can students collaboratively use hypermedia to …mvhs.shodor.org/riverweb/azevedo2004.pdf · while...
Embed Size (px)
TRANSCRIPT

J. EDUCATIONAL COMPUTING RESEARCH, Vol. 31(3) 215-245, 2004
CAN STUDENTS COLLABORATIVELY USE HYPERMEDIA
TO LEARN SCIENCE? THE DYNAMICS OF SELF-
AND OTHER-REGULATORY PROCESSES
IN AN ECOLOGY CLASSROOM*
ROGER AZEVEDO
FIELDING I. WINTERS
DANIEL C. MOOS
University of Maryland
ABSTRACT
This classroom study examined the role of low-achieving students’ self-
regulated learning (SRL) behaviors and their teacher’s scaffolding of SRL
while using a Web-based water quality simulation environment to learn about
ecological systems. Forty-nine 11th and 12th grade students learned about
ecology and the effects of land use on water quality by collaboratively
using the RiverWebSM water quality simulation (WQS) during a two-week
curriculum on environmental science. The students’ emerging understanding
was assessed using pretest and posttest scores. Students’ self-regulatory
behaviors and teacher’s scaffolding of SRL were assessed through an analysis
of their discourse during several collaborative problem-solving episodes.
Findings indicate that students learned significantly more about ecology
after working collaboratively with the WQS. However, these learning
gains were quite small and were related to the self-regulatory behaviors
observed in the dyads and their teacher’s scaffolding and instruction.
Analyses of video data indicate that a large amount of time was spent by the
dyads and teacher in using only a few strategies, while very little time was
spent on planning, monitoring, and handling task difficulties and demands.
Further analysis revealed that both the dyads and teacher were using low-level
*This research was supported by funding from the National Science Foundation (REC#0133346)
awarded to the first author.
215
� 2004, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc.

strategies (e.g., following procedural tasks, evaluating the content, searching,
and selecting informational sources in the WQS) to learn about the topic. Our
results provide a valuable initial characterization of the complexity of self-
and other-regulated learning in a complex, dynamic, technology-enhanced,
student-centered science classroom. We discuss how the results will be used
to inform the design of computers as MetaCognitive tools designed to foster
students’ learning of conceptually challenging science topics.
INTRODUCTION
Educational researchers have successfully used student-centered methods to
enhance students’ understanding of science with computer-based learning
environments (CBLEs) (e.g., Azevedo, Verona, & Cromley, 2001; Biswas,
Schwartz, Bransford, & CTGV, 2001; Clark & Linn, 2003; Hickey, Kindfield,
Horowitz, & Christie, 2003; Kozma, Chin, Russell, & Marx, 2000; Lajoie &
Azevedo, 2000; Lajoie, Guerrera, Munsie, & Lavigne, 2001; Reiser et al., 2001;
Slotta & Linn, 2000; Suthers & Hundhausen, 2003; Vye et al., 1998; White,
Shimoda, & Frederiksen, 2000). These studies, however, also provide evidence
that students have difficulties regulating certain aspects of their learning when
they use CBLEs to learn about complex and challenging science topics such as the
circulatory system, Newtonian physics, and genetics (e.g., Hickey et al., 2003;
Jacobson & Archodidou, 2000). More specifically, research shows that students
have difficulties regulating several aspects of their learning, including their
cognitive resources (e.g., activating prior knowledge), motivation and affect
(e.g., task value, self-efficacy, interest in the topic), behavior (e.g., engaging in
help-seeking behavior), and the instructional context (e.g., negotiating task struc-
ture and dealing with dynamically changing structure of the instructional context).
While the majority of these studies have examined specific aspects of students’
self-regulated learning (SRL) such as planning, metacognitive monitoring, and
reflection in isolation (e.g., Lajoie et al., 2001), most have not examined the
complex nature of the dynamics between the phases of SRL and how this com-
plexity may affect how students regulate their learning of science topics in
student-centered classrooms using CBLEs.
It is therefore critical that we understand how students working collaboratively
regulate their own learning as well as how teachers facilitate students’ SRL
by using different scaffolding and instructional approaches in student-centered,
technology-rich science classrooms. To that end, we have adopted and extended
existing models of SRL (see Boekaerts, Pintrich, & Zeidner, 2000; Zimmerman
& Schunk, 2001 for extensive reviews) and have used them as a lens to examine
the complex interactions between students’ self-regulatory behavior and teachers’
scaffolding of their students’ SRL when using CBLEs collaboratively in the
science classroom for extended periods of time.
216 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

Theoretical Framework: Self-Regulated Learning
By adopting SRL as framework, we make certain theoretical and empirically-
based assumptions about learning. Self-regulated learners are active learners who
efficiently manage their own learning in many different ways (Boekaerts et al.,
2000; Butler, 1998; Paris & Paris, 2001; Perry, 2002; Winne, 2001; Winne &
Hadwin, 1998; Winne & Perry, 2000; Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001). Students
are self-regulating to the degree that they are cognitively, motivationally, and
behaviorally active participants in their learning process (Zimmerman, 2001).
These students generate the thoughts, feelings, and actions necessary to attain their
learning goals. Models of SRL describe a recursive cycle of cognitive activities
central to learning and knowledge construction activities (e.g., Pintrich, 2000;
Schunk, 2001; Winne, 2001; Winne & Hadwin, 1998; Zimmerman, 2001).
Several models of SRL posit that SRL is an active, constructive process
whereby learners set goals for their learning and then attempt to monitor, regulate,
and control their cognition, motivation, and behavior in the service of those goals
(see Pintrich, 2000; Zimmerman, 2001). SRL is guided and constrained by both
personal characteristics and the contextual features of the environment (Pintrich,
2000). Thus, these models offer a comprehensive framework with which to
examine how students learn and how they adapt during learning with hypermedia.
Recent research has begun to examine the role of students’ ability to regulate
several aspects of their cognition, motivation, and behavior during learning of
complex science topics with hypermedia in laboratory experiments and in tutoring
situations (e.g., Azevedo, Cromley, & Seibert, 2004; Azevedo, Guthrie, & Seibert,
2004). Thus far, this research has demonstrated that students have difficulties
benefiting from hypermedia environments because they fail to engage in key
mechanisms related to regulating their learning with hypermedia.
Self-Regulated Learning with CBLEs
In CBLEs such as Web-based hypermedia learning environments, students
are given access to a wide range of information represented as text, graphics,
animation, audio, and video, which is structured in a non-linear fashion (Jonassen,
1996). Learning in such an environment requires a learner to regulate his or
her learning; that is, to make decisions about what to learn, how to learn it, how
much to learn, how much time to spend on it, how to access other instructional
materials, determine whether he or she understands the material, when to abandon
and modify plans and strategies, and to increase effort (Shapiro & Niederhauser,
2004; Williams, 1996; Winne & Hadwin, 1998). Specifically, students need to
analyze the learning situation, set meaningful learning goals, determine which
strategies to use, assess whether the strategies are effective in meeting the learn-
ing goal, evaluate their emerging understanding of the topic, and determine
whether the learning strategy is effective for a given learning goal. They need
to monitor their understanding and modify their plans, goals, strategies, and
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 217

effort in relation to contextual conditions (e.g., cognitive, motivational, and task
conditions) (Azevedo, Cromley, & Seibert, 2004). Further, depending on the
learning task, they may need to reflect on the learning episode and modify their
existing understanding of the topic (Winne, 2001; Winne & Hadwin, 1998).
Therefore, despite the potential for fostering learning, hypermedia environments
are proving to be ineffective unless learners can regulate their learning. Being
able to regulate one’s own learning is therefore extremely important in enhancing
one’s ability to learn about complex topics with hypermedia. Researchers have
recently begun to identify several problems associated with learners’ inability
to regulate their learning with hypermedia (Azevedo, Cromley, Winters, Xu,
& Iny, 2003). The next step in this line of research is to extend these results
by focusing on whether students can collaboratively use Web-based hypermedia
learning environment to learn about the effects of land use on water quality
indicators in a student-centered classroom by focusing on the complex interactions
between students’ SRL.
Self-Regulated Learning with CBLEs in the
Classroom: Complex Contextual Interactions
The importance of the classroom context for students’ self-regulated has been
well-established (e.g., Butler, 1998; Meyer & Turner, 2002; Paris & Paris, 2001;
Perry, 2002; Perry & VandeKamp, Mercer, & Nordby, 2002). Current research
has focused on the importance of the classroom as a complex context within
which to study students’ SRL and the role of teachers (as external self-regulating
agents) who foster students’ understanding by using scaffolding or any other
instructional methods to support their learning (e.g., Rasku-Puttonen, Etelapelto,
Arvaja, & Hakkinen, 2003). However, what has not been investigated is the
complex dynamics between students’ SRL while working collaboratively and
using a CBLE to learn about a challenging science topic, and their teacher’s
scaffolding of their SRL in the classroom.
Meyer and Turner (2002) emphasized that in a classroom context there is a
shared responsibility among teachers and students for establishing and main-
taining relationships as they coordinate multiple goals through available oppor-
tunities and scaffolding (McCaslin & Hickey, 2001). Scaffolding is an instruc-
tional process in which the teacher supports students cognitively, motivationally,
and emotionally in learning while helping further develop their autonomy
(Meyer & Turner, 2002). During instructional scaffolding, the teacher supports the
students’ self-regulation, as needed, in three ways: a) helping students build
competence through increased understanding; b) engaging students in learning
while supporting their socioemotional needs; and c) helping students build and
exercise autonomy as learners. The interactions between a teacher and a class full
of students with varying needs and abilities to regulate their learning while
working collaboratively with a CBLE to learn about a challenging science topic
218 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

pose theoretical and methodological challenges which need to be addressed in
order to study whether students working collaboratively can use CBLE to learn
about challenging topics.
In this study, we address the question of whether academically low-achieving
students, who have failed several science classes in high school, can regulate their
learning of conceptually challenging science topics such as the effects of land use
on water quality in a student-centered technology-enhanced science classroom.
Our study contributes to a large body of literature on fostering students’ learning of
conceptually challenging science topics with CBLEs in student-centered class-
rooms. It examines low-achieving students’ SRL and the teacher’s scaffolding
of students’ SRL in a classroom setting. It converges product data (i.e., learning
gains) and process data (i.e., time and frequency of use of several SRL variables
used by students working collaboratively, as well as teacher scaffolding and
instructional moves). This classroom study was conducted to investigate the
following research questions: 1) Can low-achieving high school students effec-
tively use a water-quality simulation1 (WQS) collaboratively to learn about
water quality indicators and land use? 2) Do the students and teacher spend the
same amount of time using various regulatory behaviors to facilitate students’
conceptual understanding? and 3) Do the students and teacher differ in the
frequency of use of these self-regulatory behaviors to facilitate students’ con-
ceptual understanding?
METHOD
Participants
Forty-nine 11th and 12th grade high school students (22 girls and 27 boys) from
four ecology science classes volunteered to participate in this study. Ages ranged
from 16 to 18 years. The students came from diverse socioeconomic and racial
backgrounds. Forty-nine percent (n = 24) were African American, 41% (n = 20)
were Hispanic, 6% (n = 3) were Caucasian, and 4% (n = 2) were Asian. All of
these students were placed in an environmental science class after failing several
required science classes in high school. The students had limited exposure to
environmental science, and therefore low prior knowledge about it. In addition, we
observed that students lacked both general learning skills (e.g., comprehension
monitoring) and specific science-related skills (e.g., how to round numbers).
Data were collected in April and May 2003, one month prior to the end of
the school year.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 219
1 The original designers and developers of the RiverWeb WQS labeled it as a simulation-based
environment, however, we consider it to be a Web-based hypermedia learning environment since it
provides students access to multiple representations of scientific information in multiple formats
including text, and diagrams, graphics in a non-linear fashion.

Research Design
We used a one-group pretest-posttest design (49 students). Video and audio
data were collected while the students worked in groups of two using the
RiverWeb Water Quality Simulator (WQS) to learn about ecological systems.
We collected process data on dyads of students from two of four classes taught
by two environmental science teachers.
Instructional Context
In a typical high school environmental science course, a unit is devoted to
water quality and the impact of land use on the pollutants that find their way
into streams, rivers, and eventually, the ocean. In order for students to understand
the differential impact of agricultural, commercial, urban, suburban, manufac-
turing, and foresting/lumbering land uses on water quality, students need to have
some kind of experience with each. An ideal project for students in such an
environmental science course would be to investigate how water quality changes
across different locales; however, that would require field trips that are both
expensive and time intensive and therefore often infeasible. The WQS is designed
to provide students with a simulated field trip through a prototypical watershed.
The RiverWebSM Water Quality
Simulation (WQS) Environment
The WQS is a Web-based environment (http://mvhs1.mbhs.edu/riverweb/
index.html) developed at Maryland Virtual High School and linked to the
RiverWebSM Program originating from the National Center for Supercomputing
Applications (NCSA). Targeted at the grades 8 through 12 science and math
curriculum, the WQS depicts the effects of various land uses on water quality in
an archetypal watershed. Students access WQS monitoring stations to explore how
different land uses (i.e., a pristine forest, agricultural, lumbering forest, residential
area, commercial/industrial area, wetlands, and urban area) affect water quality.
Each water monitoring station allows students to test for physical and chemical
characteristics of the tributary (termed indicators), such as total flow and nitrogen
concentration. By limiting each sub-watershed to one land use, the effect of that
land use can be seen on the quality of the water that students “test” within its
boundaries. The cumulative effect of the combined land use determines the water
quality shown by the indicator values found at a common river outflow.
After the user logs in, a map of the archetypal watershed appears. Water quality
monitoring stations located throughout the watershed are depicted on the map.
The user may click on the map to investigate any sub-watershed using the WQS
graph window. After clicking on a station, the student is presented with a default
graph that displays the variation of nitrogen over time in the top window and
precipitation over time in the bottom window. However, other indicators may
220 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

be selected; for example, the student could compare different indicators at
the same station such as water temperature and toxins (as shown in Figure 1),
or could compare nitrogen concentration between two stations (e.g., residential
area vs. wetlands). In addition, by reducing the range of days for each graph,
the student can zoom in on a particular time period and investigate daily or
seasonal variations.
A tour, which may be selected at login, uses frames to combine the WQS with
instructions leading the user through most of the simulator’s capabilities. Students
can examine time series reports and scatterplots: using the time series graphs,
students are able to see the impact of seasonal changes on certain indicators;
using the scatterplots, students are able to discover correlations between variables,
such as the relationship between water temperature and dissolved oxygen. The
goal is to foster student understanding of the complex relationships among the
many land uses, indicators, and water quality in the watershed. To gain this
understanding, students are first given instruction in graph interpretation. They
then answer open-ended questions, using the WQS, which demand explanations
of concepts and relationships, and finally they construct concept maps.
Data Sources and Measures
Several data sources were used to obtain an in-depth understanding of students’
emerging understanding of science phenomena while using the WQS. A total of
6.12 hours of video and audio data were collected on four dyads in each of the two
science classes during all online science inquiry sessions with WQS. This allowed
for an in-depth analysis of students’ behavior as well as teacher instruction and
scaffolding while students engaged in collaborative science inquiry activities
with the WQS. The students’ emerging understanding and ability to regulate their
learning was assessed through an analysis of student and student-teacher inter-
actions during these activities. In addition to the video and audio data from student
pairs, we also collected students’ pretests and posttests.
The paper-and-pencil materials consisted of a pretest and a posttest, which were
constructed by a consulting science teacher based on information presented in
the WQS. The pretest and posttest, which were identical, included seven complex
questions which required students to: 1) demonstrate understanding of runoff
in relation to different water quality indicators and sources and effects of different
indicators; 2) describe and hypothesize daily and seasonal variations depicted
on several line diagrams; 3) determine relationships between different indicators
at different land use areas using scatter plots; and 4) construct a concept map
illustrating the relationships between water quality indicators and land use. The
range of total possible points for each test was 0–62.
The questions were designed to give each student an opportunity to demonstrate
his or her understanding of various issues related to water quality. Question 1
was designed to determine if students understood whether pollutants found at the
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 221

222 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS
Figure 1. RiverWeb interface illustrating a student comparing the
variation in water temperature (top window) and toxins (bottom window)
over time in the wetlands area (Station 5).

mouth of a river come from runoff from land uses areas upstream (range 0–5).
Question 2 was designed to assess whether students understood the importance of
trees as a stream buffer, particularly in developed areas (range 0–6). Question 3
was designed to assess whether students understood how land use can cause an
increase in runoff and, subsequently, more sediments and toxins in the water
(range 0–6). Question 4 was designed to assess whether students understood that
there is seasonal variation in these variables and why that might be (range 0–6).
Question 5 was designed to assess two concepts; each scored separately, whether
students understood that the scale on the y-axis of any graph must be included in
the interpretation of the graph, and that land usage affects nitrogen concentra-
tion (range 0–9). Question 6 also had two parts, scored separately, and was
designed to assess whether students understood that the scale on the x-axis of
any graph must also be included in the interpretation of the graph and that
both precipitation and land use affect runoff (range 0–8). In question 7, students
had to draw a concept map to show that they understood how various factors
affect water quality (range 0–22). With regard to format, questions 1, 2, and 3
were constructed-response questions. Questions 4 and 5 combined time series
graph interpretation and constructed response. Question 6 was a scatter plot graph
interpretation question, and question 7 asked the students to draw and label
a concept map.
Procedure
For this study, students in the environmental science course spent two weeks
using the WQS. For most of the students, this was their first experience learning
about water quality. On the first day of week one, the students took a pretest
to assess their understanding of water-quality indicators and land use. They
were then introduced to the concept of water quality and engaged in mini-labs
such as testing water for nitrogen or identifying the directionality and strength
of relationships between graphed water-quality indicators. During the end of
week one, the students were given a tour of the WQS and taught how to use the
various tools to analyze data. During week two, the students participated in three
“jigsaw” activities using the WQS. In Jigsaw 1, students used a time-series graph
to compare and contrast two variables, and then compared the indicators in
their land use area to those in the pristine forest. In Jigsaw 2, students used the
WQS to learn about two assigned indicators in each land use area. In Jigsaw 3,
students used scatter plots in the WQS to analyze the relationships of all the
different indicators for their specific land use area. The groups then reunited and
composed concept maps showing the links between the different indicators for
each area and presented their maps to the whole class. During the final class period
of week two, the teachers administered the posttest to all students.
Performance data, in the form of pretests and posttests, were collected for all
49 students. Process data, in the form of videotapes and audiotapes, were collected
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 223

on two separate occasions over the two-week period from a sample of eight
dyads from two classes. One consulting teacher acted as a complete participant
during the data collection period. She is an experienced environmental science
teacher who introduced the science activities to all of the students and provided
scaffolding to individual student pairs during activities. The regular classroom
teacher and the second author, a former science teacher, also provided scaf-
folding to students during their activities. The consulting teacher and the
two classroom teachers were not instructed to foster students’ SRL nor were
they asked to use specific SRL process (e.g., planning, creating sub-goals,
metacognitive monitoring) during this naturalistic classroom study. The first
author and several other graduate assistants acted as complete observers rather
than participants in the classrooms, remaining on the sidelines to take notes
and manage taping equipment, interacting minimally with the students and
teachers during class periods. Taping was done for whole class periods during
which the teachers moved in and out of interaction with individual groups
as they tackled the tasks in the jigsaw assignments. Data were therefore
gathered on groups of students while they worked both with and without teacher
assistance.
Coding and Scoring
In this section, we describe the scoring of the students’ answers to the pretests
and posttests, the coding scheme for student and teacher SRL behaviors, and
inter-rater agreement measures.
Students’ Answers to Pretest and
Posttest Questions
The second author and the consulting teacher constructed a rubric for scoring
the students’ responses to the pretest and posttest questions by initially scoring a
subset (30%) of all the pretests and posttests. The rubric was refined and the two
teachers scored all 98 tests individually using the revised rubric. Questions 1, 2, 3,
and 7 were each given a single score; question 5 had two parts so it was given
two scores; and questions 4 and 6 each had two parts so the two questions were
given four scores each. This yielded 14 separate scores for each student’s pretest
and posttest, which were combined to calculate each student’s overall (questions
1–7 on the pretest and posttest) score out of 62 for each test.
Students’ and Teacher’s Regulatory
Verbalizations and Behavior
The 6.12 hours (367 minutes) of audio and video tape recordings from 14
episodes provided the raw data for the analysis of students’ self-regulatory
224 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

behavior and teacher’s scaffolding of SRL. Azevedo, Cromley, and Seibert’s
(2004) original model of SRL was modified and used for analyzing the students’
and teacher’s regulatory behaviors and adapted to fit the use of the WQS in the
classroom. Their model is based on several recent models of SRL (Pintrich, 2000;
Winne, 2001; Winne & Hadwin, 1998; Zimmerman, 2001). It includes key
elements of these models (i.e., Winne’s (2001) and Pintrich’s (2000) formulation
of self-regulation as a four-phase process) and extends these key elements to
capture the major phases of self-regulation in a complex technology-rich student-
centered science classroom.
The classes, descriptions and examples of the planning, monitoring, strategy
use, and task difficulty and demand variables used for coding the learners’ and
teacher’s regulatory behavior are described in the Appendix. The classes are:
a) planning and goal setting, activation of perceptions and knowledge of the
task and context, and the self in relationship to the task; b) monitoring processes
that represent metacognitive awareness of different aspects of the self, task, and
context; and c) efforts to control and regulate different aspects of the self, task,
and context; and d) various kinds of reactions and reflections on the self and
the task and/or context. The model also includes behavior of the teacher when
she provided scaffolding of and instruction in SRL-related behaviors in order
to enhance students’ understanding. Teacher scaffolding refers to any teacher-
initiated move that has the intention of facilitating students’ understanding
through various self-regulatory behaviors. Teacher instruction refers to any
teacher-initiated move that has the intention of specifically directing students’
SRL. For this study and in this context, the model was adapted to include verbal
as well as non-verbal behaviors specific to the tasks students engaged in while
learning with the WQS. As such, video data as well as audio data were used to
provide evidence of teacher and student SRL behaviors.
Inter-Rater Agreement
Each teacher scored each student’s questions on the pretest and posttest
(i.e., 7 questions with a total of 14 scores × 49 students × 2 tests = 1,372 individual
scores). Each teacher was instructed to independently code all 1,372 scores on
the pretests and posttests using a scoring rubric developed by both science
teachers. Then inter-rater agreement was established for the scoring of the
learners’ pretest and posttest answers by comparing the individual coding of
the high school teacher and the second author. There was agreement on 66 out
of 66 randomly selected pretests and posttests, yielding an inter-rater agreement
of 100%. For SRL behavior variables and time, inter-rater agreement was
established on 289 out of 305 coded segments (27% of codes), yielding inter-rater
agreement of 95%. Inconsistencies were resolved through discussion between
the experimenters.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 225

RESULTS
Question 1: Can low-achieving high school students effectively
use a water-quality simulation (WQS) collaboratively to learn
about water quality indicators and land use?
We examined the shift in students’ learning (from pretest to posttest) by using
a paired samples t-test. There was a significant difference between the students’
mean pretest score and their mean posttest score (t[48] = –7.03, p < .001).
However, while there was a significant shift in the test scores, the students’ mean
score was low on both the pretest (M = 12.1%, SD = 4.5) and the posttest
(M = 19.1%, SD = 6.0).
Question 2: Do the students and teacher spend the same amount
of time using various regulatory behaviors to facilitate students’
conceptual understanding?
We examined whether the relatively small (yet significant) learning gains
from pretest to posttest were due to differences in the amount of time that both
students and teachers spent regulating students’ learning with the WQS across
each of the four SRL variables (planning, monitoring, strategy use, and handling
task difficulties and demands). A MANOVA was conducted to determine whether
students, teacher scaffolding, and teacher instruction differed in the amount of
time spent on each of these SRL variables (see Figure 2). The data met the
statistical assumptions for a MANOVA. There was a significant difference in
the mean time that learners spent regulating their own learning and the amount of
time the teacher spent on externally regulating the students by using scaffolding
and direct instruction (F[3, 117] = 19.32, p < .05). Overall, students spent sig-
nificantly more time regulating their own learning than the teacher spent on
regulating students’ learning either by instructing or scaffolding. There were
also significant differences in the amount of time students and teacher spent
using different strategies to regulate students’ learning. There were no differences
in the amount of time spent between planning, monitoring, and handling task
difficulties and demands. Multiple pairwise comparison tests found significant
differences between the time spent by students and the teacher on scaffolding
and instruction for four SRL variables. Learners spent significantly more time
using strategies to regulate their learning (F[2, 39] = 10.96, p < .05) than the
teacher did in providing either direct instruction or scaffolding students’ use
of strategies (Mean = 294.4 sec, SD = 236.3; Mean = 143.0 sec, SD = 78.0;
Mean = 39.0 sec, SD = 35.9, respectively). The teacher spent significantly
more time instructing students how to plan their learning (F[2, 39] = 10.26,
p < .05) than she did scaffolding their planning or that students spent planning
their own learning (Mean = 36.0 sec, SD = 5.5; Mean = 12.7 sec, SD = 5.5;
Mean = 1.8 sec, SD = 5.5, respectively). However, learners and teacher tended
226 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

to spend the same amount of time regulating their own and her students’ moni-
toring activities (F[2, 39] = 0.20, p > .05), and handling task difficulties and
demands (F[2, 39] = 3.14, p > .05).
Question 3: Do the students and teacher differ in the
frequency of use of self-regulatory behaviors to facilitate
students’ conceptual understanding?
Third, we examined how learners and teachers regulated the students’ learning
of the ecology system using the WQS by calculating how often teacher instruction,
teacher scaffolding, and/or student behavior reflected a variable relating to one of
the four main SRL categories. We examined whether the frequency of use of each
of the variables relating to the four main SRL categories of planning, monitoring,
strategy use, and task difficulty were used significantly differently in teacher
instruction, teacher scaffolding, and student behavior. A total of 1,143 segments
were coded based on 6.12 hours (367 minutes) of video from the four dyads in each
of the two classes. Chi-square analyses revealed a significant difference between
the frequency of these variables used between students, teacher scaffolding, and
teacher instruction (�2[6, 1143] = 145.16, p < .001) (see Table 1).
Due to the overall significance, we probed further to examine what specific
SRL behaviors the students engaged in and teachers instructed or scaffolded
while learning with the WQS. In this section, we present a detailed account of
each of the specific SRL variables used by students and teachers to regulate the
students’ learning of ecology with the WQS.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 227
Figure 2. Mean time spent by students and teacher on SRL behaviors.

Planning
Chi-square analyses demonstrated that students and teachers engaged in very
few behaviors associated with category of planning, accounting for only 4% of all
the codes. Under planning, students and teachers were coded for behaviors related
to planning (P), activating prior knowledge (PKA), stating goals (G), or recycling
a goal in working memory (RG). Students displayed no behavior associated with
prior knowledge activation or stating goals. Indeed, student moves were only
associated with 1%2 of codes in this category, due to a few instances of planning
228 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS
Table 1. Number and Proportion of Students’ Self-Regulated Learning
and Teachers’ Scaffolding of Students SRL Movesa
Student-
initiated
moves
Teacher-
instruction
moves
Teacher-
scaffolding
moves
SRL variable f % f % f %
Planning
(Planning, goals, prior knowledge
activation, and recycling goal in
working memory)
Monitoring
(Judgment of learning, partner
questioning, monitoring progress
toward goals, teacher questioning,
and feeling of knowing)
Strategy Use
(Follow directions, procedural,
content evaluation, search, and
selecting new informational
source)
Task Difficulty and Demands
(Time and effort planning and
help-seeking behavior)
Total
7
81
720
56
866
1%
9%
83%
7%
100%
33
21
131
20
205
16%
10%
64%
10%
100%
5
23
41
5
74
7%
31%
55%
7%
100%
aThis table and corresponding analyses include 73% of the codeable data (i.e., 1,143 out
of 1,562 codes), since in 27% of the segments students were engaged in off-task behavior.
2 Percentages cited are the total moves made by students and teachers.

(less than 1%) and a few instances of recycling goals in working memory (less than
1%). Teachers provided little in the way of instruction to students for planning
(only 3% of total codes) and virtually no scaffolding of these SRL behaviors (less
than 1% of total codes). The majority of teacher moves in this category were to
instruct students on their goals (2%). Teachers did not often activate students’
prior knowledge (less than 1%) and there were only a few instances of teacher
instruction of student planning (1%).
Monitoring
Chi-square analyses demonstrated that students and teachers also engaged in
very few behaviors associated with category of monitoring, accounting for 11% of
all the codes. Under monitoring, students and teachers were coded for behaviors
related to expressing a Judgment of Learning (JOL), partner questioning (PQ),
monitoring their progress toward goals (MPTG), teacher questioning (TQ),
and expressing a feeling of knowing (FOK). Student moves associated with
monitoring were responsible for 7%; but the majority of student moves in this
category were partner questioning (2%) and teacher questioning (4%). There were
few instances of judgment of learning (1%), feeling of knowing (less than 1%),
and monitoring progress toward goals (less than 1%). Teacher instruction and
scaffolding of monitoring accounted for 4%, with teachers helping students
monitor their progress toward goals (2% each for TI and TS). Teachers did not
scaffold or instruct students with respect to questioning, judgments of learning,
or feelings of knowing.
Strategies
Chi -square analyses demonstrated that students and teachers engaged in many
behaviors associated with category of strategies, accounting for 78% of all the
codes. Under strategies, students and teachers were coded for behaviors related to
following directions (FD – students only), giving procedural directions (Pro –
teachers only), content evaluation (CE), searching the environment (S), and
selecting a new informational source (SNIS). Student moves associated with
strategies were responsible for 63% of total codes. The majority of student moves
in this category were following directions (32%). Students also engaged in
selecting new information sources (18%), searching (8%), and evaluating content
(6%). These observations were expected, as the jigsaw tasks required these
behaviors for completion. Teacher scaffolding of strategies accounted for 4% of
moves, including helping students evaluate content (2%) and understand
procedures (1%). Teacher instruction of strategies accounted for 11% of moves,
including using procedures (which accounted for 10% of all moves) and
instruction on content evaluation (which accounted for 1% of all moves). Teachers
provided no other strategy help or instruction.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 229

Task Difficulty and Demands
Chi-square analyses demonstrated that students and teachers engaged in very
few behaviors associated with category of task difficulty and demands, accounting
for only 7% of all the codes. Under task difficulty and demands, students and
teachers were coded for behaviors related to time and effort planning (TEP) and
help-seeking behavior (HSB). Student moves associated with monitoring were
responsible for 5% of total codes. The majority of student moves in this category
were help-seeking behavior (5%), usually concerning procedural questions.
Students engaged in virtually no time and effort planning (less than 1%).
In summary, although the analyses found significant differences between the
students’ mean pretest and posttest scores and in the frequency of the students’ and
teachers’ use of SRL variables, the learning gains were quite small. The following
four qualitative descriptions provide valuable insight into how these students
collaborated and self-regulated their learning of ecological systems with the WQS.
The typical pattern for students who were on-task working together is demon-
strated in example #1 (see Table 2). These students were involved in selecting
variables, looking at the graphs, and determining an appropriate value by evalu-
ating the graphs. Students seemed to be merely following directions (e.g., in turn 4,
“What do we put that under—nitrogen, right?”). There is no indication that
they were thinking critically about the information they were writing down.
When teachers were present and students demonstrated that they were
lacking the necessary knowledge to effectively evaluate the content in the hyper-
media environment, the teacher typically provided only procedural instruction. In
example #2, the teacher initiated an interaction with two students by providing
procedural instruction (see Table 2). However, when the teacher presented cog-
nitive scaffolding in the form of evaluating the content (e.g., in turn 5, “So,
what is this again?”), it becomes evident that the students lacked the necessary
knowledge and/or skills to effectively use this scaffolding. Recognizing this
inadequacy, the teacher returned to providing procedural instruction.
Students often sought help from the teacher while completing the RiverWeb
task. Example #3 demonstrates a typical teacher-student interaction in which
the students sought help with carrying out a basic procedure for the task, even
though the steps for the task had been explained to the whole class at the beginning
of the session (see Table 2). The student response to such procedural instruction
was merely to follow directions (e.g., in turns 10, 12, and 17, the student either
chose variables or wrote as directed); there is no evidence that the students were
attempting to understand the concepts.
Example #4 illustrates various scaffolding and instructional techniques that
teachers provided in order to facilitate students’ learning (see Table 2). The teacher
responded to a student-initiated interaction and provided scaffolding for a relation-
ship seen between indicators in the hypermedia environment. In this example, the
teacher used both instructional and scaffolding strategies, including procedural
230 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

help (e.g., in turn 4, “Put a w.”) as well as scaffolding the evaluation of the content
(e.g., in turn 4, “Do you see it increasing or decreasing?”). In addition, the degree
and type of instruction and scaffolding provided by the teachers usually reflected
the level of students’ understanding of the information presented in the hyper-
media environment. In this example, cognitive scaffolding, as opposed to the more
superficial procedural instruction, was provided once the student demonstrated
understanding through evaluation of content (e.g., in turn 3, “Okay, this is a
weak relationship.”).
DISCUSSION
Our results show that low-achieving students tend to benefit little from Web-
based hypermedia learning environments designed to foster their learning of
challenging science topics. Despite performance data that show significant results,
the process data including the time and frequency of students’ SRL and teacher’s
scaffolding and instructional moves show the use of a disproportionate amount
of time and number of SRL moves during learning. The process data provide
evidence that students’ poor performance on the pretest-posttest shift is due to
their failure to deploy key SRL processes and mechanisms which could have led
to significant shifts in conceptual understanding. In addition, the same data show
that the teacher rarely deployed scaffolding and instructional moves aimed at
fostering students’ self-regulated learning of ecology.
With regard to the first research question, the results of this study show that
students did learn about the effects of land use during the two-week period in
which they used the WQS collaboratively in the ecology class. Despite the
statistically significant results on their scores from pretest to posttest, the shift
in scores is rather small (7%) and there is ample room for increased learning of
ecology, since the mean on the posttest was relatively low (19%). We can conclude
that while these Web-based hypermedia learning environments can potentially
foster students’ learning, not all students have the self-regulatory skills necessary
to learn from these rich environments. Our results indicate that providing low-
achieving students with complex, open-ended CBLEs without the appropriate
embedded and contextual scaffolding (e.g., from the teacher, peer, tutor, etc.) will
lead to inferior shifts in learning. This finding is consistent with the majority
of studies on non-linear, random access hypermedia environments with flexible
access and a high degree of learner control (e.g., Azevedo, Cromley, & Seibert,
2004; Azevedo, Guthrie, & Seibert, 2004; Greene & Land, 2000; Jacobson
& Archodidou, 2000).
This finding is consistent with previous research which indicates that students
lack the strategies, self-monitoring, persistence, and other self-regulatory skills
required to take advantage of the vast amounts of rich content contained in
CBLEs such as Web-based hypermedia learning environments. We hypothesize
that in addition to not having the self-regulatory skills, these students lack prior
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 231

232 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS
Table 2. Qualitative Descriptions of Students’ and Teacher Interactions
during Learning with RiverWeb [Non-Verbal Behavior] (SRL Code)
Turn Transcript
Example 1:
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
Example 2:
1
2
Student 2: [Selecting variables] (FD)
Student 1: Toxins. . . . Alrighty then, right now we can look for
pristine forests. Let me see something. That don’t work. That sucks.
What does this say— nitrogen concentration.
Student 2: [unintelligible] Um, average is .7. (CE)
Student 1: What do we put that under—nitrogen, right? 0.7 [writing
on sheet]? (FD)
Student 2: [Unintelligible] [writing on sheet] (FD)
Student 1: [Selecting variables] There is runoff. (FD) (SNIS)
Student 2: 1.7- (CE)
Student 1: No, it is 1—1.797 (CE)
Student 2: I mean 1.797
Student 1: .5. [writing on sheet] (FD)
Student 2: Change it—to what? [Selecting variables] Sediments . . .
2.—(FD) (SNIS) (CE)
Student 1: - .6 (CE)
Student 2: 2.6 [writing on sheet] (CE) (FD)
Student 1: Water temp. [Selecting variables] (FD) (SNIS)
Student 2: . . . . And its . . . . [looking at screen and writing on sheet]
(CE) (FD)
Student 1: Phosphorous . . . [Selecting variables] (FD)
Student 2: Phosphorous.
Teacher: Okay, so you are going to complete this. This first thing
that I wanted you to do is put your station number here . . . Okay,
so you are going to, you are going to fill out everything here so we
know this belongs to. Ah, this is lumbering forest. You are going to
look at average runoff. This is nitrogen. (TI-PRO) What station is
this? (TS-PRO) Wait a minute, don’t, don’t change it yet. You already
have the average nitrogen, okay. (TI-PRO). What’s the average?
[referring to information in hypermedia environment] (TS-CE)
Student 1: 0.7865 (CE)

SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 233
Table 2. (Cont’d.)
Turn Transcript
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
Example 3:
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
Teacher: Remember, I said you can, you can round that to .79,
would be good. (TI-PRO)
Student 1: Okay.
Teacher: Okay. So what is this again? [referring to information in
hypermedia environment[ (TS-CE) Where do you find that on your
table? Okay, for what station? (TS-PRO)
Student 2: I have no, I don’t know how to do none of this or
understand the problem. (JOL)
Teacher: Uh, uh. Fill that, put that in there. You will continue and fill
out the chart. (TI-PRO)
Student 1: What is this? (TQ)
Teacher: Well, you have, you have to fill this out first. (TI-PRO)
Student 1: How do I find about the [unintelligible]? (TQ)
Teacher: Well, again, click on here and it gives you all of your
different indicators. (TI-PRO)
Student 2: [Hand is raised] I need to find out what station we are.
(HSB)
Student 1: I think we are 6 [unintelligible].
Student 2: Yeah, I think we are 5 or 6. [Keeps hand raised]
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Student 1: Hey [to teacher] . . .
Teacher: Yes?
Student 1: What station? (HSB)
Teacher: Um, 6. So you can type in 6. (TI-PRO)
Student 2: What are we looking for now? (TQ)
Teacher: [referring to table on back of packet] Can I rip this? Ok.
You are going to click on your land use area. Then you are going to
use this table and you are going complete this for your indicators.
You are station 6, so I want you to write “6” in here [points to place
on sheet]. (TI-PRO)
Student 2: [Writes on sheet as directed] (FD)
Teacher: And then “urban”. Ok, now, you are going to complete this
entire table, so the first thing you are going to do (TI-P) is click on this
rectangle where it says air temp.—click there and choose runoff.
(TI-PRO)

knowledge of the topic and also lack specific science skills such as rounding
numbers. We acknowledge that one of the limitations of this study is that we used a
one-group pretest-posttest pre-experimental design and combined it with process
data. However, this was a necessary first step prior to conducting experimental
manipulations given the infancy of this type of research and the complex nature of
students’ SRL with CBLEs in the classroom (Mayer, 2003).
With regard to the second and third research questions, our extensive process
data indicated that students and the teacher differed in the amount of time and the
frequency of use of self-regulatory behaviors during learning with the WQS.
These students tend to spend more time on and use more self-regulatory skills,
followed by teacher instructional moves and then teacher scaffolding. Also, the
students and teacher use strategies as a predominant SRL variable during learning
234 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS
Table 2. (Cont’d.)
Turn Transcript
12
13
14
15
16
17
Example 4:
1
2
3
4
5
6
Student 2: [Clicks on rectangle and chooses runoff]. . . (FD)
Teacher: Ok, and then down here, what is the next one? (TS-CE)
Student 1: Sediment. (CE) [selects sediment]
Student 2: Sediment.
Teacher: Uh huh. Now when you change display—[points to screen]
you have minimum value, maximum value, and I want you to write
down the average, and again, you can round up. (TI-PRO) So that
would be 21,939. You write that here and you are done. (TS-MPTG)
Student 2: [Writes down number on sheet] (FD)
Student 1: Sketch that? [Question directed towards teacher] (TQ)
Teacher: Uh, uh. (TI-PRO)
Student 1: Okay, this is a weak relationship. [referring to scatter plot
graph in hypermedia environment] (CE)
Teacher: Uh, uh. Put a w. (TI-PRO). Negative? Do you see it
increasing or decreasing? (TS-CE). Or, if you can’t if you can’t tell if
it is going up or down, then there’s no relationship. (TI-CE)
Student 1: I can’t tell. [referring to scatter plot graph in hypermedia
environment] (CE)
Teacher: Okay, then you put no relationship. Okay, then as you
continue, you guys discuss it amongst yourselves. Make sure you
both agree. (TI-PRO)

with the WQS. The second most often used SRL variable by students and teachers
is monitoring. The quantity (i.e., amount and frequency of SRL variables) and
quality of the strategy and monitoring processes used by students may explain
why students gained so little knowledge of ecology with the WQS. The verbal
protocols provide process data to indicate that they used these SRL processes,
and together with the product data show that the use of these processes led to
significant increases in their understanding of the science topic. Students regulated
their learning by using mostly ineffective strategies and metacognitive monitoring
and did very little planning. In contrast, the teacher’s instructional and scaffolding
moves involved using strategies to facilitate students’ learning. These findings
highlight the value of converging product and trace data—i.e., while the statistical
analyses revealed significant differences in the time and frequency data, the
explanation lies in the analysis of the quality of each of these SRL moves used
by both students and teachers. For example, despite spending a significant amount
of time regulating their learning, and spending a disproportionate amount of time
using these strategies, the process data reveal students were using low-level
strategies such as following directions and searching the WQS without a specific
goal. Furthermore, the relative amount of time spent by the teacher on instructing
and scaffolding moves was relatively less than students spent, and the quality
of those moves can also be considered low-level in response to the students
level of understanding.
These findings are consistent with previous research on using trace method-
ologies to examine SRL processes used by learners during learning (Azevedo,
Cromley, & Seibert, 2004; Azevedo, Guthrie, & Seibert, 2004; Winne & Hadwin,
1998; Winne & Stockley, 1998). They also highlight current theoretical and
methodological concerns among SRL researchers that more research is required to
understand the inter-relatedness and dynamics of SRL variables during learning-
cognitive, motivational/affective, behavioral, and contextual during the cyclical
and iterative phases of planning, monitoring, control, and reflection (e.g., Butler,
2002; Meyer & Turner, 2002; Perry, 2002; Pintrich, 2000). This is definitively the
case in the area of learning with Web-based hypermedia learning environments,
where research tends to focus on learning gains (see Shapiro & Niederhauser,
2004 for examples) without considering the complexities of students’ SRL and
the teachers’ scaffolding of SRL during extended learning with CBLEs.
Implications for the Design of the CBLEs as
MetaCognitive Tools to Enhance Students’ Learning
The results of our study have implications and pose several challenges for the
design of the WQS to foster low-achieving students’ self-regulated learning
of ecology in technology-rich dynamic science classrooms. One challenge for
designing a Web-based hypermedia environment is the role of scaffolding, which
is critical in fostering students’ conceptual understanding and enhancing SRL. The
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 235

first issue related to scaffolding is the system’s ability to detect that students need
assistance in regulating their learning. For example, how can the WQS detect that
a student or the pair is/are using ineffective strategies or need(s) assistance in
monitoring their learning? Second, what types and levels of scaffolding methods
should be designed for low-self-regulating learners?
According to our results, these students typically use ineffective learning
strategies, do not monitor their learning, fail to engage in help-seeking behavior,
and have difficulties in handling task difficulties and demands. So, how can we
“build” system components (e.g., a student model, instructional model, interface,
inference engine, etc.) to determine if: a) a learner, in the complex interactions
during individual and collaborative knowledge construction is having difficul-
ties with several phases and areas of SRL; and b) what effects will this deter-
mination have on the detection, monitoring, and fostering of learners’ overall
self-regulation? How can we make our SRL model “visible” to the learners and
flexible enough to allow learners to explore advanced topics related to the
ecological systems, including its content and structure? How can the CBLE adapt
and exhibit flexibility during learning?
Recent research on pedagogical agents (e.g., Baylor, 2003; White et al., 2000)
suggest that these agents could be used to facilitate students’ SRL by scaffolding
students’ planning (e.g., include a planning net with a hierarchy of sub-goals,
model the phases of science inquiry), monitoring (e.g., prompting student to
periodically relate their emerging understanding with their prior knowledge and
instructional goals), strategy use (e.g., embed video clips that model effective
strategies such as making inferences and hypothesizing, model basic science
skills such as rounding off numbers), and handling task difficulties and demands
(e.g., prompts to help the student monitor their time and effort planning, provide
a digital library of video clips that deal with FAQs related to basic science
skills, inquiry skills, etc.).
Another critical issue is the system’s ability to construct an adequate model
of the learner’s emerging understanding of the domain (i.e., a mental model). How
can we design ways of detecting, monitoring, and fostering shifts in learners’
mental models of ecological systems? Can we have students create concept maps
and/or drawings which can be used to dynamically assess their existing mental
model, which could interact with other system components? For example, what
kind of instructional decisions should be made in a case where the system has
determined that a student has a sophisticated mental model of the ecological
system, but has expressed low interest in the task, versus a student who has
constructed a less-sophisticated mental model has expressed a high level of
interest in the topic, yet lacks the ability to plan learning and is using ineffective
strategies (e.g., “blindly” searching the WQS to find the representations of infor-
mation needed to explore the issues of the current goal)? Would making the learner
construct an “external” visual representation of his/her emerging mental model
of the domain allow him/her to self-regulate? At another level, would this visual
236 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

representation allow a peer to assist with regulating their learning? Would this
external representation allow learners to jointly regulate their co-construction of
the topic? Can this information provide the WQS with other “variables” with
which to make informed instructional decisions? Also, would these external
representations, which are visible and accessible, allow the learner (and perhaps
others such as peers, and teachers) to share, inspect, critique, modify, and assess
the learners’ understanding of a complex system?
These issues pose several challenges for the learning sciences community. First,
there is a greater need to draw on the strengths of the members of our research
community in dealing with the complexities of studying SRL in complex instruc-
tional settings and in designing and developing CBLEs to trace, detect, and foster
students’ SRL. Second, the role of teachers needs to be examined in terms of how
different instructional methods and scaffolding support students’ SRL, and their
role as external regulatory agents creating instructional settings that promote
students’ SRL (Corno & Randi, 1999; Meyer & Turner, 2002; Perry et al., 2002).
Third, theoretical issues related to SRL challenge Derry and Lajoie’s (1993) initial
characterization of three camps for using computers as cognitive tools—modelers,
middle-campers, and non-modelers. Lajoie (2000) has recently argued that we
should abandon these “camps” due to both the influx of computer technologies
made available to teachers and educators and to recent challenges made by SRL
researchers about the complexity of learning. Future research should explore the
role of SRL and teachers’ fostering of students’ understanding of complex and
challenging science topics in dynamic technology-enhanced classroom learning
environments (Azevedo, 2002).
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 237

AP
PE
ND
IX
Cla
sses,D
escri
ptio
ns,an
dE
xam
ple
so
fth
eV
ari
ab
les
Used
toC
od
eLearn
ers
’S
elf-R
eg
ula
tory
Beh
avio
ran
d
Co
-Reg
ula
ted
Beh
avio
r(b
ased
on
Azeved
o,C
rom
ley,&
Seib
ert
,2
00
4).
Vari
ab
leD
escri
ptio
na
Exam
ple
Pla
nn
ing
Pla
nn
ing
(P)
Go
als
(G)
Pri
or
Kn
ow
led
ge
Activatio
n(P
KA
)
Recycle
Go
alin
Wo
rkin
gM
em
ory
(RG
)
Ap
lan
invo
lves
co
ord
inatin
gth
esele
ctio
no
f
op
era
tors
.Its
exe
cu
tio
nin
vo
lves
makin
g
beh
avio
rco
nd
itio
nalo
nth
esta
teo
fth
ep
rob
lem
an
da
hie
rarc
hy
ofg
oals
an
dsu
b-g
oals
Co
nsis
teith
er
ofo
pera
tio
ns
that
are
po
ssib
le,
po
stp
on
ed
,o
rin
ten
ded
,o
ro
fsta
tes
that
are
exp
ecte
dto
be
ob
tain
ed
.G
oals
can
be
iden
tified
becau
se
they
have
no
refe
ren
ce
to
alr
ead
y-e
xistin
gsta
tes
Searc
hin
gm
em
ory
for
rele
van
tp
rio
rkn
ow
l-
ed
ge
eith
er
befo
reb
eg
inn
ing
perf
orm
an
ce
of
ata
sk
or
du
rin
gta
sk
perf
orm
an
ce
Resta
tin
gth
eg
oal(e
.g.,
qu
estio
no
rp
art
so
fa
qu
estio
n)
inw
ork
ing
mem
ory
(WM
)
Stu
de
nt:
“Next
we
are
go
ing
tolo
ok
at
that”
[refe
rrin
gto
new
data
en
try]
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:“W
hen
yo
uare
do
ne,yo
ucan
loo
kat
sed
imen
ts”
Tu
tor
Scaff
old
ing
:“I
have
giv
en
yo
uan
exam
ple
to
use
toco
mp
lete
this
pag
e.”
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:“Y
ou
will
be
wri
tin
gre
latio
nsh
ips.”
Tu
tor
Scaff
old
ing
:“W
hy
mig
ht
sed
imen
tsan
d
nitro
gen
go
tog
eth
er?
”
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:“Y
ou
need
tore
mem
ber
wh
at
we
did
du
rin
gth
eto
ur.
”
Stu
de
nt:
“It
says
wri
teth
ere
latio
nsh
ips.”
238 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

Mo
nit
ori
ng
Ju
dg
men
to
fLearn
ing
(JO
L)
Part
ner
Qu
estio
nin
g
(PQ
)
Mo
nito
rP
rog
ress
To
ward
Go
als
(MP
TG
)
Teach
er
Qu
estio
nin
g
(TQ
)
Feelin
go
fK
no
win
g
(FO
K)
Str
ate
gy
Use
Fo
llow
Dir
ectio
ns
(FD
)
Pro
ced
ura
l(P
ro)
Learn
er
beco
mes
aw
are
that
they
do
n’t
kn
ow
or
un
ders
tan
devery
thin
gth
ey
read
Learn
er
dir
ects
learn
ing
or
pro
ced
ura
lb
ased
qu
estio
nto
ward
sco
llab
ora
tive
part
ner
Assessin
gw
heth
er
pre
vio
usly
-set
go
alh
as
been
met
Learn
er
dir
ects
learn
ing
or
pro
ced
ura
lb
ased
qu
estio
nto
ward
ste
ach
er
Learn
er
isaw
are
ofh
avin
gre
ad
so
meth
ing
in
the
past
an
dh
avin
gso
me
un
ders
tan
din
go
fit,
bu
tn
ot
bein
gab
leto
recall
ito
nd
em
an
d
Learn
er
isin
vo
lved
ino
ne
ofth
efo
llow
ing
learn
ing
-based
beh
avio
rs:
(1)
Read
ing
wo
rksh
eet,
(2)
en
teri
ng
data
,(3
)calc
ula
tin
g,
or
(4)
co
llab
ora
tin
gw
ith
part
ner
Can
be
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avi
or
Teach
er
pro
vid
es
scaffo
lsd
ing
/in
str
uctio
n
on
pro
ced
ure
ofle
arn
ing
task
Stu
de
nt:
[Refe
rrin
gto
teach
er
scaffo
ldin
go
fco
nte
nt
evalu
atio
n]
“Id
on
’tkn
ow
ho
wto
use
tho
se
gra
ph
s.”
Stu
de
nt:
[Dir
ecte
dto
ward
sp
art
ner]
“Ho
wd
ow
e
kn
ow
wh
at’s
air
tem
p?”
Stu
de
nt:
“We
are
do
ne.”
Tu
tor
Scaff
old
ing
:“A
reyo
ug
uys
fin
ish
ed
?”
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:“Y
ou
sh
ou
ldb
esta
rtin
go
nyo
ur
seco
nd
ch
art
.”
Stu
de
nt:
[Dir
ecte
dto
ward
ste
ach
er]
“Wh
at
are
we
su
pp
ose
tod
ow
ith
the
calc
ula
tor
part
?”
Stu
de
nt:
“Id
idre
ad
them
”[b
ut
do
es
no
tkn
ow
the
an
sw
er]
.
Stu
de
nt:
Typ
es
data
into
co
mp
ute
r,calc
ula
tes,w
rite
s
info
rmatio
no
nw
ork
sh
eet,
dis
cu
sses
task
ste
ps
with
part
ner.
Te
ach
er
Scaff
old
ing
:[R
efe
rrin
gto
wo
rksh
eet]
“Wh
ich
lan
du
se
isit
for?
”
Te
ach
er
Instr
ucti
on
:“Y
ou
on
lyn
eed
toavera
ge”
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 239

AP
PE
ND
IX(C
on
t’d
.)
Vari
ab
leD
escri
ptio
na
Exam
ple
Co
nte
nt
Evalu
atio
n
(CE
)
Searc
h(S
)
Sele
ctin
gN
ew
Info
rmatio
nal
So
urc
e(S
NIS
)
Mo
nito
rin
gco
nte
nt
rela
tive
tog
oals
Searc
hin
gth
eh
yp
erm
ed
iaen
vir
on
men
t
with
ou
tsp
ecifyin
ga
sp
ecific
pla
no
rg
oal
Can
be
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avi
or
Th
esele
ctio
nan
du
se
ofvari
ou
sco
gn
itiv
e
str
ate
gie
sfo
rm
em
ory
,le
arn
ing
,re
aso
nin
g,
pro
ble
mso
lvin
g,an
dth
inkin
g.M
ay
inclu
de
sele
ctin
ga
new
rep
resen
tatio
n,co
ord
inat-
ing
mu
ltip
lere
pre
sen
tatio
ns,etc
.
Can
be
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avio
r
Stu
de
nt:
[Refe
rrin
gto
scatt
erp
lot
inh
yp
erm
ed
ia]
“Th
at’s
po
sitiv
e;
that’s
str
on
g.”
Tu
tor
Scaff
old
ing
:[R
efe
rrin
gto
scatt
erp
lot
in
hyp
erm
ed
iaen
vir
on
men
t]“D
oyo
usee
itin
cre
asin
g
or
decre
asin
g?”
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:[R
efe
rrin
gto
scatt
erp
lot
in
hyp
erm
ed
iaen
vir
on
men
t]:
“Th
ink
ab
ou
tw
hy
the
str
on
go
nes
are
str
on
g.”
Stu
de
nt:
Learn
er
man
ipu
late
sh
yp
erm
ed
iaen
vir
on
-
men
tb
yscro
llin
geith
er
up
or
do
wn
Stu
de
nt:
[Learn
er
en
ters
new
data
]th
en
ad
van
ces
ton
ew
line
or
scatt
erp
lot
gra
ph
240 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

Task
Dif
ficu
lty
an
d
De
man
ds
Tim
ean
dE
ffo
rt
Pla
nn
ing
(TE
P)
Help
Seekin
g
Beh
avio
r(H
SB
)
Oth
er
Un
co
deab
le(U
C)
Off-t
ask
(OT
)
Att
em
pts
toin
ten
tio
nally
co
ntr
olb
eh
avio
r
Learn
er
seeks
assis
tan
ce
reg
ard
ing
eith
er
the
ad
eq
uate
ness
ofth
eir
an
sw
er
or
their
instr
uctio
nalb
eh
avio
r
Can
be
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avi
or
Du
eto
vid
eo
cam
era
an
gle
an
d/o
rau
dio
,
beh
avio
ran
d/o
rverb
aliz
atio
no
fle
arn
er(
s)
can
no
tb
ere
liab
lyco
ded
usin
go
ne
ofth
e
SR
Lco
des
Learn
er
exh
ibits
beh
avio
rth
at
iscle
arl
yn
ot
with
inth
ele
arn
ing
task,in
clu
din
gta
lkin
g
off-t
op
icw
ith
co
llab
ora
tive
part
ner
an
d/o
r
peer.
Can
be
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avi
or
Stu
de
nt:
[To
part
ner]
“Wh
at
do
we
do
no
w?”
Te
ach
er
Scaff
old
ing
:[R
efe
rrin
gto
on
eo
fth
e
co
llab
ora
tive
learn
ers
]“A
reyo
uh
elp
ing
him
?”
Tu
tor
Instr
ucti
on
:“I
will
giv
eyo
ua
co
up
lem
ore
min
ute
s.”
Stu
de
nt:
Rais
es
han
dan
d/o
rverb
ally
so
licits
help
fro
mte
ach
er
or
researc
her.
aA
llco
des
refe
rto
wh
at
was
reco
rded
inth
ecla
ssro
om
an
dcap
ture
do
nth
eau
dio
an
dvid
eo
tap
es
(i.e
.,w
hat
stu
den
tsre
ad
,saw
,h
eard
,o
r
did
),in
clu
din
gw
hat
the
teach
er
initia
ted
an
dscaffo
lded
du
rin
gle
arn
ing
with
the
WQ
S.S
om
eco
des
inclu
de
no
n-v
erb
alb
eh
avio
r.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 241

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Stacy Pritchett for constructing the lesson
plans, assisting the classroom teachers, and scoring the pretests and posttests; and
Susan Ragan and Mary Ellen Verona at the Maryland Virtual High School for
allowing us to use RiverWeb in this classroom study. We also thank Jennifer G.
Cromley for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript; Laura Smith,
Lucia Buie, and Jennifer G. Cromley for assisting in the data collection; and
the high school teachers (Wendell Hall and Philip Johnson) and their students
for participating in this study.
REFERENCES
Azevedo, R. (2002). Beyond intelligent tutoring systems: Computers as MetaCognitive
tools to enhance learning? Instructional Science, 30(1), 31-45.
Azevedo, R., Cromley, J. G., & Seibert, D. (2004). Does adaptive scaffolding facilitate
students’ ability to regulate their learning with hypermedia, Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 29, 344-370.
Azevedo, R. Cromley, J. G., Winters, F. I., Xu, L., & Iny, D. (2003). Is strategy instruction
effective in facilitating students’ ability to regulate their learning with hypermedia?
In U. Hoppe, F. Verdejo, & J. Kay (Eds.), Artificial intelligence in education: Shaping
the future of learning through intelligent technologies (pp. 193-200). Amsterdam:
IOS Press.
Azevedo, R., Guthrie, J. T., & Seibert, D. (2004). The role of self-regulated learning in
fostering students’ conceptual understanding of complex systems with hypermedia.
Journal of Educational Computing Research, 30, 87-111.
Azevedo, R., Verona, M. E., & Cromley, J. G. (2001). Fostering students’ collaborative
problem solving with RiverWeb. In J. D. Moore, C. L. Redfield, & W. L. Johnson
(Eds.), Artificial intelligence in education: AI-ED in the wired and wireless future
(pp. 166-175). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
Baylor, A. (2003). Evidence that multiple agents facilitate greater learning. In U. Hoppe,
F. Verdejo, & J. Kay (Eds.), Artificial intelligence in education: Shaping the
future of learning through intelligent technologies (pp. 377-379). Amsterdam:
IOS Press.
Biswas G., Schwartz, D., Bransford, J., & CTGV. (2001). Technology support for complex
problem solving: From SAD environments to AI. In K. Forbus & P. Feltovich (Eds.),
Smart machines in education. Cambridge Press, MA: MIT Press.
Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P., & Zeidner, M. (2000), Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Butler, D. (1998). The strategic content learning approach to promoting self-regulated
learning: A report of three studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(4), 682-697.
Butler, D. (2002). Individualizing instruction in self-regulated learning. Theory Into
Practice, 41(2), 81-92.
Clark, D., & Linn, M. (2003). Designing for knowledge integration: The impact of
instructional time. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(4), 451-493.
242 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

Corno, L., & Randi, J. (1999). A design theory for classroom instruction in self-
regulated learning? In C. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional-design theories and
models: A new paradigm of instructional theory (Vol. II; pp. 293-318). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Derry, S. J., & Lajoie, S. P. (1993). Computers as cognitive tools (1st ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Greene, B., & Land, S. (2000). A qualitative analysis of scaffolding use in a resource-
based learning environment involving the World Wide Web. Journal of Educational
Computing Research, 23(2), 151-179.
Hickey, D., Kindfield, A., Horowitz, P., & Christie, M. (2003). Integrating cur-
riculum, instruction, assessment, and evaluation in a technology-supported
genetics learning environment. American Educational Research Journal, 40(2),
495-538.
Jacobson, M., & Archodidou, A. (2000). The design of hypermedia tools for learning:
Fostering conceptual change and transfer of complex scientific knowledge. Journal
of the Learning Sciences, 9(2), 149-199.
Jonassen, D. (1996). Computers as mind tools for schools. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Kozma, R., Chin, E., Russell, J., & Marx, N. (2000). The roles of representations and
tools in the chemistry laboratory and their implications for chemistry learning.
Journal of the Learning Sciences, 9(2), 105-144.
Lajoie, S. P. (2000). Computers as cognitive tools II: No more walls: Theory change,
paradigm shifts and their influence on the use of computers for instructional purposes.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lajoie, S. P., & Azevedo, R. (2000). Cognitive tools for medical informatics. In S. P. Lajoie
(Ed.), Computers as cognitive tools II: No more walls: Theory change, paradigm shifts
and their influence on the use of computers for instructional purposes (pp. 247-271).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lajoie, S. P., Guerrera, C., Munsie, S., & Lavigne, N. (2001). Constructing knowledge in
the context of BioWorld. Instructional Science, 29(2), 155-186.
Mayer, R. (2003). Learning environments: The case for evidence-based practice and
issue-driven research. Educational Psychology Review, 15(4), 359-366.
McCaslin, M., & Hickey, D. (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement:
A Vygtoskian perspective. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated
learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 227-252). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Meyer, D. & Turner, J. (2002). Using instructional discourse analysis to study the scaf-
folding of student self-regulation. Educational Psychologist, 37(1), 17-25.
Paris, S. G., & Paris, A. H. (2001). Classroom applications of research on self-regulated
learning. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 89-101.
Perry, N. (2002). Introduction: Using qualitative methods to enrich understandings of
self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 37(1), 1-3.
Perry, N., VendeKamp, K., Mercer, L., & Nordby, C. (2002). Investigating teacher-
student interactions that foster self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist,
37(1), 5-15.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In
M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
(pp. 451-502). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 243

Rasku-Puttonen, H., Etelapelto, A., Arvaja, M., & Hakkinen, P. (2003). Is successful scaf-
folding an illusion? Shifting patterns of responsibility and control in teacher-student
interaction during a long-term learning project. Instructional Science, 31, 377-393.
Reiser, B., Tabak, I., Sandoval, W., Smith, B., Steinmuller, F., & Leone, A. (2001).
BGuILE: Strategic and conceptual scaffolds for scientific inquiry in biology class-
rooms. In S. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years
of progress (pp. 263-306). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schunk, D. (2001). Social cognitive theory and self-regulated learning. In B. Zimmerman
& D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical
perspectives (pp. 125-152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shapiro, A., & Niederhauser, D. (2004). Learning from hypertext: Research issues and
findings. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research for education communications
and technology (2nd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Slotta, J. D., & Linn, M. C. (2000). The knowledge integration environment: Helping
students use the Internet effectively. In M. Jacobson & R. Kozma (Eds.), Learning
the sciences of the 21st century. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Suthers, D., & Hundhausen, C. (2003). An experimental study of the effects of repre-
sentational guidance on collaborative learning processes. Journal of the Learning
Sciences, 12(2), 183-218.
Vye, N., Schwartz, D., Bransford, J., Barron, B., Zech, L., & CTGV. (1998). SMART
environments that support monitoring, reflection, and revision. In D. Hacker,
J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice
(pp. 305-346). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
White, B. Y., Shimoda, T. A., & Frederiksen, J. R. (2000). Facilitating students’ inquiry
learning and metacognitive development through modifiable software advisers. In
S. P. Lajoie (Ed.), Computers as cognitive tools II: No more walls: Theory change,
paradigm shifts and their influence on the use of computers for instructional purposes
(pp. 97-132). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Williams, M. (1996). Learner control and instructional technologies. In D. Jonassen (Ed.),
Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (pp. 957-983).
New York: Scholastic.
Winne, P. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning viewed from models of information proc-
essing. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic
achievement: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 153-189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Winne, P., & Hadwin, A. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker,
J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice
(pp. 277-304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts,
P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 531-566).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Winne, P., & Stockley, D. (1998). Computing technologies as sites for developing
self-regulated learning. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulated learning:
From teaching to self-reflective practice (pp. 106-136). New York: Guilford.
Zimmerman, B. (2001). Theories of self-regulated learning and academic achievement:
An overview and analysis. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated
learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 1-37). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
244 / AZEVEDO, WINTERS AND MOOS

Zimmerman, B., & Schunk, D. (Eds.). (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic
achievement (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Direct reprint requests to:
Roger Azevedo, Ph.D.
University of Maryland
Dept. of Human Development
3304 Benjamin Bldg., 3304E
College Park, MD 20742
e mail: [email protected]
SRL AND COLLABORATIVE LEARNING WITH A CBLE / 245