coping theory

Upload: lesli-perez-garcia

Post on 06-Mar-2016

17 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

xc zd

TRANSCRIPT

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Adapting to and Coping with the Threat and Impacts of Climate Change

    Joseph P. Reser

    Griffith University, Queensland, Australia

    and Janet K. Swim

    Pennsylvania State University

    Article manuscript submitted to the American Psychologist for inclusion in proposed special

    edition focus on Psychology and Climate Change, 15 July 2010

    Resubmission with amendments on 30 November 2010

    1

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Abstract

    The article addresses the nature and challenge of adaptation in the context of global

    climate change. The complexity of climate change as threat, environmental stressor, risk

    domain, and impacting processes with dramatic environmental and human consequences

    requires a synthesis of perspectives and models from diverse areas of psychology to

    adequately communicate and explain how a more psychological framing of the human

    dimensions of global environmental change can greatly inform and enhance effective and

    collaborative climate change adaptation and mitigation policies and research. An integrative

    framework is provided which identifies and considers important mediating and moderating

    parameters and processes relating to climate change adaptation, with particular emphasis

    given to environmental stress and stress and coping perspectives. This psychological

    perspective on climate change adaptation highlights crucial but neglected aspects of

    adaptation in the climate change science arena. Of particular importance are intra-individual

    and social psychological adaptation processes which powerfully mediate public risk

    perceptions and understandings, effective coping responses and resilience, overt behavioral

    adjustment and change, and psychological and social impacts. This psychological window on

    climate change adaptation is arguably indispensable to genuinely multidisciplinary and

    interdisciplinary research and policy initiatives addressing the impacts of climate change.

    Keywords: Climate change, psychological adaptation, environmental stress, stress and

    coping, psychologically significant behaviour

    2

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Adapting to and Coping with the Threat and Impacts of Climate Change

    Yet, even with the most ambitious mitigation actions, the inertia of the system will ensure that the impacts of climate change will continue for centuries, if not beyond a millennium. Knowledge of impacts and the manner in which they would grow over time is therefore critical to the development of capacity and measures for adaptation to climate change. (Pachauri, 2009, xiv)

    Adaptation to the threat and rapidly unfolding impacts of climate change has become

    a pressing and urgent issue, given the alarming rapidity with which predicted climate changes

    are taking place. The question of what can be done to address the global and very human

    crisis which is now upon us is refocusing world attention on climate change adaptation

    (Pielke, et al., 2007). The threat of what will be very likely consequences of climate change,

    have been given palpable reality by extensive media coverage, at times apocalyptic in nature

    (e.g., Smith & Joffe, 2009). Addressing the challenges of adapting to climate change is

    important not only to ensure the safety and security of human and nonhuman populations in

    many regions of the world, but to ensure that immediate and pressing needs do not derail still

    vital national and international climate change mitigation policies.

    Defining climate change adaptation

    In this article climate change refers to the threat and unfolding environmental

    impacts of current climate change, with a clear understanding that what is typically referred

    to in everyday conversation and with respect to climate change adaptation are the threatened

    environmental and human consequences of climate change, not changes in global climate

    patterns. Climate change constitutes a complex risk domain, attitudinal object, and social

    representation, with respect to a phenomenon which is as much a social phenomenon as it is

    a physical phenomenon (e.g., Grauman & Kruse, 1990; Hulme, 2009; Wagner & Hayes,

    2005).

    3

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    The Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) defines adaptation as

    adjustment in natural or human systems in response to actual or expected climatic stimuli or

    their effects, which moderates harm or exploits beneficial opportunities (IPCC, 2007b).

    According to this IPCC definition, adaptation may include responses made in anticipation of

    climate change impacts, responses that are a result of deliberate policy decisions based upon

    awareness of current or upcoming changes, and autonomous or spontaneous responses

    that represent unplanned responses triggered by ecological changes in natural systems and

    by market or welfare changes in human systems.. Adaptation in this climate change science

    context often refers to structural changes, such as building more resistant human settlements

    and infrastructure or providing ways to ensure adequate and sustainable water and food

    availability and micro and macro human system adjustments, such as those relating to

    households, communities, institutions, and regional, national, and global governance

    structures and policies.

    A psychological perspective on adaptation includes many of these human setting and

    system considerations, and both anticipated and reactive responses to climate change, but

    goes beyond these in encompassing human experience and psychological well being.

    Psychological forms of adaptation are very infrequently referenced or addressed in the

    current climate change science adaptation literature (e.g., IPCC, 2007; Leary et al., 2008;

    Schipper & Burton, 2009). Adaptation as a construct and foundational process has been used

    nonetheless in a rich and convergent variety of ways in psychology and in the health and

    social sciences generally (e.g., Lazarus, 1991; Martin, 1964; White, 1974). Like evolutionary

    biologists, evolutionary psychologists have used adaptation to refer to genotypic changes that

    have increased reproductive success and survival, including hardwired behavioral

    adaptations. A classic and biological system-based use of adaptation refers to specific

    psychophysiological responses, such as sensory habituation to changing stimuli (e.g., noise,

    4

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    temperature, or amount of light). But adaptation also encompasses the diverse types of coping

    responses individuals can make to changes in their physical and social environments,

    including natural disasters and the ongoing threat of war and terror (e.g., Bell et al., 2001;

    Holahan, 1982; Marshall et al., 2007). Adaptation is also commonly used to refer to intra-

    individual and extra-individual processes and actions that involve, for instance,

    accommodating, assimilating, or adjusting to various contexts and new or difficult life

    circumstances (e.g., work situations, new cultures, globalization, adoption, chronic illness).

    What is distinctive about psychologys use of the term adaptation, particularly when

    used to refer to adaptation processes, is that it encompasses and integrates both intra-

    individual parameters and processes (e.g., appraisals of situations, affective responses,

    cognitive analysis and reframing, disengagement, defensive responses, and emotion

    regulation) as well as extra-individual social and situation processes (e.g., proximity and

    exposure, collective sense making, social comparison, social construction, social

    amplification of risk, and collective efficacy) that influence how individuals and communities

    respond to challenging circumstances. This more encompassing set of meanings and contexts

    for adaptation is integral to and greatly informs both environmental stress and stress and

    coping approaches to understanding peoples responses to difficult and taxing situations

    (e.g., Evans & Stecker, 2004; Stokols, 1978).

    An environmental stress perspective on the adaptation demands of global climate

    provides a particularly appropriate framework for considering adaptation in the context of

    climate change (e.g., Bell et al., 2001; Evans & Cohen, 1991, Evans & Stecker, 2004). This

    framework brings environmental and human ecological perspectives to the complex

    phenomenon of climate change. It encompasses the requisite multiple levels of analysis

    needed to adequately frame the adaptation challenges of dramatic climate change impacts and

    to strategically address planning considerations and interventions enhancing individual and

    5

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    community adaptations (e.g., Winkel, Saegert & Evans, 2009). This perspective encompasses

    and articulates with research on environmental perception and evaluation, risk appraisal,

    communication, and management, and disaster preparedness, response, and recovery (e.g.,

    Reyes & Jacobs, 2006). Disaster research is particularly germane because many of the

    projected impacts of climate change will take the form of acute and longer term natural

    disasters. Finally, an environmental stress perspective also informs and complements research

    on stress and coping, which itself examines and addresses individual level psychological

    processes as well as community level coping mechanisms (e.g., Baum & Fleming 1993;

    Holahan & Wandersman, 1991; Lazarus & Cohen, 1977).

    An illustrative synthesis model

    There exist many models of environmental stress, and stress and coping, but for the

    purpose of providing a synthetic model that might more usefully articulate with the schemas

    and models of the IPCC and climate change scientists, while underscoring the psychological,

    we have developed a further organizational framework for a number of the considerations

    which follow in this article (see Figure 1). This derives from related and convergent

    psychological models (e.g., environmental stress, adaptation, stress and coping models,

    protection motivation theory, and the health belief model (e.g., Glanz, Rimer, & Lewis, 2002;

    Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Rogers & Prentice-Dunn, 1997; Vaughn, 1993). We first provide

    an overview of the model and then describe specific elements of the model as they might

    apply to climate change adaptation.

    The initiators of the adaptation process, noted on the far left of the figure, are

    conceptualized as stressors, and in this context they include direct and indirect experiences

    with the threat and impacts of climate change. Initial responses to these threatening changes,

    impacts, or conditions include cognitive responses in the form of appraisals of the impacts

    relative to resources (threat appraisals), appraisals of possible responses (coping appraisals),

    6

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    and simultaneous emotion-based and cultural meaning system informed interpretative and

    motivational responses and processes. For instance, individuals who reside in a coastal

    community will assess the probability and extent to which they and their family will be

    affected by rising sea levels and whether they have resources to respond to rising sea levels

    (threat appraisals). They may also assess what they think they could or should do about rising

    sea levels and whether what they might do would make a difference (coping appraisal). Their

    risk perceptions and coping appraisals though may be equally influenced by the nature of

    climate change as a risk domain, the possible symbolic import, dread, and uncertainty

    associated with such a catastrophic scenario, prior personal or vicarious experience with

    inundation or dramatic environmental change or displacement, and protection motivation and

    psychological distancing mechanisms to counter anxieties, concerns, and possible felt

    responsibility for the very changed world which climate change may well usher in. (e.g.,

    Slovic et al., 2004; Vaughan, 1993; Weber, 2006).

    These initial responses influence each other as well as the selection of intra-individual

    and behavioural responses at both the individual and community level, which in turn mediate

    individual and community impacts. For instance, greater perceived threat can lead to more

    worry. Together these responses can lead to intra-individual emotion regulation and defensive

    responses or participation in civic action to encourage ones community to develop protective

    measures to address rising sea levels (behavioral response). Different coping responses result

    in and themselves mediate different types of impacts on individuals and communities.

    Doherty & Clayton (2010) in this issue address the psychological impacts of climate change.

    These psychological responses feed back into appraisals, affective responses, attributions,

    and motivations already mentioned. As the example illustrates, adaptation processes can

    change over time, for instance, as particular problems are addressed or as coping resources

    diminish (e.g., Lepore & Evans, 1996). Finally, many moderators can exercise influence at

    7

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    each stage in the model and examples are listed at the bottom of the figure. For instance,

    individuals and communities with fewer resources and institutionalised histories of

    powerlessness and disadvantage are likely to be more vulnerable and less resilient to climate

    changes due to, for instance, the inability to engage in effective coping responses (e.g.,

    Cutter, 2003; Norris et al., 2008).

    Climate change threat and environmental impacts and change as stressors

    In what follows we extrapolate from research on environmental stress and stress and

    coping to the context of global and local climate change, and refer to research from

    convergent areas of disaster preparedness and response, risk perception and appraisal, the

    psychology of ongoing threat, and applied research employing stress and coping models. The

    disaster literature is particularly relevant to this climate change focus given the nature of the

    threats and impact events projected in the context of climate change. Yet there are a number

    of aspects of global climate change which make this phenomenon and aggregate of stressors

    distinct from other stressors and disasters and may alter the extent to which generalization is

    appropriate. These considerations include the global scope, magnitude, and temporal horizon

    of climate change, which may encompass many generations and likely many centuries, and

    the unprecedented character of such dramatic and consequential global changes in known

    human history.

    Stressors are typically understood as events or circumstances that tax normal

    environmental transactions and relationships and initiate and motivate adaptation responses

    and stress and coping processes. In the climate change context stressors encompass direct,

    indirect, and mediated experiences with global climatic patterns and region-specific weather

    conditions and physical environmental impacts. Some communities, such as those located in

    regions of Alaska, Northern Canada, and Northern Europe are currently responding to direct

    contact with evident physical environmental impacts of climate change (e.g., Kolbert, 2006).

    8

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Yet most communities in other regions of the world are responding to media images and

    coverage of climate change and social exchanges, with these images, texts, sound bites,

    documentaries, and conversations constituting powerful but indirect and virtual social

    representations of climate change and unfolding impacts. It is noteworthy that much of the

    media coverage of natural disasters around the world is being discussed, framed, and

    explained as manifestations of climate change. This suggests that the public in many parts of

    the world increasingly understand and see current and major natural disasters as dramatic,

    prophetic, and unfolding evidence of climate change. Current in-depth national survey

    research findings in Australia provide strong support for such public perceptions and

    understandings of the nexus between natural disasters and climate change (Reser et al., 2010)

    and along with more recent survey findings in North America (e.g., Yeager, Larson &

    Krosnick, 2010; Leiserowitz, Smith & Marlon, 2010) suggest that public belief in climate

    change, confidence in climate change science, and concern about climate change impacts

    remains very high, notwithstanding the media attention given to the views of skeptics and

    selective survey findings where issues with the framing of statements and questions and

    problematic response formats have led to distorting interpretations and reported findings.

    Those who directly experience the biophysical environmental impacts of climate change will

    likely experience stress due both to their immediate exposure to and personal experiences

    with climate change impacts as well as because of their shared and socially constructed

    anxieties, expectations, and understandings about future impacts of climate change.

    Types of stressors.

    Discrete and continuous stressors. Climate change threat and impacts can be

    experienced as discrete events and as continuous environmental stressors (e.g., Aldwin &

    Stokols, 1988; Wheaton, 1999). Discrete events represent sudden, extreme, environmental

    phenomena or life changing events, including natural disaster events such as hurricanes or

    9

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    tornados, that occur with little or no warning and impact a large number of people, and

    personal stressors (i.e., stressful life events), such as death and illness, that affect fewer

    people and may or may not be anticipated (e.g., Bell et al., 2001; Evans & Cohen, 1991).

    Climate researchers have warned of more frequent and severe weather-related events

    including the increased frequency, heightened intensity, and extent of impact of natural

    disasters such as severe storms, hurricanes, tornadoes, floods, bush fires, and other rapid

    onset and largely unpredictable events.

    In contrast, continuous stressful events or prolonged and adverse environmental

    conditions such as drought or a contaminated housing estate or mining region are viewed as

    chronic stressors and not event-specific. Ambient stressors are a type of chronic stressor

    particularly characteristic of environmental stressors (e.g., Bell et al., 2001). Ambient

    stressors can represent regional conditions of the environment, such as pollution or toxicity,

    that affect a large number of people but which may not be considered acute because they

    approximate low level background noise and may go unnoticed either because they are subtle

    or because people habituate to them (e.g., Adeola, 2000; Edelstein, 2002). Climate change

    can be understood as an ambient stressor encompassing periodic acute stressor events.

    Climate researchers have projected multiple and chronic stressor conditions, in the form of

    drought and other more incremental and persistent environmental changes, such as soil loss

    and erosion, salination, and desertification. Further, climate changes are often in the

    background due to these changes being embedded in natural variations in climate, the

    patterns being difficult to detect, the slow progression of the changes which can lead to a

    normalized habituation and expectancy, or the effects being perceived to be more relevant for

    future generations than ones own..

    Natural and technological disasters. In the disaster literature researchers point to

    both natural and technological disasters (resulting from technological processes and products)

    10

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    as differing types of cataclysmic events (e.g., Baum, Fleming, & Davidson, 1983; Baum &

    Fleming, 1993; VandenBos & Bryant, 1987). Natural disasters are more sudden, cataclysmic,

    uncontrollable, acute (as distinct from chronic), and are characterised by enormous

    destructive power and magnitude. Technological disasters are attributed to human behavior

    (not the product of natural forces) and are less predictable, typically accompanied by no

    warning, are often chronic, often having no visible manifestation, are less familiar, more

    likely to threaten feelings of control, more likely to have complex impacts, less likely to elicit

    supportive and cohesive community response, and more likely to foster anger, frustration,

    resentment, felt helplessness, and blame.

    Global climate change straddles this classification. Human forcing of naturally

    occurring climate change is largely the product of technological processes and products,

    though consequent meteorological and climate change phenomena manifest as natural

    disasters. Indeed, climate change elicits some of the same responses found in the case of

    technological disasters, including distrust of government, corporations, regulatory authorities,

    and indeed science itself (e.g., Earle, 2004). Global climate change is also unique in that it

    presents multifaceted global impacts that will be chronic over a dramatic time frame, and

    constitutes a phenomenon not amenable to conventional national or jurisdictional agencies, or

    disaster policies and procedures (e.g., Marshall et al., 2007). Many authors have suggested

    that framing global climate change in global disaster terms provides a clearer and more

    realistic picture of interacting processes and impacts, their true magnitude and extent, the

    nature and scale of human impacts, and the imperative to take immediate disaster mitigation

    and preparedness measures (e.g., Spratt & Sutton, 2008; Reser & Morrissey, 2008).

    Mediating transactions between stressors and coping responses

    Threat appraisals. Adaptation and stress and coping models highlight the roles that

    cognitive and affective processes play in risk appraisal and selection of coping responses.

    11

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    The more cognitive processes identified in these stress models focus on appraising or

    evaluating the stressor and possible adverse impacts on oneself and those important to oneself

    (e.g., friends, family members, colleagues). These appraisals include assessing the perceived

    risk of events, the severity of current or future damage, and who is vulnerable to the risks

    (e.g., Weber & Stern, 2010). It is important to note that appraisals include assessing

    perceived psychological and human costs as well as the physical consequences of events.

    Climate changes can also be appraised as threatening because of their broader environmental

    impacts on all life on the planet and can be the cause of anticipatory grief and felt loss.

    Risk perception and appraisal are influenced by social factors. Much information

    about the world and potential threats and problems comes mediated by way of our social

    world (e.g., Berger & Luckmann, 1967; Gergen, 2009), through interactions with friends,

    overheard conversations, observations of others, media coverage, and specific risk

    communications from health professionals and climate change scientistswith these risk

    messages also being communicated through and edited by journalists and media

    organizations (e.g., Carvalho, 2007; Danesi, 2002), and via information and communication

    technologies including the internet (e.g., Olson & Rejeski, 2005; Pettenger, 2007). Such

    vicarious experience, information exchange, and social learning includes the individual and

    cultural learning of adaptive practices and competencies with respect to risk, danger, and

    uncertainty (e.g., Bandura, 1999; Douglas & Wildavsky, 1982; Kahan et al., 2007).

    Social construction, social representation, and social amplification processes are three

    theoretical frameworks describing the complex factors which mediate and substantially

    influence the publics appraisals of risk, environmental threat, and global environmental

    change (e.g., Bauer & Gaskell, 2002; Pidgeon et al., 2003). These perspectives help explain

    variation in understandings of and responses to climate change across cultures, regions, and

    communities, and across environmental experts, journalists, scientists, and laypeople.

    12

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Social construction as a process refers to how people collectively and through social

    interaction impose meaning and order on their world, how they perceive and interpret,

    construct and shape, their shared reality (e.g., Burr, 2003; Gergen, 2009). Social

    constructions are also understood as consensual understandings and operating constructs and

    classifications, thoughts and ideals, shared by members of a society which emerge through

    everyday conversation and transactions with each other and with the environment and world

    they share. Such entities as nature, the environment, environmental problems, natural

    and technological disasters, sustainability and climate change itself are viewed by

    many theorists and researchers as, in substantial part, social constructions (e.g., Macnaughten

    & Urry, 1998; Pettenger, 2007). A considerable body of research helps us understand the

    nature and dynamics of such socially constructed and media-disseminated environmental

    threat representations and understandings (e.g., Adam, 1998; Johndon-Cartee, 2005; Weber,

    2006). Hence this is an encompassing perspective of particular relevance to adaptation and

    coping, and public risk perceptions, understandings, and responses to climate change.

    Social representations are shared assumptions and understandings about the social

    and physical world (e.g., Moscovici, 2000). They include material culture expressions,

    images, texts, other information and communication technology products and information

    environments which capture and reflect particular world views. They provide a framework

    for the interpretation and communication of our experiences. It is through these commonly

    shared and collectively elaborated social representations that we make sense of the world and

    communicate that sense to each other (e.g., Deaux & Philogene, 2001; Flick, 1998). Social

    representations of climate change include media images, articles, books, magazine covers,

    documentary and popular culture films, research findings, and public discourse and shared

    understandings about climate change and its nature, causes and environmental and human

    consequences. Many studies have been undertaken in North America and Europe which

    13

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    examine public risk perceptions of climate change, but few studies have undertaken in-depth

    investigations of the nature of media representations of climate change, or the underlying risk

    domain of climate change vis--vis other known environmental risks, or how or why climate

    change might be quite different from other risk domains in representation and with respect to

    public risk perceptions and appraisals and related psychological responses.

    Social processes can both amplify and attenuate understandings of climate change

    (e.g., Pidgeon et al., 2003; Sjoberg, 2006). The social amplification of risk framework holds

    that, as a key part of that communication process, risk, risk events, and the characteristics of

    both become portrayed through various risk signals (images, signs, symbols), which in turn

    interact with a wide range of psychological, social, institutional, or cultural processes in ways

    that intensify or attenuate perceptions of risk and its manageability (Kasperson et al., 2003,

    p. 15). The research challenge has been to distil what these research findings and past policy

    applications of evidence-informed risk management principles have to say about how

    individuals and communities might best prepare themselves for what will be, for many, a

    very changed environmental and regulatory landscape in the context of climate change.

    Coping appraisals. A second and more individually-oriented response domain related

    to cognitive processing of experienced and anticipated stressors focuses on the evaluation of

    responses one might make to the stressor. This includes assessing ones ability to engage in a

    behavior (i.e., self-efficacy), the perceived likelihood of a behavior to result in the desired

    outcome (i.e., response efficacy), perceived constraints on response options, and the relative

    perceived costs and benefits of respective responses. Costs and benefits are, of course, often

    unknown and therefore reflect a type of risk appraisal. Other coping appraisals involve

    assessing characteristics and resources of ones immediate social environment and

    community, such as the strength of ones social networks and neighborhood organization

    (e.g., Benight, 2004; Holahan & Wandersman, 1991). Coping responses to various climate

    14

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    change impacts will be influenced by primary appraisals of the specific impacts experienced

    or anticipated, and secondary appraisals of the adaptation and mitigation responses could be

    made to these threats and/or impacts. Social processes and media portrayals are also very

    likely to influence primary and secondary threat appraisals and coping responses.

    Interpretive and motivational responses and processes. How individuals respond to

    the perceived threat of climate change is likely influenced by the causal and responsibility

    attributions made in the context of climate change. Psychological research shows that

    peoples willingness to restore or prevent damage is mediated by perceptions of the causes of

    the damage (e.g., Weiner, 1995). Such attributions, for instance, to either natural or human

    processes can influence appraisals of and the impact of events (Brun, 1992; Slovic et al.,

    2004). The distinction between natural and human-influenced causes may appear irrational in

    the face of consequential considerations, but plays a crucial role in considerations of

    perceived responsibility, accountability, and adaptation motivations. Even if people agree

    climate change is anthropogenically forced, they may not take personal responsibility for

    adjusting to current consequences or for preventing future impacts. Indeed research findings

    suggest that many may perceive global and distant others to be largely responsible for this

    global and distant environmental problem instead of attributing personal or collective

    responsibility (e.g., Uzzell, 2000; 2004). Research is needed to specifically examine such

    interrelations in the context of global climate change and how these sense-making and human

    agency dynamics might relate to assessing and allocating blame and accountability for

    disasters.

    The emotional side of risk perceptions, appraisals, and responses to climate change

    are likely to influence and mediate behavioral responses to climate change, and the

    importance of risk-as-feeling is not limited to individual level responses (Loewenstein et al.,

    2001; Slovic et al., 2004). Societal and cultural level emotional responses to media images

    15

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    and coverage of salient and menacing threats, such as radiation or seemingly cataclysmic

    future scenarios, both imbue and reflect strong affective and symbolic responses to threat,

    and are informed by culturally elaborated risk domains (e.g., Adam, 1998; Douglas &

    Wildavsky, 1982). While only limited research has considered the nature and status of

    climate change as a risk domain vis a vis other risk domains (e.g., Townsend, Clarke, &

    Travis, 2004), it is of particular importance to ask how emotional and symbolic aspects of

    climate change risk appraisals and sense making are influencing the nature and levels of

    public concern and underlying adaptation and protection motivation processes (e.g., Bohm,

    Nerb, McDaniels, & Spada, 2001; Weinstein, 1989).

    Perhaps the most frequently studied affective responses to stressful events relate to

    anxiety, fear, and worry, though other appraisal and self-efficacy related emotions are salient.

    Environmental stressors characteristically undermine peoples perceived ability to predict and

    control the environments in which they live. A perceived lack of personal environmental

    control is one of the most ubiquitous determinants of aversiveness, anxiety, and distress (e.g.,

    Evans & Cohen, 1991; Shapiro, Schwartz & Astin, 1995). Worry is an important

    psychological impact of climate change (see Doherty & Clayton, 2010) which can also

    influence other parts of the adaptation process (e.g., Davey & Wells, 2006). Fear, for instance

    in Protection Motivation Theory, is conceptualized as a response to and mediator of ones

    evaluation of the stressor (e.g., Rogers & Prentice-Dunn, 1997). Fear and anxiety, while

    adaptive responses to threat, can often get in the way of clear thinking and very necessary

    adaptive responding in the context of imminent natural disaster warning situations (Reser,

    2004). Other affective responses, such as hope, may act like optimism, by enhancing the

    likelihood that individuals will select coping strategies that engage one with the situation

    (Snyder, 2002). A taken for granted assumption within psychology that such intra-individual

    responses to the threat and perceived impacts of climate change are an important form of

    16

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    adaptation and a powerful mediator of overt adaptation behaviors is not widely appreciated or

    understood in the climate change science community. The nature and status of emotional

    responses to climate change is an important but currently unresolved conceptual and

    theoretical issue, as is the status of environmental concern(s).

    Motivational processes are fundamental to considerations of psychological responses,

    impacts, and behavior change in the context of climate change. Most stress and coping

    models assume that the reduction of appraised threats motivates individuals to initiate coping

    responses. The Health Belief Model, for instance, is premised on the assumption that people

    are prepared to undertake preventive behavior(s) as a function of their appraisal of the

    severity of a threat, the perceived benefits of a recommended health action, and the perceived

    barriers to taking such action (e.g., Janz & Becker, 1984). Cognitive adaptation approaches

    (e.g., Aspinwall, 2004; Lehman & Taylor, 1987; Taylor, 1983; Taylor & Shepperd, 1998;

    Taylor & Stanton, 2007) are also central to understanding intra-individual psychological

    adaptation processes, and the suite of cognitive and emotional heuristic strategies employed

    to achieve a manageable world (e.g., Slovic, 2000). Motivated reasoning perspectives argue

    that functional, self-serving needs lead us to selectively seek information, evaluate evidence,

    and form conclusions that validate existing, unreflective, beliefs and enhance self perception

    and esteem (e.g., Kunda, 1990; Leary, 2006). Other motivational responses to environmental

    threat and stress have received substantial psychological attention: instinctive fight or flight

    responding, drives to survive as described in evolutionary psychology, psychoanalytic

    defense mechanisms, goal setting, and various manifestations of protection motivation,

    whether through attitudinal stance, value expression, avoidance, defensive pessimism or

    unrealistic optimism (e.g., Taylor & Brown, 1988; Weinstein & Kline, 1996). These and

    other motivational and sense-conferring considerations can substantially inform our

    understanding of adaptation and coping responses in the face of climate change.

    17

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    A central emphasis over the past several decades in the area of environmental

    psychology (e.g., Bell et al., 2001; Bonnes & Bonaiuto, 2002; Gifford, 2007) has been that of

    environmental concern(s), and the role which this risk appraisal process and outcome, and

    accompanying motivational state, plays in adopting pro-environmental behaviors and

    possibly mediating psychological distress (e.g., Hansla, Gamble, Juliusson, & Garling, 2008;

    Schmuck & Schultz, 2002). This focus on concern has also been very typical of popular

    culture coverage and debate with respect to the human impacts of climate change (e.g.,

    Carvalho, 2007; Kluger, 2006; Lowe et al., 2006). But current conceptualizations of

    environmental concern(s) as construct, risk response, and motivational state have not

    adequately addressed the nature, scope, and uncertainty of global climate change, nor its

    important spatial, temporal, and cultural referents and meanings.

    These convergent literatures are routinely drawn upon by psychologists when

    addressing environmental risks and natural and technological hazard preparedness and

    response (e.g., Cvetkovich & Earle, 1992; ORiordan, 1995). Such psychological

    considerations and research findings are often not recognized or utilized in interdisciplinary

    considerations and discourses, with climate change being a particularly salient case in point.

    More recently a number of psychology research teams have begun to systematically compare

    and contrast public risk perceptions, appraisals and psychological response to global climate

    change as contrasted with nuclear energy facilities (e.g., Pidgeon, Lorenzoni & Poortinga,

    2008; Spence, Pidgeon & Uzzell, 2008). This research draws on an extensive research base

    compiled since 1979 in the wake of Three Mile Island (TMI) and other nuclear power station

    accidents (e.g., Baum & Fleming, 1993; Baum, Fleming & Davidson, 1983) and has since

    been directed to many technological and natural environmental threats (e.g., Edelstein, 2002).

    The research with nuclear facilities and accidents, such as that at TMI has conclusively

    shown that information itself about technological risks can be threatening and anxiety-

    18

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    inducing, leading to very real mental and physical health impacts. In this context, for

    example, emotionally-focused coping strategies were associated with less stress than

    problem-focused coping and denial. In many large scale disaster contexts, being able to

    anticipate and manage ones risk perceptions and psychological response in the context of

    largely uncontrollable external events and consequences confers very real and

    psychologically adaptive benefits (e.g., Taylor, 1983; Aspinwall, 2010; Aspinwall & Taylor,

    1997; Reser & Morrissey, 2008).

    Types of adaptation and coping responses

    Coping responses include actions or inhibitions of single, multiple, and repeated

    behaviors engaged in by individuals or groups as well as intra-individual responses to climate

    change. These responses can be proactive (also known as anticipatory adaptation and

    psychological preparedness), made in anticipation of an event, or reactive, made after an

    event (e.g., Aspinwall, 2010; Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997). The two merge when responses are

    made to an event in order to diminish the impact of a current event while simultaneously

    addressing future events. For instance, individuals who rebuild their homes after a natural

    disaster may be adapting to changes that have occurred while at the same time enhancing

    protection from future disasters. Nonetheless, the differentiation is useful when thinking

    about coping with climate change because many people may not be responding to currently

    occurring events attributable to climate change but are instead responding to anticipated

    events. Thus, addressing successful coping in the context of global climate change requires a

    thoughtful consideration of prevention and preparedness (e.g., Ball, 2008; Keim, 2008).

    Different literatures emphasize different types of coping responses. The stress and

    coping literature has emphasized individual coping responses. Intra-individual responses to

    experienced or anticipated experiences include responses such as denial, environmental

    numbness, cognitive reappraisals, and emotion regulation (e.g., Carver & Sheier, 1998).

    19

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Other individual responses are overtly behavioral such as seeking information, seeking social

    support, or engaging in problem solving by changing ones habitat to adjust to climate

    changes, or engaging in mitigation. In contrast, research on environmental stressors and

    natural disasters has been more likely to include community level responses (e.g., Gow &

    Paton, 2008; Peek & Mileti, 2002). Community responses to stressors include volunteerism

    and helping neighbors cope with lack of water, basic amenities, or destruction of ones home.

    It is not uncommon for groups to emerge after disasters that help communities cope with

    crises, reflecting the interactive dynamics of collective coping, community resilience, and a

    crisis-initiated and renewed group identity and sense of community (e.g., Holahan &

    Wandersman, 1991; Gow & Paton, 2008). These community responses may be particularly

    important to take into account when considering coping with the impacts of climate change

    given the breadth and duration of the impacts, and the differential impacts of climate change

    for communities in differing geographic and socioeconomic circumstances. In the context of

    climate change, additional and specific types of psychological and social responses that have

    not typically been examined in past research may need to be addressed, such as abandoning

    social or moral order, reliance on dogmatic beliefs, or rejecting consumer driven lifestyles

    (e.g., Eckersley, 2008).

    Moderators of adaptation and coping processes

    Many personal and contextual variables have been theorized and tested as predictors

    and moderators of individuals and communities adaptation and coping responses and many

    of these are likely to be important factors in public adaptation to climate change (e.g., Bell et

    al., 2001; Winkel et al., 2009). Several examples are listed in Figure 1. Sometimes these

    variables predict appraisals and preferences for coping responses, such as when optimism

    predicts the tendency to use problem-focused coping in reaction to a stressor (e.g., Scheier,

    Weintraub, & Carver, 1986). At other times the constructs will moderate relations between

    20

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    the variables in the model such as when the constructs predict the impact of these appraisals

    on the coping response (i.e., moderators of the impact of appraisals on coping responses) and

    when the construct predicts the consequences of coping responses (i.e., moderate the relations

    between coping responses and outcomes; the latter are discussed in Doherty & Clayton,

    2010). For instance, neuroticism has been shown to influence not only the choice of coping

    responses but also the impact of coping responses on wellbeing (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995).

    Two constructs often discussed in the climate change literature are resilience and

    vulnerability. In this literature resilience typically refers to the adaptive capacity of resilient

    social-ecological systems (e.g., Schipper & Burton, 2009). Within psychology, and in the

    case of individuals, the construct of resilience typically refers to inner strengths and coping

    resources for necessary adaptation to situational demands. In the case of communities, it

    refers to social strengths and capacities of a community such as in the form of pooled

    resources, knowledge, social supports, and social capital (e.g., Bonanno, 2004; Luthar, 2003;

    Masten, 2001; Schoon, 2006). Resilience has become the principal theme in the APAs

    web-based help line and brochures providing advice and guidance in the context of disasters

    and terrorism (e.g., American Psychological Association, 2007; Newman, 2005).

    Vulnerability refers to the extent to which systems and individuals are susceptible to,

    and unable to cope with, adverse effects of climate change. Vulnerability is a function of the

    characteristics of climate change impacts (e.g., their magnitude and rate of change) and

    variation in systems and individuals (e.g., degree of exposure to climate change impacts,

    individual and community adaptive capacity, and connectedness to communities). It has also

    been well documented, most recently and dramatically in the aftermath of Hurricane Katrina,

    that vulnerabilities can be endemic to systems and places as well as integral to life

    circumstances, prior experiences, and socioeconomic and racial disadvantage (e.g., Cutter,

    Boruff & Shirley, 2003; Cutter et al., 2006). Vulnerability in psychological contexts is also a

    21

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    very important experiential and risk appraisal domain and psychological response, which is

    often not commensurate with objective risk assessment and which typically reflects cultural

    and often symbolic meanings and associations as well as individual difference considerations.

    Discussion

    The question addressing adaptation in the context of climate change framed by the

    APA Taskforce was How do people adapt to and cope with the perceived threat and

    unfolding impacts of climate change? This brought necessary specification to that which

    people are adapting to, and to the construct and processes of adaptation, and facilitated a

    clearer and strategic consideration of convergent theoretical and research areas which seemed

    of particular value in identifying and profiling relevant psychological work. The matter of

    what people are adapting to is in our view critical, albeit complex, as climate change is

    clearly far more than the objective environmental consequences and impacts of altered

    atmospheric climate patterns. It is also one thing to review how individuals and communities

    have coped with a devastating set of environmental changes such as a decades-long drought,

    or a catastrophic event such as 9/11 or Hurricane Katrina, or that body of literature relating to

    more personal life changes and crises, but quite another to address how individuals and

    communities will adapt and cope with an encompassing spectrum of global environmental

    changes profound in scope and consequences and possibly extending into the next

    millennium. Yet adaptation to climate change also constitutes a quintessentially

    psychological matter and our discipline has considerable experience and depth in related risk,

    disaster, crises, and promotion of health and wellbeing contexts, and the matter at issue is

    human response to dramatically changing behavior contexts, settings, and natural

    environments.

    While we focused on adaptation in this article, it is difficult to separate psychosocial

    and mental health impacts from adaptation processes and responses, either analytically or

    22

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    operationally. They are intertwined aspects of psychological response to the complex

    phenomenon of global climate change. Public perceptions, appraisals, understandings,

    motivations, and consequent psychological and behavioral responses to climate change

    representations and physical environmental consequences are all integral and inter-related

    features of psychological adaptation to the ongoing and unfolding psychosocial impacts of

    global climate change. Having said this, it is important to closely consider, and to bring

    theoretical and analytic clarity to, the construct and processes of adaptation in the context of

    climate change, both to communicate the too-often neglected mediating roles and dynamics

    of psychological processes when discussing adaptation in the context of climate change,

    and to profile the crucial value of an encompassing ecological and multi-level psychological

    perspective when considering climate change impacts, interventions, and policy

    considerations (e.g., Winkel et al., 2009).

    A challenge in addressing adaptation in the context of climate change is that

    adaptation is such a fundamental part of psychologys assumptive and theoretical world when

    addressing human behavior and in particular people-environment transactions that it is not

    always easy or useful to differentiate adaptation from closely related and/or interacting

    processes such risk perception and appraisal, sense making, coping, psychological impacts,

    and multiple types of intra-individual and extra-individual responses and adjustments. These

    processes all fall within the compass of adaptation and the reciprocal adjustments which

    characterize characterize people-environment transactions. Indeed from a psychological

    vantage point it is arguable that climate change adaptation and mitigation are closely

    interlinked, in that it is ones psychological response to the climate change threat and ones

    changed thinking, feelings, motivations and concerns that powerfully mediate the extent to

    which one engages in environmentally significant behavior (i.e., behavior which reduces

    ones carbon footprint, e.g., Stern, 2010). What has received insufficient attention is that it is

    23

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    often the psychological significance of ones behavior and response to perceived

    environmental issues that is personally meaningful and motivating. The prevailing distinction

    between adaptation and mitigation made in the climate change science literature is

    understandable and arguably useful, but problematic in terms of the motivations,

    meaningfulness, and consequences of ones actions and their relationship to psychological

    adaptation. Indeed more recent interdisciplinary discussions are acknowledging that

    adaptation and mitigation are in fact closely interlinked, with this realization itself suggesting

    an important research front (e.g., Brewer, 2008).

    The challenges of addressing the threat and environmental impacts of global climate

    change highlight multiple areas of research need and cross-domain collaboration possibilities,

    both within psychology and across the social and natural sciences. A more environmental

    psychological perspective places particular emphasis on important changes taking place in

    the human landscape in response to environmental changes and impacting processes. In the

    context of human response and adaptation to climate change, there exist a number of areas of

    crucial need and strategic importance.

    There is a clear need to be able to more adequately and sensitively measure, document, and monitor significant changes over time taking place in the human

    landscape with credible and meaningful psychological and social indicators which

    relate to psychological and social responses to and the psychosocial impacts of the

    threat and environmental consequences of climate change. (e.g., Li, 2010; Stokols et

    al., 2009).

    A pressing research challenge is to more closely address the matter of local versus global environments and places; how these space/place perceptions and connections

    relate to environmental concerns, engagements, responsibility attributions, and

    perceived efficacy; and how very consequential psychological adaptations to climate

    24

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    change might well involve making the global more local, and the local more global,

    through meaningful personal engagement at a local level with this global threat and

    challenge (e.g., Uzzell, 2004; Whitmarsh, ONeill & Lorenzoni, 2010).

    An important and related research domain relates to the relative importance of direct exposure to and experience with environmental changes and impacts associated with

    climate change as distinct from indirect or vicarious experience through media

    coverage, environmental documentaries, on line inquiries, and interpersonal

    exchange. Given the pervasiveness of media coverage of climate change, it is

    important to further explore and document emotional and other reality-conferring

    message features which can make virtual exposure to and experience with the realities

    of climate change more direct, personal, and motivating (e.g., Moser & Dilling, 2004;

    Ornstein & Ehrlich, 1990 ).

    There exist very few studies of the risk domain status and cultural meaning contexts and parameters of climate change. This neglected research domain relates closely to

    that of the social representations of global climate change, and how media images and

    popular science depictions are influencing public understandings of, perceived risk,

    other psychological responses to the phenomenon of climate change (e.g., ONeill &

    Hulme, 2009). Using what is known about adaptation and coping could be used to

    develop interventions to aid psychological and physical adaptation, as has been done

    in health psychology (e.g., Aspinwall, 2010; Taylor, 2006), and to develop policy

    recommendations.

    Psychological perspectives on climate change adaptation highlight a number of crucial but currently neglected aspects of adaptation in multi- and interdisciplinary

    perspectives on adaptation. These include multi-leveled approaches and analytic

    frameworks that encompass individual and experience-focused levels of analysis,

    25

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    social psychological and motivational process responses to the threat and unfolding

    impacts of climate change, and environmental psychological models, constructs, and

    indicators relevant to assessing the psychosocial contexts and impacts of climate

    change (e.g., Gifford, Steg & Reser, 2011; Wapner et al, 2000).

    Psychological research on human response to global environmental change

    conservatively spans three decades (e.g., Chen, Boulding & Schneider, 1983; National

    Research Council, 1992, 2009; Stern & Gardner, 1981), providing particularly helpful

    perspectives and insights on human adaptation and adjustment to environmental threat,

    natural and technological disasters, and stressful and challenging environmental changes. But

    this highly relevant and extensive body of theoretical approaches, research findings, and

    evidence-based applications continues to be a relatively unfamiliar disciplinary landscape in

    the environmental sciences. Of particular importance is the conceptual framing and

    theoretical elaboration of intra-individual and social psychological adaptation processes

    which would appear to be powerfully mediating public risk perceptions and understandings,

    effective coping responses and resilience, overt behavioral adjustment and change, and

    psychological and social impacts in the context of climate change. This psychological

    window on climate change adaptation is arguably indispensable to genuinely

    multidisciplinary and interdisciplinary research and policy initiatives addressing the impacts

    of climate change (Stokols et al., 2008).

    26

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    References

    Adam, B. (1998). Timescapes of modernity: The environment and invisible hazards. London:

    Routledge.

    Adeola, F. (2000). Endangered community, enduring people: Toxic contamination, health,

    and adaptive responses in a local context. Environment and Behavior, 32, 209-249.

    Aldwin, C. & Stokols, D. (1988) The effects of environmental change on individuals and

    groups: Some neglected issues in stress research. Journal of Environmental

    Psychology, 8, 57-75.

    American Psychological Association. (2007). The Road to Resilience. Retrieved June 15,

    2009, from http://www.APAHelpCenter.org/resilience

    Aspinwall, L.G. (2010) Future-oriented thinking, proactive coping, and the management of

    potential threats to health and well-being. In S. Folkman (Ed) The Oxford handbook of

    stress, health and coping. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Aspinwall, L. G. (2004). Dealing with adversity: Self-regulation, coping, adaptation, and

    health. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology:

    Intraindividual processes (pp. 591-614). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive

    coping. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 417-436.

    Ball, M. (2008). Prevention psychologys forgotten mission. The Psychologist, 21, 602-603.

    Bandura, A. (1999). Social learning. In A. S. R. Manstead, M. Hewstone, S. T. Fiske, M. A.

    Hogg, H. T. Reis & G. R. Semin (Eds.), The Blackwell encyclopedia of social

    psychology (pp 576-581). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.

    Bauer, M. W. & Gaskell, G. (Eds). (2002). Biotechnology: The making of a global

    controversy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    27

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Baum, A., Fleming, R., & Davidson, L. M. (1983). Natural disaster and technological

    catastrophe. Environment and Behavior, 15, 333-354.

    Baum, A., & Fleming, I. (1993). Implications of psychological research on stress and

    technological accidents. American Psychologist, 48, 665-672.

    Bell, P. A., Greene, T. C., Fisher, J. D., & Baum, A. (2001). Environmental psychology (5th

    ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt College Publishers.

    Benight, C.C. (2004) Collective efficacy following a series of natural disasters. Anxiety,

    Stress and Coping, 17, 401-420.

    Berger, P. L. & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. Hammondsworth:

    Penguin.

    Bohm, G., Nerb, J., McDaniels, T., & Spada. (Eds). (2001). Environmental risks: Perception,

    evaluation, and management [Special Issue]. Research in Social Problems and Public

    Policy, 9, xi-xx.

    Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personality in the stress

    process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 890-902.

    Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the

    human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, 59,

    20-28.

    Bonnes, M., & Bonaiuto, M. (2002). Environmental psychology: From spatial-physical

    environment to sustainable development. In R.B. Bechtel & A. Churchman (Eds.),

    Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 28-54). New York: Wiley.

    Brewer, J.F. (2008) New directions in climate change vulnerability, impacts, and adaptation

    assessment: Summary of a workshop. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press.

    Brun, W. (1992). Cognitive components in risk perception: Natural versus manmade risks.

    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5, 117-132.

    28

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Burr, V. (2003). An introduction to social constructionism. Second edition. London, UK:

    Routledge.

    Carvalho, A. (2007). Ideological cultures and media discourses on scientific knowledge: Re-

    reading news on climate change. Public Understanding of Science, 16, 223-243.

    Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Chen, R.S., Boulding, E. & Schneider, S.H. (1983) (Eds) Social science research and climate

    change. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Company.

    Cutter, S.L., Emrich, C.T., Mitchell, J.T., Boruff, B.J., Gall, M., Schmidtlein, M.C. Burton,

    C.G. & Melton, G. (2006) The long road home: Race, class and recovery from

    Hurricane Katrina. Environment, 48(2) 8-20.

    Cutter, S.L, Boruff, B.J. & Shirley, W.L. (2003) Social vulnerability to environmental

    hazards. Social Science Quarterly, 84 (2) 242-261.

    Cvetkovich, G. & Earle, T. C. (Eds.). (1992). Public responses to environmental hazards.

    Journal of Social Issues, 48(4). [Entire Issue].

    Danesi, M. (2002). Understanding media semiotics. London: Arnold.

    Davey, G.C.L. & Wells, A. 2006) Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, assessment

    and treatment. Chichester: Wiley.

    Deaux, K, & Philogene, G. (Eds). (2001). Representations of the social. Oxford, UK:

    Blackwell Publishers.

    Douglas, M., & Wildavsky, A. (1982). Risk and Culture: An essay on the selection of

    technological and environmental dangers. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Earle. T. C. (2004). Thinking aloud about trust: A protocol analysis of trust in risk

    management. Risk Analysis, 24, 169-183.

    29

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Eckersley, R. (2008). Nihilism, fundamentalism, or activism: Three responses to fears of the

    apocalypse. The Futurist, January-February, 35-39.

    Edelstein, M. R. (2002). Contamination: The invisible built environment. In R. B.Bechtel &

    A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 559-588). New

    York: Wiley.

    Evans, G. W. & Cohen, S. (1991). Environmental Stress. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.),

    Handbook of Environmental Stress. (Vol. 1: pp. 571-610). Malabar, FL: Krieger.

    Evans, G. W. & Stecker, R. (2004). Motivational consequences of environmental stress.

    Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 143-165.

    Flick, U. (1998). Everyday knowledge in social psychology. In U. Flick (Ed.), The

    psychology of the social (pp. 41-59). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Gergen, K.J. (2009) An invitation to social construction. Second edition. London: Sage.

    Gifford, R. (2007). Environmental psychology: Principles and practice. Colville, WA:

    Optimal Books.

    Gifford, R., Steg, L. & Reser, J.P. (2011) Environmental psychology. In P.R. Martin, F.M.

    Cheung, M.C. Knowles, M Kyrios, L. Littlefied, J.B. Overmeier & J.M. Prieto (Eds)

    The IAAP handbook of applied psychology (pp 440-470). London: Blackwell

    Publishing.

    Gifford, R., Scannell, L., Kormos, C., Smolova, L., Biel, A., Boncu, S., et al. (2009).

    Temporal pessimism and spatial optimism in environmental assessments: An 18-

    nation study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29, 1-12.

    Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Lewis, F. M. (2002). Health behavior and health education:

    Theory, research and practice. San Francisco: Wiley & Sons.

    Gow, K., & Paton, D. (Eds.). (2008). The phoenix of natural disasters: Community resilience.

    New York: Nova Science Publishers.

    30

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Grauman, C.F. & Kruse, L. (1990) The environment: Social construction and psychological

    problems. In H.T. Himmelweit & G. Gaskell (Eds) Societal psychology (pp 212-229).

    London: Sage.

    Hansla, A., Gamble, A., Juliusson, A., & Garling, T. (2008). The relationships between

    awareness of consequences, environmental concern, and value orientations. Journal of

    Environmental Psychology, 28, 1-9.

    Holahan, C. J. (1982). Environmental Psychology. New York: Random House.

    Holahan, C. J. & Wandersman, A. (1991). The community psychology perspective in

    environmental psychology. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of

    environmental stress (Vol 1, pp. 827-861). Malabar, FL: Krieger.

    Hulme, M. (2009) Why we disagree about climate change?: Understanding controversy,

    inaction and opportunity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [IPCC]. (2007a). Climate Change 2007:

    Synthesis Report. Pachauri, R.K. & Reisinger, A. (Eds.). IPCC: Geneva, Switzerland.

    Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [IPCC]. (2007b). Glossary of Terms used in the

    IPCC Fourth Assessment Report: Working Group II (WG2). Retrieved June 15, 2009,

    from http://www.ipcc.ch/glossary/

    Janz, N. K., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief model: A decade later. Health

    Education Quarterly, 11(1), 1-47.

    Johndon-Cartee, K. S. (2005). News narratives and news framing: Constructing political

    reality. Lanhan, MD: Rowan & Littlefield.

    Kahan, D.M., Braman, D., Slovic, P., Gastil, J. & Cohen, G. (2007) The second national risk

    and culture study: Making sense of and making progress in the American culture

    war of fact. Report of the Cultural Cognition Project at Yale Law School. New Haven

    CN: Yale Law School.

    31

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N. & Marlon, J.R. (2010) Americas knowledge of climate change.

    New Haven, CT: Yale University, Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.

    Kasperson, J.X., Kasperson, R.E., Pidgeon, N. & Slovic, P. (2003) In N. Pidgeon, R.E.

    Kasperson & P. Slovic (Eds) The social amplification of risk (pp13-46). Cambridge,

    UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Keim, M. E. (2008). Building human resilience: The role of public health preparedness and

    response as an adaptation to climate change. American Journal of Preventive

    Medicine, 35, 508-516.

    Kluger, J. (2006b, April 3). Global warming. Time, 28-42.

    Kolbert, E. (2006). Field notes from a catastrophe: Man, nature and climate change. New

    York: Bloombury.

    Kunda, Z. (1990) The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108 (3) 480-498.

    Leary, M.R. Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology,

    58, 317-344.

    Lazarus, R.S. (1991) Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Lazarus, R. S., & Cohen, J. B. (1977). Environmental stress. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill

    (Eds), Human behavior and environment: Advances in theory and research (pp. 90-

    127). New York: Plenum.

    Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). (Eds.). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York:

    Springer.

    Leary, N., Adejuwon, J., Barros, V., Burton, I., Kulkarni, J. & Lasco, R. (2008) (Eds)

    Climate change and adaptation. London: Earthscan.

    Lehman, D., & Taylor, S. E. (1987). Date with an earthquake: Coping with a probable,

    unpredictable disaster. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 546-555.

    32

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Lepore, S. J. & Evans, G. W. (1996). Coping with multiple stressors in the environment. In

    M. Zeidner, & N.S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research and

    applications (pp. 350-377). New York: Wiley.

    Li, R.M. (2010) The importance of common metrics for advancing social science theory and

    research: A workshop summary. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

    Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk as feelings.

    Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267-286.

    Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., DeFrancea, D. M., Haynes, K. & Vincent, K. (2006). Does

    tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perception of climate change.

    Public Understanding of Science, 15, 435-457.

    Luthar, S. A. (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood

    adversities. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Macnaghten, P., & Urry, J. (1998). Contested natures. London: Sage.

    Marshall, R. D., Bryant, R. A., Amsel, L. Suh, E. J., Cook, J. M. & Neria, Y. (2007). The

    psychology of ongoing threat: Relative risk appraisal, the September 11 attacks, and

    terrorism-related fears. American Psychologist, 62, 304-316.

    Martin, I. (1964) Adaptation. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 35-44.

    Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American

    Psychologist, 56(3), 227-238.

    Moscovici, S. (2000). Social representations: Explorations in social psychology. Cambridge:

    Polity Press.

    Moser, S.C. & Dilling, L. (2004) Making climate change hot: Communicating the urgency

    and challenge of climate change. Environment, 46 (10) 32-46.

    Morrissey, S. A., & Reser, J. P. (2007). Natural disasters, climate change and mental health

    considerations in rural Australia. Australian Journal of Rural Health, 15, 120-125.

    33

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    National Research Council. (1992). Global environmental change: Understanding the human

    dimensions. Stern, P. C., Young, O. R., & Druckman, D. (Eds.). Washington, DC:

    National Academies Press.

    National Research Council. (2009). Understanding and responding to climate change.

    Washington, DC: National Research Council/The National Academies.

    Newman, R. (2005). APAs resilience initiative. Professional Psychology: Research and

    Practice, 36, 227-229.

    Norris, F.H., Stevens, S.P., Pfefferbaum, B., Wyche, K.F. & Pfefferbaum, R.L. (2008)

    Community resilience as a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for

    disaster readiness. American Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 127-150.

    Olson, R., & Rejeski, D. (2005). Environmentalism and the technologies of tomorrow:

    Shaping the next industrial revolution. Washington, DC: Island Press.

    ONeill, S.J. & Hulme, M. (2009) An iconic approach for representing climate change.

    Global Climate Change, 19, 402-410.

    ORiordan, T. (Ed.). (1995). Perceiving environmental risks. New York: Academic Press.

    Ornstein, R. & Ehrlich, P. (1990) New world: New mind. New York: Simon & Schuster. Pachauri, R.K. (2008) Foreword. In N Leary, J. Adejuwon, V. Barros, I. Burton, J. Kulkarni,

    & R. Lasco (Eds) Climate change and adaptation (xiv). London: Earthscan.

    Peek, L. A. & Miletti, D. S. (2002). The history & future of disaster research. In R. B.

    Bechtel, R. B., & A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp.

    511-524). New York: Wiley.

    Pettenger, M.E. (2007). The social construction of climate change. London: Ashgate.

    Pidgeon, N. F., Kasperson, R. K. & Slovic, P. (2003). The social amplification of risk.

    Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    34

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Pidgeon, N., Lorenzoni, I., & Poortinga, W. (2008). Climate change or nuclear power No

    thanks! A quantitative study of public perceptions and risk framing in Britain. Global

    Environmental Change, 18, 69-85.

    Pielke, R., Prins, G.P., Rayner, S. & Sarewitz, D. (2007) Lifting the taboo on adaptation.

    Nature, 445, 597-598.

    Reser, J. P. (2004). The experience of natural disasters: Psychological perspectives and

    understandings. In J. P. Stoltman, J. Lidstone & L. M. DeChano (Eds.), International

    perspectives on natural disasters: Occurrence, mitigation, and consequences (pp.

    369-384). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Reser, J. P. (2010). A psychological perspective on thinking globally and acting locally in

    the context of climate change. Keynote address to the InternationalCongressof

    AppliedPsychologyJuly1116,Melbourne.

    Reser, J.P., Pidgeon, N., Spence, A., Bradley, G.; Glendon, I. & Ellul, M. (2010) Public

    perceptions of climate change in Australia and Great Britain. Gold Coast, Qld:

    Griffith University, Griffith Climate Response Program.

    Reser, J. P. & Morrissey, S. A. (2008). Situating and framing individual and community

    experience and response: A psychological perspective. In K. Gow & D. Paton (Eds.),

    The phoenix of natural disasters: Community resilience (pp. 47-72). New York: Nova

    Science Publishers.

    Reyes, G. & Jacobs, G.A. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of international disaster psychology

    (Vol.1-4). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Rogers, R. W., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (1997). Protection motivation theory. In D. S. Gochman

    (Ed.), Handbook of health behavior research 1: Personal and social determinants

    (pp. 113-132). New York: Plenum Press.

    35

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Scheier, M. F., Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with stress: Divergent

    strategies of optimists and pessimists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

    51(6), 1257-1264.

    Schipper, E.L.F. & Burton, I. (2009) (Eds) The Earthscan reader on adaptation to climate

    change. London: Earthscan.

    Schoon, I. (2006). Risk and resilience: Adaptations in changing times. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Schmuck, P., & Schultz, W. P. (Eds.). (2002). Psychology of sustainable development.

    Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Shapiro, D. H., Schwartz, C. E., & Astin, I. A. (1996). Controlling ourselves, controlling our

    world. American Psychologist, 51, 1213-1230.

    Sjoberg, L. (2006). As time goes by: The beginnings of social and behavioral science risk

    research. Journal of Risk Research, 9(6) 601-604.

    Slovic, P. (2000). The perception of risk. London: Earthscan.

    Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., et al. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings:

    Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24, 311-322

    Smith, N.W. & Joffe, H. (2009) Climate change in the British press: The role of the visual.

    Journal of Risk Research, 12, 647-663.

    Snyder, C. R. (2002). Hope theory: Rainbows in the mind. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 249-

    275.

    Spence, A., Pidgeon, N., & Uzzell, D. (2008). Climate change psychologys contribution.

    The Psychologist, 21, 108-11.

    Spratt, D., & Sutton, P. (2008). Climate code red: The case for a sustainable emergency.

    Fitzroy, Melbourne: Friends of the Earth.

    36

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Stern, P.C. & Gardner, G. (1981) Psychological research and energy policy. American

    Psychologist, 36, 329-342.

    Stokols, D. (1978). Environmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 253-295.

    Stokols, D., Misra, S., Hall, K., Taylor, B. & Moser, R. (2008) The ecology of team science:

    Understanding contextual influences on transdisciplinary collaboration. American

    Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35, (2, Suppl. 1) S96-S115.

    Stokols, D., Misra, S., Runnerstrom, M. G. & Hipp, A. (2009). Psychology in an age of

    ecological crisis. American Psychologist, 64, 181-193.

    Taylor, K. M., & Shepperd, J. A. (1998). Bracing for the worst: Severity, testing, and

    feedback timing as moderators of the optimistic bias. Personality and Social

    Psychology Bulletin, 24, 915-926.

    Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation.

    American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173.

    Taylor, S. E. (2006). Health psychology (6th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological

    perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193-210.

    Taylor, S. E. & Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health.

    Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 377-401.

    Townsend, E., Clarke, D. D. & Travis, B. (2004). Effects of context and feelings on

    perceptions of genetically modified food. Risk Analysis, 24, 1369-1384.

    Uzzell, D. L. (2000). The psycho-spatial dimensions of global environmental problems.

    Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20(4), 307-318.

    Uzzell, D. (2004). From local to global: A case of environmental hyperopia. IHDP Update, 4,

    6-7.

    37

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Vaughan, E. (1993). Individual and cultural differences in adaptation to environmental risks.

    American Psychologist, 48, 673-680.

    VandenBos, G.R. & Bryant, B.K. (1987) (Eds.), Cataclysms, crises, and catastrophes.

    Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Wagner, W. & Hayes, N. (2005) Everyday discourse and common sense: The theory of social

    representations. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Wapner, S., Demick, J., Yamamoto, T. & Minami, H. (2000) (Eds) Theoretical perspectives

    in environment-behavior research: Underlying assumptions, research problems, and

    methodologies. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Weber, E. U. (2006). Evidence-based and description-based perceptions of long-term risk:

    Why global warming does not scare us (yet). Climatic Change, 77, 103-120.

    Weiner, B. (1995) Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct.

    New York: Guilford Press.

    Weinstein, N. D. & Kline, W. (1996). Unrealistic optimism: Present and future. Journal of

    Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 1-8.

    Weinstein, N. H. (Ed.). (1988). Taking care: Understanding and encouraging self-protective

    behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Wheaton, B. (1999). The nature of stressors. In A. V. Horwitz, & T. L. Scheid (Eds.), A

    handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (pp.

    176-197). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    White, R.W. (1974) Strategies of adaptation: An attempt at systematic description. In G.

    Coehlo, D.Hamburg & J. Adams (Eds) Coping and adaptation (pp 47-68). New York:

    Basic Books.

    Whitmarsh, L., ONeill, S. & Lorenzoni, I. (Eds) (2010) Engaging the public with climate

    change. London: Earthscan.

    38

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    39

    Yeager, D.S., Larson, S.B. & Krosnick, J. (2010) Measuring Americans issue priorities: A

    new version of the most important problem question reveals more concern about

    global warming and the environment. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University, Woods

    Institute for the Environment. http:woods.stanford.edu/research/surveys.html.

    Winkel, G., Saegert, S. & Evans, G.W. (2010) An ecological perspective on theory, methods,

    and analysis in environmental psychology: Advances and challenges. Journal of

    Environmental Psychology, 29, 318-328.

  • PsychologyandGlobalClimateChange

    Interpretive and motivational responses

    and processes Attributions Affective responses Motivational processes

    Coping appraisal (aka secondary appraisals)

    Assessing anticipated effects of possible responses and capacity to respond

    Self-efficacy Response-efficacy Cost-benefits Situation constraints Strength of community

    Threat appraisal (aka primary or risk appraisals)

    Assessing whether impacts on self, family, community, culture, and society exceed resources

    Perceived probability of risk Perceived severity Perceived vulnerability & resilience

    Continuous & Dynamic Process

    Proactive and reactive coping response

    Intra-individual responses (examples)

    environmental numbness cognitive reappraisal such as

    altering risk assessment

    emotion regulation

    Behavioral responses (examples)

    seeking information seeking interpersonal or community

    social support

    compensatory behavior such as changing structure of habitat Mitigation Civic participation

    Direct, indirect & mediated threats and

    impacts

    Discrete (e.g., Cataclysmic) events

    Chronic events including

    ambient stressors

    Psychosocial Impacts

    on Individuals and

    communities

    40

    Examples of Moderators of each step in the process

    Characteristics of Individual Resilience and vulnerability Prior experience Dispositional optimist

    Characteristics of community Resilience and vulnerability Social capital and social networks

    Media representations, social construal & reactions

    Cultural norms Neighborhood organizations

    Characteristics of incident and physical environment

    Severity & intensity of impacts Region specific biophysical impacts Proximity & exposure to impacts

    Figure 1: Psychological processes that influence adaptation and coping with climate change.