69662253

Upload: laly-hernandez

Post on 02-Apr-2018

227 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    1/8

    OPIOIDS, SUBSTANCE ABUSE &ADDICTIONS SECTION

    Original Research Article

    Longitudinal Observation of Changes in PainSensitivity during Opioid Tapering in Patientswith Chronic Low-Back Painpme_1276 1720..1726

    Haili Wang, MD, Michael Akbar, MD,

    Nina Weinsheimer, MD, Simone Gantz, Dipl-SozWiss, and Marcus Schiltenwolf, MD

    Department of Orthopedics, Trauma Surgery and

    Paraplegiology, University Hospital of Heidelberg,

    Heidelberg, Germany

    Reprint requests to: Haili Wang, MD, Division of PainManagement, Department of Orthopedic Surgery,University of Heidelberg, Schlierbacher Landstrasse200a, 69118 Heidelberg, Germany. Tel:+49-6221-966505; Fax: +49-6221-966230; E-mail:[email protected].

    There are no financial or other relationships that mightlead to a conflict of interest.

    The first two authors contributed equally to thisproject.

    Abstract

    Objective. Several studies have shown that expo-sure to opioids for short or long periods alters painsensitivity. Little is known about changes in painsensitivity during and after tapering of long-termprescribed opioid treatment in chronic low-back

    pain (cLBP) patients.

    Design. The goal of this prospective longitudinalstudy was to investigate pain sensitivity in a homo-geneous patient population (cLBP patients only)after tapering of long-term (17 months) opioid useand to monitor the changes in pain sensitivity for 6months.

    Methods. Pain sensitivity (thermal sensation andthermal pain thresholds in low back and nondomi-nant hand) was measured by quantitative sensorytesting (QST) at 1 day before (T1), 3 weeks after (T2),

    and 6 months after the start of opioid tapering (T3) in

    35 patients with both cLBP and opioid medication(OP), 35 opioid-nave cLBP patients (ON), and 28individuals with neither pain nor opioid intake (HC).

    Results. Significant differences in heat pain thresh-olds were found among the three groups at allthree time points (T1: P= 0.001, T2: P= 0.015, T3:P= 0.008), but not between the two patient groups.OP patients showed lower cold pain thresholdsat T2 than ON patients and HC. At T3, the heatpain thresholds of OP patients still remained lowerthan HC (P= 0.017), while those of ON patients werenormalized.

    Conclusions. Our findings suggest that long-termuse of opioids does not reduce pain sensitivity incLBP patients; opioid tapering may induce briefhyperalgesia that can be normalized over a longerperiod.

    Key Words. Opioid Tapering; Pain Sensitivity;Chronic Low-back Pain; Quantitative SensoryTesting

    Introduction

    To date, evidence of long-term amelioration of pain andimprovements in function attributable to opioid therapy is

    lacking. Meta-analyses and systemic reviews of random-ized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials of opioids for

    chronic noncancer pain found these trials were of briefduration (average 5 weeks), subject to publication bias

    from pharmaceutical company sponsorship, and limited

    by eligibility criteria that excluded subjects with a historyof substance abuse [14]. Furthermore, patients on long-

    term opioid treatment often complain of symptoms suchas agitation, sleep disturbance, nausea, emesis, consti-

    pation, and cognitive disturbance, even hyperalgesia[510]. These studies showed that exposure to opioids

    alters pain sensitivity. Angst et al. showed direct evi-dence in humans that short-term administration of an

    Pain Medicine 2011; 12: 17201726Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

    1720

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    2/8

    opioid can enhance hyperalgesia, as observed during

    withdrawal, and they pointed to a potential role of the

    N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-receptor system in medi-ating such a hyperalgesic response. Compton et al.

    confirmed the presence of hyperalgesia in four healthynon-opioid-dependent men using the acute opioid physi-

    cal dependence (APD) model and showed that painthresholds and tolerance to the cold pressor uniformly

    decreased across all APD induction methods. Thesefindings provide initial support for the existence of

    opioid-induced hyperalgesia, which has been conceptu-

    alized as a coexisting opponent process to opioid-induced analgesia and proposed to be an alternative

    explanation for the development of analgesic toleranceto opioids.

    To date, however, little is known about changes in pain

    sensitivity during and after opioid tapering in chronic low-back pain (cLBP) patients treated with long-term opioids.

    A previous study by our group compared the intensity ofpain before and after multidisciplinary pain therapy in

    patients with cLBP who underwent opioid withdrawal. Thepain intensity via visual analog scale was significantly

    decreased at discharge [11].

    Another study of hyperalgesia under opioid withdrawaldemonstrated that 714 days after withdrawal 12 patients

    showed decreased tolerance for cold in the cold pressortask, although their quality of life had significantly

    improved [12]. A study using quantitative sensory testing(QST) suggested that the pain thresholds of pre-heroin-

    dependent addicts were still different several months after

    detoxification [13]. In contrast, some studies indicate that

    the pain threshold values of chronic pain patients treatedwith opioids do not differ significantly from those of chronicpain patients treated with non-opioid analgesics [14].

    Therefore, longitudinal studies are needed to resolve the

    question of whether long-term opioid therapy and opioidtapering causes the increase in pain sensitivity reported in

    chronic pain patients. The goal of this prospective longi-tudinal study was to investigate in a homogeneous patient

    population (cLBP patients only) the long-term (17 months)

    effects of opioid analgesics on pain sensitivity and tofollow for 6 months the changes in pain sensitivity during

    opioid tapering and withdrawal. We used the QST tech-nique to assess pain sensitivity over a period of 6 months.

    To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study of 6months duration regarding pain sensitivity during and

    after opioid tapering.

    Methods

    Study Subjects

    The study was approved by the ethics committee of theUniversity of Heidelberg, Germany and was funded by the

    research fund of the Department of Orthopedic Surgery ofthe University of Heidelberg. The subjects were recruited

    from the Department of Orthopedic Surgery, and all gaveinformed consent.

    Three groups of subjects were studied: patients with both

    cLBP and opioid therapy (group 1, N= 35); those whohad cLBP but had not been on opioid therapy for at least

    3 months (group 2, N= 35); and healthy controls withneither pain nor opioid therapy (group 3, N= 28).

    The inclusion criteria for patients with cLBP were as

    follows: 1) age between 20 and 70 years; 2) a history of at

    least 6 months of chronic myofascial low-back painwithout radicular sensory or motor deficits or radicular

    pain symptoms (herniation or disc protrusion excluded bymagnetic resonance imaging), but sick leave amounting to

    less than 6 months within the 12 months immediatelybefore enrolment in this study (grade II chronification

    according to von Korff [15]); 3) opioid therapy was definedas intake of a morphine equivalent dose of at least 30 mg

    per day every day for more than 3 months. For the stan-

    dardized data analysis, we used the following conversionratios between an oral dose of morphine and other opioid

    analgesics: 1 mg morphine = 0.65 mg oxycodone,0.25 mg methane, 5 mg tilidine, 0.01 mg fentanyl,

    0.13 mg hydromorphone, 5 mg tramadol [16,17]; and 4)non-opioid pain medication in the group of opioid-nave

    patients was allowed.

    The exclusion criteria for both groups were: 1) sensorydeficits because of diabetes, alcoholic neuropathy, spinal

    stenosis, or severe thyroid, liver, or kidney disease; 2)

    interventional pain management procedures that mightalter QST responses, including neuraxial or local anes-

    thetic block, within the previous 3 months; 3) major psy-

    chiatric disorders requiring recent hospitalization, such asschizophrenia or psychosis; and 4) infection or acute injuryat the QST site.

    Study Procedure

    The study lasted 6 months (Figure 1). Before admission

    into the study, each patient filled in a questionnaire giving

    information on demographic data, pain location, painintensity on a numeric rating scale (NRS: 0 = no pain,10 = worst pain imaginable), pain pattern, pain duration,clinical diagnosis, and medications, including opioid and

    non-opioid analgesics. During the whole study, all patientskept records of the medications they used.

    Opioid Tapering Procedure

    Upon admission to the study, the dosage of opioid prepa-ration was halved every 3 days until the patients became

    opioid clean. In the first week, patients usually complainedof symptoms such as nausea, weakness, increased

    sweating, or sleep disturbance. During the opioid taper-ing, doxepine was applied for damping of withdrawal

    symptoms until 2 weeks after the patient had becomeopioid clean. All patients had stopped the use of opioids

    by T2, and 85% of them remained opioid clean 6 monthsafter opioid withdrawal. Only data from these 85% were

    1721

    Pain Sensitivity under Opioid Therapy

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    3/8

    used for statistical analysis (group 1, N= 17; group 2,N= 22, group 3, N= 18); the remaining 15% of patientswho continued to use opioids at the 6-month time point

    were considered as dropouts.

    QST

    QST was performed using the Thermal Sensory Analyzer

    (TSA) II (Medoc Inc., Ramat Ishai, Israel) and was carriedout at three time pointsat admission into the study (T1)

    and at 3 weeks (T2) and 6 months (T3) thereafterin a

    quiet room maintained at 2025C. A contact thermode(3 3 cm) was gently attached and secured with a bandonto the lower back (bilaterally) and the nondominanthand in each subject. The baseline temperature was

    32C. The cutoff temperatures were 0C and 50C. Eachsubject was told to stop stimulation as soon as he/she felt

    a change in temperature. The measured parameters were:

    cold sensation (CS), warm sensation (WS), cold painthreshold (CP), and heat pain threshold (WP).

    Testing of CS and WS

    Subjects were instructed to stop stimulation as soon asthey perceived an increase or decrease in temperature

    from the baseline (32C). The testing was performed fourtimes in each patient.

    Testing of CP and WP

    Subjects were instructed to stop stimulation as soon asthey perceived a painful sensation as temperature

    decreased (CP) or increased (WP) from the baseline tem-perature of 32C. The testing was performed three times

    Intention-to-treat

    99 patients

    98 patients recruited

    1 patient refused participation

    35 opioid-positive

    (Group 1)

    35 opioid-nave

    (Group 2)

    28 healthy controls (HC)

    (Group 3)

    T1:

    34 patients

    1 patient excluded

    (spinal claudication)

    2 patients dropped out

    (invalid QST values)

    T1:

    33 patients

    T2:

    32 patients

    2 patients dropped out 4 patients dropped out

    T2:

    29 patients

    T3:

    20 patients

    12 patients dropped out

    T3:

    19 patients

    10 patients dropped out

    T1:

    22 HC

    6 subjects dropped out

    (invalid QST values)

    Figure 1 Flow diagram of study participants during 6-month follow-up. T1 = day 0; T2 = day 21; T3 = day

    180; HC = healthy controls; QST= quantitative sensory testing.

    1722

    Wang et al.

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    4/8

    in each patient. The temperature at which the subject

    stopped the stimulation was recorded as threshold tem-

    perature (C).

    Statistical Analysis

    According to the ShapiroWilk test, the values of QST

    were not normally distributed, so nonparametric testswere used. As an overall test the Friedman test was

    used for comparisons of the differences among the QSTresults among three time points. The Wilcoxon test

    was used to compare differences of the QST results

    between two time points. The overall KruskalWallis testwas used to compare the three unpaired groups, the

    MannWhitney U-test for two groups. Spearmans rhoand Pearsons correlation coefficients were used to

    examine the relationships between QST value and depres-sion, duration of pain, duration of opioid medication, dose

    of opioids, and subjective pain intensity. Only completedatasets are included. All tests were performed with Sta-

    tistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 17.0 soft-ware for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). For each

    statistical test, the significance level was set at P 0.05.

    Bonferroni correction was used to correct for multipletesting.

    Results

    Table 1 shows characteristics of the subjects in all threegroups. Overall, there were no statistically significant dif-

    ferences in age and sex among the groups. The depres-sion rate, the mean duration of pain, and pain medication

    with nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs and antidepres-

    sants were comparable between OP and ON patients.The only differentiating characteristic between these two

    groups was the intake of opioids.

    There were significant differences in heat pain thresholdsat all three time points (KruskalWallis test: T1, P= 0.001;

    T2, P= 0.015; T3, P= 0.008) among all three groups.However, this difference was confirmed only between

    group 1 and group 3 at all time points (MannWhitney test:T1, P= 0.007; T2, P= 0.007; T3, P= 0.017) and betweengroups 2 and 3 at T1 (P= 0.001) and at T2 (0.002); therewas no difference between the two patient groups at any

    time point (Figure 2A).

    At T2, significant differences in CPs were found among allthree groups (KruskalWallis test: P= 0.003 for hand,P= 0.023 for left low back). The opioid-positive patients ofgroup 1 had a significant lower threshold to cold stimulithan patients in group 2 (P= 0.004 for hand) and healthycontrols (P= 0.001 for hand, P= 0.021 for left low back).Opioid-nave patients had a pain threshold similar to that

    of healthy controls (Figure 2B).

    Compared with T1, group 1 patients reacted to warmstimuli significantly faster at T2 (P= 0.029), while group 2patients did not change their pain profile. At T3, group 1

    patients showed a further increase in speed of reaction towarm stimuli (P= 0.01) and a further lowering of CP(P= 0.026) relative to T2. Group 2 patients showed sig-nificant changes in heat pain thresholds (P= 0.028) at T3compared with T1.

    No correlations between QST values and depression, painduration, opioid dose, or opioid duration were found. NRS

    scores showed moderately significant correlations withthermal sensation, but not with pain thresholds. Statisti-

    cally significant correlations were found between sensa-

    tion of warmth in the hand and both age (r= 0.308,P= 0.015) and sex (r= 0.271, P= 0.003).

    Discussion and Conclusions

    This prospective study is the first to demonstrate the

    changes of pain sensitivity in patients with cLBP underlong-term opioid use and opioid tapering in a longitudinal

    Table 1 Characteristics of study subjects before therapy

    Group 1 (N= 20) Group 2 (N= 19) Group 3 (N= 22) P-value

    Age (year) 49.3 12.4 (3169) 46.6 8.9 (2865) 47.1 10.3 (3366) P> 0.05

    Female 40.0% 63.2% 59.1% P> 0.05

    Male 60.0% 436.8% 40.9%Diagnosis cLBP cLBP No pain

    Depression 45.5% 42.5% 5% P> 0.05*

    Other psychic comorbidity 20.5% 18.2% 0 P> 0.05*

    Duration of pain (years) 10.3 10.5 7.3 5.6 0 P> 0.05*

    Pain intensity (NRS) 6.23 1.9 5.3 2.3 0 P> 0.05*

    Duration of opioid intake (months) 16.9 21.6 0 0

    Morphine equivalent dose (mg/d) 107.3 77 0 0

    Other pain medications NSAIDs (20%) NSAIDs (17.1%) 0 P> 0.05*

    Antidepressants (17.1%) Antidepressants (3%)

    * Comparison between patient groups.

    cLBP = chronic low-back pain; NSAIDs = nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs; NRS = numeric rating scale.

    1723

    Pain Sensitivity under Opioid Therapy

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    5/8

    setting. We found 1) that long-term (average 17 months)opioid use did not cause increased or reduced pain sen-

    sitivity and 2) that opioid tapering leads to significantlydecreased pain thresholds as early as 3 weeks after opioid

    tapering, but this effect is no longer present 6 months

    later.

    To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal documen-

    tation of opioid tapering-induced hyperalgesia in a homo-geneous patient population with cLBP under chronic

    opioid medication with such a long observation period(mean 17 months). This finding correlates with those of

    Prosser et al. [13] and indicates opioid-induced enhance-ment of hyperalgesia after opioid cessation. The mecha-

    nisms of opioid tapering-induced hyperalgesia are poorlydescribed, especially in humans. One possible mecha-

    nism of opioid tapering-induced hyperalgesia is the acti-

    vation of central neuroimmune responses as described byRaghavendra et al. [18], who demonstrated that chronic

    administration of morphine to sham-operated rats acti-vated spinal glia and upregulated proinflammatory cytok-

    ines (interleukin [IL]-1b, IL-6, and tumor necrosis factor-a),which could mediate hyperalgesia. This neuroimmune

    activation was further enhanced in nerve-injured rats after

    chronic morphine treatment. Spinal inhibition of pro-inflammatory cytokines restored acute morphine antinoci-

    ception in nerve-injured rats and also significantly reversedthe development of morphine tolerance and withdrawal-

    induced hyperalgesia and allodynia in nerve-injured orsham-operated rats. Two years later, the same research

    group investigated the effect of propentofylline, a glialmodulator, on the expression of analgesic tolerance and

    withdrawal-induced hyperalgesia in chronic morphine-treated rats. They found that chronic morphine adminis-

    tration through repeated subcutaneous injections inducedglial activation and enhanced pro-inflammatory cytokine

    levels in the lumbar spinal cord [19].

    Another study showed that during opioid abstinence-induced withdrawal, administration of glutamate receptorantagonists and the kappa agonist to the nucleus raphe

    magnus suppressed the withdrawal-induced hyperalge-sia, which is thought to be mediated by activation of those

    pain-facilitating neurons during opioid withdrawal [20].

    Finally, the withdrawal of chronic opioid use could inducesensitization of spinal NMDA receptors, presynaptic/

    central glutamate receptors, and peripheral a2-adrenergic/adenosine receptors, thus decreasing the painthresholds to stimuli and causing the hyperalgesia.

    Our results showed that the pain thresholds of opioid-

    positive and opioid-nave patients did not differ from eachother at T1. Compared with healthy subjects, both opioid-

    positive and opioid-nave patients had significantlydecreased pain thresholds to heat stimuli. This indicates

    that low-back pain itself might induce peripheral sensiti-

    zation [21], which seems not to be counteracted by opioidmedication, as shown in our study and by other authors

    [22,23]. In contrast, long-term opioid therapy (in our studythe mean duration of opioid use was 17 months) intensi-

    fied the peripheral sensitization rather than reducing it.

    Potential confounding factors were considered in thisstudy. We tested both healthy controls and opioid-nave

    WP

    40

    A

    B

    T1 T2 T3

    T1 T2 T3

    41

    42

    43

    44

    45

    46

    47

    Time

    TemperatureC

    group 1

    group 2

    group 3

    *** ** ** **

    *

    CP

    0

    2

    4

    6

    8

    10

    12

    14

    16

    18

    Time

    TemperatureC

    group 1

    group 2

    group 3

    ** **

    *

    Figure 2 Pain thresholds as measured by quantitative

    sensory testing in patients with chronic low-back pain.

    * P< 0.05; ** P< 0.01; T1 = day 0; T2 = day 21;

    T3 = day 180. Group 1: Patients with chronic low-back

    pain and under opioid therapy before study; group 2:

    patients with chronic low-back pain without opioid

    therapy before study; group 3: healthy controls. (A)

    Heat pain thresholds (WP). Both patient groups

    showed significantly lower WP than healthy controls at

    T1 and T2. At T3, group 1 still had lower WP than group

    3 but in group 2 pain sensitivity had improved signifi-

    cantly after 6 months. (B) Cold pain thresholds (CP). AtT2 but not at T1 or T3, the CP were significantly

    different between groups 1 and 2 and between groups

    1 and 3. Group1 patients were more sensitive to cold

    stimuli, and perceived the same cold stimuli as pain

    earlier, than those in group 3.

    1724

    Wang et al.

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    6/8

    patients and used their baseline QST responses to ensure

    differentiation of QST findings because of opioid usage or

    to preexisting low-back pain. The palm of the hand wasadditionally chosen as a control region to enable detection

    of any opioid-induced hyperalgesia independent of preex-isting low-back pain [24].

    To rule out any influence of fluctuations in ambient tem-

    perature on QST results, the temperature of the roomwhere QST was carried out was kept stable at 2025C

    during testing [25,26].

    Age and sex influenced the perception of warmth in our

    study, so we considered both as potential confoundingfactors. All three groups turned out to be comparable for

    age and sex. Both of the two persons conducting thetests were female, excluding sex of the test person as

    confounding factor [27,28]. Duration of pain, pain intensity,comorbidity with depression, and use of antidepressants

    were all similar in both patient groups. We found no cor-

    relation between depression score and QST parametersat any time point.

    We found in this study that subjective pain intensity cor-

    related with objective thermal perception, in agreementwith our previous results [11] Correspondingly, we

    observed that heat pain thresholds of opioid-positivepatients did not change over the whole study, even at the

    end of the follow-up period when patients were alreadyopioid clean. This indicates that it is not pain thresholds

    with QST but rather thermal sensation that determinespatients subjective pain intensity, as previous studies

    have reported [19,29].

    In conclusion, this study makes two important observa-tions. First, it confirms that cLBP patients demonstrateheat hyperalgesia when nociception is measured using

    QST. Opioid exposure additionally changes the thermalsensation in this patient population. Second, it shows that

    opioid tapering may induce cold hyperalgesia shortly afterdose reduction but that this effect disappears after long-

    term (6 months in our study) abstinence from opioids.

    Acknowledgments

    We would like to acknowledge financial support from the

    Orthopedic University Hospital, University of Heidelberg,Germany.

    References

    1 Kalso E, Edwards JE, Moore RA, McQuay HJ. Opioidsin chronic non-cancer pain: Systematic review of effi-

    cacy and safety. Pain 2004;112(3):37280.

    2 Furlan AD, Sandoval JA, Mailis-Gagnon A, Tunks E.

    Opioids for chronic noncancer pain: A meta-analysisof effectiveness and side effects. CMAJ 2006;

    174(11):158994.

    3 Martell BA, OConnor PG, Kerns RD, et al. Systematicreview: Opioid treatment for chronic back pain: Preva-

    lence, efficacy, and association with addiction. AnnIntern Med 2007;146(2):11627.

    4 Moore RA, McQuay HJ. Prevalence of opioid adverse

    events in chronic non-malignant pain: Systematicreview of randomised trials of oral opioids. Arthritis Res

    Ther 2005;7(5):R104651.

    5 Angst MS, Koppert W, Pahl I, Clark DJ, Schmelz M.Short-term infusion of the mu-opioid agonist remifen-

    tanil in humans causes hyperalgesia during with-

    drawal. Pain 2003;106(12):4957.

    6 Compton P, Athanasos P, Elashoff D. Withdrawalhyperalgesia after acute opioid physical dependence

    in nonaddicted humans: A preliminary study. J Pain2003;4(9):5119.

    7 Celerier E, Rivat C, Jun Y, et al. Long-lasting hyperal-

    gesia induced by fentanyl in rats: Preventive effect of

    ketamine. Anesthesiology 2000;92(2):46572.

    8 Rivat C, Laulin JP, Corcuff JB, et al. Fentanyl enhance-ment of carrageenan-induced long-lasting hyperalge-

    sia in rats: Prevention by the N-methyl-D-aspartatereceptor antagonist ketamine. Anesthesiology 2002;

    96(2):38191.

    9 Koppert W, Sittl R, Scheuber K, et al. Differentialmodulation of remifentanil-induced analgesia and

    postinfusion hyperalgesia by S-ketamine and clonidine

    in humans. Anesthesiology 2003;99(1):1529.

    10 Chou R, Fanciullo GJ, Fine PG, et al. Opioids for

    chronic noncancer pain: Prediction and identificationof aberrant drug-related behaviors: A review of theevidence for an American Pain Society and American

    Academy of Pain Medicine clinical practice guideline.J Pain 2009;10(2):13146.

    11 Gartner CM, Schiltenwolf M. [Limited efficacy of

    opioids in chronic musculoskeletal pain]. Schmerz

    2004;18(6):50614.

    12 Younger J, Barelka P, Carrol I, et al. Reduced coldpain tolerance in chronic pain patients following opioid

    detoxification. Pain Med 2008;9(8):115863.

    13 Prosser JM, Steinfeld M, Cohen LJ, et al. Abnormalheat and pain perception in remitted heroin depen-

    dence months after detoxification from methadone-maintenance. Drug Alcohol Depend 2008;95(3):

    23744.

    14 Fillingim RB, Doleys DM, Edwards RR, Lowery D.

    Clinical characteristics of chronic back pain as a func-tion of gender and oral opioid use. Spine (Phila Pa

    1976) 2003;28(2):14350.

    15 Von Korff M, Ormel J, Keefe FJ, Dworkin SF. Gradingthe severity of chronic pain. Pain 1992;50(2):13349.

    1725

    Pain Sensitivity under Opioid Therapy

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    7/8

    16 Reznikov I, Pud D, Eisenberg E. Oral opioid adminis-

    tration and hyperalgesia in patients with cancer or

    chronic nonmalignant pain. Br J Clin Pharmacol2005;60(3):3118.

    17 Lawlor PG, Turner KS, Hanson J, Bruera ED. Dose

    ratio between morphine and methadone in patientswith cancer pain: A retrospective study. Cancer

    1998;82(6):116773.

    18 Raghavendra V, Rutkowski MD, DeLeo JA. The role ofspinal neuroimmune activation in morphine tolerance/

    hyperalgesia in neuropathic and sham-operated rats.J Neurosci 2002;22(22):99809.

    19 Raghavendra V, Tanga FY, DeLeo JA. Attenuation

    of morphine tolerance, withdrawal-induced hyperalge-sia, and associated spinal inflammatory immune

    responses by propentofylline in rats. Neuropsychop-harmacology 2004;29(2):32734.

    20 Bie B, Pan ZZ. Presynaptic mechanism for

    anti-analgesic and anti-hyperalgesic actions of kappa-opioid receptors. J Neurosci 2003;23(19):72628.

    21 Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L. Peripheral and

    central sensitization in musculoskeletal pain disorders:

    An experimental approach. Curr Rheumatol Rep2002;4(4):31321.

    22 Deyo RA, Mirza SK, Turner JA, Martin BI. Overtreating

    chronic back pain: Time to back off? J Am Board FamMed 2009;22(1):628.

    23 Deshpande A, Furlan A, Mailis-Gagnon A, Atlas S,

    Turk D. Opioids for chronic low-back pain. Cochrane

    Database Syst Rev 2007;(3):CD004959.

    24 Mao J. Opioid-induced abnormal pain sensitivity:

    Implications in clinical opioid therapy. Pain 2002;100(3):2137.

    25 Meh D, Denislic M. Quantitative assessment ofthermal and pain sensitivity. J Neurol Sci 1994;

    127(2):1649.

    26 Hirosawa I, Dodo H, Hosokawa M, et al. Physiologicalvariations of warm and cool sense with shift of envi-

    ronmental temperature. Int J Neurosci 1984;24(34):2818.

    27 Levine FM, De Simone LL. The effects of experimenter

    gender on pain report in male and female subjects.

    Pain 1991;44(1):6972.

    28 Feine JS, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH. Sex

    differences in the perception of noxious heat stimuli.

    Pain 1991;44(3):25562.

    29 Kelly KG, Cook T, Backonja MM. Pain ratings at thethresholds are necessary for interpretation of quanti-

    tative sensory testing. Muscle Nerve 2005;32(2):17984.

    1726

    Wang et al.

  • 7/27/2019 69662253

    8/8

    Copyright of Pain Medicine is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to

    multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users

    may print, download, or email articles for individual use.