brain correlates of aesthetic expertise_a parametric fmri study (2009)

Upload: re985

Post on 03-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    1/10

    Brain correlates of aesthetic expertise: A parametric fMRI study

    Ulrich Kirk a,b,*, Martin Skov a, Mark Schram Christensen a,c, Niels Nygaard d

    a Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance, Copenhagen University Hospital, Hvidovre, Kettegaard All 30, DK-2650 Hvidovre, Denmarkb Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, Anatomy Department, University College London, Darwin Building, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UKc Department of Exercise and Sport Sciences, University of Copenhagen, The Panum Institute, Blegdamsvej 3, DK-2200 Copenhagen N, Denmarkd Institute for Architecture and Aesthetics, Aarhus School of Architecture, Norreport 20, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:Accepted 1 August 2008

    Available online 9 September 2008

    Keywords:

    Neuroaesthetics

    Expertise, orbitofrontal cortex

    Subcallosal cingulate gyrus

    Architecture

    Faces

    a b s t r a c t

    Several studies have demonstrated that acquired expertise influences aesthetic judgments. In this para-digm we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study aesthetic judgments of visually

    presented architectural stimuli and control-stimuli (faces) for a group of architects and a group of

    non-architects. This design allowed us to test whether level of expertise modulates neural activity in

    brain areas associated with either perceptual processing, memory, or reward processing. We show that

    experts and non-experts recruit bilateral medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and subcallosal cingulate

    gyrus differentially during aesthetic judgment, even in the absence of behavioural aesthetic rating differ-

    ences between experts and non-experts. By contrast, activity in nucleus accumbens (NAcc) exhibits a dif-

    ferential response profile compared to OFC and subcallosal cingulate gyrus, suggesting a dissociable role

    between these regions in the reward processing of expertise. Finally, categorical responses (irrespective

    of aesthetic ratings) resulted in expertise effects in memory-related areas such as hippocampus and pre-

    cuneus. These results highlight the fact that expertise not only modulates cognitive processing, but also

    modulates the response in reward related brain areas.

    2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    In psychological models of aesthetic experience it is generally

    assumed that art-related expertise influences subjects preference

    for works of art (Leder, Belke, Oeberst, & Augustin, 2004). Indeed,

    a substantial number of behavioural studies have confirmed that

    level of expertise modulates the aesthetic evaluation of art objects

    (Eysenck& Castle, 1970; Gordon, 1951/1952, 1956; Hekkert, Peper,

    & van Wieringen, 1994, Hekkert & van Wieringen, 1996a, 1996b;

    OHare, 1976; Schmidt, McLaughlin, & Leighten, 1989). It is there-

    fore likely that art experts use different neural processes for deter-

    mining aesthetic evaluation than non-experts. The question we

    wish to raise here is whether this putative difference in aesthetic

    evaluation can be detected as a difference in neural activity

    through the use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

    It has been shown by imaging experiments that acquired exper-

    tise is associated with changes in brain structures underlying per-

    ceptual and memory processes, even on a macro-anatomical scale.

    For example, in a study using voxel-based morphometry analysis,

    Maguire and colleagues (2000) found that grey matter volume in

    the posterior hippocampus of London taxi drivers is greater than

    in age-matched controls, and that the size of this increase corre-

    lates positively with time spent taxi driving. Furthermore, several

    experiments have demonstrated that musicians, after years of

    playing, respond differently to musical inputs as compared to

    non-musicians (for a review, see Schlaug, 2003). For example, in

    a recent fMRI study, Bangert and colleagues (2006) compared brain

    activity in groups of musicians and non-musicians as they pas-

    sively listened to a piano sequence and found elevated activity in

    the musicians in regions of the temporal lobe associated with audi-

    tory processing, and in frontal regions associated with motor

    control.

    Several neuroimaging studies have investigated cortical areas

    that are recruited when subjects make aesthetic evaluations from

    a variety of stimulus modalities such as paintings (Cela-Conde

    et al., 2004; Kawabata & Zeki, 2004; Vartanian& Goel, 2004), music

    (Blood & Zatorre 2001; Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez, & Evans, 1999;

    Koelsch, Fritz, von Cramon, Mller, & Friederici, 2006; Brown, Mar-

    tinez, & Parsons, 2004; Menon& Levitin, 2005), faces (Aharon et al.,

    2001; Nakamura et al., 1998; ODoherty et al. 2003; Winston,

    ODoherty, Kilner, Perrett, & Dolan, 2007) and geometrical figures

    (Jacobsen, Schubotz, Hfel, & Cramon, 2006). Taken together, these

    studies suggest that the computation of aesthetic preferences for

    objects predominantly relies on the activity of cortical and subcor-

    tical areas implicated in the processing of reward; especially stria-

    tum, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)

    (for a review, see Skov, in press.) It is therefore important to inves-

    0278-2626/$ - see front matter 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004

    * Corresponding author. Address: Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Reso-

    nance, Copenhagen University Hospital, Hvidovre, Kettegaard All 30, DK-2650

    Hvidovre, Denmark.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (U. Kirk).

    Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Brain and Cognition

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / b & c

    mailto:[email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02782626http://www.elsevier.com/locate/b&chttp://www.elsevier.com/locate/b&chttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02782626mailto:[email protected]
  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    2/10

    tigate whether expertise influences aesthetic evaluation through

    the modulation of neural activity in these areas. Since the medial

    OFC is not only found to correlate with subjective hedonic value

    in most of the studies mentioned above, but have also been dem-

    onstrated to be involved in coding stimulus value of a variety of

    other sensory modalities, including taste (ODoherty et al., 2001;

    Small, Zatorre, Dagher, Evans, & Jones-Gotman, 2001; Small et al.,

    2003), olfactory (Anderson et al., 2003; Gottfried, Deichmann, Win-

    ston, & Dolan, 2002; Rolls, Kringelbach, & de Araujo, 2003), and

    somatosensory (Rolls, ODoherty et al., 2003), we hypothesized that

    this region would reflect a modulation of aesthetic assessment

    according to level of expertise.

    To accomplish this experimental aim, nave subjects (i.e. sub-

    jects professing to have no great interest or expertise in art or

    architecture) and expert subjects (i.e. graduate students in archi-

    tecture and professional architects) were asked to rate the aes-

    thetic value of a series of images containing both buildings and

    faces during an event-related fMRI paradigm (see Fig. 1). We

    hypothesized that the expert-specific conditions (i.e. building

    images) would significantly affect both aesthetic ratings and neural

    activity differentially in the two groups. Since earlier psychometric

    studies have found that people in different cultures, and of both

    sexes, tend to agree as to which faces are attractive (Langlois

    et al., 2000), we predicted the two groups aesthetic ratings and

    neural processing would not differentiate for face images.

    2. Experimental methods

    2.1. Subjects

    A total of 24 healthy volunteers (11 experts/13 non-experts; 6

    female experts/7 female non-experts; experts mean age: 30.8

    years; age range 2642 years; non-experts mean age: 27.2 years;

    age range 2232 years; all subjects were right-handed) were

    scanned. We excluded two subjects (both male non-experts) from

    the analysis for clinical reasons. The experts were recruited from

    architectural offices and schools where they were graduate or

    post-graduate students. Non-experts were all undergraduate or

    graduate students with no formal education in any art-related

    field. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects

    and ethical approval (KF-01-131/03) was obtained before the

    experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision,

    and none had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders.

    2.2. Stimulus set

    Visual achromatic stimuli belonging to two categories, build-

    ings and faces, were used as stimulus material. One hundred and

    sixty-eight building stimuli were selected from various online re-

    sources. The surrounding of the building image was shaded so that

    the building was in focus for each stimulus. This was accomplished

    in Photoshop (version 7.0, Adobe, USA). Any image noticeably dis-

    torted (e.g., proportion and illumination) by this process was ex-

    cluded from the stimulus pool. Building stimuli were presented

    with a resolution of 600 pixels in height and varying width with

    a maximum of 1024 pixels. Prior to scanning the building stimuli

    were exposed to an aesthetic judgment scale in a behavioural pilot

    study by a separate cohort of subjects (7 experts/6 non-experts; 3

    female experts/3 female non-experts; experts mean age 34.3 years;

    age range 2744 years; non-experts mean age 29.2 years; age

    range 2730 years). Level of appeal was measured using an aes-

    thetic rating scale from 1 to 5, where 1 was defined as very unap-

    pealing and 5 as very appealing. The stimuli conformed to a

    balanced distribution in the frequency of each rating bin between

    experts and non-experts. To investigate whether there were differ-

    ences between the two groups for rating-specific stimuli, i.e.

    whether there was an image-wise difference between experts

    and non-experts for buildings, further analyses were applied. The

    building stimuli were selected according to two sub-classes: a for-

    mal/stylistic sub-classification (modernist and non-modernist

    architecture) and a typological sub-classification (private and

    public architecture). This was done in order to further control

    for a potential skewed preference distribution between the groups;

    for instance, experts might all prefer modernist and non-experts

    might all prefer non-modernist buildings. However, this poten-tially confounding effect did not amount to significant differences

    between stimuli sub-classes across groups in subsequent statistical

    analyses (F(7,40) = 1.78; p > .1).

    The face database was provided by the Max-Planck Institute for

    Biological Cybernetics in Tuebingen, Germany. 168 face stimuli

    were selected; half of the stimuli were female faces. Stimuli were

    rated by a separate group of subjects (n = 10/4 females; mean

    age 28.4 years; age range 2630 years) for level of appeal in a

    behavioural pilot study prior to scanning. Level of appeal was rated

    using the same aesthetic rating scale as described above. One hun-

    dred and sixty-eight faces were selected from the high, middle and

    low ends of the appeal ratings in order to obtain a balanced distri-

    bution. The face stimuli were masked in order to remove hair and

    were adjusted to be of equal size and luminance by using Photo-shop (version 7.0, Adobe, USA). The faces were centred in a

    588 600 pixel black background and presented at a screen reso-

    lution of 1024 768 pixels.

    2.3. Experimental paradigm

    The experimental protocol consisted of an event-related design

    in which subjects were scanned while being presented with each of

    the 168 face stimuli and the 168 building stimuli in a pseudoran-

    dom order, making a total of 336 presentations. On each trial, a fix-

    ation cross was presented for 1000 ms on a grey background

    followed by a stimuli presentation for 3000 ms. Subjects were in-

    structed to press one of five buttons on a response key-pad with

    their right hand to indicate their aesthetic judgment (1 = veryunappealing, 5 = very appealing). Randomly interspersed with the

    Fig. 1. Experimental paradigm. A fixation cross was shown for1000 ms followed by

    stimuluspresentation with a duration of 3000 ms in whichsubjectswere instructed

    to indicate the level of aesthetic appeal by means of button-press on a scale from 5

    (highest appeal) to 1 (lowest appeal). Examples of stimuli used in the scanningsessions are displayed.

    U. Kirk et al./ Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315 307

  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    3/10

    stimuli presentations were 56 null event trials (grey screen). Total

    scanning-time per subject was 26 min. in one session. The long

    paradigm lasting 26 min could have potentially given rise to fati-

    gue in the subjects. However, we did not observe any deviations

    in behavioural differences when the first part of the scanning

    was compared with the last part of the scanning. In particular

    the trial to trial judgment variability was assessed as a measure

    of fatigue, under the assumption that fatigued subjects would tend

    to evaluate similarly from trial to trial when they were fatigued.

    Missing trials and the reaction time were also used as indirect

    measures, and these did not change either. Prior to scanning, sub-

    jects were informed that the study was concerned with investigat-

    ing aesthetic judgments, but no reference was made to the

    experimental aims. After the scanning task was complete, subjects

    were presented with the stimuli again, this time outside the scan-

    ner, where they rated each stimulus for familiarity. Familiarity rat-

    ings were entered into the design matrix as regressors of no

    interest. Stimuli were presented and responses collected using E-

    prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). The stimuli were back-

    projected via a LCD projector onto a transparent screen positioned

    over the subjects head and viewed through a tilted mirror fixed to

    the head coil.

    2.4. fMRI data acquisition

    The functional imaging was conducted by using a 3 Tesla scan-

    ner (Siemens, Magnetom Trio, Erlangen, Germany) to acquire gra-

    dient T2* weighted gradient echo (GR) echo planar images (EPI) to

    maximize the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast

    (echo-time, TE = 30 ms; repetition time, TR = 2400 ms; flip angle,

    F A=90). The EPI sequence was optimized in order to reduce signal

    drop-out in OFC (Deichmann, Gottfried, Hutton, & Turner, 2003).

    Each functional image was acquired in an interleaved way, begin-

    ning with 2nd slice (slice No. 2,4,. . .,40, 1,3,. . .,39) when counted

    from the bottom, comprising 40 axial slices each 3.0 mm thick,

    consisting of a 64 64 matrix with an in-plane resolution of

    3 3 mm. This gave near whole-brain coverage, excluding inferiorparts of the cerebellum. Each session consistedof 654 volumes. The

    subjects pulse and respiration were recorded using an MRI-com-

    patible pulse oximeter, and a respiration belt, both sampled at

    50 Hz. After the functional scan, a T1 weighted MPRAGE structural

    sequence was acquired, using a phased array head coil to provide

    high-resolution anatomical detail.

    2.5. fMRI data analysis

    Image pre-processing and data analysis was performed using

    SPM2 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London,

    UK). The EPI images were spatially realigned (Friston et al.,

    1995). This was followed by temporal realignment, which cor-

    rected for slice-time differences using the middle slice as referenceslice. Images were then normalized to the Montreal Neurological

    Institute (MNI) EPI-template provided in SPM2. Finally, a spatial

    filtering was performed by applying a Gaussian smoothing kernel

    of 8 mm FWHM (full width at half-maximum).

    Following pre-processing a general linear model (GLM) was ap-

    plied to the time course data, where each event was modelled with

    a separate single impulse response function time-locked to middle

    stimulus time and then convolved with the canonical haemody-

    namic response function (HRF), including its temporal and disper-

    sion derivatives in order to capture small variations in the onset

    and width of the BOLD responses.

    A parametric regression analysis was used (Buchel, Holmes,

    Rees, & Friston, 1998) that allowed us to model on/off, linear and

    non-linear haemodynamic responses using orthogonalized polyno-mial expansion functions. This was performed for each of the two

    stimulus conditions using subject-specific aesthetic ratings in or-

    der to model a potential parametric modulation of aesthetic rat-

    ings. The on/off or 0th order parametric regression analysis

    allows inferences to be made about variations in the response

    across the two subject groups independent of the aesthetic ratings.

    First-level analysis was performed on each subject to generate a

    single mean parameter corresponding to each term of the polyno-

    mial expansion. In order to correct for the structured noise inducedby respiration and cardiac pulsation we included RETROICOR (RET-

    ROspective Image based CORrection method) nuisance covariates

    in the design matrix (Glover, Li, & Ress, 2000). These regressors

    are a Fourier expansion of the aliased cardiac and respiratory oscil-

    lations. We included six regressors for respiration and ten regres-

    sors for cardiac pulsation. We also included 24 regressors that

    remove residual movement artefacts with spin history effects,

    which have been shown to remain even after image realignment

    (Friston, Williams, Howard, Frackowiak, & Turner, 1996). This set

    of nuisance regressors have also been shown to reduce inter and

    intra subject variation significantly (Lund, Nrgaard, Rostrup,

    Rowe, & Paulson, 2005). Having all four types of nuisance regres-

    sors in the design improves the assumption of independently and

    identically distributed errors (Lund, Madsen, Sidaros, Luo, & Nic-

    hols, 2006). For the analysis we also applied a high pass filter with

    a cut-off frequency at 1/128 Hz. This high pass filter removes any

    temporal drift that oscillates slower than once every 128 s, and it

    will therefore remove slowly varying drift caused by hardware

    instabilities.

    The statistical parametric maps were entered into a second-le-

    vel, random effects analysis (RFX) accounting for the between sub-

    ject variance. Experts and non-experts were treated as separate

    groups in an ANOVA model using the beta-estimates of the two

    groups and the two stimuli conditions for the linear and the qua-

    dratic expansions. Equal variance was not assumed, thus SPM2s

    options for non-sphericity correction was applied (Glaser& Friston,

    2004).

    Using t-contrasts allowed us to test for correlations of the fMRI

    BOLD signal and the parameters of interest performed as on/off,linear and non-linear parametric modulations, respectively. Re-

    ported p-values were set at a threshold of p < .001, uncorrected,

    unless otherwise stated. In order to correct for multiple compari-

    sons in the medial orbitofrontal cortex, a regionin which activation

    was predicted on the basis of our a priori hypothesis, we used

    small volume corrections (SVC) (Worsley et al., 1996) constraining

    our analysis to this region using a sphere with a 10 mm radius. We

    used the coordinates reported in Kawabata and Zeki (2004) for

    medial OFC. Before using SVC, we transformed coordinates given

    by Kawabata and Zeki (2004) from Talairach space to MNI space

    (http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk). The coordinates of all activations

    are reported in MNI space.

    3. Results

    3.1. Behavioural results

    We first inspected the two groups behavioural responses, i.e.

    aesthetic ratings, collected during scanning (see Fig. 2). A two-

    way ANOVA with two factor levels (buildings, faces) and groups

    (experts, non-experts) revealed significant differences between

    stimulus conditions (F(1,10) = 54.42; p < 2 107) (see Fig. 2A),

    but no significant differences between groups (F(1,10) = 1.89;

    p > .1). Furthermore, no significant interactions between stimulus

    conditions and groups was observed (F(1,10) = 1.44; p > .2). The

    same analysis was applied to the reaction-time data (RT) collected

    during scanning. However no significant differences were foundbetween groups (F(1,10) = 0.45;p > .5). Analysing the mean ratings

    308 U. Kirk et al. / Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315

    http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/
  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    4/10

    of the two stimulus classes, these results suggest that experts and

    non-experts did not display significantly different behaviour in

    making an aesthetic judgment of either faces or buildings. We next

    inspected whether experts and non-experts differed in the fre-

    quency of each rating bin (see Fig. 2C and D), as such a difference

    might not be reflected when inspecting the mean aesthetic ratings

    (see Fig. 2A). Moreover, such a potential difference in the distribu-

    tion of ratings could also have consequences for interpreting thefMRI results, since a different distribution in the frequency of rat-

    ing bins between groups could potentially account for differences

    in the linear fits between the two groups. No significant differences

    were found between groups in the frequency of each rating bin for

    face stimuli (see Fig. 2D). For building stimuli (see Fig. 2C) no sig-

    nificant differences between groups was observed for rating bin 4

    and 5 (reflecting high appeal) and bin 1 and 2 (reflecting low ap-

    peal). However, the middle rating bin resulted in a significant dif-

    ference between groups (two sample t= 3.02; df = 10; p < .01).

    Finally, we looked at the variance of RTs between the two groups.

    Although we found no significant differences between the two

    groups in mean ratings and RTs, it was possible that a difference

    between groups would be reflected in greater variance in RTs be-

    tween the groups. However, we found no such difference in vari-ance across groups (F(1,4) = 0.73; p > .4), rating bin (F(1,4) = 0.24;

    p > .9), or stimulus type (F(1,4) = 2.25; p > .1). Likewise, we ob-

    served no significant interactions (group rating, group stimu-

    lus, and group rating stimulus).

    3.2. fMRI results

    3.2.1. Correlation between the BOLD signal and linear aesthetic ratings

    To test whether architectural expertise modulated brain activ-ity associated with making aesthetic judgments a 1st order para-

    metric regression model using the subject-specific behavioural

    responses was applied. We defined the expertise effect as the inter-

    action between the two subject groups and the two stimulus

    conditions [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] [Non-Expert_Build

    Non-Expert_Faces]. This analysis allowed us to focus on voxels

    for which the difference between the responses for the two stimu-

    lus conditions varied across the two subject groups. The interaction

    revealed significant activations in bilateral subcallosal cingulate

    gyrus (4, 30, 2; z= 4.47; p < .05, corrected for multiple compari-

    sons using false discovery rate, FDR; 14, 40, 2; z= 4.32; p < .05,

    FDR) (see Fig. 3). When we performed small volume corrections

    (SVC) we observed significant activity in bilateral medial OFC

    (

    8, 30,

    20; z= 3.83; p < .05, FDR, SVC; 6, 34,

    16; z = 3.40;p < .05, FDR, SVC). These significant interactions were further

    Fig. 2. Behavioural responses collected during scanning. (A) Mean aesthetic ratings for the two stimulus conditions and both subject groups. The mean rating for buildingstimuli for experts was 3.29 (SD = 0.21) and for non-experts 3.28 (SD = 0.32). For face stimuli the mean rating for experts was 2.7 (SD = 0.25) and for non-experts 2.79

    (SD = 0.33).(B) Mean reactiontimes (RT) forstimulus conditions and subject groups. Examination of RTsrevealedthat average RT forbuildingstimulifor experts was 2003 ms

    (SD = 357.4) and for non-experts 2011 ms. (SD = 562.4). The average response time for face stimuli for experts was 1810 ms (SD = 256.6) and for non-experts 1696ms

    (SD = 440.4). Response latencies between groups and stimulus conditions did not differ significantly in a one-way ANOVA (F(3,40) = 1.48; p < .23). (C) Distribution of ratings

    acrossthe fiverating bins forbuildingstimuli, where each ratingbin is bears a scale from 5 (high appeal) to 1 (low appeal) (x-axis) and the response frequency across subjects

    in percent is shown (y-axis). Error bars indicate SD. (D) Distribution of ratings across the five rating bins for face stimuli. Error bars indicate SD.

    U. Kirk et al./ Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315 309

  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    5/10

  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    6/10

    the clusters from the building-specific conjunction, and found that

    left NAcc (10, 8, 4; z= 3.42; p < .05, FDR, SVC) was significantly

    more active in both stimuli conditions in both groups (see Fig. 4).

    The activation in left anterior thalamus did not survive SVC. These re-

    sults suggest that left NAcc plays a role in encoding high and low aes-

    thetic values that is not modulated by expertise or stimulus modality.

    3.2.3. Correlation between the BOLD signal and expertise irrespective

    of aesthetic ratings

    Finally, in order to identify voxels that responded differentially

    in the two groups per sei.e., irrespective of aesthetic ratingan

    interaction analysis using regressors from a zero-order parametric

    regression analysis was conducted. An interaction analysis [Ex-

    pert_BuildExpert_Faces] [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces]

    showed distinct specificity for buildings compared to faces in

    experts relative to non-experts in bilateral hippocampus, left pre-

    cuneus and cerebellum (see Fig. 5 and Table 1).

    In the converse interaction [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Fa-

    ces] [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] we observed significant activa-

    tions in bilateral calcarine gyrus bilateral and fusiform gyrus

    located adjacent to the collateral sulcus and inferior lingual gyrus

    posterior to the parahippocampal gyrus (see Fig. 6 and Table 1).

    We furthermore conducted a conjunction analysis using the

    building-specific main effects for both groups [Expert_BuildEx-

    pert_Faces] and [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces]. We ob-

    served bilateral activation of the parahippocampal place area

    (PPA) [30, 42, 14; 30, 46, 10, FDR] (see Supplementary

    material) that has been found to respond selectively to houses,

    landscapes and other environmental sceneries (Epstein& Kanwish-

    er, 1998).

    4. Discussion

    The present experiment extends other studies of expertise to

    suggest that acquired expertise not only impacts on cognitive

    and perceptual systems (Bangert et al., 2006; Maguire et al.,

    2000), but also modulates the response of brain areas associated

    with the processing of reward. However, the processing of rewardhas been linked to several brain areas, including the ventral teg-

    mental area, ventral striatum, amygdala and OFC (for a review,

    see McClure, York, & Montague, 2004), and our results show that

    only parts of this system are modulated by expertise during aes-

    thetic judgment. In contrast to expertise effects observed in OFC

    and subcallosal cingulate gyrus, we found that activity in left NAcc

    was elevated in both groups and stimuli conditions in response to

    appealing and non-appealing stimuli.

    The response profile of medial OFC in both groups exhibited a

    positive linear correlation with aesthetic ratings. However, when

    compared to each other the increase was significantly higher in

    the experts than in the non-experts. The fact that the medial part

    of OFC shows sensitivity to the magnitude of aesthetic value is in

    accordance with studies on reward processing showing that therelative reward value of stimuli is reflected by the amplitude of

    Fig. 4. The upper panel shows activation in left NAcc using the2nd order non-linear

    term. The lower panel shows the parameter estimates in both groups and for both

    stimulus conditions in left NAcc (10, 8, 4) where the x-axis reflects the

    experimental conditions consisting of both groups and both stimulus conditions,

    and the y-axis shows BOLD signal changes. Activations are overlaid on sagittal,

    coronal and axial sections of the canonical SPM structural image. Activation isdisplayed at p < .008, FDR). Error bars indicate 90% confidence interval.

    Fig. 5. The upper panel display activation of the left hippocampus from theinteraction [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces]

    using the zero-order parametric analysis that display voxels activated irrespective

    of aesthetic rating. The lower panel shows parameter estimates for the left

    hippocampus, where the x-axis reflects the experimental conditions, and the y-axis

    shows BOLD signal changes. Activation is displayed at p < .001, uncorrected. Error

    bars indicate 90% confidence interval.

    Table 1

    Summary of interaction effects for the parametric regression analysis irrespective of

    aesthetic ratings

    Brain region MNI coordinates z Score Number of voxels

    [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces]

    R hippocampus 38, 28,8 3.60 18

    L hippocampus 26,14,14 3.29 17

    34,16,20L precuneus 6, 54, 22 3.61 37

    R cerebellum 16, 62,16 3.57 26

    26, 68,24 3.44 13

    [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces] [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces]

    R fusiform gyrus 32, 54,4 3.35 9

    L fusiform gyrus 30, 56, 0 3.73 42

    R calcarine gyrus 18, 58, 16 3.59 21

    L calcarine gyrus 18,64, 14 3.79 30

    R pons 16, 18, 30 4.41 69

    Activations are shown at (p < .001, uncorrected). L, left hemisphere; R, right

    hemisphere.

    U. Kirk et al. / Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315 311

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    7/10

    neural activity in OFC (Kringelbach, 2005; Tremblay & Schultz,

    1999). For instance, in studies comparing subjects ingesting food

    in states of hunger and satiety a contrast of these two states reveals

    different neural responses in OFC, indicating that OFC neurons

    codes reward aspects of a stimulus rather than sensory aspects

    (Kringelbach, ODoherty, Rolls, & Andrews, 2003). Furthermore,several studies suggest that the medial aspect of the human OFC

    represents the hedonic attributes involved in preference judg-

    ments of various stimulus types (Aharon et al., 2001; Anderson

    et al., 2003; Blood et al., 1999; Gottfried et al., 2002; Kawabata

    et al., 2004; ODoherty et al., 2001; ODoherty et al., 2003; Rolls,

    Kringelbach, ODoherty, Rolls, & Andrews, 2003; Rolls, ODoherty,

    et al., 2003; Small et al., 2001, 2003). The implication of these pre-

    vious findings for the present results is that medial OFC may be en-

    gaged under conditions where behavioural decision making based

    on stimulus reward value is required (Bechara, Damasio, & Dama-

    sio, 2000; Wallis, 2007). Recently, several studies have demon-

    strated that medial OFC responses can be modulated by

    top-down information such as knowledge of the price of a wine

    (Plassmann et al., 2008), brand information (McClure, York et al.,2004), and visual word descriptors influence preference for odours

    (de Araujo, Rolls, Velazco, Margot, & Cayeux, 2005). The novelty of

    our results is that the representation of stimulus value, or possibly

    intrinsic motivation, in medial OFC varies with expertise level to

    such an extent that the experts displayed higher activation to the

    building stimuli than the non-experts, but not to the control-stim-

    uli, i.e. faces.

    In contrast to OFC, voxels in the subcallosal part of the anterior

    cingulate gyrus responded inversely to high and low ratings in the

    experts compared to non-experts. This result supports the growing

    recognition that anterior cingulate gyrus and OFC contribute dis-

    tinct component processes to decision making (Rushworth, Beh-

    rens, Rudebeck, & Walton, 2007). The anterior aspect of the

    cingulate gyrus forms an anatomical interface between the OFCand premotor cortex. Since it also receives afferents from subcorti-

    cal dopaminergic neurons and inputs from dorsolateral prefrontal

    cortex, it has been suggested that the anterior cingulate integrates

    the affective drive and action strategies for the purpose of selecting

    appropriate motor responses, i.e. making decisions how to act

    (Paus, 2001). Another possibility might be that subcallosal cingu-

    late gyrus activity reflects the subjects monitoring of their own

    emotional state (Ochsner et al., 2004), whereby appealing buildings

    are more arousing to the experts than to the non-experts. Indeed,the subcallosal aspect of the cingulate gyrus has previously been

    implicated in such forms of emotional processing, including the re-

    call of happy autobiographical memories (Lane, Reinman, Ahern,

    Schwartz, & Davidson, 1997) and attending to emotionally stimu-

    lating words (Maddock, Garrett, & Buonocore, 2003). Interestingly,

    decreasing musical dissonance, associated with an elevated experi-

    ence of pleasure (Blood et al., 1999) and passive listening to unfa-

    miliar, pleasant musical compared to a rest condition (Brown et al.,

    2004) has also been shown to produce enhanced activity in subcal-

    losal cingulate gyrus. Finally, as pupillometry data was not re-

    corded in the present study we are unable to assess whether or

    not such effects would have detected differences between the

    two groups. Indeed there is evidence for involvement of the subcal-

    losal cingulate in generating and monitoring autonomic interocep-

    tive states (Critchley, 2004).

    It is notable that while activity in OFC and the subcallosal cin-

    gulate gyrus were sensitive to the level of expertise, the behav-

    ioural responses did not parallel this difference in neural

    activation between the two groups. As a number of previous

    behavioural studies have found an effect of expertise on aesthetic

    evaluation to be robust, our result was somewhat surprising

    (Eysenck & Castle, 1970; Gordon, 1951/1952; Gordon, 1956;

    OHare, 1976; Schmidt et al., 1989). One possible reason for this

    discrepancy betweenour study and earlier ones couldbe the meth-

    od of comparison used in our study. It is conceivable that compar-

    ing means of rating is not sufficiently sensitive to detect

    differences in experts and non-experts ratings. Experts and non-

    experts are known to respond differentially to dimensions of stim-

    ulus qualities such as craftsmanship and quality (Hekkert & vanWieringen, 1996b), or chromatic versus achromatic versions of

    paintings (Hekkert & van Wieringen, 1996a). It is conceivable that

    our set of buildings stimuli lack perceptual properties that system-

    atically influence the differential aesthetic assessment of experts

    and non-experts. On the other hand, the fact that we did not ob-

    serve a behavioural difference in the two groups responses to

    the building stimuli strengthens the neural result. If both a differ-

    ence in behaviour and neural activity between the two groups had

    been observed, the difference in neural activity might have been

    confounded by the differences in behavioural responses. In the

    present situation where the two groups differ in neural activity

    but not in behaviour we can be sure that what we observe is a true

    group difference.

    Our finding of a positive bivalent response in the ventral stria-tum, specifically the left NAcc, in both subject groups and to both

    stimuli types replicates and extends previous findings that NAcc

    and OFC play different functional roles in reward processing (Knut-

    son, Fong, Adams, Varner, & Hommer, 2001; ODoherty et al., 2002;

    ODoherty et al., 2003; Tremblay& Schultz, 1999; Watanabe, 1999).

    Whereas the OFC is thought to process reward outcomes, the NAcc

    is generally believed to subserve the prediction of reward and to

    compute the variance between reward expectation and the actual

    reward (for reviews, see Knutson & Cooper, 2005; Montague, Hy-

    man, & Cohen, 2004). Although earlier results have found non-lin-

    ear response profiles in the NAcc as discussed below, to our

    knowledge there has been no previous descriptions of NAcc activ-

    ity with negative aesthetic ratings. Electrophysiological recordings

    in animals have demonstrated that NAcc neurons increase theirresponse to positive reward prediction errors (situations that are

    Fig. 6. The figure displays the interaction [Non-Expert_BuildNon-Expert_Fa-

    ces]

    [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] from the zero-order parametric analysis thatshows brain areas activated, irrespective of the actual aesthetic ratings. The upper

    panel displays a bilateral activation of the fusiform gyrus on slices where z-

    coordinates for the three slices are ascending from 6, 4 and 2, respectively.

    Evident on the slices are the collateral sulcus just lateral to the activation in the

    fusiform gyrus. The lower panel displays the corresponding parameter estimates in

    the right fusiform gyrus. The x-axis reflects the experimental conditions. The y-axis

    shows BOLD signal changes. Activations are displayed atp < .001, uncorrected. Error

    bars indicate 90% confidence interval.

    312 U. Kirk et al. / Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315

  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    8/10

    better than expected) and decrease their response to negative pre-

    diction errors (Apicella, Ljungberg, Scarnati, & Schultz, 1991;

    Schultz, Apicella, Ljungberg, Romo, & Scarnati, 1993). Neuroimag-

    ing work has subsequently replicated these findings (e.g., ODoher-

    ty et al., 2006; Seymour, Daw, Dayan, Singer, & Dolan, 2007; Spicer

    et al., 2007). However, recently another hypothesis has been put

    forth, suggesting that activations of the ventral striatum are not

    sensitive to errors in prediction, but rather encode salience dimen-

    sions of the stimulus. This idea heralds from imaging studies where

    the ventral striatum has been shown to correlate with prediction

    errors regardless of valence (Jensen et al., 2007; Zink, Martin-Skur-

    ski, Chappelow, & Berns, 2004). A recent fMRI study by Cooper and

    Knutson (2008), though, suggests that the ventral striatum may

    compute the interaction of valence and salience, depending upon

    the context wherein motivational behaviour takes place. Directly

    comparing degrees of valence and salience, Cooper and Knutson

    found that both the valence and the salience of anticipated incen-

    tives correlated with NAcc activation. More specifically, in this

    study when outcomes were uncertain and salience high, NAcc acti-

    vation increased for anticipated loss and gain, whereas NAcc acti-

    vation increased for anticipated gain and decreased for

    anticipated loss when outcomes were certain and salience low. In

    our study the experimental set-up might be seen as similar to the

    first situation (uncertain outcome and high salience). However, we

    neither manipulated the salience of the pictures nor the relation

    between anticipation and reward, so this remains conjecture. Since

    it is possible that the subjects covertly anticipated the reward out-

    come of the upcoming stimuli based on the recently transpired

    judgment act, we cannot rule out the possibility that the NAcc acti-

    vation reflects prediction error signalling.

    The zero-order parametric regression analysis identified brain

    regions showing a typological response to the two stimulus con-

    ditions irrespective of aesthetic ratings. Hence, this analysis de-

    tects differences in the two groups response to the stimulus

    material beyond those differences specifically related to making

    an aesthetic judgment. Inspection of the parameter estimates in

    significant voxels from the interaction showed that experts hadsignificantly more activity in hippocampus, precuneus and cere-

    bellum relative to non-experts for expert-stimuli (buildings) com-

    pared to control-stimuli (faces). Precuneus is often reported to

    play a role in integrating the current input with prior established

    knowledge (Fletcher et al., 1995; Maguire, Frith, & Morris, 1999)

    and in episodic memory retrieval (Krause et al., 1999). Our data

    suggests that the demand on the precuneus is higher in experts

    when perceiving expert-stimuli, as building conditions presum-

    ably depend more on connections between retrieved information

    and prior knowledge for this group relative to non-experts. The

    hippocampus has also been consistently implicated in episodic

    memories (Brown & Aggleton, 2001; Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum,

    Young, & Bunsey, 1996; Eichenbaum, Yonelinas, & Ranganath,

    2007). Hippocampus activation has been associated with condi-tions where subjects correctly recollect contextual information

    compared to conditions where they do not (Cansino, Maquet, Do-

    lan, & Rugg, 2002). Our findings support these data and suggest

    that the hippocampus and precuneus may be selectively engaged

    during memory retrieval in experts. As we have ruled out famil-

    iarity effects in the data, experts may have attempted to organize

    new information into a framework of prior knowledge and use

    this information to guide and bias aesthetic judgments. The pos-

    sibility that the hippocampus and the precuneus are specifically

    involved in biasing preference judgments based on recruitment

    of episodic memory has also been suggested by other studies

    (Jacobsen et al., 2006; McClure et al., 2004). Specifically, a charac-

    teristic of episodic memories in the present study might be in-

    creased encoding of associations in experts (Eichenbaum et al.,2007) responding to expert-stimuli, which is dissociable from

    stimuli recognized based on familiarity (Eldridge, Knowlton, Fur-

    manski, Bookheimer, & Engel, 2000).

    For the converse interaction analysis we found no activation in

    areas involved in episodic memory formation. However, we found

    activity in regions of the visual cortex and in the ventral temporal

    cortex, such as the calcarine gyrus and in the fusiform gyrus. The

    bilateral activation of the fusiform gyrus is interesting as this re-

    gion, although distinctly demarcated by the collateral sulcus, isanatomically closely located to an area straddling the anterior

    end of lingual gyrus which has been deemed the location of a

    building-sensitive region (Aguirre, Zarahn, & DEsposito, 1998).

    Further evidence for building selectivity shows that the medial

    portion of the fusiform gyrus, including the collateral sulcus, dem-

    onstrates greater fMRI signal change in response to buildings as

    compared to faces and chairs (Ishai, Ungerleider, Martin, Schouten,

    & Haxby, 1999). There is disagreement about the exact anatomical

    location of a building-sensitive region, but it seems to include both

    inferior lingual gyrus and the fusiform gyrus surrounding the col-

    lateral sulcus. Our activation, clearly located in the fusiform gyrus,

    is, however, distinct from the face-sensitive region within the fusi-

    form gyrus (Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997), which is lo-

    cated inferior and lateral to our building-sensitive voxels. This is

    furthermore evidenced by the parameter estimates in the build-

    ing-sensitive region of the fusiform gyrus, where it is shown that

    this area is unresponsive to face stimuli in both groups (see

    Fig. 6). The region we observed in the fusiform gyrus is located just

    adjacent to the parahippocampal gyrus. Several neuroimaging

    studies have demonstrated that the posterior portion of the para-

    hippocampal gyrus is involved in the representation of large-scale

    places and scenes (Epstein& Kanwisher, 1998; Maguire, Frith, Bur-

    gess, Donnett, & OKeefe, 1998). It is noteworthy that we observed

    activity in the PPA in the conjunction analysis between [build-

    ings > faces] for experts and non-experts. Evident in this conjunc-

    tion is also activation, beyond the PPA, of the entire ventral

    temporal cortex and visual cortex (see Supplementary material),

    suggesting that the response to buildings is not restricted to the re-

    gion that responds maximally to that object category located in thefusiform gyrus. However, this effect may also be driven by differ-

    ences in visual stimulation across the two stimulus conditions, be-

    cause building trials were presented with varying pixel width. The

    fact that building stimuli activate the entire ventral temporal

    cortex, albeit to varying degrees, suggests, in agreement with other

    reports (Ishai et al., 1999), that the representation of buildings in

    this portion of the cortex may be feature-based rather than build-

    ing-sensitiveper se. Such an interpretation may account for the dif-

    ferential activation of the fusiform gyrus between experts and non-

    experts evident in the interaction analysis. The demand on this

    portion of the fusiform gyrus is higher for non-experts compared

    to experts presumably due to experts recruitment of episodic

    memory, whereas non-experts are more sensitive to specific per-

    ceptual features of building stimuli, which finds further supportby the relative stronger activation in the calcarine gyrus in non-ex-

    perts relative to experts.

    In conclusion, we have demonstrated that expertise modulates

    brain areas to both aesthetic processing and to cognitive or typo-

    logical processing irrespective of aesthetic ratings. Specifically,

    our new discovery is that the representation of stimulus value in

    medial OFC and bilateral subcallosal cingulate gyrus is modulated

    by expertise. We found that only some regions associated with the

    processing of reward are modulated by expertise (OFC, subcallosal

    cingulate gyrus), while activity in NAcc was typical of both experts

    and non-experts, suggesting that these regions play different roles

    in reward processing. Furthermore, we have demonstrated that ex-

    perts and non-experts differ in their neural response to expertise

    stimuli per se, irrespective of aesthetic ratings. This typological re-sponse was observed bilaterally in the hippocampus and precu-

    U. Kirk et al./ Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315 313

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    9/10

    neus, and suggests that experts may integrate current input into a

    framework of prior knowledge and use this information to orga-

    nize aesthetic judgments.

    Acknowledgments

    We thank Prof. S. Zeki, Dr. O.J. Hulme and Dr. T. Lund for helpful

    discussions. Prof. C. Frith, Dr. M.Self,Dr. V. Cardin and Dr. T. Ramsy

    provided useful comments on the manuscript. P. Neckelmann pre-

    paredthe stimulusmaterial. U. Kirk was supportedby a Ph.D. schol-

    arship from the Danish Medical Research Council; M. Skov was

    supported by Hvidovre Hospitals research foundation; M.S. Chris-

    tensen wassupported by a Ph.D. scholarship from theFacultyof Sci-

    ence, University of Copenhagen; N. Nygaard was supported by a

    Ph.D. scholarship by the Danish Research Council for the Humani-

    ties. The MR-scanner was donated by the Simon Spies Foundation.

    Appendix A. Supplementary data

    The figure displays the building conjunction from the zero-or-

    der parametric analysis derived from the building-specific main

    effects for both groups [Expert_BuildExpert_Faces] and [Non-Ex-

    pert_BuildNon-Expert_Faces]. Glass-brain activation is displayed

    at FDR-corrected threshold. Supplementary data associated with

    this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/

    j.bandc.2008.08.004.

    References

    Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., & DEsposito, M. (1998). An area within the human ventral

    cortex sensitive to building stimuli: Evidence and implications. Neuron, 21,373383.

    Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., OConnor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001).

    Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioural evidence.

    Neuron, 32, 537551.Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Stappen, I., Panitz, D., Ghahremani, D. G., Glover, G.,

    et al. (2003). Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in

    human olfaction. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 196202.

    Apicella, P., Ljungberg, T., Scarnati, E., & Schultz, W. (1991). Responses to reward indorsal and ventral striatum. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 491500.

    Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., & Hinrichs, H. (2006).

    Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists:

    Evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage, 30, 917926.Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Decision making and the

    orbitalfrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295307.Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music

    correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion.

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98 ,1181811823.

    Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional responses to

    pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain

    regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382387.Brown, M. W., & Aggleton, J. P. (2001). Recognition memory: What are the roles of

    the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature Review Neuroscience, 2, 5161.Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., & Parsons, L. M. (2004). Passive music listening

    spontaneously engages limbic and paralimbic systems. NeuroReport, 15,20332037.

    Buchel, C., Holmes, A. P., Rees, G., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Characterizing stimulusresponse functions using nonlinear regressors in parametric fMRI experiments.

    Neuroimage, 8, 140148.Cansino, S., Maquet, P., Dolan, R. J., & Rugg, M. D. (2002). Brain activity underlying

    encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 10481056.Cela-Conde, C. J., Marty, G., Maest, F., Ortiz, T., Munar, E., Fernndez, A., et al.

    (2004). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic

    perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States ofAmerica, 101, 63216325.

    Cooper, J. C., & Knutson, B. (2008). Valence and salience contribute to nucleus

    accumbens activation. Neuroimage, 39, 538547.Critchley, H. D. (2004). The human cortex responds to an interoceptive challenge.

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,101, 63336334.

    de Araujo, I. E., Rolls, E. T., Velazco, M. I., Margot, C., & Cayeux, I. (2005). Cognitive

    modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron, 46, 671679.Deichmann, R., Gottfried, J. A., Hutton, C., & Turner, R. (2003). Optimized EPI for

    fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 19, 430441.

    Eichenbaum, H., Schoenbaum, G., Young, B., & Bunsey, M. (1996). Functionalorganization of the hippocampal memory system. Proceedings of the

    National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93,1350013507.

    Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe

    and recognition memory. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 21, 123152.Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Engel, S. A.

    (2000). Remembering episodes: A selective role for the hippocampus during

    retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 11481152.Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual

    environment. Nature, 392, 598601.Eysenck, H. J., & Castle, M. (1970). Training in art as a factor in the determination of

    preference judgments for polygons. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 6581.Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M., Shallice, T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J.

    (1995). Brainsystems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory:An

    in vivo study in humans. Brain, 118, 401416.Frey, S., & Petrides, M. (2002). Orbitofrontal cortex and memory formation. Neuron,

    36, 171176.Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Frith, C. D., Poline, J. B.,Heather, J. D.,& Frackowiak, R. S. J.

    (1995). Spatial registration and normalization of images. Human Brain Mapping,2, 165189.

    Friston, K. J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Turner, R. (1996).

    Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine,35, 346355.

    Glaser, D., & Friston, K. J. (2004). Variance components. In R. S. J. Frackowiak, K. J.

    Friston, C. D. Frith, R. J. Dolan, C. J. Price, & S. Zeki, et al. (Eds.), Human brainfunction (pp. 781792). Elsevier Academic Press.

    Glover, G. H., Li, T. Q., & Ress, D. (2000). Image-based method for retrospective

    correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. MagneticResonance in Medicine, 44, 162167.

    Gordon, D. A. (1951/1952). Methodology in the study of art evaluation. Journal ofAesthetics and Art Criticism, 10, 338352.

    Gordon, D. A. (1956).Individual differences in theevaluation of artand the natureof

    art standards. Journal of Educational Research, 50, 1730.Gottfried, J. A., Deichmann, R., Winston, J. S., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Functional

    heterogeneity in human olfactory cortex: An event-related functional magnetic

    resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1081910828.Hekkert, P., Peper, L. E., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1994). The effect of verbal

    instruction and artistic background on the aesthetic judgment of rectangles.

    Empirical Studies of the Arts, 12, 185203.Hekkert, P., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1996a). The impact of level of expertise on

    the evaluation of original and altered versions of post-impressionistic paintings.

    Acta Psychologia, 94, 117131.Hekkert, P., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1996b). Beauty in the eye of expert and

    nonexpert beholders: A study in the appraisal of art. American Journal ofPsychology, 109, 389407.

    Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., Schouten, J. L., & Haxby, J. V. (1999).

    Distributed representation of objects in the human visual pathway. Proceedingsof the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96,

    93799384.Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Hfel, L., & Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Brain correlates of

    aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage, 29, 276285.Jensen, J., Smith, A. J., Willeit, M., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., Vitcu, I., et al. (2007).

    Separate brain regions code for salience vs valance during reward prediction in

    humans. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 294302.Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A

    module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal ofNeuroscience, 17, 43024311.

    Kawabata, H., & Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal ofNeurophysiology, 91, 16991705.

    Knutson, B., & Cooper, J. C. (2005). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of

    reward prediction. Current Opinion of Neurobiology, 18, 411417.Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Adams, C. M., Varner, J. L., & Hommer, D. (2001).

    Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI.

    NeuroReport, 12, 36833687.Koelsch, S.,Fritz,T., vonCramon,D.Y., Mller,K., &Friederici,A. D.(2006).Investigating

    emotionwith music: An fMRi study. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 239250.Krause, B. J., Schmidt, D., Mottaghy, F. M., Taylor, J., Halsband, U., Herzog, H., et al.

    (1999). Episodic retrieval activates the precuneus irrespective of the imagerycontent of word pair associates: A PET-study. Brain, 122, 255263.

    Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to

    hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691702.Kringelbach, M. L., ODoherty, J., Rolls, E. T., & Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the

    human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its

    subjective pleasantness. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 10641071.Lane, R. D., Reinman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1997).

    Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. The AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 154, 926933.

    Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M.

    (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review.

    Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390423.Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic

    appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489508.Lund, T. E., Madsen, K. H., Sidaros, K., Luo, W. L., & Nichols, T. E. (2006). Non-white

    noise in fMRI: Does modeling have an impact? Neuroimage, 29, 5466.Lund, T. E.,Nrgaard, M. D.,Rostrup, E., Rowe, J. B.,& Paulson, O. B. (2005). Motion or

    activity: Their role in intra- and inter-subject variation in fMRI. Neuroimage, 26,960964.

    314 U. Kirk et al. / Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004
  • 7/28/2019 Brain Correlates of Aesthetic Expertise_A Parametric fMRI Study (2009)

    10/10

    Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., & Buonocore, M. H. (2003). Posterior cingulate cortex

    activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task.

    Human Brain Mapping, 18, 3041.Maguire, E. A., Frith, C. D., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., & OKeefe, J. (1998). Knowing

    where things are: Parahippocampal involvement in encoding object locations in

    virtual large-scale space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 6176.Maguire, E. A., Frith,C. D., & Morris, R. G. M. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of

    comprehension and memory: The importance of prior knowledge. Brain, 122,18391850.

    Maguire, E. A.,Gadian, D. G.,Johnsrude,I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.

    S., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxidrivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of

    America, 97, 44144416.McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., & Montague, P. R.

    (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks.

    Neuron, 44, 379387.McClure, S. M., York, M. K., & Montague, P. R. (2004). The neural substrates of

    reward processing in humans: The modern role of fMRI. Neuroscientist, 10,260268.

    Menon, V., & Levitin, D. J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: Response and

    physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage, 28, 175184.Montague, P. R., Hyman, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Computational roles for dopamine

    in behavioural control. Nature, 431, 760767.Nakamura, K., Kawashima, R., Nagumo, S., Ito, K., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., et al. (1998).

    Neuroanatomical correlates of the assessment of facial attractiveness.

    NeuroReport, 9, 753757.Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T., Glover, G.,

    et al. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: An fMRI study of neural systems

    supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience, 16, 17461772.

    ODoherty, J. P., Buchanan, T. W., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Predictive neural

    coding of reward preference involves dissociable responses in human ventral

    midbrain and ventral striatum. Neuron, 49, 157166.ODoherty, J. P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Neural responses

    during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron, 33, 815926.ODoherty, J., Rolls,E. T.,Francis, S., Bowtell, R.,& McGlone,F. (2001). Representation

    of pleasant andaversivetaste in the human brain.Journal of Neurophysiology, 85,13151321.

    ODoherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2003).

    Beauty in a smile; the role of medial orbitiofrontal cortex in facial

    attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147155.OHare, D. P. A. (1976). Individual differences in perceived similarity and preference

    for visual art: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Perception and Psychophysics,20, 445452.

    Paus, T. (2001). Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and

    cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 417424.

    Plassmann, H., ODoherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can

    modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105 , 10501054.

    Rolls, E. T., Kringelbach, M. L., & de Araujo, I. E. (2003). Different representations of

    pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain. European Journal ofNeuroscience, 18, 695703.

    Rolls, E. T., ODoherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Francis, S., Bowtell, R., & McGlone, F.

    (2003). Representations of pleasant and painful touch in the human

    orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 308317.Rushworth, M. F., Behrens, T. E., Rudebeck, P. H., & Walton, M. E. (2007). Contrasting

    roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour.Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 168176.

    Schlaug, G. (2003). The brain of musicians. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), Thecognitive neuroscience of music (pp. 366381). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Schmidt, J. A., McLaughlin, J. P., & Leighten, J. (1989). Novice strategies for

    understanding paintings. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 3, 6572.Schultz, W., Apicella, P., Ljungberg, T., Romo, R., & Scarnati, E. (1993). Reward-

    related activity in the monkey striatum and substantia nigra. Progress in BrainResearch, 99, 227235.

    Seymour, B., Daw, N., Dayan, P., Singer, T., & Dolan, R. (2007). Differential encoding

    of losses and gains in the human striatum. Journal of Neuroscience, 27,48264831.

    Skov, M. (in press). The pleasure of art. In M. Kringelbach & K. Berridge (Eds.),

    Pleasures of the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., & Parrish, T.

    (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective

    valuation in human gestation. Neuron, 39, 701711.Small, D. M., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2001).

    Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate. Brain, 124, 17201733.Spicer, J., Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2007). Sensitivity

    of the nucleus accumbens to violations in expectation of reward. Neuroimage,34, 455461.

    Tremblay, L., & Schultz, W. (1999). Relative reward preference in primate

    orbitofrontal cortex. Nature, 398, 704708.Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2004). Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference

    for paintings. NeuroReport, 15, 893897.Wallis, J. D. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making.

    Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 30, 3156.Watanabe, M. (1999). Attraction is relative not absolute. Nature, 398, 661662.Winston, J. S., ODoherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain

    systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195206.Worsley, K. J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P., Vandal, A. C., Friston, K., & Evans, A. C. (1996). A

    unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of

    cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping, 4, 5873.Zink, C. F., Martin-Skurski, M. E., Chappelow, J. C., & Berns, G. S. (2004). Human

    striatal responses to monetaryreward depend on saliency. Neuron, 42, 509517.

    U. Kirk et al./ Brain and Cognition 69 (2009) 306315 315