332_866(2)

62
Facilitating Student Learning Through Contextualization Dolores Perin February 2011 CCRC Working Paper No. 29 A WORKING PAPER IN THE CCRC ASSESSMENT OF EVIDENCE SERIES Across the first year of a major grant from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, with supplemental funding from Lumina Foundation for Education, CCRC has gathered and synthesized a large body of research evidence regarding strategies that may improve the success of students who attend community college. Working papers in the Assessment of Evidence Series use the research literature to draw conclusions and provide evidence-based recommendations in eight major topic areas: developmental assessment, developmental acceleration, developmental mathematics pedagogy, contextualization of basic skills instruction, online learning, non-academic support, institutional and program structure, and organizational improvement. All the papers in the series are made available on CCRC’s website (http://ccrc.tc.columbia.edu ) as they are released. Address correspondence to: Dolores Perin Professor of Psychology and Education and Senior Research Associate, Community College Research Center Teachers College, Columbia University 525 West 120 th Street, Box 174 New York, NY10027 Email: [email protected] This research was funded by the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation. Larry Mikulecky, Shanna Jaggars, Thomas Bailey, Nikki Edgecombe, and Katherine Hughes provided helpful comments on earlier drafts. Leila Brandt assisted in searching the literature and compiling information for the paper.

Upload: vikaspsy

Post on 04-Oct-2014

71 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: 332_866(2)

Facilitating Student Learning Through Contextualization

Dolores Perin

February 2011

CCRC Working Paper No. 29

A WORKING PAPER IN THE CCRC ASSESSMENT OF EVIDENCE SERIES

Across the first year of a major grant from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, with supplemental funding from Lumina Foundation for Education, CCRC has gathered and synthesized a large body of research evidence regarding strategies that may improve the success of students who attend community college. Working papers in the Assessment of Evidence Series use the research literature to draw conclusions and provide evidence-based recommendations in eight major topic areas: developmental assessment, developmental acceleration, developmental mathematics pedagogy, contextualization of basic skills instruction, online learning, non-academic support, institutional and program structure, and organizational improvement. All the papers in the series are made available on CCRC’s website (http://ccrc.tc.columbia.edu) as they are released.

Address correspondence to: Dolores Perin Professor of Psychology and Education and Senior Research Associate, Community College Research Center Teachers College, Columbia University 525 West 120th Street, Box 174 New York, NY10027 Email: [email protected] This research was funded by the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation. Larry Mikulecky, Shanna Jaggars, Thomas Bailey, Nikki Edgecombe, and Katherine Hughes provided helpful comments on earlier drafts. Leila Brandt assisted in searching the literature and compiling information for the paper.

Page 2: 332_866(2)

Abstract

This paper is a literature review that explores the nature and effectiveness of

contextualization as a way to improve outcomes for academically underprepared college

students. Two forms of contextualization have been studied: “contextualized” and

“integrated” instruction. There is more descriptive work on the contextualization of basic

skills than studies with student outcome data. In addition, many studies with quantitative

evidence on the effectiveness of contextualization have methodological flaws that limit

conclusions. Further, only a small number of studies are with college students. However,

despite these problems, contextualization seems to be a promising direction for

accelerating the progress of academically underprepared college students. The method of

contextualization is grounded in a conceptual framework relating to the transfer of skill

and student motivation; practitioners who use it observe positive results, and the available

quantitative evidence indicates that it has the potential to increase achievement.

Page 3: 332_866(2)

Table of Contents

1. Introduction................................................................................................................... 1 

2. Definitions, Examples, and Extent of Use of Contextualization ............................... 2 2.1 Components of Contextualization............................................................................ 4 2.2 Examples of Contextualization ................................................................................ 5 2.3 Contextualization Beyond Basic Skills.................................................................... 5 2.4 Extent of Contextualization of Basic Skills ............................................................. 7 

3. Two Forms of Contextualization of Basic Skills Instruction .................................... 7 3.1 Commonalities in the Two Forms of Contextualization........................................ 10 

4. Underlying Mechanisms............................................................................................. 10 

5. Evidence on Contextualization .................................................................................. 13 5.1 Evidence on Contextualized Instruction ................................................................ 14 5.2 Evidence on Integrated Instruction ........................................................................ 21 

6. Trends in the Research............................................................................................... 28 

7. Practical Implications................................................................................................. 30 

8. Future Research Directions ....................................................................................... 33 

9. Conclusion ................................................................................................................... 34 

References........................................................................................................................ 36 

Appendix.......................................................................................................................... 49

Page 4: 332_866(2)
Page 5: 332_866(2)

1

1. Introduction

Skills in reading, writing, and mathematics are key to academic learning, but

conventionally, these skills are taught separately from the discipline areas to which they

must be applied. For example, students may be taught writing skills in the morning in an

English course and then be expected to apply them to writing an essay in a history class

in the afternoon. Several problems arise with this structure. First, for reasons still to be

determined (Barnett & Ceci, 2002), students do not necessarily transfer their morning

writing skills to the afternoon history assignment. Second, students may not be motivated

to learn writing skills in the English class because they do not consider such skills to be

relevant to their personal goals (Cavazos, Johnson, & Sparrow, 2010). Third, weaknesses

in essay-writing skills may not be addressed by the afternoon content-area teacher, who

aims to teach subject knowledge rather than basic skills (Fisher & Ivy, 2005).

These problems have serious implications for the academic trajectory of the many

underprepared students who enter postsecondary education. Despite the allocation of

considerable resources to providing developmental education courses that intend to bring

the reading, writing, and math skills of underprepared students to the college level

(Boylan, Bliss, & Bonham, 1997), many students in college-credit courses display

continuing difficulties in applying these foundational skills to the learning of subject

matter (Perin & Charron, 2006). A growing literature, especially in the field of adolescent

literacy, suggests that bringing basic skills and subject-area instruction closer together

may be a solution to this problem (Heller & Greenleaf, 2007; Lee & Spratley, 2010). One

way to create this relationship is through contextualization, or the teaching of basic skills

in the context of disciplinary topic areas.

Postsecondary developmental educators have recommended that pre-college

academic skills instruction be directly related to the content of college-level courses

(Simpson, Hynd, Nist, & Burrell, 1997). Contextualization, which achieves this purpose,

can be seen as a form of “deep learning” that comes about through linking ideas and

concepts across courses (Moltz, 2010). For example, Simpson et al. (1997) suggested that

instruction to improve outcomes for low-skilled college students “would probably use

authentic materials like the textbooks used in college courses such as psychology or

Page 6: 332_866(2)

2

biology” (p. 41). Drawing on research on the transfer of learning, Simpson et al. (1997)

contrasted such “embedded” instruction with the predominant “generic” instruction

(Simpson et al., 1997, p. 42), where technical aspects of literacy or math are taught apart

from content, such as when students are taught to analyze text structure or find main

ideas using short reading passages that have no obvious relation to each other, to content-

area courses, or to students’ goals. In the generic writing approach, language is

fragmented into decontextualized segments such as sentence- and paragraph-writing

skills, with no connection to authentic uses. Accordingly, generic instruction has been

criticized as uninteresting and ineffective (Grubb, 1999).

The purpose of this review is to consider the hypothesis that low-skilled students

can learn more effectively and advance to college-level programs more readily through

contextualization of basic skills instruction. The review begins with an overview of

definitions and uses of contextualization. Then, two major forms of contextualization are

presented, and mechanisms by which it benefits students are suggested. Evidence for the

effectiveness of contextualization is then discussed in order to determine what is known

about possible advantages for low-skilled students. The review ends by discussing

practical implications and future directions for research on the relation between

contextualization and academic outcomes for low-skilled college students.

2. Definitions, Examples, and Extent of Use of Contextualization

As E. Baker, Hope, and Karandjeff (2009) point out, contextualization has been

defined in numerous ways. Here, I follow the definition proposed by Mazzeo, Rab, and

Alssid (2003):

A diverse family of instructional strategies designed to more seamlessly link the learning of foundational skills and academic or occupational content by focusing teaching and learning squarely on concrete applications in a specific context that is of interest to the student. (Mazzeo et al., 2003, pp. 3–4)

Of interest to many students is completing course work that counts toward a

college credential. The content of this course work can serve as a context for teaching

Page 7: 332_866(2)

3

basic academic skills. Thus, the contextualization of basic skills is defined here as an

instructional approach that creates explicit connections between the teaching of reading,

writing, or math on one hand, and instruction in a discipline area on the other, as, for

example, when writing skills are taught with direct reference to topics covered in a

history class.

Many terms have been used to refer to contextualization, including contextual

teaching and learning (E. Baker et al., 2009; Johnson, 2002), contextualized instruction

(Parr, Edwards, & Leising, 2008; Wisely, 2009), content-area literacy (McKenna &

Robinson, 2009), embedded instruction (Simpson et al., 1997), writing-to-learn (Klein,

1999), integrative curriculum (Dowden, 2007), situated cognition (Stone, Alfeld,

Pearson, Lewis, & Jensen, 2006), theme-based instruction (Dirkx & Prenger, 1997),

anchored instruction (Bottge, Rueda, Serlin, Hung, & Jung, 2007), curriculum integration

(Badway & Grubb, 1997), academic-occupation integration (Grubb & Kraskouskas,

1992; Perin, 2001), infused instruction (Badway & Grubb, 1997; Perin, 2001),

developmental education learning communities (Weiss, Visher, & Wathington, 2010),

workplace literacy (Mikulecky & Lloyd, 1997), and functional context education (Sticht,

2005).

Whatever term is used, the work tends to converge on several key themes:

teaching skills with direct reference to real world events and practices (Berns & Erickson,

2001; Carrigan, n.d.; Dirkx & Prenger, 1997; Fuchs & Fuchs, 2001; Goldman &

Hasselbring, 1997; Johnson, 2002; Jurmo, 2004; Karweit, 1998; Orpwood, Schollen,

Marinelli-Henriques, & Assiri, 2010; Sticht, 2005; Stone et al., 2006; Weinbaum &

Rogers, 1995); and instruction in the basic skills needed in content courses (Boroch et al.,

2007; Martino, Norris, & Hoffman, 2001; Perin, Hare, Peverly, & Mason, 2010; Snyder,

2002; Wisely, 2009). In some cases, contextualization occurs through the merging of

basic skills and subject-area instruction (Grubb, 1996; Guthrie, Anderson, Alao, &

Rinehart, 1999; Paquette & Kaufman, 2008). Further, the connection between basic skills

and disciplinary learning is also seen in the newly developed national literacy standards

for career and college readiness, which specify competencies for reading and writing in

history, social studies, and science (National Governors’ Association & Council of Chief

State School Officers, 2010).

Page 8: 332_866(2)

4

Instruction that contextualizes basic skills is often associated with career and

technical education (Association for Career and Technical Education, 2010; Kalchik &

Oertle, 2010), and since 1985 it has been written into the Carl Perkins Vocational

Education Act, which is legislation governing secondary vocational programs (Stone et

al., 2006). Various occupational areas have been used as a context for basic skills

instruction such as marketing courses in high school and college career and technical

education (CTE) (Artis, 2008; Berns & Erickson, 2001), and college automotive and

wind technology certificate programs (E. Baker et al., 2009; California Community

Colleges, 2008). However, contextualization is also found in academic programs in

elementary, secondary, and undergraduate college education (Caverly, Nicholson, &

Radcliffe, 2004; Misulis, 2009; Tilson, Castek, & Goss, 2010).

2.1 Components of Contextualization

In any one program, contextualization of basic skills instruction contains one or

more of the following components: interdisciplinary learning (Berns & Erickson, 2001;

National Council for Workforce Education & Jobs for the Future, 2010), use of students’

informal, out-of-school knowledge (Goldman & Hasselbring, 1997), active, student-

centered learning (Dirkx & Prenger, 1997; Dowden, 2007), student collaboration

(Johnson, 2002), use of explicit literacy strategies (Paquette & Kaufman, 2008), authentic

assessment (Johnson, 2002), and teacher collaboration to identify real world examples

(Orpwood et al., 2010). Professional development may be given (Stone et al., 2006) but

seems rare. Guidelines for contextualization have been provided for workplace and

transition programs (Kalchik & Oertle, 2010; National Council for Workforce Education

& Jobs for the Future, 2010), and instructions for integrating literacy instruction in high

school science courses have also been offered (Krajcik & Sutherland, 2010).

Workplace literacy focuses on a broader range of skill than do other forms of

contextualization; oral language, problem solving, teamwork, research skills, and basic

computer operations are taught in addition to reading, writing, and math (Jurmo, 2004).

Some programs have a vocational English language component where oral language is

taught to English language learners using the content of specific jobs for which they are

preparing (Mazzeo et al., 2003).

Page 9: 332_866(2)

5

2.2 Examples of Contextualization

Contextualization is implemented using many different instructional techniques,

and over the years a fairly large number of program descriptions have accumulated,

although many are not accompanied by student outcome data. The following examples

illustrate the range of approaches. In high school CTE, plumbing content was the basis of

instruction in an English course (Darvin, 2000). Community college allied health and

criminal justice students learned to write documentation relevant to their respective fields

(Perin & Charron, 2006). Also in a community college, allied health students in a

developmental math course learned to solve math problems drawn from curricula in

respiratory therapy, radiology, occupational therapy, medical laboratory, physical therapy,

and nursing courses they were or would be taking (Shore, Shore, & Boggs, 2004). A

learning community linked literature, criminal justice, and business courses (Badway &

Grubb, 1997) while another one linked developmental English and psychology courses

such that writing was taught using material from the psychology course (Cargill &

Kalikoff, 2007). In two studies, developmental education and middle school students

worked collaboratively to create “publishing companies” in order to share their writing or

math products (Goode, 2000; Reilly & Pagnucci, 2007). Students’ informal, out-of-

school knowledge was used to teach middle school pre-algebra by organizing the class

around a project centered on a fictitious pizza company (Brenner, 2002). Developmental

education courses in a learning community utilized content from a service learning

experience, or paired English instruction and college success courses using the theme of

African American culture, literature, and experience (E. Baker et al., 2009). In a final

example, service learning in local organizations was used as an instructional foundation

across the whole curriculum in a high school serving Native Hawaiian students

(Yamauchi, 2003).

2.3 Contextualization Beyond Basic Skills

Our focus is the contextualization of basic reading, writing, and math skills, but it

should be mentioned that contextualization is also used in discipline area instruction

without a basic skills dimension. In this iteration, content area teachers contextualize

instruction by referring to authentic practices related to the topics being taught in order to

Page 10: 332_866(2)

6

deepen domain knowledge (Baldwin, 2003; Biermann & Sarinsky, 1993; Cammarata,

2009; Chaplin & Manske, 2005; Craig, 2006; Englert, 2009; Englert et al., 1995;

Keselman, Kaufman, Kramer, & Patel, 2007; Macaulay, Van Damme, & Walker, 2009;

Nikitina, 2006; Nokes, Dole, & Hacker, 2007; Rivet & Krajcik, 2008; Schultz, 2003;

Wooden, 2008). For example, studies have been conducted to teach high school history

students to think like historians by learning about the social, cultural, and economic

environment in which a primary document was written (Nokes et al., 2007), or to teach

science to community college science students by having them conduct and interpret

experiments employing methods used by professional scientists (Biermann & Sarinsky,

1993).

In a related manner, problem-based learning situates the learning of content in

authentic, everyday life situations (Barron et al., 1998).1 Writing-across-the-curriculum

and writing-to-learn, in which teachers assign writing tasks in order to enhance subject-

area knowledge (Klein & Samuels, 2010), are another type of contextualization. These

approaches are not intended to meet the needs of low-skilled learners in particular, and in

fact, their benefits may be greater for higher-achieving students. For example, to benefit

from writing-to-learn, the writing process must be proficient, which by definition is not

the case with most low-skilled students.2

Contextualization is also used in the teaching of oral language skills to English

language learners where course work and everyday life practices are the simultaneous

focus of instruction (Crandall, 1993; Mak & Coniam, 2008). Contextualized approaches

have also been used within teacher education to prepare pre-service teachers to integrate

literacy into content area instruction (Marri et al., in press; Perin et al., 2009) and to

increase their sensitivity to their students’ real life situations (Darling-Hammond &

Snyder, 2000; Pugach, Longwell-Grice, Ford, & Surma, 2008). Another version of

contextualization is found in “situated learning,” which conceptualizes education as a

1 Problem-based learning generally does not set out to teach basic skills. However, the term problem-based learning was used by Shore et al. (2004) to describe contextualized math instruction described below. 2 Sometimes students are taught writing skills prior to receiving writing assignments in a discipline area. For example, Klein and Samuels (2010) taught middle school students to write persuasive essays using generic materials for four months, and then, when the writing instruction was finished, assigned persuasive essays on a science topic to investigate whether engaging in the writing task itself furthered the content knowledge. Since writing-to-learn does not embed writing instruction in a discipline area, it is not classified here as contextualization of basic skills instruction.

Page 11: 332_866(2)

7

network of social interactions that form the basis of knowledge and skill (Anderson,

Reder, & Simon, 1996).3 Having noted these other uses, we return to the current concern,

the contextualization of basic skills instruction.

2.4 Extent of Contextualization of Basic Skills

The extant literature does not provide information on the frequency of use of

contextualization of basic skills instruction, but it appears that the approach is used more

often in elementary, secondary, and adult literacy education than in college programs. A

study of contextualization in credit-bearing vocational courses in community colleges in

one state found very few examples, and those found were almost exclusively in math

(Wisely, 2009). A broader search for postsecondary contextualization in the form of

academic-integration in community colleges in multiple states found similar low usage

(Perin, 2001). One reason may be cost: Jenkins, Zeidenberg, and Kienzl (2009) reported

that an adult basic education program used in community colleges across the state of

Washington receives 75 percent more funds per student than for students in traditional

basic skills courses.4

3. Two Forms of Contextualization of Basic Skills Instruction

Perusal of the ways in which contextualization is implemented reveals that it

occurs in two distinct forms: contextualized and integrated instruction. This distinction

has not been made explicitly in previous literature, but it is an important contrast because

each form involves different teaching staff and instructional emphases. Contextualized

instruction would be employed by instructors of reading, writing,5 and math, while

integrated instruction would be the province of discipline-area instructors in both

academic and career and technical areas. To maintain consistency with previous

3 The term “situated learning” is used in various ways in the literature. For example, Guthrie et al. (1999) use the term to refer to the integration of reading and content-area instruction. 4 The literature search for this review did not identify any other information about the cost of contextualization of basic skills instruction. 5 Reading and writing includes English language arts, developmental reading, developmental writing, college English, and freshman composition.

Page 12: 332_866(2)

8

literature, the umbrella term “contextualization” is used here to refer collectively to the

two forms of instruction.

Contextualized basic skills instruction involves the teaching of academic skills

against a backdrop of specific subject matter to which such skills need to be applied, such

as philosophy (Snyder, 2002), statistical process control (E. Baker et al., 2009), allied

health (Shore et al., 2004), history (De La Paz, 2005) and earth science (Bulgren,

Marquis, Lenz, Schumaker, & Deshler, 2009). The primary emphasis of contextualized

basic skills instruction is the teaching of reading, writing, or math, and, as mentioned

above, the instruction is delivered by developmental education, English, and math

teachers. The primary instructional objective is to teach academic skills rather than the

subject matter, although there may be some implicit content learning as students are

exposed to subject-area material in the course of practicing basic skills. Workplace

literacy programs (e.g., Mikulecky & Lloyd, 1997) provide contextualized basic skills

instruction. Here, reading, writing, or math is taught in the context of job documents and

tasks. Another example is instruction in a secondary English language arts class on the

procedures for writing a persuasive essay on topics being taught in a concurrent history

class (De La Paz, 2005). The latter model is also used in learning communities that pair

developmental education with, for example, sociology, psychology, business, or student

orientation courses (Weiss et al., 2010).

In contrast, integrated basic skills instruction is the incorporation of reading,

writing, or math instruction into the teaching of content, such as in secondary social

studies (De La Paz & Felton, 2010; Massey & Heafner, 2004; Nokes, 2008), elementary

and secondary science (Barton, Heidema, & Jordan, 2002; Bulgren et al., 2009;

McDermott, 2010; Tilson et al., 2010),6 and college-level courses in marketing (Artis,

2008) or agricultural technology (Parr et al., 2008). For example, using integrated

instruction, a high school science teacher can teach students strategies for comprehending

information depicted in graphics, or how to write an argument showing why evidence

supports one conclusion rather than another on a scientific issue (Krajcik & Sutherland,

6 The integration of reading or writing instruction in secondary disciplinary courses is known as content area instruction.

Page 13: 332_866(2)

9

2010).7 Integration is also seen when a community college career and technical course

instructor teaches students how to write a summary of a business text or when an allied

health instructor teaches students how to write log entries on patient care (Badway &

Grubb, 1997; Perin, 2001). While contextualized instruction is provided by language arts

and literacy teachers, integrated instruction is found in discipline-area classrooms, with

the academic skills serving as a means of developing critical thinking about disciplinary

content (Pearson, Moje, & Greenleaf, 2010).

Integrated instruction may also be needed when a content instructor observes that

many students are having difficulty with the basic skills needed to learn the material,

such as, in one example, when teachers “sneak in” reading comprehension strategies in a

college course on symbolic logic (Higbee, Lundell, & Arendale, 2005, p. 328). Other

types of integrated instruction are the use of “content enhancement routines” in secondary

content instruction (Bulgren et al., 2009; Deshler, 2007; Sencibaugh, 2008), where a

variety of techniques are used to support reading comprehension, including advanced

organizers, charts that visually depict and organize information in text, mnemonic

devices, and peer collaboration.

Integrating basic skills instruction involves providing explicit instruction in

strategies for reading, writing, and math in discipline-area classrooms. Content teachers

routinely assign reading, writing, or math tasks; what is different about integrated basic

skills instruction is that the teacher provides procedural knowledge, i.e. tells the students

how to perform these tasks using modeling techniques (e.g., Vaughn et al., 2009) rather

than merely assigning them. The integration of reading, writing, and math skills is

relatively easy to accomplish in elementary school classrooms since one teacher teaches

all subjects. For instance, the writing skills of fifth and sixth grade students improved

when teachers provided explicit strategy instruction in argumentative writing as part of

social studies and science lessons (Klein & Kirkpatrick, 2010). Complications for

integrating basic skills into content area instruction come in secondary education, where

teachers specialize in subject areas. In this case, content-area teachers need to be

7 However, although content area literacy preparation is a staple of secondary teacher education programs, the integration of instruction in basic skills in content classrooms has not become the norm (Perin et al., 2009).

Page 14: 332_866(2)

10

persuaded of the value of integrated instruction and be provided with professional

development in appropriate techniques.

3.1 Commonalities in the Two Forms of Contextualization

Academic skills and subject-area teachers may collaborate to plan instruction in

both contextualized and integrated instruction (E. Baker et al., 2009; Berns & Erickson,

2001; Jenkins, Zeidenberg, & Kienzl, 2009), and both approaches may be used within

learning communities (Cargill & Kalikoff, 2007). In addition, “hybrid” courses that

combine basic skills and career content in equal measure have also been described

(Badway & Grubb, 1997; Grubb & Kraskouskas, 1992; Perin, 2001; Wisely, 2009). Since

these tend to be taught by content instructors (Perin, 2001), hybrid courses are considered

a form of integrated instruction in the current overview. Both contextualized and

integrated instruction are a departure from traditional basic skills instruction, where

reading, writing, and math are taught in the abstract, with little or no reference to

authentic applications (Johnson, 2002; Jurmo, 2004). Because instruction must be

customized for specific contexts, both approaches can require considerable effort on the

part of instructors. However, given the high incidence of difficulty with basic academic

skills among many college students in the United States (Bailey, Jeong, & Cho, 2010a,

2010b; Grigg, Donahue, & Dion, 2007; Salahu-Din, Persky, & Miller, 2008), it is

important to find instructional methods that can promote improved outcomes. Both forms

of contextualization seem to be a promising direction for this purpose.

4. Underlying Mechanisms

The goal of contextualization is to create conditions for more effective learning,

expressed for example in higher grades and rates of retention in courses, and through

progression to more advanced course work. Whether instruction is contextualized or

integrated, the connection of basic skills instruction to applications and life goals is

consistent with constructivism, which places students’ interests and needs at the center of

education (Dewey, 1966; Dowden, 2007). The theoretical literature suggests that both

Page 15: 332_866(2)

11

cognitive and affective mechanisms underlie the expected improvement in learning

outcomes.

From a cognitive perspective, contextualization is thought to promote transfer of

learning and improve the retention of information (Boroch et al., 2007; Carrigan, n.d.;

Dirkx & Prenger, 1997; Fuchs et al., 2003; Gillespie, n.d.; Karweit, 1998; Stone et al.,

2006; Weinbaum & Rogers, 1995). When information is learned in a context similar to

that in which the skills will actually be needed, the application of learning to the new

context may be more likely. Stone et al. (2006) hypothesized that “The creation of

explicit connections between situations is critical if students are to transfer their

knowledge and skills outside the classroom, whether it is to another context or to an

abstract testing situation” (p. 11). However, knowing when and where one should apply a

previously-learned skill requires metacognitive and self-regulation abilities that low-

skilled students may lack (Bailer, 2006; Fox, 2009; Mayer & Wittrock, 1996; Nash-

Ditzel, 2010). Linking basic skills in developmental education instruction directly to

authentic content-area applications that students will encounter in a disciplinary course

may increase the likelihood of transfer of skill to that particular setting.8 It has been

suggested that by using authentic academic texts as part of academic assistance services,

low-skilled students become more active learners and are then more inclined to use their

skills in college courses (Simpson & Nist, 2002).

Barnett and Ceci (2002) proposed that the extent of transfer of skill will vary

according to the type of skill being targeted, how transfer is measured, the demands

placed on memory of the skill to be transferred, and the distance between learning and

8 Cognitive theory on transfer has a long history of unresolved debates (Anderson et al., 1996; Barnett & Ceci, 2002; Billing, 2007; Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 2000; Detterman & Sternberg, 1993; Greeno, 2009; Mikulecky, 1994; Perkins & Salomon, 1989; Smagorinsky & Smith, 1992; Son & Goldstone, 2009). One problem is the lack of a commonly agreed-upon definition of transfer (Barnett & Ceci, 2002), but a more pressing question is that of “dosage,” i.e., how much contextualization is required to facilitate the transfer of learning. More specifically, the debate has focused on creating flexible learners who will apply knowledge and skill to diverse situations. It has been theorized that over-contextualization limits learners’ flexibility in applying new knowledge and skills (Bransford et al., 2000). The debate has a slightly different focus from that in the current review, which is narrower in its concern with the learning and application of basic literacy and math skills by low-achieving students. From a pragmatic point of view, although too much contextualization may inhibit flexibility in the application of skills, the simple application of basic skills to a subject area would be an improvement over the current situation in which many low-skilled students do not apply the basic skills they learned once in remedial settings to the content classroom. Further, it appears that transfer is difficult to discern even when explicit instruction in transfer is provided (Hendricks, 2001).

Page 16: 332_866(2)

12

transfer. According to this framework, the distance between original learning and

eventual transfer can be measured in terms of the similarity of the two domains, as well

as the physical, temporal, functional, and social contexts, and the modality for expressing

transfer. In the present context, modality is the application of a skill such as verbalizing

how a math problem is solved in an accounting class or writing a summary in a history

class.

In addition to the cognitive mechanism of transfer of learning, the possible

benefits of contextualization may be explained by the affective mechanism of intrinsic

motivation, where a learner is drawn to engage in a task because it is perceived as

interesting, enjoyable, and/or useful (L. Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Becker, McElvany, &

Kortenbruck, 2010; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Academically underprepared college students

may not have high levels of intrinsic motivation to learn basic skills that they should have

learned much earlier in their academic history (Cavazos et al., 2010; Dean & Dagostino,

2007; Gardenhire-Crooks, Collado, Martin, & Castro, 2010). The low motivation may

occur because, having graduated from high school, students may not realize that their

academic skills are not at college standard, so they may resist the need to sit yet again in

classrooms that teach basic skills. Further, underprepared students may not be motivated

to attend class regularly and apply themselves to learning because of a dislike of

appearing incompetent (Dean & Dagostino, 2007) or because of competing job and

family responsibilities (Caverly et al., 2004; Kozeracki, 2005).

Connecting developmental reading, writing, and math instruction directly to the

content courses students must pass in order to earn a postsecondary credential may

improve intrinsic motivation to learn the skills. The assumption is that students will be

more engaged in the learning process if they perceive it to be useful and meaningful

(Berns & Erickson, 2001; Bond, 2004; Boroch et al., 2007; Guthrie et al., 1999; Johnson,

2002; National Council for Workforce Education & Jobs for the Future, 2010; Shore et

al., 2004; Sticht, 2005).9 Similarly, workplace literacy students, who may not generally

9 The hypothesis here is that the level of intrinsic motivation predicts the level of future engagement in course work. However, it is noted that intrinsic motivation to read has not been found to be a statistically significant predictor of future reading ability. Rather, level of intrinsic motivation to read loses its independent predictiveness once prior reading ability is accounted for (Becker et al., 2010). The same may be true for intrinsic motivation as a predictor of students’ engagement in learning, so that motivation may be confounded with prior academic achievement in predicting future course engagement.

Page 17: 332_866(2)

13

see the appeal of basic skills instruction, may be more motivated to learn the skills when

instruction is connected to immediately-useful applications (Ekkens & Winke, 2009;

Jenkins et al., 2009; Sticht, Armstrong, Hickey, & Caylor, 1987; WSBCTC, 2005).

5. Evidence on Contextualization

The literature was searched for evidence on the contextualization of basic skills

instruction,10 positive or negative. Basic skills was defined as ability in reading, writing,

and mathematics. Because there were few studies with college samples, it was decided to

screen in research from elementary and secondary education as well. Studies were

selected if they contextualized reading, writing, or math instruction and used quantitative

measures of student academic outcomes. Twenty-seven studies were found, 17 on

contextualized instruction, nine on integrated instruction, and one on both contextualized

and integrated instruction (Wisely, 2009). These studies are summarized in Table 1

(Appendix), ordered in turn by educational sector (college, secondary, or elementary

education), type of contextualization (contextualized or integrated instruction), skill area

(reading, writing, or math), and alphabetical order by author name. The table and this

paper are both confined to studies reporting student outcomes, although some studies, e.g.

Greenleaf et al. (2010), and Stone et al. (2006), also reported findings on teachers’

practice and perceptions. In Table 1, the design,11 analysis, sample, nature of instruction,

and dependent variables for the student outcomes are summarized for each study.

Quantitative studies of contextualized instruction were conducted with college

academic programs (six studies), adult basic education (six studies), and K-12 academic

education (four studies of each) but no studies were found for this form of

contextualization with college or high school CTE students. Five of the six studies on

contextualized instruction in college involved developmental education (Caverly et al.,

10 The literature search produced many examples of contextualization, but most of the work was descriptive, without supporting data on evidence. For example, E. Baker et al. (2009) described 11 different programs involving contextualization in postsecondary settings but no evidence on their effectiveness was reported. 11 Some studies did not explicitly state whether an experimental or quasi-experimental design was used. In these cases, it is assumed that a quasi-experimental design with a comparison rather than control group was used, with the control group defined as a group created through random assignment.

Page 18: 332_866(2)

14

2004; Perin et al., 2010; Shore et al., 2004; Snyder, 2002; Wisely, 2009) and one

(Martino et al., 2001) focused on low-achieving students in a college-level content

course. Among the six studies with adult basic education students, five were with

workplace literacy programs (Ekkens & Winke, 2009; Lazar, Bean, & Van Horn, 1998;

Mikulecky & Lloyd, 1997; Perin, 1997; Sticht, 1995) and one was with a prison sample

(Dirkx & Crawford, 1993). Three of four studies of K-12 contextualized instruction

focused solely on math (Bottge, 1999; Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Brenner et al., 1997),

and one taught writing (De La Paz, 2005).

Four of the 10 studies on integrated instruction were with CTE programs: two in

college (Cox, Bobrowski, & Spector, 2004; Jenkins et al., 2009) and two in secondary

education (Parr et al., 2008; Stone et al., 2006). The other six studies were in academic

programs in elementary (Guthrie et al., 1999; Tilson et al., 2010) and secondary

education (Bulgren et al., 2009; De La Paz & Felton, 2010; Greenleaf et al., 2010;

Vaughn et al., 2009).

Many of the studies had methodological weaknesses that limited the conclusions

about the effectiveness of contextualization. Sometimes there was no control group,

group assignment was through self-selection, comparison samples were not equated for

pre-intervention skill levels, groups received different amounts of instruction, or there

was possible contamination of treatment and comparison instruction. Some studies

reported outcomes but not inferential statistics, and others used only self-report measures

or researcher-designed measures, the properties of which were not reported. These flaws

are indicated in Table 1. The studies that offer the best evidence12 are summarized in the

following sections.

5.1 Evidence on Contextualized Instruction

College settings. Working with developmental reading and writing students,

Perin et al. (2010) contextualized reading and writing instruction in biology. Students

used a curricular supplement to practice written summarization, question-generation,

vocabulary, and persuasive writing skills, with a strong emphasis on summarization. The

12 Most of the studies of contextualization in college settings have serious limitations. However, all of the studies of contextualization in college identified in the search for this review are included in the following section because we are most concerned with this particular educational sector.

Page 19: 332_866(2)

15

supplement consisted of 10 units, which were completed independently outside of the

classroom weekly over one college semester. The reading and writing practice centered

on a biology text drawn from anatomy and physiology textbooks. To address difficulties

in discriminating between important and less important information in the text (Johns,

1985; Perin, Keselman, & Monopoli, 2003; Selinger, 1995), instructions in the units led

students to the main ideas in the biology text prior to writing a summary.

Students in 12 developmental reading and English classrooms were randomly

assigned to use the contextualized intervention or a generic version of the intervention

that was identical, except that generic text from developmental education textbooks was

used. A third group from a purposive sample of four classrooms served as a business-as-

usual comparison group. Both intervention groups showed statistically significantly

higher gain on several summarization variables (the proportion of main ideas from source

text, accuracy of information, and word count) than the comparison group, and the

contextualized biology group showed greater gain than the comparison group on two

summarization variables (inclusion of main ideas and the accuracy of written summaries),

with effect sizes of 0.33 to 0.62 SD units. However, pre-post gain on a generic,

standardized test of reading was minimal, and neither intervention group showed greater

gain than the comparison group on this measure. The findings for the summarization

measure suggest that systematic practice contextualized in content-specific text helps

students learn to summarize the type of material they need to read in order to learn in

college-credit courses. At the same time, the study is limited by the fact involved

independent practice rather than direct instruction, and students received only a small

amount of feedback, raising the possibility that student-related variables rather than

solely the intervention may explain the results. Also, since randomization occurred within

classrooms, there may have been contamination between conditions.

Caverly et al. (2004)13 investigated the use of a contextualized reading

comprehension strategy with first-semester students in developmental reading classrooms

in a four-year college. Instruction was anchored in chapters from textbooks used in core

curriculum courses that the students would have to pass to complete their degrees. The

13 Two experiments were conducted using the same form of contextualization. Since only the second used a comparison group, only that study is reported here.

Page 20: 332_866(2)

16

instruction focused on a strategy based on the mnemonic “PLAN” (Predict, Locate, Add,

and Note).14 With this strategy, students first predicted what would be in the textbook

chapters, and then examined the title, introduction, subtitles, the use of boldface and

italics, pictures, graphs, and summaries. From the predictions and examination of the text,

the students created a concept map (visual display of the information) and ascertained

how they would approach the reading task. In the next step of the strategy, students

checked items in the concept that they already knew and marked unfamiliar information

with a question mark. Next, student read the text and expanded the concept map using

new information. In the last step of the strategy, they reflected on what they had learned

and estimated how well they thought they could now satisfy the task demands they had

identified before reading. To teach the strategy, the teacher modeled it using think-alouds

and by providing a demonstration of metacognitive skills,15 and the students practiced in

small groups. The groups applied the strategy to both “considerate” and “inconsiderate”

text, defined by how clearly it was written (McKeown, Beck, Sinatra, & Loxterman,

1992).16 Also, to promote transfer of learning, the students were asked to apply the

strategy in other classes and were required to summarize this in writing.

Outcomes for n = 56 students who took the contextualized reading course were

compared to those of a random sample of students (n = 72) who had the same reading

levels at pretest but did not take developmental education. Pre- and post-test levels were

measured by using both a statewide standardized reading test and grades in a subsequent

college-level history course with high reading demands. Statistically significantly higher

scores were found for the treatment group on both measures. This study suggests that the

strategy promoted achievement in college-credit courses, but conclusions are tentative

because the comparison group did not take developmental education, leaving a question

14 Mnemonics are frequently used in literacy instruction for low-skilled students. This device is a type of scaffolding that aims to make processes in reading and writing that are characteristic of proficient reading explicit and easy to remember. 15 Metacognitive skills include self-monitoring to check for whether what is being read is being understood, and repair strategies to improve comprehension when the reader determines that he or she is not understanding (Simpson & Nist, 2002). 16 Many textbooks are poorly written, particularly in terms of conveying information coherently. Interactions have been found between reading ability and text coherence such that the comprehension of low-skilled readers is poorer on minimally coherent texts compared to highly coherent texts. In contrast, highly skilled readers are able to comprehend minimally coherent text by referring to their background knowledge (McNamara, Kintsch, Songer, & Kintsch, 1996).

Page 21: 332_866(2)

17

as to whether the developmental education course in general or the strategy instruction, or

a combination of the two, was responsible for the improved performance. Also, students

who choose to take versus not take developmental education may differ on variables such

as motivation that may explain the group difference.

Similar to Caverly et al. (2004), Shore et al. (2004) contextualized basic skills

instruction in college course content. Community college developmental math students

who were preparing for degrees in various health professions were taught problems based

on topics from allied health (respiratory therapy, radiology, occupational therapy,

medical laboratory, and physical therapy) to nursing curricula. The problems were

developed collaboratively by a group of health and developmental education instructors

who observed each other’s classes. For example, a problem was developed to teach

students to interpret a graph illustrating the relationship between percent of normal

glomerular filtration as measured by creatine clearance, and blood urea nitrogen, to yield

a function needed by a nurse to analyze a patient’s kidney function. Data were collected

for cohorts over a three year period.

Compared to a comparison group made up of sections of a traditional

developmental math course, students receiving the contextualized instruction in the first

two years of the study earned better math scores and were more likely to respond on a

questionnaire that they found the instruction useful. The proportion of contextualized

problems on the math test increased each year over the three year project period, in the

third year increasing to 70%. The contextualization group participating in the third year

did not show an advantage over the comparison group. The researchers attributed this

change to a larger number of seriously underprepared students than in previous years and

to the fact that the contextualized problems were harder than the traditional problems.

Thus while the positive findings for contextualization in the first two years of the study

are encouraging, firm conclusions cannot be drawn, because it was not stated how

classrooms were assigned to conditions nor whether the groups had equivalent math

scores at pretest. Further, the authors referred to pre- and post-tests but neither the

specific amount of gain nor statistics were reported.

Adult basic education. Based on a program evaluation, Mikulecky and Lloyd

(1997) reported outcomes of contextualized instruction for 180 incumbent workers in six

Page 22: 332_866(2)

18

companies who participated in work-related literacy classes. The instruction was

provided in five of the companies for 20 to 60 hours, and for 200 hours in another, which

is equivalent, as the authors pointed out, to six or seven weeks of high school.

Participants’ initial reading levels ranged from high elementary school grades to college

level. The industries in which the instruction was contextualized were the automobile and

other manufacturing, prison, insurance, and hospital industries. For example, hospital

workers and correctional officers were taught the writing skills needed to improve the

quality of written reports and memoranda, and gasket-makers were taught reading skills

using company newsletter articles, procedure manuals, and productivity graphs. Some of

the participants were taught skills to prepare for promotion tests.

Literacy gains were measured using self-reports gathered in pre- and post-

interviews focusing on the workers’ literacy practices, beliefs, and plans, as well as

strategies they used to read a workplace newsletter and performance on a work-related

reading scenario. The researchers created scores from the interview responses, compared

the pre- and post-scores using t-tests, and finally, expressed the amount of gain on a

three-point scale (positive, neutral, and negative gain). Statistically significant gains were

found for the reading scenario, reading strategies, and for literacy beliefs and plans, and

the gains were higher than those for a waiting list comparison group in one of the

participating companies. In particular, increases in skill were found for students in

classrooms in which more than 70 percent of instructional time was spent on reading and

writing activities, and students discussed and received feedback on reading and writing

processes. However, this evidence is tentative since it is based on self-reports, which can

be subjective.

Secondary education. De La Paz (2005) created a learning community of sorts

by pairing instruction in social studies and English language arts for eighth graders. In the

social studies class, students learned the “historical reasoning” strategy to build an

understanding of a topic in the eighth grade social studies curriculum, the history of

westward expansion in the United States. This strategy involved reading and reconciling

four sets of primary and secondary documents on each issue. In the language arts class,

the students received instruction in self-regulated strategy development (SRSD) (Graham,

Harris, & Mason, 2005), which involves setting goals for reading and writing and

Page 23: 332_866(2)

19

monitoring progress toward those goals. In the iteration of SRSD used by De La Paz

(2005), students were taught how to write persuasive (argumentative) essays through a

process of modeling and guided practice. The language arts teacher taught this strategy

based on materials used in the social studies class, consisting of set textbook passages,

primary documents, and secondary sources besides the textbook. Following this strategy,

which was taught over 10 days, steps in writing a persuasive essay were taught using two

SRSD mnemonics: STOP (Suspend judgment; Take a side, Organize ideas; Plan as you

write), and DARE (Develop a topic sentence; Add supporting ideas; Reject an argument

for the other side; End with a conclusion). At the beginning of the intervention, the

students wrote personal diary entries about trying to convince someone of something and

then discussed the outcomes of these situations in small group. Contextualized instruction

in persuasive writing then began, in which the students learned the mnemonics, reviewed

sample argumentative essays on the social studies topics, and discussed the five-

paragraph structure they would be expected to use. The teacher modeled different

planning processes and led students though guided practice using a graphic organizer that

depicted the process of writing a persuasive essay. Students were required to reach the

criterion of planning and composing an essay of at least five paragraphs within one class

period after reading a set of social studies documents.

A comparison group received no instruction in the historical reasoning or

argumentative writing strategy, although they read the same social studies texts. Instead

of the intervention, the comparison group created a journal about native Americans and

other actors in the historical events being studied, involving the recording of factual

information. The intervention group showed greater gain than the comparison group on

measures of essay length, persuasive quality, the number of arguments included in the

essay, and historical accuracy. Interviews with students at the end of the intervention

indicated that both groups showed gaps in understanding some of the material but that

both groups answered more questions correctly after instruction. The comparison of the

two groups’ post-test scores showed moderate to strong effect sizes (d = 0.57 to d =

1.23), suggesting that the contextualized writing instruction was an effective approach.

Further, the effects were seen for learners over a range of ability levels, from students

with learning disabilities to average- and high-achieving learners. However, a post-only

Page 24: 332_866(2)

20

design was used, and although data gathered before the instruction began did not show

statistically significant differences since the comparison group was made up of English

language learners, there may have been unmeasured differences between groups.

Brenner et al. (1997) conducted a contextualized intervention that focused on

math rather than on reading or writing skills. Contextualized instruction in this case

occurred through the use of an everyday life scenario rather than through content-area

material. Over a period of 20 days, seventh and eighth graders in a pre-algebra class in an

urban school were taught problem solution skills, including the manipulation of symbols

in equations. Specifically, students learned to produce and represent functions such as

y = mx + b.

The problems were cast in a hypothetical scenario where a decision had to be

made concerning which of three pizza companies should be selected as a vendor for the

school cafeteria. Lessons included taste tests with data collection on student preferences,

a computer malfunction scenario where students searched for errors in the pizza maker’s

order forms and invoices, a pizza delivery game where students had to determine the

correct destination, formulas related to advertising the pizza, and tables about profit and

loss in the pizza business. Students frequently worked in cooperative groups to discuss

and solve the problems. Three teachers taught two sections each, one contextualized and

one traditional; the classes were randomly assigned among teachers to treatment and

control conditions. The classrooms for each teacher were randomly assigned to

conditions. Several curriculum-based and transfer measures were administered to test

students’ ability to represent and solve word problems. Participants in the intervention

showed greater gains than those in the control group in the representation of problems,

such as depicting word problems in the form of tables and graphs. Both fluent speakers

and English language learners showed this benefit.

Several variables could explain the positive outcome. The intervention and

control conditions differed not only in the use of contextualized materials but in whether

or not cooperative learning was used. Further, because the materials were contextualized,

the treatment focused more on problem representation than on the symbol manipulation

that, according to the researchers, is characteristic of traditional math instruction at this

level. (In fact, the performance of the control group was significantly higher than that of

Page 25: 332_866(2)

21

the intervention group on symbol manipulation.) Future research would be needed to

determine whether contextualization, cooperative learning, the problem representation

approach, or a combination of these strategies is most instrumental in explaining the

effects of the contextualized intervention.

5.2 Evidence on Integrated Instruction

As mentioned earlier, there are fewer studies of integrated instruction than there

are of contextualized instruction, and no studies of integrated instruction were identified

within college developmental or academic programs. In this section, examples of

integrated instruction in college and secondary career technical education (CTE), and K-

12 academic programs are summarized.

CTE: college. Jenkins et al. (2009) studied student outcomes in the Integrated

Basic Education and Skills Training (I-BEST) program, a special initiative that combines

CTE and adult basic education in community colleges throughout the state of

Washington. Students in this program are enrolled in non-credit adult basic education and

simultaneously take a college-credit occupational course that integrates instruction in

occupationally-related reading, writing, and math. Instruction lasts one college quarter, in

accordance with the statewide community college calendar. Although the content and

number of hours of instruction varies across sites, there is a stipulation that both an

occupational and a basic skills instructor must be present in the classroom for at least half

of the total instructional time (it is not reported how this time is distributed across class

sessions).

Two-year outcomes were compared between a cohort of 900 I-BEST students and

two other samples of adult basic education students: one group who did and another

group who did not enroll in a traditional, college level CTE course at the same time as the

I-BEST students. The comparisons controlled for age, gender, intent (vocational or

academic), enrollment full or part time, when first enrolled, and educational history. Net

of controls, I-BEST students were more likely than the traditional group to take

subsequent credit-bearing courses, earn credits toward a certificate or degree, persist to

the next college year, and to show gain in basic skills. I-BEST students’ basic skills

improvement was 18 percentage points higher than adult basic education students who

Page 26: 332_866(2)

22

did not enroll in a traditional occupational course and 9 percentage points higher than

adult basic education students who took an occupational course. Thus, the major

advantage of I-BEST was seen when the comparison group took only adult basic

education but not an occupational course. These results provide encouraging evidence for

integrated instruction, but conclusions remain tentative as the sample was self-selected,

presenting the possibility of a confound with student motivation. As the authors noted, I-

BEST correlated with, but did not necessarily cause, the positive outcomes.

CTE: secondary education. Stone et al. (2006) investigated the effects of

integrating math instruction into five CTE areas—agriculture, auto technology,

business/marketing, health, and information technology—using a “Math-in-CTE” model.

The purpose of the instruction was to broaden students’ knowledge of math concepts they

learned in CTE and have students “recognize how to solve practical problems by using

mathematics in their occupational area; recognize math occurring in other contexts; and

do so without diminishing the acquisition of technical knowledge in the course” (p. 5).

However, it was not explained why technical knowledge might diminish by a broadened

approach to math instruction. Math instruction assumed prior knowledge of algebra.

Initially, highly contextualized math problems were taught, along with the inclusion of

more abstract examples. For instance, when students used a T-square during instruction

in agricultural mechanics, the teacher presented the Pythagorean theorem by showing the

formula a2 + b2 = c2. However, ultimately, the goal was that “students would see the

math as an essential component of the CTE content, a tool—like a saw, wrench, or

thermometer—needed to successfully solve workplace problems” (Stone et al., 2006, p.

6).

Teachers of 57 classrooms in 12 states were recruited on a volunteer basis and

randomly assigned to conditions (57 experimental and 74 control). The CTE teachers in

the experimental condition collaborated with math teachers to identify math problems

embedded in the existing CTE curricula and to create lessons highlighting mathematical

operations. The math-enhanced CTE lessons constituted 10 percent of instructional time

over one academic year. The math lessons contained seven elements: introduce the CTE

lesson; assess math skills relating to the CTE lesson; work through a math problem

embedded in the CTE lesson; work through related, contextualized examples; work

Page 27: 332_866(2)

23

through traditional math examples; have students demonstrate their understanding;

include math questions in formal assessment at the end of the CTE unit or course (Stone

et al., 2006, Figure 1, p. 12).

Pre- and post-tests on two standardized math tests, the TerraNova and

Accuplacer, showed significantly greater gain for the experimental group (effect sizes

0.42 and 0.55). When occupational tests used in each participating classroom were

administered at post test, no significant differences were found between the experimental

and control groups. The authors interpreted this to mean that the math instruction was not

detrimental to a growth of knowledge in the CTE field, but since the math enhancement

was presumably in the interest of an increase in occupational knowledge, the findings can

also be interpreted to mean that the math enhancement did not advance CTE

performance. Again, as mentioned above, it was not clear why there might be any chance

of the math instruction lowering students’ occupational knowledge; given the theory that

math should enhance occupational performance, one would expect the opposite. It seems

more logical to conclude that the intervention was not effective in improving job-related

knowledge.

Following on the work of Stone et al. (2006), Parr et al. (2008) randomly assigned

38 teachers of an agricultural power and technology course in high schools across the

state of Oklahoma to teach a math-enhanced or traditional CTE curriculum. Teachers in

the experimental group worked with math teachers to insert course-related math

instruction into nine of the CTE lessons. There was no statistically significant difference

between the students in the experimental (206 students) and control conditions (241

students) in math aptitude prior to the intervention. After the intervention period, students

were given a standardized test on course content (NOCTI Agriculture Mechanics

examination). At the end of the intervention, both groups had similar scores. As in the

Stone et al. (2006) study, this result was interpreted to mean that enhancing the

curriculum with math did not reduce students’ knowledge of the agricultural content.

However, it can also be interpreted to mean that the math enhancement did not promote

students’ content knowledge.

Academic programs: K-12. Building on De La Paz’s (2005) eighth grade study

of contextualized instruction described earlier, De La Paz and Felton (2010) investigated

Page 28: 332_866(2)

24

the effects of an intervention that taught both historical reasoning and persuasive writing

in an 11th grade 20th century history course. Whereas in the earlier study the writing skills

were taught by language arts teachers, in the De La Paz and Felton study, history teachers

provided this instruction.

Participants were n = 79 students in experimental classrooms and n = 81 students

in comparison classrooms in two schools. In the experimental (integrated instruction)

condition, which lasted several weeks, the history teachers introduced and modeled steps

in the writing of persuasive essays on historical topics and then proceeded to teach the

content using the historical reasoning strategy used in the earlier study. Toward the end

of a two-week instructional period, the students were given guided practice in the writing

of two persuasive essays on the topics taught. The STOP and DARE mnemonics from De

La Paz’s 2005 study, described above, were used to help students plan their essays. The

writing instruction and guided practice focused on specific elements of persuasive

writing: writing a topic sentence that stated a position on a historical controversy,

providing reasons for their position, using evidence to support claims, presenting a

counterargument with evidence, and refuting the opposing point of view, presenting new

evidence. Participants had low to average writing skills, and none received special

education or English language services. A persuasive writing task was administered pre-

and post-test to measure the effects of instruction. Students in the comparison classrooms

received traditional instruction, without the historical reasoning or writing strategy

instruction. They were assigned to write the same two essays as in the experimental

condition during the course of instruction, without the support of writing instruction.

Pre- and post-test persuasive essays were analyzed for length, persuasive quality,

and historical accuracy. At post-test, the essays written by the experimental group were

longer (effect size d = 0.66), approximately one third more likely to include elaborated

claims, three times more likely to include elaborated rebuttals than the comparison group

when controlling for essay length, and cited historical documents in support of claims

more often (effect size 1.42 SD units). The finding that rebuttals of historical claims

became more elaborated among the group receiving integrated instruction is notable, as

this aspect of persuasive writing is particularly challenging (Ferretti, Lewis, & Andrews-

Weckerly, 2009). These results support the practice of integrated instruction, although it

Page 29: 332_866(2)

25

is not possible to determine from this study whether the positive outcome is attributable

to the writing or history strategy, or both in combination.

A well-controlled study of integrated instruction was conducted by Vaughn et al.

(2009) with low-income seventh grade social studies students, approximately one third of

whom spoke Spanish as a native language and who were not proficient speakers of

English.17 Assignment to condition was unusually rigorous; first, students were randomly

assigned to classrooms, and then classrooms were randomly assigned to an intervention

or to a business-as-usual control condition. The social studies material was identical in

both conditions. The intervention involved explicit reading comprehension and

vocabulary instruction; the control condition did not receive any literacy instruction but

only focused on the social studies content. The integrated instruction was delivered for 50

minutes per day, five days per week for nine to 12 weeks. Four new vocabulary words

were taught per day. All vocabulary was drawn directly from the social studies text.

To teach vocabulary, after giving an overview of a “big idea” relating to the

historical topic, the teacher pronounced each vocabulary word, identified a Spanish

cognate or translated the word into Spanish, provided a definition in everyday language,

showed a visual representation of the word, and put each word into two sentences, one in

historical context from the class reading and the other relating to students’ everyday life

experience. The students then discussed each word in pairs. A 2- to 4-minute video clip

on the topic was then shown and discussed. Then a graphic organizer was used to support

silent and oral reading comprehension, and students worked in pairs to read the text and

answer questions. In the paired reading, one student read while the other followed along,

with the first student interrupting to correct the reader as needed. The teacher then led a

whole-class discussion of the answers to the questions, and, as a writing activity, worked

with students to summarize information on the topic using the graphic organizer.

On researcher-developed measures of vocabulary matching and reading

comprehension, the experimental group showed greater gain than the control group, with

effect sizes of g = 1.12 for reading comprehension and g = 0.53 for vocabulary.

Importantly, the integrated instruction was equally effective with proficient speakers of

17 Twenty-five percent of the Spanish speakers were designated as English language learners (ELLs) and the others had completed ELL instruction but were still considered Limited English Proficient (LEP).

Page 30: 332_866(2)

26

English and ELL/LEP students. The study only evaluated literacy outcomes; the effect of

the integrated instruction on content knowledge was not measured. However, what is

impressive about this study and the one conducted by De La Paz and Felton (2010) is that

content-area instructors, who generally shy away from teaching literacy skills (Fisher &

Ivy, 2005), not only taught reading and writing skills as part of their disciplinary

instruction, but also obtained positive results.

Bulgren et al. (2009) conducted a short-term “content-enhancement routine”

intervention (two 89-minute sessions taught five days apart) with 36 typically developing

and learning disabled (LD) students in grades 9–12 in an inner-city school. While the

teachers in Vaughn et al.’s (2009) study were social studies teachers, in Bulgren et al.’s

(2009) intervention, the instructors were researchers rather than classroom teachers. In

this study, students were randomly assigned to intervention and control groups, using

stratification to ensure equal representation of LD and non-LD students. The intervention

group learned a strategy for taking notes and learning vocabulary based on a 30-minute

film on ozone depletion (Vaughn et al., 2009 also used a film during instruction) in order

to prepare to write an essay on climate change.

The note-taking process was taught using a “question exploration guide,” which is

an organizational structure for recording important information in the film. Sections of

the guide listed several questions that student had to answer, including: “what is the

critical question?”, “what are they key terms and explanations?”, “what are the

supporting questions and answers?”, and “what is the main idea?”. Other questions

related to experiments that could be conducted and to applications of knowledge about

the ozone depletion issue to students’ individual lives. The control group viewed the film

twice and was asked to take notes, with no instruction.

Outcomes were measured using an essay on ozone depletion. Using two separate

measures, the essays were scored for the quality of the writing, and, unlike the study by

Vaughn et al. (2009) above, content knowledge was also assessed. Writing quality

referred to the ideas expressed in the essay, organization, voice, word choice, sentence

construction, and the use of written English conventions. The content score measured

identification of the problem, cause, effect, solution, and a conclusion on the issue. At

post-test, the writing quality scores of the experimental group were 25 percent better than

Page 31: 332_866(2)

27

those of the control group (d = 1.32). Superior gains for the treatment group were seen for

every writing quality variable except writing conventions. The intervention group also

showed greater gain than the control group on content knowledge (d = 0.74). However,

when the scores for the learning disabled and typically developing students were

disaggregated, only the typically developing students showed greater gain than the

control group (d = 2.0). The results of this integrated instruction approach are

encouraging, but conclusions are limited by the fact that the activity in the control

condition seems considerably less compelling. Other methodological limitations are that

instruction was delivered by researchers rather than by classroom teachers and that the

intervention was very short, only lasting two sessions.

Similar to Bulgren et al. (2009), Tilson et al. (2010) taught an experimental

science unit that integrated literacy instruction. While Bulgren et al.’s (2009) study was a

small-scale experiment in secondary education, Tilson et al.’s (2010) participants were

fourth graders in 94 classrooms in 48 elementary schools. Students were randomly

assigned to experimental (n = 217) or control (n = 241) classrooms. The science unit

focused on physical science (light and energy) and was taught in forty 60-minute

sessions. Forty-percent of instructional time was spent on science, 40 percent on literacy

(reading, writing, speaking, and listening),18 and the remaining 20% of the time on

formative assessment.

Several types of science-related writing were embedded in the science instruction,

including the recording of data, written responses to informational text, and reports on

what students learned in group discussions. Instruction was provided on constructing

topic sentences, providing supporting evidence, and using scientific vocabulary in a

precise way. The teacher modeled the entire writing process at the beginning of a unit.

Also, the students were taught to use graphic organizers and worked in pairs to plan

writing tasks. As an example of the integrated instruction, one of the lessons involved

testing various materials to investigate the phenomenon of reflection. Students created a

data table and read a text on the topic, after which they wrote explanations to show their

understanding of the nature of reflection.

18 Literacy is often defined in terms of these four functions (National Governors’ Association & Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010).

Page 32: 332_866(2)

28

In the control condition, students used the same text, experiential activities, and

were assigned reading and writing tasks but did not receive any explicit literacy

instruction. All students were tested pre and post on writing skills using an experimenter-

designed instrument. The quality of students’ writing was scored on the accuracy of the

science content, the use of evidence, the quality of the introduction and conclusion,

clarity of expression, vocabulary usage, and vocabulary count, defined as how many of

32 science terms targeted during instruction were included in the writing sample. The

treatment group showed statistically significantly greater gain from pre to post than the

control group on all of the writing measures except vocabulary usage and quality of

conclusion (effect size on a composite score of all of the writing dimensions d = 0.69).

6. Trends in the Research

The studies identified in this review provide preliminary support for the

hypothesis that low-skilled students can learn more effectively and advance to college-

level programs more readily through the contextualization of basic skills instruction.

Conclusions are very tentative at present because of the shortage of rigorous studies with

academically-underprepared students in college or adult basic education programs.

Research with K-12 samples was included in the review since there was relatively little

information on the use of contextualization in adult basic education or college settings,

but there does not seem to be any reason why findings from elementary and secondary

education cannot be extrapolated to older adolescent and adult learners.

As shown in Table 1 (Appendix), 27 studies reporting quantitative evidence on

the contextualization of basic skills were found. Outcome measures for almost all of the

studies focused exclusively on, and found gains for, specific basic skills outcomes, i.e.

reading, writing, or math scores. All of the outcomes of contextualization for basic skills

achievement were positive, although there was minor variation in outcomes for subskills

and different measures. For example, in a college CTE study integrating writing

instruction in a business course, students improved their ability to write a business

abstract but not to express business concepts in their own words. In another study of

integrated instruction, Tilson et al. (2010) found that students receiving writing

Page 33: 332_866(2)

29

instruction performed better than a control group on the use of science content and

evidence, the quality of the introduction, clarity and vocabulary count in an essay, but the

control group performed better on the quality of the conclusion to the essay as well as

vocabulary use. Workplace literacy and developmental education students receiving

contextualized instruction in studies by Ekkens and Winke (2009), Perin (1997), and

Perin et al. (2010) showed gain on researcher-developed but not standardized measures.

However, despite these differences, there is a very strong trend in the research toward

positive findings for both contextualized and integrated instruction.

It should also be noted that most of the studies compared contextualization to a

business-as-usual comparison group, indicating that contextualization is more effective

than standard, non-contextualized practice. This is a good start in examining the potential

of contextualization, but more definitive conclusions can only be made when

contextualization is compared to another intervention in addition to conventional

instruction so that results can be attributed to contextualization itself and not to other

dimensions of the research such as novelty or the added attention that may be given to

participants in a “treatment.”

One of the assumptions underlying integrated instruction is that when basic skills

instruction is incorporated in disciplinary instruction, ability in both academic skills and

content knowledge should increase. However, in five studies of integrated instruction that

measured outcomes on knowledge development in a content area (Bulgren et al., 2009;

De La Paz & Felton, 2010; Parr et al., 2008; Stone et al., 2006; Tilson et al., 2010), two

found no improvement in content knowledge (Parr et al., 2008; Stone et al., 2006). Both

of these studies embedded math in occupational courses in high school CTE. Since strong

claims are made for the advantages of combining literacy with subject area instruction,

these mixed findings are disappointing and warrant further research.

Only two studies, Wisely (2009) and Jenkins et al. (2009), provided data on the

second part of the hypothesis under consideration in this review, i.e. on college

advancement. Wisely (2009) found that participation in contextualization was associated

with the completion of developmental education courses, and the speed of entry into, and

performance and completion of, college level courses. However, these positive effects

were limited to non-white students; no effects for contextualization were found for white

Page 34: 332_866(2)

30

students. Jenkins et al. (2009) found that adult education students who attended

occupational classes that integrated basic skills instruction were more likely than adult

education students who either did or did not enroll in a traditional occupational course to

take subsequent credit-bearing courses, earn credits toward a college credential, persist to

the next college year, as well as show greater gain in basic skills. Given practitioners’

enthusiasm about the value of contextualization (see program descriptions in E. Baker et

al., 2009; Boroch et al., 2007; California Community Colleges, 2008), it is unfortunate

that more evidence is not available.

7. Practical Implications

The presence of large numbers of low-skilled students in colleges, especially

community colleges, along with low rates of retention and progress in course work

(Bailey, Jeong, & Cho, 2010a, 2010b) and recent findings that traditionally low

graduation rates are not increasing (Radford, Berkner, Wheeless, & Shepherd, 2010),

suggests that the method of instruction of academically underprepared college students

needs to be reformed. Among the many different innovations underway that attempt to

promote the learning of low-skilled college students (Perin & Charron, 2006),

contextualization seems to have the strongest theoretical base and perhaps the strongest

empirical support. Both forms of contextualization, i.e. contextualized and integrated

instruction, are supported by quantitative studies that include control or comparison

groups. There are more studies on contextualized instruction than there is on integrated

instruction, but both forms of contextualization appear potentially valuable.

Moving toward contextualization in general and toward contextualized or

integrated instruction in particular will depend on practical conditions internal to the

colleges. Most important among these conditions are instructors’ willingness to modify

their instruction and colleges’ ability to provide incentives and support for this change.

Many developmental education instructors are not highly aware of the day-to-day reading

and writing requirements that students find so difficult in college credit disciplinary

courses. Further, they are strongly committed to the generic, decontextualized instruction

in reading, writing, and math that predominates in developmental education (Grubb,

Page 35: 332_866(2)

31

1999). On the other hand, disciplinary instructors may be equally unwilling to consider

contextualization because they feel that basic skills instruction is beyond their range of

responsibility and/or competence (Marri et al., in press; McDermott, 2010). Strong

college leaders will need to provide ongoing direction and support for either version of

contextualization.

The following recommendations may support the implementation of

contextualization for low-achieving students in a college setting:

1. Create conditions for interdisciplinary collaboration so that basic skills and content area instructors can familiarize each other with their curricula, assessment approaches, standards, and teaching techniques (E. Baker et al., 2009; Greenleaf et al., 2010; Kalchik & Oertle, 2010; Perin, 2005; Shore et al., 2004; Stone et al., 2006). It is important that instructors visit each others’ classrooms, discuss their educational philosophy and instructional techniques, jointly analyze the literacy and math demands of content instruction, look for intersects between their instructional topics, and collaborate to align curricula so that students can be taught reading, writing, or math skills that are directly applicable to the subject areas they are learning. Substantial time is required for this effort.

2. Provide ongoing professional development, led by trainers who have experience in contextualization, to initiate and support contextualization. Professional development leaders should be experts from within the institution rather than outsiders (Kozeracki, 2005). Formal professional development should be conducted with interdisciplinary groups of instructors and should be designed to meet tangible targets for implementing contextualized or integrated courses. Evidence-based professional development methods should be utilized, such as interdisciplinary inquiry-based approaches that involve coaching and intensive institutes (Greenleaf et al., 2010). Further, professional development should be guided by common cross-discipline agreement on desired learning outcomes for contextualization and means of achieving them (E. Baker et al., 2009). Follow-up activities and supportive monitoring should be provided after the conclusion of formal training sessions to maintain instructors’ interest in and ability

Page 36: 332_866(2)

32

to contextualize or integrate basic skills instruction. Greenleaf et al. (2010) noted that “A long history of research in reading has demonstrated that reading comprehension strategies are not often taught in subject-area classes, even when teachers are trained to use these strategies during subject-area teaching.” (p. 15). To avoid this situation, follow-up coaching and support of respected instructional leaders will be needed.

3. Develop assessment procedures that incorporate both basic skills and content area knowledge to evaluate the effects of contextualization. For example, in Shore et al.’s (2004) study, developmental math and allied health instructors collaborated to create allied health math problems. Both De La Paz and Felton (2010) and Perin et al. (2010) included measures of content accuracy in instruments to measure contextualized writing, and Guthrie et al. (1999) developed fine-grained assessment methods that simultaneously measured reading comprehension strategies and science knowledge. It appears that such measures will need to be locally developed, because disciplinary curricula tend to change, and conventional standardized tests do not capture students’ progress in contextualized basic skills (Greenleaf et al., 2010), although customized subject-specific basic skills tests can be developed and normed (Lazar et al., 1998).

4. As the basis of contextualization of basic skills instruction in community colleges, select discipline-area courses that are needed for graduation by large numbers of students but that also have high failure rates. Because contextualization is a labor-intensive initiative, it will be necessary to select courses for implementation. Initial attempts should focus on courses that have the highest need, represented by failure rates. Anecdotal evidence suggests that introductory science courses such as anatomy and physiology that are required for graduation by popular majors such as allied health may be a useful place to start, since these courses display high failure rates, and descriptive and quantitative studies are available on the contextualization of basic skills instruction in science content (Bulgren et al., 2009; Guthrie et al., 1999; McDermott, 2010; Perin et al., 2010; Shore et al., 2004).

Page 37: 332_866(2)

33

5. When contextualized courses are established, collect outcome data for examination by instructors and administrators alike. For example, the use of evidence to guide instructional practice in community colleges is a central reform strategy of Lumina Foundation’s Achieving the Dream initiative (Achieving the Dream, 2005). Instructors who implement contextualization and administrators who support this effort should be made aware of both short- and longer-term outcomes, such as the rate of passing basic skills and disciplinary courses, grade point average, semester-to-semester retention, and degree or certificate attainment. Evaluating contextualization in this way will indicate whether the effort is worthwhile, and may point to the need to modify teaching techniques.

8. Future Research Directions

The lack of rigorous research suggests that it is premature to invest substantial

funds in a contextualization intervention at this time. However, practitioners have been

enthusiastic about contextualization for many years, trends in the available research have

been positive, and it is consistent with contemporary theories of learning and motivation.

For these reasons, it would be worthwhile to mount a rigorous research and development

effort to gather information about the potential efficacy of this approach, specifically with

low-skilled adult learners, whether in community college degree and certificate programs,

or in adult basic education programs.

A major premise underlying the practice of contextualization of basic skills is that

students are more likely to transfer the skills to subject-area learning when the instruction

is connected to these subject areas rather than taught abstractly. A topic that has not been

addressed in studying the effects of contextualization on transfer of learning is possible

interactions between student ability, student motivation, type of skill to be learned, and

amount of contextualization. Thus, in either research and development, or basic research

investigations, moderators of the possible effects of contextualization should be

identified. Experiments investigating contextualization as an instructional intervention

should include comparisons with performance on alternate interventions as well as with

Page 38: 332_866(2)

34

business-as-usual comparison groups to ensure that effects of contextualization are not

attributable simply to novelty or to increased attention.

Anecdotal evidence from practitioners (E. Baker et al., 2009; Boroch et al., 2007;

Johnson, 2002) suggests that lower-skilled students benefit from contextualization, not

because it helps them become flexible learners but because it increases their mastery of

basic skills as well as increases the likelihood of transfer of basic skills to content courses

that is not occurring in traditional, decontextualized learning environments. There is very

little research on the relation between the contextualization of basic skills instruction and

subsequent course work, and among the little information that exists, it is not possible to

attribute the gains exclusively to contextualization. Future research paradigms should

control for variables such as the nature of the course, teacher expertise, and cognitive and

affective characteristics of learners.

The issue of dosage of contextualization should also be studied, in light of claims

that instruction can be overcontextualized and as such can be counterproductive (e.g., see

Bransford et al., 2000). Another area that needs attention is the nature of the dependent

variable used in studies of contextualization. The studies in this review varied on whether

they measured both basic skills and subject-area gains, or just the former. Dependent

variables in future research on contextualization of basic skills should include both basic

skills and subject matter learning, since the intent of the intervention is to bring the two

areas closer together and increase learning in both.

9. Conclusion

The contextualization of basic skills in disciplinary content is used in elementary,

secondary, and postsecondary education as a way to engage students, deepen content

learning, and promote transfer of skill. The approach is well grounded in psychological

theories of transfer (although there is debate in this area on dosage) and motivation.

There is support in the literature for the two forms of contextualization identified in this

review, contextualized instruction, which is taught by developmental education

instructors and English and English language arts teachers, and integrated instruction,

which is provided by discipline area instructors.

Page 39: 332_866(2)

35

A greater amount of literature is descriptive than evaluative, but the 27 studies

found in this review that reported evidence suggest that contextualization has the

potential to promote short-term academic achievement and longer-term college

advancement of low-skilled students. However, the studies also indicate that considerable

effort is needed to implement contextualization because instructors need to learn from

each other and collaborate across disciplines, a practice that is not common in college

settings. Further, there is very little information on cost or what would be needed to scale

up contextualization. However, the available evidence, taken in combination with

practitioners’ considerable enthusiasm for contextualization, suggests that this approach

would be a useful step toward improving the outcomes of academically underprepared

college students.

Page 40: 332_866(2)

36

References

Achieving the Dream. (2005). About Achieving the Dream: Overview. Retrieved from http://www.achievingthedream.org/aboutatd/default.tp

Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Situated learning and education. Educational Researcher, 25(4), 5–11.

Artis, A. B. (2008). Improving marketing students’ reading comprehension with the SQ3R method. Journal of Marketing Education, 30(2), 130–137.

Association for Career and Technical Education. (2010). What is “career ready”? Alexandria, VA: Author. Retrieved from http://www.acteonline.org/uploadedFiles/Publications_and_Online_Media/files/Career_Readiness_Paper.pdf

Badway, N., & Grubb, W. N. (1997). A sourcebook for reshaping the community college: Curriculum integration and the multiple domians of career preparation (Vols. 1–2). Berkeley, CA: National Center for Research in Vocational Education.

Bailer, D. L. (2006). A multivariate analysis of the relationship between age, self-regulated learning, and academic performance among community college developmental education students (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. 3201703)

Bailey, T., Jeong, D. W., & Cho, S.-W. (2010a). Referral, enrollment, and completion in developmental education sequences in community colleges. Economics of Education Review, 29, 255–270.

Bailey, T., Jeong, D. W., & Cho, S.-W. (2010b). Student progression through developmental sequences in community colleges (CCRC Brief No. 45). New York, NY: Columbia University, Teachers College, Community College Research Center. Retrieved from http://ccrc.tc.columbia.edu/Publication.asp?uid=812

Baker, E. D., Hope, L., & Karandjeff, K. (2009). Contextualized teaching and learning: A faculty primer. Sacramento, CA: The Research and Planning Group for California Community Colleges, Center for Student Success. Retrieved from http://www.careerladdersproject.org/docs/CTL.pdf

Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children’s motivation for reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(4), 452–477.

Baldwin, M. J. (2003). A literature-based, one-quarter inorganic chemistry laboratory course. Journal of Chemical Education, 80(3), 307–310. doi: 10.1021/ed080p307

Page 41: 332_866(2)

37

Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637.doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.612

Barron, B. J. S., Schwartz, D. L., Vye, N. J., Moore, A., Petrosino, A., Zech, L., . . . The Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1998). Doing with understanding: Lessons from research on problem- and project-based learning. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 7(3–4), 271–311.

Barton, M. L., Heidema, C., & Jordan, D. (2002). Teaching reading in mathematics and science. Educational Leadership, 60(3), 24–29.

Becker, M., McElvany, N., & Kortenbruck, M. (2010). Intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation as predictors of reading literacy: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 773–785. doi: 10.1037/a002008

Berns, R. G., & Erickson, P. M. (2001). Contextual teaching and learning: Preparing students for the new economy (The Highlight Zone: Research @ Work No. 5). Louisville, KY: University of Louisville, National Research Center for Career and Technical Education.

Biermann, C. A., & Sarinsky, G. B. (1993). Hands-on versus remediation: Alternative strategies for a community college biology preparatory course. Community College Review, 21(3), 53–61.

Billing, D. (2007). Teaching for transfer of core/key skills in higher education: Cognitive skills. Higher Education, 53(4), 483–516.

Bond, L. P. (2004, January). Using contextual instruction to make abstract learning concrete. Techniques. Alexandria, VA: Association for Career and Technical Information. Retrieved from http://www.acteonline.org

Boroch, D., Fillpot, J., Hope, L., Johnstone, R., Mery, P., Serban, A., . . . Gabriner, R. S. (2007). Basic skills as a foundation for student success in California community colleges. Sacramento, CA: The Research and Planning Group for California Community Colleges, Center for Student Success. Retrieved from http://www.cccbsi.org/Websites/basicskills/Images/Lit_Review_Student_Success.pdf

Bottge, B. A. (1999). Effects of contextualized math instruction on problem solving of average and below-average achieving students. The Journal of Special Education, 33(2), 81–92.

Bottge, B. A., & Hasselbring, T. S. (1993). A comparison of two approaches for teaching complex, authentic mathematics problems to adolescents in remedial math classes. Exceptional Children, 59(6), 556.

Page 42: 332_866(2)

38

Bottge, B. A., Rueda, E., Serlin, R. C., Hung, Y.-H., & Jung, M. K. (2007). Shrinking achievement differences with anchored math problems: Challenges and possibilities. Journal of Special Education, 41(1), 31–49.

Boylan, H. R., Bliss, L. B., & Bonham, B. S. (1997). Program components and their relationship to student performance. Journal of Developmental Education, 20(3), 2–9.

Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Brenner, M. E. (2002). Chapter 5: Everyday problem solving and curriculum implementation: An invitation to try pizza [Monograph: Everyday and Academic Mathematics in the Classroom]. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 11, 63–92.

Brenner, M. E., Mayer, R. E., Moseley, B., Brar, T., Durán, R., Reed, B. S., Webb, D. (1997). Learning by understanding: The role of multiple representations in learning algebra. American Educational Research Journal, 34(4), 663–689.

Bulgren, J., Marquis, J., Lenz, B. K., Schumaker, J. B., & Deshler, D. D. (2009). Effectiveness of question exploration to enhance students’ written expression of content knowledge and comprehension. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(4), 271–289.

California Community Colleges. (2008). Teaming up for green technology. Getting It Done with WpLRC, 6(5), 1–2. Retrieved from http://www.wplrc.org/app/doc/WPLRC%20Getting%20It%20Done%20Vol6-5.pdf

Cammarata, L. (2009). Negotiating curricular transitions: Foreign language teachers’ learning experience with content-based instruction. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 65(4), 559–585.

Cargill, K., & Kalikoff, B. (2007). Linked psychology and writing courses across the curriculum. The Journal of General Education, 56(2), 83–92.

Carrigan, V. L. (n.d.). Contextualizing basic skills and Career Technical Education (CTE) curricula. Sacramento, CA: Workplace Learning Resource Center, Los Rios Community College District. Retrieved from http://www.wplrc.org/app/doc/Contextualizing%20Basic%20Skills%20into%20CTE%20curricula.pdf

Page 43: 332_866(2)

39

Cavazos, J. Jr., Johnson, M. B., & Sparrow, G. S. (2010). Overcoming personal and academic challenges: Perspectives from Latina/o college students. Journal of Hispanic Higher Education, 9(4), 304–316.

Caverly, D. C., Nicholson, S. A., & Radcliffe, R. (2004). The effectiveness of strategic instruction for college developmental readers. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 35(1), 25–49.

Chaplin, S. B., & Manske, J. M. (2005). A theme-based approach to teaching nonmajors biology. Journal of College Science Teaching, 35(1), 47–51.

Cox, P. L., Bobrowski, P. E., & Spector, M. (2004). Gateway to business: An innovative approach to integrating writing into the first-year business curriculum. Journal of Management Education, 28(1), 62–87.

Craig, S. A. (2006). The effects of an adapted interactive writing intervention on kindergarten children’s phonological awareness, spelling, and early reading development: A contextualized approach to instruction.Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(4), 714–731.

Crandall, J. A. (1993). Content-centered learning in the United States. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 13, 111–126.

Darling-Hammond, L., & Snyder, J. (2000). Authentic assessment of teaching in context. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(5–6), 523–545.

Darvin, J. (2000). Poetry meets plumbing: Teaching English in a vocational classroom. The English Journal, 89(6), 59–64.

De La Paz, S. (2005). Effects of historical reasoning instruction and writing strategy mastery in culturally and academically diverse middle school classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), 139–156.

De La Paz, S., & Felton, M. K. (2010). Reading and writing from multiple source documents in history: Effects of strategy instruction with low to average high school writers.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(3), 174–192.

Dean, R. J., & Dagostino, L. (2007). Motivational factors affecting advanced literacy learning of community college students. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 31(2), 149–161.

Deshler, D. (2007). Informed choices for struggling adolescent readers: A research-based guide to instructional programs and practices. New York, NY: Carnegie Corporation of New York.

Page 44: 332_866(2)

40

Detterman, D. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (1993). Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Dewey, J. (1966). Democracy and education. New York, NY: The Free Press.

Dirkx, J. M., & Crawford, M. (1993). Teaching reading through teaching science: Development and evaluation of an experimental curriculum for correctional ABE programs. Journal of Correctional Education, 44(4), 172–176.

Dirkx, J. M., & Prenger, S. M. (1997). A guide for planning and implementing instruction for adults: A theme-based approach. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Dowden, T. (2007). Relevant, challenging, integrative and exploratory curriculum design: Perspectives from theory and practice for middle level schooling in Australia. Australian Educational Researcher, 34(2), 51–71.

Ekkens, K., & Winke, P. (2009). Evaluating workplace English language programs. Language Assessment Quarterly, 6(4), 265–287.

Englert, C. S. (2009). Connecting the dots in a research program to develop, implement, and evaluate strategic literacy interventions for struggling readers and writers. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 24(2), 104–120.

Englert, C. S., Garmon, A., Mariage, T., Rozendal, M., Tarrant, K., & Urba, J. (1995). The early literacy project: Connecting across the literacy curriculum. Learning Disability Quarterly, 18(4, Research on Writing and Literacy), 253–275.

Ferretti, R. P., Lewis, W. E., & Andrews-Weckerly, S. (2009). Do goals affect the structure of students’ argumentative writing strategies? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 577–589.

Fisher, D., & Ivy, G. (2005). Literacy and language as learning in content-area classes: A departure from “every teacher a teacher of reading.” Action in Teacher Education, 27(2), 3–11.

Fox, E. (2009). The role of reader characteristics in processing and learning from informational text. Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 197–261.

Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2001). Principles for the prevention and intervention of mathematics difficulties. Learning Disabilities: Research & Practice, 16(2), 85–95.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett, C. L., Owen, R., .. . Jancek, D. (2003). Explicitly teaching learning for transfer: Effects on third-grade students’ mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 293–304.

Page 45: 332_866(2)

41

Gardenhire-Crooks, A., Collado, H., Martin, K., & Castro, A. (2010). Terms of engagement: Men of color discuss their experiences in community college Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) Document No. ED508982. New York: MDRC.

Gillespie, M. (n.d.). EFF research principle: A contextualized approach to curriculum and instruction. Knoxville, TN: Equipped for the Future. Retrieved from http://www.edpubs.gov/document/ED001934W.pdf

Goldman, S. R., & Hasselbring, T. S. (1997). Achieving meaningful mathematics literacy for students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(March/April), 198–208.

Goode, D. (2000). Creating a context for developmental English. Teaching English in the Two-Year College, 27(3), 270–277.

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Mason, L. H. (2005). Improving the writing performance, knowledge, and self-efficacy of struggling young writers: The effects of self-regulated strategy development. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 207–241.

Greenleaf, C. L., Litman, C., Hanson, T. L., Rosen, R., Boscardin, C. K., Herman, J., . . . Jones, B. (2010). Integrating literacy and science in biology: Teaching and learning impacts of Reading Apprenticeship professional development. American Educational Research Journal. Advance online publication. doi: 10.3102/000283121038483

Greeno, J. G. (2009). A theory bite on contextualizing, framing, and positioning: A companion to Son and Goldstone. Cognition and Instruction, 27(3), 269–275.

Grigg, W., Donahue, P., & Dion, G. (2007). The nation’s report card: 12th grade reading and mathematics 2005 (NCES 2007-468). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.

Grubb, W. N. (1996). Working in the middle: Strengthening education and training for the mid-skilled labor force. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Grubb, W. N., & Kraskouskas, E. (1992). A time to every purpose: Integrating academic and occupational education in community colleges and technical institutes (MDS-251). Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley, National Center for Research in Vocational Education.

Grubb, W. N. (with Worthen, H., Byrd, B., Webb, E., Badway, N., Case, C., . . . Villeneuve, J. C.). (1999). Honored but invisible: An inside look at teaching in community colleges. New York, NY: Routledge.

Page 46: 332_866(2)

42

Guthrie, J. T., Anderson, E., Alao, S., & Rinehart, J. (1999). Influences of concept-oriented reading instruction on strategy use and conceptual learning from text. The Elementary School Journal, 99(4), 343–366.

Heller, R., & Greenleaf, C. L. (2007). Literacy instruction in the content areas: Getting to the core of middle and high school improvement. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.

Hendricks, C. C. (2001). Teaching causal reasoning through cognitive apprenticeship: What are results from situated learning? Journal of Educational Research, 94(5), 302–311.

Higbee, J. L., Lundell, D. B., & Arendale, D. R. (Eds.). (2005). The General College vision: Integrating intellectual growth, multicultural perspectives, and student development. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, Center for Research on Developmental Education and Urban Literacy.

Jenkins, D., Zeidenberg, M., & Kienzl, G. S. (2009). Educational outcomes of I-BEST, Washington State Community and Technical College System’s Integrated Basic Education and Skills Training Program: Findings from a multivariate analysis (CCRC Working Paper No. 16). New York, NY: Columbia University, Teachers College, Community College Research Center. Retrieved from http://ccrc.tc.columbia.edu/Publication.asp?UID=692

Johns, A. M. (1985). Summary protocols of “underprepared” and “adept” university students: Replications and distortions of the original. Language Learning, 35(4), 495–512.

Johnson, E. B. (2002). Contextual teaching and learning: What it is and why it’s here to stay. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.

Jurmo, P. (2004). Workplace literacy education: Definitions, purposes, and approaches. Focus on Basics, 7(B), 22–26. Retrieved from http://www.ncsall.net/?id=629

Kalchik, S., & Oertle, K. M. (2010).The theory and application of contextualized teaching and learning in relation to programs of study and career pathways (Transition Highlights Issue No. 2). Champaign, IL: University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, College of Education, Office of Community College Research and Leadership.

Karweit, N. (1998). Contextual learning: A review and synthesis. In A. M. Milne (Ed.), Educational reform and vocational education (pp. 53–84). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement, National Institute on Postsecondary Education, Libraries, and Lifelong Learning.

Page 47: 332_866(2)

43

Keselman, A., Kaufman, D. R., Kramer, S., & Patel, V. L. (2007). Fostering conceptual change and critical reasoning about HIV and AIDS. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(6), 844–863.

Klein, P. D. (1999). Reopening inquiry into cognitive processes in writing-to-learn. Educational Psychology Review, 11(3), 203–270.

Klein, P. D., & Kirkpatrick, L. C. (2010). A framework for content area writing: Mediators and moderators. Journal of Writing Research, 2(1), 1–46.

Klein, P. D., & Samuels, B. (2010). Learning about plate tectonics through argument-writing. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 56(2), 196–217.

Kozeracki, C. (2005). Preparing faculty to meet the needs of developmental students. New Directions for Community Colleges, 129, 39–49.

Krajcik, J. S., & Sutherland, L. M. (2010). Supporting students in developing literacy in science. Science, 328(5977), 456–459.

Lazar, M. K., Bean, R. M., & Van Horn, B. V. (1998). Linking the success of a basic skills program to workplace practices and productivity: An evaluation. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 41(5), 352–362.

Lee, C. D., & Spratley, A. (2010). Reading in the disciplines: The challenges of adolescent literacy. New York, NY: Carnegie Corporation of New York’s Council on Advancing Adolescent Literacy.

Macaulay, J. O., Van Damme, M.-P., & Walker, K. Z. (2009). The use of contextual learning to teach biochemistry to dietetic students. Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Education, 37(3), 137–142.

Mak, B., & Coniam, D. (2008). Using wikis to enhance and develop writing skills among secondary school students in Hong Kong. System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 36(3), 437–455.

Marri, A. R., Perin, D., Crocco, M. S., Riccio, J. F., Rivet, A. R., & Chase, B. J. (in press). Content-driven literacy: One approach to urban secondary teacher education. The New Educator.

Martino, N. L., Norris, J., & Hoffman, P. R. (2001). Reading comprehension instruction: Effects of two types. Journal of Developmental Education, 25(1), 2–12.

Massey, D. D., & Heafner, T. L. (2004). Promoting reading comprehension in the social studies. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 48(1), 26–40.

Page 48: 332_866(2)

44

Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 47–62). New York, NY: Macmillan.

Mazzeo, C., Rab, S. Y., & Alssid, J. L. (2003). Building bridges to college and careers: Contextualized basic skills programs at community colleges. Brooklyn, NY: Workforce Strategy Center.

Mazzeo, C. (2008). Supporting student success at California Community Colleges: A white paper. Career Ladders Project, Bay Area Workforce Funding Collaborative (unpublished manuscript).

McDermott, M. (2010). More than writing-to-learn: Using multimodal writing tasks in the science classroom. The Science Teacher, 77(1), 32–36.

McKenna, M. C., & Robinson, R. D. (2009). Teaching through text: Reading and writing in the content areas (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education.

McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Sinatra, G. M., & Loxterman, J. A. (1992). The contribution of prior knowledge and coherent text to comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 78–93.

McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 1–43.

Mikulecky, L. (1994). Literacy transfer: A review of the literature. Philadelphia: National Center on Adult Literacy, University of Pennsylvania. Retrieved from http://www.sbctc.ctc.edu/College/abepds/literacy_transfer-a_review_of_the_literatutre_tr94-05_june_1994.pdf

Mikulecky, L., & Lloyd, P. (1997). Evaluation of workplace literacy programs: A profile of effective instructional practices. Journal of Literacy Research, 29(4), 555–585.

Misulis, K. E. (2009). Promoting learning through content literacy instruction. American Secondary Education 37(3), 10–19.

Moltz, D. (2010, November 11). Encouraging deep learning. Inside Higher Ed. Retrieved from http://www.insidehighered.com

Nash-Ditzel, S. (2010). Metacognitive reading strategies can improve self-regulation. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 40(2), 45–63.

National Council for Workforce Education & Jobs for the Future. (2010). Breaking through: Contextualization toolkit. Big Rapids, MI and Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from http://www.jff.org/sites/default/files/BT_toolkit_June7.pdf

Page 49: 332_866(2)

45

National Governors Association and Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common core state standards for English language arts and literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects. Retrieved from http://www.corestandards.org/assets/CCSSI_ELA%20Standards.pdf

Nikitina, S. (2006). Three strategies for interdisciplinary teaching: Contextualizing, conceptualizing, and problem-centring. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 38(3), 251–271.

Nokes, J. D. (2008). Aligning literacy practices in secondary history classes with research on learning. Middle Grades Research Journal, 3(3), 29–55.

Nokes, J. D., Dole, J. A., & Hacker, D. J. (2007). Teaching high school students to use heuristics while reading historical texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 492–504.

Orpwood, G., Schollen, L., Marinelli-Henriques, P., & Assiri, H. (2010). College Mathematics Project 2009 (Final report). Seneca, Ontario: Seneca College of Applied Arts and Technology, York-Seneca Institute for Mathematics, Science and Technology Education.

Paquette, K., & Kaufman, C. C. (2008). Merging civic and literacy skills. The Social Studies, 99(4), 187–190.

Parr, B. A., Edwards, M. C., & Leising, J. G. (2008). Does a curriculum integration intervention to improve the mathematics achievement of students diminish their acquisition of technical competence? An experimental study in agricultural mechanics. Journal of Agricultural Education, 49(1), 61–71.

Pearson, P. D., Moje, E., & Greenleaf, C. (2010). Literacy and science: Each in the service of the other. Science, 328(5977), 459–463.

Perin, D. (1997). Workplace literacy assessment. Dyslexia, 3(3), 190–200.

Perin, D. (2001). Academic-occupational integration as a reform strategy for the community college: Classroom perspectives. Teachers College Record, 103(2), 303–335.

Perin, D. (2005). Institutional decision making for increasing academic preparedness in community colleges. New Directions for Community Colleges, 129,27–38.

Perin, D., & Charron, K. (2006). Lights just click on every day. In T. Bailey & V. S. Morest (Eds.), Defending the community college equity agenda (pp. 155–194). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Page 50: 332_866(2)

46

Perin, D., Crocco, M., Marri, A., Riccio, J., Rivet, A., & Chase, B. (2009). Integrating literacy in content classrooms. Academic Exchange Quarterly, 13(2), 97–105.

Perin, D., Hare, R. J., Peverly, S. T., & Mason, L. H. (2010). Contextualized written summarization by academically-underprepared community college students. Unpublished manuscript.

Perin, D., Keselman, A., & Monopoli, M. (2003). The academic writing of community college remedial students: Text and learner variables. Higher Education, 45(1), 19–42.

Perkins, D. H., & Salomon, G. (1989). Are cognitive skills context-bound? Educational Researcher, 18(1), 16–25.

Pugach, M. C., Longwell-Grice, H., Ford, A., & Surma, M. A. (2008). A programmatic view of portfolios for urban teacher preparation. New Educator, 4(2), 107–132.

Radford, A. W., Berkner, L., Wheeless, S., & Shepherd, B. (2010). Persistence and attainment of 2003–04 beginning postsecondary students: After six years. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Educational Statistics. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2011151

Reilly, E. M., & Pagnucci, G. S. (2007). Mathematics, art, research, collaboration, and storytelling: The high M.A.R.C.S. project. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 12(9), 497–502.

Rivet, A. E., & Krajcik, J. S. (2008). Contextualizing instruction: Leveraging students’ prior knowledge and experiences to foster understanding of middle school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45(1), 79–100.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54–67.

Salahu-Din, D., Persky, H., & Miller, J. (2008). The nation’s report card: Writing 2007 (NCES 2008-468). Washington, DC:, U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.

Schultz, M. (2003). Analyzing argumentative strategies. Teaching English in the Two-Year College, 31(1), 77–80.

Selinger, B. M. (1995). Summarizing text: Developmental students demonstrate a successful method. Journal of Developmental Education, 19(2), 14–16, 18, 20.

Page 51: 332_866(2)

47

Sencibaugh, J. M. (2008). A synthesis of content enhancement strategies for teaching students with reading difficulties at the middle and secondary level. Reading Improvement 45(2), 84–98.

Shore, M., Shore, J., & Boggs, S. (2004). Allied health applications integrated into developmental mathematics using problem based learning. Mathematics and Computer Education, 38(2), 183–189.

Simpson, M. L., Hynd, C. R., Nist, S. L., & Burrell, K. I. (1997). College academic assistance programs and practices. Educational Psychology Review, 9(1), 39–87.

Simpson, M. L., & Nist, S. L. (2002). Encouraging active reading at the college level. In C. C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 365–381). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Smagorinsky, P., & Smith, M. W. (1992). The nature of knowledge in composition and literary understanding: The question of specificity. Review of Educational Research, 62(3), 279–305.

Snyder, V. (2002). The effect of course-based reading strategy training on the reading comprehension skills of developmental college students. Research and Teaching in Developmental Education, 18(2), 37–41.

Son, J. Y., & Goldstone, R. L. (2009). Contextualization in perspective. Cognition & Instruction, 27(1), 51–89.

Sticht, T. G. (1995). The military experience and workplace literacy: A review and synthesis for policy and practice. Philadelphia, PA: National Center on Adult Literacy.

Sticht, T. G. (2005). Functional context education: Making learning relevant in the 21st century:Workshop participant’s notebook. Retrieved from http://www.nald.ca/library/research/fce/FCE.pdf

Sticht, T. G., Armstrong, W. B., Hickey, D. T., & Caylor, J. S. (1987). Cast-off youth: Policy and training methods from the military experience. New York, NY: Praeger.

Stone, J. R., III, Alfeld, C., Pearson, D., Lewis, M. V., & Jensen, S. (2006). Building academic skills in context: Testing the value of enhanced math learning in CTE (Final study).St. Paul, MN: National Research Center for Career and Technical Education. Retrieved from http://136.165.122.102/UserFiles/File/Math-in-CTE/MathLearningFinalStudy.pdf

Page 52: 332_866(2)

48

Tilson, J. L., Castek, J., & Goss, M. (2010). Exploring the influence of science writing instruction on fourth graders’ writing development. In R. T. Jimenez, V. J. Risko, M. K. Hundley & D. W. Rowe (Eds.), 59th Yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 117–134). Oak Creek, CA: National Reading Conference.

Vaughn, S., Martinez, L. R., Linan-Thompson, S., Reutebuch, C. K., Carlson, C. D., & Francis, D. J. (2009). Enhancing social studies vocabulary and comprehension for seventh-grade English language learners: Findings from two experimental studies. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(4), 297–324.

Weinbaum, A., & Rogers, A. M. (1995). Contextual learning: A critical aspect of school-to-work transition programs. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement. Retrieved from ERIC database. (ED381666)

Weiss, M. J., Visher, M. G., & Wathington, H. (with Teres, J., & Schneider, E.). (2010). Learning communities for students in developmental reading: An impact study at Hillsborough Community College. New York, NY: Columbia University, Teachers College, National Center for Postsecondary Research.

Wisely, W. C. (2009). Effectiveness of contextual approaches to developmental math in California community colleges(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of the Pacific, Stockton, CA.

Wooden, J. A. (2008). “I had always heard Lincoln was a good person, but. . . ”: A study of sixth graders’ reading of Lincoln’s views on Black-white relations. Social Studies, 99(1), 23–32.

WSBCTC. (2005). I-BEST: A program integrating adult basic education and workforce training(Research Report No. 05-2). Olympia, WA: Washington State Board for Community and Technical Colleges.

Yamauchi, L. A. (2003). Making school relevant for at-risk students: The Wai’anae High School Hawaiian Studies Program. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 8(4), 379–390.

Page 53: 332_866(2)

49

Appendix

Table 1 Evidence for Contextualization 

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

COLLEGE Caverly et al. (2004) (Experiment 2) ‐ C 

Quasi‐experimental,  treatment vs. no‐treatment comparison (paired t‐tests and Mann‐Whitney U)  

Developmental education: reading 

Reading strategy taught using chapters from textbook assigned in concurrent college‐level history course. Strategy focuses on predicting information, applying prior knowledge, confirming prior knowledge through close reading, and representing the information in the text on a chart. 

Standardized reading test (TASP‐Reading), grade in reading‐intensive course (history) 

Comparison < treatment on pre‐post gain on reading test, and post‐only history grade.  

Comparison group did not take developmental education—confounding of course and strategy taught; 4‐year gap between pre and post tests; no covariates 

Martino et al. (2001) ‐ C 

Quasi‐experimental, comparison of two contextualized reading instruction treatments (ANOVA) 

Biology students with low reading levels 

Treatment 1, fully contextualized: Communicative Reading Strategies (CRS), used only biology textbook, instruction on graphophonic, phonemic, lexical, syntactic, conceptual and discourse structure skills; prior knowledge, oral reading, factual questions, making inferences, generalizing information to new situations. Treatment 2, partly contextualized: Skills Instruction, used traditional reading text and reading skills worksheets related to biology text. 

Standardized test (Nelson Denny Reading Test) and researcher‐designed weekly biology comprehension probes  

Both groups gained on standardized reading test, no group x time interaction.  Same amount but faster increase in biology comprehension for Treatment 1.  

Number too small (8 total) to make statistical inferences  

Page 54: 332_866(2)

50

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Perin et al. (2010) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental, treatment vs. no‐treatment comparison; randomization to treatments within classrooms (ANCOVA/ regression) 

Developmental education: reading and writing 

Weekly independent practice in written summarization, question‐formulation, and vocabulary development; use of reading comprehension quizzes and persuasive writing using biology text 

Researcher‐designed summarization & standardized reading test: Nelson‐Denny comprehension 

Controlling for student background, comparison < treatment on pre‐post gain on 3 summarization variables (effect sizes 0.33–0.62 SD units) but not standardized reading test; performance better with science than generic text on 2 summarization variables. 

Intervention did not provide feedback to students; unknown level of contamination between conditions in same classrooms  

Snyder (2002) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental, treatment vs. no‐treatment comparison (within and between‐subjects t‐tests) 

Developmental education: reading 

Reading comprehension strategies (question‐generation, clarification, prediction, and locating main ideas) taught using text assigned in freshman seminar on writing, philosophy, and speech 

Standardized reading test: Nelson‐Denny comprehension 

Comparison < treatment on amount of pre‐post gain but still significantly lower than comparison group on post‐test 

Comparison group began with higher reading levels (tested out of developmental education) 

Shore et al. (2004) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental, treatment vs. no‐treatment comparison group (no inferential statistics) 

Developmental education, allied health: math 

Math taught using problems drawn from allied health curricula. Developmental and content instructors collaborated to create problems. 

Researcher‐designed math test 

In first 2 years but not in 3rd year of program, higher math scores for contextualization than comparison group 

Test scores and inferential statistics not reported, little information on methodology provided 

Page 55: 332_866(2)

51

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Wisely (2009) ‐ C and I  3 groups compared: contextualized, traditional course but contextualized offered at college, traditional course, contextualized not offered at college, demographic controls (logistic regression) 

College CTE: math 

33 credit math courses contextualized or integrated math skills in material from CTE courses taken by students. 

Dichotomous: pass basic skills course, persist to enrollment in credit course work next term 

Contextualization predicted non‐white students’ completion of math course, and speed of entry into, performance in, and completion of other college level courses 

Teachers self‐reported on contextualization, not independently verified; CTE students not tested for math skills, may have been low skilled (developmental level) 

Cox et al. (2004) ‐ I  One group, pre‐post (within‐subjects t‐test) 

CTE, business: writing 

Instruction in writing business abstracts and research reports on specific industries; writing tasks assigned to support learning of business content; instruction provided within business course 

Pre‐post researcher‐designed test of ability to write abstract of business article  

Increase in ability to write business abstract but not using own words (statistically significant decrease); increase in quality of business reports 

No control group, no moderators included in statistical analysis 

ADULT BASIC EDUCATION Dirkx & Crawford (1993) ‐ C 

Quasi‐experimental: treatment vs. no‐treatment comparison (no inferential statistics) 

Adult basic education, maximum security prison inmates: reading and writing  

Reading and writing instruction contextualized in earth, life, and physical sciences on assumption that learning about natural world would motivate students. 

Pre‐post reading test, self‐reported reading habits attendance, observations of learner engagement 

5 of 9 treatment students compared to 2 of 9 comparison students scored higher on post than pre reading test. Attendance and student engagement: comparison < treatment  

Small sample size (9 in each group); little information on reading test; reading habits self‐reported 

Page 56: 332_866(2)

52

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Ekkens & Winke (2009) ‐ C 

One group, pre‐post (within‐subjects correlations and t‐tests)  

Adult basic education, workplace literacy, English language learners: reading 

Grammar and vocabulary contextualized in time management, culture of work, and communication topics using health and manufacturing materials 

Standardized test: Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment System (CASAS), reading and listening subtests; self‐ratings of satisfaction and learning  

Little or no gain on standardized tests of listening and reading but high self‐ratings of satisfaction and learning  

No control group; use of self‐ratings. 

 Lazar et al. (1998) ‐ C  One group, pre‐post (within‐subjects t‐tests) 

Adult basic education, workplace literacy, health care workers, and administrative assistants: reading, writing, and math 

Literacy instruction contextualized in simulations of on‐the‐job teamwork using materials used in health care and administrative assistance jobs, for job upgrading. 

Standardized tests: Adult Learning Employment‐Related Tasks (ALERT) and Tests of Adult Basic Education (TABE) reading and math subtests; researcher designed writing quality, writing fluency, and vocabulary measures; employee and supervisor ratings of job literacy; employer ratings of job performance  

Statistically significant increase on all basic skills and job literacy measures except writing fluency. 

No control group.  

Mikulecky & Lloyd (1997) ‐ C 

One group, pre‐post (within‐subjects t‐tests) 

Adult basic education, workplace literacy: reading 

Reading instruction in several worksites contextualized in materials from manufacturing, prison, insurance, and hospital settings. 

Researcher designed measures of reading practices, self‐efficacy, and job‐related reading created from interview responses 

Statistically significant pre‐post gain on all measures except amount of reading on and off the job   

Scores based on self‐reports. Waiting list control group for one worksite but pre‐post data not reported for this group. 

Page 57: 332_866(2)

53

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Perin (1997) ‐ C  One group, pre‐post (within‐subjects t‐tests) 

Adult basic education, workplace literacy, psychiatric health care aides: reading and writing 

Reading and writing contextualized in health care jobs 

Test of Applied Literacy Skills (TALS), prose subtest; Tennessee Self‐Concept Scale; researcher designed job‐related reading (2 versions, harder and easier), writing, and self‐efficacy tests 

Gains shown on all measures except TALS and harder version of researcher‐designed reading test 

No control group.  

Sticht (1995) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental, treatment vs. traditional instruction comparison (no inferential statistics) 

Adult basic education, army recruits: reading 

Reading instruction (“FLIT” program) contextualized in job materials such as manuals. 

Researcher‐designed general and job‐related reading tests 

More gain on job‐related than general reading in treatment group. Comparison < treatment in gain on job‐related reading 

Little information on nature of measures; inferential statistics not reported.  

Jenkins et al. (2009) ‐ I  Treatment and comparison groups compared (linear and logistic regressions). Treatment = enrollment not completion 

Adult basic education: I‐BEST program: reading, writing and math 

Non‐credit basic skills course paired with credit occupational education course. Reading, writing, and math instruction used job content; team teaching by academic skills and occupational instructors. (Comparison took adult basic education and, for some, a traditional occupational course.)         

College credits, persistence to following year, earn degree or certificate, gain on standardized reading, writing, math skills tests 

Comparison < treatment on college persistence, and literacy and math gain    

Possible selection bias 

Page 58: 332_866(2)

54

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

SECONDARY EDUCATION  

           

De La Paz (2005) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental:  post‐only treatment vs. comparison(one‐way ANOVA – condition by writing quality) 

Typically developing, high achieving and learning disabled 8thgraders: writing 

Language arts teacher taught strategy for persuasive writing contextualized in history of U.S. westward expansion; course paired with history course. 

Researcher designed measures of essay length, persuasive quality, number of arguments, historical accuracy of persuasive writing sample 

Comparison < treatment on all measures (d = 0.57 to d = 1.23) 

Treatment group consisted of typically achieving and learning disabled students; comparison group consisted of English language learners; no pretest 

Bottge (1999) ‐ C  Quasi‐experimental: comparison of two treatments taught to different classes: contextualized problem and word problem, matched treatment groups (repeated measures ANCOVA) 

Typically developing and remedial students, 8th grade: math 

Math instruction contextualized in videotaped scenarios of real‐life shopping problems, compared with traditional math instruction 

Pre‐post fractions computation, word problem, contextualized problem, and two transfer tests 

Word problem < contextualized problem condition on contextualized problem and transfer tests; no differences in fractions computation or word problems 

Despite matching process, higher pre‐existing math and self‐efficacy scores in contextualization group 

Page 59: 332_866(2)

55

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Bottge & Hasselbring (1993) ‐ C 

Experimental: random assignment of matched students to contextualized problem and word problem conditions (t‐tests and repeated measures ANOVA) 

Students with learning disabilities in 7–9th grade: math 

Same as Bottge (1999): math instruction contextualized in videotaped scenarios of real‐life shopping problems, compared with traditional math instruction 

Researcher designed fractions computation, contextualized problem and transfer tests 

Word problem < contextualized problem group on gain on contextualized and transfer problems 

 

Brenner et al. (1997) ‐ C 

Experimental: random assignment of classes to contextualized or traditional condition (ANCOVA) 

7–8th grade pre‐algebra students: math 

Pre‐algebra problems taught using real life scenario involving selection of pizza company as vendor for school cafeteria 

Pre‐post researcher designed function word problem, word problem representation, word problem solving, and equation solving (symbol manipulation) tests 

Control < treatment in gain on function and representation tests; treatment < control on symbol manipulation 

Instructional differences besides contextualization: treatment group received cooperative learning and focused more on problem representation than symbol manipulation 

Bulgren et al. (2009) ‐ I  Random assignment to treatment or traditional instruction (ANCOVA) 

9th–12th grade science students: learning disabled and typically developing: reading and writing 

Short‐term intervention embedding reading and writing instruction in science instruction. 

6‐Trait Model of Analytic Scoring (ideas, organization, voice, word choice, sentence fluency, and researcher‐designed conventions in writing sample) and content score (name problem, cause of problem, effect of problem, solution, and main idea)  

Control < treatment on both writing and content scores, effect sizes d = 1.44 and d = 0.74 

Instruction by researchers, not teachers in classrooms, short duration (only 2 sessions) 

Page 60: 332_866(2)

56

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Greenleaf et al. (2010) ‐ I 

Experimental: schools randomized to treatment or control (hierarchical regression) 

High school biology, 2 samples (longitudinal—parental consent; cross‐sectional—no consent, anonymous, delinked data): reading 

High school science teachers integrated reading instruction after receiving professional development 

Statewide standardized tests: California Standards Test, biology, English language arts (ELA) and reading comprehension subtests 

Longitudinal sample: no difference treatment and control; cross‐sectional sample (author states more representative): control < treatment, effect sizes 0.23., 0.24, 0.28 SD units for biology, ELA and reading  

Attrition: 54% of teachers remained to end of study; because of selective attrition of teachers, treatment classes had higher proportions of low skilled and ELL students than control 

De La Paz & Felton (2010) ‐ I 

Quasi‐experimental: integrated compared to traditional history instruction (repeated measures ANCOVA and ordinal regression) 

11th grade history students: writing 

History teachers taught historical reasoning strategy and integrated writing instruction in course on 20th century American history 

Measures based on persuasive writing sample: essay length, essay quality, development of arguments, claims, rebuttals, reference to documents 

At post‐test, comparison < treatment on essay length, quality, rebuttals, and accuracy 

Experimental condition involved innovative practice in both history (historical reasoning strategy) and writing, not possible to attribute findings specifically to integration of writing instruction 

Vaughn et al. (2009) (Experiment 2) ‐ I 

Experimental: students randomly assigned to course sections and sections randomly assigned to treatment or control condition (ANCOVA) 

7th grade social studies: English language learners (ELLs) and native speakers of English: reading 

Reading comprehension and vocabulary instruction integrated in social studies 

Pre‐post vocabulary and comprehension measures 

Control < treatment on both comprehension and vocabulary measures (g = 1.12 and g = 0.53); equally effective for ELLs and native speakers 

Comprehension measure may have underestimated performance in requiring written rather than forced choice responses 

Page 61: 332_866(2)

57

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Parr et al. (2008) ‐ I  Experimental: post only, teachers randomly assigned to treatment or control condition (ANOVA)   

High school CTE: math 

Math taught in 9 lessons in agricultural power in a technology course (math‐enhanced CTE) 

NOCTI Agriculture Mechanics examination 

No difference in treatment and control on occupational content (authors argue this is positive finding—math instruction did not diminish occupational learning) 

Math was taught but math outcomes not measured.  

Stone et al. (2006) ‐ I  Experimental: teachers randomly assigned to treatment or control condition (hierarchical regression)  

High school CTE: math 

Math‐enhanced CTE instruction in agriculture; auto technology; business and marketing; health; and information technology courses (math‐enhanced CTE); 10% of instruction in 1 academic year 

Post‐test: (1) students randomly assigned to take TerraNova CTBS Basic Battery, ACCUPLACER Elementary Algebra test, or WorkKeys Applied Mathematics Assessment; (2) occupational tests, varied across schools. Pre test: Terra‐Nova scores 

Control < treatment on gain on TerraNova (d = 0.55) and ACCUPLACER (d = 0.42) math but not occupational tests (authors make same argument as Parr et al. above) 

 

ELEMENTARY EDUCATION 

           

Brenner et al. (1997) ‐ C 

Quasi‐experimental: post‐only treatment vs. comparison (t‐test) 

Native Hawaiian kindergartners: math 

Math taught in context of everyday life money problems, and Native Hawaiian themes; math vocabulary introduced in Hawaiian Creole. 

Metropolitan Achievement Test, math and reading subtests 

Comparison < treatment on math but not reading   

No pretest 

Page 62: 332_866(2)

58

Reference  & Type of 

Contextualization 

Design & Analysis 

Sample & Skill Area 

Nature of Instruction  Dependent Variables 

Results  Methodological Flaws 

Guthrie et al. (1999) ‐ I  Quasi‐experimental: post‐only treatment vs. comparison, pretest reading covariate (MANCOVA) 

3rd and 5th graders: reading 

“Concept Oriented Reading Instruction:” integration of science and reading comprehension instruction 

Pre test: California Test of Basic Skills (3rd graders), Metropolitan Achievement Test (5th graders). Posttest: performance assessment of reading and language arts—2 forms, familiar and unfamiliar topics: prior knowledge, strategy use, drawing, writing, conceptual transfer, informational text comprehension, narrative interpretation  

Controlling for student background, comparison < treatment on strategy use, conceptual learning, and text comprehension; better transfer to new domain at grade 3 than grade 5. 

Probable contamination of treatment and comparison instruction 

Tilson et al. (2010) ‐ I  Experimental: random assignment of classrooms to treatment or control (ANCOVA) 

4th graders: writing 

Writing instruction embedded in science instruction. Students taught to write explanations of light and energy: constructing topic sentences, including supporting evidence, and using scientific vocabulary 

Researcher‐designed writing measures: science content, use of evidence, quality of introduction and conclusion, clarity, vocabulary usage, and vocabulary count 

Control < treatment on composite writing score  d = 0.69. On separate writing skills: control < treatment: science content, use of evidence, quality of introduction, clarity, vocabulary count; treatment < control on quality of conclusion, and vocabulary usage 

 

 Note. C = contextualized instruction; I = integrated instruction. Comparison group attended traditional college‐level courses but not developmental education. “No‐treatment,” unless otherwise indicated, is defined as business as usual, i.e., following the conventional curriculum.