anchoring and consistency (keith dowd)

1
Anchoring and Consistency: A New Metacognitive Anchoring and Consistency: A New Metacognitive Approach to Understanding the Anchoring-and- Approach to Understanding the Anchoring-and- Adjustment Effect Adjustment Effect Keith W. Dowd John V. Keith W. Dowd John V. Petrocelli Petrocelli Wake Forest University Wake Forest University Wake Forest University Wake Forest University ABSTRACT ABSTRACT The purpose of the present research is to test the hypothesis that a metacognitive consistency factor is a mechanism that drives the anchoring- and-adjustment heuristic. In a preliminary study, we asked participants to respond to a series of trivia questions requiring them to generate estimates based on a) their prior knowledge (self-generated anchor; SGA) and b) a value that functions as a marker to adjust upward or downwards from (experimenter-provided anchor; EPA). We predicted that one’s final estimate would be adjusted away from the EPA when the consistency between the SGA and the EPA is low. In addition, we hypothesized that SGAs play an important role when estimating unknown quantities and that participants would be aware of their SGAs, regardless of whether they were consciously instructed to report them. Support was found for both of our hypotheses which suggests that consistency may play an important role in the cognitive processes underlying the anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic. INTRODUCTION Anchoring-and-adjustment refers to a cognitive heuristic used in the estimation process whereby people attempt to adjust their estimation from an accessible starting value until an acceptable estimate is reached; often the adjustment is insufficient (Kahneman & Tversky, 1974). Previous research (who? Cite them) has demonstrated it to be robust in and outside of the laboratory and has implicated it as one explanation for numerical judgments and estimates of future performance. Despite numerous studies chronicling its effect, little research has explored the nature of the cognitive processes underlying its operation. Theoretical and Conceptual Framework Epley & Gilovich (2007) suggested that some types of anchors (i.e., SGA) activate processes of anchoring- and-adjustment whereas others do not (EPA). They argued that semantic priming is occurring in the case of EPAs rather than anchoring-and-adjustment. We propose that adjustment does occur with EPAs but that it does so in conjunction with SGAs and is driven by a metacognitive consistency factor between the two types of anchors. Because people also have the ability to think more deeply about their initial thoughts, we suggest that a metacognitive view (Nelson & Narens, 1994) of anchoring-and-adjustment may be fruitful to our understanding of its operation. Goal • To test the hypothesis that consistency between EXPERIMENT HYPOTHESES 1. We predicted that final estimates would be associated greater adjustment away from the EPA when the consistency between the SGA and the EPA is low. 2. We also predicated that SGAs operate to influence final estimates regardless of whether SGAs are consciously reported or not. PROCEDURE • Participants (N=84) completed 12 item sets • The question used in each item set was constructed such that participants would not know the correct answer • Randomly assigned to 1 of 6 conditions that varied on: • The experimenter-provided anchors provided (high v. low) • The request to self-generate (presence v. absence) • The location of the request to self-generate (first v. last) • Each item set consisted of 4 sequential parts: 1. Presentation of the question and self-generation of internal anchor (SGA) 2. Presentation of the experimenter-provided anchor (EPA) 3. Presentation of the comparative assessment question (CA) 4. Request for final estimate (FE) • 6 of the items served as filler questions Dependent variable: Amount of adjustment in FE from EPA SGA SGA FE FE EPA EPA CA CA Conditions 1 & 2 SGA SGA FE FE EPA EPA CA CA Conditions 3 & 4 SGA SGA FE FE EPA EPA CA CA Conditions 5 & 6 RESULTS Average correlation between consistency and adjustment = -.xx (all ps <.0?) • Average correct detection of high/low EPAs = xx% • All 6 of our items evidenced either a significant main effect for consistency or an interaction between the experimenter-provided anchors and consistency, even after controlling for participants’ self-generated anchors as a covariate . Evidence suggests that SGA are generated spontaneously (no difference in total adjustment between conditions 3/4 and conditions 1/2 and 5/6). • The temporal location in the items sets of the request for participants to form self-generated anchors did not DISCUSSION The results of our experiment suggest two ways in which a metacognitive process may operate when people respond to classic anchoring questions. One is the pattern of correct CA judgments made by participants; that is, participants were largely accurate in determining whether or not the EPA was high or low. This finding suggests that they are not only using a self-generated standard of comparison (e.g., SGA) but also actively thinking about it when making estimates about unknown qualities. The other way in which our data support a metacognitive process comes from the recurrent interaction that occurred between consistency and EPAs. Participants appear to be consciously reflecting on the consistency between their SGAs and the EPAs in order to a render a final estimate. Our data also speaks to the spontaneity of SGAs. That is, these estimates were generated by participants regardless of whether or not they were instructed to explicitly report them; which suggests that they play an important role in the operation of the anchoring-and- adjustment heuristic because they provide a standard of comparison to contrast against EPAs. The consistency between these two anchors moderates the amount of adjustment in FEs and without the contribution of both of them the anchoring effect may fail to operate. Figure 1 Adjustment from experiment-provided anchor means by experiment- provided anchors and consistency level between self-generated and experiment-provided anchors. U N I V E R S I T Y WAKE FOREST

Upload: abramrickards

Post on 06-Aug-2015

202 views

Category:

Education


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Anchoring and Consistency (Keith Dowd)

Anchoring and Consistency: A New Metacognitive Approach to Anchoring and Consistency: A New Metacognitive Approach to Understanding the Anchoring-and-Adjustment EffectUnderstanding the Anchoring-and-Adjustment EffectKeith W. Dowd John V. PetrocelliKeith W. Dowd John V. Petrocelli Wake Forest University Wake Forest UniversityWake Forest University Wake Forest University

ABSTRACTABSTRACT

The purpose of the present research is to test the hypothesis that

a metacognitive consistency factor is a mechanism that drives the

anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic. In a preliminary study, we

asked participants to respond to a series of trivia questions

requiring them to generate estimates based on a) their prior

knowledge (self-generated anchor; SGA) and b) a value that

functions as a marker to adjust upward or downwards from

(experimenter-provided anchor; EPA). We predicted that one’s

final estimate would be adjusted away from the EPA when the

consistency between the SGA and the EPA is low. In addition,

we hypothesized that SGAs play an important role when

estimating unknown quantities and that participants would be

aware of their SGAs, regardless of whether they were

consciously instructed to report them. Support was found for both

of our hypotheses which suggests that consistency may play an

important role in the cognitive processes underlying the

anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic.

INTRODUCTION

Anchoring-and-adjustment refers to a cognitive heuristic used in the

estimation process whereby people attempt to adjust their estimation

from an accessible starting value until an acceptable estimate is

reached; often the adjustment is insufficient (Kahneman & Tversky,

1974).

Previous research (who? Cite them) has demonstrated it to be robust

in and outside of the laboratory and has implicated it as one

explanation for numerical judgments and estimates of future

performance. Despite numerous studies chronicling its effect, little

research has explored the nature of the cognitive processes

underlying its operation.

Theoretical and Conceptual Framework

Epley & Gilovich (2007) suggested that some types of anchors (i.e.,

SGA) activate processes of anchoring-and-adjustment whereas others

do not (EPA). They argued that semantic priming is occurring in the

case of EPAs rather than anchoring-and-adjustment. We propose that

adjustment does occur with EPAs but that it does so in conjunction

with SGAs and is driven by a metacognitive consistency factor

between the two types of anchors.

Because people also have the ability to think more deeply about their

initial thoughts, we suggest that a metacognitive view (Nelson &

Narens, 1994) of anchoring-and-adjustment may be fruitful to our

understanding of its operation.

Goal• To test the hypothesis that consistency between SGAs and EPAs

moderates the degree of adjustment represented in final estimates

from SGAs after being exposed to the EPAs.

EXPERIMENT

HYPOTHESES

1. We predicted that final estimates would be associated greater

adjustment away from the EPA when the consistency between the SGA

and the EPA is low.

2. We also predicated that SGAs operate to influence final estimates

regardless of whether SGAs are consciously reported or not.

PROCEDURE

• Participants (N=84) completed 12 item sets• The question used in each item set was constructed such that

participants would not know the correct answer• Randomly assigned to 1 of 6 conditions that varied on:

• The experimenter-provided anchors provided (high v. low)• The request to self-generate (presence v. absence)• The location of the request to self-generate (first v. last)

• Each item set consisted of 4 sequential parts:

1. Presentation of the question and self-generation of internal anchor

(SGA)

2. Presentation of the experimenter-provided anchor (EPA)

3. Presentation of the comparative assessment question (CA)

4. Request for final estimate (FE)• 6 of the items served as filler questions• Dependent variable: Amount of adjustment in FE from EPA

SGASGA FEFEEPAEPA CACA

Conditions 1 & 2

SGASGA FEFEEPAEPA CACA

Conditions 3 & 4

SGASGAFEFEEPAEPA CACA

Conditions 5 & 6

RESULTS

• Average correlation between consistency and adjustment = -.xx (all ps <.0?)• Average correct detection of high/low EPAs = xx%• All 6 of our items evidenced either a significant main effect for consistency or

an interaction between the experimenter-provided anchors and consistency,

even after controlling for participants’ self-generated anchors as a covariate.

• Evidence suggests that SGA are generated spontaneously (no difference in

total adjustment between conditions 3/4 and conditions 1/2 and 5/6).

• The temporal location in the items sets of the request for participants to form

self-generated anchors did not have a significant effect on their final estimates.

DISCUSSION

The results of our experiment suggest two ways in which a metacognitive

process may operate when people respond to classic anchoring questions.

One is the pattern of correct CA judgments made by participants; that is,

participants were largely accurate in determining whether or not the EPA

was high or low. This finding suggests that they are not only using a self-

generated standard of comparison (e.g., SGA) but also actively thinking

about it when making estimates about unknown qualities.

The other way in which our data support a metacognitive process comes

from the recurrent interaction that occurred between consistency and EPAs.

Participants appear to be consciously reflecting on the consistency between

their SGAs and the EPAs in order to a render a final estimate.

Our data also speaks to the spontaneity of SGAs. That is, these estimates

were generated by participants regardless of whether or not they were

instructed to explicitly report them; which suggests that they play an

important role in the operation of the anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic

because they provide a standard of comparison to contrast against EPAs.

The consistency between these two anchors moderates the amount of

adjustment in FEs and without the contribution of both of them the anchoring

effect may fail to operate.

Finally, these data build on the findings of Epley & Gilovitch (2007) because

they provide further evidence for the importance of SGAs for the anchoring

heuristic while also offering a preliminary process account for its operation.

Figure 1

Adjustment from experiment-provided anchor means by experiment-provided anchors and

consistency level between self-generated and experiment-provided anchors.

U N I V E R S I T Y

WAKE FOREST