a head start on reading for children in a head start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… ·...

38
APPROVED: Janet Ellis, Major Professor Smita Mehta, Committee Member Mary Estes, Committee Member Richard Smith, Chair of the Department of Behavior Analysis Thomas Evenson, Dean of the College of Public Affairs and Community Service Michael Monticino, Interim Dean of the Robert B. Toulouse School of Graduate Studies A HEAD START ON READING FOR CHILDREN IN A HEAD START PRESCHOOL PROGRAM Kristin R. Osley, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS May 2009

Upload: others

Post on 24-Jun-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

APPROVED: Janet Ellis, Major Professor Smita Mehta, Committee Member Mary Estes, Committee Member Richard Smith, Chair of the Department of

Behavior Analysis Thomas Evenson, Dean of the College of

Public Affairs and Community Service

Michael Monticino, Interim Dean of the Robert B. Toulouse School of Graduate Studies

A HEAD START ON READING FOR CHILDREN IN A

HEAD START PRESCHOOL PROGRAM

Kristin R. Osley, B.S.

Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

MASTER OF SCIENCE

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS

May 2009

Page 2: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool

program. Master of Science (Behavior Analysis), May 2009, 33 pp., 2 figures, 1 table,

references, 56 titles.

Literacy is a fundamental to all areas of learning. Early reading experiences prior

to elementary school and kindergarten years are critical factors for later reading

success. This study evaluated the effect Direct Instruction® reading procedures vs.

Scholastic Early Childhood Program® reading procedures on the production of letter

names, letter sounds, CV and CVC blends by preschool-aged students in a Head Start

program. Results showed the intervention group improved in all areas, while the control

group improved only in letter naming and letter sounds. This study discusses reading as

a behavioral cusp as well as limitations, and recommendations for future research.

Page 3: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

ii

Copyright 2009

by

Kristin R. Osley

Page 4: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank my friends and family for always being there throughout my

graduate career. I would also like to thank Dr. Janet Ellis for being my advisor, mentor,

and friend. I also want to thank Tiffani Martin for all her help with data collection. Finally,

I would like to thank Dr. Smita Mehta and Dr. Mary Estes for taking time out of their

busy schedules in order to serve on my thesis committee.

Page 5: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS……………..…….………………………………………………...iii

LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES…………...………………………………………………..v

INTRODUCTION………………....……………………………………………………………..1

METHODS…….………………..……………………………………………………………...11

RESULTS…………..……….…………………………………………………………………16

DISCUSSION…….……..……………………………………………………………………..20

Appendices

A. INFORMED CONSENT FORM………………………………………………23

B. DIRECT INSTRUCTION OUTLINE………………………………………….27

REFERENCES ………………………………………………………………………………..29

Page 6: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

v

LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES

Table: 1 Interobserver Agreement ……..……………………………………………………15

Figure 1: Intervention Group….………………………………………………………………18

Figure 2: Control Group…….…………………………………………………………………19

Page 7: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

1

INTRODUCTION

Literacy is fundamental to all areas of learning, and is highly correlated with

success in society (Chandler, 2000). An estimated 44 million Americans are illiterate

and another 50 million read below a 5th grade level (Kirsch, Jungeblut, Jenkins, &

Kolstad, 1993). Lack of literacy has been correlated with high school dropout rates

(Slavin, Karweit, Wasik, Madden, & Dolan, 1994), chronic unemployment (Schonhaut &

Satz, 1983) and criminal behavior (Bureau of Justice and Statistics, 1997; Haigler,

Harlow, O’Connor, & Campbell, 1994).

National longitudinal studies based on NICHHD (2000) data predicted that more

than 17.5 % (10 million) students will encounter reading problems within the crucial first

3 years of their schooling (NICHHD, 2000). Based on these data, legislation has come

to acknowledge the fact that early reading experiences prior to elementary school and

kindergarten years are critical factors for later reading success (No Child Left Behind

Act, 2001).

Reading is a complex behavior built upon many prerequisite skills (Cunningham,

Erickson, Spadorcia, Koppenhaver, Cunningham, & Yoder, 1999). Phonemic

awareness is a fundamental step in the process of learning to read, concerns the

structure of words vs. their meaning. To understand the construction of our written code,

readers need to be able to comprehend spelling-to-sound correspondence. Students

need to understand that the written word is composed of graphemes (individual

letters)For example, the word “ship” is composed of four graphemes, s, h, i, p. These

correspond to phonemes (letter sounds). For example, the word “ship” is composed of

three phonemes (sh, i, p). Beginning readers first must have some understanding that

Page 8: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

2

words are composed of sounds (phonemic awareness) vs. conceiving each word as a

single indivisible sound stream (Hempenstall, 1997). McCormick (1999) identified two

levels of phonemic awareness: simple (sound isolation) and compound (word-to-word

matching).

In addition to sound isolation and word-to-word matching is the concept of

abstraction. Abstraction is a discriminative behavior based on a single property of a

stimulus (Catania, 1998; Skinner, 1953). An abstraction must be explicitly taught

because it is “the analysis of things into their constituents…with some pragmatic

disregard of the residuum of qualities or other features” (Kantor, 1949). Pre-readers will

not “acquire an abstract response until a reinforcing agency sets up the required

contingency” (Skinner, 1953). Important component skills involved in acquisition of the

alphabetic principle are still under investigation (Byrne, 1998; Martens & Daly, 1999). At

a minimum these skills must include auditory abstraction (phoneme awareness) and

visual abstraction (Byrne, 1992; Saunders, 2002); both skills include discrimination of

printed letters and words, discrimination of phonemes and the corresponding

relationship between the letters and phonemes (Adams, 1990; Byrne & Fielding-

Barnsley, 1991, 1993, 1995; Saunders, 2002). Most children who have not started to

read will not demonstrate an auditory abstraction skill without appropriate training (see

Treiman & Breaux, 1982).

Various methods have been applied to teach pre-reading skills to preschool and

kindergarten populations. A study by Walsh and Blewitt (2006) examined the effects of

adult questioning on children’s novel word acquisition during storybook reading. In this

study 353 3-year-old children were assigned to one of three conditions: vocabulary-

Page 9: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

3

eliciting questions, non-eliciting questions, and no questions (control group). General

vocabulary comprehension and novel word knowledge were equivalent across the

groups before the storybook-reading intervention. Children were read three storybooks

repeatedly across 4 reading sessions and were tested for production and

comprehension of novel words in the final session. Children’s novel word

comprehension increased more in both question conditions than in the control condition.

Novel word production was not strongly affected by any of the reading conditions.

However, this study examined only novel word production and none of the prerequisite

skills associated with reading words.

Another study (Whitehurst et al., 1994) used four Head Start centers randomly

assigned to one of two conditions: the Head Start curriculum or an add-on curriculum

modeled after Sounds Foundations curriculum (Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1991). The

add-on curriculum included interactive story reading (dialogic reading) and a phonemic

awareness component. The study continued for 1 year and involved 94 children in the

intervention group and 73 in the control group. The add-on curriculum entailed reading

and discussing stories in small groups 5 times/week and one-on-one reading at home.

The phonemic awareness component focused on development of 7 consonant sounds

(s, m, p, g, l, t, sh) and 2 vowel sounds (e.g., a, e). No significant effects were obtained

on language and linguistic awareness. The greatest improvement was noted in blends,

CVC words, and letter naming for the experimental group. Although the study did report

some positive changes, the components responsible for the change were not identified.

Piertrangelo's (1999) study examined the effects of a 12-week supplementary

classroom literacy program on the emergent literacy skills of children in a Head Start

Page 10: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

4

program. This study consisted of 10 Head Start classrooms (9 intervention groups and 1

control group), all of which were administered pre - and post - intervention tests (12

tests each). These pre- and post - tests examined areas of semantic processing,

rhyming, print conventions, sentence memory, and word memory. The intervention

consisted of supplementing Head Start curriculum with activities designed to teach letter

names, letter sounds, and phonemic composition of words. Teachers were trained to

add large and small reading groups, letter name and sound instruction, phonological

awareness activities, word-play activities, and to monitor the children’s progress weekly.

This study demonstrated that supplemental instruction in the intervention classrooms

increased pretest vs. posttest scores for the control group. This study also

demonstrated the impact of individualized instruction, although the components

responsible for the changes were not identified.

Several studies have used matching-to-sample (MTS) procedures in combination

with fading procedures to teach visual discrimination of individual alphabet letters. In

these studies the child was asked to select the letter identical to the sample letter, with

the salient features of the letters exaggerated then slowly reduced to produce accurate

discriminations (Bijou, 1968; Caldwell & Hall, 1969; Popp, 1964; Williams, 1969). This

method was used more recently to teach prereaders to discriminate onset letters (first

letter in the word) in CVC words (Yoo, 2003). While these studies effectively teach letter

name discrimination they do not teach letter sounds or how to blend the letters together

to form a word.

Mueller et al. (2000) taught 3 kindergarten children to select CVC words on a

computer when they heard the corresponding spoken word. The children were taught

Page 11: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

5

six sets of words; each set consisting of four words with overlapping letters (e.g., sat,

mat, sop, sug). The experimenters tested for recombinative generalization, determined

by whether the children selected novel words with the same components as the trained

words (e.g., mop, mug). This was accomplished over four phases. The 3 children

demonstrated recombinative generalization after one or two training sets. Then these 3

children demonstrated highly accurate printed-word-to-picture matching and correctly

read the majority of printed words. While this study was effective in teaching word sets

to 3 kindergarten students, this method has not been implemented with preschool

children.

Rule, Dockstader, and Stewart, (2006) took a different approach when teaching

phonological awareness to elementary students; they examined the effectiveness of

object box activities, environmental print card activities, and kinesthetic/oral activities

used in two before-school programs for Title 1 students. In this study 34 1st -3rd grade

students in two different before-school programs (Group 1, Group 2, and control group)

were taught by the same certified teacher. These students met 3 mornings/week

(Tuesday–Thursday) for 45 min/day, for a total of 18 hrs of phonological awareness

instruction.

Activities for Group 1 included songs, phonic card games, word games, and

writing. Students rotated through four stations with a writing station added 1 day/ week.

The Group 1 activities included bingo and concentration games involving different

phonological concepts along with reading sight words and phonics games. Group 2's

program focused on hands-on activities in which small objects were manipulated.

Students worked individually and in small groups with boxes of materials. These boxes

Page 12: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

6

were of two basic types: environmental print sets of words cut from food boxes and

other product boxes. These words were then mounted on colored cardboard and object

boxes containing toys, miniature facsimiles, and small household items long with printed

word cards or headings to be sorted by the concept of the day (phonemes, syllables,

vowel sounds, etc.). Students rotated through four stations with a writing station added

1day/ week.

The Scholastic Early Childhood Program (SECP) curriculum was implemented in

the Head Start classrooms in this study. The literacy/reading component of this

curriculum was developed from Early Reading First curriculum and focused on four

main areas: oral language, phonological awareness, print awareness, and letter

recognition (Scholastic Inc., 2003).

The oral language component consisted of reading aloud during story time and

circle time. The fiction or nonfiction stories are in the context of a theme, as to enable

students to develop language and vocabulary associated with the theme.

The phonological awareness unit consisted of identifying and making oral

rhymes; identifying and working with syllables in spoken words; identifying and working

with “onsets and “rimes”; and identifying and working with individual sounds in words.

Only one of the four sections was presented daily during teacher- initiated circle time

unless a student initiated another activity. Phonological awareness activities included

rereading books, singing songs and rhymes, listening to poems, and working with the

sounds and patterns of language.

Print awareness exposes children to a variety of print (e.g. fiction and nonfiction

books, poems and song charts, and posters). Print awareness activities included

Page 13: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

7

teachers and children constructing print in a variety of forms and for many different

purposes. These activities are designed to help children understand that print carries a

message.

The alphabet knowledge section is derived from Teaching Letters and Sounds

(Block & Carnizares, 2003). This component focuses on letter recognition, letter sounds,

and word recognition. This is accomplished by reading a rhyme (a story with focus on a

particular letter/sound), having the students listen for the letter sound, write the letter,

and engage in various activities using that letter.

While the SECP curriculum provides many different pathways to learning letters,

letter sounds, and recognizing words that begin with a particular letter, it does not

provide instruction on how to blend letter sounds together to form a word.

The National Reading Panel Report (2000) indicated that large gains in reading

scores were possible when instruction was directed systematically and explicitly to one

or two types of phonemic awareness activities, when those activities were provided to

small groups. There small groups involved associating phonemes with letters (such as

segmenting and blending) activities.

Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction® Method

Teaching basic skills to children at a young age provides a better foundation for

building critical skills in their later education. This position was exemplified in a study by

Stipek (1991) who used a Direct-Verbal Program® procedure (Berteiter & Englemann,

1966). This program consisted of oral drills in verbal and logical patterns which

eventually evolved into a curriculum known as Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction

System for Teaching Arithmetic and Reading or Scientific Research Associates

Page 14: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

8

DISTAR® method (Stipek, 1991). Programs using this methodology consisted of

teaching component skills first (e.g., for reading, identify sounds, blends sounds, then

follow a left-to-right sequence, and sound out the word at a normal rate) to form a more

complex skill (reading phonetic words). This approach has reported positive gains

(Meyer, Gersten, & Gutkin, 1983). Aukerman (1984) described four large-scale follow-

up studies all with positive results in favor of Scientific Reseach Associates DISTAR®.

The Selectionist Approach

Two critical features of the selectionist approach are its use of generative

procedures, specifically generative instruction, and its insistence that skills are to be

built to fluency (Johnson & Layng, 1992). This method focuses on effective teaching to

establish key component skills and their underlying tool elements to fluency (Johnson,

1990, 1991). The term “fluency” refers to behavior performed with high degrees of

accuracy and speed (Binder, 1996; Johnson & Layng, 1992). The National Institute for

Child and Human Development (2000) indicated, “It is generally acknowledged that

fluency is a critical component of skilled reading…. nevertheless, fluency is often

neglected in classroom instruction” (p. 3). Kuhn and Stahl (2003) also encouraged

children to use fluency techniques in classrooms more often because of the positive

benefits that accrued to reading. Lindsley’s Precision Teaching® method (Lindsley,

1972, 1990, 1991, 1997) is a method for measuring behavior and making data-based

decisions to achieve and maintain reading behavior goals. Kubina (2005) recommended

reading fluency for students with and without disabilities in an inclusive classroom.

These recommendations came from applications of Lindsley’s Lindsley’s Precision

Teaching® (Binder, 1996; Haughton, 1972; Lindsley, 1997; Maloney, 1998) and

Page 15: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

9

suggested how this systematic practice procedure could benefit students during reading

instruction:

● Allocate time during the reading period for practice activities.

● Pinpoint the behavior selected for practice.

● Select the range of behaviors.

● Itemize steps involved in the practice routine.

● Select an optimal “counting time” for the practice routine.

● Select a “fluency aim” (the goal) for the pinpointed behavior.

● Combine practice steps with the selected counting time and initiate

practice.

● Reinforce correct performance and provide feedback for incorrect

performance after the practice session has ended.

● Graphically display the data on the Standard Celeration Chart graph.

Direct Instruction ® and Lindsley’s Lindsley’s Precision Teaching ®, are

successful examples of how generative instruction and fluency, have taught various

reading skill components (Barnes & Pennypacker, 2001; Legault, Maloney, & Giroux,

2001; White, 2002).

There are a variety of ways to teach pre reading skills. Current research and

legislation (No Child Left Behind Act, 2001) support the need for children to begin pre-

reading skills at the preschool level, in order to gain a solid foundation in pre-reading

skills prior to entering elementary school. These approaches increased literacy rates in

public school systems. However, previous research has primarily focused on children in

elementary school; the small portion on research done involving children enrolled in a

Page 16: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

10

preschool has not implemented any form of generative instruction or fluency

methodology. This study addresses the following question: How will Direct Instruction ®

reading procedures vs. Scholastic Early Childhood Program reading procedures affect

the production of letter sounds and CVC blends by preschool-aged students?

Page 17: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

11

METHOD

Participants

The participants were part of a clinical study using an AB design. Thirty consent

form letters were sent describing the study to the parents of children in two Head Start

classrooms. Twenty-two letters were returned the following week, 11from each

classroom. Subsequently there forms were separated into male and female groups from

each classroom. Eight students, 3-4 years of age (2 boys and 2 girls from each

classroom) were selected to participate in this study in order to avoid any gender bias.

One group was labeled the Control group. These students received baseline, probe,

and post test assessments. No training was provided by the experimenter. The same

procedure was followed for the students in the intervention group. The baseline

assessment was administered to both groups at the onset of the study. However, only

the intervention group received pre-reading training in the school library.

Materials

Stimuli consisted of all lower case alphabet letters printed horizontally in size 20

New Times Roman font taped onto 26 3x5 index cards. Other materials included a

pencil, timer, a binder (for storing student graphs), stickers (picturing popular TV and

movie cartoon characters, cars, happy faces, and various animals), a toy bin containing

a Poof Products Slinky ® toy, Hasbro Play-Doh ® putty, Ja-ru Flarp ® noise putty,

miniature farm and prehistoric animals, a toy cellphone, a Fisher Price Magnadoodle ®

board, Hasbro Mr. Potato Head ® toy, crayons, and construction paper delivered

contingent on correct responses.

Page 18: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

12

Training Procedures

Baseline

The baseline assessment was administered to all participants at the beginning

of the study, and included all 26 lower case letters, 10 CV blends, and 10 CVC blends.

The 26 lowercase letters were individually presented to the participant when the

experimenter asked “What is the name of this letter?”, and “What sound does this letter

make?” for each alphabet letter. The blends and common words were also presented

individually when the experimenter asked the participant, “What does this say?” No

feedback or reinforcement was delivered following correct or incorrect responding. After

this baseline assessment the participants received a sticker of their choice.

Training Letter Sounds

Sessions started with the letter sound program which began with three letter

sounds. Letter sounds the participants answered correctly on the baseline assessment

were used as the initial letter sounds. If the participants were unable to emit correct

letter sounds the session began with three letters they had correctly named. All the

participants began with 15-s timings on three letter sounds. Criteria for adding a new

letter was 90% accuracy and a rate above15 sounds/min on the previous timing. When

each participant reached 10 letters, timings increased from 15s to 30s providing

adequate time to present each letter at least once per timing. At the beginning of the

session the participant was told the goal (one more correct than in the previous timing)

to earn free play time. The participant then chose a reinforcer from the toy bin. If the

participant met his/her goal the participant accessed the reinforcer for 3 min.

Page 19: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

13

Training CV Blends

The CV blends program was introduced once the participants had correctly

emitted 6 letter sounds. All participants began training with blends sa, and ta. These

blends were selected based on the letter sounds each participant had learned in the

letter sound program. Criteria for adding a new blend and increasing the length of the

timings was the same as in the letter sound program timings. The consequence

procedures were the same as in the letter sound program.

Training CVC Blends

The CVC blends program began in the fourth month of intervention. This

program consisted of participants sounding out each letter sound. Then the student to,

"Say all sounds fast." During this CVC blends program 1-2 new blends were

introduced/session in addition to practicing previously learned blends. The consequence

procedures were the same as in the letter sound and CV Blends programs.

Probe Procedures

The probe was administered to all participants the last day before winter break

(December 19, 2008) and was conducted in the same manner as the baseline

assessment.

Post Assessment Procedures

The post assessment occurred at the end of the study and was the same as the

previous assessments except the CV and CVC blends consisted of letter sounds the

participants had correctly labeled on the probe.

Error Correction Procedures

Prior to the participant gaining access to free play time, errors were corrected.

Page 20: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

14

The experimenter said “What sound is this?” if the participant answered correctly praise

was delivered. If the participant answered incorrectly the experimenter said, “No, this is

/a/ (or the correct sound)”, “What is this sound?” until the participant correctly labeled

the sound independently.

Data Collection

Target responses measured in this study included correct and incorrect

responses. A correct response was scored if the participant said the appropriate letter

sound, CV, and/or CVC blend after the stimulus was presented. Self-corrects made

before the next stimulus was presented were also scored as correct. An incorrect

response was scored if the participants made the incorrect sound, said “I don’t know,” or

made no response within 5s.

For each participant the number of correct and incorrect answers, along with the

number and length of the timings was recorded on a Timings chart for each program.

The best timing for the session was then recorded on a Daily Standard Celeration Chart

for each program.

Interobserver Agreement (IOA)

Table 1

Interobsever Agreement

Sessions Attended

Sessions IOA

Range

Mean

Oscar 51 11 92.25% - 100% 99.29% Jane 42 10 100% 100% Bill 56 11 88.2%-100% 98.59% Ava 57 13 91.7%-100% 99.36%

Page 21: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

15

Table 1 depicts the number of sessions the participants attended, the number of

sessions during which IOA was collected, the range and mean of IOA.

IOA percentages for all participants were computed for approximately 20% of the

sessions/student. IOA was collected once a week for 13 weeks by a trained

independent observer who sat at the opposite end of the table (approximately 5ft. away)

while the researcher and participant were in session. IOA percentages between the

experimenter and the trained independent observer were calculated by dividing the total

number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100, and ranged from

88.2% to 100% with a mean of 99.3%.

Page 22: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

16

RESULTS

Overall in the baseline assessment, the participants in the control group emitted

more correct responses for letter names and letter sounds than occurred in the

intervention group. Results of the intervention group indicated that Oscar was able to

correctly name only 2 letters; Jane named only 1 letter; Bill named 4 letters correctly

and 1 letter sound; Ava named 6 letter sounds correctly and 1 letter sound. Results of

the control group showed that Don correctly named 8 letters and 2 letter sounds; Tracy

correctly named 13 letters and 7 sounds; Sara accurately named 17 letters and 11

sounds; while Matt correctly named 14 letters and 11 sounds. No participants in either

group correctly identified any CV or CVC blends.

Generally the results from the probe assessment show both groups improved in

the probe assessment compared to the baseline assessment; however, the control

group improved only in letter names and sounds, while the intervention group showed

gains in letter names, sounds and blends. Results of the intervention group revealed

that Oscar named 7 letters, 6 sounds and 1 CV blend; Jane named 7 letters, 7 sounds,

and 1 CV blend; Bill named 11 letters, 7 sounds, and 1 CV blend; while Ava named 11

letters, 10 sounds, 2 CV blends, and 1 CVC blend. The performance of all participants

in the intervention group improved from their previous assessment. Results of the

control group showed that Don named 23 letters and 5 sounds; Tracy named 16 letters

and 6 sounds; Sara correctly labeled 13 letters and 9 sounds, while Matt named 21

letters and 7 sounds. In the control group Don and Matt improved over their baseline

assessments, however, Tracy improved in letter names but dropped in letter sounds.

Sara's letter naming and sounds performances decreased compared to her baseline

assessment.

Page 23: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

17

Overall in the post assessment, both groups improved in letter naming and

sounds; however, only the intervention group improved in CV and CVC blends: Oscar

named 13 letters, 10 correct sounds, 2 CV blends and 2 CVC blends; Jane named 10

letters, 10 sounds, 3 CV blends and 2 CVC blends; Bill labeled 20 letters, 19 sounds, 6

CV blends and 6 CVC blends; Ava identified 22 letters, 21 sounds, 6 CV blends and 6

CVC blends. In the control group outcomes, Don named 24 letters and 9 sounds; Tracy

identified 14 letters and 7 sounds; Sara labeled 22 letters and 16 sounds; and Matt

named 21 letters and 14 sounds. Don and Sara improved in letter names and sounds,

while Tracy improved in letter sounds and decreased in letter names. Matt's

performance in letter names did not change from baseline; however, his performance

decreased in letter sounds.

Figure 1. Results of the baseline, probe and post assessments for the intervertion group. All participant names are psedonyms.

Page 24: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

18

Figure 2. The results of the baseline, probe, and post assessments for the control group. All participant names are psedonyms.

Page 25: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

19

DISCUSSION

Overall, results indicate that most participants improved in their letter-naming

skills and letter sounds. However, only the intervention group was able to correctly label

any CV and/or CVC blends. This is a significant outcome, because recent literacy

literature has emphasized teaching letter names (graphemes) to their corresponding

letter sounds (phonemes) (Cunningham, Erickson, Spadorcia, Koppenhaver,

Cunningham, & Yoder, 1999). However, results of this study suggest that a focus on

teaching letter names and sounds in isolation does not lead to sound blending. The

Lindsley’s Precision Teaching®/Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction® approach

applied in this study may be a useful procedure for teaching this pre-school population

the concept of blending sounds together. Sound blending is a necessary and critical

component of pre-reading (National Reading Panel Report, 2000).

One explanation of why none of the 4 participants in the control group failed to

correctly blend sounds indicates that sound blending is a behavioral cusp. A behavioral

cusp is a behavior change that “exposes the individual’s repertoire to new

environments, especially new reinforcers and punishers, new stimulus controls, and

new communities of maintaining or destructive contingencies” (Rosales-Ruiz & Baer,

1997). Learning to read leads to many new reinforcers, such as increasing performance

efficiency in the general as well as academic culture. Ability to read enables these

economically deprived students to ultimately (through high school education) acquire

skills necessary to earn a living in today's workplace. Reading is important to the child’s

future development which defines a cusp. A child cannot learn to read without first

acquiring the prerequisite skills needed to achieve the final behavior of reading

Page 26: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

20

accurately and fluently. Each prerequisite skill is a building block for the next and opens

up new opportunities for reinforcement. First, the child must be able to discriminate the

different letter sounds, then, learn to blend the sounds together, this will lead to forming

words and being able to sound out novel words, and then reading multiple words in a

sentence etc. The point is that each component of learning to read needs to be taught

and we cannot expect a student to be able to read the word “cat” just because the

student can say, “c-a-t” and make the sounds /c/ /a/ /t/.

Another interesting finding from this study is the speed with which the

intervention group improved compared to the control group. The baseline assessment

(Figure 1) showed the intervention group correctly naming 1-6 letter names, while only 2

participants in the intervention group correctly labeled 1 letter sound vs. the control

group which began with naming 8-16 letter names and 2-11 letter sounds. The post

assessment (Figure 2) shows the intervention group correctly labeled 10-21 letters

sounds while the control group correctly labeled 7-16 letter sounds, approximately a 5-

letter increase from baseline. Also, all 4 participants in the intervention group were able

to blend some CV and CVC sounds together.

The variation in assessment scores in the intervention group may be a function of

session attendance. There were a total of 60 sessions in this study, Ava was absent

twice; Bill was absent three times; Jane was absent 18 times, and Oscar was absent 9

times. Results suggest that consistent attendance is important for skill acquisition to

occur.

One confound in this study was that the intervention and control groups received

Scholastic Early Childhood Program (SECP) curriculum which consisting of the pre-

Page 27: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

21

reading program described in detail in the introduction of this study. The SECP pre-

reading program focused mainly on letter names and letter sounds, thus making it

difficult to assess the effectiveness of the letter names and sounds procedure. However,

the SECP program did not teach blending and the results suggest that the Lindsley’s

Lindsley’s Precision Teaching®/Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction® method was

responsible for the academic achievement in that area.

Another limitation of this study is the lack of follow-up retention data. This

limitation was imposed by time restraints.

Conclusion

The present study demonstrated that Lindsley’s Lindsley’s Precision

Teaching®/Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction® produced positive gains in CV

and CVC blends and is an effective methodology for teaching pre-reading skills to

students in Head Start programs. Research focused on applying this technology in a

small group classroom setting may prompt the classroom teacher to implement this

training procedure when teaching pre-reading skills. Further research with follow-up is

needed in this area and to assess retention. Future research could include other

populations such as children with Autism. This procedure could also be effective for

teaching pre-reading skills in adult education classes, children with developmental

disabilities, and as an approach to teaching mathematics. Further research that enables

answers to how to most effectively train academic skills might benefit from the

outcomes of this behavior analysis research project.

Page 28: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

24

APPENDIX A

INFORMED CONSENT FORM

Page 29: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

24

University of North Texas Institutional Review Board

Informed Consent Form

Before agreeing to your child’s participation in this research study, it is important that

you read and understand the following explanation of the purpose and benefits of the

study and how it will be conducted.

Title of Study: A Head Start on Reading for Children in Head Start Program

Principal Investigator: Kristin Osley, a graduate student in the University of North Texas

(UNT) Department of Behavior Analysis.

Purpose of the Study: The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of a

procedure to teach reading to children (3-5yrs old) in the preschool program Head Start.

This procedure is based on a method that tracks student’s progress by conducting 15-

30 second timings in which the numbers of correct and incorrect responses are noted

and goals for the next day are derived.

All participants will be assessed at the beginning and end of the study on letter sounds,

and blends (consonant/vowel sound e.g. “ba” as in “bag”). If this teaching method is

effective in teaching reading to this population, then their after test scores should

increase.

Study Procedures: If your child is selected to participate, his/her current pre-reading

skills (letter sounds, constant/vowel (C/V) blends (“ba” as in bag), and

constant/vowel/constant (CVC) blends (e.g., "cat," "mit") will be assessed. After the pre-

assessment your child will be taught any unknown letter sounds and C/V blends. During

each timing, correct responses will be praised and incorrect responses will be ignored.

After each timing, incorrect responses will be corrected and briefly practiced. If your

Page 30: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

25

child meets his/her goal for the day s/he will receive a sticker of their choice and a short

2 minute free play time with age-appropriate toys. The session will take 10 -15 minutes

each school day from 11/03/08- 03/09/09. At the end of the study an assessment will be

given.

Foreseeable Risks: No foreseeable risks are involved in this study.

Benefits to the Subjects or Others: Benefits to your child are expected to be building

pre-reading skills such as, letter sounds, C/V blends, and CVC blends, which are

important building blocks in learning to read.

Procedures for Maintaining Confidentiality of Research Records: All research

records will be maintained in a locked filing cabinet in the Department of Behavior

Analysis at the University of North Texas. Signed consent forms will be filed separately

from any other identifying information in order to maintain confidentiality, and these data

will be destroyed three years following the completion of the study. The confidentiality of

your child’s individual information will be maintained by numeric code (e.g. Adam Cade

= 1-3) or pseudonyms in any publications or presentations regarding this study.

Questions about the Study: If you have any questions about the study, you may

contact Kristin Osley at telephone number 555-555-5555, or the faculty advisor, Dr.

Janet Ellis, 999-999-9999.

Review for the Protection of Participants: This research study has been reviewed

and approved by the UNT Institutional Review Board (IRB). The UNT IRB can be

contacted at 999-999-9999 with any questions regarding the rights of research subjects.

Research Participants’ Rights: Your signature below indicates that you have read or

have had read to you all of the above and that you confirm all of the following:

Page 31: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

26

Kristin Osley has explained the study to you and answered all of your questions.

You have been told the possible benefits and the potential risks and/or

discomforts of the study.

You understand that you do not have to allow your child to take part in this study,

and your refusal to allow your child to participate or your decision to withdraw

him/her from the study will involve no penalty or loss of rights or benefits. The

study personnel may choose to stop your child’s participation at any time.

You understand why the study is being conducted and how it will be performed.

You understand your rights as the parent/guardian of a research participant and

you voluntarily consent to your child’s participation in this study.

You have been told you will receive a copy of this form.

_______________________________ Printed Name of Parent or Guardian

________________________________ __________ Signature of Parent or Guardian Date

Page 32: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

27

APPENDIX B

DIRECT INSTRUCTION OUTLINE

Page 33: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

28

The following outline of Englemann & Becker’s Direct Instruction is from Distar

Reading I: A Instructional System (1974).

Pre-reading Activities

The purpose of these activities is to prepare the children for the reading of simple

regularly spelled words.

Symbol Identification. The children must be able to recognize the letter symbols and

produce the sounds they represent.

Sequencing. The children need to know the order in which to read the symbols.

Blending. The children need to know that a word can be pulled apart by sounding out

and that once a word is sounded out it can be put back together by saying the sounds

fast.

Rhyming. The children need rhyming skills so that they will be aware of similarities

among words.

Once the children master these skills they will be ready to begin reading words.

Page 34: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

29

REFERENCES

Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Aukerman, R.C. (1984). Approaches to reading. New York: Wiley.

Barnes, W. S., & Pennypacker, H. S. (2001). Fluency research: An example from history. Journal of Precision Teaching and Celeration, 17, 92-96.

Bereiter, C., & Engelmann, S. (1996). Teaching disadvantaged children in the preschool. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice –Hall.

Bijou, S.W. (1968). Studies in the experimental development of left-right concepts in retarded children using fading techniques. In N.R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation, 3, New York: Academic Press.

Binder, C. (1996). Behavioral fluency: Evolution of a new paradigm. Behavior Analyst, 19,163-197.

Block, C., & Canizanres, S. (2003). Early literacy, New York, NY: Scholastic Inc.

Bureau of Justice and Statistics (1997). Juvenile offenders and victims 1997 national report. Washington, DC: National Center for Juvenile Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.

Byrne, B. (1992). Studies in the acquisition procedure for reading: Rationale, hypotheses, and data. In P. B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 1-34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Byrne, B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 805-812.

Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 104-111.

Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2- and 3- year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 488-503.

Catania, A, C. (1998). Learning. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Caldwell, E.C., & Hall, V.C. (1969). The influence of concept training on letter discrimination. Child Development, 40, 63-71.

Page 35: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

30

Chandler, K. (2000). Functional illiteracy. Advisor, 26, 3.

Cunningham, J. W., Erickson, K. A., Spadorcia, S. A., Koppenhaver, D. A., Cunningham, P. M., Yoder, D. E., et al. (1999). Assessing decoding from an onset-rime perspective. Journal of Literacy Research, 31, 391-414.

Distar Reading I: An Instructional System, (1974). Science Research Associates, Chicago, IL.

Haigler, K. O., Harlow, C., O’Connor, P., & Campbell, A. (1994). Literacy behind prison walls, U. S. Department of Education. National Center for Educational Statistics (NAES), Washington, DC.

Haughton, E. C. (1972). Aims: Growing and sharing. In J. B. Jordan & L. S. Robbins (Eds.), Let’s try doing something else kind of thing (pp. 20-39). Arlington, VA: Council for Exceptional Children.

Hempenstall, K. (1997). The role of phonemic awareness in beginning reading: A review. Behaviour Change, 14(4), 201-14.

Johnson, K. R. (1990, November). Literacy training: JTPA adults and youth at risk. Ninth International Precision Teaching Conference, Boston, MA.

Johnson, K. R. (1991). About Morningside Academy. Future Choices, 3, 64-66.

Johnson, K. R. & Layng, T. V. J. (1992). Breaking the structuralist barrier: Literacy and numeracy with fluency. American Psychologist, 47, 1475-1490.

Kantor, J.R. (1949) Principles of psychology. Chicago, IL: Principia.

Kirsch, I., Jungblut, A., Jenkins, L., & Kolstad, A. (1993). Adult literacy in America: A first look at the results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington: US Department of Education National Center for Education Statistics.

Kubina, R. (2005). Focus on inclusion: developing reading fluency through a systematic practice procedure. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 21, 185-192.

Kuhn, M. R. & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 3-21.

Legault, A., Maloney, M., & Giroux, N. (2001). Learning rates with direct instruction, precision teaching and the corrective reading series. Journal of Precision Teaching and Celeration, 17, 89-91.

Lindsley, O. R. (1972). From Skinner to precision teaching: Teaching the child knows best. In J.B. Jordan & L. S. Robbins (Eds.), Let’s try doing something else kind of thing (pp. 1-11). Arlington, VA: council for Exceptional Children.

Page 36: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

31

Lindsley, O. R. (1991). Precision Teaching’s unique legacy from B. F. Skinner. Journal of Behavioral Education, 1(2), 253-266.

Lindsley, O. R. (1997). Precise instructional design: Guidelines from Precision Teaching. In C.R. Dills & A. J. Romiszowski (Eds.), Instructional development paradigms (pp. 537- 554). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications.

Maloney, M. (1998). Teach your children well: A solution to some of North America’s educational problems. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies.

Mann, V.A. (2003). Part III: Phonological awareness, speech development, and letter knowledge in preschool children. Speech Development, Perception, and Production; Components of Reading; Defining Mathematics Learning Disability, 53, 149.

Martens, B. K., Daly, E.J. (1999). Discovering the alphabetic principle: A lost opportunity for education reform. Journal of Behavioral Education, 9, 35-43.

Meyer, L. A., Gersten, R.M., & Gutkin, J. (1983). Direct instruction: A project follow through. Elementary School, 94, 380-394.

Mueller, M., Olimi, D., & Saunders, K. (2000). Recombinative generalization of within-syllable units in prereading children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 515-531.

National Institute for Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instructions. Reports of the subgroups. NIH publication No. 00-4754. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Retrieved 12/2/03 from http://www.nationalreadingpanel.org

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. Education. Intergovernmental relations, 20 USC §6301 (2002).

Piertrangelo, D. J. (1999). Outcomes of an enhanced literacy curriculum on the emergent literacy skills of head start preschoolers. (Doctoral dissertation, State University of New York, 1999.) Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 60, 4.

Popp, H. (1964). Visual discrimination of alphabet letters. Reading Teacher, 18, 221-226.

Page 37: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

32

Rosales-Ruiz, J. & Baer, D. (1997). Behavioral cusps: A developmental and pragmatic concept for behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 533-544.

Saunders, K. J. (2002, October). Abstraction and recombinative generalization of within-syllable units. Invited presentation at the Southeastern Association for Behavior Analysis, Charleston, SC.

Scholastic Inc. (2003, July), Scholastic early childhood program aligned to early reading first, Retrieved February 20, 2009, from http://www.scholastic.com/administrator/funding/fundingconnection/pdf/SECP_ERF_Alignment1.05Final.pdf

Schonhaut, S., & Satz, P. (1983). Prognosis for children with learning disabilities: A review of follow-up studies. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 542-563). New York: Guilford Press.

Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Free Press.

Slavin, R., Karweit, B., Wasik, N., Madden, N., & Dolan, L. (1994). Success for all: A comprehensive approach to prevention and early intervention. In R. Slavin, N. Karweit, & B. Wasik (Eds.), Preventing early school failure (pp.175-205). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Stipek, D. (1991). Characterizing early childhood education programs. In L. Rescorla, M.C. Hyson, K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Academic instruction in early childhood: Challenge or pressure (pp. 47-55). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Rule, A., Dockstader, C., & Stewart, R. (2006). Hands-on and kinesthetic activities for teaching phonological awareness. Early Childhood Education Journal, 34, 195-201.

Treiman, R., & Breaux, A. M. (1982). Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: Their use by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 569-598.

Walsh, B., & Blewitt, P. (2006). The effect of questioning style during storybook reading on novel vocabulary acquisition of preschoolers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33, 273-278.

White, L. (2002). Rick’s reading progress. Journal of Precision Teaching and Celeration, 18, 69-77.

Whitehurst, G., Epstein, J., Angell, A., Payne, A., Crone, D., & Fischel, J. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention in Head Start. Journal of Educational Psychology. 86, 542-555.

Williams, J.P. (1969). Training kindergarten children to discriminate letter-like forms. American Education and Research Journal, 6, 501-514.

Page 38: A head start on reading for children in a Head Start .../67531/metadc10985/m2/1/high_res… · Osley, Kristin R. A head start on reading for children in a Head Start preschool program

 

33

Yoo, J. H. (2004). The abstraction of onset letters in consonant-vowel-consonant words by pre-readers. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, 2003). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Physical Sciences and Engineering, 65, 4.