue positions. The Western Political Quarterly, 35, 228-244.

Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1989). Candidate perceptions in an ambiguous

world: Campaigns, cues, and inference processes. American Journal of

Political Science, 33, 912-940.

Cooper, W. H. (1981). Ubiquitous halo. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 218-244.

Dalton, R. J. (1979). Citizen attitudes and political behavior. Comparative

Political Studies, 33, 912-940.

Dawes, R., D. Singer & P. Lemons (1972). An experimental analysis of the

Page 131: (1) Information and Political Perception

118

contrast effect and its implications for intergroup communication and

indirect assessment of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 21, 281-295.

Delli Carpini, M., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and

why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

DePaulo, B. M., Kenny, D. A., Hoover, C., Webb, W. & Oliver, P. (1987).

Accuracy of person perception: Do people know what kinds of impressions

they convey. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52, 303-315.

DiDonato, T. E., & Kruegar, J. I. (2007). Intergroup differentiation and ingroup

favoritism: Evidence for the sufficiency of egocentric processes.

Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper.

Ebenbach, D. H., & Keltner, D. (1998). Power, emotion, and judgmental

accuracy in social conflict: Motivating the cognitive miser. Basic and

Applied Social Psychology, 20, 7-21.

Erber, R., & Fiske, S. T. (1984). Outcome dependency and attention to

inconsistent information about others. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 47, 709-726.

Feldman, S., & Conover, P. J. (1983). Candidates, issues, and voters: The role

of inference in political perception. The Journal of Politics, 45, 810-839.

Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from

category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and

motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental

Social Psychology, 23, 1-74.

Page 132: (1) Information and Political Perception

119

Golebiowska, E. A. (2001). Group stereotypes and political evaluation.

American Politics Research, 29, 535-565.

Goodman, C., & Murray, G.R. (2007). Do you see what I see? Perceptions of

party differences and voting behavior. American Politics Research, 35,

905-931.

Graber, D. A. (1984). Processing the news: How people tame the information

tide. New York: Longman.

Granberg, D., & Brent, E. (1974). Dove–hawk placements in the 1968 election:

Application of social judgment and balance theories. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 29, 687-695.

Hamill, R., Lodge, M., & Blake, F. (1985). The breadth, depth, and utility of

class, partisan, and ideological schema. American Journal of Political

Science, 29, 850-870.

Hastie, R. & Kumar, P. A. (1979). Person memory: Personality traits as

organizing principles in memory for behaviors. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 37, 25-38.

Hayes, T., Murphy, C., Johnson, M., & Bowler, S. (2008). Strategies of

ambiguity: Modeling rhetoric in primary election campaigns. Paper

presented at the Western Political Science Association Annual Meeting,

San Diego, CA.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John

Wiley.

Hellweg, S. A. (1995). Campaigns and candidate images in American

Page 133: (1) Information and Political Perception

120

presidential elections. In K. L. Hacker (Ed.), Candidate images in

presidential elections, (pp. 1-17). Westport: CT: Praeger.

Hofstetter, C. R., Zukin, C., & Buss, T. F. (1978). Political imagery and

information in an age of television. Journalism Quarterly, 56, 106-115,

152.

Holbrook, T. M. (1996). Do campaigns matter? Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Publishing.

Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Krosnick, J. A., Visser, P. S., & Boninger, D. S.

(2005). Attitude importance and the accumulation of attitude-relevant

knowledge in memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,

749-769.

Jefferson, T. (1900). The Jeffersonian Cyclopedia. J.P. Foley (Ed). New York:

Funk and Wagnalls Company.

Jones, D. R. (2001). Party polarization and legislative gridlock. Political

Research Quarterly, 54, 125-141.

Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & Krosnick, J. A. (1983). Judging the positions of

political candidates: Models of assimilation and contrast. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 952-963.

Kaid, L. L., & Hirsh, R. O. (1973). Selective exposure and candidate image: A

field study over time. Central States Speech Journal, 24, 48-51.

Kam, C. D. (2005). Who toes the party line? Cues, values, and individual

differences. Political Behavior, 27, 163-182.

Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of

Page 134: (1) Information and Political Perception

121

representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.

Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction.

Psychological Review, 80, 237-251.

Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American

Psychologist, 39, 341-350.

Keeter, S. (1987). The illusion of intimacy: Television and the role of candidate

personal qualities in voter choice. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 344–358.

Keeter, S., & Zukin, C. (1982). New romances and old horses: The public

images of presidential candidates. In D. Graber (Ed.), The president and

the public. Philadelphia, PA: Institute for Study of Human Issues.

Kendall, K. E., & Yum, J. O. (1984). Persuading the blue collar voters: Issues,

images, and homophily. In R. N. Bostrom (Ed.), Communication yearbook

8, (pp. 707-722). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Kenny, D. A. (1983). BLOCKO: A FORTRAN program for blocked data

structures. Unpublished manuscript, University of Connecticut.

Kenny, D. A. (2004). PERSON: A general model of interpersonal perception.

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 265-280.

Key, V. O. (1958). Politics, parties, and pressure groups (4th ed.). New York:

Cornell.

Kinder, D. R. (1978). Political person perception: The asymmetrical influence of

sentiment and choice on perceptions of presidential candidates. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 859-871.

Kinder, D. R. (1983). Diversity and complexity in American public opinion. In A.

Page 135: (1) Information and Political Perception

122

W. Finifter (Ed.). Political science: The state of the discipline.

Washington, DC: American Political Science Association.

Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and action in the realm of politics. In D. T. Gilbert,

S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology, Vol 1,

778-867. New York: Oxford University Press.

Krosnick, J. A. (1990). Americans' perceptions of presidential candidates: A test

of the projection hypothesis. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 159-182.

Krosnick, J.A. (2002). The challenges of political psychology: Lessons to be

learned from research on attitude perception. In J.H. Kuklinski (Ed.),

Thinking about political psychology, 115-152. New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Krosnick, J. A. & Alwin, D. F. (1989). Aging and susceptibility to attitude change.

Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57, 416-425.

Krueger, J. I. (2007). From social projection to social behavior. European

Review of Social Psychology, 18, 1-35.

Krueger, J. I. & Rothbart, M. (1988). Use of categorical and individuating

information in perceptions of group characteristics. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 27, 878-888.

Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (1997). Voting correctly. American Political

Science Review, 91, 585-599.

Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). An experimental study of information

Page 136: (1) Information and Political Perception

123

search, memory, and decision making during a political campaign. In J.

Kuklinski (Ed.), Political psychology and public opinion, 136-159. New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Lau, R.R., & Redlawsk, D.P. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing

during election campaigns. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Layman, G. C., & Casey, T. M. (2002). Party polarization and party structuring

of policy attitudes: A comparison of three NES panel studies. Political

Behavior, 24, 199-236.

Lippmann, W. (1955). Essays in the public philosophy. Boston: Little, Brown.

Lodge, M., & Hamill, R. (1986). A partisan schema for political information

processing. American Political Science Review, 80, 505-519.

Lodge, M. & Taber, C. (2000). Three steps towards a theory of motivated

political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M.D. McCubbins, and S.L. Popkin (Eds.),

Elements of reason, 183-213. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lupia, A., McCubbins, M.D., & Popkin, S.L. (2000). Beyond rationality: Reason

and the study of politics. In A. Lupia, M.D. McCubbins, and S.L. Popkin

(Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of

rationality, 1-20. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Miller, W., & Shanks, J. M. (1996). The New American Voter. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Miller, A., Wattenberg, M. P., & Malanchuk, O. (1986). Schematic assessments

of presidential candidates. American Political Science Review, 80, 521-

540.

Page 137: (1) Information and Political Perception

124

Mondak, J. J. (1993). Public opinion and heuristic processing of source cues.

Political Behavior, 15, 167-192.

Moreno, K. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1999). Resisting stereotype change: The

role of motivation and attentional capacity in defending social beliefs.

Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 2, 5-16.

Nicholson, S. P. (2005). Voting the agenda: Candidates, elections, and ballot

propositions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Nicholson, S. P., Pantoja, A., & Segura, G. M. (2006). Political knowledge and

issue voting among the Latino electorate. Political Research Quarterly,

59, 259-271.

Nisbett, R. E., Zukier, H., & Lemley, R. (1981). The dilution effect: Nondiagnostic

information weakens the effect of diagnostic information. Cognitive

Psychology, 13, 248-277.

Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes:

Replicated factor structure in peer nominated personality ratings. Journal

of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 574-583.

Page, B. I. (1976). The theory of political ambiguity. American Political Science

Review, 70, 742-752.

Page, B. I. (1978). Choices and echoes in presidential elections. Chicago:

University of Chicago Press.

Page, B. I., & Jones, C. C. (1979). Reciprocal effects of policy preferences,

party loyalties, and the vote. The American Political Science Review, 73,

1071-1089.

Page 138: (1) Information and Political Perception

125

Park, B. & Judd, C. M. (1989). Agreement on initial impressions: Differences

due to perceivers, trait dimensions, and target behaviors. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 493-505.

Park, B. & Rothbart, M. (1982). The perception of out-group homogeneity and

levels of categorization: Memory for the subordinate attributes of in-group

and out-group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

42, 1051-1068.

Patterson, T. E. (1980). The mass media election: How Americans choose their

president. New York: Praeger.

Pennefather & Park, B. (2006). Unpublished data.

Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in

Presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of the beholder:

Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological Review,

111, 781-799.

Quist, R. M., & Crano W. D. (2003). Assumed policy similarity and voter

preference. Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 149-162.

Raven, B. H., & Gallo, P. S. (1965). The effects of nominating conventions,

elections, and reference group identification upon the perception of

political figures. Human Relations, 18, 217-229.

Riggle, E. D., Ottati, V. C., Wyer, R. S., Kuklinski, J., & Schwarz, N. (1992).

Bases of political judgments: The role of stereotypic and nonstereotypic

information. Political Behavior, 14, 67-87.

Page 139: (1) Information and Political Perception

126

Robbins, J. M., & Krueger, J. I. (2005). Social projection to ingroups and

outgroups: A review and meta-analysis. Personality and Social

Psychology Review, 9, 32-47.

Robinson, M. J. & Sheehan, M. A. (1983). Over the wire and on TV: CBS and

UPI in campaign '80. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Rothbart, M., Evans, M. & Fulero, S. (1979). Recall for confirming events:

Memory processes and the maintenance of social stereotypes. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 343-355.

Sears, D. O. (1969). Political behavior. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The

handbook of social psychology: Volume 5, (pp. 315-458). Reading, MA:

Addison-Wesley.

Sears, D. O. (1975). Political sophistication. In F.I. Greenstein and N.W. Polsby

(Eds.), Handbook of political science, 2, 93-127. Menlo Park, CA:

Addison-Wesley.

Sears, D. O., & Funk, C. (1991). The role of self-interest in socal and political

attitudes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social

psychology, 2, 1-91. New York: Academic Press.

Shanteau, J. (1988). Psychological characteristics and strategies of expert

decision makers. Acta Psychological, 68, 203-215.

Shapiro, M. J. (1969). Rational political man: A synthesis of economic and

social psychological perspectives. American Political Science Review, 63,

1106-1119.

Shepsle, K. (1972). The strategy of ambiguity: Uncertainty and electoral

Page 140: (1) Information and Political Perception

127

competition. American Political Science Review, 66, 555-563.

Shively, W. P. (1992). From differential abstention to conversion: A change in

electoral change, 1864-1988. American Journal of Political Science, 36,

309-330.

Sigel, R. (1964). Effect of partisanship on the perception of political candidates.

Public Opinion Quarterly, 28, 483-496.

Sniderman, P. M. (1993). The new look in public opinion research. In A. W.

Finifter (Ed.). Political science: The state of the discipline II. Washington,

DC: American Political Science Association.

Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice:

Explorations in political psychology. New York: Cambridge University

Press.

Srull, T. K., Lichtenstein, M. & Rothbart, M. (1985). Associative storage and

retrieval processes in person memory. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 316-345.

Stasavage, D. (2007). Polarization and publicity: Rethinking the benefits of

deliberative democract. The Journal of Politics, 69, 59-72.

Sunstein, C. (2002). The law of group polarization. The Journal of Political

Philosophy, 10, 175-195.

Swann, W. B. & Gill, M. J. (1997). Confidence and accuracy in person

perception: Do we know what we think we know about our relationship

partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 747-57.

Turner, J. C., Brown, R. J. & Tajfel, H. (1979). Social comparison and group

Page 141: (1) Information and Political Perception

128

interest in ingroup favouritism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9,

187-204.

Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: Aheuristic for judging

frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-232.

Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics

and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.

Von Neuman, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic

behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Visser, P. S. & Krosnick, J. A. (1998). Development of attitude strength over the

life cycle: Surge and decline. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,

75, 1389-1410.

Wakshlag, J. J., & Edison, N. G. (1979). Attraction, credibility, perceived

similarity, and the image of public figures. Communication Quarterly, 27,

27-34.

Waldman, P., & Jamieson, K. H. (2002). Rhetorical convergence and issue

knowledge in the 2000 presidential election. Presidential Studies

Quarterly, 33, 145-163.

Wänke, M. & Wyer, R. S. (1996). Individual difference s in person memory: The

effects of socio-political ideology and ingroups vs. outgroups membership

on responses to socially relevant behavior. Personality & Social

Psychology Bulletin, 22, 742-754.

Wattenburg, M. J. (1992). The rise of candidate-centered politics. Cambridge:

Harvard University Press.

Page 142: (1) Information and Political Perception

129

Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of

stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,

961-977.

Wilder, D. A. (1984). Predictions of belief homogeneity and similarity following

social categorization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 323-333.

Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Page 143: (1) Information and Political Perception

130

Appendix A:

Photographs of Political Candidates

Page 144: (1) Information and Political Perception

131

Appendix B:

Study One Politically Irrelevant Statements 1- Republican Steven Johnson

For his last vacation Steven Johnson took a weeklong horseback tour of Argentina. He enjoys tennis, reading and can often be seen running with his two dogs Jack and Pepper. His favorite song is “Good Vibrations” by the Beach Boys. Steven Johnson’s favorite quote is by Olympic gold medalist Carl Lewis: “Life is about timing”. 2 – Democrat William McCartell

William McCartell is an avid golfer and also enjoys refurbishing antique furniture. His favorite song is “Thunder Road” by Bruce Springsteen. His last vacation was scuba-diving off the Great Barrier Reef in Australia. William McCartell’s favorite quote is “You can’t build a reputation on what you are going to do” by inventor Henry Ford. 3 – Republican Michael Larson

Michael Larson’s favorite song is Tony Bennett’s “Blue Velvet”. He has been waterskiing for most of his life and recently won an amateur competition. His favorite quote is by Albert Einstein, “Anyone who has never made a mistake has never tried anything new”. He recently spent a week in Manhattan enjoying great food a number of Broadway plays. 4 – Democrat Stan Martin

Stan Martin’s favorite quote is “Believe that life is worth living and your belief will help create the fact”, by psychologist and philosopher William James. His last vacation was exploring the Mayan ruins south of Mexico City. Stan Martin is a member of a local softball team, and until recently played center for a local over-40 hockey team. His favorite song is “Walk the Line” by Johnny Cash. 5 – Republican David Herring David Herring enjoys all things nautical, including sailing, rowing, and building model boats. His most recent vacation was a tour of the America South, exploring historical sights and enjoying local food and music. David Herring’s favorite song is “Norwegian Wood” by The Beatles, and his favorite quote is “A prudent question is one-half of wisdom”, by the chemist Francis Bacon.

6 – Democrat James Nelson

James Nelson recently went on a weekend Napa Valley wine tour, staying in Bed and Breakfasts. His favorite song is “You Mean Everything to Me” by Neil Sedaka. James Nelson enjoys running and has taken part in a number of half-marathons. His favorite quote is “Success is a science; if you have the conditions, you get the result”, by novelist, playwright, and poet Oscar Wilde. 7 – Republican Dean Hayes

Dean Hayes favorite quote is by chemist and microbiologist Louis Pasteur, “Let me tell you the secret that has led me to my goal. My strength lies solely in my tenacity.” His favorite song is “Slip Slidin Away” by Paul Simon. Dean Hayes is a musician who co-founded a local jazz quartet. He recently took a California golfing vacation, including two days at Pebble Beach. 8 – Democrat Gregory Davis Gregory Davis took his last vacation sailing the Greek Isles on a 45 foot sloop with friends and family. His favorite quote is “If you don't know where you are going, you'll end up some place else”, by Baseball Hall of Famer Yogi Berra. He enjoys spending time outdoors; fishing, hiking, and camping. His favorite song is “The Best is Yet to Come”, by Frank Sinatra.

Page 145: (1) Information and Political Perception

132

Appendix C:

Study One Politically Relevant-Undiagnostic Statements 1- Republican Steven Johnson In the past few years our country has faced many challenges. We now have a chance to move ahead with confidence, resolve, and hope. We need a candidate who will maintain stability in an unstable time. Steven Johnson is that candidate. He has a plan to create jobs, and improve

education. Steven Johnson, keeping our country safe and secure. 2 – Democrat William McCartell Our nation needs to re-examine its priorities. We are at a cross-road in our history and need a leader with the vision to guide us to a better future. William McCartell is the leader to do it. He has plans to refocus our country’s priorities on job creation, education, and health care. William McCartell, defending America. 3 – Republican Dean Hayes Staying healthy is a concern that every American has. We need to know that if we get sick we will receive the care we need. Unfortunately, that is not always the case. Dean Hayes has plans to change our health care system so that every American will receive the health care he or she needs. Dean Hayes cares. 4 – Democrat Gregory Davis It is a travesty that in the most advanced country in the world there are people without health care. We need to develop a system in which every American can receive medical treatment. His goal is to insure that no American will have to go without. He is committed to improving social security. Gregory Davis is ready to help heal America. 5 – Republican Michael Larson Our country is the economic power-house of the world. We need leaders who will continue to carry on this tradition. Michael Larson will ensure that our country’s economy remains strong and. He understands the necessity of tightening our country’s borders and increasing national security spending. He is a strong supporter of our men and women serving overseas. Michael Larson is a leader for today’s world. 6 – Democrat Stan Martin Now is the time to invest in our nation’s future. We need leaders dedicated to strengthening our economy. Stan Martin is one of those leaders. He is committed to investing in education and health care. He will protect our national security and defend our interests oversea. Stan Martin supports initiatives to increase governmental support of our servicemen and women. Together we can move America forward. 7 – Republican David Herring Our world is changing. We are nearing a time when important decisions about our use of energy will need to be made. David Herring is prepared to make those tough choices and has a plan for decreasing our reliance on foreign oil. He has plans for improving health care and restructuring social security. He plans to improve education and the tax system. David Herring, steady leadership in times of change. 8 – Democrat James Nelson Energy is the lifeblood of our country. Without it we would stop moving ahead. Our country has become overly reliant on foreign oil. James Nelson has outlined a policy to increase our energy independence. He has plans to revitalize our education system and create a more equitable tax system. James Nelson, dedication, integrity, focus.

Page 146: (1) Information and Political Perception

133

Appendix D:

Study One Politically Relevant-Diagnostic Statements Note: The bolded statements are diagnostic, stereotype-consistent positions on the issues. The statements in the relevant-undiagnostic condition (Appendix C) are identical, with the exception that the bolded statements are left out. The judges will not see the bolding.

1- Republican Steven Johnson (National Security) In the past few years our country has faced many challenges. We now have a chance to move ahead with confidence, resolve, and hope. We need a candidate who will maintain stability in an unstable time. Steven Johnson is that candidate. He is dedicated to protecting our country and keeping our families safe, by increasing the military budget to combat terrorism overseas. He has a plan to create jobs, improve education, and lower the cost of health car. He has a plan to decrease health care costs by making it more difficult to bring frivolous law-suits against doctors and HMOs. Steven Johnson, keeping our country safe and secure.

2 – Democrat William McCartell (National Security) Our nation needs to re-examine its priorities. We are at a cross-road in our history and need a leader with the vision to guide us to a better future. William McCartell is the leader to do it. He has plans to refocus our country’s priorities on job creation, education, and health care. He plans to increase Medicare coverage, and has outlined a universal health care plan. He will focus on keeping our families and our country safe, by bringing estranged nations such as Syria and Iran to the bargaining table. William McCartell, defending America. 3 – Republican Michael Larson (Economy) Our country is the economic power-house of the world. We need leaders who will continue to carry on this tradition. Michael Larson will ensure that our country’s economy remains strong and competitive by helping large businesses that are the backbone of our economy and making the recent tax cuts permanent. He understands the necessity of tightening our country’s borders and increasing national security spending. He is a strong supporter of our men and women serving overseas. He will improve our energy problems by increasing the use of America’s abundant coal resources. Michael Larson is a leader for today’s world.

4 – Democrat Stan Martin (Economy) Now is the time to invest in our nation’s future. We need leaders dedicated to strengthening our economy, by focusing on the under-taxation of large corporations, and increasing tax breaks for the lower and middle classes. Stan Martin is one of those leaders. He will deal with our energy crisis by increasing the use of alternative energy sources such as wind and solar power. He is committed to investing in education and health care. He will protect our national security and defend our interests oversea. Stan Martin supports initiatives to increase governmental support of our servicemen and women. Together we can move America forward. 5 – Republican David Herring (Energy) Our world is changing. We are nearing a time when important decisions about our use of energy will need to be made. David Herring is prepared to make those tough choices and has a plan for decreasing our reliance on foreign oil by increasing domestic oil exploration and coal energy capability. He has plans for improving health care and restructuring social security. He will protect our nation’s security by increasing the budget for the Armed Services and research into new technologies to protect the American military overseas. David Herring,

steady leadership in times of change. 6 – Democrat James Nelson (Energy)

Page 147: (1) Information and Political Perception

134

Energy is the lifeblood of our country. Without it we would stop moving ahead. Our country has become overly reliant on foreign oil. James Nelson has outlined a policy to increase our energy independence by relying more on alternative renewable energy sources including wind and solar power and seriously restricting oil and gas drilling. He is committed to improving social security and has a plan for making health care available to all Americans. He will redirect money to overseas goodwill projects by focusing on international aid to underdeveloped countries. James Nelson, dedication, integrity, focus.

7 – Republican Dean Hayes (Health Care) Staying healthy is a concern that every American has. We need to know that if we get sick we will receive the care we need. Unfortunately, that is not always the case. Dean Hayes has plans to change our health care system so that every American will receive the health care he or she needs, by allowing Americans to deduct health care costs from their income tax. He plans to improve education and the tax system, by abolishing the Estate Tax and decreasing taxes across the board which will have a trickle down effect on the nation’s economy. Dean Hayes cares. 8 – Democrat Gregory Davis (Health Care) It is a travesty that in the most advanced country in the world there are people without health care. We need to develop a system in which every American can receive medical treatment. Gregory Davis has a plan to implement a Universal Health care system and equal drug coverage. His goal is to insure that no American will have to go without. He has plans to revitalize our education system and create a more equitable tax system, by making the tax system more progressive by increasing taxation of the very rich and decreasing corporate loopholes. Gregory Davis is ready to help heal America.

Page 148: (1) Information and Political Perception

135

Appendix E:

Study One Self Rating Questionnaire

Section 1: Self-ratings of Personality Traits

Using the scale below, rate yourself on each of the following traits. To what extent would you use each trait to describe this yourself? Circle the number that you feel best describes yourself.

Extroverted [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Shy

Disagreeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Agreeable

Trustworthy [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Untrustworthy

Self-Doubting [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Self-Confident

Knowledgeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Uninformed

Reserved [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Outgoing

Good Natured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Irritable

Irresponsible [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Responsible

Self-Assured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Insecure

Unintelligent [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Intelligent

Page 149: (1) Information and Political Perception

136

Section 2: Political Attitudes, Ideology, and Knowledge. Below are some statements of possible attitudes for several currently important issues. To what

extent do you agree or disagree with each attitude statement? Use the scale below in making

your judgments.

Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I support a woman’s right to abortion on demand. _________ It is important to support equal rights for gays and lesbians. _________ There should be no limitations on the right to bear arms. _________ The United States should engage in more diplomatic discussions rather than using the military _________ HMOs ensure that American health care stays competitive with the rest of the world. _________ The government should move away from oil and invest in alternative energy. _________ I support decreasing taxes on large corporations to increase the nation’s economic vitality. _________ Our country needs to invest in a universal health care plan. _________ I view abortion as the taking of a human life. _________ Marriage should be reserved for heterosexual couples. _________ Limits on gun sales (including handguns) should be enacted into law. _________ The U.S. should drill almost all of its available oil resources. _________ I support increased spending to support the government’s overseas military operations. _________ I believe that tax breaks for big-business hurt the average American.

Page 150: (1) Information and Political Perception

137

Please think about the your overall political stand. To what extent would you say that you are conservative versus liberal? (circle a number on the scale below) Very Conservative

Very

Liberal

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 What is your political affiliation? Democrat Republican Independent No Affiliation How strongly do you identify with your political party? Very Weakly

Very Strongly

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How knowledgeable are you about politics Very Unknowledgeable

Very Knowledgeable

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How interested are you in politics? Very Interested

Not at all Interested

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 151: (1) Information and Political Perception

138

Appendix F:

Study One Rating of Political Candidates Questionnaire

Section 1: Ratings of Candidates’ Personality Traits

Steven Johnson

Republican

Using the scale below, rate the candidate on each of the following traits. To what extent would you use each trait to describe this candidate? Circle the number that you feel best describes the candidate.

Extroverted [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Shy

Disagreeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Agreeable

Trustworthy [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Untrustworthy

Self-Doubting [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Self-Confident

Knowledgeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Uninformed

Reserved [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Outgoing

Good Natured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Irritable

Irresponsible [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Responsible

Self-Assured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Insecure

Unintelligent [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Intelligent

Page 152: (1) Information and Political Perception

139

Section 2: Political Attitudes, Ideology, and Knowledge. Below are some statements of possible attitudes for several currently important issues. To what extent do you believe this candidate would agree or disagree with each attitude statement? Use the scale below in making your judgments. Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I support a woman’s right to abortion on demand. _________ It is important to support equal rights for gays and lesbians. _________ There should be no limitations on the right to bear arms. _________ The United States should engage in more diplomatic discussions rather than using the military _________ HMOs ensure that American health care stays competitive with the rest of the world. _________ The government should move away from oil and invest in alternative energy. _________ I support decreasing taxes on large corporations to increase the nation’s economic vitality. _________ Our country needs to invest in a universal health care plan. _________ I view abortion as the taking of a human life. _________ Marriage should be reserved for heterosexual couples. _________ Limits on gun sales (including handguns) should be enacted into law. _________ The U.S. should drill almost all of its available oil resources. _________ I support increased spending to support the government’s overseas military operations. _________ I believe that tax breaks for big-business hurt the average American. .

Page 153: (1) Information and Political Perception

140

How likely would it be for you to vote for this candidate? Very Unlikely

Very

Likely

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Please think about the candidate’s overall political stand. To what extent would you say that he is conservative versus liberal? (circle a number on the scale below) Very Conservative

Very

Liberal

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How strongly affiliated (attached) to his political party? Not at all Affiliated

Very

Affiliated

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How attractive do you think this candidate is? Very Unattractive

Very

Attractive

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How much do you like this candidate? Dislike Very Much

Neither Like or Dislike

Like

Very Much ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

In general how similar do you think this candidate is to you? Very Dissimilar

Neither Similar or Dissimilar

Very

Similar

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 154: (1) Information and Political Perception

141

Appendix G:

Study Two Target. Popular:

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

Recently Democrat voters in Stephen

Johnson’s home district were polled

about whom they planned to vote for

in the upcoming Congressional primary.

79 percent of registered Democrats stated

they would vote for Stephen Johnson. The

next most popular Democrat candidate

received only 6 percent support.

Unpopular:

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

Recently Democrat voters in Stephen

Johnson’s home district were polled

about whom they planned to vote for

in the upcoming Congressional primary.

Only 6 percent of registered Democrats

stated they would vote for Stephen Johnson.

The most popular Democrat candidate

received 79 percent support.

Page 155: (1) Information and Political Perception

142

Appendix H:

Study Two Consistent Target Information

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

- Voted yes on a measure that

would allow current illegal aliens

the opportunity to participant in

Social Security.

- Wants to place restrictions on

assault weapons and ammunition

types.

- For his last vacation Stephen

Johnson took a weeklong

horseback tour of Argentina.

- Voted yes to a bill requiring the

CIA to report on Guantanamo

detainees’ condition and

interrogation methods.

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

- Stephen Johnson enjoys tennis,

reading and can often be seen

running with his two dogs Jack and

Pepper.

-Wants to invest money into clean

energy alternatives such as solar

and wind, not dirty technology like

nuclear.

- Voted no on a measure that

would subject federal employees to

random drug tests.

- His favorite song is “Good

Vibrations” by the Beach Boys.

- Feels that we need a

Constitutional amendment

protecting the right of a woman to

have an abortion.

Page 156: (1) Information and Political Perception

143

Appendix I:

Study Two Inconsistent Target Information

Note: Statements in bold are the inconsistent statements. All other statements are identical to the statements for the consistent target.

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

- Voted yes on a measure that

would allow current illegal aliens

the opportunity to participant in

Social Security.

- Opposes restrictions on

assault weapons and

ammunition types.

- For his last vacation Stephen

Johnson took a weeklong

horseback tour of Argentina.

- Voted yes to a bill requiring the

CIA to report on Guantanamo

detainees’ condition and

interrogation methods.

Stephen Johnson

Democrat

- Stephen Johnson enjoys tennis,

reading and can often be seen

running with his two dogs Jack and

Pepper.

- Wants to invest money into

efficient energy alternatives

such as nuclear, not inefficient

ones like solar and wind.

- Voted no on a measure that

would subject federal employees to

random drug tests.

- His favorite song is “Good

Vibrations” by the Beach Boys.

- Feels that we need a

Constitutional amendment

defining life from conception.

Page 157: (1) Information and Political Perception

144

Appendix J:

Study Two Self Rating Questionnaire

Below are some statements of possible attitudes for several currently important issues. To what

extent do you agree or disagree with each attitude statement? Use the scale below in making

your judgments.

Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I support a woman’s right to abortion on demand. _________ It is important to support equal rights for gays and lesbians. _________ Poverty is a large problem in our country and should be addressed by governmental support. _________ There should be no limitations on the right to bear arms. _________ The United States should engage in more diplomatic discussions rather than using the military _________ The death penalty is a deterrent for crime, and will decrease the number of murders. _________ Increased security at the expense of an individual’s rights is unacceptable. _________ Illegal immigrants are a drain on our resources, and every effort should be made to deport them. _________ HMOs ensure that American health care stays competitive with the rest of the world. _________ The government should move away from oil and invest in alternative energy. _________ I support decreasing taxes on large corporations to increase the nation’s economic vitality. _________ I do not mind surrendering some personal freedoms to ensure our country’s safety. _________ Charities not the government should be responsible for caring for the poor. _________ Our country needs to invest in a universal health care plan. _________ The death penalty is morally wrong, and does nothing to decrease crime.

Page 158: (1) Information and Political Perception

145

Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I view abortion as the taking of a human life. _________ Drug use is a crime and should be treated as such with stiffer penalties. _________ I believe that immigrant workers should be allowed to become American citizens. _________ Marriage should be reserved for heterosexual couples. _________ Limits on gun sales (including handguns) should be enacted into law. _________ The U.S. should drill almost all of its available oil resources. _________ I support increased spending to support the government’s overseas military operations. _________ Non-violent drug offenders should receive treatment rather than prison time. _________ I believe that tax breaks for big-business hurt the average American. Below are some current political issues. Please indicate how important each of these issues is to

you. This question is not designed to determine what your position is on the following issues,

rather it is designed to determine how important you think the issues are. Use the scale below in

making your judgments.

Not at all Important

Very Important

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _____ Gun Laws _____ Taxation _____ Health Care _____ Drug War _____ Foreign Policy _____ Homeland Security _____ Illegal Immigration _____ Abortion _____ Gay Marriage _____ Death Penalty _____ Welfare _____ Energy

Page 159: (1) Information and Political Perception

146

Please think about your overall political stand. To what extent would you say that you are conservative versus liberal? (Circle a number on the scale below) Very Conservative

Very

Liberal

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 What is your political affiliation?(Circle one) Democrat Republican Independent No Affiliation How strongly do you identify with your political party? Very Weakly

Very Strongly

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How knowledgeable are you about politics Very Unknowledgeable

Very Knowledgeable

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How interested are you in politics? Very Interested

Not at all Interested

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 160: (1) Information and Political Perception

147

Appendix K:

Study Two Rating of Political Candidates Questionnaire

Steven Johnson

Democrat Below are some statements of possible attitudes for several currently important issues. To what extent do you believe this candidate would agree or disagree with each attitude statement? Use the scale below in making your judgments. Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I support a woman’s right to abortion on demand. _________ It is important to support equal rights for gays and lesbians. _________ Poverty is a large problem in our country and should be addressed by governmental support. _________ There should be no limitations on the right to bear arms. _________ The United States should engage in more diplomatic discussions rather than using the military _________ The death penalty is a deterrent for crime, and will decrease the number of murders. _________ Increased security at the expense of an individual’s rights is unacceptable. _________ Illegal immigrants are a drain on our resources, and every effort should be made to deport them. _________ HMOs ensure that American health care stays competitive with the rest of the world. _________ The government should move away from oil and invest in alternative energy.

Page 161: (1) Information and Political Perception

148

Strongly Disagree

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Strongly Agree

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _________ I support decreasing taxes on large corporations to increase the nation’s economic vitality. _________ I do not mind surrendering some personal freedoms to ensure our country’s safety. _________ Charities not the government should be responsible for caring for the poor. _________ Our country needs to invest in a universal health care plan. _________ The death penalty is morally wrong, and does nothing to decrease crime. _________ I view abortion as the taking of a human life. _________ Drug use is a crime and should be treated as such with stiffer penalties. _________ I believe that immigrant workers should be allowed to become American citizens. _________ Marriage should be reserved for heterosexual couples. _________ Limits on gun sales (including handguns) should be enacted into law. _________ The U.S. should drill almost all of its available oil resources. _________ I support increased spending to support the government’s overseas military operations. _________ Non-violent drug offenders should receive treatment rather than prison time. _________ I believe that tax breaks for big-business hurt the average American.

Page 162: (1) Information and Political Perception

149

For the following questions please circle your answers. How likely would it be for you to vote for this candidate? Very Unlikely

Very

Likely

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Please think about the candidate’s overall political stand. To what extent would you say that he is conservative versus liberal? (circle a number on the scale below) Very Conservative

Very

Liberal

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How strongly affiliated (attached) to his political party? Not at all Affiliated

Neither Similar nor Dissimilar

Very

Affiliated

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 How attractive do you think this candidate is? Very Unattractive

Very

Attractive

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 163: (1) Information and Political Perception

150

How much do you like this candidate? Dislike Very Much

Neither Like nor Dislike

Like

Very Much ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- ---------------

-- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

In general how similar do you think this candidate is to you? Very Dissimilar

Neither Similar nor Dissimilar

Very

Similar

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

What about this candidate would make you want to, or not want to vote for him? Use the

space provided below to answer.

Page 164: (1) Information and Political Perception

151

Using the scale below, rate the candidate on each of the following traits. To what extent would you use each trait to describe this candidate? Circle the number that you feel best describes the candidate.

Extroverted [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Shy

Disagreeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Agreeable

Trustworthy [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Untrustworthy

Self-Doubting [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Self-Confident

Knowledgeable [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Uninformed

Reserved [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Outgoing

Good Natured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Irritable

Irresponsible [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Responsible

Self-Assured [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Insecure

Unintelligent [---1---|---2---|---3---|---4---|---5---|---6---|---7---] Intelligent

Page 165: (1) Information and Political Perception

152

Appendix L:

Study Two Recall Measure Next we are interested in what statements you can recall about the candidate. In the space below please list as many of the 9 statements that you read, one in each blank. Your recall does not need to be verbatim (word-for-word) but please try to be as complete and accurate in listing each of the 9 statements you read about the candidate as possible. 1) ______________________________________________________________ 2) ______________________________________________________________ 3) ______________________________________________________________ 4) ______________________________________________________________ 5) ______________________________________________________________ 6) ______________________________________________________________ 7) ______________________________________________________________ 8) ______________________________________________________________ 9)_______________________________________________________________

Page 166: (1) Information and Political Perception

153

Appendix M:

Study Two Recognition Measure For each of the following statements please indicate whether or not this statement was used in describing the candidate. Say yes only if the statement matches exactly one used in describing the candidate, not just if you think he would agree with it. Finally, in the second space indicate how sure you are about this.

Not at all

Confident Somewhat Confident

Very Confident

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3

Yes/No _______ _______ Wants to do all he can to protect Roe vs. Wade. _______ _______ Has argued that medical marijuana issues should not be decided

between patients and doctors, but by the government. _______ _______ Wants to place restrictions on assault weapons and ammunition

types. _______ _______ Wants to invest money into efficient energy alternatives such as

nuclear, not inefficient ones like solar and wind. _______ _______ Voted yes to a bill requiring the CIA to report on Guantanamo

detainees’ condition and interrogation methods. _______ _______ Voted no on a measure that would allow current illegal aliens the

opportunity to participant in Social Security. _______ _______ Feels that we need a Constitutional amendment defining life from

conception. _______ _______ Believes we should strengthen existing gun restrictions, and

penalize criminal use. _______ _______ Has argued that medical marijuana issues should be decided

between patients and doctors, not the government. _______ _______ Voted yes on a proposition to drill for oil and gas in the Arctic

National Wildlife Refuge.

Page 167: (1) Information and Political Perception

154

Not at all

Confident Somewhat Confident

Very Confident

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 Yes/No _______ _______ Voted yes on a bill to allow warrantless wiretapping of telephone

and electronic communications. _______ _______ Voted yes on a measure that would give foreign “Guest Workers”

the chance to apply for American citizenship. _______ _______ Wants to do all he can to overturn Roe vs. Wade. _______ _______ Voted no on a measure that would subject federal employees to

random drug tests. _______ _______ Wants to invest money into clean energy alternatives such as solar

and wind, not dirty technology like nuclear. _______ _______ Believes we should repeal existing gun restrictions, but still

penalize criminal use. _______ _______ Voted yes on a measure that would allow current illegal aliens the

opportunity to participant in Social Security. _______ _______ Voted no to a bill requiring the CIA to report on Guantanamo

detainees’ condition and interrogation methods. _______ _______ Feels that we need a Constitutional amendment protecting the right

of a woman to have an abortion. _______ _______ Voted yes on a measure that would subject federal employees to

random drug tests. _______ _______ Opposes restrictions on assault weapons and ammunition types. _______ _______ Voted no on a bill to allow warrantless wiretapping of telephone and

electronic communications. _______ _______ Voted no on a measure that would give foreign “Guest Workers” the

chance to apply for American citizenship. _______ _______ Voted no on a proposition to drill for oil and gas in the Arctic

National Wildlife Refuge.

Page 168: (1) Information and Political Perception

155

Appendix N:

Study Two Subtyping Measure Think about your impression of this political candidate. How typical or atypical are his attitudes and beliefs compared to your impression of the attitudes and beliefs of the other members of his political party

Very Typical

Neither Typical nor

Atypical

Very Atypical

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

-----------------

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 169: (1) Information and Political Perception

156

Appendix O:

Study Three 1980 ANES Political Issues Note: The first four measures were used in the 1980 ANES model based on the results of the factor analysis. The name of the variable is in brackets along with whether the variable was reverse scored or not. In the model the variables run from Liberal to Conservative. 1. (Job) Some people feel the government in Washington should see to it that every person has a job and a good standard of living. Others think the government should just let each person get ahead on his own. Where would you place yourself on this scale or haven't you thought much about this?

1. Government see to a job and good standard of living . .

7. Government let each person get ahead on own 2. (Aid) Some people feel that the government in Washington should make every effort to improve the social and economic position of blacks and other minority groups, even if it means giving them preferential treatment. Suppose these people are at one end of the scale at point number 1. Others feel that the government should not make any special effort to help minorities because they should help themselves. Suppose these people are at the other end at point 7. And, of course, some other people have opinions somewhere in between at points 2, 3, 4, Where would you place yourself on this scale or haven't you thought much about this?

1. Government should help minority groups . .

7. Minority groups should help themselves 3. (Serv, Reverse-scored) Some people think the government should provide fewer services, even in areas such as health and education, in order to reduce spending. Other people feel it is important for the government to continue the services it now provides even if it means no reduction in spending. Where would you place yourself on this scale, or haven't you thought much about this?

1. Government should provide many fewer services; reduce spending a lot . .

7. Government should continue to provide services; no reduction in spending 4. (Unemp, Reverse-scored) Some people feel the federal government should take action to reduce the inflation rate, even if it means that unemployment would go up a lot. Others feel the government should take action to reduce the rate of unemployment, even if it means that inflation would go up a lot. Where would you place yourself on this scale, or haven't you thought much about this? 1. Reduce inflation even if unemployment goes up a lot . .

Page 170: (1) Information and Political Perception

157

7. Reduce unemployment even if inflation goes up a lot 5. Some people believe that we should spend much less money for defense. Others feel that defense spending should be greatly increased. Where would you place yourself on this scale, or haven't you thought much about this?

1. Greatly decrease defense spending . .

7. Greatly increase defense spending 6. There has been some discussion about abortion during recent years. Which one of the opinions on this page best agrees with your view? You can just tell me the number of the opinion you choose.

1. By law, abortion should never be permitted. 2. The law should permit abortion only in case of rape, incest or when the woman's life is in danger. 3. The law should permit abortion for reasons other than rape, incest, or danger to the woman's life, but only after the need for the abortion has been clearly established. 4. By law, a woman should always be able to obtain an abortion as a matter of personal choice.

7. Which of these statements best describes what you would like to see happen over the next three years? Over the next three years, federal income taxes 1. Should not be cut 2. Should be cut by 10% 3. Should be cut by 20% 4. Should be cut by 30% 5. Should be cut by more than 30% 0. Inap, coded 5,8 or 9 in q.k19 8. Dk 9. Na 8. Recently there has been a lot of talk about women's rights. Some people feel that women should have an equal role with men in running business, industry, and government. Others feel that women's place is in the home. Where would you place yourself on this scale or haven't you thought much about this?

1. Equal role . .

7. Women's place in home

Page 171: (1) Information and Political Perception

158

Appendix P:

Study Three 1980 ANES Knowledge Measures Note: All measures reverse-scored in the 1980 ANES model.

1. (Info) Some people don't pay much attention to campaigns. How about you? Would you say that you have very much interested, somewhat interested, or not much interested in the political campaign so far this year? 1. Very much interested 3. Somewhat interested 5. Not much interested 2. (ObsInterest) Overall, how great was R's interest in interview? 1. Very high 2. Above average 3. Average 4. Below average 5. Very low 3. (Polls) During the last month, television, newspapers and magazines have reported the results of national polls which measure the popularity of the candidates running for. Have you read or heard any of the results from these national polls taken during the last month? Yes No 4. (Self-Int) Respondent's general level of information about politics and public affairs seemed: 1. Very high 2. Fairly high 3. Average 4. Fairly low 5. Very low

Page 172: (1) Information and Political Perception

159

Appendix Q:

Study Three 1988 ANES Political Issues Note: All measures reverse-scored in the 1988 ANES model. 1. (InterestGov) Some people seem to follow what's going on in government and public affairs most of the time, whether there's an election going on or not. Others aren't that interested. Would you say you follow what's going on in government and public affairs most of the time, some of the time, only now and then, or hardly at all? 1. Most of the time 2. Some of the time 3. Only now and then 4. Hardly at all 2. (InterestPres) How about the presidential campaign? Would you say that you are very much interested, somewhat interested, or not much interested in following the presidential campaign in 1988? 1. Very much interested 3. Somewhat interested 5. Not much interested 3. (InterestPaper) How much attention did you pay to articles in the newspaper about the presidential primary campaigns -- a great deal, quite a bit, some, very little, or none? 1. A great deal 2. Quite a bit 3. Some 4. Very little 5. None 4. (InterestTV) How much attention did you pay to news on TV about the presidential primary campaigns -- a great deal, quite a bit, some, very little, or none? 1. A great deal 2. Quite a bit 3. Some 4. Very little 5. None

Page 173: (1) Information and Political Perception

160

Appendix R:

Study Three 1988 ANES Ideology Measure We hear a lot of talk these days about liberals and conservatives. Think about a ruler for measuring the political views that people might hold, from liberal to conservative. On this ruler, which goes from one to seven, a measurement of one means very liberal political views, and a measurement of seven would be very conservative. Just like a regular ruler, it has points in between, at 2, 3, 4, 5 or 6. Where would you place yourself on this scale, remembering that 1 is very liberal and 7 is very conservative, or haven't you thought much about this? 1. Very liberal . . 7. Very conservative