behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: a self-categorization account

17
Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account David Novelli University of Sussex John Drury University of Sussex Stephen Reicher University of St Andrews Carly Fenn University of Sussex Symposium: The positive crowd: Psychological and social dimensions. 15th General Meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology, Opatija, Croatia, June 10-14th 2008

Upload: saburo

Post on 13-Jan-2016

18 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account David Novelli University of Sussex John Drury University of Sussex Stephen Reicher University of St Andrews Carly Fenn University of Sussex - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-

categorization account

David Novelli

University of Sussex

John Drury

University of Sussex

Stephen Reicher

University of St Andrews

Carly Fenn

University of Sussex

Symposium: The positive crowd: Psychological and social dimensions. 15th General Meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology, Opatija, Croatia, June

10-14th 2008

Page 2: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Crowding 1: the negatives

Focus has been almost exclusively on the potential for negative consequences.

1. Animal studies: high levels of stress, abnormal sexual activity, increased aggression, & cannibalism (Calhoun, 1962, 1976).

2. Prison studies: high blood pressure, increased displays of negative affect, & higher death rates for inmates in crowded dorms (Paulus, Cox, McCain, & Chandler, 1975; Paulus, McCain, & Cox, 1978).

3. Lab studies: participants’ cognitive capabilities (Worchel & Teddlie, 1976), and social relations (Griffith & Veitch, 1971) deteriorated when exposed to ‘crowding’.

4. Link between urban overcrowding and increased stress and aggression (Leyhausen, 1965).

Page 3: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Crowding 2: the positives• However, review articles suggested that the experimental findings and definitions of crowding were inconsistent:

“…it should be determined whether or not some individuals may seek, even prefer, crowded situations.” (Lawrence, 1974, p. 718)

“Crowding may be pleasurable as well as painful. Some people thrill to the excitement of the crowded city. Other things being equal, a large crowd is a

good indication at the theatre, stadium, beach, or party.” (Proshansky, Ittleson, & Rivlin, 1976, p. 179).

• Examples of collective joy evident throughout history: religious rituals, carnivals, festivals, concerts, nightclubs, raves, and sporting events (Ehrenreich, 2007)

• Neville & Reicher (2008): Participants pleased to experience close proximity in crowds when social identity is salient.

• Lack of quantitative evidence

Page 4: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Personal space approach to crowdingPersonal space:

“…an area with invisible boundaries surrounding a person’s body into which intruders may not come.” (Sommer, 1969, p. 26)

Intrusions into a person’s accepted area of ‘personal space’ can influence whether they have a negative experience of crowding (e.g. Evans & Wener, 2007)

How big is a personal space zone?

Some variables which can influence personal space:

1. Culture (Hall, 1966).

2. Location (Cochran, Hale, & Hissam, 1984).

3. Gender role (Uzzell & Horne, 2006).

These variables simply provide a list, rather than a theory of variability in personal space. Intra-individual variation is left largely unexplained.

Page 5: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

V

Crowding on the underground

Page 6: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

SCT approach to personal space/crowding

- Whether a personal or social identity is salient in a particular context will determine whether relationships with others are perceived to be interpersonal, intra-group, or inter-group.

- When interacting with in-group member/s, they will be perceived as an extension of the self. When interactions are inter-group, others will be distinct from the self.

- Therefore, less personal space should be required when contexts are intra-group, rather than inter-group. Close proximity should be experienced more positively in intra-group contexts.

Page 7: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Minimal group identity

- Participants told past researchers identified two distinct cognitive categories – dot ‘over-estimators’ and dot ‘under-estimators’.

- Told that we were interested in the communication styles of category members when interacting with members of same or different category.

- Participants asked to estimate the number of dots in a series of patterns – then randomly assigned to over/under estimator condition.

Page 8: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Hypotheses:

1. Significantly less personal space required when an anticipated interaction is intra-group, rather than inter-group.

2. No difference between ‘under-estimators’’ and ‘over-estimators’’ personal space.

Participants: Female undergraduate students from the University of Sussex (n = 80) invited to participate in a ‘communication study’.

Study 1

Page 9: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 1 – procedure (a)

- Participants arrived at lab and were informed that they would be having a discussion with another participant. Told ‘other’ had already started the experiment in one of two cubicles.

- Participants asked to sit in second cubicle and briefed about ‘cognitive categories’.

- Having completed computer-based estimation tasks, participants informed ‘other’ participant had gone to look for her lost phone but would hopefully return shortly.

- Participants assigned to category (half ‘over-estimators’, half ‘under-estimators’).

- Participants informed of ‘other’s’ group membership.

Page 10: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 1 – procedure (b)

-Experimenter led participant through to ‘discussion’ room.

- Informed participant that ‘other’ participant had set herself up before rushing off to find her phone

- Participant asked to go into ‘discussion’ room, take a chair, and set herself up however she felt most comfortable.

- ‘Other’ participant’s chair was set up in room with a jacket, bag and bottle of water.

- Experimenter left lab, waited outside for several minutes, then re-entered to reveal true nature of the study and to measure the distance between the chairs.

Page 11: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 1 Results

-Intra-group participants sought significantly less personal space than inter-group participants (F (1, 76) = 7.06, p = .010).

- There was not a significant effect of the participants’ identification as a dot ‘over’ or ‘under-estimator’ on their personal space (F(1, 72) = 0.29, p = NS).

Proximity preferences for participants anticipating intra-group and inter-group interactions

3035

404550

5560

Intra-group Inter-group

Condition

Mea

n d

ista

nce

(i

nch

es)

Page 12: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 2

Investigating the experiential dimension of ‘personal space’. Proximity and group context were used as independent variables.

Hypothesis: Participants interacting with an ingroup confederate will have a more positive experience than those interacting with an outgroup confederate, especially when interaction distance is ‘close’.

Participants: Female sixth-form students (n = 60)

Page 13: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 2 – procedure (a)

-Participants given cover story about dot ‘over-estimators’ and ‘under-estimators’.

- Told that they will be taking part in an interview-style role-play so that their interaction style can be observed.

- Interview is actually with a confederate.

- Participants assigned to category and told of confederate’s category membership.

Page 14: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 2 – procedure (b)

-Participant asked to take a seat in ‘interview’ room, in which the confederate was already seated.

- Participant’s chair was either ‘close’ to (34 inches), or ‘far away’ from (54 inches) confederate’s chair.

- Participant always assigned to role of ‘interviewee’.

- Following interview, participant (and confederate) completed questionnaire, which included ‘experience’ items.

Page 15: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Study 2 Results

- Significant main effect of distance on ‘experience’ (F (1, 56) = 14.05, p < .001)

- Significant main effect of categorization on ‘experience’ (F (1, 56) = 10.76, p = .002)

- IMPORTANT: Mean ‘experience’ score for the the inter-group ‘near’ condition was not above the mid-point of the scale. It was for all three of the other conditions (to a significant degree).

Page 16: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

Conclusion

- Findings from two studies support a self-categorization approach to personal space.

- Behavioural and experiential dimensions of personal space dependant on perceived group membership of self and other.

- These findings offer quantitative support to the qualitative evidence provided in the previous presentation (Neville & Reicher, 2008)

- For the first time, we have a possible unifying theory for personal space, and hence for crowding behaviour/experience variability.

Page 17: Behavioural and experiential variations in ‘personal space’: A self-categorization account

ReferencesCalhoun, J. B. (1962). Population density and social pathology. Scientific American, 206, 139-148.

Calhoun, J. B. (1976). The role of space in animal sociology. In: H. M. Proshansky, W. H. Ittelson, & L. G. Rivlin (eds.), Environmental psychology. People and their physical settings, pp. 181-189. USA: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc.

Cochran, C. D., Hale, W. D., & Hissam, C. P. (1984). Personal space requirements in indoor versus outdoor locations. The Journal of Psychology, 117, 121-123.

Ehrenreich, B. (2007). Dancing in the Streets. A History of Collective Joy. London: Granta Books.

Evans, G. W., & Wener, W. E. (2007). Crowding and personal space invasion on the train: Please don’t make me sit in the middle. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 90-94.

Griffith, W., & Veitch, R. (1971). Hot and crowded: Influences of population density and temperature on interpersonal affective behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 92-98.

Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday.

Lawrence, J. E. S. (1974). Science and sentiment: Overview of research on crowding and human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 712-720.

Leyhausen, P. (1965). The sane community – A density problem? Discovery, 26, 27-33.

Neville, F., & Reicher, S. (2008). Positive collective experience: An exploratory study of antecedents and consequences. Paper presented to the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology. Opatija, Croatia.

Paulus, P., Cox, V., McCain, G., & Chandler, J. (1975). Some effects of crowding in a prison environment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 5, 86-91.

Paulus, P. B., McCain, G., & Cox, V. C. (1978). Death rates, psychiatric commitments, blood pressure, and perceived crowding as a function of institutional crowding. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 3, 107-116.

Proshansky, H. M., Ittleson, W. H., & Rivlin, L. G. (1976). Freedom of choice and behavior in a physical setting. In H. M. Proshansky et al. (Eds.), Environmental psychology: People and their physical settings. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston

Sommer, R. (1969). Personal space: The behavioral basis of design. Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Uzzell, D., & Horne, N. (2006). The influence of biological sex, sexuality and gender role on interpersonal distance. British Journal of Social psychology, 45, 579-597.

Worchel, S., & Teddlie, L. (1976). The experience of crowding: A two-factor theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 30-40.