acquisition of prosody at the babbling stage in...

55
ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN PROSODIC, GESTURAL, AND PRAGMATIC CONSIDERATIONS Núria Esteve Gibert Universitat Pompeu Fabra Ph.D. Project Supervisor: Dr. Pilar Prieto

Upload: vocong

Post on 15-Feb-2019

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

ACQUISITION OF PROSODY

AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

PROSODIC, GESTURAL, AND PRAGMATIC CONSIDERATIONS

Núria Esteve Gibert

Universitat Pompeu Fabra

Ph.D. Project Supervisor: Dr. Pilar Prieto

Page 2: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

2

INDEX

0. ABSTRACT/RESUM ................................. ....................................................................... 3

1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................... 5

1.1. Object of analysis ........................... .............................................................................. 5

1.2. Prior work ................................... ................................................................................... 5

1.2.1. Continuity between babbling and early words .............................................................. 5

1.2.2. Development of intonation in relation to the development of

intentional communication ...................................................................................... 6

1.2.3. Communicative use of gestures .................................................................................. 8

1.2.4. Synchronization between gesture and speech ........................................................... 11

1.2.5. Intonation as a tool for children to determine intentions in co-speech gestures…...... 13

2. GOALS OF THE DISSERTATION ...................... ............................................................. 16

3. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK .......................... ............................................................. 17

3.1. The study of prosodic development ............ ............................................................. 17

3.2. The study of intentional communication ....... ............................................................ 19

3.3. The study of gesture development ............. .............................................................. 23

4. HYPOTHESES ................................................................................................................ 25

5. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES .......................... ................................................................. 26

5.1. Study 1 ...................................... ................................................................................ 26

5.1.1. Materials ..................................................................................................................... 26

5.1.2. Codification ................................................................................................................. 27

5.1.3. Summary results ....……………………….…………………………………...…....... 30

5.2. Study 2 ...................................... ................................................................................ 33

5.2.1. Materials .................................................................................................................... 33

5.2.2. Codification ................................................................................................................ 35

5.2.3. Preliminary results ………………………………………………………....….......... 40

5.3. Study 3 ...................................... ................................................................................ 43

5.3.1. Materials .................................................................................................................... 43

5.3.2. Codification ............................................................................................................... 44

6. WORKING SCHEDULE................................ .................................................................... 45

7. SELECTED REFERENCES ............................................................................................. 47

8. GENERAL REFERENCES.............................. ................................................................. 49

Page 3: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

3

0. ABSTRACT

Children develop the ability to communicate their emotions and intentions before they

produce their first words (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975; Piaget, 1936, 1946;

Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007). Which are the tools that children use to

this end? Previous work in the field suggests that babbling children produce

vocalizations with distinct prosodic patterns depending on communicative context

(D’Odorico & Franco ,1991; Marcos, 1987; Papaeliou, Mindakis, & Cavouras, 2002;

Papaeliou & Trevarthen, 2006). Other studies have found that gestures are a crucial

tool for children to communicate their emotions and intentions with parents or

caregivers (Bates et al., 1975; Brinck, 2004; Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, &

Colonnesi, 2009; Cochet & Vauclair, 2010; Goldin-Meadow, 2007; Liszkowski, 2005;

Tomasello et al., 2007).

The goal of this dissertation is to investigate the early acquisition of prosodic and

gestural patterns by Catalan-babbling infants. Thus, three main issues will be

addressed: (1) the way in which gestural and prosodic cues signal the emergence of

intentional communication at the babbling stage, (2) the gesture-speech relation at the

babbling stage, focusing on how children temporally align gesture and speech and

comparing results with adult data, and (3) if prosodic cues are the tool that children use

to understand the different meanings behind a pointing gesture. As far as we know, no

previous research has studied the acquisition of prosody and gestures in Catalan-

babbling infants

In order to do this, the following data will be analyzed: first, a longitudinal corpus of four

Catalan-monolingual children will be used to study whether children use gesture and

prosody to communicate and also to investigate the temporal alignment between both

modalities. Next, the results of an experimental task with adults will allow us to

compare children’s results with adults’. Lastly, a perception task with an eye-tracking

system will be used to investigate whether the parents’ use of specific request prosody

while pointing to an object increases children’s comprehension of the speech act in

comparison with the pointing gesture accompanied by neutral prosodic contours. This

work, then, aims to offer a general perspective on the use of prosody and gestures

from a perception and a production point of view. In sum, we believe that this

dissertation will contribute to the study of language acquisition, giving evidence that

there is a stage before the emergence of the first words when children have already

developed very important linguistic skills.

Page 4: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

4

RESUM

És sabut que els infants desenvolupen la capacitat de comunicar emocions i intencions

abans de poder produir les primeres paraules (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975;

Piaget, 1936, 1946; Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007). Quines eines utilitzen

per a fer-ho? Alguns estudis previs suggereixen que durant el balboteig, els nens

produeixen vocalitzacions amb patrons prosòdics diferents segons la situació

comunicativa en què es trobin (D’Odorico & Franco ,1991; Marcos, 1987; Papaeliou,

Mindakis, & Cavouras, 2002; Papaeliou & Trevarthen, 2006). Altres autors han

demostrat que els infants utilitzen els gestos com a eina per a comunicar les seves

emocions i intencions amb els que els envolten (Bates et al., 1975; Brinck, 2004;

Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, & Colonnesi, 2009; Cochet & Vauclair, 2010;

Goldin-Meadow, 2007; Liszkowski, 2005; Tomasello et al., 2007).

L’objectiu de la tesi és investigar com els infants catalans adquireixen els patrons

prosòdics i gestuals durant l’etapa del balboteig. Per això, ens centrarem en tres

qüestions principals: (1) la manera com els gestos i la prosòdia demostren

l’emergència de la comunicació intencional en els infants, (2) la relació entre gest i

parla durant el balboteig, sobretot des del punt de vista de l’alineació temporal entre

ambdues modalitats, tot comparant els resultats amb dades de parlants adults de

català, i (3) la utilització de la prosòdia per part dels infants a l’hora d’entendre els

diversos sentits dels gestos díctics.

Per tal d’acomplir aquest objectiu, s’analitzaran diversos tipus de dades. En primer lloc,

l’anàlisi d’un corpus longitudinal de quatre infants catalans monolingües ens permetrà

veure com els infants utilitzen gestos i prosòdia per comunicar-se i veure com

s’adquireix l’alineació temporal de gest i parla durant el balboteig. En segon lloc, a

partir de l’anàlisi dels resultats d’una tasca experimental amb adults, podrem comparar

les dades amb els infants. Finalment, durem a terme una tasca de percepció amb eye-

tracking per a veure si quan els pares assenyalen un objecte mentre produeixen un

contorn entonatiu de petició, els infants entenen més clarament la intenció de l’adult

que no pas si el contorn entonatiu és neutre. Pel que ens consta, mai abans s’havia

estudiat l’adquisició de la prosòdia i els gestos en infants catalans. Creiem, doncs, que

aquest treball servirà per mostrar com s’adquireix la prosòdia i els gestos en català,

tant pel que fa a la producció com pel que fa a la percepció. En definitiva, pensem que

podem provar que abans de produir les primeres paraules, els infants ja han sigut

capaços de desenvolupar habilitats lingüístiques molt importants.

Page 5: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

5

1. INTRODUCTION

1.1. Object of analysis

This research focuses on the study of the early acquisition of prosody by Catalan-

speaking infants during the babbling stage. The study wants to analyze four main

topics: (1) how intentional communication at the babbling stage is signaled by gestural

and prosodic cues, (2) the development of gesture and speech before children are able

to speak, focusing on their alignment, (3) the differences between adults and children

in the temporal alignment between gesture and speech, and (4) how children who still

do not speak are able to perceive the distinct meanings of gestures and whether

prosody plays a role in this process of understanding others’ intentions.

1.2. Prior work

Studies on the acquisition of prosody have investigated the first speech patterns from

different points of view. Some studies have focused on early perception of prosody,

finding that attunement of stress perception abilities in the native language starts at 4-5

months of age, and that infants are aware of the stress properties of their mother

tongues at around 9 months of age (Jusczyk, Cutler, & Redanz, 1993; Pons & Bosch,

2007; Weber, Hahne, Friedrich, & Friederici, 2004). Less is known about the early

infant’s ability to discriminate between intonational contours. Sato, Sogabe, & Mazuka

(2006) discovered that infants at 0;4, 0;6, and 0;8 could distinguish pitch patterns of

target words, and Nazzi, Floccia, & Bertoncini (1998) found that newborns are able to

extract pitch contour information at the word level.

1.2.1. Continuity between babbling and early words

As for the early production of intonational contours, many studies support the

hypothesis of some kind of continuity between babbling and early words by finding

language-specific influences in infants’ vocalizations (Boysson-Bardies & Vihman,

1991; DePaolis, Vihman, & Kunnari, 2008; Hallé, Boysson-Bardies, & Vihman, 1991;

Levitt & Utman, 1992; López-Ornat & Karousou, 2005; Whalen, Levitt, & Wang, 1991).

Cross-linguistic research has found, for instance, that French newborns tend to

Page 6: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

6

produce rising melodies in early cries whereas Germans tend to produce falling

contours (Mampe, Friederici, Christophe, & Wermke, 2009). Likewise, Levitt & Utman

(1992) reported language-specific influences in one French-speaking child and one

English speaking child at 0;5, 0;8, 0;11, and 1;2, since the French child produced non-

final syllables closer in duration to one another than did the American infant. DePaolis

et al. (2008) found language-specific differences when comparing intonation of 10

English-, 10 French-, 5 Finnish- and 5 Welsh-speaking infants at the 4-word stage.

Whalen et al. (1991) studied 5 French-speaking children and 5 English-speaking

children from 0;7 to 0;11, finding that intonation patterns were different depending on

the language environment: French children used more rising intonation and less falling

intonation than English children. However, the authors also suggested that some of

these differences in contour direction could also be due to communicative purposes.

Similarly, Snow (2006) studied contour direction and pitch range of 60 English-learning

infants between 0;6 and 1;11 and concluded that the acquisition of intonation follows

these developmental stages: at 0;6-0;8 pitch patterns reflect pre-intentional

mechanisms linking physiological states and emotional experience; at 0;9-0;11 there is

a regression phase suppressing intonational expressiveness; and finally at 1;6-1;8

pitch patterns are actively controlled.

In general, these prosodic studies have not investigated prosody in relation to its

pragmatic intentions. In the following section some results are presented about studies

that have tried to link these two linguistic aspects, thus offering a wider perspective on

the acquisition of prosody.

1.2.2. Development of intonation in relation to the development of intentional

communication

Some studies analyzing intonational contours of babbling infants have suggested that

the development of intonation has to be studied in relation to the development of

intentional communication.

To our knowledge, the first author to study the emergence of communicative acts in

that sense was Halliday (1975). He analyzed his son’s early pitch contours from 0;9 to

2;6 and discovered that different vocal expressions were able to convey distinct

functions. Halliday found that his child produced mid falling tones when interacting with

Page 7: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

7

other people but low falling tones with narrower range when he was interested in the

modification of an object. López-Ornat, Gallego, Gallo, Karousou, Mariscal, & Nieva,

(2003) studied vocalizations from 0;8 to 2;6 in 95 Spanish-speaking infants. Their

results revealed that children as young as 0;8 were able to produce vocalizations with

pragmatic functions such as proto-conversations (defined as the child producing turn-

taking dialogues, with or without recognizable words) or proto-declaratives (defined as

the child trying to attract someone’s attention about something (s)he likes and

accompanying it with gestures).

D’Odorico & Franco (1991) studied 5 Italian-learning children from 0;4 to 0;11 months

of age, analyzing acoustically their vocalizations in terms of mean f0 values, maximum

and minimum pitch, average number of pitch change and units of vocalizations in a

prosodic unit, and mean duration. As for context types, vocalizations were classified as

vocalizations during infant manipulation of a toy (VIM), vocalizations during shared

experience (VSE, i.e. manipulating a toy but looking at the adult), vocalizations during

adult manipulation of a toy (VAM), and vocalizations during exchanges with the adult

(VEA, i.e. neither of them is manipulating the toy but they are both looking at each

other). Results offered support for a ‘selective production hypothesis’ whereby different

types of vocalizations were produced in different communication contexts until children

were 0;9. Thus, children at 0;4-0;6 used different contour directions when producing a

VIM and a VSE; at 0;6-0;8 children assimilated categories VSE and VAM; and at 0;8-

1;0 VIM vocalizations could not be distinguished from the other vocalizations. The

authors hypothesized that a child’s ability to acoustically distinguish between

categories tends to disappear as age increases. Therefore, children show a selective

production hypothesis until 0;9 but not thereafter, i.e. different patterns of non-

segmental features characterize sounds produced in different contexts. Because their

results revealed many individual differences among their infant subjects, the authors

concluded that they had failed to capture communicative differences across contexts.

In order to study how children use prosody to express distinct meaning, Marcos (1987)

analyzed 10 English-learning children between ages 1;2 and 1;10, i.e. already at the

one-word period. Results suggest that pitch range is higher for repeated requests than

for initial requests for objects, and higher for initial requests for objects and for co-

operation than for labeling. The study also analyzed vocalizations when the child was

giving and showing an object and found that their pitch range is in an intermediate rank

between requests and labeling.

Page 8: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

8

Papaeliou, Mindakis, & Cavouras (2002) analyzed six English-learning children from

0;7 to 0;11 and found that prosodic patterns were different when vocalizations

conveyed communicative functions from when they expressed emotions: vocalizations

carrying communicative functions were shorter, with lower f(0) values, and had greater

intensity than vocalizations expressing emotions. Similarly, Papaeliou & Trevarthen

(2006) observed that children displayed different prosodic patterns when vocalizations

were classified as communicative, or when classified as investigative: compared to

investigative vocalizations, communicative vocalizations had a higher mean and

maximum f(0), higher standard deviation of f(0), and shorter duration.

In sum, research on how children use prosody in order to express pragmatic meanings

suggest that at a very early stage in language development, children vocalize with

different prosodic patterns according to the context involved in the communicative

situation. However, very few studies investigate this hypothesis in children that are still

not producing words, i.e. at the babbling stage. And those studies focusing on this early

period have mainly found that babbling children use distinct prosodic patterns to

distinguish communicative from non-communicative (investigative) vocalizations.

(Papaeliou et al., 2002; Papaeliou & Trevarthen, 2006). Nevertheless, more research is

needed to investigate if babbling children use prosody as a tool not only to indicate the

communicative status of the vocalizations but also to express specific pragmatic

meaning, since it is during the babbling period that the emergence of intentionality

occurs (see section 3.2).

1.2.3. Communicative use of gestures

At the babbling stage, however, children communicate not only through prosody. At this

stage their use of gestures is crucial to communicate their emotions and intentions with

parents or caregivers. Research on the communicative role of gestures is based on the

assumption that human communication is inferential (Grice, 1957; Sperber & Wilson,

1986), which means that (1) interlocutors form intentions toward others’ intentions and

understand epistemic states to transmit and infer referential content, and that (2)

human communication is cooperative, i.e. the sender wants the recipient to understand

the message, and the recipient wants to understand the message.

Another state of affairs is the case of children’s use of gestures. Right after birth the

gestures produced by children are not communicative. However, at the age when

Page 9: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

9

children have completely developed the theory of mind, i.e., around four years of age,

they use gestures as communicatively as adults (McNeill, 1992). However, there is no

consensus about the communicative status of gestures during the period ranging from

the development of intentionality (around 0;9) and 4;0. Studies investigating the

communicative use of gestures in children have found results in two main directions.

From the one hand, infants’ gestures have been interpreted as a social tool that builds

on the emerging intentionality of children (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975) and that

would only serve one’s own benefit in an individualistic problem-solving way. According

to this view, then, it would not be clear if children gesture to influence the others’

intentional states in order to benefit the other and with cooperative motive to

communicate. From the other hand, infants’ communication is seen as a cooperative

tool from an ontogenetic point of view (Bruner, 1983; Werner & Kaplan, 1963) and a

motive for social contact in an ultra-social environment. This perspective, however, has

to face the issue of whether infants’ behavior is communicative or whether adults

interpret this behavior as communicative.

Liszkowski (2008) suggests a social-cognitively and motivationally rich referential

communication in children before they are able to speak. His research focuses on

infant deictic gestures, i.e. giving, showing, reaching, and pointing. At around 0;9,

children give and show objects to the others and they seem to cooperate and bring

objects to the attention of others. At this age, however, it may be that children are only

interacting with others in a non-referential way, simply with the purpose of establishing

social contact. Yet, at 1;0 children refer to present or absent entities by pointing,

indicating that they understand that others will attend to their gesture, that they can

attract their attention, and understand the shared background. Many other studies have

found evidence for social-cognitive and cooperative motives in infant pointing (Brinck,

2004; Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, & Colonnesi, 2009; Cochet & Vauclair, 2010;

Goldin-Meadow, 2007; Liszkowski, 2005; Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007).

Brinck (2004) distinguishes between declarative pointing –performed in order to make

the addressee do something for the subject– and imperative pointing –an attention-

seeking device to achieve joint reference with the addressee–. The author suggests

that only imperative pointing has a social motivation for cooperation, while declarative

pointing is mainly intersubjective. Contradictory results were found by Camaioni et al.

(2009), whose results suggest that declarative pointing is linked to the understanding of

others’ intentions, and that it emerges later than imperative pointing. Tomasello et al.

(2007) suggest that infants point in a social-cognitive and cooperative way, since they

Page 10: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

10

aim at directing others’ attention to entities within a joint attention frame, even with

absent entities. They distinguish between three types of pointing gestures:

• Informative (assertive) pointing: the communicator wants the recipient to know

something that he thinks she will find useful or interesting.

• Requestive (directive) pointing: the communicator wants the recipient to do

something that Hill help him, the communicator, in some way (including by

providing needed information, as in questions).

• Expressive pointing: the communicator wants the recipient to feel some attitude

or emotion that he is already feeling.

Cochet & Vauclair (2010) investigated differences between imperative pointing,

declarative expressive pointing, and declarative informative pointing. They found that

different hand shapes were used depending on the pointing gesture: imperative

gestures were associated with whole-hand pointing and declarative gestures were

more frequently characterized by an extended index finger. In terms of the speech

accompanying the gesture, declarative gestures were more frequently accompanied by

vocalizations than imperative gestures.

Pointing gestures are broadly considered the first gestures used by children with a

communicative purpose. Adults, nonetheless, use other gestures to communicate apart

from the deictic ones. However, the debate on whether gestures and language form a

single system is also very present in the literature on adult gestures. One perspective

suggests that gestures assist communication and do not provide any meaning apart

from that provided by speech (Butterworth & Beattie, 1978; Butterworth & Hadar, 1989;

Hadar & Butterworth, 1997; Krauss, Chen, & Gottesman, 2000). Another perspective

suggests that gestures are both communicative and production-aiding. Goldin-Meadow

(2005) states that gesture without speech takes a language-like form (with sentence-

level structure, word-level paradigm, and grammatical categories), whereas co-speech

gesture loses the language-like form and helps in conveying thoughts that do not easily

fit into the categorical system of language. Kita (2000) proposes the “Information

Packaging Hypothesis”, stating that some gestures help speakers organize information

in packages suitable for expression in a single utterance, since gestures also access to

a spatio-motoric way of thinking.

Finally, many studies have found evidence in favor of the hypothesis that gesture is

communicative and that it provides meaning apart from that provided by speech (Clark,

Page 11: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

11

1996; Duncan, 1996; Kendon, 2004; Loehr, 2004; McClave, 1991; McNeill, 1985,

1992). For instance, Bavelas, Kenwood, Johnson, & Phillips (2002) found that social

context in the form of visibility between speaker and addressee influences gesture rate;

Bavelas, Gerwing, Sutton, & Prevost (2008) and Gullberg (2006) found that social

context influence aspects of gesture form; Furuyama (2000) and Özyürek (2002)

discovered that the addressee location can influence speakers’ use of gesture space to

represent semantic information, and Holler & Wilkin (2009) found that speakers

accompany a higher proportion of definite references with iconic gestures than they do

with indefinite references, that ‘action gestures’ (gestures foregrounding information

about actions) accompany mainly definite references whereas ‘entity gestures’

(gestures foregrounding information about entities) mainly indefinite ones. McNeill’s

(2005) work states the existence of a growth point, i.e. a minimal unit of an imaginery-

language dialectic, a package that has both linguistic categorical and imagistic

components. Five main reasons are given in McNeill (1992) in order to justify that

gesture and speech form a single system: (1) gestures occur with speech in 90% of the

cases; (2) gesture and speech are semantically and pragmatically coexpressive; (3)

gesture and speech are synchronous; (4) gesture and speech develop together in

children, and (5) gesture and speech break together in aphasia.

In conclusion, many studies have found that children use gestures to communicate

their intentions. As far as we know, however, there are no studies dealing with the

children’s use of gestures together with speech to convey their intended meanings.

Some studies have claimed that gesture and speech form a single system. Do gesture

and speech form a single system from the beginning? Do these two modalities develop

together in children? Is there a specific point in time in children’s development where

gesture and speech start behaving like a single system? More research is needed in

order to solve these questions.

1.2.4. Synchronization between gesture and speech

Most of the research on the integration of gesture and speech, then, suggests a certain

degree of communicative status of gestures. The synchronization between gesture and

speech is used as one of the main arguments in favor of this hypothesis. The

synchronization between the two modalities can occur at three different levels (Kendon,

1980, McNeill, 1992):

Page 12: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

12

• Semantic synchronization: if gesture and speech co-occur they must refer to the

same semantic unit.

• Pragmatic synchronization: if gesture and speech co-occur they perform the

same pragmatic functions.

• Phonological synchronization: the stroke1 of the gesture precedes or ends at,

but does not follow, the phonological peak syllable of speech.

According to the phonological synchronization rule, there is a close relationship

between gesture and prosody, since the stroke of the gesture is generally synchronous

with the pitch accent of the accompanying speech. Some studies have found evidence

for the synchronization of gesture and speech at the phonological level in adult

speakers. Ferré’s (2010) results, for instance, confirm that the stroke of the gesture

starts before and ends after the semantically related word. Giorgolo & Verstraten

(2008) state that synchronization between audio and visual material influences the

addressee to consider the message as a good multimodal utterance or not, and this

decision is processed according to prosodic and semantic criteria. Loehr (2007) found

a rhythmic relation of hands, head, and voice such that each articulator produces

peaks synchronically with the other articulators. Finally, Rochet-Capellan, Laboissière,

Galván, & Schwartz (2008), discovered that arm movements and jaw movements are

related temporally, since the pitch accent occurs synchronically with the stroke of the

gesture, both in trochees and in iambs.

Very little is known about the development of the synchronization of gesture and

speech in children. It seems that at early stages of language development, children

show a preference for communication in the gestural modality over the verbal modality

(Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1985, 1988; Iverson, Capirci, & Caselli, 1994). As for the

semantic synchronization, it has been found that most of the gesture-speech

combinations contain gestures that are redundant with the information conveyed in

speech (Greenfield & Smith, 1976). However, these are not the total occurrences of

gesture-speech combinations: in some cases, children’s gestures can convey

information that is not present in speech, such as pointing at a ball while saying ‘give’

(Butcher & Goldin-Meadow, 2000; Capirci, Iverson, Pizzuto, & Volterra, 1996; Goldin-

Meadow & Butcher, 2003; Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005).

1 Stroke: the peak of effort in the gesture. For more information, see section 5.2.2.

Page 13: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

13

As far as we know, Butcher & Goldin-Meadow (2000) is the only study has dealt with

the developmental point at which children behave like adults in the production of co-

speech gestures, both in terms of the total proportion of gestures produced and in

terms of the phonological synchrony between gesture phases and prosodic patterns.

The authors analyzed 6 children from the one-word stage (14.5 months, on average)

until the two-word stage (23.5 months, on average) and found that the production of

gesture with respect to the total number of communicative acts is around 20% and

remains stable across the stage. Their results also suggest that it is at the end of the

one-word period that children produce gestures mainly in combination with speech,

such as adults do. In terms of the temporal alignment, the authors considered that

gesture and speech were adequately aligned when the vocalization occurred on the

stroke of the gesture2 or at the peak of the gesture3. They found that the

synchronization of gesture and speech does not occur until the end of the one-word

period. In terms of the semantic synchronization, they found that the combination of

communicative gestures and meaningful words (as opposed to meaningless

vocalizations, i.e. vocalizations that do not refer to any target word) increases a lot

during the one-word period. In conclusion, the authors found that it is when gesture is

combined with ‘meaningful speech’ that the two modalities are phonologically

synchronized.

Previous work, therefore, suggests that babbling children do not synchronized prosody

and gesture, since it is not until children produce two-word combinations that they start

doing so. However, no research has been actually done to investigate how the two

modalities are aligned at the babbling stage. It may be that phonological

synchronization is still not adult-like, but we no study has shown how it is. More

research is needed to describe the temporal alignment between prosody and gesture

at a stage when children use both modalities to convey their intended meanings.

1.2.5. Intonation as a tool for children to determine intentions in co-speech gestures

2 Stroke: the peak of effort in the gesture. For more information, see section 5.2.2. 3 Peak of the gesture: the farthest extension before the hand began to retract. It coincides with

the final point in the stroke. Other authors such as Loehr (2004) have called this point ‘apex’.

Page 14: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

14

In his development of the Cooperative Principle, Grice (1975) stated that humans

communicate mainly with the motivation of being cooperative. In fact, previous work by

Austin (1962) and Searle (1969) in the ‘theory of speech acts’ had established five

main types of speech acts present in any communication act: assertives, directives,

commissives, expressives, and declaratives (see section 3.2). From these five speech

acts, Tomassello et al. (2007) consider that three of them are the most basic and

defined in terms of helping and sharing:

• Assertive (or informative): the communicator wants the recipient to know

something that he thinks will be useful or interesting.

• Directive (or requestive): the communicator wants the recipient to do something

that will help the communicator in some way.

• Expressive: the communicator wants the recipient to feel some attitude or

emotion that the communicator is already feeling.

The authors state that when children are in their first stages of language development,

their motivations when communicating are mainly of these three kinds: assertive,

directive, and expressive.

There is some evidence that children can understand these motivations at an early age

in language development. Some research has found that around 3;0, children can infer

the intention hidden in an adult communicative act (Povinelli, Reaux, Bierschwale,

Allain, & Simon, 1997; Tomasello, Call, & Gluckman, 1997). And even earlier in

language development, Behne, Carpenter, & Tomasello (2005) found that at 1;2,

children can already infer the location of a hidden toy by means of the communicative

cues they observe in adults. However, they also reported that children could infer the

hidden location only when the adult produce a clear cue such as ostensive gazing or

pointing to the location, not when the adult gaze to the object in an absent-mindedly

way.

In a study of children from 0;9 to 1;5, Camaioni et al. (2009) found that comprehension

precedes production of communicative pointing gestures in typical development. Their

results suggest that children produce earlier imperative pointing than declarative

pointing, and that this difference is due to the fact the both types of pointing entail

different socio-cognitive abilities. They argue that imperative pointing is just an

understanding of others as causal agents, whereas declarative pointing reflects an

understanding of others as intentional agents. They also found that children who first

Page 15: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

15

inferred the act of another person were also the first to produce declarative pointing at

1;0 and at 1;5. However, no relation was found between the production and

comprehension of imperative pointing and intention understanding.

Aureli, Perucchini, & Genco (2009) analyzed children from 1;4 to 1;8 and found that

they are able to recognize two different social intentions in a pointing gesture,

depending on the common ground involved. Thus, children interpreted a pointing

gesture as informative when the adult had pointed at the hidden toy’s location;

however, children identified a pointing gesture as declarative when the adult pointed at

a referent in a different context, since the child only reacted by commenting or smiling

at the adult.

Very few studies address the relationship between gesture and prosody. In a recent

study, Cochet & Vauclair (2010) tried to elicit imperative, declarative expressive, and

declarative informative pointing in infants aged 1;3-2;6. They found that declarative

pointing was accompanied by vocalizations more often than imperative pointing and

that declarative pointing lasted longer than the imperative one. They state that it would

be interesting to investigate the nature of the vocalizations accompanying declarative

and imperative pointing, suggesting that imperative pointing might occur more often

with vocalizations, whereas declarative pointing might occur with words (or

pseudowords). The authors also suggest that imperative pointing could be originated

from the non-communicative reaching actions, since children might learn that their

reaching actions produce an effect on adults. However, they hypothesize that

declarative pointing might be originated from imitation processes.

Thus, it is seems reasonable to assume that children are able to produce pointing

gestures with distinct social motives at an early age in language development. And that

even before in their developmental process, they have the ability of understanding

these motives in others. Thus, some studies suggest that babbling children understand

the intention behind an adult’s pointing gesture by means of the common ground

involved and other cues such as ostensive gazing. However, more research is needed

to investigate the role of prosody in this early ability in cognitive development. Since

most of the adults’ gestures are accompanied by speech, it might be that children’s rely

on this speech to understand motivations in gestures.

In conclusion, previous work on the use of prosody to communicate intentionally has

suggested that at the babbling stage, children use prosodic contours to distinguish

Page 16: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

16

vocalizations uttered with a communicative purpose, with an emotive purpose or

without any of them. However, as far as we know, no previous study has dealt with this

issue with a deeper analysis: do babbling children use prosody not only to signal the

communicative status of their vocalizations but also to express specific pragmatic

meanings within communicative vocalizations? Previous literature has also shown a

tight relation between gesture and prosody at early stages in language development,

suggesting that children use both modalities to communicate, sometimes together and

sometimes separately. Yet, more research is needed to know when children start using

prosody and gestures as a single system, such as adults seem to do. And the broad

picture of the use of prosody —accompanied or not by gesture— to communicate

would be incomplete if we do not deal with it from a perceptual point of view. Thus, we

would be sure about the abilities that children have already acquired at this early stage

in language development.

Given that some questions still remain unsolved in the previous literature, this

dissertation aims at addressing them, both from a production and a perceptual point of

view.

2. GOALS OF THE DISSERTATION

This work has the goal to investigate the early acquisition of prosody by Catalan-

speaking infants during the babbling stage. Three main issues will be addressed: (1)

the way gestural and prosodic cues signal the emergence of intentional communication

at the babbling stage, (2) the integration of gesture and speech at the stage when

children are still not able to speak, focusing on the alignment of the two modalities, and

comparing results with adult data, and (3) if prosodic cues are a tool that children use

to understand the different meanings behind a pointing gesture.

To our knowledge, no previous research has studied the acquisition of prosody in

Catalan-babbling infants, even though prosody is accepted to be one of the first

linguistic abilities to be acquired in the language development process (Prieto et al., in

press). Crucially, before children are able to speak, they have developed intentional

communication (Bates et al., 1975; Piaget, 1936, 1946; Tomasello et al., 2007). Then,

do babbling children express somehow their recently acquired intentions? Our

hypothesis is that before children are able to produce words, prosodic cues and

gestures are the tools they use to communicate emotions, intentions, and needs. This

Page 17: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

17

study also aims at investigating if the relation between gesture and prosody at the

babbling stage occurs in an adult-like manner in terms of the temporal alignment.

Finally, this study also wants to investigate the role of prosody in determining the

specific intentionality behind a pointing gesture. Thus, this work will try to offer a

general perspective on the use of prosody and gestures from a perception and a

production point of view at an early stage in language development.

3. RELEVANT THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

3.1. The study of prosodic development

The Autosegmental Metrical model of intonation (which started with Pierrehumbert,

1980) is one of the most widely used frameworks to analyze prosodic patterns. This

framework represents intonation in terms of level pitch targets, and annotates pitch

movements associated with metrically strong syllables (pitch accents) and boundary

tones (prosodic boundaries) by means of the Tone and Break Indices system, or ToBI.

Some studies on the acquisition of prosody have successfully used the Autosegmental

Metrical model to describe the intonation system in children (Frota & Vigário, 2008;

Prieto, Estrella, Thorson, & Vanrell, in press).

Once children have acquired their first words, i.e. at the 25-word period, an analysis in

terms of pitch accents and boundary tones in AM terms has been proposed. Frota &

Vigário (2008) found that a European Portuguese child acquired the inventory of pitch

accents and boundary tones in an adult-like way at 1;9, with the emergence of such

contours as early as 1;5. For this child, intonational development occurred five months

before the onset of the two-word stage, which was at 2;2. Prieto et al. (in press)

investigated the development of prosodic patterns in four Catalan children and in two

Spanish children and demonstrated that children at 1;1 and 1;3 are able to produce a

set of adult-like intonation contours: at the one-word period, children produce

statements and calling contours, and at the two-word period they produce complex

contours such as requests, counter-expectations, (L* HL%), insistent requests (L+H*

LHL%), and interrogative contours (L* HH%). Their results, thus, indicate that Catalan

and Spanish children produced the basic phonologically distinct pitch contours from the

25-word period, and that at the two-word speech they show an important knowledge of

the adult intonational grammar.

Page 18: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

18

However, the AM model cannot be used when studying prosodic patterns at the

babbling stage because children do not produce clearly defined strong syllables in their

first vocalizations. Hence, research on prosody at the babbling stage has to study

suprasegmental issues other than those involving only pitch accents and boundary

tones. Typically, the development of prosodic patterns before children can produce

pitch accents and boundary tones has been analyzed mainly through the following

phonetic distinct features: pitch direction, pitch range, and duration (DePaolis et al.,

2008; Snow & Balog, 2002).

Pitch direction (or contour direction) is the curve that tracks the perceived pitch over

time. It indicates intonation and it can be produced in two distinct directions: rising

intonation (the pitch increases over time) or falling intonation (the pitch decreases over

time). Pitch or contour direction has been widely used in cross-linguistic studies in

order to compare children’s productions with adult’s productions in languages with a

typical contour direction. For instance, some studies comparing French and English

have observed that children follow adult-like patterns in producing their vocalizations

with a rising or falling contour, respectively (DePaolis et al., 2008; Vihman & DePaolis,

1998; Whalen et al., 1991).

Pitch range is defined as the existing interval between the valley and the peak in a

rising pitch movement, or between the peak and the valley in a falling pitch movement

(Prieto, 2003). Pitch range has been studied in early children’s productions to

investigate whether children control it in an adult-like manner, i.e. if they use it to

distinguish between semantic meanings of the utterances. Marcos (1987), for instance,

analyzed 10 English-learning children between ages 1;2 and 1;10. The author found

that pitch range is higher for repeated requests than for initial requests for objects, and

that it is higher for initial requests for objects and for co-operation than for statements.

The study also analyzed giving and showing vocalizations and found that their pitch

range is in an intermediate rank between requests and statements. Snow (2006)

suggests that the magnitude of children’s accent range develops markedly at about 18

months of age and proposes that the pattern of intonation development is U-shaped: at

0;9-0;1 there is a decline in accent range production that is equal in magnitude to the

significant increase that occurs at 1;6-1;8. Esteve-Gibert & Prieto’s (submitted) also

show that children use wider pitch range when producing communicative vocalizations

than when not intending to communicate. The authors also found that when

communicating, children use pitch range to distinguish between distinct pragmatic and

Page 19: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

19

expressive intentions: pitch range was wider when requesting and expressing

discontent, and narrower when responding to a stimulus or uttering a statement.

Many studies on the development of prosodic patterns have also analyzed duration in

children’s vocalization. Levitt & Utman (1992) found that a French child produced non-

final syllables closer in duration to one another than did the American infant. This result

is consistent with adult French, since French has greater isosyllabicity in non-final

syllables. DePaolis et al. (2008) found that duration showed the clearest difference

across infants learning different languages such as French, English, Welsh, and Finish,

since children exaggerate final syllable lengthening if the languages of their linguistic

environments exhibit this phenomenon (as is the case of French). Hallé’s et al. (1991)

results show that French children produced final lengthening on the last syllable of

prosodic groups or words whereas Japanese children did not produce final

lengthening, again in keeping with their ambient languages. Papaeliou & Trevarthen

(2006) analyzed four English-learning infants at 0;10 and found that communicative

vocalizations had shorter duration than investigative vocalizations. And Esteve-Gibert &

Prieto’s (submitted) results confirm Papaeliou & Trevarthen’s hypothesis, since

Catalan-babbling children produce longer vocalizations when communicating than

when not communicating. The authors also found that when communicating, children

use duration to distinguish between distinct pragmatic and expressive intentions:

requesting and expressions of discontent lasted longer than responses to a stimulus or

statements.

3.2. The study of intentional communication

The ‘speech act theory’ was first proposed by Austin (1962) and further developed by

Searle (1969). This theory states that every sentence that is uttered in a language

carries out these speech acts: locutions, illocutions, and perlocutions. Locutionary acts

are acts required for the making of speech, i.e. saying something (a locution);

illocutionary acts are conventional social acts recognized as such by both the speaker

and the hearer and that take place when a sentence is uttered, i.e. the performance of

an act in saying something; finally, perlocutionary acts are acts that have an effect,

planned or unplanned, on the feelings, thoughts or actions of either the speaker or the

listener. In Searle’s (1969) further development of the speech act theory, the author

stated that all speech acts can be divided in two: a propositional content (or locution),

Page 20: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

20

and a performative content (or illocution). The performative or illocutionary acts are

mainly divided into one of these categories:

• Assertives: statements describing a state of affairs in the world.

• Directives: statements to make the other’s actions fit the propositional content.

• Commissives: statements to commit the speaker to a course of action.

• Expressives: statements expressing the ‘sincerity condition’ of the speech act.

• Declaratives: statements attempting to change the world by “representing it as

having been changed”.

Besides, according to the Grice’s Cooperative Principle (1975), all communicative acts

intend to fulfill two intentions: a communicator wants not only to convey a message but

also that the addressee recognizes his intention to do so, and would not be satisfied

with the outcome of his action unless this recognition is realized. Sperber & Wilson

(1995) stated two main intentions: the communicator’s informative intention, aiming to

achieve an effect in the addressee by modifying the cognitive environment, and the

communicative intention, when the communicator aims to make his informative

intention manifest for the addressee (or mutually manifest between the communicator

and the addressee).

In general, research on language development has applied Searle’s distinction

between propositional content and performative content, finding that children provide

performative content in their vocalizations before the propositional content is

adequately displayed. As Bates et al. (1975) point out, in children a locution requires

the uttering of sounds and construction of propositions, thus implying the onset of

verbal speech, an illocution requires the intentional use of a conventional sign to carry

our any socially recognized function, and a perlocution simply requires that the signal

produced by a person have some effect on the recipient. Merging Austin’s and Searle’s

proposal, Bates et al. (1975) propose the following developmental stages:

• A perlocutionary stage: the child has a systematic effect on his listener without

having an intentional control over that effect.

• An illocutionary stage: the child intentionally uses nonverbal signals to convey

requests and to direct adult attention to objects and events.

• A locutionary stage: the child constructs propositions and utters speech sounds

within the same performative sequences that he previously expressed

nonverbally.

Page 21: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

21

Research on development of intentional communication has focused on two main

performative acts (Bates et al., 1975): imperatives and declaratives. Imperative acts

use the adult as the means to a desired action or object, whereas declarative acts use

an object (through pointing, showing, giving, etc.) as the means to obtain adult

attention. This use of human agents to obtain or operate objects, and the use of objects

to operate on human attention appears at around 0;8-0;10 months of age, and

represents the children’s development of illocutionary acts. It starts with actions like

showing and it progressively changes into giving and pointing actions. This period of

development of the illocutionary acts coincides with the fourth and fifth stages in the

sensorimotor stage of the Piaget’s theory of cognitive development (Piaget, 1936,

1946). At the fourth and fifth stages (going from 0;8 to 1;8), children begin to plan

deliberately the steps to meet an object and discover new means to meet goals.

Hence, in the study of the development of intentional communication the concept of the

theory of mind plays a central role. The theory of mind is the ability to attribute mental

states, i.e. pretending, desires, intents, belief, knowledge, to oneself and others, and to

understand that others have mental states that are different from one’s own. When

studying the cognitive development of the theory of mind in children, one of the aims of

the researchers is to establish the precise point of time in development where it is

possible to identify in children the precursors of the theory of mind. Meltzoff, Gopnik, &

Repacholi (1999) offer a review of the some findings in the development of the theory

of mind. As Camaioni et al. (2009) state, it seems that children have developed the

theory of mind at some age between 3 and 5 years old, but there is no strong evidence

about what occurs before and during this stage. Bellagamba & Tomasello’s (1999)

suggest that at 1;6 children can infer the goal of another person after seeing his/her

unsuccessful attempts, but that at 1;0 children cannot imitate unsuccessful goal-

directed actions. They claim that at 1;6 children understand that people’s actions are

goal directed and intentional. Tomasello et al. (2007), Tomasello, Carpenter, Call,

Behne, & Moll (2005), and Liszkowski (2005, 2006) propose a rich interpretation of the

prelinguistic communications, suggesting that infant pointing for an adult gives

evidence that the child tries to influence the adult’s intentional/mental state.

It is evident, then, that human interactions are produced on a common ground basis

that enables the understanding of intentions in communication. This common

background is the joint attentional frame, a triadic situation involving two people and an

object or event outside the two. As shown by Behne et al. (2005), at 1;2 infants inferred

a hidden toy’s location based on the common ground that both the infant and the adult

Page 22: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

22

knew that the infant was seeking the toy. Instead, it has been reported that apes fail to

perform the task of finding the hidden toy (Call & Tomasello, 2005). Most of the

evidence found in favor of the early ability of children to establish joint attentional

frames comes from the fact that before 1 year of age, children follow adult pointing

gestures to targets and check back to make sure of her target (Carpenter, Nagell, &

Tomasello, 1998). As explained in section 1.2.5, at 1;0 children point to establish social

interaction and cooperation with three main motives (Tomasello et al., 2007):

• They want others to feel things (expressive pointing)

• They want others to know things (informative pointing)

• They want others to do things (requestive pointing)

Thus, at the end of their first year of life infants begin to use communicative gestures to

actively direct adult attention to outside entities in triadic interactions. First and most

important are deictic gestures such as pointing, showing, and offering, usually

accompanied by the infant’s looking at the adult in alternation with looks to the object.

The alternation of gaze between the object and the adult indicates that the child is

aware of the effects his or her signals will have on the other person (Bates et al., 1975;

Bretherton, 1991). As Tomasello & Camaioni (1997) documented, the key difference

between human infants and chimpanzees is that infants use their deictic gestures —

and especially pointing— not only for imperative purposes (to obtain a desired object or

event) but also for declarative purposes (to share with another person interest or

attention to some object or event).

In sum, understanding the common background or joint attention is a prerequisite for

the development of the theory of mind and thus the adequate perception and

production of communicative intentions. Even though at 0;3-0;6 infants point towards

objects, their action is not communicative because they still do not understand

intentions, attention, and shared attentions. However, at 0;9 children already

understand goals and at 1;0 they understand perceptions. At these stages, too, infants

begin to use communicative gestures to actively direct adult attention to outside entities

in triadic interactions by means of deictic gestures such as pointing, showing, and

offering. After children’s first birthday, i.e. 1;0-1;3 children can determine what others

know, and it is also at this age that children are aware of the information they share

with another person in a joint attentional frame.

Page 23: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

23

3.3. The study of gesture development

Based on the assumption that gesture is communicative and that it forms a single

system together with speech, and that in fact they are part of speech, McNeill (1992,

2005) defines gestures as ‘everyday occurrences —the spontaneous, unwitting, and

regular accompaniments of speech that we see in our moving fingers, hands, and

arms. The classification of the types of gestures proposed by McNeill (1992) is the

most used in gesture studies. The author states that gestures can be classified into one

of these categories (although many gestures involve more than one category):

• Iconic gestures, when there is a close formal relationship to the semantic

content of speech.

• Metaphoric gestures, when the pictorial event presents an abstract idea.

• Beats, when the hand moves along with the rhythmic pulsation of speech.

• Cohesive gestures, used to tie together thematically related but temporally

separated parts of the discourse.

• Deictic gestures, such as pointing, indicating objects and events in the concrete

or abstract world.

Right after birth, however, human beings do not gesture in a communicative way. It is

not until 0;8-0;10 that children gesture in order to influence the mental state of others,

either because they want the others to do, know, or feel something. The first

communicative gestures that typically developing children produce are deictic gestures

such as pointing, giving, showing, or requesting (Bates et al., 1975; Iverson & Goldin-

Meadow 2005; Özçalişkan & Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Sansavini, Guarini, & Stefanini,

2010; Tomasello, 2007). In these first gestures that infants produce, the referents can

be identified only in the physical and social context in which communication takes

place. At this age, infants produce another type of gestures: gestural routines such as

clapping hands, kissing with the moving the hand from the mouth towards the recipient,

and waving good-bye. At around 1;0, children start reproducing actions associated with

specific objects, such as bringing a phone to the ear or brushing to the hair. They are

called object-actions (Sansavini et al., 2010). Capirci, Contaldo, Caselli, & Volterra

(2005) suggest that object-actions are later expressed in a symbolic way with iconic

gestures. Thus, children would refer to the telephone by gesturing as having a phone

by the ear and being in a conversation, showing that object-actions are then converted

into iconic gestures. When focusing on the relation between gesture and speech at this

stage, i.e. before the one-word stage, some research has suggested that children

Page 24: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

24

produce more gesture-alone acts than gesture-speech combinations (Butcher &

Goldin-Meadow 2000; Esteve-Gibert & Prieto, 2011). At the early one-word stage,

however, this tendency is inverted and children already produce more gesture-speech

combinations than gesture-alone acts. When looking at the gesture-speech

combinations that children produce around the first year of life, it has been found that

gesture and speech either convey the meaning of the same referent (for instance,

producing a bye-bye gesture and saying ‘bye’) or the gesture identifies the referent and

the word refers to its meaning (for instance, pointing to a dog and saying ‘dog’) (Capirci

et al., 1996; Pizzuto & Capobianco, 2005).

From 1;4 to 1;8, children mostly produce pointing gestures. But they also produce

iconic gestures, which at this age start appearing more and more with verbs and

adjectives (Capone & McGregor, 2004). In terms of the relation between gesture and

speech, Özçalişkan & Goldin-Meadow (2005) found that at 1;6 many children produce

constructions combining argument + predicate in gesture-speech combinations (for

instance, saying ‘mommy’ and pointing at a couch), whereas constructions like these

do not occur in speech-only acts until children are 1;10. Similarly, at 1;10 many children

produce utterances with two predicates (for instance, saying ‘I like it’ and producing an

eat gesture) in gesture-speech combinations, whereas only few of them produce these

kind of utterances only by means of speech.

Children increase significantly their usage of iconic gestures between 3;0 and 5;0, and

their gesture-speech combinations are more adequately synchronized (Sansavini et al.,

2010). At this stage, however, children still do not produce neither metaphoric gestures

nor beat gestures. It is it not until children are 5 years of age that they develop

completely the rest of the gestural system (McNeill, 1992).

The study of prosodic, intentional, and gestural development at early stages in

language development has shown evidence that very young infants are able to

communicate through gestures or prosody. In the present dissertation, nevertheless,

we would like to demonstrate that these two modalities —namely, gesture and

prosody— are very tightly related from the beginning, enabling children to

communicate without words. This research would be a step further in the investigation

in language acquisition, suggesting that babbling children use prosody and gesture as

two faces of the same coin.

Page 25: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

25

4. HYPOTHESES

Based on the research exposed in the previous sections, three main hypotheses will be

tested in three experimental studies:

1. Experimental study 1 . Gesture and prosody are the communicative cues used

by children when they are still not able to express themselves by means of

words. Following Papaeliou & Trevarthen (2006), we hypothesize that

communicative vocalizations would be shorter than investigative ones. We

further suggest that when producing communicative vocalizations children

would use prosodic and gestural cues in order to express specific pragmatic

meanings such as discontent, requests, responses to stimuli, or general

statements. In fact, our suggestion is that language does not start when

children produce their first words, but that it is already functioning by means of

the communicative usage of gestures and prosody.

2. Experimental study 2 . Children synchronize gesture and speech before than

what is suggested. Previous studies suggest that children do not integrate

gesture and speech in an adult-like way until the two-word stage (Butcher &

Goldin-Meadow 2000). We hypothesize that children produce co-speech

gestures and synchronize gesture and speech in an adult-like manner before

the two-word stage. This target alignment for children would be one in which the

pitch peak and pitch accent of the speech is produced during the stroke of the

gesture (McNeill, 1992; Kendon, 1980).

3. Experimental study 3 . In case that the first and second hypotheses are

confirmed, and given that in language development, most language abilities are

first understood than produce, we assume that children understand

communicative intentions before they can produce them. Plus, we hypothesize

that children are able to understand other’s communicative intentions relying on

gestural and prosodic communicative cues.

These three experimental studies will show whether babbling infants use prosody to

produce and comprehend communicative vocalizations with intentionality, and how

prosody and gesture work together to entail the intended meaning. The dissertation will

be organized as follows. First, an introductory chapter with theoretical framework and

Page 26: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

26

previous work in the field will be presented and discussed. Second, the goals of the

study and the hypotheses will be exposed. Then, three experimental studies to test the

hypotheses will be described, with details about materials, methods, results,

conclusion, and discussion. Finally, the conclusions that may be drawn from the three

experimental studies will be laid out.

5. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES

5.1. Study 1

5.1.1. Materials

The first study aims at investigating if gesture and prosody are the communicative cues

used by children when they are still not able to express themselves by means of words.

In order to do it, a longitudinal corpus of four Catalan-monolingual children will be used

to study if gesture and prosody are the communicative cues used by babbling children

to communicate.

This corpus, Esteve-Prieto corpus, is an online Phon database of four monolingual

Catalan speaking-children that were recorded from 0;6 to 2;0 (still in progress): An, Bi,

Ma, and On. We plan to incorporate this corpus in the CHILDES database, once it will

be phonetically and orthographically transcribed. It is available in the following website:

[http://prosodia.upf.edu/phon/en/corpora/description/esteveprieto.html]

All four children were video-recorded at their homes during weekly 30-minute sessions

using a SONY camera, model DCR-DVD202E PAL. Recordings were made by the

author of this Ph.D. project, who was previously acquainted with the families and

children. Children were always recorded in the same room of their respective homes,

typically in their living-rooms, during free play sessions. All children were recorded as

they interacted with their mothers except for one child, An, who was recorded while

interacting with both her father and her mother in most of the sessions. A tripod was

used, placed as close to the child as possible and positioned so that the camera was

pointing toward the child’s face.

Page 27: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

27

We selected for analysis vocalizations produced at three specific points in time, i.e.

when children were 0;7, 0;9, and 0;11. Table 1 summarizes the data for all sessions

included in this study. These ages were selected based on the hypothesis that these

vocalizations would display the typical features of certain stages of development:

before the onset of intentional communication, when intentionality starts, and when

intentionality is already developed (Piaget, 1936, 1946; Trevarthen, 1977, 1979, 1982,

among others).

Table 1. Recorded sessions included in the study, classified by children’s age, and duration of the

session.

5.1.2. Codification

The approximately 20 hours of recordings were segmented into more than 3,000

vocalizations. From these, around 300 were excluded from the analysis because of the

following circumstances: (1) when child and parent overlapped when vocalizing, (2)

when ambient noise was too loud, (3) when the child vocalized while having an object

Participant Age Duration Participant Age Duration

An 0;7.10 0:30:09 Bi 0;7.02 0:31:07

0;7.17 0:28:22 0;7.09 0:32:02

0;7.29 0:34:03 0;7.16 0:30:33

0;9.04 0:36:39 0;7.22 0:33:24

0;9.11 0:19:03 0;9.23 0:30:36

0;9.18 0:36:45 0;9.29 0:33:05

0;9.25 0:37:30 0;11.05 0:34:41

0;11.03 0:33:00 0;11.12 0:36:20

0;11.08 0:36:34 0;11.18 0:34:21

0;11.15 0:36:35 0;11.25 0:26:09

0;11.22 0:33:20

Ma 0;7.06 0:36:58 On 0;7.02 0:38:01

0;7.13 0:27:19 0;7.15 0:34:29

0;7.20 0:26:33 0;7.22 0:33:10

0;9.04 0:19:03 0;7.28 0:34:44

0;9.09 0:34:02 0;9.03 0:36:08

0;9.17 0:37:15 0;9.11 0:34:40

0;9.29 0:35:00 0;9.16 0:36:30

0;11.00 0:32:29 0;11.14 0:26:25

0;11.05 0:34:43 0;11.23 0:37:12

0;11.12 0:39:44

0;11.19 0:35:20

0;11.25 0:33:23

TOTAL AMOUNT OF TIME RECORDED: 23:02:10

Page 28: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

28

inside his/her mouth, and (4) when the sound did not show a visible trace on the

spectrogram.

After segmenting vocalizations, three main analyses were conducted with data: an

acoustic analysis, a gestural analysis, and a pragmatic analysis. We explain now in

more detail the specificities of these analyses.

First, the pragmatic analysis was performed using Phon software system (Rose,

McWhinney, Byrne, Hedlund, Maddocks, O’Brien, & Warehem, 2005). All vocalizations

were first annotated in terms of the communicative function they conveyed. Since the

one of the aims was to discover whether the vocalizations conveying communicative

information are different from when not intending to communicate, we first classified

them as being ‘communicative’ or ‘investigative’. Following Papaeliou & Trevarthen

(2006), a vocalization was considered to be investigative if the infant was holding an

object, inspecting an object or completing a task; a vocalization was considered to be

communicative if the child was interacting with an adult, pointing, directing eye-gaze to

the adult, and reaching or giving something. Thus, communication was established in a

context of joint attention where triadic relations between the parent, the child, and an

object or event were observed. Since we also hypothesized that children use prosody

and gestures to express specific pragmatic meanings, a more specific pragmatic

analysis was conducted. Thus, all communicative vocalizations were further divided

into these categories:

• Request (speech act)

• Response to a stimulus (speech act)

• General statement (speech act)

• Vocative (speech act)

• Discontent (emotion)

• Satisfaction (emotion)

• Surprise (emotion)

These categories could overlap. Hence, it could be the case that a communicative

vocalization was then annotated as a ‘request’ with ‘discontent’, since the child could

require the other to do something by means of showing discontent towards the actual

state of facts.

Page 29: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

29

Second, the gestural analysis was performed in parallel with the pragmatic analysis

explained above. Each vocalization was annotated in terms of the gestures displayed

by children when vocalizing, using Phon software system (Rose et al., 2005). Table 2

shows the gesture categories used (based on Allwood, Cerrato, Jokinen, Navarretta, &

Paggio, 2007).

Gaze direction absent gaze

gaze to camera

gaze to object

gaze to parent

Manual gestures clapping hands

extending arms

embracing parent

manipulating object

moving arms

pointing object

moving hands

shaking arms

no specific manual gesture

Facial gestures furrowing brows

opening eyes

closing eyes

opening mouth

closing mouth

pouting

shaking head

smiling

rising eyebrows

no specific facial gesture

Table 2. Gesture categories used in the gesture analysis.

Third, the acoustic analysis was performed to find out whether different prosodic

patterns are at play when infants try to communicate or convey a set of pragmatic

functions. In order to perform the acoustic analysis, we manually extracted all the audio

files (in the .wav format) from our Phon corpus and analyzed them with the Praat

software package (Boersma & Weenink, 2005). A set of prosodic features were

manually labeled: duration and pitch contour, i.e. pitch maximum and minimum points,

and starting and end points of vocalizations. The aim was to analyze global pitch range

of the contour and total duration, which are the features that are most commonly used

in studies of the prosody of infants’ vocalizations (Marcos, 1987; Papaeliou et al., 2002;

Papaeliou & Trevarthen, 2006; Scherer, 1986).

Page 30: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

30

On the one hand, to obtain the pitch range value, we selected three pitch points from

the fundamental frequency contour: the first pitch point (p1) representing the pitch

value at the beginning of the vocalization, also referred to as the reference level of the

speaker; the second pitch point (p2) selected at the maximum peak in the fundamental

frequency contour; and finally, the third point (p3) representing the pitch value at the

end of the vocalization. The pitch range was then calculated by subtracting the

minimum pitch value from the maximum pitch. In order to compare different pitch

ranges across the three children, the pitch values were extracted in semitones and in

Hz. On the other hand, to obtain the duration value, In order to obtain the total duration

of the vocalization, the first point (t1) and last point (t2) in the fundamental frequency

line were selected. Following Papaeliou & Trevarthen’s (2006) work, we considered

two sounds to be distinct vocalizations if they were separated by at least 50 ms. Figure

1 shows an example of the acoustic annotation in Praat.

Figure 1. Illustration of a vocalization performed by Ma, at 0;11.

5.1.3. Summary results

Statistical analysis of the data showed that pitch range and duration are both

significantly affected by the communicative status of the vocalization. As for pitch

range, vocalizations display wider pitch range when the children are communicating

than when they are performing investigative vocalizations. Further analyses of

communicative vocalizations have revealed that depending on the pragmatic intention

expressed, pitch range is wider or narrower. For instance, protests and requests have

wider pitch ranges than responses and statements.

Page 31: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

31

0;110;90;7

Pitc

h ra

nge

(st)

5,00

4,50

4,00

3,50

3,00

2,50

CommunicativeInvestigative

Communicativeness

Figure 2. Error bars of the pitch range of vocalizations for all infants, separated by age and

communicative status.

Age0;110;90;7

Pitc

h ra

nge

(st)

12,50

10,00

7,50

5,00

2,50

0,00

StatementResponseSatisfactionRequestProtest

Intention

Figure 3. Box plots of the pitch range of the vocalizations (in semitones) as a function of the specific

pragmatic function at the three different ages for all infants.

As for duration, communicative vocalizations are shorter in general than investigative

ones. Yet these results also seem to show that the duration cue is not controlled until

children are 0;9. Our subsequent analysis of communicative vocalizations, whereby

they were separated into specific pragmatic intentions, showed that the durational

patterns of the vocalizations are strongly influenced by pragmatic functions.

Specifically, responses and statements behave similarly, being shorter than the other

pragmatic functions. Protests are the longest vocalizations, followed by requests, and

Page 32: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

32

in the middle there are satisfactions, which are shorter than protests and requests but

longer than responses and statements.

0;110;90;7

Tota

l dur

atio

n (m

s)

1200

1100

1000

900

800

700

600

CommunicativeInvestigative

Communicativeness

Figure 4. Error bars of the duration of vocalizations for all infants, separated by age and communicative

status.

0;110;90;7

Tot

al d

urat

ion

(ms)

3000

2500

2000

1500

1000

500

0

StatementResponseSatisfactionRequestProtest

Intention

Figure 5. Box plots of the duration of vocalizations at three different ages for all infants, broken down

by pragmatic function.

The analysis of gestural patterns (which included annotation of gaze direction, manual

gestures, and facial gestures produced by children) revealed a very close relationship

between gestural cues and communicativeness, as well as between gestural cues and

intentionality. Results suggested that children’s gestures are clearly correlated with the

classification of a certain vocalization as communicative (for instance, pointing or

furrowing of the brows).

Page 33: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

33

5.2. Study 2

5.2.1. Materials

The second study aims at investigating how children compared to adults phonologically

synchronize gesture and speech, and if they do it before what is suggested in previous

studies. Thus, two experiments will be carried out. First, an experiment is to see if adult

Catalan-speakers temporally align gesture and speech in a way that the pitch peak and

the pitch accent of the speech is produced during the stroke of the gesture. Second, an

experiment is carried out to see if children temporally align gesture and speech in an

adult-like manner and when they start doing so.

FIRST EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN ADULTS

For the first experiment, twenty adult Central Catalan-speakers will be recorded while

performing a pointing-naming task. For the recordings, a Panasonic HD AVCCAM will

be used. Following Rochet-Capellan et al. (2008) procedure, participants seat in a chair

approximately 50 cm far from the screen. In the screen, a target (smiley symbol) and a

word will be projected at the same time (see figure 6). Participants perform a pointing-

naming task in which they have to point at the smiley face while reading the word at the

precise moment when the smiley face turned from red into green color. The target

words differ in their number of syllables and stress position: /pá/, /papá/, /pápa/, /tá/,

/tatá/, /táta/, /má/, /mamá/, /máma/, /ná/, /naná/, and /nána/.

Figure 6: Example of an image projected in the screen during the pointing-naming task.

SECOND EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN CHILDREN

Page 34: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

34

For the second experiment, recordings from the Esteve-Prieto corpus will be used. This

corpus is an online Phon database of four monolingual Catalan speaking-children that

were recorded from 0;6 to 2;0 (still in process): An, Bi, Ma, and On (for more

information, see section 5.1.1). We selected for the analysis communicative acts

produced by these children at 0;11, 1;1, 1;3, 1;5, and 1;7. Vocal speech acts, gestural

speech acts, and combinations of the two modalities were all included in the analysis.

These ages were selected in the basis of the word production. At 0;11, children were

only producing routine words such as mama or papa, so it could be stated that it was

before the one-word period. At ages 1;1, 1;3, all children started producing target

words regularly, and at 1;5 they were already at the one-word stage. At 1;7, some

children started producing their two-word combinations, such as mama aigua (‘mummy

water’) or mama ja’tà (‘mummy I am done’). Therefore, by selecting this age range we

will have an overview form the babbling stage until the beginning of the two-word

period. Table 3 summarizes the data for all sessions included in this study.

Participant Age Duration Participant Age Duration

An 0;11.03 0:33:00 Bi 0;11.05 0:34:41

0;11.08 0:36:34 0;11.12 0:36:20

0;11.15 0:36:35 0;11.18 0:34:21

0;11.22 0:33:20 0;11.25 0:26:09

0;13.10 0:37:21 0;13.07 0:34:59

0;13.24 0:41:48 0;13.20 0:34:05

0;15.07 0:29:10 0;15.15 0:35:57

0;15.28 0:34:49 0;15.29 0:35:31

0;17.20 0:25:29 0;17.03 0:37:55

0;17.29 0:34:05 0;17.17 0:37:58

0;19.05 0:35:42 0;19.26 0:37:12

0;19.16 0:34:21

Participant Age Duration Participant Age Duration

Ma 0;11.05 0:34:43 On 0;11.14 0:26:25

0;11.12 0:39:44 0;11.23 0:37:12

0;11.19 0:35:20 0;13.28 0:36:15

0;11.25 0:33:23 0;15.08 0:23:28

0:13.00 0:32:46 0;15.21 0:36:43

0;13.14 0:31.17 0;17.15 0:37:09

0;13.27 0:33:36 0;19.14 1:10:54

0;15.08 0:35:48

0;15.22 0:32:56

0;17.23 0:34:51

0;19.05 0:36:29

Page 35: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

35

Table 3. Recorded sessions included in the experiment, classified by children’s age, and duration of

the session.

5.2.2. Codification

FIRST EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN ADULTS

For the first experiment with adults, all pointing-naming instances produced by the 20

adult Catalan-speakers will be acoustically and gesturally analyzed using ELAN

(Lausberg & Sloetjes, 2009) and Praat software package (Boersma & Weenink, 2005).

The aim is to investigate how Catalan speakers synchronize gesture and speech in

their productions. Previous research has found that the stroke of the gesture precedes

or ends at, but does not follow, the phonological peak syllable in speech (Kendon,

1980; McNeill, 1992; Rochet-Capellan et al., 2008). With the present analysis we will

investigate if the same occurs in Catalan and which are the values of the precise

alignment of stroke and pitch contour. Thus, two analyses will be carried out: an

acoustic analysis and a gestural analysis.

For the gestural analysis, all pointing gestures will be annotated in terms of gestural

phases. Following McNeill (1992) the phases of deictic gestures are:

• Preparation (optional in the gesture)

• Pre-stroke hold (optional in the gesture)

• Stroke (the only compulsory one)

• Post-stroke hold (optional in the gesture)

• Retraction (optional in the gesture)

Figure 7 shows an example of these phases in an adult pointing gesture.

TOTAL AMOUNT OF TIME RECORDED: 24:06:21

Page 36: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

36

Figure 7. Gesture phases and the position of the pitch peak, intensity peak, or pitch accent in respect

to the gesture phases when the gesture-speech combination is adequately aligned.

For the acoustic analysis, three values will be annotated using Praat (see figure 8b):

• The pitch peak point in the fundamental frequency line

• The pitch accent

• The intensity peak point

Afterwards, the position of these three acoustic values will be imported in ELAN. Figure

8a shows an example of annotation of the pointing-naming task in ELAN. In this figure,

the top left panel displays the video images, the top right panel displays the controls,

and the bottom panel includes the annotation tiers where prosodic and gestural cues

are annotated: type of gesture, gesture phases, word uttered, position of the pitch

accent, and position of the pitch peak point.

preparation phase

stroke

retraction phase

(pitch peak,

intensity peak,

or pitch accent)

Page 37: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

37

Figure 8a. Example of annotation of the adults’ data in ELAN. The position of the stroke is signaled with

the top spaced red circle, the position of the pitch peak is signaled with the pointed brown circle in the

middle, and the position of the pitch peak is signaled with the green circle on the bottom.

Figure 8b. Example of vocalization annotated in Praat in terms of

position of the pitch accent, of the pitch peak, and of the intensity peak.

When these points are located, they are imported in Elan’s annotation

tiers.

SECOND EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN CHILDREN

For the second experiment with children, all communicative acts produced by children

at 0;11, 1;1, 1;3, 1;5, and 1;7 were classified as ‘speech-only’, ‘gesture-only’, or

‘gesture-speech’ combinations. Then, ‘gesture-speech’ combinations were analyzed in

terms of prosody and gestures using ELAN and Praat software package. The aim of

the analysis is to investigate the temporal synchronization between gesture and

Page 38: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

38

speech, i.e. whether the gestural stroke was aligned with the pitch peak in

vocalizations.

As for the gestural analysis, two values are annotated using Elan: type of gesture and

gesture phases. First, the type of gesture is codified. Relying on the visual cues, each

communicative act involving a gesture had to be annotated as containing a ritualized

gesture (saying bye-bye, shaking head to negate, extending the finger to signal one

year), a deictic gesture (extending his/her arm, pointing towards an object or an event),

or an iconic gesture (for instance, with the hand by the ear to signal telephone).

Second, the gesture phases of deictic gestures were further codified in order to analyze

its temporal synchronization with speech. Figure 9 shows two examples of the gesture

phases in a pointing gesture. Following McNeill (1992), the gesture phases used were:

• Preparation (optional in the gesture)

• Pre-stroke hold (optional in the gesture)

• Stroke (the only compulsory one)

• Post-stroke hold (optional in the gesture)

• Retraction (optional in the gesture)

Figure 9. Examples of gesture phases in a deictic gesture

preparation phase

stroke (peak of efford)

retraction

Page 39: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

39

For the prosodic analysis, the precise location of the pitch peak, i.e. the highest point in

the fundamental frequency line, was annotated using Praat (see figure 10b). Thus, a

‘gesture-speech’ combination was considered to be adequately aligned when the pitch

peak of the fundamental frequency line occurred during the stroke of the gesture.

Figure 10a is an example of annotation in ELAN. In the top left of the ELAN window,

the video images are displayed; on the top right, controls such as speed, volume, or

navigation thorough the cases annotated can be controlled; on the bottom panel of the

window, all gestural and prosodic cues are annotation in different tiers: type of gesture,

gesture phases, verbal speech acts, position of the pitch peak, and gesture intention.

Besides, the waveform is displayed between the top and the bottom panels, in order to

export audio to Praat and to allow annotation of the prosodic information such as pitch

range.

Figure 10a. Example of annotation of the children’s data in ELAN. In this example, the pitch peak of the

speech (in the bottom green circle) occurs after the stroke of the pointing gesture (in the top red circle).

Figure 10b. Annotation of the pitch peak in Praat. When this point is located, its

position is imported in Elan, as shown in the green circle in figure 10a.

Waveform

Time line

Annotation

tiers

Time line

line

Page 40: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

40

5.2.3. Preliminary results

FIRST EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN ADULTS

Preliminary results after analyzing only five adult Catalan-speakers show that speakers

temporally align the stroke of the pointing gestures with the pitch accent in 98% of the

cases (see figure 11). No significant differences were found between monosyllabic and

disyllabic words, and neither on the position of the pitch accent in disyllabic words.

Figure 11. Percentage of synchronized and non-synchronized gesture-speech combinations.

When analyzing the position of the pitch peak in respect of the stroke of the gesture,

results showed that the pitch peak tended to align with the beginning of the stroke (see

figure 12).

Page 41: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

41

Figure 12. Alignment of the pitch peak in respect of the stroke of the gesture depending on the number of

syllables and the pitch accent position.

SECOND EXPERIMENT: TEMPORAL ALIGNMENT IN CHILDREN

Results showed that at 0;11, from all communicative acts containing gesture, only

43,26% of them where ‘gesture-speech’ combinations. However, already at 1;1 the

‘gesture-speech’ combinations represent 54,8% of the communicative acts containing

gesture, and children produce an even higher proportion of ‘gesture-speech’

combinations in the late babbling and one-word stage (see figure 13).

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

0;11 1;1 1;3 1;5 1;7

gesture only

gesture + speech

Figure 13. Development of ‘gesture only’ and ‘gesture-speech’ combinations from the babbling stage and

until the one-word period.

general

alignment

trochees

iambs

monsyllabic

words

Page 42: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

42

The analysis of the temporal alignment of the ‘gesture-speech’ combinations and,

specifically, the deictic gestures accompanied by speech, showed that at 0;11, children

still do not synchronize gesture and speech, since they produce the pitch peak during

the stroke of the gesture only in 30% of the cases. At this early age, children produce

most of the pitch peaks either before or after the stroke of the gesture. However, at the

late babbling stage and early one-word period, results suggest that children already

produce a higher proportion of synchronized ‘gesture-speech’ combinations than

unsynchronized ones (see figure 14).

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

0;11 1;1 1;3 1;5 1;7

peak duringstrokepeak beforestrokepeak afterstroke

Figure 14. Evolution of the position of the pitch peak compared to the stroke. There is synchronization

when the pitch peak is produced during the stroke.

A closer analysis of the temporal alignment showed that at the beginning of the

babbling stage, the pitch peak tends to be aligned at the end of the stroke of the deictic

gesture. However, at the late babbling stage and the one-word period, the pitch peak

has moved to the left and it is aligned at the beginning of the stroke (see figure 15), just

like adults seemed to do in the first experiment of this study.

Page 43: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

43

Figure 15: Longitudinal evolution of the alignment of the pitch peak and the stroke. At the early babbling

stage, the pitch peak is aligned at the end of the stroke, but at the one-word period it is aligned at the

beginning of the stroke.

5.3. Study 3

5.3.1. Materials

The third study aims at investigating if children are able to understand other’s

communicative intentions relying on gestural and prosodic communicative cues. It will

be a study in collaboration with a research group will be carried out. This research

group, called Communication Before Language Group, is based in The Max Planck

Institute for Psycholinguistics and coordinated by Prof. Ulf Liszkowski. Their research

focuses on infants' gestural communication, prelinguistic infants' social-interactional

experiences across different cultures, infants' understanding of mental states, and

infants' expectations of and motivation for joint collaborative activities.

at 0;11

at 1;1

at 1;3

at 1;5

at 1;7

Page 44: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

44

Interestingly, after a first meeting with our collaborators, a proposal is starting to

emerge: the cross-linguistic comparison would give us stronger evidence of this

children’s capacity. Thus, languages like Catalan, Spanish, or Dutch use different

prosodic contours to encode requests and informative utterances. Whenever these

differences arise in the children’s ability to understand adults’ intentions, it would be

clear that they rely on prosodic cues to understand the different intentions behind a

pointing gesture.

5.3.2. Codification

The hypothesis of the third study will be tested in collaboration with the research group

Communication Before Language Group (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics,

Nijmegen). Details on this project still have to be defined. However, the first contact has

already been established, and a proposal has already been formulated.

By means of an eye-tracking system, a methodology used to record eye movements,

we will study the ability to understand adult’s intentions by babbling children. Since the

children we are planning to test are still not fluent in language production, the best

methodology will be one that does not rely on the verbalization of children but only on

how they cognitively process a certain situation.

In a first version of the proposal, a draft of the methodology to be used has been

agreed. The study aims at investigating if the parents’ use of specific request prosody

(in Catalan we have a specific contour for requests, a L* HL% or even a L* LHL% if it is

more insistent) when producing a pointing gesture increases more clear responses on

the part of children in taking the objects. The prediction would be that when the

experimenter uses request intonation types, the child will respond better to the request

than when using the neutral intonation. And even more when using the insistent

request intonation.

For Catalan, different intonation contours could be used:

- With a neutral intonation (H*L%)

- With a requestive intonation (L* HL%)

- With an insistent requestive intonation (H* LHL%)

Page 45: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

45

Thus, we predict that when the experimenter uses a requestive intonation, the babbling

child will respond better to the request than when using the neutral intonation. And this

will happen even more often when using the insistent requestive intonation.

6. WORKING SCHEDULE

June 2011 ° Submission of the Ph.D. project.

° Oral presentation of results of the second part of study 2

(alignment in children) in the conference Phonetics and

Phonology in Iberia (PaPI), held in Tarragona.

July 2011

to October 2011

° Correction of an article submitted in the Journal of Child

Language with results from study 1 (gesture and prosody

to communicate intentionally), according to the revision

proposed by the journal.

° Presentation of results of the second part of study 2

(alignment in children) in the conference AMLaP

(Architectures and Mechanisms for Language

Processing), to be held in Paris, France.

° Presentation of results of the second part of study 2

(alignment in children) in the conference GESPIN

(Gesture and Speech in Interaction), to be held in

Bielefeld, Germany.

November 2011

to January 2012

° Writing the results of the second part of study 2

(alignment in children) with results and comments

obtained from the conferences.

February 2012

to March 2012

° Gathering the complete sample of first part of study 2

(alignment in adults).

April 2012 to

Mai 2012

° Writing the results of the complete study 2 (alignment in

adults and alignment in children).

Page 46: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

46

June 2012 to

July 2012

° Preparation of materials for study 3 (children’s

understanding of intentions in pointing).

September 2012

to December

2012

° Research stay in the Communication Before Language

Group (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics) to

collaborate in the study 3.

January 2013 to

April 2013

° Analysis of results from the study 3.

° Writing study 3.

Mai 2013 to

July 2013

° Writing the Ph.D. dissertation.

September 2013

° Dissertation defense.

Page 47: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

47

7. SELECTED REFERENCES

Butcher, C.; Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to

two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In: D. McNeill (Ed.).

Language and gesture. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 235–258 .

Butcher & Goldin-Meadow (2000) analyzed 6 English-speaking children from the one-

word stage to the two-word stage in order to see (1) if communicative gestures form an

integrated system with speech in the one-word stage, (2) if there is a time early in

development when communicative gesture is used primarily without speech, and (3) if

young children do produce gesture in combination with speech, are the two modalities

integrated both temporally and semantically, as they are in adults. Their results suggest

that at some point during the one-word period, children begin to combine gesture and

speech in an adult-like way, and at the same moment produce those gestures in

temporal and semantic synchrony with that speech.

Papaeliou, C.F; Trevarthen, C. (2006). Prelinguisti c pitch patterns expressing

‘communication’ and ‘apprehension’. Journal of Child Language 33, 163-178.

Papaeliou & Trevarthen (2006) observed four English-speaking infants from 0;7 to 0;11

and classified their vocalizations as ‘communicative’ or ‘investigative’ according to

concurrent non-vocal behaviors. They found that children displayed different prosodic

patterns when vocalizations were classified as communicative, or when classified as

investigative: compared to investigative vocalizations, communicative vocalizations had

a higher mean and maximum f(0), higher standard deviation of f(0), and shorter

duration. Hence, the authors concluded that pre-linguistic vocalizations can be a tool

for both communicating and thinking.

Prieto, P.; Estrella, A.; Thorson, J.; Vanrell, M.M . (in press). Is prosodic

development correlated with grammatical development ? Evidence from

emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language.

Prieto et al. (in press) investigated the development of prosodic patterns in four

Catalan-learning children and in two Spanish-learning children between 0;11 and 2;4.

Page 48: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

48

Their results show that (1) the Autosegmental Metrical Model was successful in

transcribing early intonation contours, (2) that children’s emerging intonation is

independent of grammatical development and that it develops before the two-word

combinations, and (3) that the emergence of intonational grammar is related to the

onset of speech and the presence of a small lexicon, since they found that children at

1;1 and 1;3 are able to produce a set of adult-like intonation contours.

Rochet-Capellan, A.; Laboissière, R.; Galván, A.; S chwartz, J.L. (2008). The

speech focus position effect on jaw-finger coordina tion in a pointing task .

Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research 51(6), pp. 1507-1521.

Rochet-Capellan et al (2008) analyzed 20 Portuguese Brazilian-speakers to analyze

the effects of stress on jaw-finger coordination. To do it, participants had to point to a

target while naming it using different target words with a trochee or iamb pattern.

Results found a strong synchronization between arm and jaw movements, since the

pitch accent occured simultaneously with the stroke of the gesture, both in the case of

trochees and in the case of iambs. According to the authors, these results evidence

that the speech deictic site (namely, the part of speech that shows) is anchored in the

pointing gesture.

Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Liskowsky, U. (2007). A New Look at Infant

Pointing. Child Development 78, pp. 705-722.

Tomasello et al. (2007) propose a theory in which infant pointing is the evidence of the

infants’ early intentionality and shared intentionality. They propose three different

motives behind the infant pointing: (1) the infant want the adult to do something, (2) the

infant want the adult to know something, and (3) the infant want the adult to feel

something. The authors review many articles finding results that support the hypothesis

that infant pointing gestures reflect their motivation for cooperation and shared

intentionality, concluding that at 1;0-1;2, children build their linguistic skills on the basis

of this prelinguistic communication.

Page 49: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

49

8. GENERAL REFERENCES

Acredolo, L.P.; Goodwyn, S.W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development: A case study. Human Development, 28, pp. 40-49.

Acredolo, L. P.; Goodwyn, S.W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, pp. 450-466.

Allwood, J.; Cerrato, L.; Jokinen, K.; Navarretta, C.; Paggio, P. (2007). The MUMIN Coding Scheme for the Annotation of Feedback, Turn Management and Sequencing. In: J. C. Martin et al. (eds). Multimodal Corpora for Modeling Human Multimodal Behaviour. Special issue of the International Journal of Language Resources and Evaluation, 41(3-4), pp. 273-287.

Aureli, T.; Perucchini, P.; Genco, M. (2009). Children’s understanding of communicative intentions in the middle of the second year of life. Cognitive Development, 24, pp. 1-12.

Austin, J. L. (1962). How to Do Things With Words. Oxford University Press: Oxford, England.

Bates, E.; Camaioni, L.; Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, pp. 205-224.

Bavelas, J.B.; Gerwing, J.; Sutton, C.; Prevost, D. (2008). Gesturing on the telephone: Independent effects of dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, pp. 495-520.

Bavelas, J.B.; Kenwood, C.; Johnson, T.; Phillips, B. (2002). An experimental study of when and how speakers use gestures to communicate. Gesture, 2, 1-17.

Behne, T.; Carpenter, M.; Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8, pp. 492-499.

Bellagamba, F.; Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, pp. 277-282.

Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. (2005). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.3.01). University of Amsterdam 2005. [http://www.praat.org/].

Boysson-Bardies, B. de; Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and early words in four languages. Language, 61, pp. 297-319.

Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Brinck, I. (2004). The pragmatics of imperative and declarative pointing. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 3, pp. 429-446.

Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk. New York: Norton.

Page 50: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

50

Butcher, C.; Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In: D. McNeill (ed.). Language and gesture. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 235-258.

Butterworth B.L.; Beattie G.W. (1978). Gestures and silence as indicator of planning in speech. In Campbell R. N., Smith P. T. (Eds.), Recent Advances in the Psychology of Language: Formal and Experimental Approaches. New York: Olenum Press, pp. 347-360.

Butterworth, B.; Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: a reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96, pp. 168-74.

Call, J.; Tomasello, M. (2005). What chimpanzees know about seeing revisited: An explanation of the third kind. In: N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (eds.), Joint attention. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 45-64.

Camaioni, L.; Perucchini, P.; Bellagamba, F.; Colonnesi, C. (2009). The Role of Declarative Pointing in Developing a Theory of Mind. Infancy, 5(3), pp. 291-308.

Capirci, O.; Contaldo, A.; Caselli, M.C.; Volterra, V. (2005). From Action to Language Through Gesture: A Longitudinal Perspective. Gesture, 5, pp. 155-177.

Capirci, O.; Iverson, J.M.; Pizzuto, E.; Volterra, V. (1996). Communicative gestures during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, pp. 645-673.

Capone, N.; McGregor, K. (2004). Gesture development: a review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, pp. 173-186.

Carpenter, M.; Nagell, K.; Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 255(63), pp. 1-143.

Clark, H.H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge University Press.

Cochet, H.; Vauclair, J. (2010). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: Different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, pp. 431-441.

DePaolis, R.A.; Vihman, M.M.; Kunnari, S. (2008). Prosody in production at the onset of word use: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Phonetics, 36, pp. 406-426.

D’Odorico, L.; Franco, F. (1991). Selective production of vocalization types in different communication contexts. Journal of Child Language, 18, pp. 475-499.

Duncan, S. (1996). Grammatical form and 'thinking-for-speaking' in Mandarin Chinese and English: An analysis based on speech-accompanying gestures. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Chicago.

Esteve-Gibert, N.; Prieto, P. (in progress). Esteve-Prieto Corpus. Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona. [http://prosodia.upf.edu/phon/en/corpora/description/esteveprieto.html]

Esteve-Gibert, N.; Prieto, P. (2011). The temporal alignment between prosody and gesture in Catalan-babbling infants. 4th Biennial Conference of Experimental Pragmatics. June 2-4. Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona.

Page 51: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

51

Esteve-Gibert, N.; Prieto, P. (submitted). Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language.

Ferré, G. (2010). Timing Relationships between Speech and Co-Verbal Gestures in Spontaneous French. Language Resources and Evaluation, Workshop on Multimodal Corpora, Malta.

Frota, S.; Vigário, M. (2008). The intonation of one-word and first two-word utterances in European Portuguese. Paper presented at the Third Conference on Tone and Intonation (TIE 3), Lisbon, 15-17 September 2008.

Furuyama, N. (2000). Gestural interaction between the instructor and the learner in origami instruction: In: McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Giorgolo, G.; Verstraten, F.A.J. (2008) Perception of 'Speech-and-Gesture' Integration, submitted to the International Conference on Auditory-Visual Speech Perception 2008.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). The two faces of gesture: Language and thought. Gesture, 5, pp. 241-257.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language and creating language. Child Development, 78, pp. 741-745.

Goldin-Meadow, S.; Butcher, C. (2003). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In: S. Kita (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 85-107.

Greenfield, P.; Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.

Grice, H.P. (1957). ‘eaning. The Philosophical Review, 66, pp. 377-388.

Grice, H.P. (1975). ‘ogic and conversation. In: Cole, P.; Morgan, J. (eds.) Syntax and Semantics, Volume 3. New York: Academic Press. pp. 41-58.

Gullberg, M. (2006). Handling discourse: Gestures, reference tracking, and communication strategies in early L2. Language Learning, 56(1), pp. 155-196.

Hadar, U.; Butterworth, B. (1997). Iconic gestures, imagery, and word retrieval in speech. Semiotica , 115, pp. 147-172.

Hallé, P.A.; Boysson-Bardies, B. de; Vihman, M.M. (1991). Beginnings of prosodic organization: Intonation and duration patterns of disyllables produced by Japanese and French infants. Language and Speech, 34, pp. 299-318.

Halliday, M.A.K. (1975). Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. New York: Elsevier.

Holler, J.; Wilkin, K. (2009). Communicating common ground: how mutually shared knowledge influences the representation of semantic information in speech and gesture in a narrative task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, pp. 267-289.

Page 52: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

52

Iverson, J.M; Capirci, O.; Caselli, C.M. (1994). From Communication to Language in Two Modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, pp. 23-43.

Iverson, J.M.; Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, pp. 367-371.

Jusczyk, P.W.; Cutler, A.; Redanz, N.J. (1993). Infants’ preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development, 64, pp. 675-687.

Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In: M.R. Key (Ed.). The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication. The Hague: Mouton, pp. 207-227.

Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Kita, S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In: D. McNeill (ed.), Language and gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 162-185.

Krauss, R.M.; Chen, Y.; Gottesman, R.F. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In: D. McNeill (ed.), Language and gesture. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 261-283.

Lausberg, H.; Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 41(3), pp. 841-849.

Levitt, A.; Utman, J. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: a longitudinal two-case study. Journal of Child Language, 19(1), pp. 19-49.

Liszkowski, U. (2005). Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and helpfully provide information for a communicative partner. Gesture, 5(1-2), pp. 135-154.

Liszkowski, U. (2006). Infant pointing at twelve months: Communicative goals, motives, and social-cognitive abilities. In: N.J. Enfield; S.C. Levinson (eds.), Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and interaction. New York: Berg, pp. 153-178.

Liszkowski, U. (2008). Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture, 8(2), pp. 180-196.

Loehr, D. (2004). Gesture and intonation. Doctoral Dissertation. Georgetown University, Washington, DC.

Loehr, D. (2007). Aspects of rythm in gesture and speech. Gesture 7(2), pp. 179-214.

López-Ornat, S.; Gallego, C.; Gallo, P.; Karousou, A.; Mariscal, S.; Nieva, S. (2003). iLC: Un instrumento de medida del desarrollo lingüístico temprano basado en las Escalas MacArthur. Boletín de la AELFA (Asociación Española de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología) 3, pp. 3-17.

López-Ornat, S.; Karousou, A. (2005). Las Vocalizaciones tempranas (8-30 meses) y su relación con el vocabulario y la gramática. Su medida en el CDI-Español: Resultados preliminares. In: Mayor Cinca, M.A; Zubiauz de Pedro, B.; Díez-Villora, E.

Page 53: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

53

(Eds). Estudios sobre la adquisición del lenguaje. Universidad de Salamanca, pp. 401-420.

Mampe, B.; Friederici, A.D.; Christophe, A.; Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns' Cry Melody Is Shaped by Their Native Language. Current Biology, 19(23), pp. 1994-1997.

Marcos, H. (1987). Communicative functions of pitch range and pitch direction in infants. Journal of Child Language, 14, pp. 255-268.

McClave, E. (1991). Intonation and gesture. Doctoral Dissertation. Georgetown University, Washington, DC.

McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gesture are nonverbal?. Psychological Review, 92, pp. 350-371.

McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Meltzoff, A.N.; Gopnik, A.; Repacholi, B.M. (1999). Toddlers’ understanding of intentions, desires and emotions: Explorations of the dark ages. In: P.D. Zelazo; J.W. Astington; D.R. Olson (eds.), Developing theories of intentions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 17-46.

Nazzi, T.; Floccia, C.; Bertoncini, J. (1998). Discrimination of pitch contours by neonates. Infant, Behavior and Development, 21, pp. 779-784.

Ozçalişkan, S.; Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005) Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96, pp. 101-113.

Özyürek, A. (2002). Do speakers design their co-speech gestures for their addresees? The effects of addressee location on representational gestures. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), pp. 688-704.

Papaeliou, C.; Minadakis, G.; Cavouras, D. (2002). Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45(2), pp. 311-317.

Papaeliou, C.F; Trevarthen, C. (2006). Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing ‘communication’ and ‘apprehension’. Journal of Child Language, 33, pp. 163-178.

Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel, Delachaux et Niestlé.

Piaget, J. (1946). La formation du symbole chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel, Delachaux et Niestlé.

Pierrehumbert, J. (1980). The phonology and phonetics of English intonation. Doctoral Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Boston MA.

Pizzuto, E.; Capobianco, M. (2005). The link and differences between deixis and symbols in children’s early gestural-vocal system. Gesture, 5, pp. 179-199.

Page 54: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

54

Pons, F.; Bosch, L. (2007). The perception of lexical stress patterns by Spanish and Catalan infants. In: Prieto, P.; Mascaró, J.; Solé, J. (eds.), Segmental and prosodic issues in Romance phonology, CILT 282. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 199-218.

Povinelli, D.J.; Reaux, J.E.; Bierschwale, D.T.; Allain, A.D.; Simon, B.B. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 12, pp. 327-365.

Prieto, P. (2003). Las teorías lingüísticas de la entonación. In: Prieto, P. (ed.). Teorías de la entonación. Ariel: Barcelona, pp. 13-33.

Prieto, P.; Estrella, A.; Thorson, J.; Vanrell, M.M. (in press). Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language.

Rochet-Capellan, A.; Laboissière, R.; Galván, A.; Schwartz, J.L. (2008). The speech focus position effect on jaw-finger coordination in a pointing task. Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research 51(6), pp. 1507-1521.

Rose, Y.; MacWhinney, B.; Byrne, R.; Hedlund, G.; Maddocks, K.; O’Brien, P.; Warehem, T. (2005). Introducing Phon: A Software Solution for the Study of Phonological Acquisition. In: Bamman, D.; Magnitskaia, T.; Zaller, C. (eds.). 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 489-500.

Sansavini, B.; Guarini, S.; Stefanini, C. (2010). Early development of gestures, object-related actions, word comprehension and word production, and their relationships in Italian infants. Gesture 10(1), pp. 52-85.

Sato, Y.; Sogabe, Y.; Mazuka, R.; (2006). Discrimination of lexical pitch-accent by Japanese infants. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the XVth Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Westin Miyako, Kyoto, Japan, June 19, 2006.

Searle, J.R. (1969). Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, England.

Scherer, K.R. (1986). Studying emotion empirically: Issues and a paradigm for research. In: K.R. Scherer; H.G. Wallbott; A.B. Summerfield (eds.), Experiencing emotion: A cross-cultural study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3-27.

Snow, D. (2006). Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development, 77, pp. 281-296.

Snow, D.; Balog, H. L. (2002). Do children produce the melody before the words? A review of developmental intonation research. Lingua, 112, pp. 1025-1058.

Sperber, D.; Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sperber, D.; Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance. 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.

Tomasello, M.; Call, J.; Gluckman, A. (1997). Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Development, 68, pp. 1067-1080.

Page 55: ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALANprosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/projecte_tesi_esteve.pdf · ACQUISITION OF PROSODY AT THE BABBLING STAGE IN CATALAN

55

Tomasello, M.; Camaioni, L. (1997). A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Human Development, 40, pp. 2-24

Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Behne, T.; Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, pp. 675-735.

Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Liszkowski, U. (2007). A New Look at Infant Pointing. Child Development, 78, pp. 705-722.

Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behaviour. In: H.R. Schaffer (ed.). Studies in mother-infant interaction. Academic Press, London, pp. 227-269.

Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: M. Bullowa (ed.). Before speech. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 321-347.

Trevarthen, C. (1982). The primary motives for cooperative understanding. In: G. Butterworth; P. Light (eds.). Social cognition: Studies of the development of understanding. Brighton: Harvester Press, pp. 77-109.

Vihman, M.M.; DePaolis, R.A. (1998). Perception and production in early vocal development: Evidence from the acquisition of accent. In: M.C. Gruber; D. Higgins; K.S. Olson; T. Wysocki (eds.), Chicago Linguistic Society 34, Part 2: Papers from the panels. Chicago, IL: CLS, pp. 373-386.

Whalen, D. H.; Levitt, A. G.; Wang, Q. (1991). Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 18, pp. 501-516.

Weber, C.; Hahne, A.; Friedrich, M.; Friederici, A. D. (2004). Discrimination of word stress in early infant perception: electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 149-161.

Werner, H.; Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: John Wiley.