an examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional...

12
This article was downloaded by: [University of Auckland Library] On: 01 December 2014, At: 16:02 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Psychotherapy Research Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20 An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome Jeanne C. Watson a , Evelyn J. McMullen a , Meghan C. Prosser b & Danielle L Bedard c a University of Toronto, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Department of Adult Education and Counselling Psychology , Toronto, Ontario, Canada b Private practice c London Health Sciences Centre , London, Ontario, Canada Published online: 18 Nov 2010. To cite this article: Jeanne C. Watson , Evelyn J. McMullen , Meghan C. Prosser & Danielle L Bedard (2011) An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome, Psychotherapy Research, 21:1, 86-96, DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2010.518637 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2010.518637 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Upload: danielle-l

Post on 07-Apr-2017

216 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

This article was downloaded by: [University of Auckland Library]On: 01 December 2014, At: 16:02Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Psychotherapy ResearchPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20

An examination of the relationships among clients'affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, theworking alliance, and outcomeJeanne C. Watson a , Evelyn J. McMullen a , Meghan C. Prosser b & Danielle L Bedard ca University of Toronto, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Department of AdultEducation and Counselling Psychology , Toronto, Ontario, Canadab Private practicec London Health Sciences Centre , London, Ontario, CanadaPublished online: 18 Nov 2010.

To cite this article: Jeanne C. Watson , Evelyn J. McMullen , Meghan C. Prosser & Danielle L Bedard (2011) An examinationof the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome,Psychotherapy Research, 21:1, 86-96, DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2010.518637

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2010.518637

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) containedin the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of theContent. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon andshould be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable forany losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use ofthe Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in anyform to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

An examination of the relationships among clients’ affect regulation,in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

JEANNE C. WATSON1, EVELYN J. McMULLEN1, MEGHAN C. PROSSER2, &

DANIELLE L BEDARD3

1University of Toronto, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Department of Adult Education and Counselling

Psychology, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; 2Private practice & 3London Health Sciences Centre, London, Ontario, Canada

(Received 24 August 2009; accepted 9 August 2010)

AbstractThe objectives were to examine the relationships among clients’ affect regulation capacities, in-session emotional processing,outcome, and the working alliance in 66 clients who received either cognitive�behavioral therapy or process-experientialemotion-focused therapy for depression. Clients’ initial level of affect regulation predicted their level of emotionalprocessing during early and working phases of therapy. Clients’ peak emotional processing in the working phase of therapymediated the relationship between their initial level of affect regulation and their level of affect regulation at the end oftherapy; and clients’ level of affect regulation at the end of therapy mediated the relationship between their peak level ofemotional processing in the working phase of therapy and outcome. Clients’ affect regulation at the end of therapy predictedoutcome independently of the working alliance. The findings suggest that clients’ level of affect regulation early in therapyhas a significant impact on the quality of their in-session processing and outcome in short-term therapy. Limitations of thestudy and future directions for research are discussed.

Keywords: cognitive�behavioral therapy; depression; alliance; emotion in therapy; experiential/existential/humanistic

psychotherapy; outcome research; process research; affect regulation

In an effort to better understand the factors that

contribute to positive therapeutic outcomes, re-

searchers have turned their attention to identifying

client characteristics that influence in-session pro-

cesses and outcome (Hardy et al., 2001; Hersoug,

Høglend, Havik, von der Lippe, & Monsen,

2009; Joyce, Piper, Ogrodniczuk, & Klein, 2007;

Ogrodniczuk, Piper, Joyce, McCallum, & Rosie,

2003; Quilty et al., 2008; Zuroff et al., 2000). One

pretherapy characteristic that has not received much

attention in the literature is clients’ capacity to

regulate their affect. Affect regulation, defined as

how people process, modulate, and express their

emotional experience, is viewed as an important

determinant of psychopathology and as a target of

change in psychotherapy (Bradley, 2000; Greenberg

& Watson, 2006; Watson, 2007). Poor affect regula-

tion has been implicated in a number of different

disorders, including chronic fatigue (Godfrey,

Chalder, Ridsdale, Seed, & Ogden, 2007), bulimia

(Stice, 1999), opiate addiction (Giyaur, Sharf, &

Hilsenroth, 2005), complicated grief (Ogrodniczuk

et al., 2003), and depression (Greenberg & Watson,

2006).

Several theorists have suggested that a number of

conditions, including depression, may be character-

ized by emotional and cognitive processing difficul-

ties that interfere with the ability to modulate affect.

On the basis of the finding that depressed people

show more excessive control of emotion than others,

Beutler, Engle, Oro’Beutler, Daldrup, and Meredith

(1986) suggested that they fail to process intense

emotional information, which in turn interferes with

their expression and modulation of emotional exp-

erience. Similarly, Elliott, Watson, Goldman, and

Greenberg (2003) characterized depression as a chr-

onic state of narrowed negative experiencing. Further

evidence of restricted emotional processing in de-

pression has been found with research showing that

depressed people recall more negative memories

and selectively attend to negative stimuli in their

environments, which interferes with their ability to

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeanne C. Watson, Department of Adult Education, Community

Development and Counselling Psychology, OISE/UT, 252 Bloor Street West, 7th floor, Toronto, Ontario M5S 1V6, Canada. Email:

[email protected]

Psychotherapy Research, January 2011; 21(1): 86�96

ISSN 1050-3307 print/ISSN 1468-4381 online # 2011 Society for Psychotherapy Research

DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2010.518637

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 3: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

regulate their affective experience (Kennedy-Moore

& Watson, 1999). More recently, Watson, Goldman,

and Greenberg (2007) observed that clients’ ability

to regulate their affective experience distinguished

good from poor outcome in process-experiential

therapy, an emotion-focused, short-term psychother-

apy for depression.

The important role that clients’ ability to regulate

their affect plays in psychotherapy outcome is con-

sistent with studies that have found that the quality of

clients’ emotional processing in the session has been

linked to positive client outcomes in psychotherapy

(Castonguay, Goldfried, Wiser, Raue, & Hayes,

1996; Pos, Greenberg, Goldman, & Korman, 2003;

Stanton et al., 2000; Watson & Bedard, 2006;

Whelton, 2004). Several studies have revealed sig-

nificant differences in the manner in which good-

and poor-outcome cases process their emotional

experience during the session (Pos et al., 2003;

Watson & Bedard, 2006). Watson and Bedard

(2006) found that good-outcome clients in both

process-experiential therapy, an emotion-focused

approach (PE-EFT), and cognitive�behavioral ther-

apy (CBT) for depression began, continued, and

ended therapy at higher modal and peak experiencing

levels during the session than did clients with poor

outcome. Good-outcome clients in both PE-EFT

and CBT engaged in deeper exploration, referred to

their emotions more frequently, were more internally

focused, and examined and reflected on their experi-

ence to create new meaning and resolve their

problems in personally meaningful ways more than

poor-outcome clients. In contrast, clients with poorer

outcomes were not as engaged in processing their

emotional experience, nor did they reflect on or pose

questions about their experience during the session

to examine it and try to understand the origins and

implications of their experience more fully. As a

result, poor-outcome clients did not report important

shifts in perspective or feeling during the session.

Similarly, Pos et al. (2003) found that clients’ level of

experiencing late in therapy mediated the relation-

ship between their early experiencing and outcome,

and that the working alliance made an independent

contribution to outcome after controlling for clients’

experiencing level in client- centered therapy and PE-

EFT to predict outcome. Moreover, clients’ emo-

tional processing in the session has been found to be

beneficial across a range of therapeutic approaches,

including CBT (Castonguay et al., 1996; Godfrey

et al., 2007; Leahy, 2002; Stanton et al., 2000) and

psychodynamic therapy (Giyaur et al., 2005).

To enhance treatment effectiveness and better

understand the change process, it is important to

establish links between clients’ pretreatment char-

acteristics, psychotherapy process, and outcome.

The goal of the current study was to examine the

relationship between initial level of affect regulation,

in-session emotional processing, the working alli-

ance, and outcome in a group of clients treated for

depression in a randomized clinical trial comparing

the effectiveness of CBT and PE-EFT (Watson,

Gordon, Stermac, Kalogerakos, & Steckley, 2003).

A second objective was to test the convergent and

discriminant validity of the Observer-Rated Measure

of Affect Regulation (O-MAR).

Hypotheses

It was hypothesized, first, that clients’ level of affect

regulation at the beginning of therapy would predict

their level of emotional processing early in therapy;

second, that clients’ level of affect regulation at the

beginning of therapy would predict their level of

emotional processing during the working phase of

therapy; third, that clients’ level of emotional proces-

sing during the working phase of therapy would

mediate the relationship between their level of affect

regulation at the beginning of therapy and their level

of affect regulation at the end of therapy; fourth, that

clients’ level of affect regulation at the end of therapy

would mediate the relationship between their highest

level of emotional processing during the working

phase of therapy and outcome; and fifth, that clients’

level of affect regulation at the end of therapy would

predict outcome over and above the working alliance.

Method

Participants

Clients. The sample comprised 66 clients (67%

female, 33% male) who participated in a randomized

clinical trial comparing PE-EFT and CBT in the

treatment of depression (Watson et al., 2003). All

clients were diagnosed with major depression accord-

ing to DSM-IV criteria using the Structured Clinical

Interview for DSM-IV (Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, &

First, 1995) and were assigned to either PE-EFT (20

female, 13 male) or CBT (24 female, 9 male). Clients

ranged in age from 21 to 65 years (M�41.52, SD�10.82). Regarding ethnicity, 91% identified them-

selves as European, 6% as Asian, and 3% as Hispanic.

A majority of clients (56%) had a college degree; the

remaining 44% either completed a secondary level of

education or had a graduate degree. In terms of

marital status, 42% were married or living common

law, 42% were single, and 16% were separated,

divorced, or widowed. Thirty-four clients (51%), 17

each in the PE-EFT and CBT groups, were diag-

nosed with Axis II personality disorders: 20 (58%)

Affect regulation, emotional processing, outcome 87

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 4: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

with obsessive�compulsive, 20 with avoidant, one

with histrionic, one with schizoid, three with narcis-

sistic, two with dependent, and four with paranoid

personality disorder. The Global Assessment of

Functioning scores ranged from 51 to 65 (M�58.17). There were no significant differences be-

tween the groups in terms of demographic variables.

Therapists. A total of 15 therapists participated:

eight CBT (six female, two male) and seven PE-EFT

(six female, one male). Therapists ranged in age

from 26 to 43 years (M�32.73, SD�6.08). Four-

teen identified themselves as European and one as

Asian. The majority of the therapists were master’s/

doctoral candidates in counseling psychology at a

large metropolitan university in southern Ontario,

and two were psychologists. Level of experience

ranged from 1 to 15 years (M�5.23, SD�4.74).

There were no significant differences between CBT

and PE-EFT therapists in terms of experience level,

age, education, or gender.

Process raters. There were five process raters, all

female and all European. They ranged in age from

24 to 52 years. All were master’s/doctoral candidates

in counseling psychology at a large metropolitan

university in southern Ontario.

Treatments

CBT. The CBT protocol was conducted accord-

ing to cognitive therapy treatment for depression

outlined by Beck, Rush, Shaw, and Emery (1979).

The treatment was primarily a cognitive therapy with

some behavioral components, such as the recording

of daily activities and behavioral experiments.

PE-EFT. This treatment followed the manual

developed by Greenberg, Rice, and Elliott (1993).

PE-EFT integrates client-centered and gestalt tech-

niques, including two-chair, empty-chair, systematic

evocative unfolding, and focusing, to resolve clients’

cognitive-affective problems in therapy. When thera-

pists felt it was most appropriate, they implemented

specific interventions at client markers or statements

that indicated clients were experiencing specific

processing difficulties. There was a general expecta-

tion that therapists would implement a minimum of

one intervention every two to three sessions from

Session 3 through 15.

Process Measures

The Experiencing Scale (EXP; Klein,

Mathieu, Gendlin, & Kiesler, 1969). This pro-

cess measure assesses clients’ moment-to-moment

emotional processing during the therapy hour. Client

statements are rated on a 7-point scale in terms of

the extent to which they talk about or use their

affective experience as a referent during therapy, and

explore and reflect on their inner experience to

achieve self-understanding and problem resolution

(Klein, Mathieu-Coughlan, & Kiesler, 1986). At

Level 1 clients discuss events, ideas, or others without

any reference to the self or their emotions; at Level 2

they refer to the self without expressing emotions; at

Level 3 clients refer to their reactions about external

events; at Level 4 they share their experience and

subjective worldview; at Level 5 clients begin to

purposefully explore and examine their personal

reactions and subjective worldviews; at Level 6 clients

gain awareness of previously tacit feelings and mean-

ings; and at Level 7 they make continuous reference

to their emotions as part of an ongoing process of

understanding and devising ways of being in the

world (Klein et al., 1986). Clients’ experiencing has

been related to good outcome (Klein et al., 1986; Pos

et al., 2003; Watson & Bedard, 2006). Interrater

reliability coefficients have ranged from .76 to .91,

with rating re-rating correlation coefficients of ap-

proximately .80 (Klein et al., 1986).

In the present study, four raters (two primary and

two secondary) were trained to an acceptable level of

agreement with an expert according to the training

manual (Klein et al., 1969) for the EXP (Klein et al.,

1986). The four raters were organized into three

different pairs. One pair rated the middle 20 minutes

of Session 1, overlapping on 69% of the data. All

intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) in this

study were a two-way, fixed, single measure, abso-

lute agreement type (McGraw & Wong, 1996;

Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). For Session 1 ratings, the

ICC was .67 (pB.000) for modal ratings and .76

(pB.000) for peak ratings. The ICC is strongly

influenced by variance, and ratings for Session

1 were restricted between 2 and 4, which likely

deflated ICC scores. The coders had 89% agreement

on modal ratings and 87% on peak ratings.

The data for the hypotheses were divided across

the three pairs of raters. ICCs for the modal and

peak ratings, respectively, were as follows: .90 (pB

.001) and .90 (pB.001) for the first pair of raters;

.80 (pB.001) and .81 (pB.001) for the second pair;

and .78 (pB.001) and .78 (pB.001) for the third

pair. Levels greater than .75 can be considered

excellent agreement beyond chance (Cicchetti,

1994; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979).

O-MAR (Watson & Prosser, 2004). The

O-MAR assesses clients’ affect regulation based

on five subscales of emotional processing: (1)

Level of Awareness, (2) Modulation of Arousal, (3)

88 J. C. Watson et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 5: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

Modulation of Expression, (4) Acceptance of Affec-

tive Experience, and (5) Reflection on Experience.

Each item is rated on a 7-point Likert scale, with

higher scores signifying more optimal levels of func-

tioning. An overall score is calculated as the average of

the five ratings. Preliminary findings in an earlier

study involving 50 participants indicated that the O-

MAR has high internal consistency (early O-MAR�.86; late O-MAR�.93), and there is preliminary

evidence of construct and predictive validity (Prosser

& Watson, 2007). Preliminary data on the construct

validity of the O-MAR were obtained by correlating

the O-MAR full-scale score with the Problem-

Focused Style of Coping subscales, which distinguish

between reflective, suppressive, and reactive coping

styles. As expected, O-MAR scores were positively

correlated with a reflective coping style (r�.40) and

negatively correlated with suppressive (r��.52) and

reactive (r��.51) coping styles. The current study

expanded the data set to 66. Two independent raters

were trained on the O-MAR to an acceptable level of

agreement. They rated the data, overlapping on 70%

of the data set. Interrater reliability for the O-MAR

(N�66) was significant (ICC�.78, pB.001, early

sessions; ICC�.87, pB.001, late sessions). Each

client’s mean scores across the five subscales for early

sessions and again for later sessions were used for the

analyses.

Postsession Outcome Measure

Working Alliance Inventory�Short Form

(WAI-S; Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). The

WAI-S is a 12-item self-report questionnaire derived

from Bordin’s (1979) conceptualization of the work-

ing alliance. It measures the agreement between the

participants on the goals and tasks of therapy and

the quality of the bond. Each item is rated on a

7-point Likert scale ranging from never (1) to always

(7). This instrument possesses high internal consis-

tency (.93 for the client-rated scale) and good

construct validity. Furthermore, the WAI has been

shown to have good concurrent and predictive

validity through its correlations with other measures

of the therapeutic relationship and with outcome

measures (Horvath & Greenberg, 1986).

Outcome Measures

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck,

Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961).

The BDI is a 21-item inventory for assessing

depression. Scores of 10 and higher are regarded as

symptomatic of depression. Test�retest reliability has

been reported at .65 (Ogles, Lambert, & Sawyer,

1995). The BDI has been shown to have good

concurrent validity and internal consistency reported

at .86 (Beck & Steer, 1984). Test�retest reliability

has been reported at .65 (Ogles et al., 1995).

Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP;

Horowitz, Rosenberg, Baer, Ureno, &

Villasenor, 1988). This 127-item self-report instru-

ment measures distress arising from interpersonal

sources. Responses are scored according to a cir-

cumplex model divided into eight octants corre-

sponding to eight subscales: Domineering/

Controlling, Vindictive/Self-Centered, Cold/Dis-

tant, Socially Inhibited, Nonassertive, Overly Ac-

commodating, Self-Sacrificing, and Intrusive/Needy.

The IIP has been shown to possess high internal

consistency, reliability, and validity (Horowitz et al.,

1988) and high test�retest reliability (r�.90; Hansen

& Lambert, 1996).

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory (RSE;

Rosenberg, 1965). A 10-item version of the RSE

(Bachman & O’Malley, 1977) was used to assess

clients’ levels of self-esteem. This instrument has

shown good internal consistency and validity. High

internal reliability (.89�.94), high test�retest relia-

bility (.80�.90), and adequate sensitivity to change

have been reported.

Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R;

Derogatis, Rickels, & Roch, 1976). This 90-item

self-report questionnaire measures general psycho-

logical distress. Derogatis et al. (1976) reported

internal consistency ranging from .77 to .90 and

test�retest reliability between .80 and .90 over a 1-

week interval.

Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS;

Weissman & Beck, 1978). The DAS is a 40-item

inventory of dysfunctional attitudes measuring

vulnerability to depression. It has high internal

reliability coefficients and test�retest reliability coef-

ficients (Kuiper & Olinger, 1989).

Procedure

The BDI, RSE, DAS, IIP, and SCL-90-R are all

client self-report instruments and were completed

posttherapy. The WAI-S was completed after every

session, and mean scores were used for the analyses

in the current study. Clients’ level of affect regulation

was rated at the beginning (Session 1 or 2) and at the

end (Session 15 or 16) of therapy using videotaped

segments and transcripts when available, consisting

of the middle 20 minutes of each session. To

demonstrate O-MAR and client EXP as distinct

Affect regulation, emotional processing, outcome 89

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 6: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

constructs, the same middle-20-minute portion of

the session was also coded on EXP.

Clients’ level of experiencing over the course of

therapy was rated using transcripts of three sessions.

One session was chosen from early in therapy (Ses-

sions 2�4) and the other two were chosen from the

later working phase of therapy (Sessions 5�15).

Sessions were chosen based on clients’ levels of

observed emotional expression, for example, when

clients’ speech was ragged or there were other changes

in vocal quality, such as crying, voice cracking,

volume fluctuations (e.g., rising as a result of anger

or softening as a result of tearfulness or the expression

of some other emotion). Choosing sessions in which

clients were emotional was an attempt to address the

uniformity myth, which assumes that client process is

the same within sessions and across therapy (Elliot

et al., 2004; Greenberg & Safran, 1987; Greenberg,

Watson, & Goldman 1998; Pos et al., 2003). The

identification of sessions in which clients were

aroused ensured that clients’ emotions were activated,

and that meaningful and relevant excerpts of clients’

in-session emotional processing were being ade-

quately tested in the study. Segments of the sessions

began 5 minutes before the onset of emotional

expression and continued until the end of the session

or until the subject matter about which clients had

expressed emotion ended and it was clear that their

emotion had subsided. A change in subject matter

occurred for six of the 198 sessions that were

transcribed and rated on experiencing, and in these

cases the segment ended 5 minutes after the deviation.

Results

Prior to analyses, the variables were screened for

accuracy of data entry, missing values, and potential

univariate outliers and to test for univariate normal-

ity. To determine the internal consistency of the O-

MAR for the present sample (N�66), Cronbach’s

alpha was calculated. Consistent with Prosser and

Watson (2007), the O-MAR showed high internal

consistency: early O-MAR a�.86 and late O-MAR

a�.93. The interitem correlations for each of the

subscales ranged from r�.36 to r�.86. The correla-

tions are presented in Table I.

Intercorrelations Among Affect Regulation,

Experiencing, Alliance, and Outcome

To establish affect regulation, as measured by the O-

MAR, and emotional processing, as measured by the

EXP, as distinct constructs, Pearson product�moment correlation coefficients were calculated for

data rated on the middle 20 minutes of Session 1.

No significant relationship was found between these

variables for modal (r�.07; range, �.02 to .21) and

peak (r�.19; range, �.03 to .19) scores, providing

support that these two constructs are independent.

The intercorrelations among affect regulation, ex-

periencing, alliance, and the outcome variables used

in the analyses that follow are presented in Table II.

Early Affect Regulation Predicting Emotional

Processing Early in Therapy

To determine whether clients’ level of affect regula-

tion at the beginning of therapy was related to their

emotional processing early in therapy, linear regres-

sion analyses were performed, with clients’ O-MAR

scores from the beginning of therapy as the inde-

pendent variable and clients’ mean modal rating and

peak level of experiencing early in therapy as

the dependent variables. Clients’ O-MAR scores

significantly predicted their mean modal rating,

F(1, 64)�14.999, pB.000 (�.44, R2�.19, ad-

justed R2�.18), and peak level of experiencing,

F(1, 64)�15.053, pB.000 (�.44, R2�.19, ad-

justed R2�.18), as early as Sessions 2 to 4.

Early Affect Regulation Predicting Emotional

Processing in Midtherapy

To determine whether clients’ level of affect regula-

tion at the beginning of therapy predicted their level

of emotional processing during the working phase of

therapy, a linear regression analysis was performed

with clients’ O-MAR scores from the beginning of

therapy as the independent variable and mean modal

rating and peak level of experiencing (highest rating

of the two sessions from the working phase of

therapy) at midtherapy as the dependent variables.

Clients’ O-MAR scores significantly predicted their

mean modal rating, F(1, 64)�6.46, pB.01 (�.30,

Table I. Internal Consistency Reliability Analyses for Observer-Rated Measure of Affect Regulation (O-MAR) Subscales (N�66)

Item�total correlations Range of interitem correlations

Variable Early O-MAR Late O-MAR Early O-MAR Late O-MAR

1. Awareness and Labeling of Experience .68 .60 .37�.78 .36�.70

2. Modulation of Arousal .61 .75 .38�.61 .36�.82

3. Modulation of Expression .56 .86 .37�.58 .51�.86

4. Acceptance of Experience .76 .92 .57�.65 .63�.86

5. Reflective of Experience .76 .85 .40�.78 .66�.82

90 J. C. Watson et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 7: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

Table II. Intercorrelations Among Affect Regulation, Experiencing, Alliance, and Outcome (N�66)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Early O-MAR *2. Late O-MAR .38*** *3. Early modal EXP .44**** .32*** *4. Early peak EXP .44**** .36*** .75**** *5. Mid modal EXP .30** .43**** .66**** .45*** *6. Mid peak EXP .32*** .56**** .52**** .52**** .64**** *7. Mean WAI-S .05 .31** .13 .13 .15 .29** *8. Post BDI �.20 �.50**** �.12 �.37*** �.13 �.39**** �.49**** *9. Post RSE .19 .54**** .29** .42**** .18 .38*** .35*** �.64**** *

10. Post DAS �.11 �.47**** �.18 �.32** �.09 �.27** �.36*** .46**** �.71**** *11. Post IIP �.26** �.39*** �.19 �.25* �.07 �.30** �.45**** .68**** �.60**** .67**** *12. Post SCL�GSI �.23 �.37*** �.15 �.40**** �.03 �.34*** �.36*** .72**** �.57**** .50**** .58**** *

Note. O-MAR, Observer-Rated Measure of Affect Regulation; EXP, Experiencing Scale; WAI-S, Working Alliance Inventory� Short Form; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; RSE, Rosenberg

Self-Esteem Inventory; DAS, Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale; IIP, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; SCL�GSI, Symptom Checklist-90-Revised, Global Symptom Index.

*p�.051. **pB.05. ***pB.01. ****pB.001.

Affect

regula

tion,

emotion

al

processin

g,

outcom

e91

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 8: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

R2�.09, adjusted R2�.08), and peak level of

experiencing, F(1, 64)�7.187, pB.01 (�.32,

R2�.10, adjusted R2�.09), during the working

phase of treatment.

Hierarchical Regression of Early�Late Affect

Regulation Mediated by Peak Experiencing

To determine whether clients’ level of emotional

processing during the working phase of therapy

mediated the relationship between level of affect

regulation at the beginning of therapy and level of

affect regulation at the end of therapy, the proce-

dures outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986) were

followed. Three different regression analyses were

performed to test mediation. Table III presents

the results of the regression analyses.

The results indicated that clients’ affect regulation

(independent variable) at the beginning of therapy

significantly predicted the mediator, clients’ peak

level of emotional processing from the working phase

of therapy (Equation 1.1 in Table III), and the

dependent variable, clients’ level of affect regulation

at the end of therapy (Equation 1.2). In the final

equation (1.3), the relationship between the inde-

pendent and dependent variables remained signifi-

cant, and peak emotional processing score from the

working phase of therapy also predicted the depen-

dent variable while controlling for the independent

variable. However, as predicted, the weight for

clients’ affect regulation scores at the beginning of

therapy dropped from .38 to .22 when clients’ peak

level of emotional processing was entered into the

regression model, indicating that clients’ emotional

processing partially mediates the relationship be-

tween their early and later affect regulation scores.

Last, results from the Sobel test confirmed the

statistical significance of the mediational pathway

found through regression analysis.

Hierarchical Regression of Experiencing�Outcome Mediated by Affect Regulation

To determine whether clients’ level of affect regula-

tion at the end of therapy mediated the relationship

between level of emotional processing during the

working phase of therapy and outcome, the proce-

dures outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986) were

again followed. Results of the regression analyses

(Table IV) revealed that clients’ peak level of emo-

tional processing during the working phase of

therapy significantly predicted the mediator, clients’

O-MAR scores at the end of therapy (Equation 1.1).

In the second step, five linear regression analyses

were conducted to determine whether the indepen-

dent variable, clients’ peak level of emotional proces-

sing during the working phase of therapy, predicted

the dependent variable, clients’ outcome scores on

the BDI, RSE, DAS, IIP, and SCL-90-R at the end

of therapy (Equations 1.2 to 5.2, respectively, in

Table IV). Clients’ peak level of emotional processing

during the working phase of therapy significantly

predicted outcome on all five outcome measures.

In the third step, five hierarchical linear regression

analyses were performed to test whether the med-

iator, clients’ affect regulation at the end of therapy,

predicted the dependent variable, clients’ outcome

scores on the BDI, RSE, DAS, IIP, and SCL-90-R at

the end of therapy, while controlling for the inde-

pendent variable. The model significantly predicted

outcome on the five outcome measures at the end of

therapy. Clients’ O-MAR scores at the end of

therapy significantly predicted outcome, after con-

trolling for clients’ peak experiencing scores on the

BDI, RSE, DAS, and IIP and approached signifi-

cance for the SCL-90-R (Equations 1.1 to 5.3). As

predicted, the weight for clients’ peak level of

emotional processing dropped from �.39 to �.16

on the BDI, from .38 to .11 on the RSE, from �.27

to �.00 on the DAS, from �.30 to �.12 on the IIP,

and from �.34 to �.20 on the SCL-90-R, and

became nonsignificant as a predictor in each of the

models, indicating that clients’ affect regulation at

the end of therapy fully mediates the relationship

between their level of experiencing midtherapy and

outcome on four of the five indices. Last, results

from the Sobel tests confirmed the statistical

significance of the mediational pathways found

through regression analyses.

Table III. Hierarchical Regression of Early�Late Affect Regulation Mediated by Peak Experiencing (N�66)

Equation IV DV b t p R2 F (df) Sobel test (p): ratio PM

1.1 Early O-MAR Peak EXP .318 2.681 .009 .10 7.187(1, 64)***

1.2 Early O-MAR Late O-MAR .375 3.238 .002 .14 10.484(1, 64)***

1.3 Early O-MAR Late O-MAR .220 2.064 .043

Peak EXP .487 4.564 .000 .35 17.280(2, 63)**** 2.304 (.021) .41

Note. IV, independent variable; DV, dependent variable; O-MAR, Observer-Rated Measure of Affect Regulation; peak EXP, highest peak

score for the working phase of therapy as measured by the Experiencing Scale; Ratio PM, proportion of total effect mediated.

***pB.01. ****pB.001.

92 J. C. Watson et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 9: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

Hierarchical Regression of Affect Regulation

and Alliance Predicting Outcome

Finally, to determine whether level of affect regula-

tion at the end of therapy predicted outcome over

and above the working alliance, clients’ mean work-

ing alliance score was entered into the regression for

each of the five outcome measures. Table V presents

the results of the regression analyses. The model

significantly predicted outcome on each outcome

measure at the end of therapy, including the BDI,

RSE, DAS, IIP, and SCL-90-R (Equations 1.1�5.3, respectively). Clients’ affect regulation score

late in therapy remained a significant predictor of

outcome on all the measures, and their mean work-

ing alliance scores significantly predicted outcome

independently on three of the outcome measures,

including the BDI, IIP, and SCL-90-R (Equations

1.3, 4.3, and 5.3, respectively) but was not a

significant predictor of RSE (Equation 2.3) and

DAS (Equation 3.3).

Discussion

The objectives of the current study were to examine

the relationships among clients’ capacity for affect

regulation at the beginning of therapy, their in-session

emotional processing, the working alliance, and their

affect regulation skills at the end of therapy as well as

changes on other outcome measures. Affect regula-

tion was defined as clients’ level of awareness of their

emotional experience, their capacity to modulate

both arousal and expression, their acceptance of their

emotional experience, and their capacity to reflect on

that experience. The results show that clients’ early

level of affect regulation predicted their level of

emotional processing during the early and working

phases of therapy. In addition, the quality and depth

of clients’ emotional processing in the session

mediated the relationship between their early level

of affect regulation and their level of affect regulation

at the end of therapy; and clients’ level of affect

regulation at the end of therapy mediated the relation-

ship between their peak level of emotional processing

in the working phase of therapy and outcome on all

the measures. In addition, clients’ level of affect

regulation at the end of therapy predicted outcome

independently of the working alliance. This highlights

the importance of specific change processes that are

independent of the quality of the working alliance that

develops between client and therapist to resolve

specific issues in therapy.

The findings suggest that clients’ initial capacity for

affect regulation influences how they engage with

their emotional experience in the session, and that the

quality of their emotional processing or engagement

in the session, particularly their capacity to reflect on

their emotional experience to resolve issues, predicts

changes in their capacity to regulate their affect at the

end of therapy as well as improvement in depression,

self-esteem, dysfunctional thoughts, interpersonal

problems, and the number of complaints and symp-

toms at the end of a short-term therapy for depres-

sion. These findings support those of a number of

studies showing that the depth of clients’ emotional

processing in session is related to positive outcome

(Castonguay et al., 1996; Pos et al., 2003; Watson &

Bedard, 2006). Recently, in a series of case studies

Table IV. Hierarchical Regression of Experiencing�Outcome Mediated by Affect Regulation (N�66)

Equation IV DV b t p R2 F (df) Sobel test (p): ratio PM

1.1 Peak EXP Late O-MAR .557 5.370 .000 .31 28.834(1, 64)****

1.2 Peak EXP BDI �.385 �3.334 .001 .15 11.118(1, 64)****

1.3 Peak EXP BDI �.155 �1.194 ns

Late O-MAR �.412 �3.163 .002 .27 11.345(2, 63)**** �2.725 (.006) .60

2.2 Peak EXP RSE .379 3.229 .002 .14 10.430(1, 62)***

2.3 Peak EXP RSE .106 0.819 ns

Late O-MAR .484 3.738 .000 .30 13.293(2, 61)**** 3.068 (.002) .72

3.2 Peak EXP DAS �.265 �2.150 .036 .07 4.622(1, 61)**

3.3 Peak EXP DAS �.004 �0.028 ns

Late O-MAR �.463 �3.342 .001 .22 8.282(2, 60)**** �2.837 (.005) .99

4.2 Peak EXP IIP �.299 �2.450 .017 .09 6.003(1, 61)**

4.3 Peak EXP IIP �.116 �0.806 ns

Late O-MAR �.324 �2.262 .027 .16 5.763(2, 60)**** �2.072 (.038) .61

5.2 Peak EXP SCL-90-R �.344 �2.885 .005 .12 8.322(1, 62)***

5.3 Peak EXP SCL-90-R �.201 �1.415 ns

Late O-MAR �.253 �1.782 ns (.08) .16 5.896(2, 61)*** �1.693 (.090) .42

Note. IV, independent variable; DV, dependent variable; peak EXP, highest peak score for the working phase of therapy as measured by the

Experiencing Scale; O-MAR, Observer-Rated Measure of Affect Regulation; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; RSE, Rosenberg Self-

Esteem Inventory; DAS, Dysfunction Attitudes Scale; IIP, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; SCL-90-R, Symptom Checklist-90-

Revised; Ratio PM, proportion of total effect mediated.

**pB.05. ***pB.01. ****pB.001.

Affect regulation, emotional processing, outcome 93

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 10: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

that examined good and poor outcome in PE-EFT,

Watson et al. (2007) observed that clients who did

not improve in short-term therapy had difficulty

becoming aware of, labeling, and modulating their

feelings. In contrast, good-outcome clients valued

their emotional experience and were able to reflect on

it and use it to resolve problematic issues in therapy.

Similarly, Quilty et al. (2008) found that openness to

experience as well as being innovative, curious, and

less neurotic were patient characteristics associated

with better treatment outcomes.

The findings suggest that clients who do well in

short-term treatments come into therapy with certain

emotional processing skills and capacities that are then

potentiated in the work that they do with their

therapists, resulting in significant changes. However,

it is important to attend to those clients who do not

enter therapy with the capacity to regulate their affect

as well or engage in deep emotional processing in order

to modify our treatment packages to take account of

their different needs and respond appropriately. If we

are able to identify clients’ difficulties early, then we

may be in a position to develop interventions that can

be implemented early in therapy to assist clients with

their emotional processing. For example, it may be

possible to teach clients emotional processing skills,

such as helping them become aware of their emotional

experience, and develop labels for it so that they can

then reflect on it to better modulate their arousal and

expression of emotion. One such task in PE-EFT is

focusing (Gendlin 1981, 1996). This was initially

developed by Gendlin (1964, 1974) to help clients

engage effectively in client-centered therapy. Helping

clients to become aware of and label their feelings is

important if they are to be able to reflect on them to

resolve problematic issues, identify hot cognitions, or

engage in certain interventions like two-chair or empty-

chair tasks in PE-EFT (Safran & Greenberg, 1982;

Watson et al., 2007). Other tasks like chair work help

clients develop skills related to emotional expression.

A strength of this study is that client self-report

measures of outcome and observer-rated measures

of affect regulation and emotional processing were

used to investigate the relationship between in-

session process, outcome, and the working alliance,

thereby limiting monomethod variance. Moreover,

the findings provide support that the O-MAR is

psychometrically sound, with adequate overall relia-

bility and excellent convergent, divergent, and pre-

dictive validity. As expected, facets of emotional

processing as measured by the O-MAR were corre-

lated with a measure of clients’ in-session emotional

processing. These findings speak to the robust

nature of both constructs and provide preliminary

construct validity for both measures. The findings

provide support for the divergent validity of the O-

MAR given that it is not related to the WAI. A

strength of the O-MAR is that it is rated by third-

party observers and thus allows research clinicians to

limit monomethod variance. Most of the measures

that have been developed to date to assess clients’

affect regulation are based on clients’ self-report,

Table V. Hierarchical Regression of Affect Regulation and Alliance Predicting Outcome (N�66)

Equation IV DV b t p R2 F (df)

1.1 O-MAR BDI �.498 �4.596 .000 .25 21.124(1, 64)****

1.2 WAI-S BDI �.491 �4.503 .000 .24 20.278(1, 64)****

1.3 O-MAR BDI �.384 �3.663 .001

WAI-S �.373 �3.558 .001 .37 18.816(2, 63)****

2.1 O-MAR RSE .554 5.104 .000 .30 26.054(1, 62)****

2.2 WAI-S RSE .352 2.906 .004 .12 8.763(1, 62)***

2.3 O-MAR RSE .480 4.194 .000

WAI-S .168 1.468 ns .32 14.348(2, 61)****

3.1 O-MAR DAS �.465 �4.104 .000 .22 16.839(1, 61)****

3.2 WAI-S DAS �.361 �3.024 .004 .13 9.146(1, 61)***

3.3 O-MAR DAS �.383 �3.176 .002

WAI-S �.214 �1.777 ns .26 10.297(2, 60)****

4.1 O-MAR IIP �.390 �3.307 .002 .15 10.938(1, 61)***

4.2 WAI-S IIP �.448 �3.917 .000 .20 15.344(1, 61)****

4.3 O-MAR IIP �.251 �2.058 .044

WAI-S �.348 �2.858 .006 .25 10.197 (2, 60)****

5.1 O-MAR SCL�90-R �.367 �3.104 .003 .13 9.364(1, 62)***

5.2 WAI-S SCL-90-R �.357 �3.010 .004 .13 9.061(1, 62)***

5.3 O-MAR SCL-90-R �.269 �2.158 .035

WAI-S �.254 �2.032 .046 .19 7.126(2, 61)***

Note. IV, independent variable; DV, dependent variable; WAI-S, Working Alliance Inventory; O-MAR, Observer-Rated Measure of Affect

Regulation; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; RSE, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory; DAS, Dysfunction Attitudes Scale; IIP, Inventory of

Interpersonal Problems; SCL-90-R, Symptom Checklist-90-Revised.

***pB.01. ****pB.001.

94 J. C. Watson et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 11: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

including the Scales for Experiencing Emotion (Behr

& Becker, 2002), the Trait Meta Mood Scale

(Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai,

1995), and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation

Scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004).

The development of process measures is useful and

necessary in psychotherapy research because they not

only provide a way of assessing clients’ characteristics

to establish their relationship with outcome but also are

excellent teaching tools and are clinically valuable in

assessing treatment interventions moment to moment

in therapy. Process measures can be used to identify

treatment markers, which can help clinicians tailor

their treatments to different clients so that they can be

optimally responsive to their clients moment to mo-

ment in the session. However, more work is required to

investigate the relevance of the O-MAR with different

client groups and to see whether the current findings

can be replicated. In future work it will be important to

investigate whether clients’ affect regulation capacities

continue to differentiate good from poor outcome and

to see whether people who enter therapy with lower

levels of affect regulation can improve with different

treatments or longer term therapy.

There are several limitations with the present study.

First, it is based on a small sample size. The findings

need to be replicated with a larger sample to see

whether the findings are generalizable. Second, the

present sample consisted of a depressed population

and was relatively homogenous; thus, the results may

not be generalizable to other populations (e.g., clients

suffering form anxiety or addictive behaviors). Third,

it was only possible to rate a limited number of sessions

for each client, so the picture that emerges may not be

representative of each client’s performance in every

session. Fourth, it would be important given the ratio

of women to men (2:1) to try to increase the number of

men in future samples to see whether there are

differences between men and women in terms of their

capacities to regulate their affect overall and whether

there are differences in terms of the quality of their

emotional processing during in the session.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Social Sciences and

Humanities Research Council of Canada Research

Grant 410-2005-0222 to Jeanne C. Watson. The

authors thank the reviewers for their comments on

an earlier draft of this article.

References

Bachman, J., & O’Malley, P. (1977). Self-esteem in young men: A

longitudinal analysis of the impact of educational and occupa-

tional attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

35, 365�380.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator

variable distinction in social psychological research: Concep-

tual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Person-

ality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173�1182.

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).

Cognitive therapy of depression: A treatment manual. New York:

Guilford Press.

Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1984). Internal consistencies of the

original and revised Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 40, 1365�1367.

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.

(1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 4, 561�571.

Behr, M., & Becker, M. (2002). Congruence and experiencing

emotions: Self-report scales for the person-centered and

experiential theory of personality. In J. C. Watson, R. N.

Goldman & M. S. Warner (Eds.), Client-centered and experiential

psychotherapy in the 21st century: Advances in theory, research and

practice (pp. 150�167). Herefordshire, UK: PCCS Books.

Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., Oro’-Beutler, M. E., Daldrup, R., &

Meredith, K. (1986). Inability to express intense affect: A

common link between depression and pain? Journal of Consult-

ing and Clinical Psychology, 54, 752�759.

Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic

concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re-

search, and Practice, 16, 252�260.

Bradley, S. (2000). Affect regulation and the development of

psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press.

Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S., Raue, P. J., &

Hayes, A. M. (1996). Predicting outcome in cognitive therapy

for depression: A comparison of unique and common factors.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 497�504.

Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb

for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instru-

ments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6, 284�290.

Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Roch, A. F. (1976). The SCL-90

and the MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report

scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280�289.

Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S.

(2003). Learning emotion-focused therapy: The process-experiential

approach to change. Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Gendlin, E. T. (1964). A theory of personality change. In P.

Worchel & D. Byrne (Eds.), Personality change (pp. 100�148).

New York: Wiley.

Gendlin, E. T. (1974). Client-centered and experiential psy-

chotherapy. In D. Wexler & L. Rice (Eds.), Innovations in

client-centered therapy (pp. 211�246). Toronto: Wiley.

Gendlin, E. T. (1981). Focusing. New York: Bantam Books.

Gendlin, E. T. (1996). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy. New York:

Guilford Press.

Giyaur, K., Sharf, J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2005). The Capacity for

Dynamic Process Scale (CDPS) and patient engagement in

opiate addiction treatment. Journal of Nervous and Mental

Disease, 193, 833�838.

Godfrey, E., Chalder, T., Ridsdale, L., Seed, P., & Ogden, J.

(2007). Investigating the ‘‘active ingredients’’ of cognitive

behaviour therapy and counselling for patients with chronic

fatigue in primary care: Developing a new process measure to

assess treatment fidelity and predict outcome. British Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 46, 253�272.

Gratz, K., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of

emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor

structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion

Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral

Assessment, 26, 41�54.

Affect regulation, emotional processing, outcome 95

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 12: An examination of the relationships among clients' affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome

Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating

emotional change: The moment-by-moment process. New York:

Guilford Press.

Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy:

Affect, cognition, and the process of change. New York: Guilford

Press.

Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2006). Process-experiential

psychotherapy in the treatment of depression. Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J. C., & Goldman, R. (1998). Process-

experiential therapy for depression. In G. Lietaer (Ed.), Hand-

book of experiential psychotherapy (pp. 227�248). New York:

Guilford Press.

Hansen, N. B., & Lambert, M. J. (1997). Clinical significance: An

overview of methods. Journal of Mental Health, 5, 17�24.

Hardy, G. E., Cahill, J., Shapiro, D. A., Barkham, M., Rees, A., &

Macaskill, N. (2001). Client interpersonal and cognitive styles

as predictors of response to time-limited cognitive therapy for

depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69,

841�845.

Hersoug, A. G., Høglend, P., Havik, O. E., von der Lippe, A., &

Monsen, J. T. (2009). Pretreatment patient characteristics

related to the level and development of working alliance in

long-term psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 172�180.

Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureno, G., &

Villasenor, V. S. (1988). Inventory of Interpersonal Problems:

Psychometric properties and clinical applications. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 885�892.

Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1986). The development of

the Working Alliance Inventory. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M.

Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook

(pp. 529�556). New York: Guilford Press.

Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and

validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 36, 223�233.

Joyce, A. S., Piper, W. E., Ogrodniczuk, J. S., & Klein, R. H.

(2007). Patient characteristics and variations in termination

processes and outcomes. Washington, DC: American Psychologi-

cal Association.

Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing emotion:

Myths, realities, and therapeutic strategies. New York: Guilford

Press.

Klein, M. H., Mathieu, P., Gendlin, E. T., & Kiesler, D. J. (1969).

The Experiencing Scale: A research and training manual. Madison:

University of Wisconsin, Bureau of Audio Visual Research.

Klein, M. H., Mathieu-Couglan, P., & Kiesler, D. J. (1986). The

Experiencing Scales. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.),

The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 21�71).

New York: Guilford Press.

Kuiper, N. A., & Olinger, L. J. (1989). Stress and cognitive

vulnerability for depression: A self-worth contingency model.

In R. W. J. Neufeld (Ed.), Advances in the investigation of

psychological stress (pp. 367�391). Oxford, UK: Wiley.

Leahy, R. L. (2002). A model of emotional schemas. Cognitive and

Behavioral Practice, 9, 177�190.

McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about

some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods,

1, 30�46.

Ogles, B. M., Lambert, M. J., & Sawyer, J. D. (1995). Clinical

significance of the National Institute of Mental Health Treat-

ment of Depression Collaborative Research Program data.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 321�326.

Ogrodniczuk, J. S., Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., McCallum, M., &

Rosie, J. (2003). NEO-Five Factor personality traits as pre-

dictors of response to two forms of group psychotherapy.

International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 53, 417�442.

Pos, A. E., Greenberg, L. S., Goldman, R. N., & Korman, L. M.

(2003). Emotional processing during experiential treatment of

depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71,

1007�1016.

Prosser, M. C., & Watson, J. C. (2007, June). Beyond rapport: How

therapist empathy contributes to outcome in the treatment of

depression. Paper presented at the 38th Annual Meeting of the

Society for Psychotherapy Research, Madison, Wisconsin, USA.

Quilty, L. C., De Fruyt, F., Rolland, J. P., Kennedy, S. H.,

Rouillon, P. F., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Dimensional person-

ality traits and treatment outcome in patients with major

depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 108, 241�250.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self image.

Princeton, NJ, USA: Princeton University Press.

Safran, J. D., & Greenberg, L. S. (1982). Eliciting ‘‘hot cogni-

tions’’ in cognitive therapy. Canadian Psychology, 23, 83�87.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T.

P. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring

emotional intelligence using Trait Meta Mood Scale. In J.

Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125�154).

New York: Bantam Books.

Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses

in assessing reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420�428.

Spitzer, R., Williams, J., Gibbon, M., & First, M. (1995).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. New York: American

Psychiatric Press Inc.

Stanton, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., Cameron, C. L., Bishop, M. M.,

Collins, C. A., Kirk, S. B., et al. (2000). Emotionally expressive

coping predicts psychological and physical adjustment to breast

cancer. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 875�882.

Stice, E. (1999). Clinical implications of psychosocial research on

bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 55, 675�683.

Watson, J. C. (2007). Reassessing Roger’ necessary and sufficient

conditions of change. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,

Training, 44, 268�273.

Watson, J. C., & Bedard, D. (2006). Clients’ emotional processing

in psychotherapy: A comparison between cognitive-behavioral

and process-experiential psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 74, 152�159.

Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Case

studies in the experiential treatment of depression: A comparison of

good and bad outcome. Washington, DC: American Psychologi-

cal Association.

Watson, J. C., Gordon, L. B., Stermac, L., Kalogerakos, F., &

Steckley, P. (2003). Comparing the effectiveness of process�experiential with cognitive�behavioral psychotherapy in the

treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 71, 773�781.

Watson, J. C., & Prosser, M. (2004). Observer-rated Measure of

Affect Regulation (O-MAR). Unpublished manuscript, Ontario

Institute for Studies in Education of the University of Toronto,

Toronto.

Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978, August). Development and

validation of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale: A preliminary

investigation. Paper presented at the 86th Annual Convention of

the American Psychological Association, Toronto.

Whelton, W. J. (2004). Emotional processes in psychotherapy:

Evidence across therapeutic modalities. Clinical Psychology and

Psychotherapy, 11, 58�71.

Zuroff, D. C., Blatt, S. J., Sotsky, S. M., Krupnick, J. L., Martin,

D. J., Sanislow, C. A., III, et al. (2000). Relation of therapeutic

alliance and perfectionism to outcome in brief outpatient

treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 68, 114�114.

96 J. C. Watson et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

uckl

and

Lib

rary

] at

16:

02 0

1 D

ecem

ber

2014