acoustic clues for understanding the morphological ...linguistics.berkeley.edu/~jtmanker/manker -...

34
Acoustic Clues for Understanding the Morphological Structure of Disyllabic Stems in Hän Athabascan 2012 Athabascan Languages Conference Bellingham, WA August 15, 2012 Jonathan Manker

Upload: lamkiet

Post on 14-Mar-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


6 download

TRANSCRIPT

Acoustic Clues for

Understanding the

Morphological

Structure of

Disyllabic Stems in

Hän Athabascan

2012 Athabascan Languages Conference

Bellingham, WA

August 15, 2012

Jonathan Manker

The Hän Athabascan Language

The traditional

language of

Eastern Alaska and

Western Yukon.

Spoken in the

villages of Eagle,

Alaska, and

Dawson City,

Yukon.

The youngest speakers are in

their early 60s, so the language

was last naturally transmitted in

the 1950s and 60s.

Today there are between 8

and 15 speakers, depending

on the definition of fluency.

But there are efforts to revive the language…

Background: Stem

Prominence in Athabascan

Athabascan morphemes can be divided into stems,

prefixes, and suffixes, while words can be created

by attaching any number of prefixes to a stem (or

none at all). Stems usually consist of a single syllable

and take the form CV(C) (Krauss & Golla, 1981:69).

Several studies (Kari 1990 for Ahtna, Tuttle 2005 for

San Carlos Western Apache, Rice 2005 for Hare,

Leer 2005) have indicated that the stem syllable is

phonetically prominent in Athabascan languages.

Manker (2012) shows that duration is the

most reliable distinction in indicating stem

prominence in Hän Athabascan,

particularly in stem initial consonants.

Additional features of consonants, such as

voicing in fricatives and voice quality

following ejectives are also indicators of

stem prominence in Hän.

0 20 40 60 80 100

/Cə/

/Cək/

/Ce/

/Ci/

/Cu/

/n/ (closure)

/d/ (closure)

/t/ (release)

/ǰ/ (release)

/č/ (release)

/t'/ (closure)

/k'/ (closure)

/č'/ (closure)

/tr'/ (closure)

/ð/

Percent Length of Prefix Segments Compared to Stem

Segments

(p < .05 for all)

Disyllabic Stems

In the modern language, there are some

“stems” with two syllables that cannot be

analyzed by current speakers

(synchronically). This study investigates

the phonetic production of such disyllabic

stems--- do they display stem prominence,

and if so, has the structure of the stem

been reanalyzed? Does this occur in the

first or second syllable?

Historically, disyllabic stems in the modern language

may have consisted of a prefix plus a stem, two

stems, or prefix plus a suffix, etc.

In the modern language, such words are not

analyzable, or are only partly analyzable (such as /t’əgæ/) and may be considered disyllabic stems.

GlossPA reconstruction Structure Modern Hän

man *də-ne prefix + suffix /ǰəǰe/

moose *də-nigy-i prefix + stem (+ suffix) /ǰəǰu/

girl *t'ed -ɢaye stem + particle /t’əgæ/

Synchronic Structure of

Disyllabic Stems Some suspected disyllabic stems such as /ǰəǰe/

‘man’ or /t’əgæ/ ‘girl’ structurally appear as though

they might represent combinations of prefixes and

stems according to a synchronic interpretation, thus /ǰə-ǰe/ and /t’ə-gæ/; this analysis is based on the

fact that the phoneme /ə/ occurs phonetically as

[ə] or [ɪ] in both of these examples which is typical

of the allophone found in prefixes (in stems, /ə/ is

realized as [ɵ] (de Reuse, p.c.)).

So the first syllable of these disyllabic stems may be

realized phonetically like a prefix, but is the second

syllable realized like a stem? Some preliminary

evidence indicated it might not be.

This example shows

the

monomorphemic /ǰəǰe/ ‘man’ compared to /ǰə-ǰà/ ‘his-friend.’ Is the prefix-stem boundary being marked with a longer /ǰ/ in ‘his-friend’?

This StudySpecifically, this study tested the second

syllables of disyllabic stems for prominence (it is difficult to add prefixes to most of these stems which could be a necessary method for measuring stop closure lengths within the word).

Thus, would the words / ǰəžar/ ‘cow moose’ and /ǰə-žar/ ‘her-son’ be pronounced the same, or does only the /ž/ in ‘her-son’ display stem prominence due to the clear morpheme boundary?

101 example words were collected from one fluent

speaker of Hän (RR) within complete sentences.

These included disyllabic stems as well as

analyzable prefix + stem combinations for

comparison (with all examples being two syllable

words with the consonant being in intervocalic

position), thus:

/nàgə/ ‘fox’ (disyllabic) vs.

/a-gak/ (3rd sng / l-classifier – ‘runs’)

Six different phonemes were tested: /g/ / ǰ/ /ž/ /ɣ/ /b/ and /t’/ (Comparisons were not made across

different phonemes).

For the stop /b/, only closure length was compared.

For /ǰ/ and /g/, closure and release length were

both compared. Phonetically these phonemes are

realized as [tž] and [qʁ] respectively.

The fricatives /ž/ and /ɣ/ were compared in

duration and total voicing percent; Voiced fricative

phonemes tend to display some semi-voicelessness

in stem onsets (Holton 2000, Tanacross, Manker

2012, Hän).

The ejective /t’/ was compared for closure duration

and VOT; VOT of /t’/ was found to be longer in stem

onsets and creaky voiced followed /t’/ in prefix

onsets (Manker 2012)

Results

Initial results were a bit mixed but mostly

indicated that second syllable onsets of

disyllabic stems were realized just like

those in other stems, unlike the preliminary

results.

Most p values were very insignicant, some were around 0.1, and the results for /ǰ/

actually yielded a significant p value

initially.

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic

/g/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

120.54

90.4

216.46

134.68

107.94

243.34

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

closure duration

p = 0.42

release duration

p = 0.13

total duration

p =0.16

1 morpheme (disyllabic)

2 morpheme

For example, /nàgə/ ‘fox’ vs. /a-gak/ ‘s/he-is

running’

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic /ǰ/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

For example, /ǰəǰe/ ‘man’ vs. /ǰə-ǰà/ ‘his-friend’

96.71 93.34

190.05

118.55126.19

244.74

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

closure duration

p = 0.045

release duration

p = <0.001

avg tot

p = 0.003

1 morpheme (disyllabic)

2 morph

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic

/b/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

For example, /ǰəbe/ ‘sheep’ vs. /ǰə-bà/ ‘his-sleeve’

146.18 147.9

0

50

100

150

200

250

1 morpheme (disyllabic) 2 morpheme

closure duration

p = 0.92

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic

/t’/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

For example /hat’or/ ‘birch’ vs. /na-t’aw/ ‘it-is flying’

161.1

131.3

292.4

213.8

152.2

366

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

350

400

closure duration

p = 0.09

release duration

p = 0.09

total duration

p = 0.054

1 morpheme (disyllabic)

2 morpheme

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic /ž/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

For example, /ǰəžar/ ‘cow moose’ vs. /ǰə-žar/ ‘her-son’

178.38

166

0

50

100

150

200

250

1 morpheme (disyllabic) 2 morphemes

duration

p = 0.46

Comparison of Total Voicing Percentage of

Intervocalic /ž/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

41.23%

34.52%

0.00%

20.00%

40.00%

60.00%

80.00%

100.00%

1 morpheme 2 morpheme

Total Voicing Percentage

p = 0.38

Comparison of Duration of Intervocalic /ɣ/ in words with 1 or 2 morphemes

For example, /nàɣay/ ‘frog’ vs. /a-ɣew/ ‘s/he-carries’

200.71

212.5

0

50

100

150

200

250

1 morpheme (disyllabic) 2 morphemes

duration

p = 0.47

Comparison of Total Voicing Percentage of Intervocalic /ɣ/ in words

with 1 or 2 morphemes

34.55%32.45%

0.00%

10.00%

20.00%

30.00%

40.00%

50.00%

60.00%

70.00%

80.00%

90.00%

100.00%

1 morpheme (disyllabic) 2 morpheme

total voicing percentage

p = 0.6

After reviewing this data, which yielded

mixed but mostly insignificant results, two

additional factors were discovered that

could have an effect on the length of the

intervocalic consonant.

First of all, a preceding schwa / reduced vowel /ə/

caused the following consonant to be significantly

shorter.

Comparison of consonant duration following a

reduced or full vowels for all segments

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

140

160

180

200

reduced vowel /ə/ full vowel

duration (ms)

p < 0.001

Comparison of consonant duration following a reduced or full vowels for /g/

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

closure duration

p < 0.001

release duration

p = 0.4

total duration

p < .001

reduced /ə/

full

Thus, this explains why comparing /t’əgæ/ and /a-gak/ appeared to yield a significant difference in the preliminary results. When this environment is controlled for, there is still no significant difference (for example between /nagə/ ‘fox’ and /a-gak/ ‘he is running’ or /t’əgæ/ ‘girl’ and /ǰə-gæ/ ‘her young’).

Secondly, two forms containing a /ǰ/

actually maintained a contrast with a historically distinct /ǰ/. These originated

from *n and *d separately; thus:

*dəne < /ǰəǰe/ ‘man’

*dənig-i < /ǰəǰu/ ‘moose’

For some Hän speakers, *n remains as a fully voiced /d/ or /ǰ/ which contrasts with

a voiceless unaspirated /d/ and /ǰ/.

This speaker’s reflexes of *n varied freely in

voicing, but were always significantly

shorter than reflexes of *d. This made it appear as though a word such as /ǰə-ǰà/

was marking a morpheme boundary with a longer /ǰ/ than in /ǰəǰe/ when in fact

these were different phonemes.

When these examples (/ǰəǰe/ and /ǰəǰu/) were removed, there was no significant difference.

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

closure duration

p = 0.23

release duration

p = 0.36

total duration

p = 0.89

1 morpheme

2 morphemes

*n examples

(p values listed are between 1 and 2 morpheme

examples (gray and green) and not *n, which were

significantly different)

Overall, the onsets of the second syllables of disyllabic stems were in fact realized identically to stems, despite whatever they may have been historically.

For example, the /ž/ in the word /ǰəžar/ ‘cow moose’ was no shorter than the /ž/ in /ǰə.žar/ ‘her own son.’

Words like /t’əgæ/ ‘girl’ or /ǰəǰe/ ‘man’ are produced phonetically as if they are sequences of a prefix plus a stem, despite their analyzability in the modern language.

Analysis

In Hän, it appears that prominence is

assigned to the second syllable of

“disyllabic stems,” and such prominence

does not necessarily reflect the historical picture, for example /t’əgæ/ ‘girl,’ or /ǰəǰe/ ‘man.’

Furthermore, suffixes in Hän are only

vestigial, meaning stems are almost

always the final syllable of a word. Thus, is

prominence actually just assigned to the

final syllable of a word, or have these

categories just been redefined (-gæ and

–je are now stems)?

References Holton, Gary. 2000. The Phonology and Morphology of the Tanacross Athabaskan

Language. Ph.D. disseration, University of California, Santa Barbara.

Kari, James. 1990. Ahtna Athabaskan Dictionary. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language

Center.

Krauss, Michael E. & Victor K. Golla. 1981. Northern Athabaskan Languages.Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 6: Subarctic, ed. by J. Helm, 67-86. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian.

Leer, Jeff. 2005. How Stress Shapes the Stem-Suffix Complex in Athabascan. In Sharon Hargus and Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody. Philadelphia, Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 278-318.

Manker, Jonathan. 2012. An Acoustic Analysis of Stem Prominence in Hän Athabascan. Master’s Thesis, University of Alaska Fairbanks.

Rice, Keren. 2005. Prominence and the Verb Stem in Slave (Hare). In Sharon Hargus and Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody. Philadelphia, Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 345-368.

Tuttle, Siri. 2005. Duration, Intonation, and Prominence in Apache. In Sharon Hargus and Keren Rice (eds.), Athabaskan Prosody. Philadelphia, Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Pp. 319-344.

Thanks

To Ruth Ridley of Fairbanks, AK for

participating in this study.

To the rest of the Hän community for their

support.

To UAF and the Alaska Native Language

Center for funding and support.

To Dr. Siri Tuttle, for advising me as a

Master’s student at UAF.