behavioral development in wild bottlenose dolphin …€¦ · bottlenose dolphins form temporary...

38
BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS (TURSIOPS SP.) by JANET MANN 1,2) and BARBARA SMUTS 3,4) ( 1 Departments of Psychology & Biology, Georgetown University, Washington DC 20057; 3 Departments of Psychology & Anthropology, 4014 East Hall, 525 East University, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109, USA) (Acc. 22-III-1999) Summary Newborn characteristics, patterns of motoric and social behavioural development, and mother-infant relationships in free-ranging and semi-provisioned bottlenose dolphins (Tur- siops sp.) are examined. Nine newborns were observed for 189 hours over the rst 10 weeks of life. Newborn infants breathe more often than their mothers, and synchronize their breath- ing and swimming with her soon after birth, but show a gradual decline in synchrony as they age. Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour, mother-infant proximity, and spatial relation- ships with the mother changed as a function of infant age. Maternal activity, however, does not change over time, except that mothers decrease their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rst month to the second month of infant life. Infants spend less time 2) Corresponding author; e-mail address: [email protected] 4) We’d like to thank Julie Gros-Louis, Heidi West, Caryanne Cadman, Ferdinand Arcinue, Catherine Samson, Kylie Reid, Scott Salisbury, Jak Ayers, and David Charles with assistance in data collection and entry. This paper was prepared while the rst author was a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioural Sciences with support from the National Science Foundation #SBR-9022192, Helen V. Brach Foundation, Eppley Foundation for Research, the American Association for University Women, the American Cetacean Society, Georgetown University, private donations to The Dolphins of Shark Bay Research Foundation, and a National Science Foundation Presidential Young Investigator Award to BBS (BNS-885769). Our colleagues Richard Connor, Andrew Richards, Amy Samuels, Rachel Smolker, and Peter Tyack gave many helpful comments on this manuscript. Lynn Gale and Lincoln Moses provided statistical advice. Logistical support was provided by Dept. of Human Biology and Anatomy at The University of Western Australia. The Department of Conservation and Land Management in West Australia, the Shire of Shark Bay, The Monkey Mia Resort, Richard Holst, and the rangers of Monkey Mia are gratefully acknowledged. c ® Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 1999 Behaviour 136, 529-566

Upload: others

Post on 25-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSEDOLPHIN NEWBORNS (TURSIOPS SP)

by

JANET MANN12) and BARBARA SMUTS34)

(1 Departments of Psychology amp Biology Georgetown University Washington DC 200573 Departments of Psychology amp Anthropology 4014 East Hall 525 East University The

University of Michigan Ann Arbor MI 48109-1109 USA)

(Acc 22-III-1999)

Summary

Newborn characteristics patterns of motoric and social behavioural development andmother-infant relationships in free-ranging and semi-provisioned bottlenose dolphins (Tur-siops sp) are examined Nine newborns were observed for 189 hours over the rst 10 weeksof life Newborn infants breathe more often than their mothers and synchronize their breath-ing and swimming with her soon after birth but show a gradual decline in synchrony as theyage Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity and spatial relation-ships with the mother changed as a function of infant age Maternal activity however doesnot change over time except that mothers decrease their role in maintaining proximity totheir infants from the rst month to the second month of infant life Infants spend less time

2) Corresponding author e-mail address MannJ2GunetGeorgetownEdu4) Wersquod like to thank Julie Gros-Louis Heidi West Caryanne Cadman Ferdinand ArcinueCatherine Samson Kylie Reid Scott Salisbury Jak Ayers and David Charles with assistancein data collection and entry This paper was prepared while the rst author was aFellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioural Sciences with support fromthe National Science Foundation SBR-9022192 Helen V Brach Foundation EppleyFoundation for Research the American Association for University Women the AmericanCetacean Society Georgetown University private donations to The Dolphins of Shark BayResearch Foundation and a National Science Foundation Presidential Young InvestigatorAward to BBS (BNS-885769) Our colleagues Richard Connor Andrew Richards AmySamuels Rachel Smolker and Peter Tyack gave many helpful comments on this manuscriptLynn Gale and Lincoln Moses provided statistical advice Logistical support was providedby Dept of Human Biology and Anatomy at The University of Western Australia TheDepartment of Conservation and Land Management in West Australia the Shire of SharkBay The Monkey Mia Resort Richard Holst and the rangers of Monkey Mia are gratefullyacknowledged

creg Koninklijke Brill NV Leiden 1999 Behaviour 136 529-566

530 MANN amp SMUTS

close to their mothers less time echelon swimming (close alongside the mother) and moretime infant-positionswimming (in contact under the mother) as they age Infants spend moretime travelingand socializing independentlyover time They also separate from their mothersmore often and for longer periods of time Infants do not forage during the newborn periodbut are observed lsquopractice foragingrsquo by the end of the rst month Rubbing petting chasingand displaying with other animals (including the mother) were common forms of socializingInfants frequently initiate rubbing with their mothers with particular focus on her head re-gion Infants typically associate with young females adult females and other infants but notwith adult or subadult males Developmental shifts and overall patterns are discussed in thecontext of the bottlenosedolphinrsquos ssion-fusionsocial organizationand ecology The effectsof provisioning on dolphin behaviour are addressed

Keywords infants development Bottlenose dolphins Tursiops provisioning play locomo-tion

Introduction

Since 1988 we have conducted longitudinal observations of wild bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) mothers and infants in Shark Bay Western AustraliaThis paper describes behavioural development in nine newborn infants ( rst10 weeks) Using quantitative data derived from focal samples we examinedearly physical characteristics and motor development (diving respirationand mother-infant respiratory synchrony) developmental changes in spatialrelations and mother-infant proximity and maternal and infant activitybudgets including how mothers and infants coordinate their behaviour

Some important physical and behavioural characteristics of newborndolphins have been described in detail for captive animals (McBride amp Hebb1948 McBride amp Kritzler 1951 Essapian 1953 Tavolga amp Essapian 1957Amundin 1986 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987 Reiss 1988Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Peddemors 1990 Reid et al 1995) This studyhowever provides the rst quantitative account of early development andbehaviour in the wild Analyses of newborn development within a naturalcontext is important for this species because of their exceptionally complexsocial structure and unusual combination of ecological and life-history traits

Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and compositionchanges almost continuously as individuals join and leave in a uid manner(Wells et al 1987 Smolker et al 1992) Nevertheless some highly stablepatterns of association exist not only between mothers and infants but alsoamong female kin (Wells et al 1987) adult females (Wells et al 1987Smolker et al 1992) and particularly among adult males who form stable

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 531

alliances engaged in reproductive competition against other male alliances(Connor et al 1992a b) These data along with evidence of elaboratecommunication (Richards et al 1984 Tyack 1986 Janik amp Slater 1997)suggest a social system comparable in complexity to that of great apes theonly other mammalian group in which relative brain size is as large as it isin some delphinids including Tursiops (Connor et al 1992b Smolker etal 1992 Marino 1998) Furthermore like great apes bottlenose dolphinshave very lsquoslowrsquo life histories characterized by late age of sexual maturitylong interbirth intervals and long maximum lifespans (Goodall 1986 Smutset al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Mann et al in press) In contrast thedolphin infantrsquos precocious locomotion post-partum following responseand possible period of maternal imprinting (Mann amp Smuts 1998) closelyresemble the follower strategy characteristic of many ungulates (Ralls et al1987) Finally delphinid foraging ecology may be compared to that of aterrestrial carnivore in which mothers must accelerate rapidly to chase preybut unlike many carnivores dolphins do not cache their offspring

Given this particular combination of social structure life history andecology we would expect selection to favor developmental patterns thatenable the newborn to negotiate some tasks rapidly (such as maintainingproximity to the mother) while other skills (such as those associated withforaging and the development of social bonds) may develop much moreslowly Because adult and immature life history variables are closely related(Charnov 1991) a better understanding of newborn development combinedwith data on behavioural development over the entire period of immaturityshould help to clarify the selective pressures that favored delayed maturationlarge brains and complex cognitive abilities in delphinids

Methods

Study site and population

Our study site incorporates a 130 km2 area east of the Peron Peninsula that bisects SharkBay (25deg 47cent S 113deg 43cent E) Western Australia After a preliminary visit in 1982 (Connor ampSmolker 1985) a longitudinal eld site was established in 1984 By 1998 well over 600dolphins were identi ed and roughly 100 animals are sighted routinely Sex is determinedby views of the genital area or the presence of a dependent calf (adult females) Bottlenosedolphin births peak in the spring and summer (Wells et al 1987 Connor et al 1996) Bothcaptive and wild Tursiops infants have high rates of infant mortality especially during the rst few weeks of life (Sweeney 1977 Cornell et al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Hersh et al1990 Small amp Demaster 1995)

532 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 1 Summary of focal observations

Infant Mother Sex Ages observed Minutes of Minutes of(Wks) observation (PA) observation (non-PA)

Rab Nic M 1-8 2054 1110Nov Hol F 1-10 1206 1968Pep Puc M 1-6 1499 0Hbt Hol F 3-8 80 0Nak Nic M 2-8 90 0Pic Puc F 1-10 1142 0Shd Sur M 1-8 571 0Oyst Alg M lt 2-lt 5 0 450Mout Min F 1-8 0 1190Total 6642 4718

Infant observed both in the provisioning area (PA) and away from the PA (non-PA)t Infant observed away from provisioning area onlyFor the provisioned females date of birth was known within two days For the two non-provisioned females Moursquos exact date of birth was known but Oysrsquos birthdate was estimatedwithin a two-week range based on the last time his mother was observed pregnant and the rsttime she was observed with the infant We could thereforeclassifyOysrsquos early observationsasoccurring during the rst month of life Since later observations could have fallen at the endof the rst month or the early part of the second month of life we classi ed them as occurringduring the second month so as to provide conservative estimates of age effects (comparingmonth 1 to month 2) Fifty-seven percent of observationsoccurred during the month one Novand Pic were observed for 10 hrs during weeks 9 and 10 leaving 179 hrs of observation forthe rst 8 weeks Total hours of observation = 1893 (11360 minutes) 786 hrs away fromthe provisioning area and 1107 hrs in the provisioningarea

Since the early 1960s at least 11 bottlenose dolphins have accepted sh from people andsince the late 1970s these animals have accepted stroking and lsquoseagrass gamesrsquo from touristsstanding in knee-deep water at Monkey Mia a shing camp on the peninsula Provisioningis controlled by rangers who monitor the quality and quantity of sh fed Females of at leasttwo matrilines are fed up to 2 kg sh per day during 2-3 feedings most of which occur inthe morning Dolphins occasionally receive hand-outs from shers although this practice isstrongly discouraged Several mother-newborn infant pairs that visited the provisioning area(PA) served as subjects in this study (see Focal observations below)

This study incorporates 4 eld seasons from 1990-1994 totaling 11 months and 189 hrsof focal observations on 9 infants born to 6 mothers (Table 1 of the 9 calves in this studyonly 4 survived past their rst year Those four survived to weaning at ages 3-6) Three ofthe four provisioned adult females that regularly visit the provisioning area (Nic Hol Puc)were observed with two newborn infants the fourth regular visitor (Sur) was observed withone infant The newborn infants of two non-provisioned females were also included in thisstudy All mothers in this study were multiparous with known previous offspring except forone (Sur) who gave birth to her rst offspring (Shd) during the latter part of study Only two

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 533

infants were observed at nine and ten weeks of age (for 10 focal hours) so these data wereexcluded from most of the analyses (See Table 1)

Focal observations provisioning area (PA)

For the mother-infant study as a whole most observations were collected during focalobservations away from the provisioning area obtained by following individuals in smallboats (4-5 m dinghies equipped with 6-15 hp motors) for up to 10 hours at a time (Smolker etal 1993 Mann amp Smuts 1998) However more than half of the data presented in this paperwere collected on the provisioned females and their newborns when they were in or near theprovisioning area (Table 1 Fig 1) These data termed ldquoPArdquo focal observations are includedin this study because reliabledata on newborns require accurate estimatesof birthdateswhichwere easily obtained for provisioned mothers compared to non-provisioned mothers whowere seen less often Daily observations of provisioned mothers and their infants allowed usto record neonatal behavioural and physical development in almost as much detail as one canin captivity

PA observations typically started soon after mother and calf came into the provisioningarea (Fig 1) Observations conducted from the jetty were never started during feeds or rightafter feeds because the dolphins typically left the provisioning area immediately followingfeeds Focal samples typically lasted one-half hour although some were shorter if thedolphins left the provisioning area A combination of point sampling event and continuous

Fig 1 The provisioning area (not to scale) at Monkey Mia Shark Bay Tourists typicallystand at the edge of the 90 m beach between the jetty and buoy marker The jetty height (1-3 m above sea level depending on tides) allows observers to view dolphins within a 200 mradius of the beach Occasional boat-begging by the provisioned dolphins occurs at the ramp

east of the jetty

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 2: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

530 MANN amp SMUTS

close to their mothers less time echelon swimming (close alongside the mother) and moretime infant-positionswimming (in contact under the mother) as they age Infants spend moretime travelingand socializing independentlyover time They also separate from their mothersmore often and for longer periods of time Infants do not forage during the newborn periodbut are observed lsquopractice foragingrsquo by the end of the rst month Rubbing petting chasingand displaying with other animals (including the mother) were common forms of socializingInfants frequently initiate rubbing with their mothers with particular focus on her head re-gion Infants typically associate with young females adult females and other infants but notwith adult or subadult males Developmental shifts and overall patterns are discussed in thecontext of the bottlenosedolphinrsquos ssion-fusionsocial organizationand ecology The effectsof provisioning on dolphin behaviour are addressed

Keywords infants development Bottlenose dolphins Tursiops provisioning play locomo-tion

Introduction

Since 1988 we have conducted longitudinal observations of wild bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) mothers and infants in Shark Bay Western AustraliaThis paper describes behavioural development in nine newborn infants ( rst10 weeks) Using quantitative data derived from focal samples we examinedearly physical characteristics and motor development (diving respirationand mother-infant respiratory synchrony) developmental changes in spatialrelations and mother-infant proximity and maternal and infant activitybudgets including how mothers and infants coordinate their behaviour

Some important physical and behavioural characteristics of newborndolphins have been described in detail for captive animals (McBride amp Hebb1948 McBride amp Kritzler 1951 Essapian 1953 Tavolga amp Essapian 1957Amundin 1986 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987 Reiss 1988Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Peddemors 1990 Reid et al 1995) This studyhowever provides the rst quantitative account of early development andbehaviour in the wild Analyses of newborn development within a naturalcontext is important for this species because of their exceptionally complexsocial structure and unusual combination of ecological and life-history traits

Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and compositionchanges almost continuously as individuals join and leave in a uid manner(Wells et al 1987 Smolker et al 1992) Nevertheless some highly stablepatterns of association exist not only between mothers and infants but alsoamong female kin (Wells et al 1987) adult females (Wells et al 1987Smolker et al 1992) and particularly among adult males who form stable

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 531

alliances engaged in reproductive competition against other male alliances(Connor et al 1992a b) These data along with evidence of elaboratecommunication (Richards et al 1984 Tyack 1986 Janik amp Slater 1997)suggest a social system comparable in complexity to that of great apes theonly other mammalian group in which relative brain size is as large as it isin some delphinids including Tursiops (Connor et al 1992b Smolker etal 1992 Marino 1998) Furthermore like great apes bottlenose dolphinshave very lsquoslowrsquo life histories characterized by late age of sexual maturitylong interbirth intervals and long maximum lifespans (Goodall 1986 Smutset al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Mann et al in press) In contrast thedolphin infantrsquos precocious locomotion post-partum following responseand possible period of maternal imprinting (Mann amp Smuts 1998) closelyresemble the follower strategy characteristic of many ungulates (Ralls et al1987) Finally delphinid foraging ecology may be compared to that of aterrestrial carnivore in which mothers must accelerate rapidly to chase preybut unlike many carnivores dolphins do not cache their offspring

Given this particular combination of social structure life history andecology we would expect selection to favor developmental patterns thatenable the newborn to negotiate some tasks rapidly (such as maintainingproximity to the mother) while other skills (such as those associated withforaging and the development of social bonds) may develop much moreslowly Because adult and immature life history variables are closely related(Charnov 1991) a better understanding of newborn development combinedwith data on behavioural development over the entire period of immaturityshould help to clarify the selective pressures that favored delayed maturationlarge brains and complex cognitive abilities in delphinids

Methods

Study site and population

Our study site incorporates a 130 km2 area east of the Peron Peninsula that bisects SharkBay (25deg 47cent S 113deg 43cent E) Western Australia After a preliminary visit in 1982 (Connor ampSmolker 1985) a longitudinal eld site was established in 1984 By 1998 well over 600dolphins were identi ed and roughly 100 animals are sighted routinely Sex is determinedby views of the genital area or the presence of a dependent calf (adult females) Bottlenosedolphin births peak in the spring and summer (Wells et al 1987 Connor et al 1996) Bothcaptive and wild Tursiops infants have high rates of infant mortality especially during the rst few weeks of life (Sweeney 1977 Cornell et al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Hersh et al1990 Small amp Demaster 1995)

532 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 1 Summary of focal observations

Infant Mother Sex Ages observed Minutes of Minutes of(Wks) observation (PA) observation (non-PA)

Rab Nic M 1-8 2054 1110Nov Hol F 1-10 1206 1968Pep Puc M 1-6 1499 0Hbt Hol F 3-8 80 0Nak Nic M 2-8 90 0Pic Puc F 1-10 1142 0Shd Sur M 1-8 571 0Oyst Alg M lt 2-lt 5 0 450Mout Min F 1-8 0 1190Total 6642 4718

Infant observed both in the provisioning area (PA) and away from the PA (non-PA)t Infant observed away from provisioning area onlyFor the provisioned females date of birth was known within two days For the two non-provisioned females Moursquos exact date of birth was known but Oysrsquos birthdate was estimatedwithin a two-week range based on the last time his mother was observed pregnant and the rsttime she was observed with the infant We could thereforeclassifyOysrsquos early observationsasoccurring during the rst month of life Since later observations could have fallen at the endof the rst month or the early part of the second month of life we classi ed them as occurringduring the second month so as to provide conservative estimates of age effects (comparingmonth 1 to month 2) Fifty-seven percent of observationsoccurred during the month one Novand Pic were observed for 10 hrs during weeks 9 and 10 leaving 179 hrs of observation forthe rst 8 weeks Total hours of observation = 1893 (11360 minutes) 786 hrs away fromthe provisioning area and 1107 hrs in the provisioningarea

Since the early 1960s at least 11 bottlenose dolphins have accepted sh from people andsince the late 1970s these animals have accepted stroking and lsquoseagrass gamesrsquo from touristsstanding in knee-deep water at Monkey Mia a shing camp on the peninsula Provisioningis controlled by rangers who monitor the quality and quantity of sh fed Females of at leasttwo matrilines are fed up to 2 kg sh per day during 2-3 feedings most of which occur inthe morning Dolphins occasionally receive hand-outs from shers although this practice isstrongly discouraged Several mother-newborn infant pairs that visited the provisioning area(PA) served as subjects in this study (see Focal observations below)

This study incorporates 4 eld seasons from 1990-1994 totaling 11 months and 189 hrsof focal observations on 9 infants born to 6 mothers (Table 1 of the 9 calves in this studyonly 4 survived past their rst year Those four survived to weaning at ages 3-6) Three ofthe four provisioned adult females that regularly visit the provisioning area (Nic Hol Puc)were observed with two newborn infants the fourth regular visitor (Sur) was observed withone infant The newborn infants of two non-provisioned females were also included in thisstudy All mothers in this study were multiparous with known previous offspring except forone (Sur) who gave birth to her rst offspring (Shd) during the latter part of study Only two

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 533

infants were observed at nine and ten weeks of age (for 10 focal hours) so these data wereexcluded from most of the analyses (See Table 1)

Focal observations provisioning area (PA)

For the mother-infant study as a whole most observations were collected during focalobservations away from the provisioning area obtained by following individuals in smallboats (4-5 m dinghies equipped with 6-15 hp motors) for up to 10 hours at a time (Smolker etal 1993 Mann amp Smuts 1998) However more than half of the data presented in this paperwere collected on the provisioned females and their newborns when they were in or near theprovisioning area (Table 1 Fig 1) These data termed ldquoPArdquo focal observations are includedin this study because reliabledata on newborns require accurate estimatesof birthdateswhichwere easily obtained for provisioned mothers compared to non-provisioned mothers whowere seen less often Daily observations of provisioned mothers and their infants allowed usto record neonatal behavioural and physical development in almost as much detail as one canin captivity

PA observations typically started soon after mother and calf came into the provisioningarea (Fig 1) Observations conducted from the jetty were never started during feeds or rightafter feeds because the dolphins typically left the provisioning area immediately followingfeeds Focal samples typically lasted one-half hour although some were shorter if thedolphins left the provisioning area A combination of point sampling event and continuous

Fig 1 The provisioning area (not to scale) at Monkey Mia Shark Bay Tourists typicallystand at the edge of the 90 m beach between the jetty and buoy marker The jetty height (1-3 m above sea level depending on tides) allows observers to view dolphins within a 200 mradius of the beach Occasional boat-begging by the provisioned dolphins occurs at the ramp

east of the jetty

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 3: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 531

alliances engaged in reproductive competition against other male alliances(Connor et al 1992a b) These data along with evidence of elaboratecommunication (Richards et al 1984 Tyack 1986 Janik amp Slater 1997)suggest a social system comparable in complexity to that of great apes theonly other mammalian group in which relative brain size is as large as it isin some delphinids including Tursiops (Connor et al 1992b Smolker etal 1992 Marino 1998) Furthermore like great apes bottlenose dolphinshave very lsquoslowrsquo life histories characterized by late age of sexual maturitylong interbirth intervals and long maximum lifespans (Goodall 1986 Smutset al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Mann et al in press) In contrast thedolphin infantrsquos precocious locomotion post-partum following responseand possible period of maternal imprinting (Mann amp Smuts 1998) closelyresemble the follower strategy characteristic of many ungulates (Ralls et al1987) Finally delphinid foraging ecology may be compared to that of aterrestrial carnivore in which mothers must accelerate rapidly to chase preybut unlike many carnivores dolphins do not cache their offspring

Given this particular combination of social structure life history andecology we would expect selection to favor developmental patterns thatenable the newborn to negotiate some tasks rapidly (such as maintainingproximity to the mother) while other skills (such as those associated withforaging and the development of social bonds) may develop much moreslowly Because adult and immature life history variables are closely related(Charnov 1991) a better understanding of newborn development combinedwith data on behavioural development over the entire period of immaturityshould help to clarify the selective pressures that favored delayed maturationlarge brains and complex cognitive abilities in delphinids

Methods

Study site and population

Our study site incorporates a 130 km2 area east of the Peron Peninsula that bisects SharkBay (25deg 47cent S 113deg 43cent E) Western Australia After a preliminary visit in 1982 (Connor ampSmolker 1985) a longitudinal eld site was established in 1984 By 1998 well over 600dolphins were identi ed and roughly 100 animals are sighted routinely Sex is determinedby views of the genital area or the presence of a dependent calf (adult females) Bottlenosedolphin births peak in the spring and summer (Wells et al 1987 Connor et al 1996) Bothcaptive and wild Tursiops infants have high rates of infant mortality especially during the rst few weeks of life (Sweeney 1977 Cornell et al 1987 Wells et al 1987 Hersh et al1990 Small amp Demaster 1995)

532 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 1 Summary of focal observations

Infant Mother Sex Ages observed Minutes of Minutes of(Wks) observation (PA) observation (non-PA)

Rab Nic M 1-8 2054 1110Nov Hol F 1-10 1206 1968Pep Puc M 1-6 1499 0Hbt Hol F 3-8 80 0Nak Nic M 2-8 90 0Pic Puc F 1-10 1142 0Shd Sur M 1-8 571 0Oyst Alg M lt 2-lt 5 0 450Mout Min F 1-8 0 1190Total 6642 4718

Infant observed both in the provisioning area (PA) and away from the PA (non-PA)t Infant observed away from provisioning area onlyFor the provisioned females date of birth was known within two days For the two non-provisioned females Moursquos exact date of birth was known but Oysrsquos birthdate was estimatedwithin a two-week range based on the last time his mother was observed pregnant and the rsttime she was observed with the infant We could thereforeclassifyOysrsquos early observationsasoccurring during the rst month of life Since later observations could have fallen at the endof the rst month or the early part of the second month of life we classi ed them as occurringduring the second month so as to provide conservative estimates of age effects (comparingmonth 1 to month 2) Fifty-seven percent of observationsoccurred during the month one Novand Pic were observed for 10 hrs during weeks 9 and 10 leaving 179 hrs of observation forthe rst 8 weeks Total hours of observation = 1893 (11360 minutes) 786 hrs away fromthe provisioning area and 1107 hrs in the provisioningarea

Since the early 1960s at least 11 bottlenose dolphins have accepted sh from people andsince the late 1970s these animals have accepted stroking and lsquoseagrass gamesrsquo from touristsstanding in knee-deep water at Monkey Mia a shing camp on the peninsula Provisioningis controlled by rangers who monitor the quality and quantity of sh fed Females of at leasttwo matrilines are fed up to 2 kg sh per day during 2-3 feedings most of which occur inthe morning Dolphins occasionally receive hand-outs from shers although this practice isstrongly discouraged Several mother-newborn infant pairs that visited the provisioning area(PA) served as subjects in this study (see Focal observations below)

This study incorporates 4 eld seasons from 1990-1994 totaling 11 months and 189 hrsof focal observations on 9 infants born to 6 mothers (Table 1 of the 9 calves in this studyonly 4 survived past their rst year Those four survived to weaning at ages 3-6) Three ofthe four provisioned adult females that regularly visit the provisioning area (Nic Hol Puc)were observed with two newborn infants the fourth regular visitor (Sur) was observed withone infant The newborn infants of two non-provisioned females were also included in thisstudy All mothers in this study were multiparous with known previous offspring except forone (Sur) who gave birth to her rst offspring (Shd) during the latter part of study Only two

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 533

infants were observed at nine and ten weeks of age (for 10 focal hours) so these data wereexcluded from most of the analyses (See Table 1)

Focal observations provisioning area (PA)

For the mother-infant study as a whole most observations were collected during focalobservations away from the provisioning area obtained by following individuals in smallboats (4-5 m dinghies equipped with 6-15 hp motors) for up to 10 hours at a time (Smolker etal 1993 Mann amp Smuts 1998) However more than half of the data presented in this paperwere collected on the provisioned females and their newborns when they were in or near theprovisioning area (Table 1 Fig 1) These data termed ldquoPArdquo focal observations are includedin this study because reliabledata on newborns require accurate estimatesof birthdateswhichwere easily obtained for provisioned mothers compared to non-provisioned mothers whowere seen less often Daily observations of provisioned mothers and their infants allowed usto record neonatal behavioural and physical development in almost as much detail as one canin captivity

PA observations typically started soon after mother and calf came into the provisioningarea (Fig 1) Observations conducted from the jetty were never started during feeds or rightafter feeds because the dolphins typically left the provisioning area immediately followingfeeds Focal samples typically lasted one-half hour although some were shorter if thedolphins left the provisioning area A combination of point sampling event and continuous

Fig 1 The provisioning area (not to scale) at Monkey Mia Shark Bay Tourists typicallystand at the edge of the 90 m beach between the jetty and buoy marker The jetty height (1-3 m above sea level depending on tides) allows observers to view dolphins within a 200 mradius of the beach Occasional boat-begging by the provisioned dolphins occurs at the ramp

east of the jetty

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 4: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

532 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 1 Summary of focal observations

Infant Mother Sex Ages observed Minutes of Minutes of(Wks) observation (PA) observation (non-PA)

Rab Nic M 1-8 2054 1110Nov Hol F 1-10 1206 1968Pep Puc M 1-6 1499 0Hbt Hol F 3-8 80 0Nak Nic M 2-8 90 0Pic Puc F 1-10 1142 0Shd Sur M 1-8 571 0Oyst Alg M lt 2-lt 5 0 450Mout Min F 1-8 0 1190Total 6642 4718

Infant observed both in the provisioning area (PA) and away from the PA (non-PA)t Infant observed away from provisioning area onlyFor the provisioned females date of birth was known within two days For the two non-provisioned females Moursquos exact date of birth was known but Oysrsquos birthdate was estimatedwithin a two-week range based on the last time his mother was observed pregnant and the rsttime she was observed with the infant We could thereforeclassifyOysrsquos early observationsasoccurring during the rst month of life Since later observations could have fallen at the endof the rst month or the early part of the second month of life we classi ed them as occurringduring the second month so as to provide conservative estimates of age effects (comparingmonth 1 to month 2) Fifty-seven percent of observationsoccurred during the month one Novand Pic were observed for 10 hrs during weeks 9 and 10 leaving 179 hrs of observation forthe rst 8 weeks Total hours of observation = 1893 (11360 minutes) 786 hrs away fromthe provisioning area and 1107 hrs in the provisioningarea

Since the early 1960s at least 11 bottlenose dolphins have accepted sh from people andsince the late 1970s these animals have accepted stroking and lsquoseagrass gamesrsquo from touristsstanding in knee-deep water at Monkey Mia a shing camp on the peninsula Provisioningis controlled by rangers who monitor the quality and quantity of sh fed Females of at leasttwo matrilines are fed up to 2 kg sh per day during 2-3 feedings most of which occur inthe morning Dolphins occasionally receive hand-outs from shers although this practice isstrongly discouraged Several mother-newborn infant pairs that visited the provisioning area(PA) served as subjects in this study (see Focal observations below)

This study incorporates 4 eld seasons from 1990-1994 totaling 11 months and 189 hrsof focal observations on 9 infants born to 6 mothers (Table 1 of the 9 calves in this studyonly 4 survived past their rst year Those four survived to weaning at ages 3-6) Three ofthe four provisioned adult females that regularly visit the provisioning area (Nic Hol Puc)were observed with two newborn infants the fourth regular visitor (Sur) was observed withone infant The newborn infants of two non-provisioned females were also included in thisstudy All mothers in this study were multiparous with known previous offspring except forone (Sur) who gave birth to her rst offspring (Shd) during the latter part of study Only two

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 533

infants were observed at nine and ten weeks of age (for 10 focal hours) so these data wereexcluded from most of the analyses (See Table 1)

Focal observations provisioning area (PA)

For the mother-infant study as a whole most observations were collected during focalobservations away from the provisioning area obtained by following individuals in smallboats (4-5 m dinghies equipped with 6-15 hp motors) for up to 10 hours at a time (Smolker etal 1993 Mann amp Smuts 1998) However more than half of the data presented in this paperwere collected on the provisioned females and their newborns when they were in or near theprovisioning area (Table 1 Fig 1) These data termed ldquoPArdquo focal observations are includedin this study because reliabledata on newborns require accurate estimatesof birthdateswhichwere easily obtained for provisioned mothers compared to non-provisioned mothers whowere seen less often Daily observations of provisioned mothers and their infants allowed usto record neonatal behavioural and physical development in almost as much detail as one canin captivity

PA observations typically started soon after mother and calf came into the provisioningarea (Fig 1) Observations conducted from the jetty were never started during feeds or rightafter feeds because the dolphins typically left the provisioning area immediately followingfeeds Focal samples typically lasted one-half hour although some were shorter if thedolphins left the provisioning area A combination of point sampling event and continuous

Fig 1 The provisioning area (not to scale) at Monkey Mia Shark Bay Tourists typicallystand at the edge of the 90 m beach between the jetty and buoy marker The jetty height (1-3 m above sea level depending on tides) allows observers to view dolphins within a 200 mradius of the beach Occasional boat-begging by the provisioned dolphins occurs at the ramp

east of the jetty

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 5: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 533

infants were observed at nine and ten weeks of age (for 10 focal hours) so these data wereexcluded from most of the analyses (See Table 1)

Focal observations provisioning area (PA)

For the mother-infant study as a whole most observations were collected during focalobservations away from the provisioning area obtained by following individuals in smallboats (4-5 m dinghies equipped with 6-15 hp motors) for up to 10 hours at a time (Smolker etal 1993 Mann amp Smuts 1998) However more than half of the data presented in this paperwere collected on the provisioned females and their newborns when they were in or near theprovisioning area (Table 1 Fig 1) These data termed ldquoPArdquo focal observations are includedin this study because reliabledata on newborns require accurate estimatesof birthdateswhichwere easily obtained for provisioned mothers compared to non-provisioned mothers whowere seen less often Daily observations of provisioned mothers and their infants allowed usto record neonatal behavioural and physical development in almost as much detail as one canin captivity

PA observations typically started soon after mother and calf came into the provisioningarea (Fig 1) Observations conducted from the jetty were never started during feeds or rightafter feeds because the dolphins typically left the provisioning area immediately followingfeeds Focal samples typically lasted one-half hour although some were shorter if thedolphins left the provisioning area A combination of point sampling event and continuous

Fig 1 The provisioning area (not to scale) at Monkey Mia Shark Bay Tourists typicallystand at the edge of the 90 m beach between the jetty and buoy marker The jetty height (1-3 m above sea level depending on tides) allows observers to view dolphins within a 200 mradius of the beach Occasional boat-begging by the provisioned dolphins occurs at the ramp

east of the jetty

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 6: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

534 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 2 Ethogram

Behavior De nition

Rest Slow travel lt 2 mph frequent oating at surface frequentdirection changes

Travel Straight movement gt 2 mph (not scored near PA except whenarriving or leaving the PA)

Infant position swimming Infant swims under mother melon or head lightly touchesmotherrsquos abdomen Upon surfacing to breathe the infant breaksinfant position only for a couple of seconds then angles back un-der the mother Infant position swimming can be determined bythe infantrsquos surfacing position slightly behind and lt 05 m fromthe mother

Echelon swimming Also referred to as contact swimming when the calf is closelt 30 cm alongside them other mdash roughly parallel touching themotherrsquos ank above midline

Forage Rapid surfacing frequent direction changes fast swims shchases observationsof sh catches

Snack forage Foraging characterizedby belly-up swimming near water surfacePrey are typically trapped at surface

Begpeople interaction Dolphinrsquos head lt 2 m from and oriented towards boat or personis within 2 m of dolphin in PA People interaction also includedtouching

Social Physical contact with conspeci cs including petting rubbingmounting chases genital inspections gooses pokes play bond-ing displays

Petting Type of lsquogroomingrsquo where one dolphin moves hisher pectoral nor ukes on body of another

Rubbing Gentle body-to-body contact where one individual rubs a bodypart on another conspeci c

Genital inspections Rostrum near genital area of conspeci c and makes light or nocontact to genital area

Goose Rostrum makes hard contact with genital area of conspeci cPoke Rostrum makes contact with body part (not genital area) of

conspeci cDisplays Surface displays that involve slapping a body part (belly face

jaw pectoral n dorsal side) on the water surface also spyhops (bring head vertically out of water perpendicular to watersurface)

Play Involves chasing displays occasionally carrying objects such asseagrass

Aggressive behavior Hits with tail ukes pectoral ns head head jerks (sharp lateralmovement of head) open jaw

Nursing Rostrum in contact with mammary slit for gt 2 sApproach Animal swims within 2 m radius of another

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 7: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 535

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Behavior De nition

Leave Animal departs from 2 m radius of anotherBreath Determined by seeing the blowhole open at surface the resulting

water spray or hearing the breathSurfacing bout Breath series in-between divesPeduncle dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent but tail ukes

do not break surfaceTail-out dive Dolphin at surface arches tail-stock before descent and tail ukes

clear water surface

sampling was used to record maternal and infant activity the distance between mother andinfant and approaches and leaves within the pair (Table 2) All animals in the vicinity(lt 200 m radius from the PA beach) were scored as present and potential associates of theinfant during each sample However nearest neighbors of the infant were scored at each pointsample only if they were within 10 m For two infants (Nak Hbt) behaviour and distancefrom the mother were coded from videotape their approachleave data were not included inthe analysis due to small sample sizes For one infant (Pep) observationswere scheduled for15 minute intervals and certain types of data were not collected (infant and maternal activityand associates in provisioningarea)

For two infants (Nov Rab) we periodically (1-2 times per week) counted exact breathrates of mother and infant for 10 minute periods in order to determine whether infantsbreathed more often than their mothers

Focal observations non-provisioningarea (non-PA)

Focal samples of infants away from the provisioningarea began as soon as the motherinfantpair was located and lasted for as long as we were able to stay with them or until they returnedto the provisioning beach (average follow was 3 hrs) Typical dolphin-boat distance rangedfrom 2-20 m in 57 plusmn 12 m water Visibility into the water from the boat typically variedbetween 2-8 m For behavior de nitions see Table 2

Four types of observational methods were used (Altmann 1974) We employed contin-uous sampling (all observed occurrences of a behaviour are recorded and duration andorfrequenciesare maintained in the sampling record) to record the durations of surfacing bouts oating at the surface and snack foraging the proportion of time that infants spent in thelsquoinfantrsquo or lsquoechelonrsquo positions synchronous breathssurfacings and those types of social in-teractions that generally lasted for at least several seconds occurred at the surface such thatwe could reliably detect each occurrence (eg displays rubbing petting) We conducted in-stantaneous samples (at ve-minute intervals) of party direction party composition distancebetween mother and infant identitiesof nearest neighbors depth of water speed (when track-ing) and miles traveled

Predominant activity sampling (PAS activity must occur for a minimum of 50 of aprede ned interval to be considered a predominant activity) was used to record predominantdirection predominant speed (six classi cations) and maternal and infant activity Thisconvention of interval sampling has demonstrated validity (Tyler 1979 Mann 1999)

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 8: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

536 MANN amp SMUTS

Finally we used ad libitum sampling to record behaviours such as nursing that typicallyhave very short durations such that they can only be observed opportunistically Ad libitumdata provided descriptive information outside of focal samples The rangers also providedus with details about events in the provisioning area that we considered ad libitum Whereappropriate ad libitum data are indicated For some rare behaviors we describe their rstoccurrences but do not present rates or durations

Party composition was de ned using a 10 meter chain rule where for any animal whosurfaces within 10 meters of any other animal within the party is considered a party member

Analyses

To determine the effects of provisioning we used permutation tests to compare PA andnon-PA group size association maternal and infant behaviour and spatial relationshipsWe combine non-PA observations of provisioned and non-provisioned mothers and theiroffspring and contrast them to PA observations (see Table 3)

Age comparisons involved two types of analysis First to look at rapid changes in earlydevelopment we compared the rst week of life to weeks 2-8 for convenience we refer tothis comparison as lsquoweek one vs laterrsquo Second to look at broader changes the rst month(wks 1-4) was compared to the second month of life (wks 5-8) we refer to this comparisonas lsquomonth one vs tworsquo Behaviour durations frequencies and point samples were collapsedacross weeks and converted to rates or proportions Non-parametric tests were used andall probabilities were two-tailed Means plusmn standard deviations are presented for descriptivepurposes For both age comparisons we increased the sample size by combining non-PAand PA data (for Rab and Nov who were observed both in and away from the provisioningarea we included only their non-PA data) Three infants were excluded from the weeklycomparisons (Hbt and Nak were not observed during week one and Oys was eliminated dueto uncertainty about the exact date of birth) Because the sample sizes vary for differentanalyses we include a summary table of signi cant ndings with associated sample sizes(see Table 5)

The Hinde Index (Hinde amp Atkinson 1970) was used to calculatewho was responsibleformaintaining proximity within each mother-infant dyad The Hinde Index (HI) is determinedby the proportion of approaches (A) and leaves (L) that are initiated by the mother (M) andinfant (i) (HI = [AmAm + Ai] shy [LmLm + Li]) The index ranges from shy 10 whenthe infant is totally responsible for maintaining proximity to + 10 when the mother istotally responsible for maintaining proximity An index close to 0 would indicate equalresponsibility ApproachLeave data were collected continuously at the provisioning areafor 5 infants but were collected ad libitum away from the provisioning area (within focalsamples) Only one non-provisioned infant (Mou) had enough non-PA approachleave data(gt 20 approaches and leaves) to include in the analysis

Results

Description of physical characteristics

At birth Shark Bay Tursiops have 5-7 distinctive fetal folds and white (non-pigmented) lines called fetal lines at each crease Fetal folds or lumpiness

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 9: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 537

are pronounced during the rst week after birth but may not be detectableduring the second week (Hbt Nak Rab) However fetal lines resulting fromthe folds remain for many weeks Their visibility can depend on lightingconditions individual differences and perhaps the health of the animal Forone infant fetal lines remained visible through week 6 for another throughweek 7 and for a third through week 10 One small emaciated infant whowas eventually killed by a shark had very faint fetal lines visible until herdeath at 110 days of age (Mann amp Barnett 1999) In summary fetal lines areclearly visible in month one sometimes visible in month two and barely ornot visible in month three

Newborn coloration varies Some infants are born a very light greywhereas others are nearly black in appearance The underside tends to bewhite or slightly pink The pink coloration typically disappears within afew months Teeth erupt by the 10th or 11th week (Nak Hbt) but may bepresent earlier Rostral whiskers were visible for 3-4 days post-partum ontwo infants but not thereafter

Mother-infant behaviour in provisioning area

All of the provisioned mothers visited the PA within two days after givingbirth During their rst two days in the PA mothers swam rapidly in deeperwater with the newborn avoiding the line of people standing close toshore The rangers kept tourists from touching the mother and calf or fromventuring any deeper than approximately 2 ft of water During feedings in the rst few days after birth rangers waded into deeper water and often tossedthe sh to the mother rather than feeding by hand Mothers with newbornswere offered more than their 2 kg per day quota (up to 4 kg per day) but asof 1994 the practice of feeding extra to new mothers ceased By the thirdday mothers came closer to tourists and would take sh by hand while thenewborn swam in circles a meter or two away As soon as the feed was overmothers and their calves left the PA (ad libitum notes)

Mothers typically kept themselves between the line of tourists and theirnewborns sometimes rushing between them if the infant wandered too closeThey appeared to be herding or steering their infants away from the beachand tourists For example on the rst day that Nic brought Rab into thebeach area she herded him away from the people 6 times per hour Motherswere not always effective however Several newborns (Rab Nov Pic Pep

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 10: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

538 MANN amp SMUTS

Shd) temporarily beached themselves during the rst few days of life butwere quickly moved back into the water by the rangers One mother (Puc)knocked a tourist down in an effort to herd her calf (Pic) away from the beach(ad libitum notes)

Newborns were never observed interacting directly with people but theirmothers occasionally did During the rst week mothers rarely interactedwith people outside of feedings but during subsequent weeks they occasion-ally did so (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks wk one vs later p lt 007)Some of their interactions were aggressive and involved hitting (with tails ipper or jaw) or threatening (head jerked at) people attempting to touchthem For example the most aggressive mother engaged in at least 23 ag-gressive interactions in the PA over the course of two months (07 times perhour) Nine (39) of those were clearly directed at humans For all provi-sioned females pooled the rate of aggressive behaviour was 026 times perhour (compared to 001 times per hr non-PA) When in the PA mothers spentthe greatest proportion of their time within 2 m of people (499 plusmn 246 Ta-ble 3) Direct touching feeding or interacting with humans occurred for amuch smaller proportion of the time (02 plusmn 036)

During the rst week of infant life provisioned mothers continued tochase and beg from incoming boats as their newborns followed By thesecond week the calves typically rested or oated a few meters away fromthe beach while their mothers stayed close to the people or begged fromincoming boats Mothers no longer herded their infants away from boats orpeople The newborns now kept their distance (ad libitum notes)

The effects of provisioning on mother-infant behaviour and associations

Maternal behaviour at the provisioning area differed from that away from itin several respects (Table 3) Away from the PA under natural conditionsmothers foraged and socialized more often than in the PA (permutationtests p = 003) In the PA mothers spent roughly 50 of their timeattending to people or boats at close range (lt 2 m) Even away from thePA provisioned mothers occasionally begged from boats but we neverobserved non-provisioned mothers approaching boats Travel data could notbe compared because traveling mothers rapidly left the provisioning area

For infants the clearest behavioural differences between the PA and non-PA involved echelon swimming Echelon swimming was common away

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 11: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 539

TABLE 3 Differences in association and behaviour in non-provisioning andprovisioning areas

Non-PA (N = 4) PA (N = 7)

Mean SD Mean SD p =

Maternal behaviourRest 4852 778 4678 2376 045Travel 1917 795 na na ndashSocialize 246 213 026 048 003Forage 2746 1257 310 405 003Peopleboats 239 358 4987 2459 003

Infant behaviourRest 2070 799 4992 548 073Travel 826 520 na na ndashSocialize 795 419 1532 2090 077Forage 0 0 0 0 ndashPeopleboats 0 0 0 0 ndashEchelon swim 5210 1750 2742 1872 003Infant position swim 1100 562 691 461 020

Mother-infant proximitylt 2 m 8873 833 7851 1170 0022-10 m 749 431 1784 1185 002gt 10 m 207 199 364 362 019

Party compositionMother-Infant only 4441 724 798 1087 003Female 4582 592 8013 1707 003Mixed 617 547 729 1175 077Mother infant immatures 151 119 0 0 ndashInfant alone 136 108 0 0 ndashInfant ad males 012 025 0 0 ndashInfant female 052 038 0 0 ndashInfant immatures 013 026 0 0 ndash

Party size1 088 117 0 0 ndash2 4599 846 798 1087 0033 to 5 2597 1181 3921 1422 0106 to 8 1888 1386 4934 1753 0039 to 12 513 448 347 451 093gt 13 210 215 0 0 003

All values represent time engaged in the behaviour or in those types of groupsndash Too few data to run testPermutation tests are non-parametric to estimate probabilities of obtaining a particulardistribution of ranks

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 12: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

540 MANN amp SMUTS

from the PA during the rst two months of life (occupying on average521 of the infantrsquos time) but was not so prevalent in the provisioning area(x = 274) Other infant behaviours did not differ signi cantly betweenthe PA and non-PA

Provisioning had obvious consequences for mother-infant proximity In-fants spent less time in close proximity (lt 2 m) and more time further away(2-10 m) in the PA compared to non-PA (Table 3) In the provisioning areamothers often stayed near the tourists standing in shallow water while theirinfants circled several meters away in deeper water

Differences in party composition must be considered in light of differ-ences in de nition of party composition in and away from the PA In the PAanimals did not form tight cohesive parties thus we de ned associates morebroadly as including any animal observed within 200 m of the provision-ing area Based on these somewhat different criteria associations betweenthe mother-infant dyad and other dolphins differed in the two contexts Forexample mothers and infants were often alone when away from the PA (av-eraging 444 of the time) but infrequently so in the PA (averaging 80of the time) Because feedings typically occurred in the morning and therangers waited until most of the regularly provisioned females showed upbefore feeding all the provisioned females tended to show up around thesame time and all tended to leave right after the feeding Thus in the PAlsquofemalersquo parties (de ned as mother infant and at least one adult female)were the norm (801 of the time vs 459 of the time non-PA) and theseparties often (459 of the time) included 6-8 individuals (3-4 provisionedmothers and their infants) whereas away from the provisioning area partiesof 6-8 occurred only 189 of the time Very large groups (gt 13 animals)were more common away from the provisioning area than in the provision-ing area despite the larger radius used to de ne party composition in theprovisioning area

The total number of associates differed signi cantly between the PA andnon-PA (permutation test p = 003) In the PA infants had 7-19 associatesin total during the rst two months of life (N = 6) but away from the PAinfants had 29-37 in total (N = 4) Even though the provisioning data werecollected over the course of four years the same 24 individuals tended tovisit the provisioning area and they accounted for all associations in thatarea During non-PA observations of newborns however we recorded 71different associates over the course of two consecutive eld season years

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 13: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 541

Developmental changes

I Locomotion respiration and diving and breathing synchronyFor the results described in this sub-section only non-PA data were usedwith one exception Exact respiration rates were observed for two mothersand their infants in the provisioning area

Speed All infants were observed swimming rapidly (gt 2 mph) with theirmothers during the rst two days after birth By the third day traveling wasnoticeably slower From then on infants and mothers tend to swim lt 2 mphSwimming speed (measured by the depth sounder log and parallel tracking)over the rst eight weeks of life averaged 14 plusmn 015 mph for the four calvesobserved away from the provisioning area

Respiration and diving During the rst two days of life newborns bobbedlsquocorklikersquo to the surface to breathe By the third day corklike surfacings wereatypical but still occurred (ad libitum notes) During the rst 2-3 weeks oflife but rarely thereafter newborns often surfaced with most of the torsoclearing the water and with the jaw angled at roughly 30deg or greater from thewater surface upon diving the jaw slaps down against the water surface Incontrast older infants and adults roll at the surface without bringing the jawfully out of the water (ad libitum notes) We call these newborn surfacingslsquochin-upsrsquo to distinguish them from lsquochin-slaprsquo displays which are moreexaggerated are not part of a breath series and tend to appear in a socialcontext

The earliest dives were observed on days 5 and 7 (peduncle dives) Noinfants were observed doing tail-out dives before day 12 Most infants werenot observed doing tail-out dives until the fth week of life or later eventhough their mothers were tail-out diving throughout the rst month of infantlife One infant was never observed doing a tail-out dive All infants didpeduncle dives by the second week of life and regularly thereafter

Not only are newborns buoyant remaining at the surface more than theirmothers they also breathe more frequently During deep water (gt 5 m) non-PA observations of mothers and newborns (4 dyads) involving 930 surfacingbouts for mothers and 960 bouts for infants 227plusmn 108 of infant surfacingbouts were longer ( ugrave 1 s) than those of the mother 700 plusmn 1072 were ofequivalent duration and only 73 plusmn 136 were shorter Infants surfaced and

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 14: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

542 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 4 Surfacing bouts

Mean (s) SD

Mean surfacing bout durationInfant (all mos) 3529 4023Mother (all mos) 3331 3970Infant (mo 1) 3693 1149Mother (mo 1) 3470 1410Infant (mo 2) 3827 2208Mother (mo 2) 3692 2157

of surfacing boutsInfant = Mother (all mos) 7000 1072Infant gt Mother (all mos) 2272 1082Infant lt Mother (all mos) 727 136Infant = Mother (mo 1) 6956 1171Infant gt Mother (mo 1) 2376 1180Infant lt Mother (mo 1) 667 224Infant = Mother (mo 2) 7695 1067Infant gt Mother (mo 2) 1046 1212Infant lt Mother (mo 2) 1259 714

N = 960 infant bouts 930 maternal bouts for 4 mother-infant pairsFor 30 infant bouts the infant surfaced and dove without overlapping with the motherSurfacing bout durations did not change signi cantly over time but during month 1 infantbouts were longer than maternal bouts much more often than the reverseMean interval between surfacing bouts (time submerged) = 65 s (SD = 30 s)

dove without their mothers on only 30 occasions For all surfacing bouts ofthe same duration for mothers and infant mother and infant surfaced anddove together and only 31 of newborn surfacing bouts showed no overlapin time with the motherrsquos time at the surface No age changes were detectablein infant or maternal surfacing bout durations (Table 4) The mean intervalbetween surfacing bouts was 65 plusmn 30 s and the longest interval betweensurfacing bouts was 208 s Breaths within a bout were typically 3-15 s apart

Actual respiration rates were measured weekly for two infants (RabNov) and their mothers during PA observations from birth to eight weeksof infant age (sampled for one 20 minute period each week for a totalof 13 sampling sessions) Breaths were audible in air For all samplingsessions (pooled data) newborns breathed more often than their mothers did(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks p lt 0001 each sample period istreated independently N = 13) The average breath rate for infants was38 plusmn 03 per minute maternal breath rate was 26 plusmn 01 per minute

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 15: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 543

Fig 2 This shows the rate of synchronous breaths between mothers and infants (N = 4pairs) Each line represents one mother-infant pair observed away from the provisioningarea

All dyads showed a decline in synchrony from month one to month two

Breathing synchrony Exact breathing synchrony occurs when mother andinfant break andor submerge into the water in perfect unison mdash a more spe-ci c and ne-tuned measure than the comparisons of overlapping maternaland infant surfacing bouts above During synchronous breaths the motherand infant are typically close (lt 2 m) parallel or slightly staggered in po-sition All infants showed their highest rate of synchrony with the motherduring the rst week of observations (range = 90-541 times per hr) Withbreath rates of 3-4 times per minute perfect synchrony occurred in 13 orfewer of all infant breaths All four infants observed away from the PAshowed a decline in breathing synchrony from month one to month two(Fig 2) One infant (Rab) showed a very low rate of synchrony overall Hismother spent the most time foraging a time when synchrony tends to be low

II Maternal and infant proximity and maintenancePA and non-PA data were combined to examine developmental changes inmother-infant proximity As expected from week one to later and frommonth one to month two the proportion of time infants spent at closedistances to their mothers gradually decreased and the proportion of time

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 16: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

544 MANN amp SMUTS

TABLE 5 Summary of ndings

Behaviour Non-provisioned amp Provisioned onlyprovisioned combined

Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vs Wk 1 vs Mo 1 vsWks later Mo 2 Wks later Mo 2

Mother-infant proximity ( time) (N = 6) (N = 9) (N = 5) (N = 7)lt 30 cm gt gt gtdagger gt

lt 2 m gt gt gtdagger gt

2-10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

gt 10 m lt lt ltdagger lt

SeparationsRate ( per hour) lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt

Mean bout duration na lt (N = 7) na na

Infant behaviourEchelon gt gt (N = 7) gtdagger gtdagger(N = 5)Infant position lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger NSSocial lt lt (N = 7) ltdagger lt (N = 6)

= p lt 005 dagger = p lt 010 NS = not signi cant na = not applicable because no testswere done We report signi cance levels of p lt 010 because due to small sample sizesfor some analyses 006 was the highest possible signi cance-level obtainable All tests wereWilcoxon-matched pairs signed ranks test lsquolt rsquo indicates direction of change from week 1 toweeks later or month 1 to month 2 Some infants were excluded because (a) they were notobserved during the rst week of life or (b) relevant data were not systematically collected

they spent at far distances gradually increased (Table 5) More speci callythe time spent next to the mother ( lt 30 cm) (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs mo two p lt 0005)and the time spent at close distances from the mother ( lt 2 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) decreased over time In complementary fashion the timespent at moderate distances from the mother (gt 2 m but lt 10 m) (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test wk one vs later p lt 004 mo one vs motwo p lt 0005) and the time spent at far distances from her (gt 10 m)(Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test one wk vs later p lt 004 moone vs mo two p lt 0005) increased over time

Proximity maintenance and the Hinde index The mean rate of mother-infant separation (gt 2 m apart) was signi cantly higher in the provisioning

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 17: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 545

area (143 plusmn 44 times per hr range = 79-195) compared to the rate ofseparation away from the provisioning area (318 plusmn 122 times per hr range= 221-491 times per hr permutation test p lt 003) All infants observedin the PA had a higher rate of separation than all infants observed away fromthe PA

The identity of who approached or left the other member of the pair wasconsistently scored during PA observations but could only be recorded adlibitum during non-PA observations For the two infants observed in bothcontexts we combined their approaches and leaves Infants rarely separatedfrom their mothers during the rst week of life and two of ve infantsobserved during the rst week did not separate from their mothers at allduring that time By the second week all infants separated from theirmothers for brief periods (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Only one mother wasobserved leaving her infant during the rst two weeks of life

Infants were responsible for the majority of approaches (8024 range =587-897) and almost all leaves (965 range = 934-100) Howeverbecause the Hinde Index was positive (the proportion of approaches by themother was greater than the proportion of leaves by the mother) motherswere technically more responsible than their newborns for maintainingproximity within the pair The Hinde Index declined signi cantly frommonth one (028 plusmn 015) to month two (011 plusmn 009) (Wilcoxon matchedpairs signed ranks test p = 003) indicating that maternal responsibility forproximity decreased over time

Separations from the mother Away from the provisioning area newbornsspent an average of 814 plusmn 440 of their time separated (gt 2 m) fromthe mother (range = 219-1238) Infants were observed traveling to upto 100 m away from their mothers At the provisioning area infants wereobserved going even further up to 200 m away During the majority of non-PA separations infants were alone (6681 plusmn 1758 range = 4754-8333)Patterns of association during separations are examined in detail elsewhere(Mann amp Smuts 1998)

All four infants observed away from the PA spent more time separatedfrom their mothers during month two (2473plusmn 1297 range = 612-3599)compared to month one (250 plusmn 287 range = 029-659) When the PAdata are included this pattern is signi cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 9) Similarly the rate of separation increased for

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 18: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

546 MANN amp SMUTS

all infants from month one (non-PA 195plusmn 172 per hour range = 038-222)to month two (non-PA 778 plusmn 226 per hour range = 581-1091 Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) The mean bout durationsof the separations increased as well from month one (38 plusmn 13 s range = 2-307 s) to month two (133 plusmn 68 s range = 2-1058 s N = 4) If mean boutdurations of separations are estimated for PA infants based upon the numberof point sample intervals between approaches and leaves then the combineddata reach statistical signi cance (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 002 N = 7) In sum as infants grew older they were separated fromtheir mothers more often and for longer periods of time

III Infant activityInfant activities were collapsed into ve main behavioural states echelonswimming infant position swimming resting traveling and socializingInfants did not forage (but see description of lsquoplay snackingrsquo below) Infantsdid not initiate or interact with people at the provisioning area until three orfour months of age (unpublished data) Other infant activities were brief andare described as lsquoinfant behavioursrsquo (see below)

Echelon position All newborns decreased the time spent echelon swim-ming between wk one and later (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks testp lt 004 N = 6) and between month one and month two (Wilcoxonmatched pairs signed ranks test p lt 002 N = 7) During the rst monthinfants spent most of their time echelon swimming (693 plusmn 208 range =493-985) but by the second month this behaviour had decreased dramat-ically (106 plusmn 114 range = 0-240)

Infants spent much less time swimming in echelon when close to people( lt 2 m away) than when further from them (Wilcoxon matched pairs signedranks test p lt 004 N = 6) Thus the differences between animals withinand away from the PA in time spent echelon swimming (see Table 3) may beattributable to the dyadrsquos proximity to people

Infant position During the rst week of life infant position was rare Thethree infants observed away from the PA during the rst week of life (NovRab Mou) were all observed in infant position but never for longer than74 s and more typically for less than 30 s Typically the infant stayed ininfant position long enough to nurse for several bouts (often nursing several

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 19: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 547

seconds on one side and then on the other) Of the 19 bouts of infant positionobserved in the rst week nursing (de ned as infant rostrum to motherrsquosmammary slit) de nitely occurred during 17 bouts during the other twocases we were not close enough to document nursing or its absence By thesecond week infant position bout length increased and the infant remainedthere when not nursing often for several minutes at a time (mean boutduration = 61 plusmn 72 s range = 2-411 s N = 60 bouts nursing observedin 9 bouts)

At the provisioning site we did not see Rab and Nov in infant positionuntil days nine and ten respectively despite intensive observations duringthat period whereas several brief bouts of infant position were observedaway from the provisioning area on days one and two Most of the newbornswho were observed only in the provisioning area spent very little or no timein infant position Thus none of the age changes for time spent in infantposition were statistically signi cant at the provisioning area Howeverwhen the time spent in infant position was examined as a function of locationwith in the provisioning area and each sample week was treated separatelyinfants spent much less time in infant position when near people ( lt 2 m) thanwhen further away (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test p lt 004N = 6) In shallow water infants cannot stay under their mothers and itsometimes appeared as if mothers used the shallows to prevent their infantsfrom gaining infant position contact

Away from the provisioning area age changes were more evident Duringthe rst month of life the four infants spent an average of 766 plusmn 714 ofthe time in infant position (range = 0-136) but by month two they spent anaverage 180plusmn 607 of the time in infant position (range = 120-243) twiceas much time as spent in echelon position When these data are combinedwith the PA data newborns showed a signi cant increase in time spent ininfant position from month one to 2 (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rankstest p lt 004 N = 7)

Resting and oating Resting is characterized by slow movement andoccasional oating or hanging at the surface During the rst week infantswere not seen to stop moving for more than a few seconds but oldernewborns often oated in place for up to several minutes After the rstweek infants in the provisioning area oated much more (42 plusmn 61 ofthe time) than outside of the provisioning area (01 plusmn 022) even though

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 20: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

548 MANN amp SMUTS

time resting overall did not differ (Table 3) Away from the provisioning areainfants engaged in oating bouts at the rate of 03 per hour these bouts weretypically short averaging only 8 plusmn 6 s (range = 2-28) Floating bouts werenot timed in the provisioning area

Traveling During the majority of the motherrsquos traveling the infant swamwith her in either echelon or infant position We de ned infant traveling asmovement gt 2 mph in a particular direction when not in echelon or infantposition Away from the provisioning area all four infants increased the timethey spent traveling from month one (623 plusmn 521 range = 0-127) tomonth two (135 plusmn 462 range = 80-176)

Socializing The percentage of time that non-PA infants spent socializingincreased from month one (206 plusmn 078 range = 15-317) to month two(264plusmn 201 range = 676-4773) With the PA data added this pattern wassigni cant (Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test N = 7 p lt 004)Note that our method (predominant activity sampling during a 5-minuteinterval) is likely to underestimate the amount of time spent socializingbecause social bouts tended to be of short duration Below we include moredetailed descriptions of social interactions

IV Infant behavioursRubbing and petting Rubbing was the most common form of newborn so-cializing beginning in the rst days of life with the mother Infants initiatedvirtually all (999) rubbing interactions with their mothers Typically new-borns rubbed their lateral side or full body on the motherrsquos jaw or rostrumsometimes near the melon or eye Of 1311 mother-infant rubbings observedduring the rst eight weeks of life (PA and non-PA) 480 focused on themotherrsquos head area The second most favored body part was the motherrsquoslateral side (262 of all rubbings) Otherwise newborns rubbed on theirmothersrsquo dorsal side (150) or ventrum (102) but rarely in her tail re-gion (07) A speci c type of rubbing called a keel-rub which is commonbetween adult females and males involves the actor (usually female) swim-ming belly-up under the recipient (usually male) and rubbing the base of thetail ukes vigorously across the recipientrsquos chest or between the pectoral nsTwo newborn females were observed lsquokeel-rubbingrsquo with older females on a

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 21: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 549

total of 5 occasions but newborn males were never observed keel-rubbingNewborns were never seen to rub or pet with subadult or adult males

Sociosexual rubbing (rubbing involving the infantrsquos genital area) is easierto detect among males than females because of the visibility of erectionsMales were involved in sociosexual rubbing within the rst two weeks oflife Rabrsquos erection was rst seen on day 9 and he rubbed his penis on hismotherrsquos dorsal side and peduncle multiple times on day 11 On day 17 herubbed his penis on her peduncle dorsal side and dorsal n Oys mountedhis mother during the second month Shd Nak and Pep were all observedrubbing their penises on their mothers during the rst two months of life (adlibitum and focal observations) Older female infants have been observedrubbing their genital area on others but we did not observe this behaviouramong newborn females

Petting occurs when the pectoral n of the lsquopetterrsquo is actively moving upand down on the body parts of another animal This was seen only 26 timesamong newborns In 22 of the 26 cases the infant received petting newbornsplayed the active lsquopetterrsquo role on only four occasions Two cases involved themale infant Rab who pet his mother twice on her blowhole area and melonon day 17 (earliest newborn petting observation) The other two instancesinvolved the female Nov who pet an adult female on days 51 and 52

Bonding During lsquobondingrsquo the lsquobonderrsquo swims alongside the lsquobondeersquo butshifted back a little so that the bonderrsquos pectoral n can rest on the bondeersquosposterior ank Bonding is similar to echelon swimming except that duringechelon swimming animals may or may not be in contact and the infantrsquospectoral n is placed either beneath the dorsal n of the mother or on theanterior ank of the mother Bonding is a highly distinctive behaviour thatoccurs between two females at least one of whom is typically cycling in thepresence of adult or subadult males (Richards 1996) Although newbornsoccasionally swam echelon with other animals only one clear case ofbonding was observed Nov when 52 days old swam lsquobondedrsquo to her 11year-old sister Joy for approximately 7 minutes During this time Joy wasbeing pursued by three subadult males who displayed mounted goosed andswam in formation behind her The mother of Joy and Nov was 20-50 maway throughout

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 22: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

550 MANN amp SMUTS

Social play Play is dif cult to de ne in any species (Fagen 1981) Wecoded infant activity as play when the infant was involved in reciprocalchasing andor displaying with another dolphin (Table 2) Social play wasinfrequent so we describe the types of play and when they rst occurredPlay was rst observed at the end of the rst week but it was not commonuntil after the third week As early as the rst month reciprocal chasingoccasionally involved objects For example when Rab was 4 weeks old heand Sur (then a subadult female) played with the same piece of seagrass Rabhad the seagrass Sur stole it and Rab chased her snapping at the seagrassbut did not succeed in retrieving it A week later Rab and a one-year oldmale chased each other each carrying seagrass in their mouths Seagrassplay continued to occur during the second month

By the second month of life nearly all newborns were involved inreciprocal chasing At 6 weeks Nov frequently played chase with an oldermale infant her nephew Pic and Shd chased one another during the secondmonth of life as did Oys and Mou Infants appeared to lsquoinvitersquo a chase byswimming past the target animal onside or belly-up and veering away just asthey reach the potential playmate Mothers occasionally chased their infantsand then would turn away apparently lsquoallowingrsquo their infants to chase them

Displays infrequently occurred We describe their rst occurrences andthe types of displays observed Initially infants sometimes engaged indisplays without any clear social context but by the third week displaystypically occurred during social interaction Some displays occurred as earlyas the end of the rst week of life For example Rab was rst observed doinga lsquoface slaprsquo (slapping the side of his face on the water surface) alone onday 6 By Day 18 Rab expanded his repertoire doing spy hops chin slapsback slaps and belly slaps Shd began displaying spy hops and swimmingbelly-up in pursuit of playmates in his third week

Displays that we interpret as aggressive among adults jaw claps and headjerks (Table 2) were rare in the newborn period but did occur during the 3rdand 4th weeks of life Mou jaw clapped in week 3 and Rab clapped his jawand jerked his head multiple times during the 4th week of life All of the jawclaps and head jerks observed occurred when the infant was alone (gt 10 mfrom others)

Play forage and object play Older infants (gt 3 months) frequently engagein what we call lsquosnackrsquo foraging which involves chasing small sh belly-up

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 23: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 551

and trapping them at the water surface The sh are often visible jumping atthe surface or causing small ripples in the water Most infant foraging is ofthis type We have not observed newborns catching sh but behaviours thatmimic snack foraging which we term lsquoplay snackingrsquo were seen as earlyas the third week of life Infants sometimes snapped at weed or repeatedlyjaw clapped at the surface Rab was the most precocious in this regardFor example on days 21 amp 22 Rab was observed swimming belly-upand snapping his jaw as if snacking On day 23 Rabble chased seagrass(Amphibolis antarctica) belly up and snapped at it as older calves do with sh He would let the seagrass go swim away and then again chase afterit belly-up snapping at the same piece Three calves were observed playsnacking at 7 weeks of age (Nak Hbt Nov) but not earlier Oys was neverobserved play snacking Play snacking was not recorded for the other calves

Object play was not common but was seen occasionally (ad libitum)Seagrass weed (Amphibolis antarctica) was the most common object usedSeagrass play occurred both alone and with partners Rab carried seagrassaround for long periods on day 18 On day 22 Rab again swam rapidlyaround with weed in his mouth As mentioned above seagrass can standin for sh during play snacking On day 22 Rab chased and lsquogoosedrsquo acormorant with his rostrum several times

V Maternal activity and behaviourOutside of and in the provisioning area maternal behaviours showed nosigni cant changes from month one to month two Thus only overallpatterns (months one and two combined) are summarized here (Table 3)Outside of the PA mothers spent most of their time resting (485 plusmn 78range = 403-590) or foraging (275plusmn 126 range = 1336-408) Restingincluded bouts of oating at the surface and although mothers oated muchmore often than their infants (25 bouts per hour vs 03 infant bouts perhour) it still made up a very small proportion of their time (074 plusmn 021)The mean duration for all maternal oating bouts was 10 plusmn 10 s (range =2-70) Traveling was the next most prevalent activity (192 plusmn 80 range= 130-3069) The two provisioned females begged from boats away fromthe provisioning area (199 and 76 of the time respectively) Motherssocialized an average of 24 plusmn 36 of the time (range = 036-543) muchless than their infants The proportion of time spent socializing is likely to beslightly underestimated by PAS sampling because bouts of social behaviourtend to be short

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 24: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

552 MANN amp SMUTS

Nursing The infant was considered to be nursing if the beak was in contactwith one of the motherrsquos mammary slits We cannot reliably record everyinstance of nursing nor estimate milk intake but we often saw nursingduring non-PA follows (see previous section on infant position swimming)In the provisioning area calves apparently attempted to get into infantposition by bumping or pushing under the mother often accompanied byloud whistling (audible in air) but we did not see them nurse until afterthey left the shore area Since visibility was better in the provisioning areathan away from it the absence of nursing in the provisioning area probablyindicates that mothers did not allow infants to nurse or remain in infantposition near people

All newborns except for the two who were videotaped (Nak Hbt) wereobserved nursing During the rst two months of life all but three of the 58observed nursing bouts involved the infant going belly-up or onside whilethe mother remained stationary or moved very slowly Only one motherwas observed turning on her side to let the infant nurse (3 nursing bouts)Infant lsquobeak to mammary slitrsquo times ranged between 3-10 s similar to valuesreported in captivity (eg Reid et al 1995)

Associations with others

As described earlier dolphins did not form lsquopartiesrsquo near the provisioningarea the way they did outside of it and patterns of association differeddramatically between the PA and non-PA contexts Here we describe furtherthe association patterns of the four newborns observed away from theprovisioning area The mean non-PA party size was 4 plusmn 28 (range 3-5Table 3) thus on average mothers and infants were accompanied by twoothers The minimum party size was one animal (infant alone) and themaximum was 14 animals Large parties tended to be mixed with both adultfemales and adult males present Party size did not change from month oneto two

The most common or modal party size was two animals mother andinfant Thus motherinfant pairs were more likely to be alone than withanyone else Mothers and infants were alone together an average of 44 plusmn724 of the time Otherwise they were usually in a lsquofemalersquo groupde ned as a party with at least one other adult female and no adult males(458plusmn 592) Motherinfant pairs were in mixed groups (groups containing

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 25: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 553

at least one other adult female and one adult male) an average of 617 plusmn547 of the time (Table 3) Mothers and infants were never in parties withadult males only but one infant associated with adult males away from hismother Interactions between males and infants rarely occurred but af liativebehaviours (petting and rubbing) between adult males and mothers occurredon several occasions (see Mann amp Smuts 1998 for more detailed analysesof patterns of association and allomaternal care over the newborn period)

Summary of developmental changes

Since we have described developmental changes for each behaviour sepa-rately we conclude this section with a brief summary that provides a moreintegrated picture of developmental trends

Virtually all patterns of infant behaviour mother-infant proximity andspatial relationships with the mother changed as a function of infant ageMaternal activity however did not change over time except that mothersdecreased their role in maintaining proximity to their infants from the rstmonth to the second month of infant life

Fetal folds (lumpiness) distinguished infants in the rst week of lifeDuring this time infants swam rapidly and almost exclusively in echelonposition The infant adopted infant position only brie y presumably justto nurse Surfacings were lsquocorklikersquo and chin-up surfacings were commonas were synchronous breaths between mother and infant Infants rarelyremained still at the surface Although week-old infants engaged in littlesocial behaviour they frequently rubbed on their mothers especially aroundthe head area Rubbing continued to be the most common form of socialactivity for infants during the newborn period

During the second week of life the mother tolerated further and longerseparations and separations occurred more often Infants engaged in pro-longed (up to several minutes) bouts of socializing (either rubbing with theirmothers or turn-taking chase games with others) Synchronous breaths withthe mother declined but the overwhelming majority of mother-infant sur-facings overlapped Infants began to dive (peduncle and tail-out dives) andremained in infant position for several minutes (outside of the nursing con-text) They also began to oat at the surface for several seconds at a time

The frequency of separation from the mother playing and displayingall increased during the third and fourth weeks Also infants began to

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 26: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

554 MANN amp SMUTS

pet with others although this behaviour was not common any time in the rst 10 weeks Infants occasionally carried or played with objects such asseagrass During the fourth week infants engaged in reciprocal chase gamesoccasionally involving objects We rst observed play snacking (practiceforaging) during the fourth week but it was not common at this age

During the second month fetal lines remained visible Infants typicallyswam in infant position and echelon position became uncommon Infantssometimes spent 10 minutes or more over 20 m from their mothers andthe degree of coordination and synchronization between mother and infantdeclined relative to the rst month Play involving turn-taking and displaysincreased Most infants engaged in play snacking by the end of this period

Discussion

Young dolphins undergo dramatic developmental changes during the new-born period with signi cant changes in proximity to the mother and activityover time In contrast maternal activity budgets changed little over the new-born period These behavioral patterns are discussed in relation to (a) theeffects of provisioning (b) comparisons between captive and wild newborndolphin behaviour and (c) the life-history and ecology of delphinids andother cetaceans

The effects of provisioning

Because over half of the observations reported here come from semi-provisioned dolphins it is important to address the effects of provisioningand its implications of provisioning for ethological research As the popular-ity of eco-tourism increases so do the economic incentives of making largesocial animals available for close viewing Provisioning can often facilitatethis process and it too is likely to become increasingly common Becauseprovisioned animals are easier to nd and observe researchers sometimes nd them ideal subjects The reverse situation also occurs when ethologistsinitiate provisioning to make observations easier and tourism develops later(eg Pan troglodytes chimpanzees mdash Goodall 1986 Pan paniscus bonobosmdash Kano 1982) Similarly the success of tourism at Monkey Mia has beencited in support of the development of tourist oriented-dolphin provision-ing elsewhere in Australia and North America (Orams et al 1996 National

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 27: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 555

Marine Fisheries Service [US] 1994) Thus increasingly research on an-imals like dolphins and primates may involve subjects who are arti ciallyprovisioned andor who have extensive contact with tourists (eg bottlenosedolphins mdash Orams et al 1996 bonobos mdash Kano 1982 Macaca sp mdashOleary amp Fa 1993 Hill 1994) These trends make research on the effectsof provisioning on behaviour and the animalsrsquo well-being (eg health mor-tality stress) essential for those lsquomanagingrsquo arti cial feeding and tourismfor those interested in conservation and well-being of wild populations andfor researchers interested in documenting natural behaviour and how it isaffected by human intervention of this type

Arti cial provisioning of wild mammals in Shark Bay and elsewhere canfacilitate research but it also in icts costs The bene ts include increasedaccess to animals that are otherwise dif cult to study (Connor amp Smolker1985 Wrangham 1974) For example in the absence of the provisioningarea comparable data on newborns would have taken many years to obtainThe costs include distortions in behaviour and life histories (eg Cercopithe-cus aethiops vervets Lee et al 1986 Macaca fuscata Japanese macaquesSugiyama amp Ohsawa 1982 Papio cynocephalus savanna baboons Altmannamp Muruthi 1988) For example female aggression was observed only onceduring 79 hours of non-PA observations of newborns but occurred often(026 times per hour) in the provisioning area For two reasons we concludethat provisioning triggered aggression (1) Since aggressive behaviours arefairly obvious even away from the PA it is unlikely that the higher ratesof aggression in the provisioning area are solely attributable to the superiorobservation conditions there and (2) Arti cial feeding is associated withincreased aggression among primates (eg Loy 1970 Wrangham 1974Oleary amp Fa 1993) and other mammals ( Ursus sp Herrero 1985 bot-tlenose dolphins mdash Orams et al 1996)

Although we were struck by how similarly provisioned females and non-provisioned females behaved when outside of the PA (ie the two provi-sioned female-infant pairs observed away from the PA never both rankedbelow or above non-provisioned female-infants pairs on any character-istic except for boat-begging and synchrony) provisioned females spentseveral hours per day near human activity which altered activity bud-gets association patterns and maternal behaviour for substantial portionsof the day Data from Shark Bay indicate that calves of provisioned fe-males experience greater mortality than calves of non-provisioned females

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 28: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

556 MANN amp SMUTS

(Mann et al in press) While the exact causes of this difference remain amystery behavioural differences between provisioned and non-provisionedmothernewborn pairs may play a role In the PA mothers focused on hu-mans were less likely to forage or socialize and had available only a limitednumber of potential social partners compared to outside of the PA Infantsin turn experienced reduced opportunities to be in contact with the motheran increased frequency of separations from the mother and reduced num-bers of associates compared to away from the PA In the provisioning areainfants also incurred increased risks of becoming beached or entangled in shing lines (Mann et al 1995) Increased proximity to fast moving boatsmay also be hazardous (Wells amp Scott 1997) but no infants have yet beenharmed this way

Although provisioning clearly in uenced the frequencies of differentbehaviours the order in which new behaviours appeared and the directionof developmental changes in infant behaviour were very similar within andoutside of the provisioning area (see Table 5) For example although infantsseparated more often from their mothers in the provisioning area than whenoutside of the provisioning area the rate of separation increased in bothcontexts as infants aged

Boat follows may also in uence dolphin behavior Janik amp Thompson(1996) found that bottlenose dolphins in the Moray Firth Scotland dove forlonger periods when being followed by a large tour boat than when boatswere just passing by In Shark Bay the fact that dolphins regularly engagein intimate behaviors such as nursing petting and sexual interactions nearour boats but not near the people at the provisioning area suggests to usthat the dolphins are well habituated to our boats Calf mortality for focalanimals (who were followed in boats) does not differ from mortality for non-focal calves who were only surveyed (unpubl data) However the effectsof boat follows on dolphin behavior their predators or prey have not beensystematically examined in Shark Bay

Below we discuss major developmental trends documented in our studyand compare them when possible with captive data These comparisonsare useful for managing captive dolphins particularly during the vulnerablenewborn period and determining the external validity of captive studies

Breathing synchrony locomotor coordination and mother-infant contact

The early breathing patterns of Shark Bay mothers and calves resemble thosereported by for captive Indian Ocean Tursiops (Peddemors 1990 Kastelein

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 29: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 557

et al 1990) For example mean breathing rates documented for two calvesin the provisioning area were similar to those reported by Kastelein etal (1990) However maximum breathing intervals were much greater forShark Bay newborns than for captive newborns (Peddemors 1990) possiblybecause wild infants sometimes dove to greater depths than is possible ina tank Alternatively Peddemors recorded breathing rates during rest onlywhereas the Shark Bay breathing data were derived from all behaviouralstates of mother and calf With different behavioural de nitions of rest wecannot make direct comparisons

Peddemors (1990) found that 90 of mother-calf respirations were syn-chronous a much higher gure than we reported However this differenceis likely attributable to differences in how synchrony was de ned it is un-clear whether Peddemoresrsquo de nition of synchrony refers to exact synchronyoverlapping breaths or overlapping time at the surface during surfacingbouts In Shark Bay most (70) of mother and infant surfacing bouts werethe same length (70) indicating a high degree of coordinated breathingand 97 of infant surfacing bouts overlapped with those of the mother Thesevalues did not change signi cantly from the rst to second month of lifebut the frequency with which mother and calf surfaced in exact unison diddecline as the calf aged Peddemors (1990) also reported that breathing syn-chrony declined over time with most of the decline occurring after 60 daysof calf age beyond the newborn period of the current study

During the rst month of life but not the second a signi cant minority(about one-quarter) infant surfacing bouts were longer than those of themother outside of the provisioning area indicating that newborns maytake more breaths than their mothers This was the case during the rsttwo months of life for two motherinfant pairs whose breathing rates wereprecisely documented in the provisioning area Similarly in sperm whalesPhyseter macrocephalus a species that dives deeply immatures breathemore often than adults (Whitehead 1996)

Motor patterns of surfacing and diving seem to be similar in the wild andin captivity Wild newborns rarely showed the lsquochin-uprsquo surfacing patternafter week three Peddemors similarly reported that ldquoby the 25th day postpartum the calves had all acquired the smooth roll which characterizesrespiratory surfacing of the adultrdquo (1990 p 179) Thus observations of thechin-up surfacing pattern may prove useful in estimating the ages of youngcalves in the wild

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 30: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

558 MANN amp SMUTS

Wild infants appeared to nd diving dif cult at rst but two infants wereseen peduncle diving at the end of the rst week and all calves did so bythe second week Similarly in captivity infants can dive by the 6th day oflife (Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) (Determination of dives in their study wasbased on underwater observations of infant swimming near the bottom ofthe tank) Most Shark Bay infants did not perform tail-out dives which aretypically deeper than peduncle dives until after week ve The increase intime spent in infant position swimming during week two appeared to dependon the development of skillful swimming and diving such that the infantcould more easily remain beneath the mother

Although dolphin calves clearly exhibit motoric precociousness com-pared to for example nonhuman primates or terrestrial carnivores severalobservations indicate that motor coordination improves with age First thedecline in chin-up surfacings over the rst few weeks corresponds with thedevelopment of epaxio-musculature (see Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) that en-ables the infant to smoothly break and re-enter the water surface Secondthe infantrsquos increasing ability to remain in infant position to dive to deeperdepths and to oat relatively motionless at the surface suggest steady im-provements in physical and respiratory competence Third most infants ofprovisioned females became beached accidentally during the rst week butnone did so after this time Fourth the increasing distances and durationsof separations from the mother exemplify the infantrsquos growing competencein negotiating the marine environment and nding the mother rapidly Fifththe declining breathing synchrony (Fig 2) between mother and infant mayre ect the fact that the infant has achieved respirationdiving competencesuf cient to guide its own surfacing and diving patterns independent of themother

Several studies of captive Tursiops have described developmental shiftsin patterns of mother-infant contact During the rst weeks of life infantstended to swim in echelon formation (also called contact swimming) (egTavolga amp Essapian 1957 Chirighin 1987 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987Reiss 1988 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990 Reid et al 1995) In captive studiesand our eld study echelon swimming declined rapidly in the rst few weeksof life but for some infants continued into the second month of life

Echelon swimming may bene t the infant by aiding in swimming Whenin echelon position the infant is essentially led alongside the mother andthus reaches the surface to breathe around the same time she does Almost

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 31: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 559

certainly this helps the newborn coordinate movements breathing anddiving patterns with the mother In contrast when in infant position theinfant must brie y break the position to surface alongside of the motherto synchronize breaths Norris amp Prescott (1961) proposed that echelonswimming gives infants a hydrodynamic boost but this hypothesis has notbeen tested

In both captivity (Tavolga amp Essapian 1957 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990Reid et al 1995) and in the wild infants gradually spend less time in echelonposition and more time in infant position in contact under the motherrsquosabdomen where nursing takes place This early shift from the motherrsquos headarea to the abdomen re ects the changing abilities of the newborn who must rst be able to stay with her and learn how to use and respond to soundOnce motor ability has improved the calf switches to infant position Afterthe newborn period Shark Bay infants maintained infant position roughly30-40 of the time until weaning (Mann 1997) In terms of coordination oreffort infant position may be more challenging than echelon swimming butit provides easy access to the mammary slits and the tactile stimulation mayenhance milk production Little is known about the possible hydrodynamicbene ts of infant position swimming or the energetic costs this position maycarry for the mother

Fast-swimming is characteristic of newborn dolphins and their mothersduring the rst two weeks both in this sample and in captivity (Tavolga ampEssapian 1957 Amundin 1986 Eastcott amp Dickinson 1987) Right whale(Eubalaena australis) calves studied in the wild also go through a period offast-swimming soon after birth (Thomas amp Taber 1984) Thomas amp Taber(1984) propose that rapid swimming is an anti-predation strategy that alsohelps the calf stay buoyant to breathe

Shark Bay newborns performed the majority of approaches and leaves thatoccurred between mother and infant but since their mothers were typicallymore likely to approach than to leave them mothers were more responsiblethan were infants for maintaining proximity Consistent with studies ofproximity maintenance in captive Tursiops (Chirighin 1987 Reid et al1995) and free-ranging right whales (Taber amp Thomas 1982) we found adecline over time in the motherrsquos responsibility for maintaining proximity toher infant

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 32: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

560 MANN amp SMUTS

Maternal behaviour

Some maternal behaviours reported in captivity or elsewhere in the wild havenot been seen in Shark Bay These include calf tossinglifting calf pinningand maternal ventral present during nursing We discuss each in turn

In the wild (dos Santos amp Lacerda 1987 Shane 1990) and in captivity(Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thurman amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross1990) mothers (or presumed mothers) have been seen lifting or propellingtheir infants clear out of the water This lsquocalf tossingrsquo behaviour is distinctfrom supporting behaviour in which another animal gently pushes a calf tothe surface We never observed calf tossing but once a mother brie y swambelly-up at the surface with her calf between her pectoral ns Cockcroft ampRoss (1990) interpreted similar behaviour in captive dolphins as maternaldiscipline

Captive studies have yielded several reports of mothers pinning younginfants to the bottom these were interpreted as cases of maternal punish-ment or aggression (eg Tavolga 1966 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Thur-man amp Williams 1986 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) Marine trainers havealso observed this behaviour soon after parturition (Amundin et al 1989N Hecker pers comm) Although in Shark Bay we can often see clearto the bottom we have observed calf-pinning only once when following ashark attack a mother pushed her dead offspring to the sea oor and held herthere (Mann amp Barnett 1999) This occasion was of particular interest be-cause the mother supported her dead calf for roughly half an hour before shepinned her It is possible that pinning is an attempt to revive not punish theinfant and its physiological underpinnings should be investigated (Amundinet al 1989)

Early nursing behaviour in captivity is characterized by the mother turningon her side while the infant nurses (Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell1972 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) For example Cockcroft amp Ross (1990)report that all suckling during the rst 4 weeks occurred in that fashionDuring the second month the infant turns on her side and the mother remainsupright We observed 58 nursing bouts during the rst weeks of life and onlyone mother showed this pattern three times In general from birth on theinfant not the mother turned on hisher side to nurse It is possible that thedifferent subspecies of Tursiops studied in captivity and in Shark Bay showdifferent nursing styles

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 33: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 561

Developmental trends and milestones

In aquaria bottlenose dolphin calves typically take sh between 6-12 mos(Essapian 1953 Caldwell amp Caldwell 1972 Amundin 1986 Chirighin1987 Cockcroft amp Ross 1990) In our study infants did not catch small shuntil they were between 4-6 months old Prior to this time infants lsquopracticersquoforage The onset of regular practice foraging during the second monthcoincides with the timing of the apparent development of echolocation skillsamong captive newborns (Reiss 1988) The distinctive belly-up lsquosnackingrsquothat Shark Bay infants engage in may enhance sensory integration of visualand echolocation skills because infants can visually track the backlit sh(or seagrass) at the surface while receiving acoustic information on thestimulus

Erections have been observed in captive dolphins at two days of age(McBride amp Kritzler 1951) and during the 3rd week of life when they havebecome more socially active (Reiss 1988) Caldwell amp Caldwell (1972)reported that all male calves mount their mothers within the rst 3 weeksof life but no speci cs were given We observed erections and occasionalmountings (rubbing the penis on the motherrsquos genital area) by newbornmales but relative to later periods of development little sexual behaviourtakes place during the newborn period At older ages infant males mount avariety of males and females ranging from peers to older adults Older maleinfants were often seen mounting and even achieving intromission with theirmothers (JM pers obs)

Social interactions and relationships

Newborns engaged in a variety of af liative behaviours such as rubbing andpetting primarily directed to their mothers Newborn play was characterizedby chasing and displays and involved very little body-to-body contact Thisstrikes us as different from older-infant play which tends to involve extensivecontact (unpubl data)

In Sarasota Florida (Wells et al 1987) the size of the parties in whichinfants were found declined with infant age Shark Bay calves are in largergroups during the newborn period compared to all other ages pre-weaning(Mann et al in press) However party size did not decrease from month oneto month two during the newborn period Our party sizes were considerablysmaller than those reported in Sarasota possibly due in part to different

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 34: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

562 MANN amp SMUTS

de nitions of a group We used a 10-meter chain rule whereas in Sarasotaanimals within 100 m radius were considered part of the same group (Wellset al 1987)

Shark Bay mothers and infants associated most often with other mother-infant pairs and adult females Adult and subadult males were rarely presentshowed no interest in calves and interacted with a newborn only onceHowever males occasionally engaged in af liative interactions (pettingand rubbing) with mothers (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Multiparous femalesrarely interacted directly with infants but females inexperienced in parentingoften did (Mann amp Smuts 1998) Infants (newborn and older) also showedconsiderable interest in young calves and engaged in numerous chase boutswith them The uidity of dolphin parties provides the young animal withfrequent and diverse social stimulation typically involving 30 or moreanimals during the rst months of life

The motor play of wild newborns resembled patterns reported in ungulates(eg Cuvierrsquos gazelle mdash Gomendio 1988) Different types of play mayhave different adaptive functions (Bateson amp Young 1981 Martin amp Caro1985 Gomendio 1988) and following their view motor play may suitthe immediate needs of newborns who must learn to negotiate the marineenvironment and in particular avoid large sharks and nd their mothersquickly Both reciprocal chase play and the frequent approaches and leaveswith the mother characteristic of newborns may enhance sensorimotor skillsnecessary to survive this critical period But why chase others and leave themother at such an early age Why donrsquot infants remain close to their mothersand avoid risky separations and play bouts with others until the newbornperiod is over Newborns must learn to separate and rejoin with their mothersearly on because maternal foraging activities guarantee frequent separationsThus given that the infant cannot always stay close to the mother learningto locate and return to the mother is of utmost importance

Integration of complex sensorimotor tasks seems especially critical fornewborn mammals in a marine environment Following an initial focus onswimming breathing and synchronizing with and identifying the mothernewborns begin to dive and negotiate brief separations with the motherBy the end of two months infants regularly socialize with others andpractice foraging skills Such precociousness appears to be paradoxical whencontrasted with their long period of dependency (median age at weaning is 4yrs range = 27-8 Mann et al in press) By the end of the newborn period

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 35: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 563

infants have developed many of the skills necessary for maneuvering in amarine environment However establishment of social bonds in a ssion-fusion society and re ned foraging skills may take many more years todevelop

References

Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour Sampling methods mdash Behaviour 49p 227-267

mdash mdash amp Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semi-provisioned and wild-feeding baboons mdash Am J Primatol 15 p 213-221

Amundin M (1986) Breeding the bottlenosed dolphin at the Kolmaringrden Dolphinarium mdashInt Zoo Yearbook 2425 p 263-271

mdash mdash Hultman S amp Roken B (1989) Bottlenose dolphin twin birth in KolmaringrdenDolphinarium mdash In International Marine Animal Trainers Association ConferenceAbstracts the Netherlands

Bateson P amp Young M (1981) Separation from the mother and the development of play incats mdash Anim Behav 29 p 173-180

Caldwell DK amp Caldwell MC (1972) The world of the bottlenose dolphin mdash JB Lip-pincott Co Philadelphia and New York

Charnov EL (1991) Evolution of life history variation among female mammals mdash ProcNatl Acad Sci 88 p 1134-1137

Chirighin L (1987) Mother-calf spatial relationships and calf development in captivebottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 5-15

Cockcroft VG amp Ross GJB (1990) Observations on the early development of a captivebottlenose dolphin calf mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeveseds) Academic Press New York p 461-485

Connor RC Richards AF Smolker RA amp Mann J (1996) Patterns of female attrac-tiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins mdash Behaviour 133 p 37-69

mdash mdash amp Smolker RA (1985) Habituated dolphins (Tursiops sp) in Western Australia mdashJ Mammal 66 p 398-400

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Richards AF (1992a) Two levels of alliance formation among malebottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) mdash Proc Natl Acad Sci 89 p 987-990

mdash mdash mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1992b) Dolphin alliances and coalitions mdash In Coalitions andcompetition in animals and humans (AH Harcourt amp FBM de Waal eds) OxfordUniversity Press New York and Oxford p 415-442

Cornell LH Asper ED Antrim JE Searles SS Young WG amp Goff T (1987)Progress report Results of a long-range captive breeding program for the bottle-nosedolphin Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops truncatus gilli mdash Zool Biol 6 p 41-54

dos Santos ME amp Lacerda M (1987) Preliminary observations of the bottlenose dolphin(Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Esturay (Portugal) mdash Aquatic Mamm 13 p 65-80

Eastcott A amp Dickinson T (1987) Underwater observations of the suckling and socialbehaviour of a new-born bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) mdash Aquatic Mamm13 p 51-56

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 36: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

564 MANN amp SMUTS

Essapian FS (1953) The birth and growth of a porpoise mdash Natural Hist November 1953p 392-399

Fagen R (1981) Animal play behaviour mdash Oxford University Press New YorkGomendio M (1988) The development of different types of play in gazelles Implications

for the nature and functions of play mdash Anim Behav 36 p 825-836Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of behaviour mdash Harvard Univ

Press CambridgeHerrero S (1985) The dangersof garbage and habituationmdash In Bear attacks Their causes

and avoidance (S Herrero ed) Winchester Press Piscataway NJ p 51-104Hersh SL Odell DK amp Asper ED (1990) Bottlenose dolphin mortality patterns in the

IndianBanana River System of Florida mdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwoodamp RR Reeves eds) Academic Press New York p 155-164

Hill DA (1994) Af liative behavior between adult males of the genus Macaca mdashBehaviour 130 p 293-308

Hinde RA amp Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintainingmutual proximity as exempli ed by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys mdashAnim Behav 18 p 169-176

Janik VM amp Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals mdash Adv Study Behav 26p 59-99

mdash mdash amp Thompson P (1996)Changes in surfacingpatternsof bottlenosedolphin in responseto boat traf c mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 597-602

Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba mdashPrimates 23 p 171-188

Kastelein RA Dokter T amp Zwart P (1990) The suckling of a bottlenose dolphin calf(Tursiops truncatus) by a foster mother and information on transverse birth bands mdashAquatic Mamm 16 p 134-138

Lee PC Brennan EJ Else JG amp Altmann J (1986)The ecology and behaviour of vervetmonkeys in a tourist lodge habitat mdash In Primate ecology and conservation (JG Elseamp PC Lee eds) Cambridge University Press Cambridge p 229-236

Loy J (1970) Behavioural responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage mdashAm J Phys Anthropol 33 p 263-272

Mann J (1997) Individual Differences in bottlenose dolphin infants mdash Family Syst 4p 35-49

mdash mdash (1999) Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans A review and critique mdash MarMamm Sci 15 p 102-122

mdash mdash amp Barnett H (1999) Lethal tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) attack on bottlenosedolphin (Tursiops sp) calf Defense and reactions by the mother mdash Mar Mamm Sci15 p 568-575

mdash mdash Connor RC Barre LM amp Heithaus MR (in press) Female reproductive successin bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp) Life history habitat provisioning and group sizeeffects mdash Behav Ecol

mdash mdash Smolker RA amp Smuts BB (1995) Responses to calf entanglement in free-rangingbottlenose dolphins mdash Mar Mamm Sci 11 p 168-175

mdash mdash amp Smuts BB (1993) The behavioural ecology of wild bottlenosedolphins from birthto weaning mdash Paper presented at the Tenth Bienniel Conference on the Biology ofMarine Mammals Conference Galveston TX November 1993

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 37: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN NEWBORNS 565

mdash mdash amp mdash mdash (1998) Natal attraction Allomaternal care and mother-infant separations inwild bottlenose dolphins mdash Anim Behav 55 p 1097-1113

Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans andanthropoid primates mdash Brain Behav Evol 51 p 230-238

Martin P amp Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behaviouraldevelopment mdash Adv Study Behav 15 p 59-103

McBride AF amp Hebb DO (1948) Behaviour of the captive bottle-nose dolphin Tursiopstruncatus mdash J Comp Physiol Psychol 41 p 111-123

mdash mdash amp Kritzler H (1951) Observations of pregnancy parturition and postnatal behaviourin the bottlenose dolphin mdash J Mammol 32 p 251-265

National Marine Fisheries Service (US) (1994) Report to Congress on results of feedingwild dolphins 1989-1994 mdash Prepared by Laurel Bryant NMFS Of ce of ProtectedServices

Norris KS amp Prescott JH (1961) Observations on Paci c cetaceans of California andMexican waters mdash Univ Cal Publ Zool 63 p 291-402

Oleary H amp Fa JE (1993) Effects of tourists on barbary macaques at Gibraltar mdash FoliaPrimatol 61 p 77-91

Orams MB Hill GJ amp Baglioni AJ Jr (1996) lsquoPushyrsquo behavior in a wild dolphinfeeding program at Tangalooma Australia mdash Mar Mamm Sci 12 p 107-117

Packer C amp Pusey AE (1982) Cooperation and competition within coalitions of malelions Kin selection or game theory mdash Nature 296 p 740-741

Peddemors VM (1990) Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphinTursiops truncatus calf mdash S Afr Tydskr Dierk 25 p 178-184

Ralls K Lundrigan B amp Kranz K (1987) Mother-young relationships in captive ungu-lates Behavioural changes over time mdash Ethology 75 p 1-14

Reid K Mann J Weiner JR amp Hecker N (1995) Infant development in two aquariumbottlenose dolphin mdash Zoo Biol 14 p 135-147

Reiss D (1988) Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dol-phins mdash In Animal sonar (PE Nachtigall amp PWB Moore eds) Plenum PublishingNew York p 121-127

Richards AF (1996) Bottlenose dolphin female relationships and reproductive parametersin Shark Bay Australia mdash PhD dissertationUniversity of Michigan

Richards DG Wolz JP amp Herman LM (1984) Vocal mimicry of computer generatedsounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus mdashJ Comp Psychol 98 p 10-28

Shane S (1990) Behaviour and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island Floridamdash In The bottlenose dolphin (S Leatherwood amp RR Reeves eds) Academic PressNew York p 245-265

Small J amp Demaster DP (1995) Survival of ve species of captive marine mammals mdashMar Mamm Sci 11 p 209-226

Smolker RA Mann J amp Smuts BB (1993) Use of signature whistles during separationsand reunions among wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and calves mdash Behav EcolSociobiol 33 p 393-402

mdash mdash Richards AF Connor RC amp Pepper JW (1992) Sex differences in patterns ofassociation among Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins mdash Behaviour 123 p 38-69

Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW amp Struhsaker TT (eds) (1987)Primate societies mdash The University of Chicago Press Chicago

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93

Page 38: BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WILD BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN …€¦ · Bottlenose dolphins form temporary parties whose size and composition changes almost continuously as individuals join

566 MANN amp SMUTS

Sugiyama Y amp Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with specialreference to the effect of arti cial feeding mdash Folia primatol 39 p 238-263

Sweeney JC (1977) Dif cult births and neonatal health problems in small cetace-nas mdash In Breeding dolphins present status suggestions for the future Washing-ton DC (SH Ridgeway amp K Benirschke eds) Marine Mammal Commission ReportNo MMC-76-07

Taber S amp Thomas P (1982) Calf development and mother-calf spatial relationships insouthern right whales mdash Anim Behav 30 p 1072-1083

Tavolga MC (1966) Behaviour of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) Socialinteractions in a captive colony mdash In Whales dolpins and porpoises (KS Norrised) University of California Press Berkeley CA p 718-730

mdash mdash amp Essapian FS (1957) The behaviour of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops trun-catus) Mating pregnancy parturition and mother-infant behaviour mdash Zoologica 42p 11-31

Thomas PO amp Taber SM (1984) Mother-infant interaction and behavioural developmentin southern right whales Eubalaena australis mdash Behaviour 88 p 41-60

Thurman GD amp Williams MC (1986) Neonatal mortality in two Indian Ocean bottlenosedolphins bred in captivity mdash Aquatic Mamm 12 p 83-86

Tyack P (1986) Whistle repertoiresof two bottlenosedolphins Tursiops truncatus mimicryof signature whistles mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18 p 251-257

Tyler S (1979) Time-sampling A matter of convention mdash Anim Behav 27 p 801-810Wells RS amp Scott M D (1997) Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on bottlenose dolphins

near Sarasota Florida mdash Mar Mamm Sci 13 p 475-480mdash mdash mdash mdash amp Irvine AB (1987) The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins

mdash In Current mammalogy Vol 1 (HH Genoways ed) Plenum Press New York andLondon p 247-305

Whitehead H (1996) Babysitting dive synchrony and indications of alloparental care insperm whales mdash Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38 p 237-244

Wrangham RW (1974) Arti cial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their naturalhabitat mdash Anim Behav 22 p 83-93