albert gatt corpora and statistical methods – lecture 3

Click here to load reader

Upload: hilary-quinn

Post on 18-Dec-2015

218 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Slide 1
  • Albert Gatt Corpora and Statistical Methods Lecture 3
  • Slide 2
  • Morphology and productivity Part 2
  • Slide 3
  • Morphology Many languages have multiple word forms related to a single base form (root form) Lexeme = base form from which related forms are produced Three classes of productive morphological processes: Inflection Derivation Compounding
  • Slide 4
  • Inflection Addition of prefixes and suffixes that leave core meaning intact leave grammatical category intact add/alter some features of meaning (especially relevant to syntax) Examples: -s to form plural nouns -ed to form past tense
  • Slide 5
  • Derivation Addition of prefixes and suffixes which: result in a more radical change in meaning often result in change of syntactic category Examples: English -ly (ADJ ADV): wide-ly English -en (ADJ V): weak-en English -able (V ADJ): accept-able
  • Slide 6
  • Compounding Combination of two independent words into a new word NB new word can be orthographically one or several words can cause recognisable changes in phonology new compound has a new meaning (not necessarily 100% compositional) Example: English N-N compounds disk drive, mad cow disease, credit crunch
  • Slide 7
  • Regular vs. irregular Inflectional and derivational rules often have exceptions. E.g. Past tense in English: regular: -ed suffix irregular: bring brought, ring - rang etc Sub-regularities observable: -ing/k verbs in English seem to display a particular pattern: rang, sank,
  • Slide 8
  • Productive vs non-productive Some morphological processes or categories seem to have greater potential to form new words than others e.g. English -able, -ness compare to English th: warmth, strength (much less productive)
  • Slide 9
  • Classical approaches to productivity Jackendoff (1975): unproductive rules are called redundancy rules: e.g. warmth is listed in the English speakers (mental) lexicon as a single word the redundancy rule captures the knowledge that it can be split into warm+th rule as such isnt really active, i.e. forms not produced online contrast with productive rules: e.g. Many adjectives with able are produced online, not stored
  • Slide 10
  • Features of classical approaches 1. Relies on a binary distinction (un/productive) 2. Productive rules are typically regular & sub-regularities not considered much (Dressler 2003) 3. Most of these approaches do not look at corpus data Related psycholinguistic model: Pinkers (1997) dual-route model of morphological processing
  • Slide 11
  • Corpus-based approaches View productivity as a gradable phenomenon: some forms become ingrained through frequent usage category can still be productive to some extent productivity estimated in terms of a categorys potential to produce new forms can account for sub-regularities: productivity of a category is due to a lot of factors, including analogy to existing words
  • Slide 12
  • The continuum Productive processes tend to: be compositional result in a lot of new words Productive morphological process lexicalised word ADJ+ness Noun ADJ+th Noun
  • Slide 13
  • Practical application (I) No finite lexicon can contain all words of a language at a certain time productive processes can be exploited to parse new/unseen lexical items this is helped by the compositionality of productive processes can also help to distinguish creative neologism from systematic rule- application. compare: well-defined, well-intentioned, well-specified lots of adjectives with a well- prefix YouTube a one-off
  • Slide 14
  • Practical application (II) Polarity/sentiment analysis: aim is to identify the overall positive/negative slant of a text concerning a topic Moilanen and Pulman (2008) obtain improvements by considering adjectives formed with well- vs infested etc
  • Slide 15
  • Theoretical implications raises interesting questions about the relationship between corpus-based measures and psycholinguistic data likelihood of a morphological process being applied depends on style, genre, speech community can give an indication of language change over time (some processes are fossilised, others become more productive)
  • Slide 16
  • Statistical measures of productivity (Baayen 2006)
  • Slide 17
  • What we need A measure of productivity of a process/category C should reflect: our intuitions about how frequently we encounter C how easily native speakers can form new words using C Is it easier to produce a noun with th (like warmth) or one with ness (like goodness)?
  • Slide 18
  • Realised productivity (RP) Given a morphological category C, RP gives a rough indication of the past utility of C in forming new words. Measured as the number of distinct types formed using C in a corpus of size N. E.g. regular past tense ed displays many more types than sub-regular forms such as keep-kept/sleep-slept
  • Slide 19
  • Realised productivity cont/d Why types, not tokens? Productive processes have lots of types which are hapaxes, or are very infrequent. Words formed from irregular processes tend to be very frequent. Some limitations: a high RP for a category does not imply that it will keep forming lots of new words RP is heavily dependent on corpus size
  • Slide 20
  • Expanding productivity (P*) P* gives a rough indication of the rate of expansion of C. Focuses on the number of hapaxes produced using C in the corpus. aka hapax-conditioned productivity NB: P* is still heavily dependent on corpus size! No. of types formed using C which occur only once in N tokens No. of hapaxes in the corpus
  • Slide 21
  • Potential productivity (P) Gives an indication of how likely a category C is to form new words in future. I.e. the potential for C to be already saturated aka category-conditioned productivity No. of types in C which occur only once in corpus of N tokens No. of tokens of category C
  • Slide 22
  • Some more on P Unlike RP and P*, P is not very sensitive to corpus size as such However, very sensitive to frequency of the category. e.g. if C is realised only once in a corpus of size N, then P = 1! Recent empirical work has shown that RP and P* correlate very strongly, but both exhibit a weak correlation with P (Vegnaduzzo 2009) pattern non-X has high RP and P*, but low P pattern X-ish has low RP and P*, but high P
  • Slide 23
  • In graphics (after Baayen 2006) Corpus size No. of types Growth curve for a specific category Slope of tangent represents growth rate
  • Slide 24
  • P vs. RP and P* A category can have low RP and P*, but high P. Corresponds to the ease with which new words can be formed using the category. Even though a category has high RP, it may have reached saturation, so have low P.
  • Slide 25
  • The psycholinguistic connection 1. Rule vs. direct access: To produce a word (e.g. illegal), you can either store it directly, or apply the rule on the fly. Evidence suggests that frequency of baseform vs. derivation is related to which of the two alternatives apply.
  • Slide 26
  • The psycholinguistic connection 2. Complexity-based affix ordering: Corpus research: more productive affixes follow less productive ones in word formation It seems that more highly predictable (low productivity) affixes are processed first. High productivity may also imply less likelihood of entering into further derivational processes.
  • Slide 27
  • Works cited S. Vegnaduzzo (2009). Morphological productivity rankings of complex adjectives. Proc. NAACL-HLT Workshop on Computational Approaches to Linguistic Creativity. K. Molinen and S. Pulman (2008). The good, the bad and the unknown: Morphosyllabic sentiment tagging of unseen words. Proc. ACL 2008 Baayen 2006 linked from web page