bi/cns/nb 150: neuroscience lecture monday, nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering...

61
Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 Ralph Adolphs Emotion 1 1

Upload: others

Post on 24-Aug-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience

Lecture

Monday, Nov. 23, 2015Ralph Adolphs

Emotion

1

1

Page 2: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Ch. 47: p. 1056-1061Ch. 48 Emotion

Emotion and Social Behavior

Emotions are functional states implemented in the brainEmotion states cause feelings, behavior, psychophysiologyEmotions also cause changes in cognition (attention, memory)Specific brain structures participate in processing emotionsThe amygdala is important for fear

Nov. 23

Ch. 64: p. 1425-1434 Autism

Autism

Autism is a spectrum with an unscientific diagnosisAutism involves atypical social cognitionAutism involves many genes that influence brain connectivityThere are animal models for autism

Nov. 25

review for final Thanksgiving

eat Turkey Nov. 27

We emphasize these points from Kandel in Bi/CNS 150

Read Lecture

2

2

Page 3: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Emotion theoriesANS

fear & disgustEmotion structures

hypothalamusOFC

Amygdala

3

Page 4: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

What is an emotion?

Not cognition

4

Page 5: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Dual Process Theories

System I:--automatic--emotional--rapid

System 2:--effortful/ controlled--rational--slow

5

Page 6: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Some Emotions

• Basic Emotions: happiness, surprise,fear, anger, disgust, sadness

• Social/Moral Emotions: guilt, shame, pride, embarrassment, jealousy

6

Page 7: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Emotions are important!

We (nearly) always feel some emotion

Emotions are what matters most about our experience

7

Page 8: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Killingsworth & Gilbert, Science 2010

A Wandering Mind Is anUnhappy MindMatthew A. Killingsworth* and Daniel T. Gilbert

Unlike other animals, human beings spenda lot of time thinking about what is notgoing on around them, contemplating

events that happened in the past, might happenin the future, or will never happen at all. Indeed,“stimulus-independent thought” or “mind wan-dering” appears to be the brain’s default modeof operation (1–3). Although this ability is a re-markable evolutionary achievement that allowspeople to learn, reason, and plan, it may have anemotional cost. Many philosophical and religioustraditions teach that happiness is to be found byliving in the moment, and practitioners are trainedto resist mind wandering and “to be here now.”These traditions suggest that a wandering mind isan unhappy mind. Are they right?

Laboratory experiments have revealed a greatdeal about the cognitive and neural bases of mindwandering (3–7), but little about its emotionalconsequences in everyday life. The most reliablemethod for investigating real-world emotion is ex-perience sampling, which involves contacting peo-ple as they engage in their everyday activities andasking them to report their thoughts, feelings, andactions at that moment. Unfortunately, collectingreal-time reports from large numbers of people asthey go about their daily lives is so cumbersomeand expensive that experience sampling has rarelybeen used to investigate the relationship betweenmind wandering and happiness and has alwaysbeen limited to very small samples (8, 9).

We solved this problem by developing aWebapplication for the iPhone (Apple Incorporated,Cupertino, California), which we used to createan unusually large database of real-time reportsof thoughts, feelings, and actions of a broad rangeof people as they went about their daily activ-ities. The application contacts participants throughtheir iPhones at random moments during theirwaking hours, presents them with questions,and records their answers to a database at www.trackyourhappiness.org. The database currentlycontains nearly a quarter of a million samplesfrom about 5000 people from 83 different coun-tries who range in age from 18 to 88 and whocollectively represent every one of 86 major oc-cupational categories.

To find out how often people’s minds wander,what topics they wander to, and how those wan-derings affect their happiness, we analyzed samplesfrom 2250 adults (58.8% male, 73.9% residing inthe United States, mean age of 34 years) who wererandomly assigned to answer a happiness question(“How are you feeling right now?”) answered on acontinuous sliding scale from very bad (0) to verygood (100), an activity question (“What are youdoing right now?”) answered by endorsing one or

more of 22 activities adapted from the day recon-struction method (10, 11), and a mind-wanderingquestion (“Are you thinking about somethingother than what you’re currently doing?”) answeredwith one of four options: no; yes, something pleas-ant; yes, something neutral; or yes, something un-pleasant. Our analyses revealed three facts.

First, people’s minds wandered frequently, re-gardless of what they were doing. Mind wanderingoccurred in 46.9% of the samples and in at least30% of the samples taken during every activityexcept making love. The frequency of mind wan-dering in our real-world sample was considerablyhigher than is typically seen in laboratory experi-ments. Surprisingly, the nature of people’s activ-ities had only a modest impact on whether theirminds wandered and had almost no impact on thepleasantness of the topics to which their mindswandered (12).

Second, multilevel regression revealed that peo-ple were less happy when their minds were wan-dering than when they were not [slope (b) = –8.79,P < 0.001], and this was true during all activities,

including the least enjoyable. Although people’sminds were more likely to wander to pleasant topics(42.5% of samples) than to unpleasant topics(26.5% of samples) or neutral topics (31% of sam-ples), people were no happier when thinking aboutpleasant topics than about their current activity (b =–0.52, not significant) and were considerably un-happier when thinking about neutral topics (b =–7.2, P < 0.001) or unpleasant topics (b = –23.9,P < 0.001) than about their current activity (Fig. 1,bottom). Although negative moods are knownto cause mind wandering (13), time-lag analysesstrongly suggested that mind wandering in oursample was generally the cause, and not merelythe consequence, of unhappiness (12).

Third, what people were thinking was a betterpredictor of their happiness than was what theywere doing. The nature of people’s activities ex-plained 4.6% of the within-person variance in hap-piness and 3.2% of the between-person variance inhappiness, but mind wandering explained 10.8%of within-person variance in happiness and 17.7%of between-person variance in happiness. The var-iance explained by mind wandering was largelyindependent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities, suggesting that the two were in-dependent influences on happiness.

In conclusion, a human mind is a wanderingmind, and a wandering mind is an unhappy mind.The ability to think about what is not happeningis a cognitive achievement that comes at an emo-tional cost.

References and Notes1. M. E. Raichle et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676

(2001).2. K. Christoff, A. M. Gordon, J. Smallwood, R. Smith,

J. W. Schooler, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8719(2009).

3. R. L. Buckner, J. R. Andrews-Hanna, D. L. Schacter,Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1 (2008).

4. J. Smallwood, J. W. Schooler, Psychol. Bull. 132, 946 (2006).5. M. F. Mason et al., Science 315, 393 (2007).6. J. Smallwood, E. Beach, J. W. Schooler, T. C. Handy,

J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 458 (2008).7. R. L. Buckner, D. C. Carroll, Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49 (2007).8. J. C. McVay, M. J. Kane, T. R. Kwapil, Psychon. Bull. Rev.

16, 857 (2009).9. M. J. Kane et al., Psychol. Sci. 18, 614 (2007).

10. D. Kahneman, A. B. Krueger, D. A. Schkade, N. Schwarz,A. A. Stone, Science 306, 1776 (2004).

11. A. B. Krueger, D. A. Schkade, J. Public Econ. 92, 1833 (2008).12. Materials and methods are available as supporting

material on Science Online.13. J. Smallwood, A. Fitzgerald, L. K. Miles, L. H. Phillips,

Emotion 9, 271 (2009).14. We thank V. Pitiyanuvath for engineering www.

trackyourhappiness.org and R. Hackman, A. Jenkins,W. Mendes, A. Oswald, and T. Wilson for helpful comments.

Supporting Online Materialwww.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/330/6006/932/DC1Materials and MethodsTable S1References

18 May 2010; accepted 29 September 201010.1126/science.1192439

BREVIA

Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.

*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:[email protected]

Fig. 1. Mean happiness reported during each ac-tivity (top) and while mind wandering to unpleas-ant topics, neutral topics, pleasant topics or notmind wandering (bottom). Dashed line indicatesmean of happiness across all samples. Bubble areaindicates the frequency of occurrence. The largestbubble (“not mind wandering”) corresponds to53.1% of the samples, and the smallest bubble(“praying/worshipping/meditating”) corresponds to0.1% of the samples.

12 NOVEMBER 2010 VOL 330 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org932

on

Nov

embe

r 22,

201

0w

ww

.sci

ence

mag

.org

Dow

nloa

ded

from

8

Page 9: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

There is also a big field of study of how we INFER what emotions people are having

Social perception--> attributions --> social behavior

9

Page 10: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

10

Page 11: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

11

Page 12: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Process Theories of Emotion

12

Page 13: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Motor output at different levels

Reflexes --spinal --central

"Fixed action patterns"Emotional reactionsActionsLong-term plans

Stimulus-coupled

Stimulus decoupled

13

Page 14: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Some characteristics of an emotion

1. Phasic (vs. moods)

2. Has onset, duration, decay

3. In humans, often regulated

4. Can be broken down into some components

ExpectationContext

Feeling

Individual Differences

Stimulus Evaluation EmotionalResponse

14

Page 15: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

15

Page 16: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

16

Page 17: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

William James (1882): "What is an emotion?"

--perception of our emotional response

--pattern of bodily response discriminates emotions

“If we fancy some strong emotion, and then try to abstract from our consciousness of it all the feelings of its characteristic bodily symptoms, we find we have nothing left behind, no “mind-stuff” out of which emotion can be constituted, and that a cold and neutral state of intellectual perception is all that remains.”

17

Page 18: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

18

Page 19: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Psychophysiology

Galvanic Skin Conductance (GSR, SCR)

Electrocardiogram (EKG)

Facial Electromyogram (facial EMG)

Pupillometry, Respiration, Skin temperature, blood pressure

19

Page 20: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Figure 10.28a

20

Page 21: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Charles Darwin (1872)

"The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals"

--3 principles:

serviceable associated habits

antithesis

direct action of nervous system

--idea of “basic” emotions

21

Page 22: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

22

Page 23: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

23

Page 24: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Neuron

Perspective

Rethinking the Emotional Brain

Joseph LeDoux1,2,*1Center for Neural Science and Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003 USA2Emotional Brain Institute, New York University and Nathan Kline Institute, Orangeburg, NY 10962 USA*Correspondence: [email protected] 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004

I propose a reconceptualization of key phenomena important in the study of emotion—those phenomena thatreflect functions and circuits related to survival, and that are shared by humans and other animals. Theapproach shifts the focus from questions about whether emotions that humans consciously feel are alsopresent in other animals, and toward questions about the extent to which circuits and corresponding func-tions that are present in other animals (survival circuits and functions) are also present in humans. Survivalcircuit functions are not causally related to emotional feelings but obviously contribute to these, at least indi-rectly. The survival circuit concept integrates ideas about emotion, motivation, reinforcement, and arousal inthe effort to understand how organisms survive and thrive by detecting and responding to challenges andopportunities in daily life.

IntroductionEmotion is a major research growth area in neuroscience andpsychology today. A search of PubMed citations for the 1960syields just over 100 papers with the word ‘‘emotion’’ in the title.With each subsequent decade, small increases resulted, untilthe last decade, when emotion titles grew exponentially—morethan 2,000 hits. Emotion has happened.But what exactly is it that has happened? What is being

studied in all these papers on emotion? Actually, the term‘‘emotion’’ is not well defined in most publications. Perhapsthis is not surprising since there is little consensus about whatemotion is, and how it differs from other aspects of mind andbehavior, in spite of discussion and debate that dates back tothe earliest days in modern biology and psychology (e.g., Dar-win, 1872; James, 1884; Cannon, 1927, 1931; Duffy, 1934,1941; Tomkins, 1962; Mandler, 1975; Schachter, 1975; Ekman,1980, 1984, 1992; Izard, 2007; Frijda, 1986; Russell, 2003;; Ek-man and Davidson, 1994; LeDoux, 1996; Panksepp, 1998,2000, 2005; Rolls, 1999, 2005; Damasio, 1994, 1999; Leventhaland Scherer, 1987; Scherer, 2000; Ortony and Turner, 1990; Oh-man, 1986, 2009; Johnson-Laird and Oatley, 1989; Ellsworth,1994; Zajonc, 1980; Lazarus, 1981, 1991a, 1991b; Barrett,2006a, 2006b; Barrett et al., 2007; Kagan, 2007; Prinz, 2004;Scarantino, 2009; Griffiths, 2004; Ochsner and Gross, 2005;Lyons, 1980).One point that many writers on this topic accept is that, while

there are unique features of human emotion, at least someaspects of human emotion reflect our ancestral past. Thisconclusion is the basis of neurobiological approaches toemotion, since animal research is essential for identifyingspecific circuits and mechanisms in the brain that underlieemotional phenomena.Progress in understanding emotional phenomena in the brains

of laboratory animals has in fact helped elucidate emotionalfunctions in the human brain, including pathological aspects ofemotion. But what does this really mean? If we don’t have anagreed-upon definition of emotion that allows us to say what

emotion is, and how emotion differs from other psychologicalstates, how can we study emotion in animals or humans, andhow can we make comparisons between species?The short answer is that we fake it. Introspections from

personal subjective experiences tell us that some mental stateshave a certain ‘‘feeling’’ associated with them and others do not.Those states that humans associate with feelings are oftencalled emotions. The terms ‘‘emotion’’ and ‘‘feeling’’ are, infact, often used interchangeably. In English we have words likefear, anger, love, sadness, jealousy, and so on, for these feelingstates, and when scientists study emotions in humans they typi-cally use these ‘‘feeling words’’ as guideposts to explore theterrain of emotion.The wisdom of using common language words that refer to

feelings as a means of classifying and studying humanemotions has been questioned by a number of authors overthe years (e.g., Duffy, 1934, 1941; Mandler, 1975; Russell,1991, 2003; Barrett, 2006a, 2006b; Kagan, 2007; Griffiths,1997; Rorty, 1980; Dixon, 2001; Zachar, 2006). Whatever prob-lems might arise from using feeling words to study humanemotion, the complications are compounded many fold whensuch words are applied to other animals. While there arecertainly emotional phenomena that are shared by humansand other animals, introspections from human subjective expe-rience are not the best starting point for pursuing these. How,then, should the aspects of emotion relevant to animals and hu-mans alike be pursued?In answering this question it is important to separate the

phenomena of interest from the overarching concept of emotion.One set of such phenomena includes responses that occur whenan organism detects and responds to significant events in thecourse of surviving and/or maintaining well-being—for example,responses that occur when in danger or when in the presence ofa potential mate or in the presence of food when hungry or drinkwhen thirsty. These are fundamental phenomena that havealways interested animal behavior scientists, and would be ofinterest even if the terms ‘‘emotion’’ and ‘‘feelings’’ never

Neuron 73, February 23, 2012 ª2012 Elsevier Inc. 653

LeDoux, Neuron 73:653-676 (2012)

- Do not use “emotion” scientifically

Why?

1. Because most people confuse “emotion” with “feelings”2. And we cannot tell if animals have feelings

24

Page 25: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

1. Emotions are Internal/Central Functional States2. Emotions are caused by particular stimuli/contexts3. Emotions CAUSE behavior4. Emotions CAUSE feelings

5. Emotions have characteristic features

Anderson & Adolphs, “A framework for studying emotion across species.” Cell (2014)

25

Page 26: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

stimuli centralemotional state

subjective reports

observed behavior

psychophysiology

cognitive changes

somatic responses

26

Page 27: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

stimuli centralemotional state

subjective reports

observed behavior

psychophysiology

cognitive changes

somatic responsescontext volitional control

27

Page 28: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

1. Scalability (intensity)

2. Valence (antithesis)

3. Persistence

4. Context and Stimulus Generalization

5. Pleiotropy

6. Priority over Behavioral Control

7. Poised for Social communication

Similarity structure (2-D)

Flexibility

Automaticity

28

Page 29: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

1. Scalability (intensity)

2. Valence (antithesis)

3. Persistence

4. Context and Stimulus Generalization

5. Pleiotropy

6. Priority over Behavioral Control

7. Poised for Social communication

Similarity structure (2-D)

Flexibility

Automaticity

Uniquely Human Aspects?

8. Control9. Subjective Report10. Stimulus Decoupling

29

Page 30: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

30

Page 31: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

31

Page 32: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Modules for recognizing emotions from faces

- Fear: amygdala- Disgust: basal ganglia (HD), insula

32

Page 33: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 11 | NUMBER 7 | JULY 2008 739

N E W S A N D V I E W S

insular cortex. Neuroimaging, as well as depth electrode recording studies, show that human insular cortex responds to disgusted faces9,10, and damage to the insula produces deficits in the recognition of disgusted faces11. Furthermore, insular pathology in Huntington’s disorder is associated with a decreased disgust response and a similar recognition deficit of disgust12. Although it is tempting to divide duties for the amygdala and insular cortex between fear and disgust and to suggest that they represent unique neural substrates for their processing, other work suggests that these reciprocally connected regions will interact along dimensions such as arousal, valence and/or attention, dimensions that cross the stark boundary between these two expression categories13.

One key point raised by Susskind et al.1 is whether sensory intake is the only selection pressure that shaped fearful and disgusted expressions. It is probable that multiple selection pressures are involved. For example, it has been suggested14 that fearful faces look the way they do because such a facial configuration increases the apparent ‘babyishness’ of the expresser and, therefore, serves to elicit nurturing or protective responses from conspecifics. So, are different hypothesized selection pressures such as these mutually exclusive or can they peacefully coexist?

are observed at times when you would do well to learn more about your surroundings and disgusted expressions are observed at times when you feel you have learned quite enough.

That eye widening should facilitate environmental monitoring is consistent with other data showing that the amygdala has a unique relationship with fearful facial expressions. Indeed, the amygdala uses the widened eyes in fearful expressions as a proxy for the presence of fearful faces2, and lesions of the human amygdala result in an inability to properly scan the eye whites in fearful faces3. Further, electrical stimulation of the human amygdala results in eye widening and heightened visual scanning of the environment4 and produces nonspecific arousal responses such as pupil dilation4. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the amygdala is sensitive to the pupil size of others5,6. Taken together, these findings support the hypothesized role of the amygdala in potentiating sensory information processing to facilitate learning about the predictive value of environmental events, a process referred to as associative orienting7,8. More generally, these results suggest that your amygdala is sensitive to the very facial responses in others that it controls in you.

If the amygdala has shown an affinity for the facial expression of fear, then the analogous brain region for disgusted expressions would be the

The human face is a canvas on which we can paint any number of different facial features to nonverbally communicate with those around us. These facial configurations need not be complicated. A simple raise of the eyebrow from a mother will halt an 11-year-old child right in his tracks.

But why do facial expressions look the way they do? Upturned corners of a mouth are seen as a good thing, whereas downturned corners are bad. Could it have gone the other way? Are the shapes of our facial expressions random or do these different configurations exist because they each support some specific adaptive purpose? In this issue, Susskind et al.1 asked these questions of fearful and disgusted expressions.

In their study, subjects posed fearful and disgusted faces in various conditions. In one condition, subjects had to detect quick flashes of light on a wall that encompassed the entirety of their peripheral vision (much like a visit to the eye doctor). In another condition, the speed of subjects’ eye movements to targets in their visual field was measured. And in yet another condition, the amount of air that subjects inspired through the nose was measured. Across all conditions, facilitated intake of sensory information was observed when subjects made fearful faces, whereas attenuated sensory intake was observed when subjects made disgusted faces.

To elaborate, when subjects posed fearful faces, they were able to detect more flashes of light in the upper extent of their peripheral visual field, they were faster to move their eyes to targets in their visual field and they took in more air through the nose. In contrast, when they posed disgusted expressions, they detected fewer flashes of light in the upper extent of their visual field, they were slower to move their eyes to visual targets and they took in less air through the nose. Thus, the converse movements of the brows, eyelids and nose enacted in the formation of these expressions had opposite effects on sensory intake (Fig. 1). This makes sense when one considers the situations where these expressions are encountered. Fearful expressions

The authors are at the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755, USA. e-mail: [email protected]

The shape of faces (to come)Paul J Whalen & Robert E Kleck

Have facial expressions evolved randomly or do their different shapes support some adaptive purpose? New work offers evidence of a selection pressure that may have shaped fearful and disgusted expressions.

Figure 1 Fearful versus disgusted expressions. Note how the various parts of the face move in opposite directions in the two expressions, and therefore have opposite effects on sensory intake. Facial images from the NimStim set of facial expressions (MacArthur Foundation, http://www.macbrain.org/resources.htm).

Lowered browNarrowed eyes

Narrowed nostrilsClosed mouth

a Disgust

Raised browWidened eyesFlared nostrilsOpen mouth

b Fear

33

Page 34: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 11 | NUMBER 7 | JULY 2008 739

N E W S A N D V I E W S

insular cortex. Neuroimaging, as well as depth electrode recording studies, show that human insular cortex responds to disgusted faces9,10, and damage to the insula produces deficits in the recognition of disgusted faces11. Furthermore, insular pathology in Huntington’s disorder is associated with a decreased disgust response and a similar recognition deficit of disgust12. Although it is tempting to divide duties for the amygdala and insular cortex between fear and disgust and to suggest that they represent unique neural substrates for their processing, other work suggests that these reciprocally connected regions will interact along dimensions such as arousal, valence and/or attention, dimensions that cross the stark boundary between these two expression categories13.

One key point raised by Susskind et al.1 is whether sensory intake is the only selection pressure that shaped fearful and disgusted expressions. It is probable that multiple selection pressures are involved. For example, it has been suggested14 that fearful faces look the way they do because such a facial configuration increases the apparent ‘babyishness’ of the expresser and, therefore, serves to elicit nurturing or protective responses from conspecifics. So, are different hypothesized selection pressures such as these mutually exclusive or can they peacefully coexist?

are observed at times when you would do well to learn more about your surroundings and disgusted expressions are observed at times when you feel you have learned quite enough.

That eye widening should facilitate environmental monitoring is consistent with other data showing that the amygdala has a unique relationship with fearful facial expressions. Indeed, the amygdala uses the widened eyes in fearful expressions as a proxy for the presence of fearful faces2, and lesions of the human amygdala result in an inability to properly scan the eye whites in fearful faces3. Further, electrical stimulation of the human amygdala results in eye widening and heightened visual scanning of the environment4 and produces nonspecific arousal responses such as pupil dilation4. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the amygdala is sensitive to the pupil size of others5,6. Taken together, these findings support the hypothesized role of the amygdala in potentiating sensory information processing to facilitate learning about the predictive value of environmental events, a process referred to as associative orienting7,8. More generally, these results suggest that your amygdala is sensitive to the very facial responses in others that it controls in you.

If the amygdala has shown an affinity for the facial expression of fear, then the analogous brain region for disgusted expressions would be the

The human face is a canvas on which we can paint any number of different facial features to nonverbally communicate with those around us. These facial configurations need not be complicated. A simple raise of the eyebrow from a mother will halt an 11-year-old child right in his tracks.

But why do facial expressions look the way they do? Upturned corners of a mouth are seen as a good thing, whereas downturned corners are bad. Could it have gone the other way? Are the shapes of our facial expressions random or do these different configurations exist because they each support some specific adaptive purpose? In this issue, Susskind et al.1 asked these questions of fearful and disgusted expressions.

In their study, subjects posed fearful and disgusted faces in various conditions. In one condition, subjects had to detect quick flashes of light on a wall that encompassed the entirety of their peripheral vision (much like a visit to the eye doctor). In another condition, the speed of subjects’ eye movements to targets in their visual field was measured. And in yet another condition, the amount of air that subjects inspired through the nose was measured. Across all conditions, facilitated intake of sensory information was observed when subjects made fearful faces, whereas attenuated sensory intake was observed when subjects made disgusted faces.

To elaborate, when subjects posed fearful faces, they were able to detect more flashes of light in the upper extent of their peripheral visual field, they were faster to move their eyes to targets in their visual field and they took in more air through the nose. In contrast, when they posed disgusted expressions, they detected fewer flashes of light in the upper extent of their visual field, they were slower to move their eyes to visual targets and they took in less air through the nose. Thus, the converse movements of the brows, eyelids and nose enacted in the formation of these expressions had opposite effects on sensory intake (Fig. 1). This makes sense when one considers the situations where these expressions are encountered. Fearful expressions

The authors are at the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755, USA. e-mail: [email protected]

The shape of faces (to come)Paul J Whalen & Robert E Kleck

Have facial expressions evolved randomly or do their different shapes support some adaptive purpose? New work offers evidence of a selection pressure that may have shaped fearful and disgusted expressions.

Figure 1 Fearful versus disgusted expressions. Note how the various parts of the face move in opposite directions in the two expressions, and therefore have opposite effects on sensory intake. Facial images from the NimStim set of facial expressions (MacArthur Foundation, http://www.macbrain.org/resources.htm).

Lowered browNarrowed eyes

Narrowed nostrilsClosed mouth

a Disgust

Raised browWidened eyesFlared nostrilsOpen mouth

b Fear

34

Page 35: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

marked increase in the upper (F1,266¼ 174.70, Po 0.0001), but not thelower visual field (F1,266 ¼ 1.13, P 4 0.29), consistent with the browand upper eye-lid raise (Figs. 2a and 3). In contrast, disgust resulted ina decrease in the upper (F1,266 ¼ 113.51, P o 0.0001) and lower fields(F1,266¼ 50.05, Po 0.0001), consistent with brow lowering and uppercheek raise (Figs. 2b and 3). Fear’s enhancement only for the uppervisual field may be argued as having limited functional importance.However, the lower visual field under neutral viewing conditions moreclosely approximates complete coverage, whereas the upper visual fieldis substantially more restrictive, in part reflecting occlusion from theeyelid and brow34. The functional importance of fear expressions mayreflect the retraction of these anatomical features, affording a morecomplete visual field.We next examined whether changes in estimated visual field size were

specifically related to the changes in physical differences in eye aperturethat are associated with expression generation. Eye regions were out-lined from film clips taken during the directed facial action task (seeMethods) to determine the extent of eye opening in each participant. Aspredicted, fear expressions resulted in greater vertical eye openingrelative to disgust (F1,18 ¼ 148.08, P o 0.0001). We next used thephysical distance between participants’ lower and upper lids to predicttheir visual-field size by correlating it with visual-field changes along thevertical meridian (Fig. 3d). Vertical eye aperture correlated significantlywith perceived changes in the upper visual field (r¼ 0.593, t(18)¼ 4.41,P o 0.001). These results show that the antagonistic eye opening andclosing associated with fear and disgust regulate visual-field size.Fear may induce subjective changes in visual-field perception, but a

stronger test of sensory vigilance is to show that peripheral stimulusdetection is altered by expression. To corroborate the subjective results,we applied an objective visual perimetry procedure in an additionalexperiment to test stimulus detection–eccentricity thresholds at upperhorizontal, oblique and vertical meridians while participants posed fear,neutral and disgust facial configurations (see Methods). Stimulusdetection differed with respect to expression condition (F2,26 ¼ 28.675,P o 0.001), with the effect of expression depending on visual-fieldlocation (F8,104¼ 14.856, Po 0.001). Specifically, in the fear condition,participants detected objects at farther eccentricities than in the neutralcondition for the upper visual field (F1,104 ¼ 84.182, P o 0.001), butnot for the horizontal meridian (F1,104¼ 0.802, P¼ 0.373). Conversely,stimulus detection in the upper visual field was reduced for disgustrelative to neutral (F1,104 ¼ 105.909, P o 0.001), but not for thehorizontal meridian (F1,104 ¼ 1.829, P ¼ 0.179). An analysis of catchtrials in which no stimulus was present did not reveal any differences in

response bias across expression types (Fried-man test, w2(3) ¼ 0.931, P ¼ 0.818). In sum,these results demonstrate that fear expressionsenhance and disgust expressions reduce stimu-lus detection in the upper visual field, consis-tent with opposing eye lid and brow actionsregulating retinal exposure35.Eye opening and brow raising in fear may

reflect not only altered functional visual-fieldsize, but also multiple functional benefits,affording a more effective sampling of theentire visual field. Eye movements are instru-mental in acquiring information from thevisual field, and fear expressions may activatemuscle units in preparation to facilitate scan-ning. We measured participants’ eye move-ments during horizontal saccades to examinewhether fear expressions are associated with

increased eye-scanning speed relative to neutral expressions andwhether fear expressions have opposing effects to disgust (see Meth-ods). Employing the directed facial-action task, participants wereprompted to pose a facial expression for 5 s while carrying out saccadesbetween two circular targets separated by 271. The facial actions for fearand disgust were modified to focus on eye configurations to accom-modate an eye tracker and chin rest. Expression configuration had asignificant effect on average velocity (measured in degrees of visualangle per second, F2,38¼ 18.001, Po 0.001; Fig. 4a), increasing linearlyfrom disgust to neutral to fear (F1,38 ¼ 33.975, Po 0.0001). Althoughdifferences were small in magnitude, eye movements for fear expres-sions were reliably faster than those for neutral expressions (t(19) ¼2.133, P o 0.05), and disgust expressions resulted in a morepronounced slowing relative to neutral (t(19) ¼ 4.42, P o 0.0003).Peak velocity also differed between expressions (F2,38 ¼ 7.812, P ¼0.001), linearly increasing from disgust to neutral to fear (F1,38¼ 15.12,P o 0.0004). A linear fit between the condition means (r2 ¼ 0.99)revealed that peak and average velocities increased from disgust tofear (Fig. 4b). Further analyses revealed that the amplitude (distance)of saccades differed across expressions (F2,34 ¼ 6.68, P o 0.004),paralleling the linear increase from disgust to fear, which mayhave contributed to expression differences in velocity. However,we found that after equating for saccade distance, the abovesignificant differences in velocity remained statistically reliable. Thus,

©20

08 N

atur

e P

ublis

hing

Gro

up h

ttp:

//ww

w.n

atur

e.co

m/n

atur

eneuroscience

120°

150°

180°

210°

DisgustFear

240°270°

300°

Mag

nitu

de e

stim

atio

n ±

s.e.

m.

Eye

ape

rtur

e

330°

–6 –4 –2 0

–6 –4 –2

Visual field

2 40

30°

1.5 0.04

0.02

0–0.01

–0.03

–0.05

1.0

0.5

–0.5

–1.0

–1.5

0

60°90°a b c d

Figure 3 Subjective visual-field changes for participants posing fear and disgust expressions.(a) Changes in visual field estimation along horizontal, vertical and oblique axes. Central ellipse isneutral baseline. Unit markings are in 9.51 of visual angle. (b) Change in estimated visual-field size(in standardized units) for fear and disgust expressions relative to neutral expressions, averaged acrossvisual-field location. (c) Average eye opening from participants posing disgust, neutral and fearexpressions (from top to bottom row). (d) Correlation of vertical eye-size measurements of participantsposing disgust and fear expressions with upper visual-field magnitude change from neutral.

10

a b555

545

535295 300

Average velocity (deg s–1) ± s.e.m.

305 310

540

5505

–5

–10

Eye

vel

ocity

min

us n

eutr

al

Pea

k ve

loci

ty (

deg

s–1)

± s.

e.m

.

–15

–2020 40

FearDisgust

60

Time (ms)

800

0

FearNeutralDisgust

Figure 4 Saccadic eye movements for participants posing fear, disgust andneutral expressions. (a) Average velocity over time profiles for fear anddisgust expressions relative to neutral. Relative velocities were scaled ateach time point by the ratio of instantaneous-to-peak neutral velocity.(b) Average versus peak velocities for each condition are plotted along withthe best-fit line between the condition means (r 2 ¼ 0.996) with within-subject error bars.

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION 3

ART ICLES

Susskind et al., Nat Neurosci 2008

35

Page 36: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

in addition to altering visual-field size, facial actions of fear and disgustenhance and decrease the speed of horizontal foveation during targetlocalization, respectively.The above studies focused on the function of facial actions in

the upper region of the face. We next examined the functionalconsequences of facial actions associated with the mid-lower regionof the face by examining whether expression appearance may also relateto more primitive chemosensory functions. Disgust has been proposedto originate in defensive reactions related to the rejection of noxiouschemical stimulation, whereby sensory exposure is reduced to potentialcontaminants25. According to the principle of form, fear configurationsshould serve the opposing action of disgust. We examined whether fear

and disgust facial expressions regulate the entry of airborne substancesto the olfactory mucosa through one of the most basic and primitiveforms of sensory intake, nasal inspiratory capacity36 (see Methods).Facial expressionwas manipulated via the directed facial-action task,

while nasal respirometry, nasal temperature and abdominal-thoracicrespiratory measures were acquired during a controlled instructedbreathing cycle (2.2 s in/out per breath). Expression configuration hada significant effect on air intake (F2,36 ¼ 9.93, P o 0.0005), linearlyincreasing from disgust to neutral to fear (F1,36 ¼ 19.73, P o 0.0001).Despite equal duration of inspiration, fear was associated with anincrease in air velocity and volume corrected for respiratory effortrelative to neutral expressions (t(18) ¼ 2.30, P o 0.04; Fig. 5a,b). Incontrast, disgust was diminished relative to neutral (t(18) ¼ 2.34, P o0.04). To ensure that these effects were not artifacts of the effect ofexpression on airflowmeasurements with a nasal respirometry mask, wefurther examined inspiratory-related temperature changes under thenostrils with a wire nasal thermistor. This revealed an identical pattern(F2,34 ¼ 12.07, Po 0.0001), showing a linear effect of expression (F1,34¼ 23.57, P o 0.0001), with fear expressions being associated withgreater temperature change relative to neutral (t(17) ¼ 2.77, Po 0.02)and disgust being diminished relative to neutral (t(17)¼ 2.07, P¼ 0.05).Altered air intake may reflect a variety of factors rather than genuine

structural changes in sensory capacity afforded by facial expressionform. We next sought to determine whether visible surface deforma-tions of the face (that is, facial expression appearance) are associatedwith invisible underlying structural changes of the internal anatomy ofthe nasal passages. In an additional control study, we examined directlywhether fear and disgust altered the structure of the nasal passages inopposing manners. High-resolution fast spoiled gradient–recalled(FSPGR) magnetic resonance images of the nasal passages wereacquired during the directed facial-action task (see Methods). Nasalvolume depended on expression (F2,72 ¼ 14.98, P o 0.0001), whichwas most evident in the more inferior aspects of the nasal passages

©20

08 N

atur

e P

ublis

hing

Gro

up h

ttp:

//ww

w.n

atur

e.co

m/n

atur

eneuroscience

0.4 0.7

0.5Fear

Disgust

FearDisgust

0.3

0.1

–0.1

–0.3

–0.5

0.2

Air

velo

city

min

us n

eutr

al

Vol

ume

min

us n

eutr

al ±

s.e

.m.

–0.2

50 500

1,000

1,500

2,000

2,500

Time (ms)3,0

004,0

003,5

00–0.4

0

a b

Figure 5 Measurement of nasal inspiratory capacity during expressions ofdisgust and fear. (a,b) Mean air-flow velocity (in standardized units) for fearand disgust expressions relative to neutral during inhalation over time (2.2-sinhalation; a) and mean volume relative to abdominal-thoracic respiratoryeffort (in standardized units) for disgust and fear expressions relative toneutral (b). Velocity was scaled such that the area under the curve for neutralsniffs was equal to 1.

200Fear

Disgust150

35FearDisgust25

15

5

–5

–15

–25

50

–50

–1001 2 3 4 5 6

Slice (inferior to superior)7 8 9 10

0

100

Vol

ume

(mm

3 ) m

inus

neu

tral

Vol

ume

(mm

3 ) m

inus

neut

ral ±

s.e

.m.

a c

d eb

Figure 6 Effects of fear and disgust expressions on internal nasal anatomy. (a,b) Axial slices from T1-weighted FSPGR MRI during fear (a) and disgust (b)expressions. (c) Passageways to the inferior turbinate of the respiratory mucosa. Relative to neutral expressions (middle panel), disgust (left) and fear (right)expressions resulted in closure and dilation, respectively. (d) Plot of the volume of air cavity of the ventral portion (12 mm) of the nasal passages for fear anddisgust expressions relative to neutral. Each slice was 1.2-mm thick with an in-plane resolution of 0.86 " 0.86 mm. (e) Bar graphs represent average overallair cavity volume for fear and disgust expressions relative to neutral.

4 ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION NATURE NEUROSCIENCE

ART ICLES

36

Page 37: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

InsulaCingulate

EmotionPerception

AssociationInduction

EmotionalResponse

FeelingAwareness

SensoryCortices

AmygdalaOFC

HypothalamusPAG

37

Page 38: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Walter Hess

A key aggression relay in the mouse

23Lin et al., Nature (2011)

LacZ

Light 1 minMount Attack

Test 1

Test 2: 6 days later

1 mW/mm0

20

40

60

80

100

% o

f T

ria

ls I

nd

uc

ing

Atta

ck

0

2

4

6

La

ten

cy (

s)

**

0

20

40

60

80

100

% o

f T

ria

ls I

nd

uc

ing

Atta

ck

f

k m

o

EYFP LacZEYFP

LacZ

EYFP

LacZ

Nissl

g

Fos

h

***

ns p

Figure 4

0

20

40

60

80

100

Non-

moving Moving

Anaesthetized

AnimalGlove

Non-

moving Moving%

of A

nim

als

Att

ackin

g

l

n

Attack

No Change

n = 27

i j

b c d e

Freeze/Flight

p = 0.06

Castrated Male IntruderFemale Intruder

Castrated Male IntruderFemale Intruder

a

Fiber Optic CableCannula

Between

Intromission

After

Ejaculation

Intromission

Before

Mounting

Attack onset Attack offset Optimized

Light Intensity

EYFP

Fos

Nissl

EYFP

Fos

EYFP

2

VMHdm (cells/section)0 20 40 600

20

40

60

VM

Hvl p

oste

rio

r (c

ells

/se

ctio

n)

A key aggression relay in the mouse

25

“sham rage”

38

Page 39: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

39

Page 40: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Putative functions of some brain structures

Amygdala:--fear, arousal, saliency

Insula:--disgust, empathy, pain

OFC:--valence,decisions, social emotions, moral judgment

Cingulate cortex:--pain, motivation ("akinetic mutism")

40

Page 41: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

41

Page 42: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Orbitofrontal Cortex

-emotional and social behavior-represents reward value of stimuli

-involved in complex decision-making

42

Page 43: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

43

Page 44: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

The equilibrium or balance, so to speak, between his (Gage’s) intellectual faculties and animal propensities seems to have been destroyed. He is fitful, irreverent, indulging at times in the grossest profanity … impatient of restraint or advice when it conflicts with his desires. John Harlow, 1868

44

Page 45: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

45

Page 46: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

46

Page 47: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

47

Page 48: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Amygdala

48

Page 49: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

1. The Amygdala’s role in implicit emotional memory (like fear conditioning) is doubly dissociable with the role of the hippocampus in declarative memory.

2. The amygdala is important for learning the value of stimuli, and for regulating social behavior, especially for fear or withdrawal-related behaviors.

3. The amygdala modulates much other cognition.

4. The amygdala shows individual differences and contributes to psychiatric illnesses such as mood disorders.

49

Page 50: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

50

Page 51: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Heinrich Kluver

“Kluver-Bucy Syndrome”

51

Page 52: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

52

Page 53: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

--tameness

--oral tendencies

--hypersexual

--hypermetamorphosis

--psychic blindness

--altered taste preferences

53

Page 54: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Amygdala Lesions in Rats

54

Page 55: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Choi & Kim, PNAS (2010)

55

Page 56: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

56

Page 57: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

The brain of patient S.M.

57

Page 58: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Measuring the Experience of Fear

1. Standardized Questionnaires2. Express fear to instruction3. Fear to films4. Fear in real life: autobiographical5. Fear in real life: pet store

Feinstein et al., Current Biology (2011)

58

Page 59: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Experience sampling with PDA

--3 months--624 samples

--0 reports of afraid/scared/frightened

Emotional films

--no endorsement of fear

Autobiography

--no fear since age 10

59

Page 60: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

60

Page 61: Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience Lecture Monday, Nov. 23, 2015 ... · iance explained by mind wandering was largely independent of the variance explained by the na-ture of activities,

Summary

Emotions are a ubiquitous and salient aspect of our conscious experience

Fear has been mechanistically dissected in great neurobiological detail, and is an excellent model system

The amygdala is necessary not only for fear behaviors in rodents, but also for fear experience in humans

Fear is not in the amygdala, but generated by all the multiple processes orchestrated by the amygdala

61