black&asl&as&aseparate&variety& · preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582...

10
10/26/10 1 Black ASL as a Separate Variety Joseph Hill, Carolyn McCaskill, Ceil Lucas and Robert Bayley TISLR 2010 Purdue University Black ASL project at a glance The objecJves of this fouryear project are: To create a filmed corpus of conversaJonal Black ASL as it is used in the South. The focus is on the structure and history of Southern Black ASL because that region is where the most radical segregaJon occurred in the educaJon of Black and White deaf children, creaJng the condiJons for the development of a separate language variety. To provide a descripJon of the linguisJc features that make Black ASL recognizable as a disJnct variety of ASL and of the history of the educaJon of Black Deaf children. To disseminate the project findings in the form of teaching materials and instrucJonal resources. Basic QuesJons for the Project (1) What are the features of the variety of ASL that people call “Black ASL”? There are many anecdotal reports about its existence “Yeah, I see something different…”; and, we have considerable evidence of differences in individual signs (lexical variaJon). Hairston and Smith (1983) there is “a Black way of signing used by Black deaf people in their own cultural milieu among families and friends, in social gatherings, and in deaf clubs” (55). Basic QuesJons for the Project (2) There also exists a 50year tradiJon of research on African American English (AAE), with unique features idenJfied at all levels of the language – phonology, morphology, syntax, lexicon – showing that AAE is a disJnct variety of English (see Mufwene et al. 1998 and Green 2004 for reviews). Can the same kind of unique features that have been idenJfied for AAE be idenJfied for Black ASL, to show that it is a disJnct variety of ASL?

Upload: others

Post on 18-Oct-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

1  

Black  ASL  as  a  Separate  Variety  

Joseph  Hill,  Carolyn  McCaskill,  Ceil  Lucas  and  Robert  Bayley  

TISLR  2010  Purdue  University  

Black  ASL  project  at  a  glance  

•  The  objecJves  of  this  four-­‐year  project  are:  –  To  create  a  filmed  corpus  of  conversaJonal  Black  ASL  as  it  is  used  in  the  South.  The  focus  is  on  the  structure  and  history  of  Southern  Black  ASL  because  that  region  is  where  the  most  radical  segregaJon  occurred  in  the  educaJon  of  Black  and  White  deaf  children,  creaJng  the  condiJons  for  the  development  of  a  separate  language  variety.    

–  To  provide  a  descripJon  of  the  linguisJc  features  that  make  Black  ASL  recognizable  as  a  disJnct  variety  of  ASL  and  of  the  history  of  the  educaJon  of  Black  Deaf  children.  

–  To  disseminate  the  project  findings  in  the  form  of  teaching  materials  and  instrucJonal  resources.    

Basic  QuesJons  for  the  Project  (1)  

•  What  are  the  features  of  the  variety  of  ASL  that  people  call  “Black  ASL”?  

•  There  are  many  anecdotal  reports  about  its  existence    –  “Yeah,  I  see  something  different…”;  and,  we  have  considerable  evidence  of  differences  in  individual  signs  (lexical  variaJon).  

•  Hairston  and  Smith  (1983)    –  there  is  “a  Black  way  of  signing  used  by  Black  deaf  people  in  their  own  cultural  milieu-­‐  among  families  and  friends,  in  social  gatherings,  and  in  deaf  clubs”  (55).  

Basic  QuesJons  for  the  Project  (2)  

•  There  also  exists  a  50-­‐year  tradiJon  of  research  on  African  American  English  (AAE),  with  unique  features  idenJfied  at  all  levels  of  the  language  –  phonology,  morphology,  syntax,  lexicon  –  showing  that  AAE  is  a  disJnct  variety  of  English  (see  Mufwene  et  al.  1998  and  Green  2004  for  reviews).    

•  Can  the  same  kind  of  unique  features  that  have  been  idenJfied  for  AAE  be  idenJfied  for  Black  ASL,  to  show  that  it  is  a  disJnct  variety  of  ASL?  

Page 2: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

2  

FormaJon  of  Language  VarieJes  

•  How  do  varieJes  of  language  (dialects)  come  about  ?    –  J.  Rickford  (1999)  explains  that  “all  languages,  if  they  have  enough  speakers,  have  dialects-­‐  regional  or  social  varieJes  that  develop  when  people  are  separated  by  geographic  or  social  barriers.”  (African  American  Vernacular  English.  Blackwell.    p.  320)    

Geographic  factors  

•  IsolaJon  of  one  community  from  another;  geographic  and  poliJcal  boundaries  – e.g.,  rivers,  mountains,  swamps,  borders;    

•  Pajerns  of  sejlement,  where  people  live  (or  are  allowed  to  live).    – “…  being  isolated  from  other  speakers  tends  to  allow  a  dialect  to  develop  in  its  own  way,  through  its  own  innovaJons  that  are  different  from  those  of  other  dialects”  (Language  files,  10th  ed.,  p.  419).  

Social  factors  

•  Language  varieJes  can  be  defined  by  factors  –  Socioeconomic  status  –  Age  –  Gender  –  Race  –  Ethnicity        

•  Examples  of  social  factors  –  Differences  in  working-­‐class  and  middle-­‐class  language  –  Caste-­‐defined  differences  in  India  –  AAE    –  Southwest  Spanish    

8

Founding  and  DesegregaJon  of  Schools  for  the  Deaf  (1)  

State 1.White school

2. Black sch./ dept.

3. Desegregati

on

Years bet. 1 & 2

Years bet. 2 & 3

DC, KDES 1857 1857, dept. 1958 0 101

N. Carolina 1845 1868 1967 23 99

Maryland 1868 1872 1956 4 84

Tennessee 1845 1881, dept. 1965 36 84

Georgia 1846 1882 1965 36 83

Mississippi 1854 1882, dept. 1965 28 83

S. Carolina 1849 1883, dept. 1966 34 83

Kentucky 1823 1884, dept. 1954-60 61 70

Florida 1885 1885 1965 0 80

Page 3: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

3  

9

Founding  and  DesegregaJon  of  Schools  for  the  Deaf  (2)  

State 1.White school

2. Black sch./ dept

3. Desegregation

Years bet. 1 & 2

Years bet. 2 & 3

Texas 1857 1887 1965 30 78

Arkansas 1850 1868 1967 23 78

Alabama 1858 1892 1968 34 76

Missouri 1861 1888, dept 1954 37 66

Kansas 1861 1888, dept 1954 27 66

Virginia 1839 1909 1965 (2 schs) 70 56

Oklahoma 1898 1909, dept 1962 11 53

Louisiana 1852 1938 1978 86 40

W.Virginia 1870 1926 1956 56 30

Timing  of  Establishment  and  IntegraJon  

•  The  average  number  of  years  between  the  establishment  of  the  White  school  and  the  establishment  of  the  Black  school  (or  department  )  is  33  –  Striking  excepJons:  Kentucky  (61),  West  Virginia  (56),  Virginia  

(70)  and  Louisiana  (86).  •  The  average  number  of  years  between  the  establishment  of  

the  Black  school  (or  department)  and  desegregaJon  is  72.8  –  The  striking  excepJons  are  Washington,  DC  (101)  and  North  

Carolina  (99)  –  Note  that  Louisiana  is  only  40  because  there  was  no  school  for  

Black  children  unJl  1938  and  desegregaJon  did  not  occur  unJl  24  years  aner  Brown  v.  Board  of  EducaJon  

CondiJons  favoring  the  formaJon  of  Black  ASL  

•  Factors  idenJfied  – Geographic  separaJon  and  isolaJon  of  school  locaJons  

– Social  division  based  on  race  •  ComplicaJng  factors  

– Who  the  teachers  were  – The  general  context  of  oralism  – What  kind  of  language  the  children  were  bringing  to  school  

Current  study  •  Sites  

–  North  Carolina  (1869);  Texas  (1887);  Arkansas  (1887);  Alabama  (1892);  Virginia  (1909);  Louisiana  (1938)  

–  NaJonal  Black  Deaf  Advocate  (NBDA)  conference  in  St.  Louis,  Missouri  in  2007  

•  ParJcipants  –  Signers  over  55  who  ajended  segregated  schools  –  Signers  under  35  who  ajended  desegregated  schools  

•  Data  collecJon    –  Filming  free  conversaJon,  interviews,  and  word  elicitaJon  –  Interviews  that  focus  on  language  use  and  school  history  

•  Analysis  –  Analyzing  the  tapes  for  specific  linguisJc  features  and  for  accounts  of  

language  use  and  school  history              

Page 4: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

4  

Black  ASL  Mosaic    

13

Amount of mouthing

Location: Forehead location vs lowered

Use of role shifting

Size of signing space

Handedness: 2-handed vs. 1 handed signs

Vocabulary differences

Incorporation of AAE into

signing

Use of repetition

Handedness:  previous  studies  

•  In  Woodward  and  DeSanJs  (1977),  Frishberg  (1975),  and  Lucas  et  al.  (2007),  factors  have  been  idenJfied  as  significant:  –  Contact  with  face  and  body  –  Preceding  and  following  signs  – GrammaJcal  category  –  Parts  of  the  target  signs  

•  Outward  movement,  high/low  locaJon,  complex  movement  

–  Social  factors  •  Race,  age,  and  region  

Handedness:  our  study  

•  Signs  that  can  be  produced  with  2  hands  or  1  hand  unremarkably  – Sign  examples:  REMEMBER,  DON’T-­‐KNOW  

•  Data  Analyzed  – 818  tokens  from  free  conversaJons,  interviews,  and  NBDA  conversaJons  

– MulJvariate  analysis  with  Varbrul  

Results  (applicaJon  value  =  1  handed)  

Factor Group Factor N % Weight

Contact Contact 286 44.8 .603

No contact 532 29.3 .444

Preceding sign Pause or 1 handed 565 39.6 .554

2 handed 253 23.7 .381

Following sign 1 handed 256 45.3 .598

Pause 288 33.0 .492

2 handed 274 26.6 .416

Age Young (35–) 349 39.5 .552

Old (55+) 469 31.1 .461

Total Input 818 34.7 .336

Log likelihood = –497.783, chi-square/cell = 0.9063.

Page 5: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

5  

Handedness:  results  

•  Black  signers  use  more  two-­‐handed  variants  than  White  signers  

•  Older  Black  signers  use  more  two  variants  than  younger  Black  signers  

•  Younger  signers  tend  to  use  1-­‐handed  signs.    •  When  compared  with  White  signers  in  an  earlier  study  (Lucas  et  al.  2001),  Black  signers  always  prefer  2-­‐handed  signs  over  1-­‐handed.    

LocaJon:  previous  studies  

•  Frishberg  (1975)  –  There  is  an  historical  tendency  for  signs  to  “move  down”  to  the  central  signing  space.  

•  Liddell  &  Johnson  (1989)  –  “many  signs  which  are  produced  with  contact  at  the  SFH  [side  of  forehead]  locaJon  in  formal  signing  may  be  produced  in  casual  signing  at  the  CK  [cheek]  locaJon.  Similarly,  signs  produced  at  the  CK  locaJon  (including  those  moved  from  the  SFH  locaJon)  may  be  produced  at  the  JW  [jaw]  locaJon.  These  same  signs  also  appear  at  Jmes  without  contact  in  the  area  immediately  in  front  of  the  iNK  [ipsilateral  neck]  locaJon  (253).  

LocaJon:  previous  studies  

•  Lucas  et  al.  (2001)  –  GrammaJcal  funcJon  is  strongest  constraint  

•  Noun,  verb,  adjecJve  disfavor  lowered  forms,  preposiJon  and  interrogaJve  favor  lowered  forms.  

–  Preceding  locaJon  •  Body  favors  lowered  forms,  head  disfavors.  

–  Following  contact    •  No  contact  favors  lowered  forms,  contact  disfavors.  

–  Social  factors  •  White  signers,  both  working  class  and  middle  class,  slightly  favor  lowered  forms  (Varbrul  weight  .555).  

•  Middle  class  Black  signers  disfavor  lowered  forms    (.445).  •  Working  class  Black  signers  disfavor  lowered  forms  (.314).    

LocaJon:  our  study  

•  Our  focus  here  is  on  signs  such  as  KNOW,  DON’T-­‐KNOW,  WHY,  FOR,  TEACH,  IMAGINE,  EDUCATION,  SUSPECT,  REMEMBER,  SEARCH.  

•  In  citaJon  (i.e.  dicJonary)  form,  such  signs  are  signed  at  the  level  of  the  forehead  but  in  actual  use  they  can  also  be  signed  at  lower  levels  ranging  from  the  face  to  the  space  in  front  of  the  signer.    

•  Data  Analyzed  –  877  tokens  from  free  conversaJons,  interviews,  and  NBDA  conversaJons  

Page 6: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

6  

LocaJon:  Results,  LinguisJc  Constraints  (App.  Value=  lowered  variant)  

Factor Group Factor N % Weight Grammatical Compound 47 48.9 .716

category Noun 111 39.6 .602

Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582

Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464

Verb 552 25.0 .448

Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562

Body, no contact 419 27.7 .505

Head, contact 106 26.4 .492

Head, no contact 54 22.2 .379

Total Input 877 29.2 .260

LocaJon:  results  

•  Black  signers  use  more  non-­‐lowered  variants  than  White  signers  

•  Older  Black  signers  use  non-­‐lowered  variants  than  younger  Black  signers  

•  Regional  differences  – Texas,  Alabama,  Virginia,  and  North  Carolina  signers  use  more  lowered  variants  than  Arkansas  and  Louisiana  signers  

Signing  space:  previous  claims  

•  Lewis  et  al.  (1995:  24)  –  MenJon  the  use  of  space  in  descripJon  of  a  person’s  answer  to  

a  quesJon  as  to  why  he  is  wearing  African  American  clothing  •  Aramburo  (1989:  115)  

–  Reports  that  when  Black  Deaf  Signers  converse  with  Black  Deaf  Signers,  they  use  larger  signing  space  than  they  do  with  Black  hearing,  White  Deaf,  and  White  hearing  signers  

•  Tabak  (2006)  –  Discusses  black  children’s  signing  at  the  BDO  (Blind,  Deaf,  

Orphan)  school  in  AusJn,  TX  –  Reports  that  the  signs  produced  by  BDO  students  were  larger  

than  those  of  their  white  peers  –  Describes  no  data  as  the  basis  for  this  observaJon  

Signing  space:  our  study  

•  Videos  of  Black  and  White  signers  were  randomly  selected  and  50  signs/signer  were  analyzed  as  follows.  

•  GrammaJcal  category  of  signs  were  coded  –  Noun,  adjecJve/adverb,  plain  verb,  depicJon/locaJve  verb,  indicaJng  verb,  funcJon  signs  

•  Intensity  of  signs  –  Tensions  in  the  arms,  torso  movement,  eye  gaze  at  the  co-­‐interlocutor  

•  Genre  –  Elicited  narraJve:  retelling  of  wordless  cartoons  with  a  lot  of  acJon  

–  Free  narraJve:  stories  spontaneously  told  by  signers  •  ImposiJon  of  a  transparent  grid  on  all  videos  

Page 7: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

7  

Signing  space:  elicited  narraJve  example   Signing  space:  locaJon  code  

V:  Above  the  top  of  the  head  

L:  Below  the  waist  

h:  A  foot  away  from  the  side  of  the  body  

H:  More  than  a  foot  away  from  the  body  

U:  Unmarked  space  within  the  boundary  

V

L

h h H H U

Signing  space:  results  

•  LinguisJc  factors  – GrammaJcal  category  is  significant  for  both  Black  and  White  signers  

•  DepicJng  and  locaJve  verbs  and  indicaJng  verbs  were  most  likely  to  extend  beyond  the  unmarked  space.  

•  AdjecJves  and  adverbs  slightly  favored  the  use  of  a  marked  variant  by  Black  signers  but  slightly  disfavored  by  White  signers.  

•  Plain  verbs,  nouns,  pronouns,  and  funcJon  signs  were  most  likely  stayed  within  the  boundary  of  the  unmarked  space.  

–  Lijle  difference  in  the  results  of  intensity  of  signs  between  Black  and  White  signers  

Signing  space:  Race  by  age    

Percentage of signs beyond the usual signing space

Page 8: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

8  

Role-­‐shining  •  Previous  research  

–  Metzger  and  Mather  (2004)  •  The  use  of  constructed  dialogue  and  constructed  acJon  of  Black  and  White  

signers  •  IndicaJon  that  Black  signers  use  them  more  onen  then  White  signers  

•  Our  study  –  24  elicited  narraJves  and  21  free  narraJves  –  7  different  events  

•  Constructed  acJon  (CA);  constructed  dialogue  (CD);  constructed  acJon/body  parJJoning  (CABP);  constructed  dialogue/body  parJJoning  (CDBP);  constructed  acJon/constructed  dialogue  (CACD);  constructed  acJon/narraJve  (CANA);  constructed  dialogue/narraJve  (CDNA)  

•  Result  –  Some  indicaJon  that  Black  signers  use  more  constructed  dialogue,  

however,  the  data  show  a  great  deal  of  individual  variaJon.  Further  research  is  needed.    

RepeJJon:  results      

•  Marked  difference  between  Black  signers  and  White  signers    

•  Marked  difference  between  older  and  younger  Black  signers,  with  more  repeJJons  among  the  older  signers  –  this  seems  to  be  a  feature  of  Black  ASL  that  is  changing.  

•  RepeJJon  serves  a  pragmaJc  funcJon  is  supported  by  the  fact  repeJJons  in  all  grammaJcal  categories  are  predominantly  declaraJve  statements  rather  than  quesJons.    

Mouthing  •  Tabak  (2006)  

–  Graduates  of  BDO  (the  Texan  insJtuJon  for  Black  deaf  children  along  with  Black  blind  and  orphan  children)  mouth  less  than  graduates  of  TSD  (Texas  School  for  the  Deaf,  historically  white)  

–  Seems  to  be  a  personal  observaJon  •  Our  study  

–  Instances  of  voiceless  mouthing  of  English  •  Full  mouthing  •  Light  mouthing    

–  iniJal  consonant  or  a  consonant  and  vowel  combinaJon    »  Examples:  ‘pay  ajenJon’  mouth  just  the  iniJal  ‘p’  and  the  iniJal  ‘a’  in  the  phrase;  

the  iniJal  consonant  and  vowel  in  ‘wife’,  “wi”  

•  No  mouthing  –  26  ten  minute  conversaJons  from  a  set  of  95  clips  

•  7  for  older  Black  signers,  6  for  younger  Black  signers,  7  for  older  White  signers,  and  6  for  younger  White  signers.  

–  CategorizaJon  of  tokens  •  Noun,  adjecJve/adverbs,  verbs/predicate,  WH  words,  funcJon  words,  phrases,  

reacJve  tokens  

Mouthing:  results  

•  Nouns  were  the  most  frequently  mouthed,  followed  by  plain  verbs  and  predicates,  phrases,  and  finally  adjecJve  and  adverbs.  

•  Some  funcJon  words  were  mouthed,  as  well  as  a  small  number  of  WH  words.    

•  Some  evidence  that  older  Black  signers  mouth  less  than  other  signers,  however,  further  research  is  necessary  with  a  larger  sample  size.    

Page 9: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

9  

Contact  with  AAE  

•  Borrowing  of  expressions  from  AAE,  e.g.  “Girl”,  “My  bad”  •  Examples  spontaneously  produced  in  interviews  and  free  

conversaJons  in  our  videotaped  •  Black  signers  incorporate  AAE  lexical  items  into  their  

signing.    –  STOP  TRIPPING  –  STUPID  #FOOL  –  WHASSUP  –  GIRL,  PLEASE  –  #DANG  

•  Younger  Black  signers  incorporate  more  AAE  lexical  items  than  their  elders,  possibly  due  to  media  exposure.  

Lexicon:  signs  for  common  items  

•  Previous  study:  Woodward  et  al.  (1976)    –  examined  a  subset  of  signs  including  MOVIE,  RABBIT,  LEMON,  

COLOR,  SILLY,  PEACH,  and  PEANUT  that  can  all  be  produced  either  on  the  face  or  on  the  hands  

–  Found  that  White  signers  produced  more  of  the  face  variants  than  did  Black  signers.    

•  Our  study  –  Differences  in  Black  and  White  signs  for  common  items  and  

concepts,  e.g.  MOVIE,  COLOR,  RABBIT,  AFRICA  [added  by  us]  –  ElicitaJon  method  

•  Pictures  of  the  items  –  Results  

•  Lexical  variaJon  persists,  but  younger  Black  signers  use  fewer  “Black  variants  than  their  elders”  

Lexicon:  older  signs  

•  Lexical  items  varied  by  regions  •  Collected  from  free  conversaJons  and  interviews  

•  SelecJve  lexical  variaJon  examples  – Noun:  SODA,  SHOES,  SCHOOL,  CORNFLAKE,  BATHROOM,  MOVIE,  TOWEL,  BIRTHDAY  

– Verb/Predictate:  FLIRT,  PEE,  SHARP  (as  in  ‘sharp  dresser’)    

Conclusion  

•  Back  to  the  quesJon:    –  Can  the  same  kind  of  unique  features  that  have  been  idenJfied  for  AAE  be  idenJfied  for  Black  ASL,  to  show  that  it  is  a  disJnct  variety  of  ASL?  

•  The  answer  is  yes  and  no.  •  In  Black  ASL,  we  have  not  idenJfied  unique  other  than  the  incorporaJon  of  AAE  and  lexical  items.    

•  Thus  far,  the  differences  between  Black  ASL  and  White  ASL  are  quanJtaJve  rather  than  qualitaJve.    

Page 10: Black&ASL&as&aSeparate&Variety& · Preposition/interrog. 107 35.5 .582 Adjective/adverb 60 21.7 .464 Verb 552 25.0 .448 Preceding contact Body, contact 150 29.3 .562 Body, no contact

10/26/10  

10  

The  History  and  Structure  of  Black  ASL:  Research  Team  

•  Project  Co-­‐Directors  –  Ceil  Lucas,  LinguisJcs,  Gallaudet  University  –  Carolyn  McCaskill,  ASL  &  Deaf  Studies,  Gallaudet  Univ.  –  Robert  Bayley,  LinguisJcs,  UC  Davis  

•  Graduate  Research  Assistants  –  Joseph  Hill,  Roxanne  King,  and  Anika  Stephen,  Gallaudet  

University  •  Undergraduate  Research  Assistants  

–  Stephanie  Johnson  and  TaWanda  Barkley,  Gallaudet  University  •  Technical  Consultant  

–  Randall  Hogue,  Gallaudet  University  •  Community  RepresentaJve  and  Archivist  

–  Pam  Baldwin,  Washington,  DC  

Acknowledgments  

•  The  research  reported  here  was  funded  by  the  Spencer  FoundaJon  and  the  NaJonal  Science  FoundaJon,  whose  support  is  gratefully  acknowledged.    

•  Special  thanks  to  the  members  of  the  African  American  Deaf  community  who  generously  shared  with  us  the  richness  of  their  experience  and  language.  

Black  ASL  Project  Website  •  hjp://blackaslproject.gallaudet.edu/  

References  •  Aramburo,  A.  1989.  SociolinguisJc  aspects  of  the  Black  Deaf  community.  In  C.  Lucas  (ed.),  The  

SociolinguisJcs  of  the  Deaf  Community.  New  York:  Academic  Press.  103-­‐122.  •  Bayley,  R.,  C.  Lucas,  and  M.  Rose.  2000.  VariaJon  in  American  Sign  Language:  The  case  of  DEAF.  

Journal  of  Sociolinguis/cs  4:  81-­‐107.  •  Hairston,  E.,  and  L.  Smith.  1983.  Black  and  Deaf  in  America:  Are  We  that  Different?  Silver  Spring,  

MD:  TJ  Publishers.  •  Lewis,  J.,  C.  Palmer  and  L.  Williams.  1995.  Existence  of  and  axtudes  toward  Black  variaJons  of  sign  

language.  In  L.  Byers,  J.  Chaiken,  and  M.  Mueller  (eds.),  CommunicaJon  Forum  1995.  Washington,  DC:  Department  of  LinguisJcs,  Gallaudet  University.  17-­‐48.  

•  Lucas,  C.,  A.  Goeke,  R.  Briesacher,  and  R.  Bayley.  2007.  Phonological  VariaJon:  2  Hands  or  1?  Paper  presented  at  NWAV  36,  University  of  Pennsylvania,  Philadelphia,  October.    

•  Lucas,  C.,  R.  Bayley,  and  C.  Valli.  2001.  Sociolinguis/c  Varia/on  in  American  Sign  Language.  Washington,  D.C.:  Gallaudet  University  Press.  

•  Metzger,  M.,  and  S.  Mather.  2004.  Constructed  Dialogue  and  Constructed  AcJon  in  ConversaJonal  NarraJves  in  ASL.  Poster  presented  at  the  Conference  on  TheoreJcal  Issues  in  Sign  Language  Research  (TISLR  8),  Barcelona,  September.    

•  Rickford,  J.  R.  1999.  African  American  Vernacular  English:  Features,  Evolu/on,  Educa/onal  Implica/ons.  Oxford:  Blackwell.  

•  Tabak,  J.  2006.  Significant  Gestures-­‐  A  History  of  American  Sign  Language.  Westport,  CT:  Praeger