c apter 15 h expressions and body language cultural ... · c15 hapter cultural differences in...

12
CHAPTER 15 Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabe th Huis in 't Veld Abstract As our environment becomes increasingly more internati onal, we are interacting increasi ngly more with people from different cultures. During social interactions. it is important to respond appropriately to the cues that are relevant in a given situation. P eople are expected to behave in a sui tabl e way to avoid offending other s. F or example, an appropriate response to a gr eeting can avoid much misund er standing. Cultu ral variat.ions have often been named as possible factors for explaining differences in processing emoti ons. Because culture is in pa rt about r egu lati ng social interaction, one expects to find t hat cultural norms define displ ay r ul es that are at least characteristic of the daily expressions of emotions. This chapter addresses whether there are important cultural factors that determine whole-body expressions of emot ion, how t hey are perceived, how they are displayed, and what they are. i Key Words: cul ture, neuroscience, body expr ess ions of emotion, social interactions. processing emotions C ultural Perspectives on the Perception o fBody Expression W i th current communi ca tion technologies and enterpri ses, our environment is beco ming :r .ore internation al eve ry day. - ll1is mea ns rhar we are nter acting increasingly more with people from dif- ;rrc nt cu l tures. During socia l i nreracrions, it is very . r.1ponam w respond appropriately to the cues that <rerelcvant in a given situation. People are ex pected :o bchavc in a suitable way in order ro avo id offend- r.g o thcrs . For exampl e, an appropriate response to •gree ting can avoid a lot of mi sunderstand in g, and , orrcctly judging the inrenrions of one's business )J rtnc r can make all the difference between reach- n ga d esi rable deal or nor. Cuirural variations have otten been named as possible factors for explaining Ji lfercn ces in processing emotions. Because cul- :ure is in part about regulating social interaction, 1 ne c xpccrs to find char cultural norms define dis- ,l ar rul es rhat are at least characteristic of the daily aprc ssi ons of emotions. This chapter addresses the quescions of whether th ere are al so imponant cui- rural factors thar determ ine whole body express ions of emotion, how they are perceived, how they are displayed, and what they are. The few studi es that ha ve investigated cul tural aspects of body exp ressions a nd emotion have examined facial expression recognition in different cultures . Again, rhe vast majority of resea rch arti cl es on cross-cultural differences in emotion pe rcept ion have examin ed rhe recognition of facia l expres- sions. ln bri eAy r eviewing this literature, one not es that there is norhing like a systematic analysis of the kinds of questions and the types of issues that belong specifi ca ll y he re. In many of th e stud ies, it is nor clear where, ar what l evel, and at what stage during individual face processing cultural effects ca n enter the processing routines. 1he notion of cul - ture is rare ly commented on, and rhat of mce is usu- ally used at a descriptive level only, sidestepp in g the sophi sticated analyses that biologists and anthro- pologists use nowadays to understand th is term. 223

Upload: hamien

Post on 16-Apr-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

CHAPTER

15 Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language

Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

Abstract

As our environment becomes increasingly more international, we are interacting increasingly more with people from different cultures. During social interactions. it is important to respond appropriately to the cues that are relevant in a given situation. People are expected to behave in a suitable way to avoid offending others. For example, an appropriate response to a greeting can avoid much misunderstanding. C ultural variat.ions have often been named as possible factors for explaining differences in processing emotions. Because culture is in part about regulating social interaction, one expects to find that cultural norms define display r ules that are at least character istic of the daily expressions of emotions. This chapter addresses whether there are important cultural factors that determine whole-body expressions of emotion, how they are perceived, how they are displayed, and what they are.

i Key Words: culture, neuroscience, body expressions of emotion, social interactions. processing

emotions

Cultural Perspectives on the Perception ofBody Expression

With current communication technologies and ~obal enterprises, our environment is becoming :r.ore international every day. -ll1is means rhar we are nteracting increasingly more with people from dif­

;rrcnt cultures. During social i nreracrions, it is very .r.1ponam w respond appropriately to the cues that <rerelcvant in a given situation. People are expected :obchavc in a suitable way in order ro avoid offend­r.g othcrs. For example, an appropriate response to •greeting can avoid a lot of misunderstand ing, and ,orrcctly judging the inrenrions of one's business )Jrtncr can make all the difference between reach­nga desirable deal or nor. Cui rural variations have otten been named as possible factors for explaining Jilfercnces in processing emotions. Because cul­:ure is in part about regulating social interaction, 1ne cxpccrs to find char cultural norms define dis­,lar rules rhat are at least characteristic of the daily aprcssions of emotions. This chapter addresses the

quescions of whether there are also imponant cui­rural factors thar determ ine whole body expressions of emotion, how they are perceived, how they are displayed, and what they are.

The few stud ies that have investigated cul tural aspects of body expressions and emotion have examined facial expression recognition in different cultures. Again, rhe vast majority of research articles on cross-cultural d ifferences in emotion perception have examined rhe recognition of facia l expres­sions. ln brieAy reviewing this literature, one notes that there is norhing like a systematic analysis of the kinds of questions and the types of issues that belong specifically here. In many of the studies, it is nor clear where, ar what level, and at what stage during individual face processing cultural effects can enter the processing routines. 1he notion of cul­ture is rarely commented on, and rhat of mce is usu­ally used at a descriptive level only, sidestepping the sophisticated analyses that biologists and anthro­pologists use nowadays to understand th is term.

223

Page 2: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

One might view this set of questions as belonging to the new field of cultural neuroscience, expecring that they will be treated more systematically in the near future. Growing insight in the neurological processes underlying face and emotion recognition, the inAuence of race and culture on these processes, and cultural variations in display rules provides a framework for the much needed research on cul­tural d ifferences in the bodily expressions of emo­tion and social interactions.

Culture, Race, and Face Perception The main aspects of faces that have been studied

from a cross-cultural perspective are identity, expres­sion, and gaze direction. A fourth aspect is equally interesring and concerns cultural differences in the way observers from d ifferent racial and cultural backgrounds take advantage of multiple and parallel channels of social information input, such as recog­nizing a person's emotion from his facial expressions as well as from his tone of voice. We brieAy review some relevant studies in each of these areas.

Race and Person Identity Face perception has been a topic of extensive

research, and whether people perceive faces of their own race differently than those of other races has been of interest ro researchers for over 50 years. The phenomenon known as the "other race effect" refers to the fact that people are better at recogniz­ing faces from their own race than faces from other racial groups (Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991; O'Toole, Deffenbacher, Valentin, & Abdi, 1994; for review, see Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Our lab performed a few experiments on the other-race effect using Chinese subjects who had no previous exposure to Caucasian faces . We used an extensive battery of tasks rhar were available from previous experiments in our laboratory. The tasks had been developed to invesrigare both neurologically intact and impaired observers, as well as various clinical populations. Therefore, these tasks raxed verbal and cognirive functions (e.g., memory) as li ttle as possible. For example, during all match-to-sample tasks, one picture was always presented on top, and a marching picture had to be chosen from the two pictures below. One experiment focused on iden­riry recognition and required marching the target stimulus shown in fro ntal view to the correct foil shown in three-fourths profile. One block presented Caucasian faces and the other Chinese. We did not find the other-race effect s ince our results showed no difference between groups or stimuli (Sinke, 2012) .

One importan t and well-studied phenom­enon in face recognition pertains to the idea that recognition of the face is more dependent on hol istic, or configura!, processing than, for exam­ple, object recognition (Tanaka & Farah, 1993). Consequently, it has been surmised that the other-race effect could be explained by improved holistic processing for same-race faces (Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006). In our lab, we recently created the idemiry face-marching task as described previously with South African faces from a Xhosa population. Caucasian stu­dents performed the task with upright and inverted Caucasian and South African faces. In line with the other-race effect, Caucasian participants showed higher accuracy rates and faster reaction t imes 011

Caucasian than 011 South African faces. This task also allows the creation of a measure of configura­tion processing as measured with the face-inversion effect (Farah, Tanaka, & Drain, 1995; Yin, 1969). Interestingly, the inversion effect was comparable for both Caucasian and South African faces, so the other-race effect as fou nd in this study could not readily be explained by an effect of race on con­figural processing. These resultS are in line with those of other studies that found evidence fo r the other-race effect even though no differences in con­figural processing could be found, giving rise to rhe idea that the other-race effect cannot be com­pletely explained by differences in holistic process­ing (Michel, Caldara, & Rossion, 2006; Mondloch et aL, 2010). In addition, these effects have been studied using an evem-relared potential (ERP) known to be specifically involved in face processing, the N 170 (Eimer, 1998). The studies investigat­ing the face inversion effect show slightly different but, again, somewhat varied resul ts, indicari ng that both same- and other-race faces are processed holis­t ically, although it may be delayed for other races (Wiese, Stahl, & Schweinberger, 2009) or increased for same-race faces (Caharel et al., 2011; Gajewski, Schlegel, & Stoerig, 2008; Montalan et al. , 20 13; Vizioli, Foreman, Rousselet, & Caldara, 2009). In contrast, Hahn, Jantzen, and Symons (20 12) found that same-race faces were processed more holisti­cally than faces from other races using a technique called "lhatcheriz.ation," rotating only the eyes and mouth within a face.

Another important aspect in cross-racial facial recognition is the extent to which people have experience with other races (Bukach, Cottle, Ubiwa, & Miller, 2012; Rhodes et al., 2009; Srahl, Wiese, & Schweinberger, 2008). In addition to

224 I CULTURAL D I FFERENCES I N EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIONS AND BODY LANGUAGE

Page 3: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

1he race of the perceived person, and the degree to which perceivers have experience with other races, Michel, Corneille, and Rossi on (20 I O) showed that 1he degree to which the same, racially ambiguous face is processed holistically depends on what the observer perceived to be the race of the stimulus. Another explanation for racial d ifferences in holis­Iic processing may be baseline differences berween people of different races. For example, there is evidence that Asians may process fuces and other stimul i more configurally rhan Caucasians (M ichel, Caldara, et al., 2006; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 1004, for an overview, see Miyamoto, 2013). Ou r lab recently conducted a part-to-whole marching

experiment in Japan. Participants marched eyes or mouths to a picture of a whole Caucasian face, o r 1hey marched doors or windows ro a picwre of a whole house. Accuracy scores and reaction rimes of 1he Japanese parricipams were similar to rhose of a Dutch control group and, furthermore, they showed the same pattern in face and object parr rec­ognition as a Dutch control sample. Tn shorr, rhe japanese participants d id nor show an other-race effect, which may be explained by these baseline dif­ferences in hol istic processing berween populations.

'I he next question concerns the time during per­ceprual processing when race influences face per­cepiion. Walker and Tanaka (2003) found this race effect already ar early srages of perceptual encoding by using a sequential matching task with natural and morphed East Asian and Caucasian faces. However, 1he N 170 ER.P has given rise to larger amplitudes for other rhan same race faces (Caharel er al., 2011; Gajewski et al., 2008; Herrmann er al., 2007; Stahl et al., 2008; Walker, Silverr, Hewsrone, & Nobre,

2008). This was not seen in the earlier PI 00 ERP (Caharel et al., 20 I I; Herrmann er al., 2007; Stahl e1 al. , 2008). Thus, a race effect seems to manifest itsel f at a later stage, relared to rhe structural encod­

ing stage of the face and larer, ar approximately 250-300 ms (Caldara, Rossion, Bover, & Hauen, 2004; Caldara et al., 2003; Tanaka & Pierce, 2009). In conclusion, the picrure of rhe orher-race effect is Slill nor very solid and rhe explanation is not clear, as one can also discern from recent meta-analysis reports (Michel, Rossion, et al., 2006; Young, Hugenberg, Bernstein, & Sacco, 20 12).

Race and Facial Expressions Facial expressions play a major role in com­

munication, bur this role may be modulated by culture or race. The debate on universal facial expressions of basic emotions has been occurring

for decades (see Nelson & Russell, 20 13; Scherer, Clark-Polner, & Morrillaro, 2011 ). Currently, how­ever, many researchers take a position rhar is a sorr of compromise berween the classical Ekman posi­tion on universal emotions enshrined in rhe Ekman facial expressions and the realization rhar rhere is a measure of cultural relariviry.

A modest position rhar only touches rhar debate rangentially concerns rhe influence of culture ar rhe level of rhe display rules for emotions. Culture provides various kinds of rules about which facial expressions to d isplay, when, and where. This is a vast topic, and deal ing with it systematically would require contributions from anthropologists and socio logists, among others. To illusrrare, there is cultural variability in rhe level of rhe display rules fo r emotions. For example, Japanese people are per­ceived as less likely ro show negative emotions such as fear or anger bur more likely to express positive emotions (Hess, Blairy, & Kleck, 2000). At the least, there are cultural differences in rhe appropriateness of expressing certain emotions in certain sirua­tions. Overall, Asians are less prone to express nega­tive emotions such as contempt and fear (Chung, 20 12; Safdar er al., 2009). A well-known experi­ment showed that Japanese participants were prone to mask negative emotions by smiling, whereas Americans did not (Friesen, 1972). Evidence for this emotion suppression was found when Asian and American participants were asked to suppress their emotion in response to images while ERPs were recorded. In Asian participants, bur not in Americans, a significant decrease in the late posi­t ive potential (LPP), a measure of emotional pro­cessing, was found (Murata, Moser, & Kitayama, 20 13). Possibly because of rhese traditions, people in some East Asian countries tend to be more emo­t ionally introverted (Bond, 1993; Matsumoto, Yoo, Hirayama, & Perrova, 2005).

Because of these cultural differences in display rules, it can be hypothesized that Asians are more experienced than Westerners in estimating emo­t ional states from looking ar the eyes because it is well-known rhat muscles around the eyes (orbicu­laris oculi) are d ifficult to control (Ekman & Friesen,

2003). An interesting day-ro-day result of this focus on the eyes can be seen in emoticons used in dif­ferent areas of the world (Pollack, 1996). Whereas

Westerners keep the eyes constant (rwo simple dots) but change the mouth ro signify different emotions,

Japanese keep the mouth as a straight line while changing the eye symbols for each emotion. To rest this cultural bias on rhe eye versus the mouth, Yuki,

DE GELDER, H UtS IN 'T VELD 225

Page 4: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

Maddux, and Masuda (2007) used borh emoticons and real faces and manipulated independently rhe eyes and mouths. They fo und rhar Japanese partici­pants rated the emotion by focusing mainly on the eyes, whereas American participants used the mouth as a prominent cue (also see Blais, Jack, Scheepers, Fiset, & Caldara, 2008). These findings are sup­ported by a study that examined the eye move­ments of Eastern and Western participants during rhe decod ing of facial expressions. It was fo und that Eastern people, in contrast ro Westerners, fix­are on the eye region instead of looking ar mulriplc regions of rhe face. In addition, these different scan patterns are likely to be an underlying cause of rec­ognition deficits in certai n emotions Uack, Blais, Scheepers, Schyns, & Caldara, 2009). In contrast, Asians and Caucasians were found ro use largely the same cortical networks while trying co decode emotion from the eyes of both their in-group and their our-group, although these networks were more active in response w eyes belonging tO rhe same race (Adams, Rule, et al. , 2010).

Also on a more physical level, emotions are nor always expressed similarly or to the same degree between d ifFerent culrures (Eife nbein, 20 13). For example, when participants fro m Quebec and Gabon were asked to freely express emotions, it was fou nd rhar the groups used different action units fo r different emotional expressions (Elfenbein, Beaupre, Levesque, & Hess, 2007). In other words, people from different cultures use different facial muscles to

express emotion. Ir may therefore not come as a sur­prise rhar what people expect to see in an emotional expression differs as wel l. -This cultural variety was demonscrared by add ing random noise ro a standard face o r by using a random action uni t generator to create random expressions, after which participants were asked to judge the emotion seen in the random noise. ' ll1ese responses were then used ro create aver­age expected emotional expressions, which differed in each cu lture Uack, Caldara, & Schyns, 20 12; Jack, Garrod, Yu, Caldara, & Schyns, 20 12). Also, there is evidence supporting the notion that rhe left side of the face may be more strongly infiuenced by culture than the right side and that there are cultural differences in these hemifacial biases (Eifenbe in, Mandai, Ambady, Harizuka, & Kumar, 2004; Mandai, Harizuka, Bhushan, & Mishra, 200 1 ). These effects are important to rake into consider­ation nor only fo r researchers who are interested in faci al responses, emotional contagion, or imitation but also because they may be of importance fo r new emerging techniques such as fac ial interpretation

softv,rare for smarrphones, security cameras, or games. In conclusion, it seems that a Facial Action Coding System (FACS; Ekman & Friesen, 1978). which maps all the muscles used fo r facial expres· sions, would need ro be different fo r each culture.

Probably because of these differences in the expression of emotion, there are subtle differences in how people from different countries interpret facial expressions of emotions (Eifenbein & Ambady. 2002). There seems to be an in-group advamagc especially for the recognition of culturally "natural· emotions and a majority advanrage for standard­ized (FACS) expressions (Beaupre & Hess, 2005: Elfenbein eta!. , 2007; for review, see Kang & Lau, 20 13). In a functional magnetic resonance imag­ing (fMRl) experimenr, Chiao et al. (2008) showed Japanese and American parricipanrs emotional faces of their in- and our-groups, and ir was fo und that che amygdala responded more strongly ro fearful expressions of che in-group. However, there may also be differences in che way rhe Japanese brain and che Caucasian brain process emotion ro begin with; it was found that the Japanese rely more on the pre· motor corcex and the insula, whereas Caucasians recruited the posterior cingulate, che supplemen· rary motor area, and che amygdala (Moriguchi et al., 2005).

A meta-analysis by Elfenbein and Ambady (2002; see also Elfenbein & Ambady, 2003) clearly showed char the in-group advantage was reduced when more intercultural concacc was establ ished. An fMRI study found that culture and duration of stay influenced amygdala activation and its relation· ship to the recognition of anger and disgust (Dernd et al., 2009). Srudies of cultural factors need to more systematically compare results obtained with groups before and after they have had extensive exposure to

the other cul ture. In any case, living in a multicul· rural environment or not must be a relevant f.1ctor. -Therefore, in our experiment, we rested Chinese stu· dents who had recenrly arrived in The Netherlands (no more rhan 2 weeks) so thar their contact with Wesrern people was at a minimum. The prcviouslv mentioned study on Chinese versus Durch idcmiry recognition also contained a memory experimen1. Dutch and Chinese parricipanrs were asked to memorize a ser of Caucasian faces . In one experi­ment, rhe faces wore neurral expressions. l l1c Dmch participants performed significantly bener than the Chinese. However, che d ifference between the two groups disappeared when the faces to remember were showing a fearfu l, sad, or happy expression. ll1is suggests that neutral faces of strangers :1re more

226 I CU LTURAL DIFFERENCES IN EMOTIONAL EX PR ESS I ONS AND BODY LANGUAGE

Page 5: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

difficult to remember. lt also indicates that memory for faces of people from another race is at least to some extend culturally specific, but it may also be dependent on emotion and probably the inrerplay between race and gender (Krumhuber & Manstead, 2011). However, the pattern was differenr when we

used a task that required matching an expression ro the correct similar one rather than a memory rest. Overall, the Dutch parricipanrs were rather f.1Ster than Chinese participants in matching the Caucasian facial expression . However, this was also dependent on the emotion; Chinese students were slower than the Dutch on all emotions except happy. Fear and surprise were recognized less accurately in rhe two groups than the other emotions. Taking rhese and previous results into consideration, happy seems to be the facial emotion most easily recog­nized across cultures.

Culture and Gaze Humans use information on the direction of

gaze of others to rapidly orienr themselves ro the location where the gaze is directed- a process that is useful to detect relevanr events in the en vi ronmenr. However, this automatic orienting behavior seems

ro be influenced by the in-group versus out-group status of rhe observed face. For example, an Italian srudy showed that black parricipants shifted atten­rion similarly to the averred gaze of black and white faces, whereas white participants selectively shifted attention only in response to individuals of their same group (Pavan, Dalmaso, Galfano, & Castelli, 20 II ) . AJlother study showed that the own-race effect for the memory of faces is also dependent on eye gaze, where same- race faces were recog­nized better only when the gaze was d irect (Adams, Pauker, & Weisbuch, 20 I 0). It is interesting to note how some effects chat were previously deemed to

be automatic attenrion effects a re now found to

be under the concrol of social factors (Shepherd, 20 10). Cerrainly, more research is needed to unravel rhe various components of these complex behaviors, and it might also shed light on che question of cui­rural or racial influences on the perception of bodily expressions. Comparable to gaze direction, body language provides directional informacion, and a disruption of these processes as a result of racial interactions can be indicative of the effect of race on

body language. Regarding the perception of emotion and gaze,

it was found char East Asians experience a face as angrier when the eye contact is direct {Akechi et al., 2013). Furthermore, another study found that the

direct gaze of our-group members is experienced as more inrense chan the gaze of in-group members in an Asian versus Caucasian compa rison (Kramer et al. , 20 13). This may be explained by greater amygdala responses co the d irect gaze of our-group members (Adams, FrankJin, et al. , 20 I 0; Richeson, Todd, Trawalter, & Baird, 2008).

Culture and Affective Cue Selection To date, cross-cultural stud ies of emotion have

typically used facial expressions not accompanied by information from the voice as expected in rhe natural environment (de Gelder & Bertelson, 2003; de Gelder, Boeker, Tuomainen, Hensen, & Vroomen, 1999; de Gelder & Vroomen, 2000; Massaro & Egan, I 996). However, there are indications of cross-cultural d ifferences in the way information from the face and information fro m the voice are combined . Cultural differences were reported nor only in unisensory stimuli bur also in the susceptibility ro ignore information in visual (Masuda er a!., 2008) and auditory (Ishii, Reyes, & Kirayama, 2003) modalities. ll1e very

process of integrating emotional cues from differ­ent modalities may also be cultu re sensitive. We investigated cultural differences in the multisensory perception of emotion between Japanese and Dutch participants. In line with che view that the Japanese are sensitive to conrexrual information (Masuda et al., 2008) and to vocal affect (Ishii et al., 2003), we expected chat they would weigh cues in the voice more than the Dutch when presented with audio­visual stimuli. We used short fragments voiced by two Japanese and two Dutch female speakers in their native language. Each fragment with neutral linguis tic meaning was uttered with happy or angry emotion, and the fragments were combined with

happy and angry facial expressions. Our results demonstrated that when the face and the voice

did nor represent rhe same emotion, rhe Japanese participants weighted cues in the voices more chan

did the Dutch participants. These findings provide the firsr evidence char culture modulates multisen­sory integration of affective information (Tanaka et al., 20 I O). The results arc consistent with rhose

of a study reporting char the Japanese are more sus­ceptible w affective prosody and less susceptible ro linguistic content compared to Americans (Ishii et al., 2003). Our results are also consistent with a lesser degree of audiovisual speech integration in the Japanese (Sekiyama & ' lo hkura, 1991). These results can be interpreted as showing rhar in mul­tisensory situations, che Japanese people rely more

DE GELDER, HUIS IN ' T VELD I 227

Page 6: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

on a vocal affect than a facia] expression because d irect or excessive eye contact can be considered rude in their culture (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969) or because they rend ro use a less efficient srraregy to judge the facial expression Qack er al., 2009). lr also may be related ro the fact that Japanese people control the display of their own feelings in rhe face (Ekman, 1972; Matsumoto, Takeuchi, Andayani,

Kouznersova, & Krupp, 1998). This may lead to a lower reliance on rhe face and higher reliance on the voice when judging a person's emotion.

Cultural Differences in the Recognition of Bodily Expressions

Although there is no doubt rhar the face, or voice, can indicate much about a person's feelings or intentions, body language is another very important indicator. Bodily expressions of emotion have only recently become a domain of interest, and knowl­edge in this area is increasing rapidly (for reviews, see de Gelder, 2006; de Gelder, Snyder, Greve, Gerard, & Hadjikhani, 2004; de Gelder er al., 20 l 0), although cultural differences in perceiv­ing bodily expressions have nor yet received much arrention. We previously stared rhar facial expres­sions can be culturally d iverse, and there is evidence that the same might be true for bodily expressions. In 1988, Scherer er al. found that rhe Japanese do nor use as many movements of the hands, arms, and body in certain emotional situations. Furthermore, the Japanese and Americans have some un ique and differing features rhar enable them to recognize emotion from body posture, whereas other fea­tures may be universal. For example, a bent head

and arms hanging next to rhe body are essential for Japanese participants to recognize sadness, but Sri Lankans and Americans accept other poses for sad­ness as well. Also, poses that are viewed as fearful by

Japanese people are perceived as angry by Sri Lankans (Kleinsmith, De Silva, & Bianchi-Berrhouze, 2006; Sogon & Masutani, 1989). The study by Kleinsmith et al. (2006) also supporrs the theory that some cul­tures may be less expressive: When American and

Japanese parricipams are asked to rare how intensive rhe emotion is rhar a person must be feeling based

on bodily expression, the ratings of the Japanese are higher than those of Americans. The same was pre­viously found for Chinese participants: The Chinese rare facial expression of negative emotions of other Chinese as more intense than those of Caucasians (Zhu, Ho, & Bonanno, 2013). Th is implies rhar a Japanese observer infers that someone might be feel­ing more than she is expressing. On the other hand,

although cultural diversity exists, it seems that there is at least some universality with regard to recogniz· ing emotional expressions in the body. For example, Americans and Indians were asked to categorize emotional dance expressions as described in ancient Indian texts, and both groups were able to do this accurately (Hejmadi, Davidson, & Rozin, 2000). Similarly, in-group and our-group effects, as dis· cussed with facia l expressions, may be similar with regard to bodily expressions: Caucasians are faster tO

recognize angry body language when a black face is combined with an angry body than when a Caucasian face is combined with the exact same angry body (Hinzman & Kelly, 2013). In contrast, African and American children judged rhe expression of emo­tional faces, faces and bodies, and bodies alone, and no cultural differences were found in the body-only condition (Tum inello & Davidson, 201 1).

Additionally, in a recent behavioural study, we

found that white Western European participants perceived black bodies of ambiguous emotion as angrier than ambiguous white bodies when in a neutral context (i.e., paired with neutral sounds); however, when rhe bodies were paired with affective sounds there was no difference in emotion carego· rization, suggesting that specific multisensory con· rexrs may be able ro reduce or eliminate race-specific biases in emotion perception. We also investigated rhe cerebral correlates of "other race" body emotion perception, scanning white European participants using fMRI . Here, we dearly saw regions which activated more to other race, vs. own race bod· ies, including the bilateral interparietal lobule and fusiform gyrus.

In our lab, we tested the same group of Chinese subjects with instrumental and emotional bodily expressions, and the procedure was rhe same as that used for faces. Here also, no differences were found between groups or stimulus categories. In contrast with the facial expression results, where happy is the best recognized expression, sad seems to be the most prominently recognized body emotion for both groups. In a follow-up experiment using images of day-to-day scenes with and without other people present, we investigated the role of the context in wh ich bodily expressions are encountered. When judging rhe emotion of bodily expressions shown in nonsocial contexts, we found that the Chinese were

general ly slower in judging the emotion of the tar· get body than the Dutch. However, because we had also found this for the previous match-to-sample tasks in general, we cannot say conclusively that ir is because of the context. Both groups were distracted

228 CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIONS AND BODY LANGUAGE

Page 7: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

by a fearful context when the body is neutral, lead­ing to more time taken tO respond.

In addition, bodily expressions in social contexts dm show other people engaging in activity with either the same or a contrasting emotional signifi­cance are more difficult to recognize by either group when the bodily expression is incongruent with the scene. All participants were influenced by an incongruent (happy or neutral) social scene show­ing fearful target bodies and also by a fearful social scene showing happy target bodies (as shown by the accuracy data). However, the Chinese did perform becter than the Dutch students in general, which was not the case when the bodies were presented in a nonsocial context or without conrexr. An explana­tion for this could be that the Chinese are in fact more influenced by the social scene, bur in a differ­em way: Because there are other people present in the scenes, they might feel some pressure to perform becter. We know that such unrealistic social cues can indeed have an influence on behavior (e.g., Bateson, Nettle, & Roberts, 2006), and because the Chinese students were raised in a collectivistic society, this could have a greater effect on them. One possible explanation of why the incongruent social scenes do nor distract could be the short stimulus duration of I 00 ms. A previous study showed that Japanese participants who had to judge a target body within a social scene looked more at the other people in the scene-but only after I s (Masuda et al., 2008). This finding, however, could nor be amibured tO

any ambiguity in the scenes: Both groups recog­nized the emotion expressed by the people in the scene equally well, and neither had any trouble rec­ognizing happy scenes.

In addition, Chinese panicipants seemed to be more specialized for faces compared to Dutch par­ticipants; they were more accurate in recognizing both Chinese and Caucasian idenrities, and they did this faster than idenrifying shoes or bodies. With regard to Caucasian emotions, Chinese par­ticipants needed more rime than their Dutch coun­terparts independenr of whether these emotions were expressed in the face or the body. They also needed more time to recognize (emotional) bod­ies in a nonsocial context- but not when bodies were perceived in a social context, in which case the Chinese were faster than the Dutch. Thus, although an incongruent scene similarly influenced both groups, a social scene did have more effect on the Chinese participants. One might speculate that this is related to the fact that traditional Chinese society is less individual istic than Western society.

Currently, however, these are rather broad and vague categorizations.

To add to the existing literature, a bodily expres­sion recognition experiment was performed in Japan and The Netherlands. Participants performed a sample-to-march task with angry, fearful, happy, and neutral expressions of Japanese and Dutch bodily expressions. Caucasian expressions were rec­ognized more accurately than Japanese stimuli by both Dutch and Japanese participants. lmerescingly, Japanese participants were better at recognizing fearful Japanese expressions than were the Dutch participants. However, it is important to note that Japanese angry and fearful bodily expressions yielded lower accuracy scores than those of Japanese happy and neutral expressions from both nation­ali ties. This has previously been found for Japanese facial expressions (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969). h can be argued that these stimuli may be less expressive than the Caucasian expressions because it is cultur­ally less accepted to express negative emotions in Japan. Further analyses on behavioral ratings of the stimuli should be taken into account.

Cultural Differences in Social Interactions Much research has focused on the one-sided

action of perceiving rhe face, or body, of another human. How does race influence the process of daily interaction? Participants of four differ­em nationalities (Irish, Guatemalan, Peruvian, and Serbian) were asked ro rate the intensity of social interactions between rwo English people. In this situation, an in-group effect was found; Irish panicipants cared the emotions as more intense than did people from other nationalities (Sneddon, McKeown, McRorie, & Vukicevic, 2011 ). Recalling the discussion of display rules, it has been found that Asians react less angrily than Americans in an anger-provoking situation with an experimenter (Mauss, Buder, RobertS, & Chu, 201 0), but they experience greater physiologi­cal responses in confrontational or intercultural social interactions (Buder, Lee, & Gross, 2009; Mauss & Buder, 2010).

Another important aspect of social interac­tion is the ability to understand other people not only by reading facial and bodily expressions but also by reading their gestures. Molnar-Szakacs, Wu, Robles, and Iacoboni (2007) measured cor­ticospinal excitability- a measure of motor reso­nance or, in other words, the responsiveness of the motor cortex- in response to actors of the in­and out-group playing out both culturally known

DE GELDER, HUIS IN 'T VELD

Page 8: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

and u nknown gestures. Inrerestingly, motor reso­nance was increased only in response ro in-group members, independent of the gestures. However, another study ind icated that in females, the oppo­site pattern can occur (Desy & lneoret, 2007). In line with these results, observing both the fam iliar and the unfamiliar gestures of in-group members also faci litates the mirror neuron networks and rhc insula (Liew, Han, & Aziz-Zadeh, 20 I I). In addition, conical nerworks involved in imitation, including rhe motor areas (Losin, lacoboni, Marrin, Cross, & Daprerro, 20 12), respond differently ro gestures performed by people of another race.

Conclusion Social interactions arc a large parr of da ily

human life, and in rhcsc interactions we use infor­mation from facial expressions, body posture, and rone of voice. Race and culrure are increasi ngly important aspects in th is regard, and they inAu­ence these processes that are at the heart of suc­cessful commun ication. In the literature reviewed , we found thar navigating in an environment with many people of other nat ionalities might inAuence one's ability ro remember the ir faces, interpret their emotions and gestures, or even misunderstand their gaze. TI1e re are many situations in da ily life in which this can have detrimental effects. For example, in docror-patienr relationships, it has been found

that race may interrupt effective doctor-patient communication or impede the rrust that patients have in their doctor due w m isinterpretations of nonverbal behavior (Levine & Ambady, 20 13). Furthermore, people were willing ro listen more ofren to a compmcr avarar giving rhcm advice when rhc avarar was a member of rhcir in-group (Pratt, Hauser, Ugray, & Patterson, 2007), which can severely impact adherence to medication or rrcar­menr in a medical scrring. It is reasonable ro assume rhar this may also play a role in other real-life inreracrions-for example, between reachers and srudcnrs, doctors and patients, therapists and cli­cnrs, and pol ice officers and rhc public . If these kinds of processes, essential for commun ication, are inAuenced by race or culture, the same might be true for the perception of body language in day-tO­day interaction. To make rhe comparison with daily life, this might inAuence the effectiveness of police officers or firefighters working in a multicultural envi ronment, requiring asture responses from oth­ers in a high-st ress situation.

Studies on display rules have consistently found that some cultures are more likely to

suppress rheir emotions, especially negative ones, in social interact ions. This again is something thar should be raken into account in imporram inte rracial interactions. For example, to rake a therapist-cl ient pe rspective, rhc role of depression on emotional expression is vastly different for peo­ple from d ifferenr culwres (Chenrsova-Durron, Tsai, & Gotlib, 20 10; Su, Lee, & Oishi, 20 13). On a more positive note, it has been suggesred that dc rrimcnral effects of emotion suppression arc also dependent on culture, where rhe negati ve effects are less for people with Asian values who are more likely ro suppress their emotion (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007).

More diverse stud ies with different popula­tions are necessary. For example, fM RI studies are predominantly done on Caucasian populations. even though people from other races may have a slightly different brain structure (Bai er al.. 20 12; lsamah et al. , 20 I 0). Some studies have examined the previously reviewed race effects in cl inical populations, such as those wirh autism (Hi rschfeld , Bartmess, White, & Frith, 2007) or schizophren ia (Pinkham et al., 2008), finding rhat these syndromes do nor inrcracr wirh racial biases. However, it was found rhar children wirh W ill iams syndrome do nor have racial stereotypes (Santos. Meyer-Lindcnberg, & Dcruclle, 2010) and thar emotion perception may be differentially affected in different populations with schizophrenia (Brekke. Nakagami, Kee, & Green, 2005; Leppanen er al., 2006).

References Adams. R. 13 .. Franklin, R. G .. Rule, N . 0 ., Freeman, J. ll ..

Kvcraga, K., l ladjikhani, N., ct al. (20 10). C ulture, gaze and

the neural processing of fear expressions. Social Cognitiur and Aff~rtive Nrurosdma, 5, 340-348. doi: I 0 . 1 093/scan/nsp04;

Adams, R. B., Pau ker, K.. & Weisbuch, M. (20 10). Looki ng

the other way: ' I he role of gaze di rection in tht· cross-r:lCr

memory effect. journal of Experimental Social l'sycbology, 46. 478-48 1. doi: I 0. 10 16/j.jesp.2009.12.0 16

Adams, R. B., Rule, N. 0., Frankl in, R. G ., Wang, E., Stevenson.

M. T.. Yoshikawa, S., e t al. (20 1 0). Cross-culrural reading thr

mind in the eyes: AniMRl investigation. journal ofCognim·t Nmrosrirncr. 22. 97- 108. doi: I 0.1162/jocn.2009.2 11 87

Akechi, H., Scnju, A., Uibo, H. , Kikuchi, Y., Hasegaw·J.

T, & Hieranen, J. K. (20 13). Arrention ro eye contact in the West and East: Autonomic responses and evaluatiw

ratings. 1'/..QS ONE, 8(3), c59312. doi:IO.I371/journal.

pone.0059312 Bai, J., Abdul-Rahman, M. F.. R.ilkin -Grabo i, A., Chong, Y. S ..

Kwck, K., Saw, S. M., er a l. (2012). Population differenct~

in brain morphology and microstructure among Chinese. Malay, and Ind ian neonates. P/..QS ONe·, 7(10), c478 16.

doi: I 0.137 1/journal.ponc.0047816

230 CULTU RA l. D I FFERENCES IN EMOTIONAL EX P RESS I ONS AND BODY LANGUAGE

Page 9: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

1\:ncson, M .. Neule, D .. & Roberts, C. (200(,). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world serring. Biowgy

Lmm, 2, 412-4 14. doi: I 0. 1 098/rsbl.2006.0509

lk:mpre. M. G., & Hess, U. (2005) . Cross-cul rural emo-

tion recogn ition amo ng Canadian

.ft'nmutl of Cross-Culwml l'syrholof.Y.

doi:l 0 11 77/0022022 104273656

ethnic groups .

36, 355-370.

lliJis, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers. C., Fiset, D .. & Caldara. R.

(2008). C ulture shapes how we look at f.1ccs. 1'/.oS ONE,

3(8), c3022. doi: I 0. 137 1/Journai.Pone.0003022 1\ond, M. H. (1993). Emotions and thei r expression in C hin<·st·

culture . .Journal of Nonverbal &havior, 17, 245-262.

doi: 10.1 007/BRJ0987240

1\r&ke, J. S .. Nak:ogami, E .. Kce, K. S., & Green. M. F. (2005).

Cross-ethnic differcnct-s in percept ion of emotion in schizo­

ph renia. Schiwplmnitt Rmarrh, 77. 289-298. doi: 10.10 16/j.

sclucs.2005.04.004 llukach. C. M .. Cottle. J .. Ubiwa. J .. & Miller, J. (20 12) .

lndi,•iduarion ~-x.per ience predicts other-race effecL~ in holistic

processing for both Caucasian and Black parricipams. Cognition.

123, 3 19-324. doi: 10. 10 16/j.cognition.201 2.02.007 Butler, E. A., Lee. ·1: L., & Gross. J. J . (2007) . Emotion regu­

lation and culture: Arc the social consequences of emo­

tion suppression culrure-specific? Emotion. 7. 30-48.

doi: 10. 103711528-3542.7.1 .30 llurler, E. A. , Lee, T. l.., & G ross, J. J. (2009). Docs cxprc>S­

ing your emotions raise or lower your blood pressure' 'J he answer depends on cultural conrext . .foumal ofCross-Cit!wml

Psychology, 40,5 10-5 17. doi: l0.1 177/0022022 109332845 Caloarel, S., Monralan, B., Fromager. E., Bernard, C., Lalonde.

R., & Mohamed, R. (2011) . O ther-ract· and inversion effects

during the structu ral encoding stage of f.,ce processing in

a race catt·gorization rask: An event-relared bra in porenrial

study. lnumationrtl journal of l'sycbopbysioi<J.(Q', 79. 26~271. doi: I 0. 10 16/j .ijpsycho.20 10. 10.0 18

Caldara, R., Rossion. B., Bovet, 1~. & Hatten, C. A. (2004).

Evem-related poremials and time course of the "other-race"

face da.-sificarion advantage. NmroReport, 15, 905-91 0.

doi: I 0.1097/0 l.wnr.OOOO 119729.93S64.3e Caldara, R. , Thur, G .. Servoir, 1~ . Michel. C. M., Bovet.

1~ . & Renault, B. (2003). l'ace versus non-f.1ce object per­

ception and the "other-race" effect: A spario-tempora l

evenr-related potential study. Clinical Neuropbysiology. 114.

5 15-528. doi: I 0. 1 016/S 1388-2457(02)00407-8

Chcntsova-Outton. Y. E., Tsai. J. L.. & Gorlib. l. H. (2010) . Further evidence for the cultural norm hypothesis: Positive

emotion in depr<'sS<·d and control European American

and A>ia n American women. Cit!tural Diversity & Ethnic

Minority Psychology, /6 , 284-295.doi: I 0.1037/ AOO 17562

Chiao, J. Y., lidaka, T. Gordon, H . L.. Nogawa, J ., Bar. M .. Aminoff. E. , er al. (2008). Cultural specificiry in amygdala

response ro ft-ar faces . .Journal of Cognitivr NeuTI>sdmre, 20,

2167-2 174. doi: I 0. 1162/jocn.2008.20 15 1

Chung, J. M. (2012) . The conrrihurion of self-deceptive

enhancement to d isplay rules in the United States and

Japan. Asian .Journal of Soria/ Psyrbology. 15, 69- 75. doi:IO. Il llfj. I467-839X.2011.01358.x

dt Gelder. B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotion al

body language. Nature Ret~ini!S Nmrosrima, 7, 242-249.

doi: I 0.1 038/Nrn 1872 de Gelder, B., & Bertelson, P. (2003) . Multisensory integra­

tion. perception and ecological val idity. Trends in Cognitit•e

Srimm. 7. 460-467. doi: I 0. 1016/j.tics.200.3.08.014

de Gelder, B., Boeker, K. B. E., Tuomainen, J. , Hensen,

M., & Vroomen, J. (1999). 1l1e combined perception of emotion from voice and fuce: Early interaction revealed by

human electric brain responses. Nmroscima Inters, 260,

133-136. doi: l 0. I 016/50304-3940(98)00963-X

de G elder. B .. Snyder, J., G reve, 0., C emrd. G .. & Hadjikhani,

N. (2004). ~car fosters night : A mechanism for fear conta­

gion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of thr National Academy ofSrienm qf rhr USA.

101, 16701-16706. doi: l0.1 073/pna;.0407042101

de Gelder. B., Va n den Srock, J. , Meeren. H. K .. Sinke, C. ~ .. Kret, M. E .. & Tamierro, M. (20 10). Standing up for th<·

body: Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in

the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Nrurosrirnrr

and Biobrhavioml Revirws. 34. 5 13-527. doi: I 0.1 0 I 6/j.

neubiorev.2009. 1 0.008 de Gelder, B .. & Vroom en, J. (2000) . ·1 he perception ol

emotions by ear and by C)'e. cognition & Emotion, 14.

289-31 1. Ucrntl. B.. Habel. U., Robinson. S., Windischberger. C ..

Kryspin-Exner, 1. , G ur, R. C., et a l. (2009). Amygdala acti­

vation during recognition of emotions in a foreign ethnic

group is associated wirh duration of stay. Social Neurosrimcr,

4. 294-307. doi: I 0.1080/ 174709 1080257 1633

Dcsy, M. C .. & Theoret, H. (2007). Modulation of motor

cortex cxcitabil iry by physical similarity with an observed hand act ion. 1'/.oS ONE, 2( 10). c971. doi: 10.1:371/journal.

pone.0000971 Eimer, M. (1998). Does the face-specific Nl 70 component

rcAect the acrivity of a specialized eye processor? NruroRrpon.

9. 2945-2948. doi: I 0.1097/00001756-199809 140-00005 Ekman. I~ ( 1972). Universals and culwral diAcrcnces in tacial

expressions of emotion. In J . Cole (Ed.), Nebrmlut symposium

on motivation. l.incoln, N E: Universiry of Nebraska Press.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V (1978). Facia/Action CodingSysmn (FACS}: A uclmique for thr measurrmmt offorial action. l'alo

Alto, CA: Consu lting Psychologists l'rcss.

Ekman. 1~ . & hiesen, W. V. (2003) . Unmasking rbe jita: A guide

ro recognizing emotions from jitcial clues. Los Altos,

CA: Malor Book~.

Elfenbein, H . A. (20 13). Nonverbal dialects and accents in

facial expressions of emoti on. Emotion R1•view, 5, 90-96.

doi: I 0.11 77/1754073912451332

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). O n the uni­

versality and cult ura l specificiry of emotion recogni­tion: A meta-analysis. f',ycbologiralllulktin. 128. 203-2.35.

doi: I 0. 1037//0033-2909.128.2.203 Elfenbein. H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003) . When fami liariry

breeds accuracy: C ultural expo;ure and facial emot ion rec­

ognition. jounwl of l'rrsOIIfllity and Social Psyrbolog_y. 85. 276-290. doi : I 0.1 037/0022-3514.85.2.276

Elfcnbein, H. A., Beaupre, M .. Levesque, M .. & HC»s, U. (2007) .

' loward a dialect theory: Cultural diAercnces in rhe expres­

sion and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion. 7.

1.31- 146. doi:IO. I 037/ 1528-3542.7.1. 13 1 Elfcnbein, H. A., Mandai, M. K .. Ambady, N .. l lariwka,

S., & Kumar, S. (2004). Hemifacial differences in the in-group

advantage in emotion recognition. Cognition & Emotion. 18,

6 13-629. doi: I 0. l 080/0269993034 1000257

Farah. M. J ., T:111ab, J . W., & Drain, H. M. (1995). What

causes rhe face inversion effecr. .Journal of Experimrmal Psychology: Humrtll Perception and Pnformanrr. 21, 628- 634.

doi: I 0.1037//0096- 1523.21 . .3.628

DE GELDE R, H U I S IN 'T VELD 231

Page 10: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

Friesen. W. V. ( 1972}. Culruml diffn-mrn in fonal o:pmriom in a sonal nruation: An o:ptrimnrral rm ofrlx con«pt of display ruin. Doctoral disscrution, University of California, San Franciscx>.

Caj~ski, P. D .• Sclllegd, K., & Sroerig, P. (2008}. Efftt:ts of human race and face inversion on the N 170: A cro~-race study. jo11rnal of PIJChopbpio/ogy, 22. 157-165. doi: I 0.1027/0269-8803.22.4. 157

Hahn, A. C .. Janru:n, K. J., & Symons, L. A. (2012). Thatchcrization impacts the proce.~sing of own-race f:1ces more so than other-race faces: An ERP study. Socitrl Nturoscima, 7. 11 3-125. doi:IO.I0801174709 19.20 11 .583080

1-lejmadi , A., Davidson, R. J., & Ro1.in , I~ (2000). Exploring Hindu Indian emotion expressions: Evidence for accurate recognition by Americans and Indians. Psyr-bolqgical Scimu, II, 183-187. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00239

llcrrmann. M. J ., Schreppel. T .. Jager. D .• Koehler, S., Ehlis, A. C .. & Fallgatter, A. J. (2007}. The other-race effect for face perception: An event-related potential study. journal of Nmral Trammission, 114, 95 1-957. doi:IO.I007/ ~00702-007 -0624-9

He~. U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). The inAuencc of fucial emotion displays. gender, and ethnicity on judgmen ts of dominance and affi liation. jo11rnal of Nonvtrbal Btbavior, 24,265-283. doi:I0.1023/A:I0066232 13355

Himman. 1.., & Kdly, S. D. (20 13). Effects of emotional

body language on rapid out-group judgments. journal of Exptrimmral Social Prycbology. 49, 152- ISS. doi:IO.IOI6/j. jesp.2012.07.010

Hirschfdd, L.. llaruness, E., White, S .. & Frith, U. (2007}. Can autistic children predict behavior by social stereo­types? Curmu Biology. 17, R4SI - R452. doi: IO.IOI6/j. cub.2007.04.051

lsamah, N .. Fai~on. W., Payne, M. E., MacFall. J., Steffens. D . C .. Beyer, J. 1.., er al. (20 I 0). Variabil ity in fromorempo­ral brain structure: The importance of recruitment of African Americans in neuroscience research. PLoS ON£, 5( I 0), c13642. doi: 10.13711journal.pone.OOI3642

Ishii, K., Reyes, J. A., & Kirayama, S. (2003). Spontaneous attention to word content versus emotional tone: Differences among three cultures. PI]Cbolqgiral Somu. 14, 39-46. doi:IO.IIlll1467-9280.01416

Jack. R. E .. Blais. C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G .. & Caldara, A. (2009). C ultural confusions show rhar f.1cial expr~ions are nor universal. Curmu Biology. 19, 1543 1548.

Jack. R. E., Caldara, R .• & Schyns. P. G. (2012). Internal rcpre~cntarions reveal cultural diversity in expectations of facial cxprcs~ions of emotion. journal of Expuimmtal Prybowgy: Cmtral, 141, 19-25. doi:IO.I037/A0023463

Jack. R. E., Garrod, 0. C. B., Yu. II .. Gldara, R., & Schyns. P. C. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are nor culrurally universal. Proc~~dinp of tbt Nmional Aradmry of Scimm of rh~ USA. 109, 7241-7244.

Kang, S. M., & Lau, A. S. (2013}. Revisiting the out-group advantage in emotion recognition in a multicultural soci­ery: Further evidence for the in-group advantage. Einorum, 13 . 203-215. doi:IO. I037/A00300 13

Kleinsmith, A., De Silva, P. R., & Bianchi-Bcrthouzc, N. (2006). Cross-cultural differences in recognizing affect from body posture. lnttracring wirh Compurm. 18, 137 1-1389. doi: I 0. 10 16/j.intcom.2006.04.003

Kramer, K., Bente, G.. Luo, S. Y., Pfeiffer, U. J.. Han, S. H., & Vogdcy, K. (2013}. lnAuence of ethnic

group-membership and gaze direction on the perct"ptiun of emotions: A cro~-culrural srudy berween Germany and China. PloS ONE, 8(6), e66335. doi:IO.I3711jourrul.

pone.006633S Krumhuber. E. G .. & Manstead, A. S. R. (2011). When mcm·

ory is bener for our-group faces: On negative emotions Jnd gender roles. jo11rnal of Nonvn-bal Bthavior, 35, 51..(>1. doi: I 0.1 007/s 109 19-010-0096-8

Leppanen, J. M., Niehaus, D. J. H .. Koen, 1.., Du Toil. E .. Schocman, R .. & Emsley, R. (2006}. Emorional face pro· = ing deficit in ~chizophrenia: A replication study in a Sourh African Xhosa population. Schizopbrmia Rtttarrh. X4.

323-330. doi: I 0.10 16/j.schres.2006.02.007 Levine, C. S., & Ambady, N. (2013}. The role of non·,·mi

behaviour in racial disparities in health care: lmplicarionsanJ solutions. Mrdirnl Education, 47, 867-876. doi:IO.IIII• Medu.l2216

Liew, S. L., Han, S. H., & Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2011). FamiliJrit~

modulates mirror neuron and mcntalizing regions dur· ing in tention understandi ng. Huma11 Brain Mapping. }!.

19116- 1997. doi : I 0.1 002/Hbm.2 11 (>4 Lindsay. D. S .. Jack, I~ C., & Christian, M.A. ( 1991}. Othcr-rJ(f

face perception. journal of Applitd Psycbowgy. 76, 587-589 doi: 10.1037110021-90 I 0.76.4.587

Losin, E. A. R.. lacoboni, M., Manin, A., C"'"· K. A., & Daprctto, M. (2012}. Race modulates ncuul

activity during imitation. N~urolmngt, 59. 3594-360.t doi: I 0. I 0 16/j.ncuroimage.20 11.10.074

Malpass. R. S., & Kravitz. J. (1969}. Recognition for faces ofc>~<n and or her race. journal of Pmonality and Soriall'rydiOIIIfl. 13. 330-334.

Mandai, M. K .. llariwka, S., Bhushan, B .. & Mishra, R. C. (200 I). Cultural variation in hemif.•cial asymmetry of emu·

tion expressions. /Jritisb journal of Social l'syrhology. 40. 385- 398. doi: I 0.1348/0 1446660 1164885

Massaro, D. W., & Egan, I~ B. (1996). Perceiving alfc:ct rwm

the voice and the face. PIJCbonomir Bulkrin & Rrvrtu-. }. 215-221. doi: I 0.3758/Bf03212421

Masuda, T., FJisworth. I~ C., Mesquit2, B., Leu, J., lanicb. S., & de Veerdonk, E. V. (2008}. Placing the face in wn· rexr: Cultural differences in rhe perception of facial emotion journal of Pmonaltty and Social Prychowgy. 94. 36S-.181 doi: I 0.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365

Matsumoto, D., Takeuchi. S., Andayani, S., Kouzncuo"~·

N., & Krupp. D. ( 1998). ll1e contribution ,,f individualism-coll ectivism to cross-na tional diffcrcmc' in display rules. Asian journal of Social l'syrbolo,rzy. !. 147- 165.

Matsumoto, D .. Yoo. S. H ., Hirayama, S., & Perrova, G. (200)). Development and v•lidation of a measure of display rule knowledge: ' I he display rule ~mem inventory. /;mori••· 5, 23-40. doi: I 0.103711528-3542.5.1.23

Ma~. I. B., & Butler, E. A. (2010). Cultural context mod· erares the relationship between emotion control 1alu<1 and cardiovascular challenge versus threat respt>n><"'· Biolqgical Psyr-hology, 84, 521-530. doi: IO.IOIMj. biopsycho. 2009.09.0 I 0

Mauss, I. B., Bu tl er, E. A., Roberts, N. A .. & Chu. A. (20 10). Emotion control values and responding ro an ang" provocation in Asian-American a.nd European-Amt'rk•n individuals. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 1026- 104.1. doi: I 0.1080/02699930903 122273

232 CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIONS AND BODY LANGUAGE

Page 11: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (200 I). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces-A mera-analytic review. l'sydJowgy Public l'olicy and Law, 7, 3-35. doi: I 0.1037//1076-8971.7.1.3

Michel, C., Caldara, R., & Rossion, B. (2006). Same-race faces arc perceived more holistically than other-race faces. Vimal Cognition, 14. 5)-73. doi: I 0.1080/13506280500158761

Michel, C., Corneille, 0., & Rossion, B. (2010). Holisric face encoding is modulated by perceived face race: Evidence from perceprual adaptation. Visual Cognition, 18, 434-455. doi: 10. 1080/135062809028 19697

Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, C. S., & Caldara, R. (2006) . Hol istic processing is finely tuned for faces of one's own race. Psycbologicnl Science, 17, 608-615. doi: I 0.1111/j.l467 -9280.2006.0 1752.x

MiyamotO, Y. (20 13). Cultu re and analytic versus hol istic cog­nirion: Toward multilevel analyses of cultural inAuences. Advanw in Expn-immtal Social l'sycbowgy, 47, 131-188. doi: I 0. 10 16/B978-0-12-407236-7.00003-6

Molnar-S7.akacs, L, Wu, A. D., Robles, E J., & lacoboni, M. (2007) . Do you sec what I mean? Corticospina l excit.~bility du ring observation of culture-specific gestures. !'LoS ONE, 2(7), e626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000626

~·londloch , C. J., Elms, N., Maurer, D., Rhodes, G., Hayward, W. G .. Tanaka, J. W., et al. (20 I 0). Processes underlying the cross-race effect: An investigation of holistic, feat ural, and relational processing of own- race versus orher-race faces. Prruption, 39, I 065- 1085. doi: I 0.1 068/1'6608

Montalan, B., Veujoz, M., Boirout, A., Leleu, A .. Camus, 0., L'llondc, R., ct al. (20 13). Investigation of effects of face rota­tion on race processing: An ERPs study. Brnin and Cognition, 81.360-369. doi: IO. IOI61j.bandc.20 12.12.004

Moriguchi , Y. , Ohnishi, T , Kawachi, R. T., Mori, ·1:. Hirakata. M., Yamada, M .• et al. (2005). Specific brain activation in Japanese and Caucasian people to fearful Gees. NmroReport, 16, 133- 136. doi: 10. 1097100001756-200502080-00012

Murata, A., Moser, J. S., & Kit.1y:1ma, S. (2013). C ulture shapes electroconical responses during emotion suppres­sion. Socittl Cognitivr 11nd Afficrivr Nruroscintcr, 8. 595- 60 I. doi: I 0.1 0931Scan/Nss036

Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2013) . Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5, 8- 15. doi: 10.1 17711 754073912457227

OToole, A. J. , Deffenbacher, K. A .. Valentin, D., & Abdi, H. (1994). Structural aspects of face recognition and the other-race effect. Mmrory & Cognition, 22, 208-224. doi: 1 0.3758/Bf03208892

Pavan, G., Dalmaso, M., Galfano, G. , & Castelli, L.. (2011) .

Racial group membership is associated ro gaze-mediated orienting in Italy. !'LoS ONE, 6( 1 0). e25G08. doi: I 0. 1.}7 1 I journal.ponc.0025608

Pinkham, A. E., Sasson, N. J. , C.1lki ns, M. E., Richard, J., Hughett, r~. C ur, R. E., et al. (2008) . 1l1e other-race effect in f:tce processing among African American and Caucasian individuals wirh schizophren ia. Amrrican }ormurl of l'sycbiatry, 165, 639-645. doi: 10. 11761appi. ajp.2007.07 1 01604

Pollack, A. ( 1996. August 12). Happy in the East (/\_/\)or smil­ing in the West:-). The New York Times, D5.

l'rnn, J. A., Hauser, K., Ugray, Z .. & Patterson, 0. (2007). Looking at human-computer interface design: Effects of ethnicity in compmer agems.lmrracting tvith Computers, 19. 512- 523. doi: I 0.10 161j.intcom.2007.02.003

Rhodes, G., Ewing, L., Hayward, W. G., Maurer, D .. Mondloch , C. J .. & Tanaka, J. W. (2009). Contact and other-race effects in configura) and component process­ing of faces. BritiJb jourr111l of Psycbowgy, 100, 7 17-728. doi: I 0.13481000712608x396503

Richeson, ] . A. , Todd, A. R., Trawalter, S .. & Baird, A. A. (2008).

Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activ­ity. Croup l'roctsus & intergroup Relations, II, 233- 246. doi: 10. 1177/ 1368430207088040

Safdar, S., Friedl meier, W., Marsumoto. D. , Yoo, S. H., Kwamcs, C. T., Kakai, H. , et al. (2009). Variations of emotional dis­play rules within and across cul rures: A comparison between C.1nada, USA, and Japan. Canadian journal of Brhaviourtrl Scimce, 41, 1- 10. doi:I0. 10371A0014387

SantOS, A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., & Deruelle, C. (20 10). Absence of racial, but not gender, stereotyping in Williams syndrome children. Currrnr Biology, 20. R307- R308. doi: 10. 10 161j.cub.20 I 0.02.009

Scherer, K. R., C lark-Polner, E., & Monillaro, M. (20 11). In the eye of the beholder? Un iversality and cultural specificity in the expreSc~ion and perception of emotion . lmrmati01111l journal of Ps)•cbology, 46. 401-435. doi: I 0.1080100207594 .20 I 1.626049

Scherer, K. R .. Matsumoto, D., Wal lbou, H. G., & Kudoh, T. ( 1988). Emotional experience in cultu ral context: A com­parison berween Europe, Japan, and the USA. In K. R. Scherer (Eel.), Farm of emotion: Ream rtuarcb (pp. 5- 30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sckiyarna, K., & Tohkura. Y. (1991). Mcgurk effect in non-English listeners-Few visual effects for Japanese sub­jects hearing Japanese syl lables of high auditory intell igibility. journal oftbt Acoustical Sudety of America, 90, 1797-1805. doi: I 0.1121 I 1.40 1660

Shepherd, S. V. (20 I 0) . Following gaze: Gaze-following behav­ior as a window into social cognition. FromitN in lnugmriv~ Neuroscima, 4. 5. doi: I 0.3389/fnint.20 I 0.00005

Sinke, C. B. A. (20 12). Perc~ption ofmrotions from fom and bod­i~s and thr inf/urncr ofcomext. Unpublished doctoral disserta­tion, Tilburg University, Ti l burg, 1l1e Nerherlands.

Sneddon, L, McKeown, G., McRorie, M. , & Vukicevic, T. (201 1). Cross-culmral patterns in dynamic ratings of posi­tive and negative natural emotional behaviour. !'LoS ONE. 6(2). e 14679. doi: I 0. 137 1/jou rnal.pone.OO 14679

Sogon, S., & Masutani, M. ( 1989). Identification of emotion from body movements-A cross-cultural study of Americans and Japanese. Psychological Reports, 65, 35-46.

Stah l, J .. Wiese. II., & Schwcinbcrger, S. R. (2008). Expenise and own-race bias in face processing: An event-related poten­tial study. NmroReport, 19, 583-587.

Su, J. C.. Lee, R. M., & Oishi, S. (2013). ·n,e role of culture and sel f-construal in the link between expressive suppression and depres.~i vc symptoms. journal of Cross-Cultuml Psy<hology. 44, 316-331. doi: 10. 1177100220221 124434 13

Tanaka, A., Koizumi, A., lmai, H. , Hiramatsu, S., Hiramoto, E .. & de Gelder, B. (20 I 0). I feel your voice: Cultural differ­ences in the multisensory perception of emotion. Psycbowgiml Scimce, 21, 1259-1262. doi: 10.11 77/0956797610380698

"Emaka, J. W., & l'arah, M. J. (1993). Pans and wholes in f,cc recognition. Quarterly journal of £xperimemal fl>ychology. 46, 225- 215.

T.1naka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2004). A holistic account of Lhc own-race effect in face recognition: Evidt·nce

DE GELDER, H U IS I N 'T VELD 233

Page 12: C APTER 15 H Expressions and Body Language Cultural ... · C15 HAPTER Cultural Differences in Emotional Expressions and Body Language Beatrice de Gelder and Elisabeth Huis in 't Veld

from a cro~·culrural study. GJf.mtion. 9]. B 1-B9.

doi: 10.1 016/j.cognition.1003.09.011

lanab. J. W .. & Pierce, L. ). (1009). lhc neural pl.lSii~ity of other-race fuce recognition. Cognilll'<' Affirllt'<' & JJ~havioral N~urommu. 9. 122- 131 . doi: I 0.3758/Cahn. ?.1.122

' lumincllo. f. R .. & Davidson, D. (10 11). What the: face and

body rcvt-al: In-group emotion effects and MCfl'Otyping

of' emotion in African American and European American

children. }oumnl of Experimrmnl Child l~ychology. 110. 258-274. doi: 10. 1016/j.jccp.20 11.02.016

Vitioli. 1 ... l;orc:man. K. , Rousselct, G . A., & Caldara, R. (2009) .

Inverting face> elicits sensiriviry to race on the N 170 com­

ponent: A cross-<:ulrural stud)'· journal of Visto/1. 10. IS. doi:IO. I167/IO.l.IS

Walker. I~ M .. SiJ,·cn, L .. H<.w>wnc, M., & Nobre. A. C. (2008).

Soc:ial cuntdct .1nd other-race face processing in the human

brain. Sol'i1d Cog11iriw 1111d Ajj~clll't' Nrurommu. J. 16-25. doi: 10.1 093/Scan/Nsm035

Walker. I~ M .. & ' J3mka. J. W. (2003). i\n cnmding advan­

tage for uwn-mcc versus other-race f~cc,. l'rrrrption, 32. I 11 7- 11 25. doi: 10.1 068/1'5098

Wiese. H .. Stahl, J .. & Schwcinberger, S. R. (200?). Configut.ol

proccs.\ing of other-race 13ccs is delayed but not dccr.:a"-.1.

Biologrrnl l~yhology. 81, 103-109. doi:IO.IOih

biopsycho.2009.03.002 Yin. R. K. ( 1969). Looking at upside-down rn<.t">. joumal DJ

F.>:paimnunl Psychology. 81. 14 1- 145.

Young, S. G., llugcnbcrg. K., Bernstein, M. J .. & )Jcco.

D . E (20 12). Perception and mot ivation in f.tcc rc<o~nl· tion: A critica l review of' theories of the cross- race: dku.

Prrsonnlity and Social Psycbowgy Revinv. 16. 116- 14!

doi: IO. I 177/ 1088868.H 14 18987

Yuki , M .. Madd tn, W. W., & Masuda , T. (2007). Arc thr \\in·

dows to th~ w ul the same in the East and West~ Cuh ur~

difference> in Uling the eyes and mouth as CLI<'S to rcu,:· nite emotion~ in Japan and the Unit<."tl Statt-... jounul 'I E·qurimnua/ Sortal l'rychology, 43, 303-311. dni: 10.101611

jesp.2006.01.004 Zhu, Z. Y .. Ho, ~. M. Y .. & Bonanno. G. A. (20U). Cuhur.J

similaritic-.. and difrcrences in the perception of cmntinn.J

valence and inten>ity: A comparison of American< and llm1F Kong Chinese. Amrrimnjoumnl ofl'sycbolngy, 116, 261 -l".l.

234 CULTU RAL DIFFEREN CES IN EMOT I ONAL EXPRESSIONS AND BODY LANGUAGE