6. wu, c. s., & cheng, f. f. (2011)

Upload: tablamaru

Post on 07-Jul-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    1/11

    The joint effect of framing and anchoring on internet buyers’ decision-making

    Chin-Shan Wu a, Fei-Fei Cheng b,⇑

    a Tunghai University, Department of Information Management, No. 181, Section 3, Taichung Port Rd., Taichung City 40704, Taiwan ROC b National Chung Hsing University, Institute of Technology Management, 250 Kuo Kuang Rd., Taichung 402, Taiwan

    a r t i c l e i n f o

     Article history:

    Received 20 August 2009

    Received in revised form 12 January 2011

    Accepted 12 January 2011

    Available online 20 January 2011

    Keywords:

    Anchor points

    Anchoring effect

    Cognitive bias

    Experimental research

    Framing effect

    Internet advertising

    Message framing

    Product attributes

    Purchase intention

    Willingness to pay

    a b s t r a c t

    This article reports on an experiment that examines the influence of message framing and anchor points,

    and the joint effect of these two information cues on Internet consumers’ judgments regarding attitude,purchase intention and willingness to pay. The role of participants’ subjective knowledge is also evalu-

    ated. The experimental results suggest that message framing, which describes a product’s attribute in

    positive or negative terms, significantly influences participants’ attitude toward and their intention to

    buy the product. In addition, participants’ willingness to pay was significantly influenced by the presen-

    tation of anchors embedded in banner advertisements. Further, a significant interaction effect for mes-

    sage framing and anchor points indicate that their congruence enhances the effects of information

    presentation on people’s responses. Specifically, describing a product attribute in positive terms along

    with a high anchor point induces more favorable response than any other framing and anchoring com-

    binations. Finally, online shoppers who are low in product knowledge are more susceptible to framing

    and anchoring influences. The findings provide guidance for designing appropriate product and price cues

    to induce Internet consumer responses that favor online retailers.

     2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    The Internet has been growing rapidly. According to a recent re-

    port (Internet World States 2010), the number of Internet users is

    estimated to have reach 180 million in December 2009, up from 36

    million in 2000. In addition, the dollar value of online retail sales

    for the third quarter of 2008 was US$34.4 billion (US Census Bu-

    reau 2008), an increase of 0.3% from the third quarter of 2007.

    The development of e-commerce in Taiwan has expanded fast also.

    More than 1.3 million users connected to the Internet in 2009

    (TWNIC 2009) and the total online market volume reached

    NT$243 million (about US$7.36 million in current exchange rate

    terms for January 2011). The growth rate of the e-commerce mar-ket reached 21.9% in 2009, which surpassed the growth rate of the

    physical retail channel, which actually declined. This number is

    also larger than the growth rate of 15.4% in the US e-commerce

    market estimated by Forrester Research. Thus, although Taiwan

    is a densely populated country, more and more consumers have

    been accepting this new distribution channel and this has pro-

    moted the development of e-commerce in Taiwan.

    In this context of e-commerce growth, it is important for both

    online retailers and researchers to look for factors that influence

    Internet consumers’ purchase decisions. Past studies have focused

    on this issue froma variety of perspectives. Theseincludethe design

    of appropriate online store atmosphere to influence online consum-

    ers’ emotional and behavioral responses (Mandeland Johnson2002,

    Cheng et al. 2009, Wu et al. 2008a,b), the influence of trust (Shnei-

    derman 2000, Pavlou 2003, Jones and Leonard 2008), consumer

    decision-making in an online environment (Häubl and Trifts 2000,

    Häubl and Murray 2003), as well as the effects of Web site quality

    on Internet users’ shopping decisions (Aladwani and Palvia 2002,

    Kim and Stoel 2004, Liu et al. 2009, Huang et al. 2009). The current

    study introduces a different perspective which draws on cognitivepsychology research and focuses on the influence of web site infor-

    mation on Internet shoppers’ purchase responses.

    Our research focus is based on the observation that while Inter-

    net consumers today are faced with a variety of information when

    they browse online retail stores, the information delivered through

    Web pages mainly includes product attribute messages and price

    information cues. Understanding how to use these two types of 

    Web site information to influence Internet shoppers’ decision-

    making is an important yet overlooked issue. Based on the cogni-

    tive psychology research, individual decision-making can be influ-

    enced by the way information is presented, which includes two

    types of cognitive biases: the framing bias and the anchoring bias.

    1567-4223/$ - see front matter  2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.elerap.2011.01.002

    ⇑ Corresponding author.

    E-mail addresses: [email protected] (C.-S. Wu), [email protected]

    (F.-F. Cheng).

    Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

    Contents lists available at  ScienceDirect

    Electronic Commerce Research and Applications

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e :   w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / e c r a

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.elerap.2011.01.002mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.elerap.2011.01.002http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/15674223http://www.elsevier.com/locate/ecrahttp://www.elsevier.com/locate/ecrahttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/15674223http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.elerap.2011.01.002mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.elerap.2011.01.002

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    2/11

    We can understand framing bias based on a well-known exper-

    iment by Tversky and Kahneman (1981). They described an Asian

    disease in terms of either the likelihood of lives saved (a positive

    framing) or the likelihood of lives lost (a negative framing) to a

    group of subjects. The results indicated that the relative attractive-

    ness of options varies when the same decision problem is framed

    in different ways. This phenomenon is referred to as  framing effect 

    or the framing bias

    . In addition to the framing bias, another cogni-

    tive bias that results from information presentation is called the

    anchoring effect . It describes the phenomenon that occurs when

    an arbitrarily chosen reference point or  anchor   influences a deci-

    sion-maker’s estimate of value (Slovic and Lichtenstein 1971). In

    the online shopping context, framing messages are most likely to

    be used in describing a product attribute as positive or negative.

    The reference price of the product shown in a Web banner ad

    may serve as an  anchor point  to influence an Internet shopper’s

    decision behavior. Because consumers who browse the Web site

    tend to perceive all its elements holistically rather than indepen-

    dently, we think that the framing message and anchor point will

    influence consumer behavior independent of one another, and that

    there may also be a joint effect on Internet shoppers’ responses.

    The Internet provides an ever-increasing amount of product

    information (Lee and Lee 2004), so online consumers often rely

    on heuristics to make decisions.  Park and Lessig (1981) assert that

    subjective knowledge provides information about decision-mak-

    ers’ systematic biases in decisions. Thus, we also investigated the

    role of Internet consumers’ knowledge level in moderating the

    occurrence of the framing effect and the anchoring effect.

    Overall then, we argue that Internet shoppers’ purchase deci-

    sions may be influencedby theway that product information is pre-

    sentedto them– in eithera positiveor a negativeways – suchthata

    discernible framing effect will occur. Also, the presentation of price

    information is another possible source that may induce the anchor-

    ing effect. Internet shoppers perceive productand price information

    in a Web site’s banner holistically, rather than independently. So

    how these two elements jointly influence consumer decisions is

    an important issue. In addition, the role of consumer productknowledge plays a role in their decision-making process. Thus, a

    goal of thisresearchto investigate howmessage framing andanchor

    points independently and jointly influence Internet shopper deci-

    sions, as well as the role of consumer knowledge in this context.

    The findings of this research can provide insights regarding how

    to present information in Web pages to induce consumer responses

    that arefavorable for online retailers. The results can also help us to

    understand under what conditions consumers are less likely to be

    affected by external reference prices provided by banner ads and

    the framing messages they contain for product attributes.

    2. Theory 

    A rational decision-maker is expected to make similar decisionsno matter how the same decision problem is described or framed.

    However, human decision-makers usually lack the knowledge,

    computational skills or time resources necessary to make decisions

    in a manner compatible with  Simon’s (1957)  economic theory of 

    rational behavior. Instead, decision-makers tend to adopt heuris-

    tics to save effort ( Johnson and Payne 1985, Payne et al. 1988,

    Thorngate 1980), but decision biases or cognitive biases may occur

    as a result. One way of viewing cognitive biases is that they are sys-

    tematic deviations from rationality. There are various kinds of cog-

    nitive biases that have been identified by decision theory

    researchers. The framing effect and the anchoring effect are both

    well-known cognitive biases that have been explored in the re-

    search of  Tversky and Kahneman (1981), and result based on the

    way information is presented to a decision-maker.

     2.1. The framing effect 

    The framing effect can be viewed in terms of three different types

    of framing: attribute framing, goal framing, and risky choice fram-

    ing effects (Levin et al. 1998). In attribute framing , a single attribute

    of a given object is framed positively or negatively. In  goal framing ,

    the persuasive message is framed to stress the positive or negative

    consequences of performing an act or not. Risky choice framing 

     oc-

    curs when the choice between a risky and a riskless option of equal

    expected value depend on whether the options are described in po-

    sitive terms or in negative terms. We will focus only on attribute

    framing because it is the simplest case, and because it is especially

    useful for gaining a basic understanding of how the  descriptive va-

    lence influences information processing (Levin et al. 1998).

    The attribute framing effect  occurs when individuals’ judgments

    vary as a function of the labels used to describe specific object attri-

    butes ( Johnson and Levin 1985). For example,  Levin et al. (1985)

    examined the effect of message framing in three tasks and a statis-

    tically reliable framing effect was observed. More favorable ratings

    were produced when the key attributes were expressed in positive

    terms than when they were expressed in negative terms.

    Moreover, in   Levin and Gaeth’s (1988)   study, the product

    ‘‘ground beef’’ was framed as either ‘‘75% lean’’ or ‘‘25% fat’’ and

    was presented to two groups of subjects. The results indicate that

    the participants’ evaluations were more favorable when the beef 

    was described in percent-lean terms than when it was described

    in percent-fat terms. Other studies (Kramer 1989, Loke and Tan

    1992, Levin et al. 1998, 2002) also have similar findings that the

    same alternative is rated more favorably when a key attribute is

    framed in positive terms rather than in negative terms.

    Levin (1987)  suggested that the framing effect occurs because

    positively-framed or negatively-framed information is believed to

    be associated with a more favorable or unfavorable encoding in a

    person’s memory. Subjects’ favorable associations in memory can

    be evoked by labeling a product’s attribute in a positive manner.

    In contrast, when the information is labeled negatively, unfavor-

    able associations in memory tend to be evoked, and this resultsin less favorable cognitive responses.

    The dependent measure of the attribute framing effect is evalu-

    ation (Levenet al.1998), whichcan take theformof ratings of favor-

    ability. This include perceptions of the quality of the product (Levin

    and Gaeth 1988), attitude toward the target product (Shiv et al.

    1997) or the advertisement (Smith 1996), willingness to purchase

    theproduct (Zhang andBuda 1999), andwillingness to pay (Howard

    and Salkeld 2009). We will measure consumer attitudes toward the

    product, purchase intention and willingness to pay for the product

    in this study to examine the effect of attribute framing messages.

    Based on the above discussion, we propose the following

    hypotheses:

    Hypothesis 1a   (The Framing and Product Attitude Hypothesis). Par-ticipants in the positive framing condition will have a more

    positive attitude toward the target product than their counterparts

    in the negative framing condition.

    Hypothesis 1b   (The Framing and Intention to Buy Hypothesis). Par-

    ticipants in the positive framing condition will reveal a higher

    intention to buy the target product than their counterparts in the

    negative framing condition.

    Hypothesis 1c   (The Framing and Willingness to Pay Hypothe-

    sis). Participants in the positive framing condition will reveal a

    higher willingness to pay for the target product than their counter-

    parts in the negative framing condition.

    C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368   359

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    3/11

     2.2. The anchoring effect 

     Anchoring and adjustment  is an heuristic that is often employed

    to make probability estimates and value predictions (Tversky and

    Kahneman 1974). In most studies, anchoring experiments are ini-

    tiated by explicitly asking people to compare the anchoring value

    to some target (Tversky and Kahneman 1974, Strack and Musswe-

    iler 1997, Wong and Kwong 2000, Kristensen and Gärling 2000,

    Mussweiler and Strack 2000). Basically, a high-value and a low-va-

    lue anchor point typically are provided to two groups of subjects,

    which serve as the basis of comparison. Subjects then use an

    adjustment process to produce their estimates (Wilson et al.

    1996, Strack and Mussweiler 1997). Finally, the adjustment pro-

    cess terminates in a range between the upper or lower bound on

    value and the actual value (Strack and Mussweiler 1997). People’s

    final estimates will be lower if they began with a lower-value an-

    chor point than if they began with a high-value anchor point. The

    gap between the final estimate and the actual value often arises

    because the person who makes the estimate does not make enough

    of an adjustment. Lopes (1982) considers this to be one of the pos-

    sible sources of the anchoring bias or anchoring effect. (See  Chap-

    man and Johnson (2002) for additional discussion.)

    The anchoring effect is a robust phenomenon that has been ob-

    served in many domains. These include numerical estimation

    (Tversky and Kahneman 1974, Mussweiler and Strack 2000), spou-

    sal preference prediction (Davis et al. 1986), and negotiation (Ritov

    1996, Kristensen and Gärling 1997). A general finding in past re-

    search suggests that the anchoring value has a pronounced effect

    on the subsequent response. A higher anchor point leads to a high-

    er final estimate than lower anchor point does (Tversky and Kahn-

    eman 1974, Northcraft and Neale 1987, Wilson et al. 1996, Strack

    and Mussweiler 1997, Mussweiler and Strack 1999). For example,

    participants in  Northcraft and Neale (1987)  were asked to make

    pricing decisions about real estate properties. The listing price for

    the property served as anchor point, and different groups of sub-

     jects were assigned to four listing price conditions. The results

    indicated that the anchor points tended to influence the subjects’value estimates. The higher the anchor points, the higher were

    the final estimates.

    Consumers form their preferences based on the available infor-

    mation, such as reference prices, when there is uncertainty about

    product quality (Scitovsky 1944–1945). In general, consumers tend

    to infer product quality from the reference price (McConnell

    1968a,b, Shapiro 1970). A higher price is believed to be associated

    with higher quality than a lower price is. Thus, it is reasonable to

    argue that consumers will have more a positive attitude and higher

    intention to buy when they are exposed to higher-value anchor

    points than those exposed to lower-value anchor points.

    In addition, marketing researchers have provided some evi-

    dence regarding the anchoring effects on consumers’ price judg-

    ments (Ariely et al. 2003, Green et al. 1998, Wu et al. 2008a,b,2011). In this study, we use Internet buyer willingness to pay to

    capture the consumer price judgments. Willingness to pay denotes

    the maximum price a buyer is willing to pay for a given quantity of 

    a good. It is a subjective value that the buyer assigns to that

    quantity (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002). External reference prices

    provided to consumers through channels such as price advertise-

    ments, catalog listings, and consumer price guides often serve as

    anchors to influence consumer price perceptions (Biswas and Blair

    1991). Thus, we assert the following group of hypotheses:

    Hypothesis 2a   (The Anchoring Condition and Product Attitude

    Hypothesis). Participants in high anchor condition will have a

    more positive attitude toward the target product than their

    counterparts in the low anchor condition.

    Hypothesis 2b   (The Anchoring Condition and Intention to Buy

    Hypothesis). Participants in high anchor condition will reveal a

    higher intention to buy the target product than their counterparts

    in the low anchor condition.

    Hypothesis 2c   (The Anchoring Condition and Willingness to Pay

    Hypothesis). Participants in high anchor condition will reveal a

    higher willingness to pay than their counterparts in the low anchorcondition.

     2.3. The joint effect of framing and anchoring 

    Message framing is an important aspect of communication

    strategy. In contrast, the reference price is an important factor that

    influences consumers’ perceived quality of the product, as well as

    their willingness to pay. Each topic has been studied in the past

    in isolation, but investigations regarding their joint effects are rare.

    We extend the previous research by proposing that the joint effect

    of message framing and anchor points will be significant with re-

    spect to Internet consumers’ attitude, purchase intention and will-

    ingness to pay.

    When consumers are exposed to positive messages, they arelikely to form a higher expectation for product quality. A positive

    message accompanied by a high-value anchor point can deliver a

    consistent message that the product is of good quality and worth-

    while for consumers to pay a higher price to buy it. Thus, the posi-

    tive messages accompanied by a high-value reference price

    embedded in a Web banner may result in higher consumer prefer-

    ences and higher willingness to pay. By contrast, when consumers

    are exposed to negative messages, they may form a lower expecta-

    tion about product quality. If the negative messages are accompa-

    nied by high-value anchor points, these conflicting elements will

    tend to result in a less favorable product attitude and purchase

    intention on the consumers’ part.

    Based on the above discussion, we assert the following addi-

    tional hypotheses:

    Hypothesis 3a   (The Product Attitude Interaction Hypothesis). The

    interaction effect between message framing and anchor point will

    be significant with respect to the participants’ attitude toward the

    target product.

    Hypothesis 3b   (The Intention to Buy Interaction Hypothesis). The

    interaction effect between message framing and anchor point will

    be significant with respect to the participants’ intention to buy.

    Hypothesis 3c   (The Willingness to Pay Interaction Hypothesis). The

    interaction effect between message framing and anchor point will

    be significant with respect to on participants’ willingness to pay.

     2.4. Consumers’ product knowledge

    The subjective knowledge is the consumers’ degree of confi-

    dence in his or her knowledge level (Brucks 1985). The role of 

    knowledge in anchoring effect studies has resulted in different

    conclusions. For example,  Wilson et al. (1996)  measured partici-

    pants’ knowledge by asking them to rate how knowledgeable they

    were about the target to be estimated. Their results demonstrated

    that the anchor manipulation is moderated by people’s knowledge.

    Specifically, the anchoring effect occurred only among people who

    with less knowledge. In addition,   Northcraft and Neale (1987)

    examined the anchoring effect for estimating the price of real

    estate properties with students amateurs and real estate agent

    360   C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    4/11

    experts. The results indicated that estimations provided by both

    subject populations were significantly biased toward the anchor

    point that was provided by the experimenter. As a result, we con-

    clude that experts are also susceptible to anchoring bias.

    When consumers make decisions, they may use price as a cue

    for product quality (Monroe and Krishnan, 1985). However, this

    price and perceived product quality relationship will be influenced

    by consumer knowledge ( Jacoby et al. 1971, Rao 1971, Shapiro

    1968). Novice buyers tend to use price as an indicator of quality

    to a greater extent than expert buyers. On the Internet, when con-

    sumers are planning to buy something, they may look for cues that

    are presented on Web pages. It has been suggested that consumers

    who have prior knowledge about products they want to purchase

    do not need to search for information from external sources

    (Brucks 1985), such as banner advertisement. Thus, we expect that

    people who know less about the target product are more suscepti-

    ble to anchoring bias than their counterparts who have a high level

    of product knowledge.

    Previous research has asserted that consumers who have less

    prior product knowledge tend to make decisions based on incom-

    plete experience (Bettman and Park 1980), while consumers with

    more product knowledge are able to develop a more complete cog-

    nitive structure to process and analyze the data they obtain (Alba

    and Hutchinson 1987).   Kühberger (1998)   suggested that, based

    on the meta-analysis results, the influence of message framing

    on novices and experts may differ. Experts are less likely to be

    influenced by the framing effect. We expect that people who are

    less knowledgeable about the target product will be more suscep-

    tible to framing bias than their counterparts who have more prior

    knowledge.

    Based on the above discussion, consumers with lower product

    knowledge are more susceptible to both framing and anchoring ef-

    fect. Thus, it is reasonable to expect that the joint effect of framing

    and anchoring will be different for participants with different prior

    knowledge levels. Consumers who are low in product knowledge

    will be more susceptible to the joint effect of framing and anchor-

    ing. We propose:

    Hypothesis 4   (The Knowledge Effect Hypothesis). The interaction

    effect between message framing and anchor point will be signif-

    icantly different for participants with different prior knowledge

    levels.

    3. Methods

     3.1. Design and subjects

    We recruited 318 undergraduate students from a several uni-

    versities in Taiwan, who were randomly assigned to one of the four

    conditions in a 2 (attribute framing: positive vs. negative) 2 (an-

    chor points: high vs. low) between-subjects design. Each partici-pant received a NT$100 (about US$3) McDonald coupon for their

    participation. There are 93, 70, 72 and 83 participants in the

    respective groups: Group 1 (negative framing, low anchor), Group

    2 (negative framing, high anchor), Group 3 (positive framing, low

    anchor) and Group 4 (positive framing, high anchor).

     3.2. Procedure and experimental manipulation

    Our experiment was conducted in a computer laboratory

    equipped with forty PCs. All the experimental stimuli and mea-

    surements were implemented on an experimental Web site. After

    entering the laboratory, the participants were instructed to enter

    the experimental Web site, which included three primary pages.

    The first page described a fictitious online store selling all types

    of electronic appliances. These included mobile phones, PDAs,

    and so on. On the second page, information of the target product

    is presented. The product is an electronic translator with a fictional

    brand name ‘‘YiShen.’’ In addition, the framing messages and an-

    chor points were also manipulated on the second page.

    We described the electronic translator as follow: ‘‘The YiShen

    electronic translator is the latest electronic translator, equipped

    with all the functions that can be found in most of the powerful

    products on the market. In addition, it offers built-in English-to-

    Chinese and Chinese-to-English two-way full-text translation.

    You can type in an English sentence and it will be translated into

    Chinese. You can also write Chinese in a sentence and YiShen will

    translate it into English. This device will serve different groups,

    especially students, foreign languages learners, businessmen, sec-

    retaries, and travelers.’’

    The key attribute of an electronic translator is the accuracy of its

    translation capability, and whether this is framed in a positive or a

    negative way. In the positive framing condition, the translator was

    described as: ‘‘The translator offers built-in English-to-Chinese and

    Chinese-to-English two-way full-text translation with up to 80%

    translation accuracy.’’ In the negative framing condition, the trans-

    lator was described in this way: ‘‘The translator offers built-in Eng-

    lish-to-Chinese and Chinese-to-English two-way full-text

    translation with up to a 20%   error rate. The manipulation of 80%

    and 20% in framing message was adopted from the attribute fram-

    ing design of  Levin et al. (2002), in which ‘‘ground beef’’ was de-

    scribed as ‘‘80% lean’’ or ‘‘20% fat.’’ Some other studies use 75%

    and 25% (Levin and Gaeth 1988) to describe their product attri-

    butes. Krishnamurthy et al. (2001) examined the effectiveness le-

    vel (10% vs. 30% vs. 50% vs. 70% vs. 90%) of message framing.

    Their results suggest the attribute framing effects are relatively

    stable across all of the different effectiveness levels.

    We manipulated the anchor in this study with a banner adver-

    tisement shown on the top of the product description page. To be

    more specific, the participants viewed the Web page, which in-

    cludes a description of the electronic translator. A banner embed-

    ded in the upper side of the same Web page illustrated anelectronic translator ad with a reference price as its anchor point

    of NT$38,800 (a high-value anchor of about US$1140) or NT$900

    (a low-value anchor of about US$26), based on the manipulation

    of the anchor points. The anchor points were selected based on

    the following steps. First, the market prices for the electronic trans-

    lator across four popular brands in Taiwan were surveyed. The re-

    sults showed that the lowest price was about NT$900, and the

    highest price was about NT$9700, with an average price at about

    NT$5000.

    Second, the target product in this study is an electronic transla-

    tor that is able to translate full English sentences into Chinese and

    vice versa. Although this translator has not been introduced to the

    market yet, the researchers interviewed five experts in electronic

    translator manufacturing for their opinions about the suggestedprices, if the translators were equipped with a full-text translation

    function. The average price suggested by the five experts was

    around NT$38,000.

    Third, there has been a study that manipulated the anchor point

    in the Internet environment (Wu et al. 2008a,b) and the low-value

    and high-value anchor points were NT$900 and NT$38,800, while

    the target product in their study was a mobile phone. Because

    the lowest price and the average suggested price of the translator

    in current study is quite close to the anchor points used in  Wu

    et al. (2008a,b), in which a significant anchoring effect was ob-

    served. Thus, we selected NT$900 and NT$38,800 as the low-value

    and high-value anchor points for our current study. (A screen shot

    of the experimental Web site with a high-value anchor point and

    positive framing condition is shown in  Fig. 1.)

    C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368   361

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    5/11

    Finally, the participants’ attitude toward the electronic transla-

    tor, their intention to buy it and their willingness to pay were mea-

    sured on the third page.

    After the participants entered the laboratory, they were in-

    structed to enter the experimental Web site, which randomly as-

    signed each subject to one of the four experimental conditions.

    The subjects in the first group were required to read the informa-

    tion about the electronic translator that described it in positive

    terms. At the same time, these subjects were exposed to a Web

    banner that embedded an electronic translator advertisement with

    a high-value anchor point of NT$38,800. Likewise, the subjects in

    the second group were exposed to positive product information

    and a Web banner with a low-value anchor point of NT$900. Par-

    ticipants in the third and fourth conditions were exposed to nega-

    tive framing treatments with the Web banner embedding either a

    high-value or a low-value anchor point.

     3.3. Experimental measures

    The instrument for this study was mainly based on existing

    scales developed in prior studies. Purchase intention was mea-

    sured using three items based on Ajzen and Fishbein (1980). Sim-

    ilar items were adapted and validated by  Moon and Kim (2001)

    and Kim et al. (2007). Subjects were asked to rate their intention

    to buy the electronic translator on a 7-point Likert scale in terms

    of the following three items: (1) I intend to buy this electronic

    translator. (2) I will suggest to my friends that they should buy this

    electronic translator (3) I will buy this electronic translator even if I

    have already have one. In addition, three items that were com-

    monly used to measure attitude in Internet shopping-related

    (Moon and Kim 2001) and framing-related (Levin et al. 2002) stud-

    ies were used in our instruments. Specifically, attitude was mea-

    sured by a three-item semantic differential on a seven-point

    scale, which included three pairs of adjectives: bad/good, unattrac-

    tive/attractive, and unlikable/likable.

    For our subsequent analysis, we averaged the ratings of the

    three items related to attitude and intention to buy into a singleitem each. In addition, we used an open question to measure the

    participants’ willingness to pay by asking the following question:

    ‘‘How much are you willing to pay for the electronic translator that

    you have just seen?’’ Finally, the participants’ subjective knowl-

    edge was measured based on the instrument used in   Park and

    Moon (2003). It includes three questions on a five-point scale:

    (1) Compared to other students, how familiar do you think you

    are with electronic translators? (2) Do you know what attributes

    of an electronic translator determine the functionality of an elec-

    tronic translator? (3) Do you think you can make a satisfactory

    purchase of a computer based on your own knowledge without an-

    other person’s help?

    4. Analysis

    4.1. Demographic information

    Demographic information on the participants in the current

    study indicated that the majority were males (62.3%) with a college

    or university educational level (98.1%), and 6–10 years of Internet

    experience (54.7%). Differences in the demographics of participantsin the above four groups were examined by conducting  v2 tests.

    The results indicated that there were no significant differences

    for all of the demographic variables collected in current study

    (gender:   v2 = 7.06, not significant; educational level:   v2 = 6.61,

    not significant; Internet experience:  v2 = 12.55, not significant).

    In addition, electronic dictionaries are a popular product among

    undergraduate students in Taiwan because most of them are using

    English textbooks and are required to read English papers. In the

    current study, about 92% of our subjects owned an electronic

    translator.

    4.2. Validity and reliability test 

    We assessed two types of validity: convergent validity and dis-

    criminant validity. Convergent validity of the constructs is assessed

    by examining the factor loadings of all indicators and the  average

    variance extracted   (AVE) from the measurements (Hair et al.

    1998). First, all of the indicator factor loadings must exceed the

    recommended value of 0.7. (See Table 1.) Second, the AVE values

    should be greater than the generally recognized 0.5 cut-off point,

    indicating that the majority of the variance is accounted for by

    the construct (Fornell and Larcker 1981). Table 2  shows that the

    average variance extracted by the measurements ranges from

    0.77 to 0.82, which is above the acceptable value.

    Discriminant validity indicates the extent to which a given con-

    struct is different from the other constructs (Bagozzi et al. 1991).

    One criterion for adequate discriminant validity is that the square

    root of the AVE for each construct should exceed the correlation

    shared between the construct and other constructs in the researchmodel (Fornell and Larcker 1981). The diagonal of  Table 2 contains

    the square root of the AVEs. All AVEs are greater than the off-diag-

    onal elements in the corresponding rows and columns, demon-

    strating discriminant validity. Thus, the above analysis confirms

    the reliability and validity of the survey instrument for further

    analysis.

    Table 1 provides the reliabilities for each multi-item construct

    calculated using Cronbach’s   a. The values range from 0.85 (for

    intention) to 0.94 (for attitude). The statistics indicate that all

    the constructs had a higher reliability coefficient than the bench-

    mark of 0.70 and this suggests a high internal reliability of the

    data.

    Fig. 1.   Screen shot of the experimental web site (high-value anchor, positive framing condition).

    362   C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    6/11

    4.3. The main and interaction effect of framing and anchoring 

    A 2 (attribute framing: positive vs. negative) 2 (anchor points:

    high vs. low) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test the

    hypotheses regarding the main effects of the attribute framing

    messages and anchors on participant attitude, intention to buy,

    and willingness to pay. The results indicate a significant main ef-

    fect for attribute framing on participant attitude and intention tobuy, and a significant main effect for anchor points on participant

    willingness to pay (see Table 2). Thus, Hypotheses 1, 2 and 4 were

    supported.

    The significant main effect for framing manipulation allowed us

    to further examine the means in each framing condition. The re-

    sults indicate that participants in the positive framing condition

    rated the product more favorably (M attitude = 5.11) and revealed a

    higher intention to buy (M intention = 4.03) than those in the negative

    framing condition (M attitude = 4.67,   M intention = 3.72). In addition, a

    significant anchoring effect was observed. Participants’ willingness

    to pay in the high-value and low-value anchoring conditions was

    also examined. The results indicate that participants on average

    are willing to pay more (M  = 8348.2) in the high-value anchoring

    condition than that in the low-value anchoring condition(M  = 3098.34).

    In addition, we observed a significant framing by anchoring

    interaction effect on all of the three dependent variables (see  Ta-

    ble 3). Thus, we conducted further analysis to examine the signif-

    icant interaction effect for framing based on the anchoring effect

    (see Fig. 2).

    The data presented in Fig. 2 indicate that the positive and neg-

    ative framing messages and high and low anchor points jointly

    influence the participants’ attitudes, intention to buy, and willing-

    ness to pay. Thus, Hypotheses 7–9 were supported by our data.

    4.4. The role of product knowledge in the framing and anchoring effect 

    The next step is to examine the role of the participants’ knowl-edge on the framing and anchoring effects. To analyze the moder-

    ating effect of knowledge on the framing and anchoring effect, we

    categorized the participants into high and low knowledge levels

    according to their scores on the instruments. A participant’s prod-

    uct category knowledge level was determined by percentile values.

    The overall knowledge scores were divided into two groups of 

    equal size split by the median, and the value of the cut point was

    3.34. Finally, we classified 160 of the participants as low in product

    knowledge, and another 158 people as high in product knowledge.The mean score in low knowledge group is 2.50 and 4.45 in high

    knowledge group. The difference between two groups is significant

    ( p < 0.001) and thus demonstrates that participants from two

    groups have different levels of product knowledge.

    We conducted three independent ANOVA tests for each knowl-

    edge level group in which the framing messages (positive and neg-

    ative) and anchor points (high and low) were the independent

    variables, and participants’ attitude, purchase intention and will-

    ingness to pay were the dependent variables. Our data analysis re-

    sults are shown in Table 4.

    As indicated in Table 4, framing has a significant main effect on

    attitude (F  = 4.24, p < 0.05), while the main effect of anchoring was

    observed on willingness to pay (F  = 45.11,  p < .001) for the low

    knowledge group. In addition, the interaction effect is significantwith respect to attitude (F  = 11.49,   p < 0.01) and intention

    (F  = 10.01,   p < 0.01) for the low knowledge group. For the high

    knowledge group, the main effect of framing is significant with re-

    spect to attitude (F  = 9.06,   p < 0.01) and intention (F  = 4.87,

     p  < 0.05), while a significant anchoring effect was also observed

    for willingness to pay (F  = 38.24,  p  < 0.001). The interaction effect

    is observed only for attitude (F  = 7.19, p < 0.01). These results seem

    to indicate that the high knowledge group is less influenced by

    framing and anchoring biases.

    We further examined the joint effect of different knowledge

    groups by drawing a set of figures. Figs. 3 and 4 illustrated the post

    hoc analysis for the low and high knowledge groups.

    As indicated in   Fig. 3, participants who know less about the

    electronic translator revealed significant anchoring effects whenthe product was described in positive terms, and high-value anchor

     Table 1

    Validity and reliability of attitude and intention scales.

    Attitude Intention Knowledge

    Likable/not likeable 0.94 –

    Attractive/unattractive 0.91 –

    Good/bad 0.87 –

    I will buy this electronic translator even if I have already have one – .85

    I intend to buy this electronic translator – 0.91

    I will suggest to my friends that they should buy this electronic translator – 0.88

    (1) Compared to other students, how familiar do you think you are with electronic translators? – – 0.888

    (2) Do you know what attributes of an electronic translator determine its functionality? – – 0.913

    (3) Do you think you can make a satisfactory purchase of a computer based on only your own knowledge,

    without another person’s help?

    – – 0.834

    Variance explained (%) 82.64 76.83 77.25

    Reliability 0.89 0.85 0.85

     Table 2

    Average variance extracted and correlations of constructs.

    AVEa Attitude Intention Knowledge

    Attitude 0.82 0.91b

    Intention 0.79 0.635 0.88Knowledge 0.77 0.109 0.207 0.88

    a Average variance extracted (AVE) = (Rstandardized loading2)/[(Rstandardized

    loading2) +Re j].b The shaded numbers in the diagonal row are square roots of the AVE.

    Off-diagonal elements are the correlations among constructs.

     Table 3

    Results of ANOVA test.

    Source Dependent variables

    Attitude Intention Willingness to pay

    Framing 11.989***

    5.825*

    0.699Anchoring 0.009 0.899 81.725***

    Framing anchoring 15.156*** 5.571* 3.959*

    *  p < 0.05.***  p < 0.001.

    C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368   363

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    7/11

    points resulted in more favorable responses and higher willingnessto pay. When the electronic translator was described in negative

    terms, a low-value anchor point in the Web banner induced higher

    intention to buy but a lower willingness to pay compared with the

    high-value anchor point group.

    The results shown in Fig. 4 are clear. The framing and anchoring

    messages were lessinfluentialfor participantswith more knowledge

    about the electronic translator. No anchoring effect occurs when the

    product is describedin positiveway. A lowanchor pointinthebanner

    will induce a more positive attitude toward the product but a lower

    willingness to pay when it is described in negative terms.

    5. Discussion

    The current study examines the effects of two well-known cog-nitive biases, the framing effect and the anchoring effect, on Inter-

    net buyer responses regarding attitudes, purchase intention andwillingness to pay. The joint influence of message framing and

    price anchor information has been rarely discussed in past studies.

    To test the proposed hypotheses, we employed a laboratory exper-

    iment and presented experimental stimuli via an experimental

    Web site. Framing messages that described the key attribute, the

    translation accuracy of an electronic translator, were presented

    to the participants along with either high-value or low-value an-

    chor points displayed in banner advertisements. We obtained sev-

    eral interesting findings observed from the experiment.

    First, we found a significant framing effect on participants’ atti-

    tude and purchase intention. Framing messages that describe the

    key attribute of the electronic translator in positive terms resulted

    in more favorable responses than negative terms did. This finding

    is consistent with the findings of prior studies of the attributeframing effect (Levin and Gaeth 1988, Kramer 1989, Loke and

    Fig. 2.   Participants’ responses in different conditions.

     Table 4

    Results of ANOVA test for the different product knowledge level groups.

    Product knowledge level group Source Dependent variables

    Attitude Intention Willingness to pay

    Low Framing 4.238* 2.503 0.122

    Anchoring 0.193 0.237 45.111***

    Framing anchoring 11.493*** 10.009** 0.337

    High Framing 9.058** 4.873* 1.661

    Anchoring 0.430 0.942 38.241***

    Framing anchoring 7.189** 1.359 3.877

    *  p < 0.05.**  p < 0.01.***  p < 0.001.

    364   C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    8/11

    Tan 1992, Levin et al. 1998, 2002) and it provides evidence regard-

    ing the influence of framing messages on Internet buyers’ judg-

    ment. However, the experimental results reveal no significant

    influence of the attribute framing message on the respondents’

    willingness to pay. Howard and Salkeld (2009)  are among a small

    number of researchers who have addressed the influence of attri-

    bute framing on consumer willingness to pay. In their study, pa-

    tients’ willingness to pay for screening for colorectal cancer

    Fig. 3.   Participants’ responses in different conditions (low knowledge group).

    Fig. 4.   Participants’ responses in different conditions (high product knowledge group).

    C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368   365

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    9/11

    under different attribute framing conditions was measured. The re-

    sults suggested that attribute framing messages influence patients’

    willingness to pay for the examination. The experimental context

    in Howard and Salkeld (2009) is medical examination. We believe

    that medical examination involves more risk-related issues be-

    cause it is related to personal health and disease concerns. Thus,

    the attribute framing messages are influential in a broader range

    than a less risky context like Internet shopping. Even though differ-

    ent results have been obtained in different studies, the influence of 

    attribute framing on people’s willingness to pay is worthy of closer

    investigation because the related empirical evidence regarding this

    issue is still limited.

    Second, participants’ willingness to pay revealed a significant

    anchoring effect and this result corresponds to the findings in pre-

    vious literature (Ariely et al. 2003, Green et al. 1998). Participants

    exposed to a higher anchor condition reveal a higher willingness to

    pay than that is seen under the low anchor condition. This result

    extends the anchoring literature in two aspects. First, the results

    provide direct evidence regarding the influence of the anchoring

    effect in the Internet shopping context. Second, most of the anchor-

    ing experiments in prior research were initiated by explicitly ask-

    ing people to compare the anchoring value to the target (a

    comparative judgment) followed by asking them to provide their

    estimate of the target value (the absolute estimate) (Tversky and

    Kahneman 1974, Strack and Mussweiler 1997, Wong and Kwong

    2000, Kristensen and Gärling 2000, Mussweiler and Strack 2000).

    However, this experimental design is inappropriate in the online

    retailing environment because one can hardly ask consumers to

    answer a comparative question before they make their purchase

    decisions. The experiment in present study is designed to embed

    the anchor points in a banner advertisement. Our results suggest

    that there is a significant anchoring effect that can be replicated

    by a research design that involves only the absolute estimate pro-

    cess. Thus, our research represents an advance in terms of the prac-

    tical design of an experiment to assess the influence of the

    anchoring effect in the Internet shopping context.

    The influence of anchor points on participants’ attitudes andbehavioral intentions was not observed though. We believe this

    is another type of evidence regarding anchor–target relevancy.

    The anchor point in this study is the reference price of product,

    which may actually be relevant or irrelevant to the target product.

    Measurements on the participants’ decisions include their attitude,

    behavioral intention and willingness to pay. Among these, only

    willingness to pay has the same dimension as the anchor, the ref-

    erence price. This argument has been supported by various anchor-

    ing studies (Chapman and Johnson, 1994,  Mussweiler and Strack

    2001). For example,   Strack and Mussweiler (1997)   demonstrated

    that the anchoring effect is weak when the anchor and the estimate

    focus on different dimensions of the same target (e.g., the height

    and the width of Brandenburg Gate).

    Further, the significant interaction effect for message framingby anchoring suggests that the congruence between the framing

    message and the anchor point strengthens the joint effect on the

    participants’ evaluations. For example, describing the product

    attribute in positive terms along with a high-value anchor point re-

    sults in more favorable responses than any other framing and

    anchoring combination. In addition, a negative attribute framing

    message together with a low-value anchor point results in more

    favorable responses than the combination of negative framing

    and a high-value anchoring advertisement. This might be ex-

    plained by the argument that consumers’ inferred beliefs about

    the product are based on other attribute beliefs (Erickson et al.

    1984, Huber and McCann 1982, John et al. 1986). Participants in

    our study infer product quality on the basis of price displayed in

    the advertisement, and so people who are exposed to a high-valueanchoring condition tend to believe that the electronic translator

    must be in high quality too. At the same time, then the electronic

    translator was described in positive terms, this seems to strength-

    en the participants’ belief regarding the quality of the product.

    Thus, we see that it results in the most favorable responses in

    terms of attitude, intention to buy and willingness to pay. Simi-

    larly, when the participants were exposed to the condition that fo-

    cused on the negative attributes of the electronic translator, less

    favorable responses are engendered. Under this condition, a low-

    value anchoring advertisement seems more plausible than a

    high-value anchor point, so more favorable responses are formed.

    In addition to the evaluation we offered regarding attitude and

    purchase intention, the results of our study also suggest that par-

    ticipants’ willingness to pay was mainly influenced by the anchor

    points, especially when the product attribute was described in po-

    sitive rather than in negative terms.

    The above discussion revealed that the attribute framing mes-

    sage will only influence consumers’ product attitudes and purchase

    intention, but not their willingness to pay. In addition, the adver-

    tisement will influence potential consumers’ willingness to pay

    but not their product attitudes and purchase intention when the

    reference price is the only information that is provided. Therefore,

    a better way is to combine the reference price with other positive

    attribute messages to strengthen the influence that the reference

    price will have on consumers’ responses.

    Finally, the results from this study suggest that the joint effect

    of framing and anchoring on consumers’ responses varies with

    the level of consumer product knowledge. Consumers with less

    product knowledge are more susceptible to the joint influence of 

    framing and anchoring than those with more product knowledge.

    Although prior research has suggested that subjects with lowprod-

    uct knowledge are more vulnerable to the framing and anchoring

    effects, their susceptibility to the joint effect is not fully under-

    stood. Our results provide evidence regarding this issue.

    6. Conclusion

    6.1. Implications

    We observed a significant framing effect that suggests the influ-

    ence of message framing can be replicated in the Internet purchase

    context, which has been neglected in prior research. Further, the

    experimental design of anchor manipulation in our experiment of-

    fers a practical way to influence participants’ price perception by

    embedding the anchor in banner ads without asking them to make

    comparative judgments before they make their final value esti-

    mates. This experimental design is more appropriate especially in

    the online retailing context, where one can hardly ask consumers

    to make any specific comparisons at all.

    The contributions of this study come from extending previous

    research on the framing effect and anchoring effect by considering

    the joint effect of these two well-known cognitive biases. The re-sults obtained in our experimental research suggest that the con-

    gruence of message framing and the anchor point enhances the

    effects of information presentation on consumer responses regard-

    ing product attitudes, purchase intention and willingness to pay. In

    addition, the results of this study provide evidence that online con-

    sumers perceive Web page elements holistically, rather than inde-

    pendently. Thus, designing all the elements in a congruent way

    (especially by presenting a positive framing message along with

    a high reference price) will be more effective in influencing online

    consumer responses. In this regard, our findings are helpful for

    practitioners to design messages related to product descriptions

    and price advertisements that will favor the online retailers.

    Further, this study contributes to the existing literature regard-

    ing the role of online consumer product knowledge, especially re-lated to the joint effects that have been evaluated in framing and

    366   C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    10/11

    anchoring studies. Previous research has suggested that consumers

    who are less knowledgeable will be more influenced by either the

    framing effect or the anchoring effect. Our results show that they

    are also susceptible to the joint influence of message framing and

    anchor points. Therefore, it is very important for the online retail-

    ers to understand their target consumers’ knowledge level. For

    consumers who are low in product knowledge, presenting positive

    product information with a high reference price will result in more

    favorable responses and higher willingness to pay. However, this

    might not be a good strategy with respect to consumers who are

    high in product knowledge. According to the elaboration likelihood

    model (Petty and Cacioppo 1983, 1986), when individuals are

    highly involved or more knowledgeable, a more central route to

    persuasion should be particularly effective, while a peripheral

    route should be better when consumer involvement is low or they

    are less knowledgeable (Petty et al. 1983). The positive and nega-

    tive framing messages and high and low anchoring effects in the

    current study are all peripheral cues and thus are more influential

    for consumers with less knowledge. Further research should incor-

    porate this concern into experimental designs that will be able to

    provide more insights regarding the joint effect of framing and

    anchoring among the different knowledge groups.

    6.2. Limitations

    There are a few limitations to this study that should be noted.

    First, according to Levin et al. (2002), there are three types of fram-

    ing effect but we only considered the influence of attribute framing

    messages and the joint effect of attribute framing and anchoring.

    The second type of framing effect, goal framing, has also been em-

    ployed in the marketing area. Thus, we think it will be beneficial to

    include the influence of this type of message framing and the joint

    effect of goal framing and anchoring on Internet consumers’ re-

    sponses in future research.

    Second, the anchor points embedded in the advertisement are

    anchors that are relevant to the electronic translator. However, will

    an irrelevant anchor like a mobile phone advertisement have thesame impact on participants’ responses? The answer to this ques-

    tion is worthwhile to understand. This question is derived based on

    the argument that some studies suggest that there is some seman-

    tic similarity between the comparative and absolute questions that

    is critical to the observation of the anchoring effect. For example,

    Strack and Mussweiler (1997) show that an anchor induces a very

    weak anchoring effect when the anchor and the estimation target

    focus on different dimensions of the same target (e.g., the height

    of Brandenburg Gate and the width of Brandenburg Gate). In con-

    trast, a strong anchoring effect occurs when the two questions

    asked about the same dimension of the same target. Thus, further

    study is needed to consider the role of anchor–target relevancy.

    Third, in the laboratory experiments involving undergraduate

    students who participated in our study, a key limitation is thatour findings may not apply to the broader Internet population.

    Fourth, although the laboratory experiments are useful to control

    for the possible factors that may affect the experimental subjects’

    responses, generalizing results to the real-world environment

    should only be done with caution. To test the framing effect and

    anchoring effect, we used only one product, and we only manipu-

    lated one product attribute and embedded one banner in the rele-

    vant experimental Web page. However, because online consumers

    actually have to deal with much more information in the online

    shopping context, future research should incorporate more prod-

    uct alternatives and information in their experimental designs.

    Finally, we should note that laboratory experiments reflect a

    methodology with high internal validity that offers precision for

    identifying key theorized relationships among different constructs.However, one of the related drawbacks of experiments is their lack

    of external validity. In our case, we only considered a limited num-

    ber of variables in our experiment. We focused only on the influ-

    ence of the framing and anchoring message on Internet shoppers’

    purchase decisions. Our experimental design would have been

    weaker if too many variables were included, even though this

    would have been more like the real-world setting. Finally, we

    should remind the reader that the Chinese–English electronic

    translator that we discussed in this article does not currently exist

    in the market. So the reader should be especially cautious in inter-

    preting the findings of this research for this reason also.

     Acknowledgment

    Fundingof thisresearchworkis supportedby theNationalScience

    Council (grant number, NSC96-2416-H-029-017-MY3), Taiwan.

    References

    Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M.   Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior .Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1980.

    Aladwani, A. M., and Palvia, P. C. Developing and validating an instrument for

    measuring user-perceived web quality. Information and Management , 39, 2002,467–476.

    Alba, J. W., and Hutchinson, J. W. Dimensions of consumer expertise.   Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 1987, 411–454.

    Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., and Prelec, D. Coherent arbitrariness: stable demand

    curves without stable preferences.  Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1, 2003,73–105.

    Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., and Phillips, L. W. Assessing construct validity in organizational

    research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 3, 1991, 421–548.Bettman, J. R., and Park, C. W. Effects of prior knowledge and experience and phase

    of the choice process on consumer decision processes.   Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 1980, 234–248.

    Biswas, A., and Blair, E. A. Contextual effects of reference prices in retail

    advertisements. Journal of Marketing , 55, 1991, 1–12.Brucks, M. The effects of product class knowledge on information search behavior.

     Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 1985, 1–16.Chapman, G. B., and Johnson, E. J. The limits of anchoring.  Journal of Behavioral

    Decision Making , 7, 1994, 232–242.Chapman, G. B., and Johnson, E. J. Incorporating the irrelevant: Anchors in

     judgments of belief and value. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, and D. Kahneman

    (eds.),  The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment: Heuristics and Biases, Cambridge

    University Press, New York, NY, 2002, 120–138.Cheng, F. F., Wu, C. S., and Yen, D. C. The effect of online store atmosphere on

    consumer’s emotional responses: an experimental study of music and color.

    Behavior and Information Technology, 28, 4, 2009, 323–334.Davis, H. L., Hoch, S. J., and Ragsdale, E. E. An anchoring and adjustment model of 

    spousal predictions. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 1986, 25–37.Erickson, G. M., Johansson, J. K., and Chao, P. Image variables in multi-attribute

    product evaluations: country-of-origin effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 11,1984, 694–699.

    Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. Evaluating structural equation models with

    unobservable variables and measurement error.   Journal of Marketing Research,18, 1, 1981, 39–50.

    Green, D., Jacowitz, K. E., Kahneman, D., and McFadden, D. Referendum contingent

    valuation, anchoring, and willingness to pay for public goods.   Resource andEnergy Economics, 20, 2, 1998, 85–116.

    Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., and Black, W. C.  Multivariate Data Analysis,5th edition. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1998.

    Häubl, G., and Murray, K. B. Preference construction and persistence in digital

    marketplaces: the role of electronic recommendation agents.   Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 1–2, 2003, 75–91.Häubl, G., and Trifts, V. Consumer decision making in online shopping

    environments: the effects of interactive decision aids.  Marketing , 19, 1, 2000,4–21.

    Howard, K., and Salkeld, G. Does attribute framing in discrete choice experiments

    influence willingness to pay? Results from a discrete choice experiment in

    screening for colorectal cancer.  Value in Health, 12, 2, 2009, 354–363.Huang, J., Jiang, X., and Tang, Q. An e-commerce performance assessment model: its

    development and an initial test on e-commerce applications in the retail sector

    of China.  Information and Management , 46, 2009, 100–108.Huber, J., and McCann, J. The impact of inferential beliefs on product evaluations.

     Journal of Marketing Research, 19, 1982, 324–333.Internet World States. Internet usage statistics: the Internet big picture, 2010.

    .

     Jacoby, J., Olson, J. C., and Haddock, R. A. Price, brand name, and product

    composition as determinants of perceived quality.   Journal of AppliedPsychology, 55, 1971, 570–579.

     John, D. R., Scott, C. A., and Bettman, J. R. Sampling data for covariation assessment:

    the effects of prior beliefs on search patterns.  Journal of Consumer Research, 13,1986, 38–47.

    C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368   367

    http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htmhttp://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm

  • 8/18/2019 6. Wu, C. S., & Cheng, F. F. (2011)

    11/11

     Johnson, R. D., and Levin, I. P. More than meets the eye: the effect of missing

    information on purchase evaluations.   Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 1985,169–177.

     Johnson, E. J., and Payne, J. W. Effort and accuracy in choice. Management Science, 31,1985, 395–414.

     Jones, K., and Leonard, L. N. K. Trust in consumer-to-consumer electronic commerce.

    Information and Management , 45, 2008, 88–95.Kim, S., andStoel, L. Dimensional hierarchyof retail website quality. Information and

    Management , 41, 2004, 619–633.Kim, D. J., Ferrin, D. L., and Rao, H. R. A trust-based consumer decision-making

    model in electronic commerce: the role of trust, perceived risk, and theirantecedents. Decision Support Systems, 44, 2, 2007, 544–564.

    Kramer, R. M. Windows of vulnerability or cognitive illusions? Cognitive processes

    and the nuclear arms race.   Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1989,79–100.

    Krishnamurthy, P., Carter, P., and Blair, E. Attribute framing and goal framing effects

    in health decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85, 2,2001, 382–399.

    Kristensen, H., and Gärling, T. The effects of anchor points and reference points on

    negotiation process and outcome.  Organizational Behavior and Human DecisionProcesses, 71, 1997, 85–94.

    Kristensen, H., and Gärling, T. Anchoring induced biases in consumer price

    negotiations. Journal of Consumer Policy, 23, 2000, 445–460.Kühberger, A. The influence of framing on risky decisions: a meta-analysis.

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 75, 1, 1998, 23–55.Lee, B. K., and Lee, W. N. The effect of information overload on consumer choice

    quality in an online environment.  Psychology and Marketing , 21, 3, 2004, 159–183.

    Levin, I. P. Associate effects of information framing.  Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 25, 1987, 85–86.

    Levin, I. P., and Gaeth, G. J. How consumers are affected by the framing of attribute

    information before and after consuming the product.   Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 1988, 374–378.

    Levin, I. P., Johnson, R. D., Russo, C. P., and Deldin, P. J. Framing effects in judgment

    tasks with varying amounts of information.  Organizational Behavior and HumanDecision Processes, 36, 1985, 362–377.

    Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., and Gaeth, G. J. All frames are not created equal: a

    typology and critical analysis of framing effects.   Organizational Behavior andHuman Decision Processes, 76, 2, 1998, 149–188.

    Levin, I. P., Gaeth, G. J., Schreiber, J., and Lauriola, M. A new look at framing effects:

    distribution of effect sizes, individual differences, and independence of types of 

    effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 1, 2002, 411–429.

    Liu, C. T., Du, T. C., and Tsai, H. H. A study of the service quality of general portals.

    Information and Management , 46, 2009, 52–56.Loke, W. H., and Tan, K. F. Effects of framing and missing information in expert and

    novice judgment. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 3, 1992, 187–190.

    Lopes, L.L., 1982. Toward a procedural theory of judgment. ONR final report,University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI.

    Mandel, N., and Johnson, E. J. When web pages influence choice: effects of visual

    primes on experts and novices.  Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 2, 2002, 235–245.

    McConnell, J. D. An experimental examination of the price–quality relationship.

     Journal of Business, 41, 1968, 439–444.MCConnell, J. D. Effects of pricing on perception of product quality. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 52, 1968, 13–19.Monroe, K.B., and Krishnan, R. The effect of price on subjective product evaluation.

    In J. Jacobyand J. Olsion (eds.), Perceived Quality: How Consumers View Stores andMerchandise. Lexington Books, Lexington, MA, 1985, 209–232.

    Moon, J. W., and Kim, Y. G. Extending the TAM for a World-Wide-Web context.

    Information and Management , 38, 2001, 217–230.Mussweiler, T., and Strack, F. Hypothesis-consistenttesting and semantic priming in

    the anchoring paradigm: a selective accessibility model.  Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 35, 1999, 136–164.

    Mussweiler, T., and Strack, F. Numeric judgment under uncertainty: the role of 

    knowledge in anchoring.   Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 2000,

    495–518.Mussweiler, T., and Strack, F. The semantics of anchoring.  Organizational Behavior 

    and Human Decision Processes, 86, 2, 2001, 234–255.Northcraft, G. B., and Neale, N. A. Experts, amateurs, and real estate: an anchoring-

    and-adjustment perspective on property pricing decisions.   OrganizationalBehavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 1987, 84–97.

    Park, D. W., and Lessig, V. P. Familiarity and its impact on consumer decision biases

    and heuristics. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 1981, 223–230.Park, C. W., and Moon, B. J. The relationship between product involvement and

    product knowledge: moderating roles of product type and product knowledge

    type. Psychology and Marketing , 21, 1, 2003, 977–997.Pavlou, P. A. Consumer acceptance of electronic commerce: integrating trust and

    risk with the technology acceptance model.   International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 7, 3, 2003, 101–134.

    Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., and Johnson, E. J. Adaptive strategy selection in decision

    making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14,

    1988, 534–552.Petty, R. E., and Cacioppo, J. T. Central and peripheral routes to persuasion:

    Application to advertising. In L. Percy (ed.), Advertising and Consumer Psychology,Health and Company, Washington, DC, 1983, 3–23.

    Petty, R. E., and Cacioppo, J. T.  Communication and Persuasion: Central and PeripheralRoutes to Attitude Change. Springer-Verlag, New York, NY, 1986.

    Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., and Schumann, D. Central and peripheral routes to

    advertising effectiveness: the moderating role of involvement.   Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 1983, 135–146.

    Rao, V.R. Salience of price in the perception of product quality: A multidimensional

    measurement approach. In N.H. Bordon Jr. (ed.),  Proceedings of the 1971 FallConference of American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, 1971, 571–577.

    Ritov, I. Anchoring in simulated competitive market negotiation.  OrganizationalBehavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 1, 1996, 16–25.

    Scitovsky, T. Some consequences of the habit of judging quality by price.  Review of Economic Studies, 12, 1944–1945, 100–105.

    Shapiro, B. The psychology of pricing.  Harvard Business Review, 46, 1968, 14–25.Shapiro, B. Price as a Communicator of Quality: An Experiment . Unpublished doctoral

    dissertation. Harvard University, 1970.

    Shiv, B., Edell, J. A., and Payne, J. W. Factors affecting the impact of negatively and

    positively framed ad messages.   Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 1997, 285–294.

    Shneiderman, B. Designing trust into online experiences.   Communications of the ACM , 43, 12, 2000, 57–59.

    Simon, H. A. Models of Man. Wiley, New York, NY, 1957.Slovic, P., and Lichtenstein, S. Comparison of bayesian and regression approaches to

    the study of information processing in judgment.   Organization Behavior andHuman Decision Processes, 6, 1971, 649–744.

    Smith, G. E. Framing in advertising and the moderating impact of consumer

    education. Journal of Advertising Research, 36, 5, 1996, 49–64.Strack, F., and Mussweiler, T. Explaining the enigmatic anchoring effect:

    mechanisms of selective accessibility.   Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 73, 1997, 437–446.

    Thorngate, W. Efficient decision heuristics. Behavioral Science, 25, 3, 1980, 219–225.Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases.

    Science, 185, 1974, 1124–1131.Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of 

    choice. Science, 211, 1981, 453–458.TWNIC. Broadband Internet usage in Taiwan. Taiwan Network Information Center,

    2009. .

    US Census Bureau. Quarterly retail e-commerce sales: 3rd quarter, 2008. .

    Wertenbroch, K., and Skiera, B. Measuring consumers’ willingness to pay at the

    point of purchase.  Journal of Marketing Research, 34, 2002, 228–241.Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. M., and Brekke, N. A new look at anchoring

    effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents.  Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 125, 1996, 387–402.

    Wong, K. F. E., and Kwong, J. Y. Y. Is 7300 m equal to 7.3 km? Same semantics but

    different anchoring effects.   Organizational Behavior and Human DecisionProcesses, 82, 2, 2000, 314–333.

    Wu, C. S., Cheng, F. F., and Lin, H. H. Exploring anchoring effect and the moderating

    role of repeated anchor in electronic commerce.   Behaviour and InformationTechnology, 27, 1, 2008, 31–42.

    Wu, C. S., Cheng, F. F., and Yen, D. C. The atmospheric factors of online storefront

    environment design: an empirical experiment in Taiwan.   Information andManagement , 45, 7, 2008, 493–498.

    Wu, C.S., Cheng, F.F., and Yen, D.C. The role of Internet buyer’s product familiarityand confidence in anchoring effect. Behaviour and Information Technology, 2011,doi:10.1080/0144929X.2010.510210.

    Zhang, Y., and Buda, R. Moderating effects of need for cognition on responses to

    positively versus negatively framed advertising messages. Journal of advertising ,28, 2, 1999, 1–15.

    368   C.-S. Wu, F.-F. Cheng/ Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 10 (2011) 358–368

    http://www.twnic.net.tw/download/200307/200307index.shtmlhttp://www.census.gov/mrts/www/data/html/08Q3.htmlhttp://www.census.gov/mrts/www/data/html/08Q3.htmlhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2010.510210http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2010.510210http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2010.510210http://www.census.gov/mrts/www/data/html/08Q3.htmlhttp://www.census.gov/mrts/www/data/html/08Q3.htmlhttp://www.twnic.net.tw/download/200307/200307index.shtml