20. origins 2013 ind diffs concl. and social cognition 1 (4)

Upload: yiren-lu

Post on 04-Jun-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    1/56

    Individual Differences

    Two ways to study individual differences:

    1. Relate earlier-developing psychological attributes (cognitiveabilities, temperament) to later-developing attributes.

    Do individual differences in geometrical form analysis

    predict performance at reading maps?

    Do individual differences in numerical discriminationpredict performance on symbolic math problems?

    2. Relate performance on tests administered earlier in devt. to

    real-world outcomes.

    Do people with higher SAT scores (or IQ scores) end upwith more degrees? higher income?

    Two updates on the first enterprise

    The second enterprise

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    2/56

    Predicting later cognitive abilities from earlier ones:

    Two Updates

    Non-symbolic numerical discrimination and symbolic number."suppose someone found that numerical discrimination in

    infancy predicted later symbolic math performance. Would that

    mean we are innately predistined to success or failure in academic

    math?"

    1. SRCD (Brannon lab):

    numerical discrimination tested at 6 months (Xu displays).

    children retested at regular intervals, from 1-3.5 years.

    num. acuity at 6 mos predicted acuity at all later ages tested.

    num. acuity at 6 mos also predicted mastery of numberwords and counting.

    2. SRCD (Gilmore lab): a real counter-example to the conclusion

    about predestination.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    3/56

    Why do some children do better at discriminating

    numbers of dots than others?

    To test number, must control for continuous variables like dot size.So some dot problems look like this....

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    4/56

    Why do some children do better at discriminating

    numbers of dots than others?

    To test number, must control for continuous variables like dot size.So some dot problems look like this....

    ....and others look like this.

    But for these problems, you have to inhibit responses to size

    differences as you focus on number.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    5/56

    Why do some children do better at discriminating

    numbers of dots than others?

    Some dot discrimination problems demand inhibitory control.

    Inhibitory control requires executive function (Lindsey's lecture).

    Individual differences in EF predict math achievement.

    Gilmore (SRCD), two experiments.

    Exp. 1: Replicates Halberda et al's finding that numerical acuity

    correlates with school achievement. Divides trials into two types:

    number positively vs. negatively correlated with item size.

    pos. correlation, low EF demands neg. correlation, high EF demands

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    6/56

    Why do some children do better at discriminating

    numbers of dots than others?

    Exp. 1 findings:Numerical discrimination predicted school achievement on the high

    EF trials, not on the low EF trials.

    Suggestion: individual differences in EF underlie this correlation.

    pos. correlation, low EF demands neg. correlation, high EF demands

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    7/56

    Why do some children do better at discriminating

    numbers of dots than others?

    Exp. 1 findings:Numerical discrimination predicted school achievement on the high

    EF trials, not on the low EF trials.

    Suggestion: individual differences in EF underlie this correlation.

    Exp. 2:

    Replicated Halberda et al again, this time with a separate test battery

    measuring executive function (like the tests Lindsey described).

    Findings: controlling for EF, numerical discrimination no longer

    predicted school math achievement. controlling for numericaldiscrimination, EF strongly predicted school math achievement.

    (further experiments by Gilmore: non-symbolic addition tests do

    predict school math achievement....)

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    8/56

    Summary

    Both core numerical abilities and executive function predict schoolmath performance.

    Both of these abilities are highly malleable and trainable.

    Individual differences do not imply that fixed differences in cognitive

    ability.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    9/56

    Predicting Real World Outcomes

    Much research in individual differences seeks to predict who will

    perform well at some future activity:

    ex: intuitive personality theories predict success at careers

    SAT-Q tests predict success in math & engineering professions

    IQ tests predict future earnings potential

    Does this predictive power have normative implications: e.g., are

    people who score higher on SAT tests all-around smarter than those

    who score lower?

    Three cases that cast doubt on this conclusion.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    10/56

    Case 1: personality traits and careers

    E.G. Boring

    (chair, Harvard Psych,

    1930s)

    Example: Jewish personality traits and academic potential

    In the 30s, no Jewish faculty at Harvard.Many Jewish students in Boring's lab.

    None got good academic jobs on leaving

    his lab.

    The defects of his race (Winston, 1998).

    A study of his letters of recommendation:

    talkative

    aggressive

    characteristic Jewish eagerness

    gesticulates excitedly

    Boring's error: confusing what was typicalof professors in his field

    with what was necessary for success in that field.

    But NB: these traits werepredictive of failure, because Jews weren't

    hired.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    11/56

    A common claim: women have lower spatial ability.

    The evidence: women perform less well at mental rotation tasks.

    Case 2: Spatial ability and gender

    My puzzlement: we had been studying spatial abilities a lot,

    and we didn't see evidence either for a unitary dimension of

    "spatial ability" or for gender differences on any core spatial

    tasks.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    12/56

    Overall Performance

    (Grace, Shutts, Izard, Dehaene & Spelke, 2006; Izard & Spelke, 2009; replicated by Dillon et al., in review)

    High Performance

    Are boys more sensitive to geometry in visual forms?

    Average Performance High Performance

    No sex difference favoring boys.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    13/56

    (Grace, Shutts, Izard, Dehaene & Spelke, 2006; c.f. Izard & Spelke, 2009)

    girls>boys

    * **

    (*pgirls

    Are boys more sensitive to any aspect of geometry?

    male superiority at mental

    rotation may trace back to

    infancy

    (Quinn & Liben, 2008; Moore & Johnson, 2008)

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    14/56

    (Grace, Shutts, Izard, Dehaene & Spelke, 2006; Izard & Spelke, 2009)

    Boys and girls show highly similar performance profiles

    Highly convergent performance by boys and girls.

    r=.888, p

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    15/56

    So why the belief in sex differences, and the focus on

    mental rotation on standardized tests?

    The goal of the tests is to predict who will succeed.For social reasons, male students are more likely to pursue &

    succeed at academic careers than females:

    discrimination, overt and covert

    a tendency of both genders to go into fields where theirown gender is represented (e.g., few male nurses or midwives...)

    Therefore, mental rotation is a better predictor of academic

    success than are the other measures.

    But this doesNOT mean that it is a better indicator of cognitive

    ability. Voice depth or hair length also will predict academic

    success....

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    16/56

    Case 3: IQ tests and earnings potential

    Herrnstein, Murray: IQ in the meritocracy

    Children with higher IQs tend to become adults with more education

    and money

    So is IQ a good measure of ability?

    Tests of ability: anagrams, reading prose passages

    not free-style rapping

    Why? High-income adults are likely to do cross-word puzzles andread novels. They aren't likely to be freestyle rappers.

    My guess: from the standpoint of cognitive psychology, effective

    rapping requires at least as much vocabulary, verbal fluency, etc etc.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    17/56

    Summary

    Attempts to measure intelligence or specific aspects of cognitive

    aptitude are not worthless or stupid. But it's hard to get them right.

    The best hope for better understanding individual differences in

    cognition comes not from research linking individual cognitive

    variables to successful real-world outcomes (like going to college,

    earning lots of money, or becoming a Harvard professor).

    Instead, it comes from research linking individual differences in core

    cognition to individual differences in constructed cognitive abilities:

    research like the studies discussed in the first half of this topic.

    To date, these studies show no gender or social class differences in

    children's performance. These findings should encourage us to look

    critically at the measures that do, like IQ and SATs.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    18/56

    Social Cognition

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    19/56

    Humans are a highly social species

    We live in communities (throughout human evolution).

    --Community members cooperate to accomplish tasks that no

    single person could perform alone.--Community members share information: most of what we

    know has been learned from others.

    These activities surely account for much of our success as a species.

    Community members share a culture, distinct from other cultures

    (language, food production & preparation, tools & technology, music

    & dance, dress, games, rituals, belief systems). Some of these

    distinctions make obvious sense (food availability & spoilage issues).

    Others are more puzzling:

    Why different languages?

    Why rituals? religions?

    A hope: insights from studies of children.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    20/56

    Learning about the social world

    To navigate the social world, the child must come to:

    1. recognize other specific individuals.

    2. learn about the social behavior of each individual toward the

    child and toward one another (friends or foes? selfish or

    cooperative?)

    3. learn the overall structure of the social landscape (what groups

    are there? how are they organized? what are the dimensions of

    status?). NB: this too varies across cultures.

    4. learn culture-specific beliefs and rituals.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    21/56

    Three topics:

    1. finding good social partners and cooperating with them: today

    2. discovering social groups and learning their norms: Lindsey,

    Thursday

    3. social group preferences and biases: last class

    Learning about the social world

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    22/56

    People as social partners

    Newborns: attend to people from birth (faces, voices).

    2 month olds: smile, engage socially, turn-taking.

    By 7-9 months: attachments, seeking proximity to known

    others.

    First question: How do infants come to distinguish people fromeach other so that they can form social relationships?

    recognizing faces

    detecting states of social engagement

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    23/56

    Face Recognition in Adults

    A special system for identifying faces.

    evidence: inversion

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    24/56

    Identifying People: Adults

    A special system for identifying faces.

    evidence: orientation specificity

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    25/56

    Identifying People: Adults

    A special system for identifying faces.

    evidence: species-specificity

    This is Mary

    Which is Mary?

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    26/56

    Identifying People: Adults

    A special system for identifying faces.

    evidence: species-specificity

    This is Fred

    Which is Fred?

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    27/56

    Identifying People: AdultsA special system for identifying faces.

    evidence: species-specificity

    Human adults: good with human faces, bad with monkey faces.

    Adult monkeys good with monkey faces, bad with human faces.

    High abilities to recognize individuals of ones own species by their

    faces.

    Familiarize:

    Test:

    Pascalis

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    28/56

    Identifying People: Adult BrainsTask: passive viewing of faces, artifact objects, scenes, ....

    Method: subtraction from scrambled (recall Epstein reading)

    Primate brains devote lots of territory to processing information about

    the faces of specific individuals. Kanwisher, Tsao

    Humans Monkeys

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    29/56

    How does this high sensitivity develop?Studies of infants

    Pascalis

    familiarize

    test

    At 9 months, longer looking at the novel face.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    30/56

    test

    Pascalis

    familiarization

    How does this high sensitivity develop?

    Studies of infants

    At 9 months, equal looking at the two faces (like adults under most

    testing conditions).

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    31/56

    How does this species-specificity develop?

    Pascalis

    Obvious hypothesis: infants learn to distinguish human faces.

    Alternative hypothesis: infants learn notto distinguish monkey

    faces.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    32/56

    How does this species-specificity develop?

    Pascalis

    9 months

    Success

    Failure

    6 months

    Success

    Success

    Between 6 & 9 months, a narrowing of the face processing domain.By the time they start learning language, children treat (most)

    animals as members of kinds (dog, monkey) but treat (most) people

    as individuals (Tom, Mary).

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    33/56

    Limits to face perception

    Not all creatures with faces, even human faces, are social partners for

    the infantsome are strangers: need further cues to determine who

    is socially related to them.

    Possible cues come from a person's behavior.

    Chicks use both behavior and visual appearance to

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    34/56

    Chicks use both behavior and visual appearance to

    recognize mom

    (M. Johnson & Horn)

    2 hours

    24 hours

    Chicks prefer objects that look like hens:

    innate template in chicks.Critical features: head and eyes.

    But, in the absence of these features, chicks will imprint to other

    objects if they show animate behavior (movement)

    Do human infants also identify social partners based on their actions?

    Same

    species

    Diff.

    species

    Measure = proximity

    Eye contact

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    35/56

    Eye contact

    (Farroni et al., 2002)

    you are engaged

    with me.

    (Mendelson, 1982)

    newborn infants look longer at

    the face with direct gaze

    so do infant monkeys

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    36/56

    Adult model Infant subject

    CameraCoding

    monitor

    for infant

    response

    Coding monitor for

    adult model

    (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977)

    Like me? Imitation in newborn infants

    imitation

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    37/56

    imitation

    (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977)(Ferrari et al., 2006)

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    38/56

    Why imitation?

    A reflex? Meltzoff argues no, a meaningful social behavior

    --motions are slow and deliberate--motions can happen after a delay (minutes in most studies, days in

    one study)

    --motions happen only in the presence of the person who initially

    performed the action.--motions happen only when the person first looked directly at the

    infant.

    When one person imitates another, she signals to the other that she is

    attending to him and tracking his behavior: a universal social code.

    For infants (and adults), imitation may have social meaning for this

    reason.

    imitation

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    39/56

    imitation

    (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977)

    I am engaged

    with you.

    (Ferrari et al., 2006)

    more on imitation

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    40/56

    more on imitation

    (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977)(Ferrari et al., 2006)

    Infants and monkeys prefer social others who imitate them, just

    as they prefer others who look at them: a sign of social

    engagement.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    41/56

    Summary

    Infants are equipped with multiple ways to identify social others,

    recognize them over time (by their faces and actions), share states of

    attention with them, and reproduce their actions.

    What do we do with these abilities?

    --we learn from others (recall section on agency)

    --we cooperate with others

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    42/56

    Cooperation

    Many human evolutionary biologists, anthopologists and

    psychologists argue that the key to our success as a species stems

    from our ability to cooperate:

    we help one another

    we pursue common goals

    we share information

    by cooperating, we achieve more than any person couldaccomplish alone (hunting big animals, building

    shelters, etc etc.)

    by sharing information, we learn more than any person could

    learn alone.

    Key question: What causes this tendency?

    An approach: When and do we develop a propensity to collaborate

    and help others, and under what conditions do we express it?

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    43/56

    Cooperation

    An old idea: Children are selfish; they have to learn to be

    cooperative through education and/or slow "socialization".

    Thomas Hobbes

    (1588-1679) Jean Piaget,

    egocentrism

    Felix Warneken,

    Harvard psych.

    New findings: Not so:

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    44/56

    Cooperation

    Warneken & Tomasello: Children are naturally predisposed bothto

    competition and to cooperation. With no rewards, instruction,or even clear requests, they are motivated to

    collaborate and cooperate.

    Felix Warneken

    Experiment 1: child (18 months) andexperimenter play a collaborative game.

    Question 1: are young children engaged by

    this?

    Question 2: if the experimenter stopscollaborating, will the children attempt

    to get him to continue?

    Acts to restore a mutual game

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    45/56

    Acts to restore a mutual game

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    46/56

    Collaboration

    18 month old children collaborate with others.

    Collaboration is actively maintained: when the experimenter

    breaks off, child actively works to get him back in the game.

    These kinds of actions appear in the middle of the second year.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    47/56

    Helping

    In a collaboration, the child and adult have a common goal.

    What happens when the adult has a goal that doesnt even

    involve the child?

    Next experiments: adult attempts to accomplish something andis thwarted. Child (14 or 18 months) watches.

    NB: the child isn't involved in the adult's activity.

    the adult doesn't ask for help.the adult doesn't reward the child for helping.

    Six situations: 3 retrieving out of reach objects; 3 others.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    48/56

    Case 1: Experimenter drops a clothespin, child = 14 mos.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    49/56

    Case 2: Exp. fails at stacking books, child = 18 mos.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    50/56

    How much are they helping?

    18 months

    14 months

    14 mos: retrieving

    out of reach objects.

    18 mos: almost all

    situations tested.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    51/56

    Cooperation: Summary and Question

    By 18 months, children help unrelated adults quite extensively,

    without being asked and without rewards.At 14 months, helping is less extensive but does occur.

    At younger ages, no such actions have been observed.

    Why this development?

    Warneken and Tomasello: children are naturally motivated to help

    others. They begin helping as they are capable of it.

    An alternative: from birth, children observe others being helpful to

    them. They learn to do the same by observation and imitation.

    To distinguish, need studies of younger infants. But younger infants

    don't have the needed behavioral capacities.

    A solution: allow young infants to observe acts of helping & test

    their evaluations of the helper. (Hamlin, Bloom & Wynn, 2007)

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    52/56

    Infants' evaluations of helpers and hinderers

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    53/56

    Infants' evaluations of helpers and hinderers

    test: at 6 months, a different experimenter, blind to condition,

    presents the two objects within the infant's reach

    test: at 3 months, the two objects appear side by side and looking

    is measured.

    Infants' evaluations of helpers and hinderers: 6 mos.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    54/56

    p

    control condition: same directions of motion but no animacy

    neutral condition: bystander who neither helps nor hinders.

    At 6 months, more reaching to helpers than hinderers.

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    55/56

    Infants' evaluations of helpers and hinderers: 3 mos.

    At 3 months, longer looking at helpers or bystanders than hinderers.

    Do infants understand helping? Are they naturally

  • 8/13/2019 20. Origins 2013 Ind Diffs Concl. and Social Cognition 1 (4)

    56/56

    Do infants understand helping? Are they naturally

    helpful?

    these questions are open: much more work to do.

    for 3 month old infants, the study of cooperation is about at the

    point of the study of depth perception in Fantz's day.

    But as in Fantz's day, psychologists have the tools to find out.

    Wow, effortful !

    Wow, 3D!

    helping and hindering!

    moving in same or

    opposite directions, or...