aggressive behavior

Upload: pascal-fernandez-pena

Post on 05-Apr-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    1/41

    1

    Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: Whats wrong?

    This is a preprint of an article accepted for publication in Aggressive Behaviorcopyright 2005 Wiley-Liss Inc.

    Gianluca Gini

    Department of Developmental and Socialisation Psychology

    University of Padova

    via Venezia 8

    35131, Padova - Italy

    e-mail: [email protected]

    Acknowledgements: The author is grateful to all schools, teachers and pupils who

    participated in the study. Moreover, I wish to thank Francesca Happ and Jon Sutton for

    their stories, translated and utilized in this study, and Elena Arimondo, Alessia

    Bortoloso, Giuseppe Carli and Simona Pischetola for their help in data collection.

    Finally, I am very grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on

    an earlier version of the manuscript.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    2/41

    2

    Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: Whats wrong?

    Abstract

    Two different models have been proposed that describe the bully alternatively as a

    child lacking in social skills (Crick & Dodge, 1994), or as a cold manipulative

    individual, who leads gangs to achieve personal goals (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham,

    1999a). The present study examined the performance of 204 8-11-year-olds in a set of

    stories that assessed understanding of cognitions and emotions, in relation to their

    Participant Role in bullying. Moreover, childrens understanding of moral emotions and

    proneness to moral disengagement was assessed. Victims showed some difficulties in

    the social cognition task, whereas bullies did not. Aggressive children, instead, were

    found to be more ready to show moral disengagement mechanisms, whereas defenders

    showed higher levels of moral sensibility. Results are discussed in relation to the two

    models, and the need for further research into empathy and moral cognition of children

    involved in bullying episodes is highlighted.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    3/41

    3

    Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: Whats wrong?

    Over the last twenty years, the literature about the development of aggressive

    behavior showed an increasing interest in the study of the relations between cognition,

    social information processing and social interaction in children and adolescents (e.g.,

    Crick & Dodge, 1994; Harvey, Fletcher, & French, 2001). In particular, several authors

    studied how the social information processing strategies used by children and the way in

    which they interpret situational cues and use their previous experiences can influence

    their aggressive conduct (Dodge & Price, 1994; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997; Randall,

    1997). One of the most important results of this field of research has been the possibility

    to identify the presence of specific deficits and systematic biases in some components of

    the social cognition of aggressive children. These results led Dodge and colleagues to

    formulate the Social Skills Deficit model (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980).

    According to the first formulation of this model, in which reactive and proactive

    categories of aggression were not clearly distinguished, aggressive children show some

    difficulties in one or more phases of their social information processing. For example,

    these authors found that aggressive children tend to interpret ambiguous situations in an

    aggressive way more than their non aggressive peers (hostile attributional bias) (Dodge

    & Frame, 1982; Dodge & Somberg, 1987). Another difference between aggressive and

    non aggressive children refers to the selection of the behavioral response in a social

    situation: the former, in fact, seem to have a more limited range of non aggressive

    answers than the latter and, for this reason, they are more inclined to choose and perform

    aggressive behaviors, especially in the case of interpersonal conflicts (Dodge, 1980;

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    4/41

    4

    Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Dodge & Newman, 1981). According to the same

    approach, victims, who are shy and withdrawn and lack social skills of assertiveness,

    show high levels of anxiety and loneliness, and have less experience in social interaction

    and play fighting (which may enhance social skills), as well as control and interpretation

    of emotional expression (Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997; Smith, Bowers, Binney, &

    Cowie, 1993).

    Some authors tried to apply this model to a particular kind of aggressive

    phenomenon: school bullying (Boulton & Smith, 1994; Slee, 1993; Smith et al., 1993).

    However, despite the popular view of the bully as an oaf boy, that is an intellectually

    simple or backward boy (Besag, 1989), very few studies confirmed that Dodges model

    can be applied to bullies in exactly the same way as to aggressive children (e.g., Hazler,

    1996; Randall, 1997). On the contrary, some other authors stressed the role of adaptive

    motivation to explain bullying behavior, arguing that bullying may be seen as an

    inappropriate way to reach a socially effective goal, such as leadership within a group.

    As a consequence, these authors stated that the bully should not necessarily to be seen as

    an individual with specific difficulties. On the contrary, he or she may sometimes be a

    skilled individual who takes advantage of his/her high social-cognitive competence to

    reach personal benefits, such as interpersonal dominance, through a Machiavellian

    conduct (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999a).

    Some data presented by Sutton and colleagues (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham,

    1999b) empirically support this view; in fact, they found that 7-10-year-old bullies

    performed better than other classmates in a social cognition task that required the

    understanding of mental states and emotions of the main character of a story (theory of

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    5/41

    5

    mind task). The relevance of these results, above all, lies in the fact that, in a bullying

    situation, having a grasp of the mental states of those involved, along with an ability to

    manipulate these thoughts and beliefs, may be crucial for the bully in developing and

    maintaining such inter-role relations (Sutton et al., 1999b, p. 437). In previous studies,

    therefore, bullies social cognitive skills, such as social perspective taking and decoding

    of emotional information, may have been underestimated (Sutton et al., 1999a, 1999b,

    1999c). However, some bullies may possess a different conception of the costs and

    benefits of aggression, and they may lack in empathic understanding of the victims

    suffering. Therefore, it appears more adequate to regard social skills as a neutral tool,

    which may be used for both prosocial and antisocial purposes (Bjrkqvist, sterman, &

    Kaukiainen, 2000; Hawley, 2003). From this perspective, the crucial point to understand

    bullying behavior refers to the kind of values that guide bullies conduct, rather than

    their higher or lower ability in social information processing (Arsenio & Lemerise,

    2001).

    Following this line of research, the present paper focuses on cognitive and

    emotional understanding of social situations in bullying, using a theory of mind task

    with children who assume different participant roles during a bullying episode. In the

    above mentioned study, however, Sutton and colleagues (1999b) did not explicitly

    consider a particular kind of emotion found to be important to account for aggressive

    and antisocial behaviors: moral emotions (in particular, guilt and shame). If bullies are

    cold, manipulative experts in social contexts, their behavior may be related to emotions

    surrounding moral transgression. These emotions are important because, as Hoffman

    (1976, 2000) pointed out, they are closely connected with empathy and they can be

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    6/41

    6

    considered mediators of conscience. Other authors suggested that moral development

    plays a major role in behavioral regulation (Arsenio, 1988; Nucci, 2001; Smetana, 1995;

    Turiel, 1998) and that guilt is an interpersonal phenomenon aimed at avoiding the

    disruption of relationships and bonds (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994;

    Tagney, 1992). Moreover, individuals who are more prone to guilt are less aggressive

    and less likely to act out behaviors (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988; Quiles & Bybee,

    1997). In contrast, if children had lower feelings of guilt, this could enable them to act

    aggressively without feeling guilty. However, the reverse causal link could be true, in

    that children who repeatedly commit acts of aggression may become inured to this type

    of behavior, exhibiting dulled emotional responses.

    Moral disengagement and aggressive behavior

    A useful contribution to the analysis of some social-cognitive components of

    aggressive behavior is made by the Social Cognitive Theory of the Moral Self (Bandura,

    1986, 1990, 1991). According to this theory, moral reasoning is linked to moral action

    through affective self-regulatory mechanisms by which moral agency is exercised. In

    their development, children need to develop standards of right and wrong and to adopt

    these standards as guides and deterrents for their conduct. This self-regulatory process

    implies that individuals usually act in ways that give them satisfaction and a sense of

    self-worth, whereas, on the other side, they tend to avoid behaviors that violate their

    moral standards in order to avoid self-condemnation.

    Moreover, Bandura (2002) claimed that the construct of moral disengagement

    could be considered as a mediator between the individuals moral principles and his/her

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    7/41

    7

    real conduct, which may not necessarily be coherent with those principles. Internal

    controls, in fact, are not fixed, and several psychological and social processes can

    contribute to their selective deactivation. In particular, Bandura described eight different

    mechanisms through which moral control can be disengaged. One set of disengagement

    practices operates on the reconstruction of the behavior itself. For example, an immoral

    conduct is made personally and socially acceptable by portraying it in the service of

    moral purposes (moral justification) or self-deplored acts can be made to appear

    righteous by contrasting them with worse conducts (advantageous comparison).

    Moreover, in some circumstances, activities can take on different appearances

    depending on what they are called (euphemistic labeling). The second set of

    disengagement practices operates by obscuring or minimizing the agentive role in the

    harm one causes (displacement of responsibility and diffusion of responsibility). Other

    ways of weakening moral control operate by minimizing, disregarding or distorting the

    effects of ones action; if minimization does not work, the evidence of harm can be

    discredited (minimizing or misconstruing consequences). Finally, disengagement

    practices may operate on the recipients of detrimental acts by stripping them of human

    qualities (dehumanization) or considering aggression as provoked by the victim

    (attribution of blame). These mechanisms can lead to aggressive behaviors through a

    process of moral disengagement, that is a partial gap between the abstract personal

    idea of moral behavior and the individuals behavior in real life. In this way, the

    individual protects him/herself from negative feelings, such as guilt or shame, that

    usually follow an immoral conduct (Bandura, 1991).

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    8/41

    8

    Different studies showed a positive relation between aggressive behavior and the

    activation of one or more of these mechanisms in both adults and children (Bandura,

    Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996; Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Bandura, 1995;

    Caprara, Barbaranelli, Vicino, & Bandura, 1996; Yadava, Sharma, & Gandhi, 2001). In

    particular, Caprara and colleagues (1995) confirmed the strong link between moral

    disengagement and physical and verbal aggression, both self- and peer-evaluated,

    especially in male children. More recently, Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli,

    and Regalia (2001) have replicated these results. They found a positive relation between

    moral disengagement and transgressive behavior, and a negative relation between moral

    disengagement and self-regulatory efficacy and prosocial behavior.

    The above mentioned studies actually referred to aggressive individuals, but a

    similar pattern of data has been found in school bullying research, as well. Bacchini,

    Amodeo, Ciardi, Valerio, and Vitelli (1998) and Menesini, Fonzi, and Vannucci (1997),

    for example, found that male bullies utilized the moral disengagement mechanisms more

    than did other peers and, most of all, they seemed to use the mechanisms of

    Dehumanization and Moral Justification. A confirmation of these results emerged in a

    cross-national study, involving Italy and Spain, conduced by Menesini and colleagues

    (Menesini, Codecasa, Benelli, & Cowie, 2003), who assessed moral reasoning of bullies,

    victims and children who assumed the role of the defender, using the Scan Bullying test

    (Almeida, del Barrio, Marques, Gutierrez, & van der Meulen, 2001). These authors

    confirmed the tendency of bullies to show higher levels of moral disengagement, and the

    presence of a profile of egocentric reasoning in these pupils.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    9/41

    9

    Quite recently, the traditional classification of schoolchildren into bullies,

    victims and controls has been criticized and several authors stressed the importance of

    considering bullying a group process (Sutton & Smith, 1999). The empirical

    confirmation of the importance of peer ecology applied to bullying phenomenon derives

    from several observational studies carried out during periods of free interaction among

    children in unstructured contexts, such as during recess and outdoor play. These studies,

    in fact, consistently found that peers are involved in 85% of bullying episodes (Atlas &

    Pepler, 1998; Craig & Pepler, 1997; Craig, Pepler, & Atlas, 2000; Pepler & Craig,

    1995). Moreover, Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Bjrkqvist, sterman, and Kaukiainen (1996)

    identified six different participant roles taken by individual children in the bullying

    process (i.e. victim, bully, reinforcer of the bully, assistant to the bully, defender of the

    victim, and outsider). Another study by Salmivalli, Huttunen, and Lagerspetz (1997) has

    also revealed a strong influence of how the members of a group behave in bullying

    situations on an individual childs behavior in such situations.

    In line with this approach that considers bullying as a group phenomenon, all

    distinct roles within the class were considered in the present study. Not only bullies and

    victims participated, but also those pupils who, in different ways, act important roles in

    reinforcing bullying behavior (the so-called Followers: the Assistants of the bully and

    the Reinforcers) or, in contrast, intervene to defend the victim (the Defenders). Finally,

    Outsiders, that is children not directly involved in the phenomenon, were considered.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    10/41

    10

    Aims of the study and hypotheses

    The first aim of the present study was to assess the performance of these pupils in

    a social-cognition task similar to the one used by Sutton et al. (1999b). In this task, the

    same two typologies of stories used by Sutton and colleagues were used: cognitive and

    emotion stories. The cognitive stories, in particular, assessed the childs ability to

    understand mental states, beliefs and intentions of the character of the story. The

    emotion stories, instead, assessed the ability to understand emotional states of the

    character and the possibility that an emotion could be dissimulated to influence and

    modify others knowledge. Contrary to the idea of the bully as a social oaf, it is

    hypothesized that the Bully group will perform better than, or at least at the same level

    as, other pupils in a theory of mind task that requires understanding the thoughts, beliefs,

    intentions that guide others behavior.

    Moreover, in addition to these two kinds of stories, a third category (moral

    stories), not used by Sutton et al. (1999b), was designed in order to explore the role of

    moral emotions in the context of aggressive conduct. The second aim of the present

    study, therefore, was to analyze the ability to understand moral emotions (guilt and

    shame) within hypothetical social scenarios by children who assume different roles.

    Considering the literature about moral cognition in aggressive behavior (e.g., Hoffman,

    2000; Quiles & Bybee, 1997; Tagney, 1992), it was expected that Bullies would perform

    worse than non-aggressive mates in moral stories, that is be less likely to make the

    correct attribution of moral emotions to the character of the story owing to his/her

    negative behavior. In contrast, Defenders should be particularly sensitive to this kind of

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    11/41

    11

    emotion as an expression of what is right and what is wrong and, therefore, we expected

    this group of children to perform better than any other in this type of stories.

    Finally, the third aim of the study was to consider the relation between moral

    disengagement and bullying behavior (e.g., Caprara et al., 1995; Menesini et al., 1999).

    The comparison among pupils who assume different roles may lead to a deeper

    understanding of the importance of these mechanisms in bullying episodes. Specifically,

    we expected that the tendency to activate moral disengagement mechanisms would be

    higher in aggressive children (the Bullies and their Followers). In contrast, we

    hypothesized that Victims and those children who frequently intervene to defend their

    victimized classmates (Defenders) would show a higher moral sensibility and,

    consequently, a lower level of moral disengagement.

    Method

    Participants

    The initial sample comprised 581 Italian primary-school children (295 boys and

    286 girls) aged between 8 and 11 years (mean age = 9;7 years, s.d. = 11 months).

    Although socio-economic status was not directly measured, our sample included

    students from a wide range of social backgrounds (low and working classes through

    upper middle class). In terms of racial/ethnic background, the sample was predominantly

    Caucasian (96%), with a small proportion of Asian (2%) and North African (2%) origin.

    All children received school and parental permission to participate prior to the collection

    of the data.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    12/41

    12

    On the basis of peer nominations, children were first assigned to one of the

    following Role groups: Bully, Assistant, Reinforcer, Defender, Outsider and Victim,

    based on the procedure described below. Seventy-three children did not receive a

    specific role because they did not satisfy the classification criteria proposed by

    Salmivalli et al. (1996) and were not included in the final sample. The distribution of

    Participant Roles for the whole sample is reported in Table I.

    As can be seen in Table I, cell sizes are rather unequal for both Participant Roles

    and gender. In order to handle problems connected with strongly unbalanced factorial

    designs (Cramer, 1998), especially with small cell numbers, we decided to balance our

    Role groups for number and gender. Therefore, a total of 204 children, divided into 6

    Role group (n=34; 17 males and 17 females for each one), were randomly selected and

    participated to the second phase of the study. The analyses presented in the result section

    have been conducted on this sample with equalized groups.

    --- Insert Table I about here ---

    Material

    Participant Role Questionnaire (PRQ)

    The Italian version (Menesini & Gini, 2000) of the Participant Role Questionnaire

    PRQ (Salmivalli et al., 1996; Sutton & Smith, 1999) was used to collect peer

    nominations. Children were asked to nominate up to five boys and/or girls in their class

    who fitted each of the 21 behavioral descriptions of bullying situations that compose the

    questionnaire. In the present study, self-nominations were not considered. The items are

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    13/41

    13

    divided into six scales, which refer to six different Participant Roles: Bully (4 items,

    e.g.: Starts bullying), Assistant (2 items, e.g.: Helps the bully, maybe by catching or

    holding the victim), Reinforcer (5 items, e.g.: Laughs at people getting bullied),

    Defender (5 items, e.g.: Stick up for the victim), Outsider (4 items, e.g.: Isnt usually

    there, stays away) and Victim (1 item, Gets bullied). Menesini and Gini (2000)

    reported a good internal reliability of these scales for the Italian population ( = .78 to

    = .94). In Table II, correlations between different role scores are reported.

    Roles1 were assigned according to the original procedure, proposed by Salmivalli

    et al. (1996):

    - for each child a Role Score is calculated in each of the six scales of the

    questionnaire. A nomination for sometimes showing a behavior is scored as

    1, and often is scored as 2. Then, the nominations were summed, divided

    by the number of nominators and multiplied by 100;

    - a Participant Role is assigned on the basis of the childs highest standardized

    role score. No role is assigned if the childs score is below the mean (Z

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    14/41

    14

    Social Cognition Task: The Stories

    A set of 15 short stories was designed to test childrens understanding of mental

    states and emotions. Some of these stories were translated from Happ (1994) and

    Sutton et al. (1999b), and additional stories were created reflecting the same structure of

    the pre-existent stories. Even though some stories (e.g., the army/war story) are less

    relevant for children than others, they all seemed to be understandable and valid for

    primary school pupils.

    The 15 stories were divided into three categories, five stories for each category:

    a) Cognitive stories: these stories require the understanding of mental states,

    thoughts, beliefs, intentions of characters, which determine their behavior.

    Example of cognitive story (Happ, 1994):

    During the war, the Red army captures a member of the Blue army. They

    want him to tell them where his army's tanks are; they know they are either by

    the sea or in the mountains. They know that the prisoner will not want to tell

    them, he will want to save his army, and so he will certainly lie to them. The

    prisoner is very brave and very clever, he will not let them find his tanks. The

    tanks are really in the mountains. Now when the other side ask him where his

    tanks are, he says, "They are in the mountains";

    b) Emotion stories: in these stories the understanding of what kind of emotion is

    appropriate within a specific situation is crucial. Moreover, the child needs to

    understand that emotions can be clearly shown or, on the contrary, dissimulated,

    to influence and modify others knowledge. In order to facilitate participants

    answers, children were presented with drawings of faces representing different

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    15/41

    15

    emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, guilt, and a neutral face). An example of an

    emotion story taken from Sutton et al. (1999b) is:

    Mike wants to go out with his friends, but he has a really bad tummy ache. He

    knows that if his Mum notices he is ill, she wont let him go out to play. Mike

    goes downstairs and asks his Mum, Can I go out to play please?;

    c) Moral stories: in these stories the appropriate emotion is a moral emotion

    because some wrongdoing has happened. In these stories children were also

    helped with the drawings. An example of moral story (new) is as follows:

    Claudia has just moved to her new school. She has brown hair, green eyes and

    she is a little bit shorter than her classmates. During the maths class, without

    being noticed by her teacher, Susan, who sits behind Claudia, sends her a

    written note like Pigmy, dwarf. Claudia reads this note and starts crying in

    front of all her mates.

    Each story was followed by a control question based on the content of the story to

    verify that the child understood it; then, an experimental question assessing the

    understanding of mental states or emotions was asked. For the cognitive stories, an

    answer was considered correct if the child referred to the mental state, thoughts, or

    beliefs of the characters (e.g. critical question: Where will the Red army look for the

    Blue army and why?; answer: They will look by the sea because they think that the

    prisoner is lying to them). For the emotion stories, a correct answer included both the

    identification of the appropriate emotion (positive or negative) and the reference to the

    intention/wish of the main character to hide his/her own real emotion to the other

    characters, for example when a character is sad but he or she appears happy (e.g.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    16/41

    16

    Mike has a bad tummy ache, but he appears happy, otherwise his Mum notices that he

    is feeling bad and won't let him go out). Finally, for the moral stories, an answer was

    considered correct if the child recognized the fact that, in the situation described, the

    character of the story should have felt a moral emotion (guilt or shame) for his/her own

    behavior (e.g. She (Susan) feels guilty because she shouldnt have said that bad things

    to her mate Claudia).

    Moral Disengagement Scale

    This scale measures the individuals tendency to use cognitive mechanisms that

    can disengage self-sanctions and justify the use of violent and aggressive behaviors. This

    version of the scale (Caprara et al., 1995), designed for primary school children, consists

    of 14 items describing individuals readiness to construe injurious conduct as serving

    righteous purposes, masquerading censurable activities by palliative language or

    rendering them benign by advantageous comparison, minimizing the harmful effects of

    ones detrimental conduct, and devaluing those who are maltreated (example items:

    Some kids deserve to be treated like animals; Kids cannot be blamed for misbehaving

    if their friends pressured them to do it). Children were asked to rate the strength of their

    endorsement or rejection of moral exoneration of detrimental conduct on a 5-point

    Likert-type scale (from 1= strongly disagree to 5= strongly agree). Therefore, higher

    scores in this scale indicate a higher tendency to engage in one or more of these

    mechanisms. Even though the items of the scale describe different mechanisms, previous

    studies using this scale with Italian samples (Caprara et al., 1995; Caprara et al., 1996;

    Menesini et al., 1997) have demonstrated a unidimensional structure of the scale,

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    17/41

    17

    especially for younger children. This was also confirmed in our sample. Therefore,

    following the suggestions of the authors of the scale, analyses was conducted on the total

    score and not on separate subscales in the present study. The reliability of the scale was

    = .69.

    Procedure

    Stage 1. The PRQ and the Moral Disengagement Scale were completed during a

    single classroom session and the order of presentation of the two measures was

    counterbalanced. This session began by introducing the general purpose of the study.

    Then, the following definition of bullying (Whitney & Smith, 1993) was presented and

    discussed with the class:

    We say a child or young person is being bullied, or picked on when

    another child or young person, or a group of children or young people,

    say nasty and unpleasant things to him or her. It is also bullying when a

    child or young person is hit, kicked, threatened, locked inside a room,

    sent nasty notes, when no one ever talks to them and things like that.

    These things can happen frequently and it is difficult for the child or

    young person being bullied to defend himself or herself. It is also

    bullying when a child or young person is teased repeatedly in a nasty

    way. But it is not bullying when two children or young people of about

    the same strength have the odd fight or quarrel.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    18/41

    18

    Stage 2. When the data from Stage 1 had been analyzed, children selected through

    the PRQ were tested individually in an area of the school that afforded privacy. During

    this session, the stories were presented in a randomized order and the childrens answers

    were audio-recorded to facilitate the successive transcription and coding procedure. In

    order to avoid any possible influence upon their responses, the experimenter who

    interviewed children did not know their Participant role.

    Results

    Results are presented using the categorical measure of Participant Roles to

    compare group means in social cognition and moral disengagement with independent 6

    (Roles) x 2 (Gender) ANOVAs. The continuous measure Role Scores is used to test

    for correlations between the extent of different types of behavior in the whole sample

    and the social cognition and moral disengagement measures

    Preliminary analysis

    A preliminary one-way ANOVA was performed to investigate the possible effect

    of Participant Roles on the answers to the control questions. No statistically significant

    differences emerged among the different roles [F(5, 192) = 0.962, p = n.s.], thus

    confirming that the level of comprehension of the stories presented was similar for all

    Participant Roles.

    Moreover, in order to control for inter-rater reliability, two independent judges

    coded the 30% of all childrens answers. Their level of agreement reached 96%. Cohens

    kappa was also calculated, resulting in K=.91. Disagreements were due to those answers

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    19/41

    19

    in which the reference to mental states was not completely clear. All discrepancies were

    resolved by consultation between the coders.

    Participant Roles and social cognition

    As the three categories into which our stories were divided (cognitive, emotion

    and moral stories) are different from each other, three separate ANOVAs were

    conducted on the mean performance in the three sets of stories, with Role and Gender as

    between factors. From the analysis, a statistically significant main effect of Role

    emerged only for the cognitive stories [F(5, 192) = 2.376, p = .04, p2 = .058]. Post hoc

    analyses revealed that Victims performance (M=2.88, s.d.=1.61) was significantly

    lower than Defenders (M=4.03, s.d.=1.38), whereas other groups did not significantly

    differ from each other (Bonferroni comparisons). The main effect of Gender and the

    Role x Gender interaction were not statistically significant.

    These data do not support the prediction of Hypothesis 1 that bullies should

    perform better than other people in the theory of mind tasks, but they also fail to show

    social deficits among bullies. In contrast to Hypothesis 2, bullies did not do worse than

    the other groups in the moral stories.

    --- Insert Table III about here ---

    Role scores and social cognition

    A partial correlation analysis between social cognition and the actual extent of

    different types of behavior (the role scores) was performed. Considering the fact that the

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    20/41

    20

    scales are correlated with each other, to examine the relation of each role with the

    cognitive, emotion and moral information processing skills, in the correlation analysis

    the remaining PRQ scales and age were partialled out. Moreover, considering the high

    number of comparisons, in order to avoid the increase of type 1 error, Bonferronis

    correction was used (adjusted level of: 0.05/24=.002). Correlation coefficients are

    shown in Table IV. In line with Hypothesis 1 Bully role is positively correlated with the

    total social cognition score and with the cognitive score (the ability to understand

    thoughts, intentions, beliefs, etc.) and the emotion score. Moreover, in accordance with

    Hypothesis 2, the Defender score is positively correlated with all social cognition scores.

    --- Insert Table IV about here ---

    Participant Roles and moral disengagement

    As concerns the third aim, a two-way ANOVA on the mean score in the Moral

    Disengagement Scale was performed with Role and Gender as between factors. A

    statistically significant main effect of Role emerged [F(5, 192) = 9.269, p

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    21/41

    21

    level of moral disengagement than both Outsiders (M=30.35, s.d.=7.63) and Victims

    (M=32.41, s.d.=8.12). These findings support Hypothesis 3.

    --- Insert Figure 1 about here ---

    Role scores and moral disengagement

    A partial correlation analysis, controlled for age of participants, between the score

    in the moral disengagement scale and the role scores was performed. Results showed a

    positive correlation between moral disengagement and the role scores in Bully

    (r(204)=.26, p

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    22/41

    22

    models have been proposed in the literature to explain the Bullys behavior in terms of

    social information processing. The first model hypothesizes the presence of deficits in

    the social information processing of bullies, similar to those found for aggressive

    children (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980). This model, therefore, proposes the idea

    of bullies as oaf children. They are considered deficient in their social intelligence,

    that is their ability to interpret and manage information deriving from social interactions

    with peers and to produce adequate social responses. The second model, in contrast,

    states that bullies are quite skilful in understanding social cues (e.g., others mental

    states) and that they exploit this ability to their own advantage, acting as skilled social

    manipulators (Sutton et al., 1999a; 1999b). However, according to these authors,

    bullies may lack the empathic reactivity towards their mates emotions and, in particular,

    towards victims suffering.

    In the first two hypotheses of the present study, it was anticipated that bullies may

    have a good theory of mind, but they may be deficient in moral cognition. The analysis

    of variance, indeed, indicated that the bully group did not show any difficulty, compared

    to non-aggressive peers, in the understanding of cognitive stories, which required them

    to assume the cognitive point of view of another person, that is to understand intentions,

    beliefs and goals that guide a determinate behavior. Moreover, correlations between the

    scores in each scale of the PRQ and the performance in the social cognition task

    indicated a positive relation between bullying behavior and the ability to understand

    others mental states. As regards the two models, therefore, our results are closer to what

    would be expected by the Skilled Manipulator model (Sutton et al., 1999a). The fact that

    our bullies did not show particular deficits in this specific area of social cognition does

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    23/41

    23

    not suggest a direct application of the Social Skills Deficit model (Crick & Dodge, 1994)

    to bullying behavior. Further analyses are needed into the social cognitive competence

    of children involved in bullying episodes. This is particularly important because theory

    of mind explanations per se are not likely to be very helpful for understanding the

    unique nature of bullying, in that having a sophisticated theory of mind can lead to

    highly prosocial behavior or to bullying behavior. In other words, having a superior

    theory of mind says nothing about how that knowledge will be utilized in social

    interactions (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001).

    As regards the two other types of stories used in the present study, bullies

    performance in emotion stories and also in moral stories, a category not used by Sutton

    and colleagues (1999b), was found to be slightly, but not significantly lower, than the

    performance obtained by other groups. Contrary to our second hypothesis about the

    possible difficulties of bullies in moral cognition, our results do not completely confirm

    the literature on moral development in aggressive children and bullies (e.g., Bandura,

    1991; Hart, Burock, London, & Atkins, 2003). In our sample, in fact, the bullies and

    their followers did not show a significant lack of ability in the attribution of appropriate

    moral emotions to the characters of the stories. These emotions, such as guilt and shame,

    play an important role in the recognition of the damage suffered by another person and

    are closely connected to the sense of responsibility of the person who shows the harmful

    behavior. Moreover, these emotions imply the need of reparation as a consequence of a

    negative outcome produced by ones behavior (Bybee, 1998; Hoffman, 2000).

    However, for both emotion and moral stories, it is important to note that they

    actually assess the cognitive ability to process emotional information in social

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    24/41

    24

    interactions rather than an emotional or empathic comprehension of others feelings.

    Therefore, the positive performance shown by aggressive children in these kinds of

    stories may misrepresent or underestimate the real deficit of these children in emotional

    understanding of others. In other words, what bullies may lack and what may

    differentiate them from prosocial children is the ability to appreciate the emotional

    consequences of their behaviors on others feelings, and to share in, and empathize with,

    the feelings of others (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998). In line

    with this perspective, for example, Gini, Albiero, and Benelli (2004) recently found a

    negative relation between bullying behavior and empathy, measured through Davis

    empathic reactivity index (1983), in a sample of Italian adolescents.

    Sutton and colleagues (1999a) also argued along these lines, referring to the cold

    cognition hypothesis proposed by Mealey (1995) as regards sociopathy. They also

    spoke of a theory of mind formulated purely in instrumental terms, without access to the

    empathic understanding that most people rely on. As a consequence, the individual may

    use aggressive behavior in a Machiavellian way, without considering its negative

    consequences on others. In contrast, high levels of empathic responsiveness act as an

    important mediator in social conduct, reducing aggression and favoring prosocial

    behavior (Bjrkqvist, sterman, & Kaukiainen, 2000; Kaukiainen, Bjrkqvist,

    sterman, Lagerspetz, & Niskanen, 1994; Kaukiainen, Bjrkqvist, sterman, &

    Lagerspetz, 1996).

    With respect to the other groups considered in the present study, victimized

    children showed some difficulties in the social cognition task. If compared to the

    defender group in particular, they seem to lack the ability to understand others mind,

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    25/41

    25

    that is their thoughts, beliefs and intentions that guide social behavior. Our data are

    consistent with results reported by Sutton et al. (1999b). Moreover, several authors share

    the idea that victims show deficits in areas like social skills, social problem solving,

    assertiveness, and emotional regulation (e.g., Champion, Vernberg, & Shipman, 2003;

    Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1997; Mahady-Wilton, Craig, & Pepler, 2000; Olweus, 1993;

    Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988). Finally, the results obtained in the social cognition task by

    the defender group should be noted. These children, in fact, showed high levels of

    performance in all the stories, and especially in the cognitive stories. Moreover, the

    score in the defender scale of the PRQ correlated positively with all the scores in the

    social cognition task, thus suggesting that the adoption of this kind of prosocial and

    helpful behavior requires a high level of social ability and a well-developed

    understanding of both cognitive and emotional states of others. However, the fact that

    the bullying score also correlated with the social cognition task, again, confirms that

    having a good theory of mind, by itself, does not necessarily mean that this ability will

    be used to act prosocially (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001).

    Bullying and moral disengagement

    As regards the third aim of the present study, the analysis of variance on moral

    disengagement showed higher levels of the tendency to disengage self-sanctions and

    justify the use of aggressive behaviors in all the aggressive roles and, in particular, in

    bullies. These results confirmed the role of specific cognitive mechanisms in the

    regulation of social conduct, such as the moral disengagement mechanisms that allow

    the assumption of violent behaviors (Bacchini et al., 1998; Caprara et al., 1995;

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    26/41

    26

    Menesini et al., 1999; Menesini et al., 2003). Regarding this aspect, as suggested by a

    reviewer, it could be interesting to collect information about the relations between

    bullying, familial socialization and moral climate at home and at school.

    In contrast, defenders high moral sensibility and empathic reactivity, already

    found in other studies (Menesini et al., 2003; Gini & Carli, 2003), may be one of the

    basic motivations for prosocial behavior, frequently shown by these children towards

    their victimized mates. These motivations are very important and must be taken into

    account during the implementation of intervention programs in schools (Cowie, 2000;

    Cowie, Naylor, Talamelli, Chauhan, & Smith, 2002). However, the fact that the ability

    to help other peers requires several interpersonal competencies, that young children do

    not necessarily possess, needs to be considered.

    In conclusion, the results presented in this paper have several potential

    implications for future research. On the one hand, our results draw attention to the

    importance of studying the role of social cognitive competencies in the complex circle of

    behaviors, attitudes and expectations upon which school bullying phenomenon is based.

    On the other hand, the relevance of adopting a more fine-grained classification of roles

    in the study of bullying behavior was confirmed.

    A possible limitation in the interpretation of our results was the fact that we did

    not consider the bully-victim group, that is those children who frequently assume both

    kinds of role, acting as a bully and being victimized as well (e.g., Boulton & Smith,

    1994; Olweus, 1993). Several authors stressed the relevance of this group of pupils, also

    called aggressive-victims (Perry et al., 1988; Schwartz, 2000), in the study of aggressive

    problems. Moreover, these children have been consistently described as lacking social-

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    27/41

    27

    emotional abilities, having psychosocial impairments and a wide range of other

    problems (e.g., Besag, 1989). A methodological difficulty in the study of these pupils is

    that it is not always simple to clearly identify bully-victims as a distinct group. Despite

    this complication, future research in this field will necessarily need to study the social-

    cognitive and moral characteristics of this particular group of children, and to compare

    their level of cognitive and emotional understanding with that of pure bullies and

    victims.

    References

    Almeida, A., del Barrio, C., Marques, M., Gutierrez, H., & van der Meulen, K. (2001).

    Scan-bullying: A script-cartoon narrative to assess cognitions, emotions and coping

    strategies in bullying situations. In M Martinez. (Ed.), Prevention and control of

    aggression and the impact on its victims (pp. 161 168). New York: Kluwer

    Academic/Plenum Pub.

    Arsenio, W. F. (1988). Children's conceptions of the situational affective consequences

    of sociomoral events. Child Development, 58, 1611-1622.

    Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2001). Varieties of childhood bullying: Values,

    emotion processes and social competence. Social Development, 10, 59-73.

    Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the classroom. The

    Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86-97.

    Bacchini, D., Amodeo, A. L., Ciardi, A., Valerio, P., & Vitelli, R. (1998). La relazione

    vittima-prepotente: stabilit del fenomeno e ricorso a meccanismi di disimpegno

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    28/41

    28

    morale [Bully-victim relationship: stability of the phenomenon and use of moral

    disengagement]. Scienze dellInterazione, 5(1), 29-46.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundation of thought and action. A social cognitive theory.

    Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1990). Selective activation and disengagement of moral control. Journal of

    Social Issues, 46(1), 27-46.

    Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In W.M.

    Kurtines, & G.L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development:

    theory, research and applications, Vol. 1 (pp. 71-129). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency.

    Journal of Moral Education, 312, 101-119.

    Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of

    moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology, 71, 364-374.

    Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Regalia, C. (2001).

    Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 801, 125-135.

    Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: an interpersonal

    approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243-267.

    Besag, V. (1989). Bullies and victims in schools. Milton Keynes: Open University Press

    Milton.

    Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (2000). Social intelligence empathy =

    aggression? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 52, 191-200.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    29/41

    29

    Boulton, M., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Bully/victim problems in middle-school children:

    Stability, self-perceived competence, peer perceptions and peer acceptance. British

    Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 315-329.

    Bybee, J. (1998). Guilt and children. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Vicino, S., & Bandura, A. (1996). La misura del

    disimpegno morale [Measuring moral disengagement]. Rassegna di Psicologia, 113,

    93-105.

    Caprara, G. V., Pastorelli, C., & Bandura, A. (1995). La misura del disimpegno morale

    in et evolutiva [Measuring age differences in moral disengagement]. Et Evolutiva,

    51, 18-29.

    Champion, K., Vernberg, E., & Shipman, K. (2003). Nonbullying victims of bullies:

    Aggression, social skills, and friendship characteristics. Journal of Applied

    Developmental Psychology, 245, 535-551.

    Cowie, H. (2000). Bystanding or standing by: Gender issues in coping with bullying in

    English schools. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 85-97.

    Cowie, H., Naylor, P., Talamelli, L., Chauhan, P., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Knowledge,

    use and attitudes towards peer support. Journal of Adolescence, 255, 453-467.

    Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (1997). Observations of bullying and victimization in the

    schoolyard. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 2, 41-60.

    Craig, W. M., Pepler, D. J., & Atlas, R. (2000). Observations of bullying in the

    playground and in the classroom. School Psychology International, 21, 22-36.

    Cramer, D. (1998). Fundamental statistics for social research. London: Routledge

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    30/41

    30

    Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-

    processing mechanisms in childrens social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115,

    74-101.

    Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a

    multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 441,

    113-126.

    Dodge, K. (1980). Social cognition and childrens aggressive behavior. Child

    Development, 51, 162-170.

    Dodge, K., Bates, J., & Pettit, G. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science,

    250, 1678-1683.

    Dodge, K., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive bias and deficits in aggressive boys.

    Child Development, 53, 620-635.

    Dodge, K., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision-making processes in aggressive

    boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 375-379.

    Dodge, K., & Price, J. M. (1994). On the relation between social information processing

    and socially competent behavior in early school-aged children. Child Development,

    65, 1385-1397.

    Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. (1987). Hostile attributional biases are exacerbated under

    conditions of threat to the self. Child Development, 58, 213-224.

    Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, & N.

    Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3, 5th ed. (pp. 701-778). New

    York: Wiley.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    31/41

    31

    Gini, G., Albiero, P., & Benelli, B. (2004). Relazioni tra bullismo, empatia, immagine di

    s ed autoefficacia percepita in un campione di adolescenti [Relations between

    bullying, empathy, self-image and self-efficacy in adolescents]. Poster presented at

    the 18th

    National Conference of Developmental Psychology, Sciacca (AG), Italy.

    Gini, G., & Carli, G. (2003). Il bullismo a scuola: analisi dei meccanismi di disimpegno

    morale in una prospettiva di gruppo [Bullying at school: analysis of moral

    disengagement mechanisms in a group perspective]. Orientamenti Pedagogici.

    Rivista Internazionale per lEducazione, 502, 303-314.

    Happ, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story

    characters thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal

    children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 242, 129-154.

    Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure of the psychopathy

    checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 741-747.

    Hart, D., Burock, D., London, B., & Atkins, R. (2003). Prosocial tendencies, antisocial

    behavior, and moral development. In A. Slater & G. Bremner (Eds.), An

    introduction to developmental psychology (pp. 334-356). Malden, MA, US:

    Blackwell Publishers.

    Harvey, R. J., Fletcher, J., & French, D. J. (2001). Social reasoning: A source of

    influence on aggression. Clinical Psychology Review, 213, 447-469.

    Hawley, P. H. (2003). Prosocial and coercive configurations of resource control in early

    adolescence: A case for the well-adapted machiavellian. Merrill Palmer Quarterly,

    493, 279-309.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    32/41

    32

    Hazler, R. J. (1996). Breaking the cycle of violence: Interventions for bullying and

    victimization. Washington, DC: Accelerated Development.

    Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). Individual risk and social risk as

    interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental

    Psychology, 33(6), 1032-1039.

    Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathy, role-taking, guilt, and development of altruistic

    motives. In T. Likona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research,

    and social issues (pp. 124-143). New York: Holt, Rinehart Winston.

    Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development. New York, NY, US:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Childrens normative beliefs about

    aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

    72, 408-419.

    Kaukiainen, A., Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1996). Social

    intelligence and empathy as antecedents of different types of aggression. In C. F.

    Ferris & T. Grisson (Eds.), Understanding aggressive behavior in children (pp.

    364-366). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 794.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    33/41

    33

    Kaukiainen, A., Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Niskanen, L.

    (1994). Social intelligence and the use of indirect aggression. Presented at the XIII

    Biennal Meetings of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural

    Development, June 28-July 2, Amsterdam, The Nederlands.

    Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. (1997). Victimized childrens responses to peers

    aggression: Behaviors associated with reduced versus continued victimization.

    Development and Psychopathology, 9, 59-73.

    Mahady-Wilton, M. M., Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (2000). Emotional regulation and

    display in classroom victims of bullying: Characteristic expression of affect, coping

    styles and relevant contextual factors. Social Development, 92, 226-245.

    Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model.

    Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 18, 523-599.

    Menesini, E., Codecasa, E., Benelli, B., & Cowie, H. (2003). Enhancing children's

    responsibility to take action against bullying: Evaluation of a befriending

    intervention in Italian middle schools. Aggressive Behavior, 291, 10-14.

    Menesini, E., Fonzi, V., & Vannucci, M. (1997). Bullies and victims in Italy: attitudes

    and moral disengagement. Paper presented at the 7th European Conference for

    Research on Learning and Instruction EARLI Athens 26-30 August 1997.

    Menesini, E., & Gini, G. (2000). Il bullismo come processo di gruppo: adattamento e

    validazione del questionario Ruoli dei partecipanti alla popolazione italiana

    [Bullying as a group process: adaptation and validation of the Participant Role

    Questionnaire to the Italian population]. Et Evolutiva, 66, 18-32.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    34/41

    34

    Menesini, E., Sanchez, V., Fonzi, A., Ortega, R., Costabile, A., & Lo Feudo, G. (2003).

    Moral emotions and bullying: A cross-national comparison of differences between

    bullies, victims and outsiders. Aggressive Behavior, 296, 515-530.

    Nucci, L. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press

    Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school. What we know and what we can do. Blackwell,

    Oxford and Cambridge.

    Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1995). A peek behind the fence: naturalistic observations

    of aggressive children with remote audiovisual recording. Developmental

    Psychology, 314, 548-553.

    Perry, D. G., Kusel, S. J., & Perry, L. C. (1988). Victims of peer aggression.

    Developmental Psychology, 24, 807-814.

    Quiles, Z. N., & Bybee, J. (1997). Chronic and predispositional guilt: Relations to

    mental health, prosocial behavior and religiosity. Journal of Personality

    Assessment, 69, 104-126.

    Randall, P. (1997). Adult bullying: perpetrators and victims. London: Routledge.

    Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. M. (1997). Peer networks and bullying in

    schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 305-312.

    Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996).

    Bullying as a group process: Participant Roles and their relations to social status

    within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1-15.

    Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in childrens peer groups.

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181 - 192.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    35/41

    35

    Sharp, S., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Tackling bullying in your school: A practical

    handbook for teachers. London: Routledge.

    Slee, P. T. (1993). Bullying: A preliminary investigation of the nature and effects on

    social cognition. Early Child Development and Care, 87, 47-57.

    Smetana, J. G. (1995). Morality in context: Abstractions, ambiguities and applications.

    In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development: A research annual vol. 10 (pp. 83-

    130). London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Smith, P. K., Bowers, L., Binney, V., & Cowie, H. (1993). Relationships of children

    involved in bully/victim problems at school. In S. Duck (Ed.), Understanding

    relationship processes. Vol. 2: Learning about relationships (pp. 184-212). Sage

    Newbury Park, Ca.

    Sutton, J., & Smith. P. K. (1999). Bullying as a group process: An adaptation of the

    Participant Role approach. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 97-111.

    Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and theory of mind: A

    critique of the social skills deficit view of anti-social behaviour. Social

    Development, 8(1), 117-134.

    Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and bullying: social

    inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,

    173, 435-450.

    Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999c). Socially undesirable needs not be

    incompetent: A response to Crick and Dodge. Social Development, 8, 132-134.

    Tagney, J. P. (1992). Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young adulthood.

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 199-206.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    36/41

    36

    Turiel, E. (1998). Moral development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook

    of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development 5th

    ed. (pp. 863-932). New York: Wiley.

    Whitney, I., & Smith, P. K. (1993). A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in

    junior/middle and secondary schools. Educational Research, 35, 3-25.

    Yadava, A., Sharma, N. R., & Gandhi, A. (2001). Aggression and moral disengagement.

    Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 172, 95-99.

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    37/41

    37

    Table I. Distribution of Participant Roles (%) within the whole sample

    Participant Roles

    Bullies Assistants Reinforcers Defenders Outsiders Victims No role Total

    Males 15.9 16.3 11.9 11.5 12.2 17.6 14.6 100

    Females 5.9 6.3 7.7 26.2 25.5 17.8 10.6 100

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    38/41

    38

    Table II. Role score correlations for the whole sample

    Bully Assistant Reinforcer Defender Outsider

    Assistant .68 *

    Reinforcer .66 * .60 *

    Defender - .16 * - .11 .10

    Outsider - .22 * - .24 * - .07 .30 *

    Victim .07 .01 .12 - .01 .13

    Note: N = 578; * p

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    39/41

    39

    Table III. Participant Roles and mean score [s.d.] in social cognition task

    Participant

    Roles a

    Cognitive

    stories b

    Emotion stories

    b

    Moral

    stories b

    Total social

    cognition task c

    Bullies 3.06 [1.30] 2.79 [2.20] 3.18 [1.94] 9.03 [4.79]

    Assistants 3.18 [1.47] 3.20 [2.04] 3.59 [1.86] 9.97 [4.85]

    Reinforcers 3.15 [1.56] 3.26 [1.76] 3.62 [1.67] 10.03 [4.28]

    Defenders 4.03 [1.38] 4.12 [1.45] 4.00 [1.67] 12.15 [4.04]

    Outsiders 3.38 [1.76] 3.68 [1.66] 4.06 [1.63] 11.12 [4.58]

    Victims 2.88 [1.61] 3.26 [1.81] 3.44 [1.76] 9.59 [4.73]

    Note:

    a n=34 for each group; b range = 0-5; c range = 0-15

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    40/41

    40

    Table IV. Partial correlations of social cognition and individual role scores

    Social cognition scoresRole scores

    Cognitive Emotion Moral Total score

    Bully .195 * .148 * .129 .172 *

    Assistant - .021 - .026 .014 - .012

    Reinforcer - .074 - .059 - .098 - .078

    Defender .214 * .226 * .197 * .223 *

    Outsider .071 .008 .060 .050

    Victim .068 .109 .112 .104

    Note: N = 204; * p

  • 8/2/2019 Aggressive Behavior

    41/41

    Figure 1. Participant Roles and Moral Disengagement

    Notes:

    n=34 for each group

    range = 14-70

    a,b; c,d Means with different superscript differ at p < .05.

    0

    510

    15

    20

    25

    30

    35

    40

    Defenders

    ,a

    Outsid

    ers,

    c

    Victim

    s,c

    Assistants,

    b

    Reinforcers,b

    Bulli

    es,b

    ,d