assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its...

28
Behavioral Sciences and the Law Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53–80 (2001) Assessment of ‘‘Juvenile Psychopathy’’ and Its Association with Violence: A Critical Review John F. Edens, Ph.D.,* Jennifer L. Skeem, Ph.D., 1 Keith R. Cruise, Ph.D., 2 and Elizabeth Cauffman, Ph.D. 3 Interest in the construct of psychopathy as it applies to children and adolescents has become an area of consider- able research interest in the past 5–10 years, in part due to the clinical utility of psychopathy as a predictor of violence among adult offenders. Despite interest in ‘‘juvenile psy- chopathy’’ in general and its relationship to violence in particular, relatively few studies specifically have exam- ined whether operationalizations of this construct among children and adolescents predict various forms of aggres- sion. This article critically reviews this literature, as well as controversies regarding the assessment of adult psycho- pathic ‘‘traits’’ among juveniles. Existing evidence indi- cates a moderate association between measures of psychopathy and various forms of aggression, suggesting that this construct may be relevant for purposes of short- term risk appraisal and management among juveniles. However, due to the enormous developmental changes that occur during adolescence and the absence of long- itudinal research on the stability of this construct (and its association with violence), we conclude that reliance on psychopathy measures to make decisions regarding long- term placements for juveniles is contraindicated at this time. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. The association between psychopathy and violence has been well established over the past 20 years by an impressive body of outcome studies examining adult samples (for reviews, see Hare, 1991; Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998; Salekin, Rogers, & Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. *Correspondence to: John F. Edens, Department of Pyschology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State University, Huntsville, TX 77341-2447, USA. E-mail: [email protected] 1 Jennifer L. Skeem, Ph.D., Law and Psychiatry Research Program, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh. 2 Keith R. Cruise, Ph.D., Department of Psychology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State University. 3 Elizabeth Cauffman, Ph.D., Law and Psychiatry Research Program, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh. Portions of this paper previously were presented as part of a symposium at the 2000 convention of the American Society of Criminology, San Francisco, CA.

Upload: others

Post on 24-Aug-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Behavioral Sciences and the Law

Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Assessment of `̀ JuvenilePsychopathy'' and ItsAssociation with Violence:A Critical Review

John F. Edens, Ph.D.,* Jennifer L.Skeem, Ph.D.,1 Keith R. Cruise, Ph.D.,2 andElizabeth Cauffman, Ph.D.3

Interest in the construct of psychopathy as it applies to

children and adolescents has become an area of consider-

able research interest in the past 5±10 years, in part due to

the clinical utility of psychopathy as a predictor of violence

among adult offenders. Despite interest in `̀ juvenile psy-

chopathy'' in general and its relationship to violence in

particular, relatively few studies speci®cally have exam-

ined whether operationalizations of this construct among

children and adolescents predict various forms of aggres-

sion. This article critically reviews this literature, as well

as controversies regarding the assessment of adult psycho-

pathic `̀ traits'' among juveniles. Existing evidence indi-

cates a moderate association between measures of

psychopathy and various forms of aggression, suggesting

that this construct may be relevant for purposes of short-

term risk appraisal and management among juveniles.

However, due to the enormous developmental changes

that occur during adolescence and the absence of long-

itudinal research on the stability of this construct (and its

association with violence), we conclude that reliance on

psychopathy measures to make decisions regarding long-

term placements for juveniles is contraindicated at this

time. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

The association between psychopathy and violence has been well established over

the past 20 years by an impressive body of outcome studies examining adult samples

(for reviews, see Hare, 1991; Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998; Salekin, Rogers, &

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

*Correspondence to: John F. Edens, Department of Pyschology and Philosophy, Sam Houston StateUniversity, Huntsville, TX 77341-2447, USA. E-mail: [email protected] L. Skeem, Ph.D., Law and Psychiatry Research Program, Western Psychiatric Institute andClinic, University of Pittsburgh.2Keith R. Cruise, Ph.D., Department of Psychology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State University.3Elizabeth Cauffman, Ph.D., Law and Psychiatry Research Program, Western Psychiatric Institute andClinic, University of Pittsburgh.Portions of this paper previously were presented as part of a symposium at the 2000 convention of theAmerican Society of Criminology, San Francisco, CA.

Page 2: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Sewell, 1996). Interest in the construct of psychopathy as it applies to children and

adolescents has become an area of considerable research interest in the past 5±10

years, due in part to the strength of the relationship between psychopathy and

violence among adult offenders. Despite the interest in `̀ juvenile psychopathy'' in

general and its relationship to violence in particular, relatively few studies speci®-

cally have examined whether this construct is associated with increased rates of

aggression and violence among children and adolescents. More generally, given that

psychopathy typically is construed as a relatively static personality disposition

(see, e.g., Monahan & Steadman, 1994; Quinsey, Harris, Rice, & Cormier, 1998),

attempts to apply this construct to juvenile populations raise several conceptual,

methodological, and practical concerns related to clinical/forensic practice and

juvenile/criminal justice policy.

This paper provides a critical review of the juvenile psychopathy literature, with

particular attention given to research examining the relationship between psycho-

pathy and violence. We begin with a brief overview of the construct of psychopathy

as it typically has been conceptualized among adults. Next, controversies regarding

the applicability of certain psychopathic `̀ traits'' to children and adolescents are

reviewed, focusing on the potential age -inappropriateness of some of these features

(e.g., parasitic lifestyle, irresponsibility, impulsivity), given their likely long-term

instability. After reviewing several recently developed measures that purport to

operationalize psychopathy speci®cally for juvenile populations, we summarize the

existing literature examining the association between juvenile psychopathy and

various forms of aggression, focusing in detail on those studies that address the

prediction of future criminal violence. We conclude with a discussion of the clinical,

juvenile justice, and public policy implications of this research, particularly as

they relate to potential misuses of this construct when conducting juvenile risk

assessments.

ADULT PSYCHOPATHY

Psychopathy as it is conceptualized among adults represents a distinct cluster of

affective, interpersonal and behavioral characteristics (Hare, 1991). In perhaps

the most in¯uential conceptualization of this syndrome, Cleckley (1941) described

the prototypical psychopath as an individual who was super®cially charming and

intelligent, but who also was insincere and untruthful, egocentric, and lacking in

remorse and shame. Psychopaths were characterized as engaging in `̀ fantastic and

uninviting behavior,'' having impersonal sex lives, failing to follow any life plan, and

being prone to `̀ inadequately motivated antisocial behavior.'' However, they gen-

erally were presumed to lack other signs of mental disorder, such as `̀ neurotic''

anxiety or delusions or other `̀ irrational'' thinking.

Cleckley's (1941) description of psychopathy has been reformulated into what

over the past several years has become the gold standard for assessing this syndrome

in forensic and correctional settings, the Psychopathy Checklist ± Revised (PCL-R;

Hare, 1991). The PCL-R consists of 20 items (see Table 1) that are intended to

correspond to Cleckley's original conceptualization of this syndrome. However,

because it initially was developed and validated on (primarily Caucasian male)

forensic and correctional samples, the PCL-R (and its predecessor, the PCL [Hare,

54 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 3: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

1980]) formulation of psychopathy is weighted much more heavily with items

related to criminality and a socially deviant lifestyle (e.g., juvenile delinquency,

revocation of conditional release, criminal versatility) than was Cleckley's original set

of descriptors (see Rogers, 1995).1

Part of the resurgent interest in psychopathy over the past few years is attributable

to the documented relationship between the PCL/PCL-R and antisocial behavior,

particularly future violence among released criminal offenders (Hare, 1991;

Hemphill et al., 1998; Salekin et al., 1996). For example, Hemphill et al. reported

average correlations of .27, .27, and .23 between the PCL measures and general

recidivism, violent recidivism, and sexual recidivism, respectively, across several

large-scale follow-up studies. Psychopathic individuals (i.e., those obtaining PCL-R

scores�30) were approximately four times as likely to commit a future violent crime

than were non-psychopathic offenders. Moreover, the PCL/PCL-R has been found

in many studies to perform as well as (and in some cases better than) statistically

derived actuarial measures designed speci®cally to predict future violence

(Hemphill et al., 1998; Serin, Peters, & Barbaree, 1990; cf. Quinsey et al., 1998).

Table 1. Items comprising the Psychopathy Checklist ± Revised and its factors

Factor

Item Interpersonal/Affective Socially Deviant Lifestyle Total score only

1. Glibness/superficial charm

2. Grandiose sense of self-worth

3. Need for stimulation/

proneness to boredom

4. Pathological lying

5. Conning/manipulative

6. Lack of remorse or guilt

7. Shallow affect

8. Callous/lack of empathy

9. Parasitic lifestyle

10. Poor behavioral controls

11. Promiscuous sexual

behavior

12. Early behavior problems

13. Lack of realistic

long-term goals

14. Impulsivity

15. Irresponsibility

16. Failure to accept

responsibility for actions

17. Many short-term

marital relationships

18. Juvenile delinquency

19. Revocation of

conditional release

20. Criminal versatility

1A screening version of the PCL-R also has been published (PCL:SV; Hart, Cox, & Hare, 1995) that hasshown considerable promise as a measure of psychopathic features in non-correctional settings.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 55

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 4: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Although often treated as a relatively homogeneous construct, factor analyses of

the PCL-R historically have identi®ed two separate, but highly correlated (r � .50)

factors: factor I re¯ects the core affective and interpersonal features (e.g., callous-

ness, egocentricity, super®cial charm, shallow affect) of psychopathy, and factor II

represents the chronic unstable and antisocial lifestyle (e.g., irresponsibility, bore-

dom proneness, parasitic lifestyle, impulsivity) associated with this construct.2

These two factors have shown differential correlations with criterion measures

across several domains, with the Interpersonal/Affective factor (factor I) being

associated more strongly with other theoretically consistent personality features

(e.g., narcissism, dependence [inverse]), and the Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor

(factor II) tending to correlate more highly with behavioral indices of a chronically

unstable and antisocial lifestyle (e.g., poor school performance, lower socioeco-

nomic status, substance abuse, Antisocial Personality Disorder criteria).

In terms of violence prediction, there has been some controversy in the literature

regarding the relative utility of the Interpersonal/Affective versus Socially Deviant

Lifestyle factors. Some reviewers (e.g., Quinsey et al., 1998; Salekin et al., 1996)

have questioned the relationship between the Interpersonal/Affective factor and

violence and have suggested that the Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor may be more

in¯uential in determining levels of risk. Based on their review of published and

unpublished studies, Hemphill et al. (1998) concluded that, although the Socially

Deviant Lifestyle factor appears to be a stronger predictor of general recidivism,

both factors contribute equally to the prediction of future violence. This conclusion

for violent recidivism, however, was based on only three studies in which no

signi®cant differences were obtained in terms of the relative magnitude of the

correlations between the two factors and violent recidivism. Other studies not

included in the Hemphill et al. review have been more equivocal about the relative

association between the Interpersonal/Affective factor and violence (see, generally,

Salekin et al., 1996).

Related to the controversy surrounding the incremental predictive validity of

the two PCL-R factors is recent criticism of the two-factor conceptualization of

psychopathy itself. Based on a comprehensive review of studies that have examined

the factor structure of the PCL-R, Cooke and Michie (in press) have called into

question the psychometric adequacy of this model, arguing that a more rigorous

analysis of the results of these studies does not offer strong support for the two-factor

conceptualization of psychopathy. Based on re-analyses of several large data sets

(n� 2,000 North American and 600 Scottish forensic and correctional participants),

they proposed a new, three-factor, hierarchical model in which what originally was

de®ned as factor I (Interpersonal/Affective) is now conceptualized as two distinct

factors, termed `̀ Arrogant and Deceitful Interpersonal Style'' (comprising PCL-R

items 1, 2, 4 and 5; see Table 1) and `̀ De®cient Affective Experience'' (comprising

items 6, 7, 8, and 16). Cooke and Michie (in press) also have suggested modi®ca-

tions to factor II (Socially Deviant Lifestyle) involving the deletion of various

behavioral items (i.e. elimination of items 10, 12, 18, and 19). Based on their

2Various labels have been applied to these two factors by authors from divergent theoretical perspectives.For the sake of simplicity, we refer to factor I as the `̀ Interpersonal/Affective'' factor and factor II as the`̀ Socially Deviant Lifestyle'' factor throughout this article. These descriptive labels are not meant to re¯ectendorsement of any particular theoretical model, however.

56 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 5: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

factor-analytic results, as well as item response theory analyses (Cooke &

Michie, 1997), they have argued that these items are not strong indicators of the

underlying construct assessed by this scale.3 The revised Socially Deviant Lifestyle

factor, which they termed `̀ Impulsive and Irresponsible Behavioral Style,'' is

comprised of PCL-R items 3, 9, 13, 14, and 15. To date no published studies

have examined the extent to which these factors may be related differentially to

external correlates (e.g., violence), although future research in this area clearly

seems warranted.

In summary, although conceptual questions remain regarding its factor structure,

among adult male samples the PCL/PCL-R has been shown to be a robust predictor

of criminal behavior generally and violence speci®cally. This association, in con-

junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are likely

to commit future violence (Cottle, Lee, & Heilbrun, 1999; Hawkins et al., 2000;

Zinger & Forth, 1998), has sparked increasing interest in the concept of `̀ juvenile

psychopathy.'' However, in the mental health ®eld, adult diagnoses or concepts

periodically have been applied to children and/or adolescents without taking into

account important developmental factors that may affect the applicability or

validity of these constructs (for a review, see Achenbach, 1995). In the following

section, we discuss developmental considerations related to the application of the

adult psychopathy construct to children and adolescents and identify speci®c

features that warrant close scrutiny in terms of their utility as markers of `̀ juvenile

psychopathy.''

ARE ADULT FEATURES OF PSYCHOPATHY

APPLICABLE TO JUVENILES?

The construct of psychopathy is widely (if not universally) construed as a stable

personality disposition (Monahan & Steadman, 1994; Quinsey et al., 1998).

Traditionally, diagnoses of personality disorders are applied to children and

adolescents only `̀ in those relatively unusual instances in which the individual's

particular maladaptive personality traits appear to be pervasive, persistent, and

unlikely to be limited to a particular developmental stage'' (American Psychiatric

Association, 1994, p. 631, emphasis added). Some authors have argued that

characteristics of psychopathy may be evident as early as childhood (e.g., Lynam,

1996).4 However, evidence supporting the claim that psychopathic traits ± presum-

ing they can be assessed reliably among juveniles ± represent a stable personality

pattern that continues into early adulthood is only indirect at present (see, e.g., Alm

et al., 1996a, 1996b; Harpur & Hare, 1994; af Klinteberg, Humble, & Schalling,

1992). Adolescence is a time of enormous developmental change, during

3Elimination of child/adolescent behavior problem items from the PCL-R is a controversial position, inthat Quinsey et al. (1998) have argued that these are central to the construct of psychopathy and account formuch of the variance in the prediction of violent recidivism that is associated with PCL-R scores amongadult offenders.4Lynam (1996) has argued that psychopathy can be construed as an information processing de®cit, thebehavioral expression of which differs across different ages ± hyperactivity/impulsivity in childhood;conduct disorder in adolescence; and psychopathy in adults.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 57

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 6: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

which some juveniles engage in considerable delinquent behavior of a transient

nature (see Mof®t's (1993) distinction between `̀ life course persistent'' and `̀ ado-

lescent limited'' antisocial behavior). Because of this, demonstration of the stability

of these traits is pivotal to evaluating the ecological validity of `̀ juvenile psycho-

pathy.''

Adolescents often are presumed to manifest psychopathy in much the same way

as adults (see, Forth, Kosson, & Hare, in press; Forth & Mailloux, 2000). However,

many of the PCL-R markers of psychopathy seem inapplicable to, or inappropriate

for, adolescents. The inapplicability of a few PCL-R items has been apparent from

the ®rst attempts to apply them to a juvenile population. Early adaptations of the

PCL-R for use with adolescents or `̀ youthful offenders'' excluded the parasiticlifestyle and many short-term marital relationships items because adolescents, particu-

larly young ones, have limited work and relationship experiences upon which to base

scoring of these items (Forth, Hart, & Hare, 1990; see also Poythress, Edens &

Lilienfeld, 1998, who also eliminated revocation of conditional release and criminalversatility).

Aside from these more obviously developmentally inappropriate items, several

additional groups of items included in the PCL-R also may be problematic when

applied to adolescents, particularly juvenile delinquents. For each of these items,

there are currently little data available for determining whether an adolescent (a)

exhibits signi®cantly `̀ more'' of the trait or behavior than normative peer groups,

and (b) will continue to manifest these traits and behaviors in a temporally and

situationally stable manner into adulthood.

Most of the PCL-R item groups that may not be age appropriate for adolescents

are from the Socially Deviant Lifestyle (II) factor. The ®rst item group includes needfor stimulation/proneness to boredom, impulsivity, and poor behavioral controls. Some

evidence suggests that sensation and thrill seeking may be stable from childhood to

mid-adolescence, increase from mid- to late adolescence (approximately ages 16±

19), and then decline over the course of adulthood (Giambra, Camp, & Grodsky,

1992; Zuckerman, Eysenck, & Eysenck, 1978). Because adolescents generally are

more impulsive and prefer higher levels of stimulation than adults, these items

arguably are age-inappropriate markers of psychopathy. Second, studies of time

perspective show that individuals become more future oriented and more aware of

the long-term consequences of their actions between childhood and adolescence

(ages 11±18), and between adolescence and young adulthood (ages 16±22; Greene,

1986; Nurmi, 1991). Similarly, studies of social perspective taking suggest that

individuals become more capable of considering situations from others' points of

view between adolescence and adulthood (Cauffman & Steinberg, in press; Selman,

1980). Because adolescents generally have more limited time and social perspective

than adults, PCL-R items such as lack of goals and irresponsibility seem less applicable

as de®nitive markers of psychopathy for adolescence than for adults. Third, several

studies indicate slow, steady increases throughout adolescence in the capacity for

self-direction (Greenberger, 1982) and in the formation of a stable sense of identity

(Marcia, 1980). This, similar to the evidence above, indicates that a lack of goals and

irresponsibility may be problematic when used to indicate the presence of psycho-

pathy among adolescents. Arguably, individuals must have a sense for who they are

and a capacity for directing their lives before they can focus on setting life goals and

acting responsibly.

58 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 7: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

The fourth and ®nal group of items is drawn from the Interpersonal/Affective (I)

factor of the PCL-R. Both the research cited above and additional evidence call into

question the applicability and stability of the lack of empathy/callousness and failure toaccept responsibility items. These features are likely to be in¯uenced by the limited

time and social perspective of adolescents noted earlier. Scoring of the grandiose senseof self-worth item also may be impacted by the ¯uctuations of identity seen during

adolescence (see, Alasker & Olweus, 1992; Block & Robins, 1993; Harter, 1990;

Nottelmann, 1987). More speci®cally, research has suggested that levels of self-

esteem among youth with externalizing behavior problems ¯uctuate considerably

from childhood through adolescence (Lochman & Dodge, 1994).

As summarized by Steinberg and Cauffman's (1996) model of maturity of

judgment, individuals develop greater capacity during adolescence for (1) respon-

sibility, (2) temporal and social perspective-taking, and (3) temperance (see also

Cauffman & Steinberg, 2000). It seems problematic to ascribe presumably im-

mutable psychopathic characteristics to adolescents whose socio-emotional abilities

are not yet developed fully. Applying adult models of psychopathy to juveniles,

however, risks doing so because many of the factors that denote adult psychopathy

exhibit considerable ¯uidity during adolescence, particularly antisocial conduct

(Mof®t, 1993). Instruments such as the PCL-R may tap construct-irrelevant

variance (Messick, 1995) associated with relatively normative and temporary char-

acteristics of adolescence rather than deviant and stable personality features. Even

among adolescents diagnosed with conduct disorder, the temporal stability of the

diagnosis varies considerably (Lahey et al., 1995) and has not been studied well on

the symptom level.

Clearly, longitudinal research that tracks the stability and course of putative

characteristics of psychopathy from adolescence through adulthood is needed.

However, even cross-sectional comparisons of adolescents matched with adults

on offense and demographics could better indicate the extent to which `̀ downward

extensions'' of conceptualizations of adult psychopathy are appropriate (see below,

`̀ Implications for clinical practice''). Existing evidence, however, suggests that the

PCL-R risks overestimating the level of psychopathic features of adolescents. As

they mature into adulthood, adolescents may cease to manifest putatively psycho-

pathic traits and behaviors, particularly the impulsive, antisocial features tapped by

the Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor. In a cross-sectional study of 14- to 18-year-old

(M� 16, SD� 1) serious offenders, Brandt, Kennedy, Patrick, & Curtin (1997)

found that adolescents' age at assessment was inversely associated with their

PCL:YV Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor scores (r � ÿ.21). This is concordant

with ®ndings by Harpur and Hare (1994), who found, based on a large cross-

sectional study, that Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor scores on the PCL measures

decrease signi®cantly from ages 13±70 (see footnote 4, p. 606). The authors reason

that this relationship may be partially attributable to the fact that this factor taps

antisocial behavior and personality `̀ traits'' that decline with age, including `̀ sensa-

tion seeking, impulsivity, venturesomeness, and monotony avoidance'' (p. 608,

citations omitted). Moreover, Forth and Burke (1998) found that, even in non-offender, community-based samples, adolescents obtain their highest average scores

on PCL items of need for stimulation, impulsivity, poor behavioral controls, and earlybehavioral problems. Thus, scores on measures of psychopathy arguably may be

in¯ated by general characteristics of adolescence.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 59

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 8: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

OPERATIONALIZATIONS OF THE PSYCHOPATHY

CONSTRUCT AMONG JUVENILES

Based on this brief review, it is clear that the wholesale application of the adult

psychopathy construct to children and adolescents may be inappropriate. A few

researchers have attempted to create more developmentally appropriate measures

of psychopathy by (a) modifying PCL-R items to facilitate scoring with adolescents

(Forth et al., in press) or (b) developing new measures with item content that may be

more applicable to children and adolescents (Frick, 1998; Frick & Hare, in press;

Lynam, 1997). Other measures that were not developed speci®cally for adolescents

(nor based speci®cally on the PCL conceptualization of psychopathy) also have

been used with this population (e.g., Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). Before summar-

izing the existing literature regarding the association between these measures and

violence, we brie¯y review their development and psychometric properties, focusing

in particular on research examining their construct validity.5

Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version

The initial studies examining psychopathy in adolescents utilized the PCL-R with

modi®cations in the scoring of individual items in order to make them more

appropriate for adolescents (Forth et al., 1990). Early scoring modi®cations

included the omission of items related to parasitic lifestyle and many short-termmarital relationships, given their limited applicability to adolescents. In addition,

scoring criteria for the juvenile delinquency and criminal versatility items were

modi®ed to account for the shorter criminal histories of adolescent offenders.

A few of the modi®cations employed in previous research have been codi®ed into

the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL:YV; Forth et al., in press). The

PCL:YV parallels the PCL-R in item composition, but was designed to better

account for adolescent life experiences by focusing more on peer, family, and school

adjustment. The PCL:YV is recommended for use with adolescents 13 years of age

and older, although an examination of existing studies suggest that it has been used

with children as young as 12 years of age (Forth & Burke, 1998). Similar to the PCL-

R, all items are scored on a three point-ordinal scale (0� item does not apply;

1� item applies but inconsistencies across interview and ®le information may be

present; and 2� item applies). The PCL:YV items are divided into Interpersonal/

Affective (factor I) and Socially Deviant Lifestyle (factor II) scales, which can be

summed to form a total score. However, few factor analytic results have been

reported for the PCL:YV. One study (Brandt et al., 1997) that has examined the

factor structure of the original modi®cation of the PCL-R (described by Forth et al.,1990) for adolescents does not strongly support the two-factor model (comparative

®t index (CFI)� .83).

5We do not review multi-scale inventories (e.g., MMPI-A, MAPI) that contain subscales ostensiblyassessing psychopathic features, given that most of these have not been shown to be useful measures ofthis construct (e.g., Hume, Kennedy, Patrick, & Partyka, 1996; but compare to Murrie & Cornell, 2000).Also, relatively few published reports speci®cally have addressed the ability of these scales to predictviolence and other forms of aggression.

60 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 9: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

De®nitive `̀ psychopath/non-psychopath'' cut scores have not been recommended

for the PCL:YV, although the authors of the instrument indicate that the traditional

PCL-R cut off of�30 serves as a reasonable marker for making this diagnosis (Forth

et al., in press; cf. Forth & Mailloux, 2000, p. 48). Utilizing a cut score �30, Forth

and Burke (1998) reported the following base rates of psychopathy averaged across

multiple studies: incarcerated settings 28.3%, probation settings 12.0%, and com-

munity settings 3.5%. Base rates reported in published studies assessing juvenile

psychopathy in incarcerated settings have ranged from 18 to 37%.

The PCL:YV has adequate levels of internal consistency (average � across

settings� .83; average inter-item r � .22) and inter-rater reliability for total scores

(average intraclass r � .93; Forth & Burke, 1998). However, estimates of inter-rater

reliability on the individual PCL:YV scales and items have not been published, nor

have estimates of test±retest reliability.

The convergent validity of the PCL:YV has been explored by examining the

relationship between the factor and total scores and conduct disorder (CD)

symptoms. Signi®cant correlations were found for both frequency of overall CD

symptoms (total r� .52; factor 1 r� .31; factor II r� .54) and frequency of

aggressive CD symptoms (total r� .47; factor 1 r� .32; factor II r� .31) in young

offenders (Forth & Burke, 1998). Sullivan (unpublished Master's thesis) has

reported modest but signi®cant correlations (r ranging from .23 to .34) between

the PCL:YV total scores and the adolescent version of the Minnesota Multiphasic

Personality Inventory (MMPI-A; Butcher et al., 1992) indices of externalizing

problems (Psychopathic Deviate Scale, Conduct Problems Content Scale, Anger

Content Scale).

Although the PCL:YV was designed to better account for adolescent life

experiences by focusing more on peer, family, and school adjustment, many of

the concerns regarding the developmental appropriateness of the PCL-R items

noted earlier also apply to the PCL:YV. For example, revised versions of the parasiticlifestyle and many short-term marital relationships items have been incorporated into

the PCL:YV, despite adolescents' limited work and relationship histories (Forth

et al., in press). Although the scoring criteria for these items have been modi®ed, the

content of the items is of questionable validity as markers of psychopathy for young

adolescents. Relatedly, although the scoring criteria for several problematic items

(e.g., impulsivity, irresponsibility, lack of goals, and need for stimulation/proneness toboredom) have been revised in an attempt to better apply to adolescents, the stability

of these items over signi®cant time periods remains an open issue.

Psychopathy Screening Device

The Psychopathy Screening Device (PSD; Frick & Hare, in press) is a 20-item

rating scale that was derived rationally from the item content of the PCL-R and

adapted for children and adolescents. Unlike the PCL:YV, which must be com-

pleted by trained raters, the PSD typically is completed by parents and teachers,

although a self-report version also has been investigated. Consistent with the

PCL:YV, the PSD items are scored on a three-point ordinal scale. Although speci®c

age ranges have not been speci®ed, the PSD previously has been used in research

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 61

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 10: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

with children and adolescents ranging in age from 6 to 18 years. Most of the

published studies have examined children in the 6 to 13 age range, however.

Tentative support for the construct validity of the PSD has been provided by

studies showing patterns of associations with theoretically relevant constructs that

are similar to the adult literature. For example, among children with emotional and

behavioral problems, PSD scores predict electrodermal responses to distress cues

and threatening stimuli (Blair, 1999) as well as differences in moral reasoning (Blair,

1997). Also, initial factor analyses of the PSD identi®ed a two-factor solution that

appeared somewhat similar to the adult two-factor model (Frick, O'Brien, Wootton,

& McBurnett, 1994). Similar to the PCL-R factors, the two PSD factors were highly

correlated (r� .50). The ®rst scale, corresponding loosely to the Socially

Deviant Lifestyle factor (i.e., factor II) of the PCL-R, was labeled Impulsive±

Conduct Problems (I/CP). Items loading on this factor consisted of (1) brags

about accomplishments; (2) becomes angry when corrected; (3) thinks he/she

is more important than others; (4) acts without thinking; (5) blames others for

mistakes; (6) teases other people; (7) engages in risky or dangerous activities; (8)

engages in illegal activities; (9) keeps the same friends; and (10) gets bored easily.

The I/CP scale evidenced good internal consistency in the initial validation study

(�� .83).

The second scale, which Frick et al. (1994) labeled Callous/Unemotional (CU),

was comprised of six items (concerned about schoolwork, feels bad or guilty,

emotions seem shallow, does not show emotions, acts charming in ways that seem

insincere, is concerned about the feelings of others) that loosely approximate the

Interpersonal/Affective factor (i.e., factor I) of the PCL-R. Acceptable internal

consistencies also have been obtained for this scale (alphas ranging from .81 to .73).

Differential correlates of the two scales have been cited as further evidence of the

PSD's construct validity. For example, Frick et al. (1994) reported that the I/CU

scale correlated more highly with the Delinquency (r� .58) subscale of the Child

Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) and the number of conduct disorder

symptoms (r� .53) and combined conduct disorder/oppositional de®ant disorder

(ODD) symptoms (r� .68) than did the CU scale (CBCL Delinquency r� .45; CD

symptoms r� .30; CD/ODD symptoms combined r� .40). Regression analyses in

this study indicated that the CU scale contributed independently to the prediction of

sensation seeking after controlling for ODD and CD symptoms, and a signi®cant

interaction between CU scales and DSM-III-R conduct problems predicted pater-

nal arrest history. Other research examining the correlates of these scales has

suggested that callous±unemotional traits are associated with a reward-oriented

response style (O'Brien & Frick, 1996) and with higher levels of fearlessness (Barry,

Frick, DeShazo, McCoy, Ellis, & Loney, 2000) among children with behavior

problems. Furthermore, conduct problems among children who score low on the

CU scale appear to be more strongly associated with dysfunctional parenting

practices (Wooton, Frick, Shelton, & Silverthorn, 1997) and de®cits in verbal

intelligence (Loney, Frick, Ellis, & McCoy, 1998). These factors are not strong

predictors of externalizing behavior among children who evidence callous±unemo-

tional traits, however.

Although initial factor analyses of the PSD suggested two interdependent factors,

a recent study by Frick, Bodin, & Barry (in press) suggests that a three-factor model

may be a more useful approach for construing psychopathic features among

62 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 11: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

children. In a large community sample of third, fourth, sixth, and seventh graders

(total n� 810), items related to narcissism ± which had been shown to load on the I/

CP factor in previous research ± formed a separate factor that was stable across both

parent and teacher ratings. Support for this model was somewhat more equivocal

when applied to a clinic-referred sample (n� 152), with con®rmatory factor

analyses (CFAs) suggesting an adequate ®t for both the two factor (NNFI� .90;

CFI� .91) and three factor (NNFI� .91; CFI� .92) models. A differential pattern

of external correlates was noted for the three subscales derived from these factors,

with narcissism being associated most strongly with ODD and CD symptomatology,

and the impulsivity subscale accounting for greater variance in symptoms of

hyperactivity. Callous±unemotional traits showed very little association with DSM

symptoms of conduct disorder after controlling for variance associated with the

narcissism and impulsivity subscales.

No speci®c cut scores have been proposed for distinguishing between `̀ psycho-

paths'' and `̀ non-psychopaths'' on the PSD.6 However, Christian, Frick, Hill, Tyler,

& Frazer (1997) used cluster analysis to investigate potential subtypes of psycho-

pathy based on the CU scale and ODD/CD symptoms. In a sample of 120 clinic-

referred children (age M� 8.68, SD� 2.07), four groups were identi®ed and

labeled: (a) clinic control (n� 39), (b) callous/unemotional (n� 41), (c) impulsive

conduct (n� 29), and (d) psychopathic conduct (n� 11). Relative to the other three

clusters, children in the psychopathic conduct cluster manifested the greatest

number of oppositional, aggressive, and covert property-destructive symptoms.

These children also had signi®cant histories of contacts with the police and parental

histories of APD. Using a somewhat different approach to identifying subtypes,

Frick et al. (in press) cluster analyzed the PSD scales derived from the three-factor

model. Five clusters were obtained in the community sample ± one of which was

comprised of children (n� 114) who displayed high scores across all three subscales.

Compared to the other clusters, these children had by far the highest rates of DSM-

IV symptoms of ODD and CD.

Child Psychopathy Scale

The content of the Child Psychopathy Scale (CPS; Lynam, 1997) was derived

rationally from the PCL-R and is composed of 41 items drawn from the CBCL

(Achenbach, 1991) and a version of the California Child Q-Set (CCQ; Block &

Block, 1980) through archival data analysis. Thirteen of the 20 PCL items were

operationalized by creating scales using combinations of the CBCL and CCQ items

(e.g., PCL-R item lack of remorse or guilt being assessed using CBCL item 26,

`̀ doesn't seem guilty after misbehaving,'' and CCQ item 72, `̀ he often feels guilty''

(reverse scored)). Similar to the original modi®cations of the PCL-R (Forth et al.,1990), CPS items that did not re¯ect childhood or adolescent experiences were not

included (promiscuous sexual behavior, many short-term marital relationships, and

revocation of conditional release). Also excluded were an additional three items;

6In one study examining the self-report version (Caputo, Frick, & Brodsky, 1999), cut-off scores beyondthe 95th percentile of a control group were employed to identify `̀ clinically signi®cant elevations'' on thePSD scales.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 63

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 12: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

grandiose sense of self-worth7 from the Interpersonal/Affective factor; need for stimula-tion/proneness to boredom and early behavior problems from the Socially Deviant

Lifestyle factor. In a footnote, Lynam indicated that a CFA of the `̀ scales''

supported the two factor model; however, the two factors were virtually redundant

with one another (r� .95). Based on these results, Lynam only used CPS total

scores for subsequent analyses. Unfortunately, little information regarding the

psychometric properties and CFA results were reported.

Lynam (1997) developed and evaluated the CPS with a sample of 430 male

fourth-graders from the Pittsburgh Youth Study, a longitudinal project tracking

delinquent and non-delinquent children. In terms of its construct validity, CPS

scores were found to correlate with various measures of impulsivity, including

self-reported (r� .25), teacher-rated (r� .26), and behavioral impulsivity (r� .32).

In comparison to their non-psychopathic counterparts, psychopathic children were

more prone to externalizing disorders and less likely to exhibit internalizing

disorders. After controlling for prior delinquency, SES, IQ, and impulsivity, the

CPS total score was found to add incremental validity in the prediction of

delinquency at ages 12 and 13 in a stepwise hierarchical regression.

P-Scan

Derived from the PCL conceptualization of psychopathy, the Hare P-Scan:

Research Version (Hare & HerveÂ, 1999) is a recently published measure consisting

of 90 items that are purported to indicate behaviors and traits related to this

personality construct. Although titled a `̀ research'' instrument, the P-Scan currently

is available for use by non-clinicians (e.g., judges, law enforcement of®cers, school

counselors) in applied settings and is intended to serve as `̀ a rough screening device

that, when properly used, may provide users with important clues or working

hypotheses about the nature of an individual of interest'' (p. 1). Notably, the P-

Scan manual describes the measure as not being a formal psychometric test, and

repeatedly cautions against the use of this device to make diagnostic decisions.

The items comprising the P-Scan are scored on the familiar 0±2 PCL scale, and

are grouped into `̀ interpersonal,'' `̀ affective,'' and `̀ lifestyle'' facets (30 items each).

A total score can be derived as well, based on the average of the three facet scores.

Total scores range from 0 to 60 and fall into one of three general levels (high,

moderate, low). Scores greater than 30 are described as `̀ cause for serious concern''

(Hare & HerveÂ, 1999, p. 11) and individuals who obtain such scores `̀ should be

referred for a professional evaluation and opinion by a psychologist or psychiatrist

quali®ed to use the PCL-R, the PCL:SV, or the PCL:YV'' (p. 7).

The P-Scan manual does not indicate age ranges for which the device is

considered to be appropriate, although it clearly is intended for use with adolescents,

based on references to the PCL:YV and to use by school counselors. The publisher

of the device is more speci®c in its catalogue than the authors are in the manual,

however, noting that the P-Scan is appropriate for ages 13 and older (Multi-Health

Systems, Inc., 2000).

7Lynam (1997) notes that this item did not correlate with the other CPS items.

64 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 13: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Published data regarding the reliability and validity of the P-Scan apparently are

unavailable at present. A review of relevant databases identi®ed no published studies

on this device, and the manual provides no information regarding normative data

or psychometric properties. (The manual actually provides no data of any sort.)

Inferences or hypotheses developed from P-Scan scores appear to lack any empirical

foundation at this time. It could be argued that, because the P-Scan is presented as a

`̀ rough screening device'' rather than a formalized psychological test, information

related to psychometric properties is unnecessary for use in non-clinical decision-

making. Nevertheless, quantifying a psychological construct and providing inter-

pretive statements regarding the potential meaning of obtained scores (Hare &

HerveÂ, 1999, pp. 6±7) clearly requires the publication of data that addresses the

validity and utility of the hypotheses derived from these scores.

Psychopathic Personality Inventory

One relatively new self-report measure of psychopathy is included in this review

because (1) preliminary studies indicate that it may be useful in assessing psycho-

pathy and in predicting aggression and violence among late adolescents, and (2)

there is some evidence supporting its validity across gender and setting (incarcerated

and non-incarcerated populations). The Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI;

Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996) is a 187-item self-report measure designed to assess

the core personality features of psychopathy in non-criminal, adult populations.

Although adequate levels of reliability (i.e., internal consistency, short-term test±

retest) have been reported in initial validation studies for the PPI as well as in

subsequent reports, the long-term stability of PPI scores has yet to be investigated.

In terms of construct validity, the PPI has been shown to correlate moderately to

highly with self-report, peer-report, and interview-based measures of psychopathy

and to correlate moderately with self-report, peer-report, and interview-based

measures of relevant cluster B (i.e. antisocial, narcissistic, histrionic) personality

disorder features in college samples (Hamburger, Lilienfeld, & Hogben, 1996;

Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996; Lilienfeld et al., 1998). Preliminary research with

`̀ youthful offender'' prison inmates (Poythress et al., 1998) suggests that this

questionnaire correlates highly (r� .54) with the PCL-R, and an optimally derived

cutting score classi®ed accurately 86% of the study participants as psychopathic/

non-psychopathic. The PPI also correlates modestly with criminal history variables

(e.g., number of prior arrests, history of juvenile delinquency) and moderately with

measures of aggression, empathy (negatively), and primitive psychological defenses

among prison and jail inmates (Edens, Poythress, & Lilienfeld, 1998; Sandoval,

Hancock, Poythress, Edens, & Lilienfeld, 2000).

Summary and Critique

Studies utilizing three of the four measures based on the PCL-R conceptualization

of psychopathy (i.e., PCL:YV, PSD, CPS) have examined their applicability across

several samples of varying ages. Although preliminary, the psychometric properties

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 65

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 14: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

of these measures as well as their pattern of external correlates provide support for

the argument that they are assessing a construct that is at least phenotypically similar

to adult psychopathy (this assertion cannot be made for the P-Scan). However,

several caveats regarding the ongoing validation of these measures warrant discus-

sion. First, relatively little is known about the underlying factor structure of the

PCL:YV. Noted earlier, the traditional two-factor model of psychopathy is in

question, with more recent research providing support for the existence of three

distinct factors among both adults and children (Cooke & Michie, in press; Frick

et al., in press). Although it may be argued that total scores on the PCL measures are

the most commonly employed metric for clinical decision-making, clari®cation of

the basic dimensions that comprise the psychopathy construct clearly is needed to

better de®ne what is being measured. If different dimensions of psychopathy are

differentially related to violence and aggression, this would have signi®cant implica-

tions for assessing risk. For example, in¯ated self-worth, empathy de®cits, and

impulsivity have been shown to be related to aggression (for reviews, see Baumeister,

Smart, & Boden, 1996; Edens, 1999; Miller & Eisenberg, 1988; Webster & Jackson,

1997) but the potential interactive effects of these factors rarely have been addressed

systematically. A more thorough explication of the relationship among these factors

will be an important area of future research among juveniles as well as adults.

A second concern relates more to the self- (and parent and teacher) report

measures (PSD, CPS, PPI) than to the PCL:YV. Relatively little is known regarding

the extent to which the former instruments are susceptible to the effects of response

sets such as social desirability or positive impression management. Performance on

these measures may be distorted signi®cantly if there are negative consequences

associated with the outcome. These measures have been studied primarily in

contexts where participation was either anonymous or con®dential ± circumstances

that are strikingly distinct from the typical forensic evaluation. The PPI is the only

instrument reviewed that contains validity scales to assess response distortion and,

to our knowledge, it is the only measure on which research examining the effects of

dissimulation has been conducted (Edens, Buf®ngton, & Tomicic, 2000; Edens,

Buf®ngton, Tomicic, & Riley, manuscript submitted for publication), with mixed

results being obtained in terms of identifying `̀ fake good'' protocols.

The third and most important limitation to all of these measures is the absence of

any long-term data regarding their temporal stability. This is a signi®cant limitation

that needs to be addressed, particularly given the developmental ¯uidity of some of

the personality and behavioral `̀ traits'' ostensibly assessed by these measures (see

above, `̀ Are adult features. . .''). In the absence of such data, one cannot conclude

that psychopathic features are stable from childhood to adulthood, let alone that

these measures can assess reliably these features over signi®cant time periods.

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH EXAMINING THE

ASSOCIATION BETWEEN JUVENILE

PSYCHOPATHY AND VIOLENCE

The research described in the preceding section suggests that, although signi®cant

questions remain to be addressed, the measures reviewed show some promise as

66 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 15: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

indices of psychopathic features and clearly warrant further empirical investigation.

A more applied issue, however, relates to the potential clinical utility of these

measures generally and their speci®c ability to identify accurately `̀ high-risk''

children and adolescents who are likely to engage in future violence. Authors

frequently have asserted that the PCL measures are strong predictors of institutional

misbehavior and long-term violence potential among juveniles (e.g., Forth &

Mailloux, 2000; Hare & HerveÂ, 1999; Lyon & Ogloff, 2000) and thus provide

important information regarding appraisals of risk. This section reviews the empiri-

cal basis of these claims.8

Table 2 provides an overview of published studies that have examined the

association between indices of aggression/violence and juvenile psychopathy.9

This list encompasses a wide range of studies in terms of samples employed,

predictor and criterion measures used, and follow-up (or follow-back) time periods

examined. For example, samples range in age from grade school to college age, and

are drawn from diverse settings with ostensibly differing levels of psychopathology

and delinquency/criminality (e.g., mental health clinics, youth detention centers,

adult prisons, university settings). The racial/ethnic composition of the samples

assessed also is relatively heterogeneous, although (similar to the adult literature)

they are disproportionately male. In terms of criterion variables, measures have

ranged from psychometrically sophisticated rating scales, such as self-report (e.g.,

Youth-Self Report Form; Achenbach, 1991), peer report (Multidimensional Per-

sonality Questionnaire ± Aggression scale; Tellegen, unpublished manuscript), and

parent/teacher (Child Behavior Checklist/Teacher Report Form; Achenbach, 1991)

inventories, to `̀ of®cial'' records such as institutional disciplinary infractions related

to aggressive/violent behavior and reconvictions for violent crimes. Criterion

measures have been collected over varying time periods as well, ranging from

methodologically weaker postdictive and concurrent designs ± in which `̀ predictor''

variables are assessed after, or in conjunction with, the criterion measures ± to more

rigorous predictive studies in which data upon which psychopathy ratings are made

are collected prior to the outcome variables. Most of the studies used the psycho-

pathy measures as continuous predictor variables and computed correlations with

outcome measures, although a few also used the psychopathy scales as categorical

predictors (e.g., Brandt et al., 1997) or as a criterion measure (e.g., Myers, Burket, &

Harris, 1995).

8Studies assessing the relationship between psychopathy and general delinquency or conduct disordersymptoms only (in the absence of speci®c measures of aggression or violence) were not included in thisreview, nor were studies assessing the association between violence and delinquency/conduct disorder (inthe absence of measures of psychopathy). Unpublished theses/dissertations, conference papers and rawdata sets also are not reviewed. It is worth noting that several researchers we contacted indicated that theyhave ongoing projects and unpublished dissertations (e.g., Gretton, unpublished doctoral dissertation)that address the predictive validity of the various psychopathy measures noted earlier as they relate toviolence among juveniles. Given that these have yet to go through the rigorous vetting process associatedwith publication in a peer-reviewed journal, however, none of these projects was included in the presentreview. Conclusions and recommendations derived from forensic evaluations arguably should be basedon research that has gone through this process (e.g., Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 1993).See Grisso (1998; pp. 80±81) for a related discussion of admissibility standards regarding juvenileassessment.9This research was identi®ed by searching various databases (e.g., PsychLit, PsychInfo, Medline) usingkeywords such as `̀ child/adolescent/juvenile,'' `̀ psychopathy/psychopathic,'' and `̀ aggression/violence/delinquency,'' and by contacting researchers who previously have published empirical studies in this area.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 67

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 16: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Tab

le2.

Pu

blish

edst

ud

ies

exam

inin

gth

eass

oci

ati

on

bet

wee

np

sych

op

ath

yan

daggre

ssio

n/v

iole

nce

am

on

gch

ild

ren

an

dad

ole

scen

ts

Au

thor(

s)P

red

icto

r(s)

Cri

teri

on

Mea

sure

sT

imef

ram

eS

am

ple

*/n

Mea

n

age/

ran

ge

Race

/

eth

nic

ityy

Ou

tcom

e(s

ign

ific

an

t

un

less

oth

erw

ise

note

d)

Bra

nd

tet

al.

(1997)

Mod

ifie

d

PC

L-R

(1)

CB

CL

Aggre

ssio

n

(2)

Majo

rin

stit

uti

on

al

infr

act

ion

s:

(a)

Ver

bal

(b)

Ph

ysi

cal

(3)

Tim

eu

nti

lvio

len

t

reoff

ense

(1)

Con

curr

ent

(2)

Pre

dic

tive

(3)

Pre

dic

tive

Inca

rcer

ate

d

del

inq

uen

ts/1

30

16.1

/14±18

28%

Cau

70%

AA

2%

His

(1)

r�

.31

(2a)

r�

.31

(2b

)r�

.28

(3)

Hig

hp

sych

op

ath

y

gro

up<

low

psy

chop

ath

ygro

up

Cap

uto

etal.

(1999)

Off

ense

cate

gory

PS

D-S

R:

(1)

I/C

P

(2)

CU

Post

dic

tive

Juven

ile

off

end

ers/

69

16.1

8/1

3±18

28.6

%C

au

68.6

%A

A

1.4

%H

is

1.4

%O

ther

(1)

ns

(2)

Sex

off

end

ers>

vio

len

t

off

end

ers/

non

con

tact

off

end

ers

Ed

ens

etal.

(1999)

(1)

Mod

ifie

d

PC

L-R

Inst

itu

tion

al

dis

cip

lin

ary

infr

act

ion

s:

Post

dic

tive

You

thfu

loff

end

er

pri

son

inm

ate

s/50

18.6

/17±21

32%

Cau

54%

AA

(1a)

r�

.18

ns

(b)

r�

.18

ns

(2)

PP

I(a

)P

hysi

cal

Aggre

ssio

n12%

His

(c)

r�

.28

(c)

Com

bin

ed2%

Oth

er(2

a)

r�

.06

ns

(b)

r�

.23

(c)

r�

.24

Fort

het

al.

(1990)

Mod

ifie

dP

CL

(1a)

Nu

mb

erof

pre

vio

us

vio

len

toff

ense

s

(1)

Post

dic

tive

Maxim

um

secu

rity

you

thd

eten

tion

16.3

/13±20

77.3

%C

au

22.7

%N

A

(1a)

r�

.27

(1b

)r�

.46

(1b

)N

um

ber

inst

itu

tion

al

char

ges

for

vio

len

tor

aggre

ssiv

eb

ehavio

r

(2)

Pre

dic

tive

cen

ter

inm

ate

s/75

(2)

r�

.26

(2)

Nu

mb

erof

charg

esor

con

vic

tion

s

for

vio

len

tre

off

ense

68 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 17: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Fri

cket

al.

(1994)

PS

D-P

/T:

(a)

1/C

P

CB

CL

Aggre

ssio

nC

on

curr

ent

Clin

ic-r

efer

red

/52

boys±

12

gir

ls

8.5

/6±13

82%

Cau

18%

Oth

er

(a)

r�

.67

(b)

r�

.36

(b)

CU

Lilie

nfe

ld&

An

dre

ws

(1996)

PP

I(1

)M

PQ

Aggre

ssio

n

(pee

rra

tin

gs)

Con

curr

ent

(1)

Colleg

est

ud

ents

/41

male

±55

fem

ale

(1)

18.7

/NR

(2)

18.7

/NR

(1)

NR

(2)

NR

(1)

r�

.43

(2)

r�

.38

(2)

MP

QA

gre

ssio

n

(sel

f-re

port

)

(2)

Colleg

est

ud

ents

/64

male

±45

fem

ale

(4N

R

gen

der

)

Lyn

am

(1997)

CP

S(1

)V

iole

nce

Cla

ssif

icati

on

(2)

YS

RA

ggre

ssio

n

(3)

TR

FA

ggre

ssio

n

(1)

Post

dic

tive

(2)

Con

curr

ent

(3)

Con

curr

ent

`̀H

igh

risk

''b

oys

an

dco

ntr

ols

/

(1)

403

(2)

401

(3)

344

NR

/12±13

54%

AA

46%

Oth

er

(ap

pro

xim

ate

)

(1)

r�

.32

(2)

r�

.19

(3)

r�

.31

Mu

rdock

Hic

ks,

Roger

s,&

Cash

el,

2000

PC

L:S

VV

iole

nt

inst

itu

tion

al

infr

act

ion

s

Pre

dic

tive

Juven

ile

off

end

ers/

95

15.7

8/N

R25.8

%C

au

48.3

%A

A

24.2

%H

is

1.7

%O

ther

r�ÿ0

.4(C

au

)n

s

r�

.39

(AA

)

r�ÿ.

29

(His

)n

s

Myer

s,B

urk

et,

&H

arr

is,

1995

Sel

f-re

port

ed:

(1)

Fig

hti

ng

(2)

Ser

iou

sin

jury

(3)

Wea

pon

use

Mod

ifie

dP

CL

-RP

ost

dic

tive

Ad

ole

scen

tp

sych

iatr

ic

inp

ati

ents

/10

male

&

20

fem

ale

15.3

3/1

4±17

83%

Cau

17%

AA

(1)

PC

L-R

(fig

hte

rs)

>P

CL

-R(n

on

figh

ters

);

t�2.4

7

(2)

PC

L-R

(ser

iou

s

inju

rygro

up

)

>P

CL

-R(n

ose

riou

s

inju

rygro

up

);t�

2.9

8

(3)

No

dif

fere

nce

s;t�

1.7

9n

s

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 69

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 18: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Table

2.

(Con

tin

ued

)

Au

thor(

s)P

red

icto

r(s)

Cri

teri

on

Mea

sure

sT

imef

ram

eS

am

ple

*/n

Mea

n

age/

ran

ge

Race

/

eth

nic

ityy

Ou

tcom

e(s

ign

ific

an

t

un

less

oth

erw

ise

note

d)

Roger

s,Jo

han

sen

,

Ch

an

g,

&S

ale

kin

,

1997

Mod

ifie

d

PC

L-R

Aggre

ssiv

e

inst

itu

tion

al

infr

act

ion

s:

Pre

dic

tive

Ad

ole

scen

toff

end

ers

inre

sid

enti

al

trea

tmen

t/81

(gen

der

NR

)

15.6

2/N

R46.9

%C

au

17.3

%A

A

33.3

%H

is

(1)

r�

.28

(2)

r�

NR

(pre

sum

edn

s)

(1)

Ph

ysi

cal

(2)

Ver

bal

Tou

pin

,M

erci

er,

Der

y,

Cote

,

&H

od

gin

s,1995

Mod

ifie

d

PC

L-R

(Fre

nch

ver

sion

)

(1)

DIS

Caggre

ssio

n

(con

curr

ent)

(2)

DIS

Caggre

ssio

n

(on

eyea

rfo

llow

-up

)

(1)

Con

curr

ent

(2)

Pre

dic

tive

Tre

atm

ent-

refe

rred

con

du

ctd

isord

ered

dole

scen

ts/5

2

(42

at

follow

-up

)

15.6

/13±17

NR

(1)

NR

(2)

r�

.30

Not

e:C

BC

L�

Ch

ild

Beh

avi

or

Ch

ecklist

(Ach

enb

ach

,1991;A

chen

bach

&E

del

bro

ck,1983);

CP

S�

Ch

ild

hood

Psy

chop

ath

yS

cale

(Lyn

am

,1997);

DIS

C�

Dia

gn

ost

icIn

terv

iew

Sch

edu

lefo

rC

hild

ren

(Fre

nch

ver

sion

;B

erger

on

,V

alla,

&B

reto

n,

1992);

MP

Q�

Mu

ltid

imen

sion

al

Per

son

ality

Qu

esti

on

nair

e(T

elle

gen

,u

np

ub

lish

edm

an

usc

rip

t);N

R�

Not

rep

ort

ed;P

CL�

Psy

chop

ath

yC

hec

klist

(Hare

,1980);

PC

L-R�

Psy

chopath

yC

hec

klist

-Rev

ised

(Hare

,1991);

PP

I�

Psy

chop

ath

icP

erso

nality

Inven

tory

(Lilie

nfe

ld&

An

dre

ws,

1996);

PS

D-P

/T�

Psy

chopath

yS

cree

nin

gD

evic

Pare

nt/

Tea

cher

Rep

ort

(Fri

ck&

Hare

,in

pre

ss);

PS

D-S

R�

Psy

chop

ath

yS

cree

nin

gD

evic

e-S

elf

Rep

ort

(Fri

ck&

Hare

,in

pre

ss);

TR

F�

Tea

cher

Rep

ort

Form

(Ach

enb

ach

,1991;

Ach

enb

ach

&E

del

bro

ck,

1983);

YS

R�

You

thS

elf

Rep

ort

Form

(Ach

enb

ach

,1991).

y Cau�

Cau

casi

an

;A

A�

Afr

ican

Am

eric

an

;H

is�

His

pan

ic;

NA�

Nati

veA

mer

ican

;N

R�

Not

rep

ort

ed.

*A

llsa

mp

les

are

excl

usi

vel

ym

ale

un

less

oth

erw

ise

note

d.

70 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 19: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

As shown in Table 2, all of the measures of psychopathy reviewed above

demonstrated at least some utility in predicting (or postdicting) violence. Perhaps

the most striking fact revealed in the table is the relative uniformity of these

correlations. Many of those reported for total scores approximated r� .30, with

the majority ranging between .20 and .40. This applies to a diverse collection of

studies that vary in the indices of aggression/violence used and in the follow-up time

frames examined.10 This suggests a robust, moderate association between the

various operationalizations of psychopathy and aggressive behavior, and is relatively

consistent with the PCL-R research that has examined this relationship among adult

offenders (Hemphill et al., 1998; Salekin et al., 1996).

Several studies that assess the utility of the PCL/PCL-R (employing the

modi®cations suggested by Forth et al., 1990) are worth noting. Only two

published studies have examined the prediction of re-arrest for a violent offense

among adolescents (Brandt et al., 1997; Forth et al., 1990). First, in a sample of

71 adolescent offenders with an average follow-up period of 27.2 months

(SD� 9.3), Forth et al. found that the PCL did not correlate signi®cantly with

general recidivism (r� .14) when coded dichotomously (yes/no), but was associated

signi®cantly (r� .26) with the number of charges or convictions for a violent

reoffense. No information regarding the frequency or distribution of violent

reoffending in this sample was reported, although the base rate for general

reoffending was 79%. The strength of the individual relationships between violence

and the Interpersonal/Affective and Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor scores also was

not reported.

Second, Brandt et al. (1997) examined the predictive validity of the PCL-R

using group level (i.e., low, medium, high psychopathy) classi®cations over an 18- to

24-month follow-up period. Although no signi®cant differences were obtained

for nonviolent re-offenses, survival analyses indicated that the high psychopathy

group took the least time to be referred for a violent re-offense and the low

psychopathy group took the longest. Other than being reported as statistically

signi®cant, no indices of the magnitude of this relationship were reported.

Notably, PCL-R total and factor scores did not increase the accuracy of predicting

violent re-offenses after controlling for several demographic, criminal history, and

clinical variables (e.g., MMPI and CBCL scales). However, both PCL-R

factor scores increased the accuracy of predicting combined violent/non-violent

reoffenses after controlling for these same variables. Speci®cally, the Socially

Deviant Lifestyle factor (II) accounted for unique variance (�R2� .05) after

entering these variables, and the Interpersonal/Affective factor (I) accounted for

unique variance (�R2� .03) after entering the other variables as well as Socially

Deviant Lifestyle factor scores. Unfortunately, analyses examining the predictive

utility of the PCL-R and its subscales in isolation (e.g., zero-order correlations) were

not reported.

Five studies have examined the utility of the PCL measures in predicting

violence while institutionalized. Three of these (Brandt et al., 1997; Murdock Hicks

et al., 2000; Rogers et al., 1997) involved the prediction of institutional infractions

that occurred after the PCL assessment, whereas the remaining two (Edens,

10Because of the diversity of samples and predictor and criterion measures employed in these studies, theuse of meta-analytic procedures to summarize this body of research was judged to be inappropriate.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 71

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 20: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Poythress, & Lilienfeld, 1999; Forth et al., 1990) were postdictive designs. As

shown in Table 2, the results of these studies are fairly similar, with most of the

correlations between verbally aggressive and violent infractions and psychopathy

approximating .30 (range of reported r� .18 to .46). Notably, both Brandt et al.(1997) and Rogers et al. (1997) obtained correlations of .28 with physical violence

for their samples. Although the study by Murdock Hicks et al. (2000) did not report

these results for the total sample, a review of the PCL:SV/violent infraction

correlations reported separately for the three racial/ethnic groups suggests that

this correlation would approach that of the other studies. Unfortunately, these

studies did not compare the incremental predictive validity of the Interpersonal/

Affective and Socially Deviant Lifestyle factors in predicting institutional violence.

However, the studies by both Brandt et al. (1997) and Rogers et al. (1997) reported

correlations of generally similar magnitude for the factors, with Interpersonal/

Affective factor (I) correlations of .23 and .30, and Socially Deviant Lifestyle factor

(II) correlations of .33 and .30, respectively. Similarly, in the study by Edens et al.(1999) both factors I and II were signi®cantly correlated with the combined

disciplinary infraction category of physical/verbal aggression (rs� .30 and .28,

respectively).

Only two of these ®ve studies examined the incremental validity of the

PCL measures in comparison to other indices of psychopathy, and have produced

mixed results. In contrast to Brandt et al. (1997; see above), Murdock Hicks et al.(2000) found the PCL:SV did not provide any incremental validity beyond

the MMPI-A in terms of the prediction of both violent and total (i.e., violent

and non-violent) infractions. Similarly, Edens et al. (1999) found that neither

the PCL-R nor the PPI evidenced any incremental validity beyond the other

measure in the prediction of verbally aggressive/physically violent disciplinary

infractions.

None of the studies noted above reported analyses for the three-factor hierarch-

ical model of the PCL measures recently espoused by Cooke and Michie (in press)

and Frick et al. (in press). However, re-analysis of the data from Edens et al.(1999)11 indicated that, whereas a subscale comprising the items from the Arrogant

and Deceitful Interpersonal Style factor correlated signi®cantly with physical/verbal

aggression (r� .29, p< .05), De®cient Affective Experience was not signi®cantly

associated with this criterion (r� .21, p�ns). Although the Socially Deviant Life-

style factor (factor II) was signi®cantly correlated (r� .28, p< .05) with physical/

verbal aggression in the original analysis, it was not signi®cantly associated (r� .14,

p�ns) with Cooke and Michie's revised factor II (Impulsive and Irresponsible

Behavioral Style) in which several of the items, including those tapping child/

adolescent behavior problems, have been eliminated. Although it could be argued

that this ®nding supports the position that early behavior problems play an

important role in the prediction of violent behavior (e.g., Quinsey et al., 1998),

given that these results are based on a re-analysis of data from only 50 youthful

offenders, they should be viewed as preliminary. Moreover, the attenuated relation-

ship between the Impulsive and Irresponsible Behavioral Style subscale and verbal/

physical aggression could be explained somewhat by a decrease in the reliability of

11A more detailed description of these results is available from the ®rst author.

72 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 21: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

this scale after the `̀ extraneous'' PCL-R items were deleted (i.e., � dropped from .75

to .58 after eliminating items 10, 12, and 18).12

Although the bulk of the studies noted in Table 2 used the PCL derivatives

to operationalize psychopathy, the other measures (i.e., PSD, CPS, PPI) showed

similar patterns of correlations with criterion measures. This ®nding is particularly

informative, given that most of the PSD and CPS studies examined relatively

younger (i.e., grade-school-aged) samples than did the PCL studies and also often

used different outcome measures. In a few of these studies, associations were

examined between measures that were completed by the same raters (e.g., parent-

report psychopathy (PSD) correlated with parent-reported aggression (CBCL

aggression)), which may have resulted in somewhat in¯ated results due to rater

bias. However, those in which different raters completed the measures produced

similar (and in some cases, stronger) results.

Summary and Critique

In summary, the studies reviewed above generally indicate that there is a moderate

association between various measures of juvenile psychopathy and various types of

aggression across a range of settings and age levels. Furthermore, the magnitude of

this association is consistent with research conducted with adults. However, there

are several limitations to this literature that should be noted. First, the extent to

which the association between psychopathic traits and aggression will remain stable

over time is unknown. Because the studies summarized above provide only a quasi-

cross-sectional analysis of this relationship, they provide little support for the

argument that psychopathy during adolescence is a robust predictor of futureviolence, particularly violence that occurs beyond late adolescence. Second, we

do not have strong evidence to suggest that these measures of psychopathy have

incremental predictive validity when compared to other leading predictors of

juvenile violence (e.g., conduct disorder, poor peer associations, actuarial risk

assessment scales; Hawkins et al., 2000).13 Third, the association between adoles-

cent psychopathy and `̀ of®cial'' indices of violence has been examined in only a few

published studies. Only two studies have used the PCL measures to predict violent

recidivism, and only ®ve have assessed the relationship between psychopathy and

institutional violence. In terms of practical applications related to juvenile offender

decision-making, these criminal justice outcome measures are more relevant than

the indices used in other studies (e.g., parent/teacher rating scales).

Although we are aware of ongoing research that will improve upon our existing

knowledge base, at present there are several reasons to exercise caution in applying

the construct of juvenile psychopathy in clinical/forensic settings. In our opinion,

12Because this sample was comprised of `̀ youthful offenders,'' items 9 (parasitic lifestyle) and 19(revocation of conditional release) on factor II were not scored. Therefore, � for this scale was basedon seven items in the original analysis. Also, � for the Impulsive and Irresponsible Behavioral Stylesubscale is based only on four (3, 13, 14, and 15) of the ®ve items comprising this factor, given that item 9was not scored.13The study by Rogers et al. (1997) was one of the few to address this issue, wherein the PCL-R was foundto be a more robust predictor of violence than CD symptoms ± but only in relation to the prediction ofinstitutional misbehavior. General and violent recidivism data were not available in this study.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 73

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 22: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

clinicians may appropriately use validated measures of juvenile psychopathy to aid in

making short-term predictions of violent behavior among adolescents if they clearly

acknowledge the measures' limitations (e.g., unclear long-term stability and factor

structure). However, measures of juvenile psychopathy should not be used to make

long-term predictions of violent behavior, nor to make clinical decisions with long-

term implications. The following section reviews speci®c concerns about using

measures of psychopathy for the latter purposes.

IMPLICATIONS FOR CLINICAL PRACTICE

IN JUVENILE JUSTICE SETTINGS

As suggested by the research presented above, our knowledge about the nature,

stability and consequences of juvenile psychopathy is relatively limited. Never-

theless, some researchers (Forth, 1999; Harris, Skilling, & Rice, unpublished

manuscript; Lyon & Ogloff, 2000; Towers, Heilbrun, & Peters, 2000) have implied

or asserted that assessments of psychopathy will aid in deciding whether to transfer

juvenile offenders to the adult court system, whether to decertify adolescents already

transferred to the adult system, and/or what type of sentence juveniles should receive

(i.e. length, security level, and availability of treatment). In Canada, diagnoses of

psychopathy have been used to justify decisions that juvenile offenders be tried in

adult court and serve longer sentences in order to protect the public (Zinger &

Forth, 1998). Although not as widely employed as in Canada, measures of

psychopathy apparently are beginning to be used in the assessment of adolescent

offenders in the United States as well (Towers et al., 2000). Because the PCL

measures almost universally are regarded as the `̀ gold standard'' for assessing

psychopathy, the Revised and Youth Version of the PCL are most likely to be

used for these purposes. This section analyzes the extent to which the use of the PCL

measures is likely to be helpful or harmful for making long-term decisions about

transferring and sentencing juvenile offenders, given the current state of relevant

research. Although clinicians and the courts clearly could bene®t from some

empirical guidance in making these decisions, we conclude, based on three

concerns, that measures of juvenile psychopathy should not be relied upon heavily

in this process at present.

The chief concern raised by using measures of psychopathy to make weighty legal

decisions about juveniles is that we fundamentally do not know whether these

measures identify a small subgroup of adolescents who, based on stable personality

traits, will engage persistently in antisocial and violent behavior throughout the

course of their lives (see Forth & Burke, 1998). To date, there have been no

published longitudinal studies of the stability of psychopathy, as assessed by the

PCL or other assessment tools, from early adolescence through adulthood. It is

unclear to what extent some juveniles identi®ed as psychopathic ± particularly those

whose classi®cations resulted primarily from elevations on the Socially Deviant

Lifestyle factor ± may be more representative of the `̀ adolescent-limited'' type of

offender whose antisocial behavior is likely to desist following adolescence (Mof®t,

1993). This fact alone should bar the courts from making consequential, long-term

decisions about a juvenile offender based on a single assessment of psychopathy

during teenage years.

74 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 23: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Although there apparently have been no direct investigations of the stability of

psychopathy, the evidence reviewed above (see `̀ Are adult features. . .'') suggests

that adolescents' scores on the PCL instruments may be in¯ated by normative

developmental characteristics. The PCL:YV rests on the untested assumption that

psychopathy is expressed in much the same way whether an offender is 13 or 43

years old. It is comprised of the same 20 items as the adult PCL-R, with only

modi®ed scoring criteria for items that seem developmentally inappropriate for

adolescents (e.g., parasitic lifestyle). Because it is based on the adult PCL-R, the

PCL:YV includes normative adolescent `̀ traits'' that have been shown to wane with

age, such as impulsivity, need for stimulation, and poor behavioral controls (see

Forth & Burke, 1998; Harpur & Hare, 1994). If the PCL:YV taps construct

irrelevant variance associated with adolescence, it risks misclassifying juvenile

offenders as psychopathic. Research is needed on adolescent and adult samples,

matched for basic demographic (e.g., race, SES) and offense characteristics, to

determine the extent to which (1) adolescents obtain higher scores on potentially

age-inappropriate items than adults, and (2) the base rates of psychopathy are higher

among adolescent offenders than adults (compare rates of Brandt et al., 1997; Forth

& Burke, 1998; and Trevethan & Walker, 1989, with those of Hare, 1991, and Hart

et al., 1995). If psychopathy is a stable personality disorder, it should not be more

prevalent among incarcerated adolescents than adults. Misclassi®cations of even a

small proportion of juvenile offenders as psychopathic based on the contribution of

age-inappropriate indicators of juvenile psychopathy would be associated with grave

consequences if they guided judicial decision-making.

Second, using psychopathy assessments in juvenile justice settings raises con-

cerns about the strength of the data supporting assumptions that psychopathy (a) is

strongly associated with future, persistent violence, and (b) is untreatable, even in

adolescence. With respect to the ®rst issue, measures of psychopathy may be useful

for winnowing a large group of juvenile delinquents into a smaller subgroup that is

moderately likely to re-offend in the relatively near future (see, e.g., Forth & Burke,

1998). However, the magnitude of the correlation between juvenile psychopathy

and future violence argues against sole reliance on psychopathy ratings to predict

violence for adolescent offenders (Edens et al., 1999; Rogers et al., 1997; see also

Forth & Mailloux, 2000, p. 48).

Given the emphasis traditionally placed by the juvenile justice system on offender

rehabilitation, the second issue regarding whether adolescents with psychopathy are

responsive to treatment is pivotal. However, we could locate only one published

study that addressed treatment compliance and one conference presentation that

more directly addressed the issue of treatment amenability. First, in a sample of 81

adolescent (age M� 16 years, SD� 1) admissions to a state hospital, Rogers et al.(1997) found a signi®cant, but `̀ modest'' (r � .25) correlation between scores on the

modi®ed PCL-R and ratings of treatment noncompliance. Second, O'Neill, Heil-

brun and Lidz (2000) studied an ethnically diverse sample of 64 adolescents (age

M� 16 years, SD� 1) who were charged with drug offenses and were referred by

the court to complete an intensive, 3-month-long partial hospitalization program to

treat substance abuse. The authors found that adolescents' total scores on the

PCL:YV were signi®cantly associated with the number of days that they attended

the treatment program (r � ÿ.42), and with graduate students' global, 5-point

ratings of adolescents' clinical improvement coded from discharge summaries

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 75

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 24: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

(r� ÿ.58). This research is welcome, given the absence of data in this area, but has

two limitations. First, as noted by the authors, the treatment in this study was a

relatively short, intensive program designed to address substance abuse, rather than

psychopathy-relevant symptoms including criminogenic needs (see Andrews,

Bonta, & Hoge, 1990; Simourd & Hoge, 2000). Second, the measurement of

psychotherapy outcome is complex (see Gar®eld & Bergin, 1994), and dif®cult to

reduce to a single rating gleaned from a discharge summary. In short, given available

data on the subject, using measures of psychopathy to provide juvenile courts with a

prediction regarding how an offender will respond to treatment, particularly

treatment designed to address psychopathy, seems inappropriate. In fact, even the

empirical literature on the treatment of adults diagnosed with psychopathy is

`̀ inconclusive at best,'' indicating only that the therapeutic community model,

which also is not founded upon the principles of successful rehabilitation, is

ineffective (Zinger & Forth, 1998). As recently noted by Forth and Burke (1998),

relative to adult offenders, `̀ psychopathic young offenders may be more malleable

and may bene®t more from treatment. Given the implications that psychopathy has

for society in general and the criminal justice system in particular, future research

initiatives are needed to address the issues surrounding the development of effective

intervention and management strategies for psychopathic youth'' (p. 224, citations

omitted).

Third and ®nally, the use of psychopathy measures in juvenile justice settings

raises ethical issues. We have argued that there is signi®cant potential for error

associated with using existing measures to classify adolescents as psychopaths

and, on that basis, to predict that they are likely to engage in persistent violence,

regardless of whether they are provided with treatment. Because these errors may

result in serious infringements on adolescents' liberty and treatment options, mental

health professionals are obligated to meet high standards in assessing psychopathy

(Zinger & Forth, 1998). Given that there is currently limited scienti®c justi®cation

for issuing conclusions about an adolescent's diagnosis, long-term violence poten-

tial, and treatability based on measures of psychopathy, doing so is ethically

questionable (see American Psychological Association (APA), 1992; x2.01(b);

x2.02(a); x7.02). Psychologists are bound to identify situations in which assessment

techniques `̀ may not be applicable'' based on factors such as an individual's age

(APA, 1992, x2.04).

As acknowledged by Forth et al. (1990), even if one were able to obtain stable,

age-appropriate assessments of juvenile psychopathy that strongly predicted long-

term violence potential and treatment response, measuring psychopathy during

adolescence still raises ethical issues. Diagnoses of psychopathy may be issued

with `̀ dispassion and scienti®c neutrality'' (Toch, 1998, p. 151), but are far from

morally neutral in content (see also Blackburn, 1988; Gunn, 1998; cf. Frick

et al., 1994). Individuals labeled as psychopathic are presumptively (and perma-

nently) `̀ sleazy, unsavory, repugnant, and dangerous'' (Toch, 1998, p. 151).

Labeling a young adolescent as a psychopath may violate ethical principles of social

responsibility (APA, 1992, Principle F) and doing no harm (x1.14). Arguably,

one should assign such a damning label to a young and developing adolescent

only (a) when the diagnosis is based on empirically validated methods and (b)

when justi®ed by a high likelihood that the information will prevent serious harm

to others.

76 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 25: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Adolescence is a time of signi®cant developmental change. Because most

adolescents manifest some `̀ traits'' and behaviors during this period that may be

phenotypically similar to symptoms of psychopathy, adolescence may be the most

dif®cult stage of life in which to detect this personality pattern. It is imperative that

we learn more about the stability, nature, and manifestations of psychopathy during

the adolescent years, and develop and re®ne age-appropriate risk assessment tools

based on this knowledge (e.g., Borum, 2000). These developments are a prerequi-

site for embracing the construct of psychopathy as a valid, useful component in the

evaluation of juvenile offenders.

REFERENCES

Achenbach TM. 1991. Integrative Guide for the 1991CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF Pro®les. University ofVermont Department of Psychiatry: Burlington, VT.

Achenbach TM. 1995. Developmental issues in assessment, taxonomy, and diagnosis of child andadolescent psychopathology. In Developmental Psychopathology: Theory and Methods, Cicchetti D,Cohen D (eds). Wiley: New York; pp. 57±80.

Achenbach TM, Edelbrock, 1983. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Pro®le. Burlington, VT:University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

af Klinteberg B, Humble K, Schalling D. 1992. Personality and psychopathy of males with a history ofearly criminal behavior. European Journal of Personality 6: 245±266.

Alasker F, Olweus, D. 1992. Stability of global self-evaluations in early adolescence: A cohort longitudinalstudy. Journal of Research on Adolescence 1: 123±145.

Alm PO, af Klinteberg B, Humble K, Leppert J, Sorensen S, Tegelman R, Thorell LH, Lidberg L. 1996a.Criminality and psychopathy as related to thyroid activity in former juvenile delinquents. ActaPsychiatrica Scandinavica 94: 112±117.

Alm PO, af Klinteberg B, Humble K, Leppert J, Sorensen S, Tegelman R, Thorell LH, Lidberg L. 1996b.Psychopathy, platelet MAO activity and criminality among former juvenile delinquents. Acta Psychia-trica Scandinavica 94: 105±111.

American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edn.Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychological Association. 1992. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct.American Psychologist 47: 1597±1611.

Andrews DA, Bonta J, Hoge RD. 1990. Classi®cation for effective rehabilitation: Rediscoveringpsychology. Criminal Justice and Behavior 17: 19±52.

Barry CT, Frick PJ, DeShazo TM, McCoy MG, Ellis M, Loney BR. 2000. The importance of callous±unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology109: 335±340.

Baumeister RF, Smart L, Boden JM. 1996. Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression:The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review 103: 5±33.

Bergeron L, Valla JP, Breton JJ. 1992. Pilot study for the Quebec Child Mental Health Survey: Part I.Measurement of prevalence estimates among six to 14 year olds. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 37:373±380.

Blackburn R. 1988. On moral judgments and personality disorders: The myth of the psychopathicpersonality revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry 153: 505±512.

Blair RJR. 1997. Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and IndividualDifferences 22: 731±739.

Blair RJR. 1999. Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality andIndividual Differences 27: 135±145.

Block J, Block JH. 1980. The California Child Q-Set. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Block J, Robins RW. 1993. A longitudinal study of consistency and change in self-esteem from early

adolescence to early adulthood. Child Development 64: 909±923.Borum R. 2000. Assessing violence risk among youth. Journal of Clinical Psychology 56: 1263±1288.Brandt JR, Kennedy WA, Patrick CJ, Curtin JJ. 1997. Assessment of psychopathy in a population of

incarcerated adolescent offenders. Psychological Assessment 9: 429±435.Butcher JN, Williams CL, Graham JR, Archer RP, Tellegen A, Ben-Porath YS, Kaemmer B. 1992.

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory ± Adolescent (MMPI-A): Manual for Administration, Scoring,and Interpretation. University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 77

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 26: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Caputo AA, Frick PJ, Brodsky SL. 1999. Family violence and juvenile sex offending: The potential roleof psychopathic traits and negative attitudes toward women. Criminal Justice and Behavior 26:338±356.

Cauffman E, Steinberg L. 2000. (Im)maturity of judgment in adolescence: Why adolescents may be lessculpable than adults. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 18: 741±760.

Christian RE, Frick PJ, Hill NL, Tyler L, Frazer DR. 1997. Psychopathy and conduct problems inchildren: II. Implications for subtyping children with conduct problems. Journal of the AmericanAcademy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36: 233±241.

Cleckley H. 1941. The mask of sanity. Mosby: St. Louis.Cooke DJ, Michie C. 1997. An item response theory analysis of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist ±

Revised. Psychological Assessment 9: 3±14.Cooke DJ, Michie C. In press. Re®ning the construct of psychopathy: Towards a hierarchical model.

Psychological Assessment.Cottle CC, Lee RJ, Heilbrun K. 1999. The prediction of criminal recidivism in juveniles: A meta-analysis.

Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Psychological Association. Boston, MA.Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 112 S. Ct. 2786 (1993).Edens JF. 1999. Aggressive children's self-systems and the quality of their relationships with signi®cant

others. Aggression and Violent Behavior 4: 151±177.Edens JF, Buf®ngton JK, Tomicic TL. 2000. An investigation of the relationship between psychopathic

traits and malingering on the Psychopathic Personality Inventory. Assessment 7: 281±296.Edens JF, Poythress NG, Lilienfeld SO. 1998. Validation of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in

correctional and community samples. Paper presented at the 106th annual conference of the AmericanPsychological Association: San Francisco, CA.

Edens JF, Poythress NG, Lilienfeld SO. 1999. Identifying inmates at risk for disciplinary infractions:A comparison of two measures of psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 17: 435±443.

Forth AE. 1999. Psychopathy in adolescence: Implications for the transfer of juveniles to adult criminalcourt. In Psychopathy and Risk Assessment, symposium conducted at the meeting of the AmericanPsychology±Law Society and European Association of Psychology and Law, Dublin, Hart S. (Chair).

Forth AE, Burke HC. 1998. Psychopathy in adolescence: Assessment, violence, and developmentalprecursors. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research, and Implications for Society, Cooke D, Forth A, Hare R.(eds). Kluwer: Dordrecht; 205±230.

Forth AE, Hart SD, Hare RD. 1990. Assessment of psychopathy in male young offenders. PsychologicalAssessment 2: 342±344.

Forth AE, Kosson DS, Hare RD. In press. The Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version manual. Multi-HealthSystems: Toronto.

Forth AE, Mailloux DL. 2000. Psychopathy in youth: What do we know? In The Clinical and ForensicAssessment of Psychopathy, Gacono CB. (ed). Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ; 25±54.

Frick PJ. 1998. Callous±unemotional traits and conduct problems: Applying the two-factor model ofpsychopathy to children. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research, and Implications for Society Cooke D, ForthA, Hare R. (eds). Kluwer: Dordrecht; 161±188.

Frick PJ, Bodin SD, Barry CT. In press. Psychopathic traits and conduct problems in community andclinic-referred samples of children: Further development of the Psychopathy Screening Device.Psychological Assessment.

Frick PJ, Hare RD. In press. The Psychopathy Screening Device. Multi-Health Systems: Toronto.Frick PJ, O'Brien BS, Wootton JM, McBurnett K, 1994. Psychopathy and conduct problems in children.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology 103: 700±707.Gar®eld AE, Bergin SL. (eds). 1994. Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change 4th edn. Wiley: New

York.Giambra L, Camp C, Grodsky A. 1992. Curiosity and stimulation seeking across the adult life span:

Cross sectional and 6 to 8 year longitudinal ®ndings. Psychology and Aging 7: 150±157.Greenberger E. 1982. Education and the acquisition of psychosocial maturity. In The Development of

Social Maturity McClelland D. (ed). Irvington: New York; 155±189.Greene A. 1986. Future-time perspective in adolescence: The present of things future revisited. Journal of

Youth and Adolescence 15: 99±113.Grisso T. 1998. Forensic Evaluation of Juveniles. Professional Resource Press: Sarasota, FL.Gunn J. 1998. Psychopathy: An elusive concept with moral overtones. In Psychopathy: Antisocial, Criminal

and Violent Behavior Millon T, Simonsen E, Birket-Smith M, Davis R. (eds). Guilford: New York;32±39.

Hamburger ME, Lilienfeld SO, Hogben M. 1996. Psychopathy, gender, and gender roles: Implicationsfor antisocial and histrionic personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders 10: 41±55.

Hare RD. 1980. A research scale for the assessment of psychopathy in criminal populations. Personalityand Individual Differences 1: 111±119.

Hare RD. 1991. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist ± Revised Manual. Multi-Health Systems: Toronto.Hare RD, Herve HF. 1999. Hare P-Scan: Research Version Manual. Multi-Health Systems: Toronto.

78 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 27: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Harpur TJ, Hare RD. 1994. Assessment of psychopathy as a function of age. Journal of AbnormalPsychology 103: 604±609.

Hart SD, Cox DN, Hare RD. 1995. Manual for the Screening Version of the Psychopathy Checklist(PCL:SV). Multi-Health Systems: Toronto.

Harter S, 1990. Identity and self development. In At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent, Feldman S,Elliott G. (eds). Harvard University Press Cambridge; 352±387.

Hawkins JD, Herrenkohl TI, Farrington DP, Brewer D, Catalano RF, Harachi TW, Cothern L. 2000.Predictors of Youth Violence. Of®ce of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention: Washington DC.

Hemphill JF, Hare RD, Wong S. 1998. Psychopathy and recidivism: A review. Legal and CriminologicalPsychology 3: 139±170.

Hume MP, Kennedy WA, Patrick CJ, Partyka DJ. 1996. Examination of the MMPI-A for the assessmentof psychopathy in incarcerated adolescent male offenders. International Journal of Offender Therapy andComparative Criminology 403: 224±233.

Lahey BB, Loeber R, Hart EL, Frick PJ, Applegate B, Zhang Q, Green SM, Russo, MF. 1995. Four-yearlongitudinal study of conduct disorder in boys: Patterns and predictors of persistence. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology 104: 83±93.

Lilienfeld SO, Andrews BP. 1996. Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measureof psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of Personality Assessment 66:488±524.

Lilienfeld SO, Hess T, Penna S, Morgan A, Cale E, Zolondek S. Asmundon G. 1998. Psychopathy andundersensitivity to threat cues: A test of Grey's model. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of theAssociation for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy: Washington, DC.

Lochman JE, Dodge KA. 1994. Social±cognitive processes of severely violent, moderately aggressive, andnonaggressive boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62: 366±374.

Loney BR, Frick PJ., Ellis ML, McCoy MG. 1998. Intelligence, psychopathy, and antisocial behavior.Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 20: 231±247.

Lynam DR. 1996. Early identi®cation of chronic offenders: Who is the ¯edgling psychopath? PsychologicalBulletin 120: 209±234.

Lynam DR. 1997. Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the ¯edgling psychopath in a nomological net.Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106: 425±438.

Lyon DR, Ogloff JRP. 2000. Legal and ethical issues in psychopathy assessment. In The Clinical andForensic Assessment of Psychopathy, Gacono CB. (ed). Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ; 139±173.

Marcia J. 1980. Identity in adolescence. In Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, Adelson J. (ed.). Wiley:New York; 159±187.

Messick S. 1995. Validity of psychological assessment: Validation of inferences from persons' responsesand performances as scienti®c inquiry into score meaning. American Psychologist 50: 741±749.

Miller PA, Eisenberg N. 1988. The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocialbehavior. Psychological Bulletin 103: 324±344.

Mof®t TE. 1993. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmentaltaxonomy. Psychological Review 100: 674±701.

Monahan J, Steadman HJ. (eds). 1994. Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment.University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.

Multi-Health Systems, Inc. (2000). MHS 2000 Catalogue. Author: Toronto.Murdock Hicks M, Rogers R, Cashel M. 2000. Predictions of violent and total infractions among

institutionalized male juvenile offenders. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:183±190.

Murrie DC, Cornell DG. 2000. The Millon Adolescent Clinical Inventory and psychopathy. Journal ofPersonality Assessment 75: 110±125.

Myers WC, Burket RC, Harris HE. 1995. Adolescent psychopathy in relation to delinquent behaviors,conduct disorder, and personality disorders. Journal of Forensic Sciences 40: 436±440.

Nottelmann ED. 1987. Competence and self-esteem during transition from childhood to adolescence.Developmental Psychology 23: 441±450.

Nurmi J. 1991. How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientationand planning. Developmental Review 11: 1±59.

O'Brien BS, Frick PJ. 1996. Reward dominance: Associations with anxiety conduct problems, andpsychopathy in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 24: 223±240.

O'Neill ML, Heilbrun K, Lidz V. 2000. Adolescent psychopathy in a substance abusing cohort:Predictors and treatment outcomes. In Psychopathy Among Juveniles/Adolescents, Part I, Kruh I. (Chair),symposium conducted at the biennial meeting of the American Psychology±Law Association: NewOrleans, LA.

Poythress NG. Edens JF, Lilienfeld SO. 1998. Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic PersonalityInventory in a prison sample. Psychological Assessment 10: 426±430.

Quinsey VL, Harris GT, Rice ME, Cormier CA. 1998. Violent Offenders: Appraising and Managing Risk.American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.

Juvenile psychopathy and violence 79

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)

Page 28: Assessment of 'juvenile psychopathy' and its …risk-resilience.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/journal...junction with increasing pressure to identify juvenile delinquents who are

Rogers R. 1995. Diagnostic and Structured Interviewing: A Handbook for Psychologists. PAR: Odessa, FL.Rogers R, Johansen J, Chang JJ, Salekin RT. 1997. Predictors of adolescent psychopathy: Oppositional

and conduct-disordered symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 25:261±271.

Salekin RT, Rogers R, Sewell KW. 1996. A review and meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist andPsychopathy Checklist±Revised: Predictive validity of dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: Science andPractice 3: 203±215.

Sandoval AR, Hancock D, Poythress NG, Edens JF, Lilienfeld SO. 2000. Construct validity of thePsychopathic Personality Inventory in a correctional sample. Journal of Personality Assessment 74:262±281.

Selman R. 1980. The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding: Developmental and Clinical Analyses.Academic: New York.

Serin RC, Peters R, Barbaree HE. 1990. Predictors of psychopathy and release outcome in a criminalpopulation. Psychological Assessment 2: 419±422.

Simourd DJ, Hoge RD. 2000. Criminal psychopathy: A risk-and-need perspective. Criminal Justice andBehavior 27: 256±272.

Steinberg L, Cauffman E. 1996. Maturity of judgment in adolescence: Psychosocial factors in adolescentdecision making. Law and Human Behavior 20: 249±272.

Toch, H., 1998. Psychopathy and antisocial personality in forensic settings. In Psychopathy: Antisocial,Criminal and Violent Behavior, Millon T, Simonsen E, Birket-Smith M, Davis R. (eds), Guilford: NewYork; 144±158.

Toupin J, Mercier H, Dery M, Cote G, Hodgins S. 1995. Validity of the PCL-R for adolescents. Issues inCriminological and Legal Psychology 24: 143±145.

Towers KD, Heilbrun KS, Peters L. (2000, August). Decisions to decertify juveniles in Philadelphia:What matters, what doesn't. Paper presented at the 108th annual conference of the AmericanPsychological Association: Washington, DC.

Trevethan SD, Walker LJ. 1989. Hypothetical versus real-life moral reasoning among psychopathic anddelinquent youth. Development and Psychopathology 1: 91±103.

Webster CD, Jackson MA. (eds) 1997. Impulsivity: Theory, Assessment, and Treatment. Guilford: NewYork.

Wootton JM, Frick PJ, Shelton KK, Silverthorn P. 1997. Ineffective parenting and childhood conductproblems: The moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology 65: 301±308.

Zinger I, Forth AE. 1998. Psychopathy and Canadian criminal proceedings: The potential for humanrights abuses. Canadian Journal of Criminology 40: 237±276.

Zuckerman M, Eysenck S, Eysenck HJ. 1978. Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural,age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46: 139±149.

80 J. F. Edens et al.

Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Behav. Sci. Law 19: 53±80 (2001)