corner - university of toronto t-space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. i...

130
This manuscript has been reproduoed from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of cornputer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard rnargins, and irnproper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduoed by sedioning the original, beginning at the upper lefi-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced f o n at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this mpy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographie prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. Bell & Howell Information and Leaming 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1346 USA 800-521 -0600

Upload: others

Post on 19-Jun-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

This manuscript has been reproduœd from the microfilm master. UMI films the

text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and

dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of

cornputer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy

submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and

photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard rnargins, and irnproper alignment

can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and

there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright

material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduœd by sedioning

the original, beginning at the upper lefi-hand corner and continuing from left to

right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in

one exposure and is included in reduced f o n at the back of the book.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced

xerographically in this mpy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographie

prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for

an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

Bell & Howell Information and Leaming 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1346 USA

800-521 -0600

Page 2: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons
Page 3: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPEECHLANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT

AND YOUNG ADULTHOOD ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOUR

David Aaron Rebuck, Hon. B.Sc.

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirernents for the degree of Master of Science, Graduate Department of Community Health,

Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto

O Copyright by David Aaron Rebuck, 1998

Page 4: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

National Libmry 1+1 of Canada Bibliothèque nationale du Canada

Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographie Services services bibliographiques

395 Wellington Street 395. rue Wellington Ottawa ON K I A ON4 Ottawa ON K I A ON4 Cana& Canada

The author has granted a non- L'a~lfeur a accordé une licence non exclusive licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la National Library of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou copies of this thesis in microfoq vendre des copies de cette thèse sous paper or electronic formats. la fonne de microficl?e/fïh, de

reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

The author retains ownership of the L'auteur conserve la propriété du copyright in this thesis. Neither the droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. thesis nor substantid extracts fiom it Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otherwise de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimes reproduced without the author's ou autrement reproduits sans son permission. autorisation.

Page 5: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EARLY CHlLDHOOD SPEECH/LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT AND YOUNG ADULTHOOD

ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOUR

David Aaron Rebuck, Hon. %.Sc.

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science, Graduate Departrnent of Cornrnunity Health, Faculty of Medicine,

University of Toronto.

ABSTRACT

This investigation used data from the Ottawa Language Study to examine the

relationship between early chiIdhood speechnanguage impairments and young adulthood

antisocial behaviour. At age 5, 284 chiidren were categonzed as either speech-only

impaired, language irnpaired or controls. While accounting for a large variety of age 5

covariates, their age 19 antisocial behaviour was assessed. Children who were

Ianguage impaired had the worst outcome. These children were at significant risk for a

diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (odds ratio=3.66, 95%CL: 1.49-9.01) and were

also considered by parents and/or teachers to be significantly more antisocial than those

without a history of such im pairrnents. However, when antisocial behaviour was

measured in terms of criminal activity or when subjects rated their own antisocial

behaviour, no significant differences were found arnong the age 5 speechtlanguage

groups. Despite this, these findings are the first to report the long-term, young adulthood

psychiatrie consequences of early childhood language impairments.

Page 6: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

I am especially grateful to Dr. Joseph H. Beitchrnan for the use of the Ottawa

Language Study (OLS) data on which this study is based as well as for his guidance and

supervision of this thesis. I am also indebted to my thesis cornmittee members: Prof.

Susan J. Bondy, for her patience, assistance and support and Prof. Michael Escobar for

his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness

to Beth Wilson, whose knowledge of the OLS data was invaluable to me throughout the

development and completion of this work. Finally, I acknowledge the constructive

feedback provided by rny thesis examiners: Prof. Mary Chipman, Prof. Bruce Ferguson

and Prof. Rhonda Love.

Page 7: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

TABLE OF CONTENTS

-. ABSTRACT ............ ...,. ............................................................................................. t I

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.. ......................................................................................... iii ... LIST OF TABLES.. ................................................................................................... vil I

UST OF FIGURES .................................................................................................... x

LIST OF APPENDICIES.. ................................................................................... .... xi

1. Introduction ......................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Objective ............. ... .......................................................................... 1

1.2 Study Rationale and Strengths ....................................................... 1

2. Background ........................................................................................................ 2

2.1 Speech and Language Irnpairments .............................................. 3

............................................................................ 2.1.1 Definitions ..3

................................................. ...................... 2.1 -2 Prievalence ,. 4

2.1 .3 Problems in Defining Speechllanguage

Impairments ... .................................................................... 6

2.2 Longitudinal Outcome of Speechllanguage Impairnent ......... 7

2.2.1 Behavioual Findings .......................................................... 7

2.3 Relevance ............................................................................................. 1 O

2.4 Evidence that Suggests an Association Between

SpeechRanguage Impairment and Antisocial Behaviour.. 1 1

2.4.1 Speechllanguage Impairment as an

......................................................... Antecedent Variable 1 1

2.4.2 Speechllanguage Impairment, ADHD and Antisocial

Behaviour ............................................................................ 1 3

2.5 From Early Childhood Impairment to Young Adulthood Outcome:

...................................................... Possible Mechanisms 14

Page 8: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2.6 The Present Study .............................................................................. 1 5

2.6.1 Data Source and Conceptual Framework ..................... 15

2.6.2 Antisocial Behaviour: Conceptualizations ..................... 18

2.6.3 Risk Factors to be Controlled ........................................ 19

2.6.4 Hypothesis ............................................................................ 28

3 . Methods ............................................................................................................... 29 3.1 The Ottawa Language Study ......................... .. ............................... 29

3.2 lndependent Variable of Primary Interest: Speechllanguage

.............................................................................................. Status 2 9

3.3 Covariates ............................................................................................ 30

............................................................................ 3.3.1 Intelligence 30

3.3.2 Socioeconomic Status (SES) .......................................... 31

3.3.3 Childhood Adversity ........................................................... 31

3.3.4 Inattention, Hyperactivity and Conduct Problems ....... 33

3.3.5 Speechllang uage Treatrnent ......................................... - 3 3

......................................................................... 3.4 Dependent Variables 33

3.4.1 Antisocial Behaviour as a Diagnosis .............................. 33

3.4.2 Antisocial Behaviour as a Legal Entity ........................... 34

3.4.3 Antisocial Behaviour as a Dimension ............................. 34

.. .................................................................... 3.5 Statistical Analysis ... 37

3.5.1 Data Exploration ............................................................... 37

......................................................... 3.5.2 Logistic Regression 38

.......................................... 3.5.3 Linear Regression .............. ... -40

3.6 Missing Data ........................................................................................ 40

...................................................... 3.6.1 Independent Variables 4 0

................................ ............ . 3.6.2 Dependent Variables ... ....... 4 0

.......... 3.7 Descriptive Statistics ; .......................................................... 41

............................................................................ 3.8 Power Calculations 41

......................................... 3.9 Tests of Reproducibility of the Results 42

Page 9: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3.9.1 Independent Variables ....................................................... 42

.......................................................... 3.9.2 Dependent Variables 44

..................... 3.9.3 Reproducibility of the Results: Surnmary -44

4 . Results 1: Prirnary Analyses ...................... .. ................................................ 46 . .

4.1 Summary Stahstics ................ .. .................................................... 46

4.2 Conceptualization 1: Diagnoses of ASPD ....................................... 48

4.2.1 Unadjusted Resuits ............................................................. 48

4.2.2 Adjusted Results ............................................................... 49

4.2.3 Summary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the

Analysîs of ASPD ................................................... 52

4.3 Conceptualization II: Criminal Activity ............................................ 52

4.3.1 Unadjusted Results ............................................................. 52

.............................................................. 4.3.2 Adjusted Results 53

4.3.3 Sumrnary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the

Analysis of Criminal Activity ........................................... 56

4.4 Conceptualization III: Antisocial Behaviour as a Dimension .... 57

............................................... 4.4.1 Non-su bject Measures 57

4.4.2 Subject Measures ............................................................ 6 1

4.5 Summary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results ........................ 64

5 . Results II: Supplementary Analyses ............................................................ 67

5.1 The Composite Nature of the Dimensional Measure of

Antisocial Behaviour ..................................................................... 67

5.2 The Composite Nature of the Non-subject Measure of

Antisocial Behaviour .......... .... ................................................. 69

5.3 Group Differences in Subject-perceived Social Suppo R ......... 71

5.4 ASPD and Subject Measures of Antisocial Behaviour .............. 71

Page 10: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

.......................................................................................................... . 6 Discussion 74

6.1 Ovenriew of Findings ....................................................................... 74

6.2 Poorer Outcome Among those Language versus Speech-only

Impaired ........................................................................................... 76

6.3 Evidence of Antisocial Personality Disorder versus Criminal

Behaviour ............... ,., .................................................................... -77

6.4 Subject versus Non-subject Measures ..................................... 79

6.5 Confounding ......................................................................................... 82

.................... 6.5.1 The Covariates as Confounding Variables 83

......................................... 6.5.2 Ancillary Findings ............ .... 84

6.6 Implications .......................................................................................... 86

6.6.1 Language-based Problems and Vulnerability for

.............. Antisocial Behaviour .............................. ...... 86

6.6.2 Implications for Preventi on ............................................. 88

6.7 Study Limitations ................................................................................ 89

......................................................................... 6.7.1 Missing Data 89

6.7.2 lntegrated Analysis of the Data for Both Males and

............-...........*.........*.. ............... ..................... Fernales .,, ... 90

................... 6.7.3 Merging of Parent and Teacher Measures 90

6.8 Recommendations .....................................~....................................... 90

6.9 Summary .............................................................................................. 91

.................................,,.............. .............................. REFERENCES , -92

APPENDIX A Missing Data . Tables A1 and A2 ............................................ 103

APPENDW B Comparisons of subjects with and without missing time-3

antisocial outcome data . Tabfes B I OB5 .................................. 106

APPENDIX C Multicollinearity and Diagnostics . Tables Cl-C6 .................. 112

vii

Page 11: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

UST OF TABLES

Table 3.1 :

Table 3.2:

Table 3.3:

Table 4.1 :

Table 4.2:

Table 4.3:

Table 4.4:

Table 4.5:

Table 4.6:

Table 4.7:

Table 4.8:

Table 4.9:

Table 4.1 0:

Table 4.1 1 :

Correlations (and their p-values) of the Aggression and Delinquency Subscale T scores and raw scores for the CBCL, TRF and YSR instruments, respectively ........... ..,, ..........~........................................ 36

Correlations (and their p-values) of the parent-rated (from the CBCL) and teacher-rated (frorn the TRF) rnean antisocial measures for the T scores and raw scores,respectively ....................................... 37

Approximate decile groups for the Conners variables of Hyperactivity, Conduct and Inattention, the range of values within each group and

............................................................................................. their size 43

Descriptive statistics (and standard deviations) for the speech1 language groups on time-1 sample size, each of the time-1 covariates, and time-3 speechnanguage treatment variables. ........... 47

Unadjusted proportions with diagnoses of ASPD for each speechllanguage group .....................~............................................... 48

Odds ratios (Y) and their 95% CLs for the diagnosis of an ASPD for the speech-only impaired and language irnpaired groups unadjusted for any covariates .............~............................................. 48

Results from the univanate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate and for each alternately defined covariate for the diagnosis of an ASPD.. ...................................................................... 50

Relevant odds ratios and their 95% CLs for the variables in ............................................................................................... Model 1 51

Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted speechllanguage odds ratio estimates and their 95%CLs for the analysis of ASPD ..... 5 2

Unadjusted proportions of subjects who reported some fonn of criminal behaviour for each speechllanguage group ......................... 53

Odds ratios (Y) and their 95% CLs for the occurrence of criminal behaviour for the speech-only irnpaired and language irnpaired groups unadjusted for any covariates ............................................ 53

Results from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate for the occurrence of criminal behaviour ........................... 54

Odds ratios and their 95% CLs for the variables in Model2a ............ 55

Relevant odds ratios and their 95% CLs for the variables in .................................................. ..................................... Model 2b .. 55

S..

Vlll

Page 12: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.12: Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model Sa) speechtlanguage odds ratio estimates and their 95%CLs for the

* .

analysis of criminal activity .....................,.....+~~.................. 5 ï

Table 4.13: Mean non-subject measured antisocial T ratings for each speechllanguage impaired group, unadjusted for any covariates ..... 57

Table 4.14: Results from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate for the non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour ...... 58

Table 4.15: Adjusted means for each speech/language group from Mode1 3a .... 59

Table 4.16: Adjusted means for each speechftanguage group frorn Mode! 3b .... 60

Table 4.17: Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model 3a) speechtlanguage mean antisocial behaviour estimates for the analysis of non-subject measures ..................................................... 61

Table 4.18: Mean subject measured antisocial T ratings for each speech/ .................. language impaired group, unadjusted for any covanates 61

Table 4.19: Results from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate for the subject measures of antisocial behaviour .............. 62

Table 4.20: Adjusted means for each speechtianguage group from Model 4...... 63

Table 4.21: Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model 3a) speech/ language mean antisocial behaviour estirnates for the analysis of subject measures ............................................................ 64

Table 4.22 Sumrnary table of the results unadjusted for any of the covanates for the dichotomous outcomes (diagnoses of ASPD and occurrence of criminal behaviour) and dimensional outcomes (non-subject and subject measures), respectively.. ...................................................... 65

Table 4.23: Summary table of the results adjusted for the covariates for the dichotomous outcornes (diagnoses of ASPD and occurrence of criminal behaviour) and dimensional outcomes (non-subject and subject measures), respectively ....................................................... 66

Table 5.1: SpeechAanguage group mean T scores and raw scores for the subject-measured Delinquency and Aggression Subscales ............ .68

Table 5.2: Speech/language group mean T scores and raw scores for the ...... non-subject measured Delinquency and Aggression Su bscales 68

Table 5.3: Subject-measured mean antisocial behaviour T scores for those with and without a diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) ............................................................................................... 71

Table 5.4: Subject measures of antisocial behaviour for those with and without an ASPD and the associated Pearson correlation coefficients for each speechnanguage group ................................... 72

Page 13: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 2.1 : Follow-up and attrition in the Ottawa Language Study ...................... 17

Figure 5.1 : Adjusted subject and non-subject measures of antisocial be haviour by time-1 speechflanguage grou p .................. ..... .... -70

Page 14: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

UST OF APPENDICES

. ................................................. APPENDIX A: Missing Data Tables A I and A2 103

APPENDK B: Cornparisons of subjects with and without missing time-3 antisocial outcome data . Tables BI-85 ................... ,.. ............. 106

APPENDiX C: Multicollinearity and Diagnostics . Tables Ci .C6 ..... ..... ...................... 112

Page 15: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

1. Introduction

1.1 Objective

This thesis made use of data from the Ottawa Language Study (OLS), a

cornmunity-based, three-wave panel investigation. Children were recruited into the study

ai age five and were categorized as either speech-only impaired, language impaired or

as controls. The present investigation adds to the literature by exploring the relationship

between early childhood speechltanguage impairnent and subsequent antisocial

behaviour in young adulthood. Specifically, it assesses whether speechllanguage

impairrnents, identifieci in children 5 years of age. are associated with antisocial

behaviour in young adults 19 years of age.

1.2 Study Rationale and Strengths

The rationale and strengths of this study are many and each justified its

implernentation. First, the study is unique as it is the first to address the long-term

psychiatric outcome of early chiidhood speechnanguage impairment. Second, it

addresses this outcome during a critical period of Iife, young adulthood, during which

success and failure have long-term consequences. Third, multiple measures of the

antisocial outcorne are used, thus allowing for the cornparison of different data sources

and different charactenzations of antisocial behaviour. Fourth, a large variety of possible

confounding variables are controlled, thus strengthening the conclusions drawn from the

results. Fifth, inferences frorn this study are strengthened by the nature of the data on

which it is based. For example, the data represents a community (rather than chic)

sample of children, thereby improving its extemal validity. As well, the OLS followed both

well defined speechllanguage impaired and control subjects over a period of 14 years - one of the longest follow-up periods in the speechnanguage literature.

Page 16: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2. Background Language is a means of representing and cornmunicating information. In young

children, the development of language and thought are believed to develop in a parallel

and reciprocal fashion with each contributing to the growth and evolution of the other

(Paul & Jackson, 1993). One of the hallmarks of psychopathology, however, is irrational

thought (Crittenden, 1996) and that children with impaired speech or language would be

at riçk for psychopathology seems likely. Thus, the effects of speech and language

impairment could be particularly handicapping in two ways: first, by interfering with

normal cognitive development and promoting the onset of irrational thought and

psychopathology, and second, by isolating the child, restricting social, educational and

occupational opportunities which may also increase the risk of subsequent

psychopathology.

Indeed, the CO-existence of speech and language impairment and psychiatric

disorders has k e n well docurnented and empirical research continues to show that this

association occurs ai a rate greater than chance (Beitchman et al., 1994). This has been

observed in a number of cross-sectional and retrospective studies that find large

proportions of speechllanguage irnpaired children with psychiatric disorder and

conversely, in studies that find large proportions of psychiatrically disturbed children with

speechllanguage disorders (Howlin 8 Rutter, 1987). Moreover, prospective longitudinal

studies that have followed their subjects through to middle or late childhood have shown

that speechllanguage impairment puts children at greater risk for subsequently

developing psychiatric disorders of al1 kinds (Baker & Cantwell, 1987; Cohen, Devine,

Horodezky, Lipsett, & Issacson, 1993).

Unfortunately, there have been no studies of speechllanguage impairment that

have explored the long-term psychiatrie outcome of these subjects in young adulthood.

This period in life is a pivotal transition point in the passage from adolescence to

adulthood during which decisions have Iifelong consequences. This study addresses

this conspicuous lack of information on the long-term psychiatric outcome of

speechtlanguage irnpaired children. Specifically, it explores the relationship between

early childhood speechllanguage impairment and young adulthood antisocial behaviour.

Page 17: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2.1 Speech and Language Irnpainnents

2.1.1 Definitions

Perhaps the rnost general definition of speech and language irnpairment was

given by Pinker (1995). He describes them as "any speech or language syndrome in

which the person fails to develop properly the requisite skills and the blarne cannot be

pinned on hearing deficits, low intelligence, social problems or difficulty controlling the

speech muscles" (p. 481). This definition is useful in that it highlights the nature of these

impainnents as k i n g specific to the domain of speech or language rather than being the

resuIt of some other physical, neurological, psychological or intelledual deficit.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IIIR, Arnerican Psychiatric Association,

1987) also recognizes speech and language disorders and provides sirnilar definitions. It

describes these disorders as usually first evident in infancy, childhood or adolescence

and therefore refers to thern as "developrnental disorders" characterized by "the

inadequate development of specific language or speech skills and that are not due to

demonstrable physical or neurological disorders, a pervasive developmental disorder

(such as Autism), mental retardation or deficient educationai opportunities" (p. 3940).

The essential features of a child with only an articulation disorder is a constant

failure to produce the correct speech sounds at the developrnentally appropriate age. It

is manifested by frequent misarticulations, substitutions or omissions. The rnost

frequently rnisarticulated sounds are those acquired late in the deveioprnental sequence:

r, sh, th, f, z, 1 and ch. This is in contrast to the linguistic features in children with

language delay which include, among others, sirnplified and Iimited grammatical

structures, unusual word orders and difficulty in the acquisition of new words (DSM-IIIR,

American Psychiatric Association, 1987).

Both of the above definitions raise important issues. First, there is an important

distinction between disorders in the ability to express language and disorders in the

ability to produce speech. The proficiency and clarity of speech is only one aspect of

language expression. As well, the [iterature has shown differences in the amount and

severity of social, emotional, academic and behavioural problems associated with varying

degrees of expressive language ability (Beitchman, Nair, Clegg, Ferguson, & Patel,

1986b). These differences have been obsewed between both normal children and

children with speech-only problems and children with speech-only problems and children

with language-only problems. For exarnple, children with unclear speech articulation are

known to be more Iikely to have slow language development and behaviour problems

than children with clear speech irrespective of IQ (Morely, 1965; Silva, Justin, McGee, &

Williams, 1984) but there is also evidence that a "pure articulation disorder" is less likely

3

Page 18: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

than a "pure language disorder" to be associated with psychiatn'c and psychosocial

problems (Cantwell & Baker, 1980; Baker & Cantwell, 1982a).

Second, these definitions raise an issue conceming the relationship between

language ability and intelligence. Thought (or cognition) can be regarded as the mind's

ability to integrate and manipulate information, (Crittenden, 1996) while intelligence, a

closely related concept, can be regarded as a rneasure of this ability (Paul & Jackson,

1993). Language and thought are closely intertwined in development, so too are

language and intelligence. Indeed, the items in tests of language and intelligence are

often the same and, while some language tests appear to assess language, they also

require the child's sustained attention and involve a certain amount of reasoning (Silva,

1987). Likewise, many IQ test items involve language comprehension and expression.

For example, the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale and Reynell Developrnental Language

Scale correlate highly (0.6-0.7) and both show similar correlations with both WSC-IQ

(Wechsler, 1974) and reading tests given several years later (Silva, Bradshaw, &

Spears, 1978). For these reasons, intelligence may easily confound language ability and

must therefore be controlled in speech and language disorder studies. However, much

of the literature has failed to incorporate 10 when defining speech and ianguage impaired

groups thus Iimiting the intemal validtty of their findings.

Third, as language includes both the ability to express and to comprehend

information, the DSM-IIIR krrther divides specific language disorders into developmental

expressive and receptive subcategories. Although this distinction seerns intuitive, much

of the early Iiterature does not report data for each type of disorder, but rather refers

only to a "language impaired" group perhaps consisting of children with one or the other

or both impairrnents grouped together (Silva, 1987). Although many recent studies

continue to group subjects in this way, a number of studies that have used language

disorder subgroups have found differences between them (e-g. Silva, Williams, & McGee,

1987; Beitchman, Hood, Rochon, & Peterson, 1989a; Beitchman, Kruidenier, Clegg, Hood,

& Comadini, 1989b).

2.1.2 Prevalence

Many of the early epidemiological studies of speech and language disorders, as

reviewed by Mac Keith and Rutter (1972), were handicapped by the lack of standardized

tests of speech/language development. Instead, they generally used criterion-referenced

approaches and assessed aspects such as "intelligibility" of expressive language. They

also tended to be based on expressive language only, as language comprehension is

more dificult to assess (Silva, 1987). In recent years, however, standardized tests have

Page 19: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

becorne available (eg. Test of Adolescent and Adult Language, Harnmill, Brown, Larsen,

& Wiederhold, 1994). Furthemore, atmost al1 studies of disordered language focus on

preschool children (0-5 years of age) because it is during this period that language

develops dramatically. Both clinic- and cornmunity-based studies comprise the literature

on prevalence. Since clinic-based studies are lirnited in their extemal validity, only

community-based epidemiological surveys will be reviewed.

Studies conducted over the last three decades have observed prevalence rates

of speecManguage disorders ranging from approximately 3%-25%. The most notable

studies include the Waltham Forest Study (Stevenson & Richman, 1976), the Dunedin

Study (Silva, McGee, & Williams, 1983), the Newcastle study (Fundudis, Kolvin, &

Garside, 1979) and the Ottawa Language Study (OLS) (Beitchman, Nair, Clegg, & Patel,

1986a). The Waltham Forest Study reported a prevalence rate of expressive language

delay of 3.1 O h . The authors used standardized tests which assessed a 1 -in-4 sample of

3 year old children in an outer London (UK) borough, the criterion for delayldeviance

being that the child fell at least 6 months below the chronologid age nom. IQ, however,

was not controlled in this study.

The Newcastle study reported a 4% prevalence rate of "speech retardation"

which was based on the sample of al1 children born in Newcastle Upon-Tyne, UK.

However, this figure is lirnited by its initial definition of speech retardation: "the failure to

use three or more words stning together to make some sort of sense by the age of 36

months." This definition is insensitive to the extent that children with only the more

severe forms of delay would have been identified. ChiIdren with mild and moderate

articulation problems, as well as those with disorders primarily of syntax, would have

been missed.

The Dunedin study measured different subtypes of language disorder. The

investigators found a prevalence rate of 3% in delayed verbal comprehension, a 2.5%

rate in delayed verbal expression, a 3% rate for those delayed in both and an overall

prevalence rate of 8.4%. These figures were based on al1 infants born at one maternity

hospital, however, and the survey iesults may not have been representative of the

population as there were large social class and family status differences between the

children in the sample and those not included.

Beitchman et al. (1986a) in the OLS found a speech-only impairment prevalence

rate of 6.4%, a language-only impairmentprevalence rate of 8.04%. a speech and

language impairment rate of 4.56% and an overall prevalence rate of 19.0%. These

figures were based on a 1-in-3 sample of al1 5 year old English-speaking children from

the Ottawa-Carlton region. The investigators used a 2-stage series of standardized tests

Page 20: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

where cases had to have failed (greater than or equal to 1 standard deviation below the

mean on) any 1 of 4 tests, or failed (greater than or equal to 2 standard deviations below

the mean on) any subtest of the Test of Language Development (Newcomer & Harnmill,

1 977).

Another investigation, the lowa Epidemiologic Study of Specific Language

Impairment (SLI) (Tomblin, Buckwater, Zhang, O'Brian, & Smith, in press; Tomblin, 1996)

represents a well-controlled, population sampled prevalence study of SLI. Among the

rnonolingual English-speaking kindergarten population, 7,218 children (age 5) were

sampled via a stratified cluster sampling of children living in lowa or western Illinois.

Among those sampled, 216 children (3%) were diagnosed as SLI. This diagnosis was

based on 1) a language assessrnent battery of standardized tests; 2) performance-IQ

estimates; 3) a pure tone hearing test, and 4) parental reports of developmental, sensory

and neuromotor problems. A diagnosis of SLI consisted of an oral language impairment in

association with normal performance-IQ (>87), normal hearing and no parental report of

mental retardation, autism, visual impairment, cerebral palsy or severe head injury.

A final study worthy of mention is one performed by Tuomi and lvanoff (1977).

These investigators found a prevalence of speech articulation problems to be 24.5% and

of language problems to be 6.7% among almost 900 kindergarten and grade 1 children in

public schools near London, Ontario. These figures were based on a 2-stage procedure,

the second of which made use of a standardized test. However, the initial screening

stage was quite subjective (college students judged a child's response to the question

"What do you do at home?" as well as the descriptions of three pictures). Because the

critical screening stage was not standardized, it is difficult to be confident of these

results.

2.1 3 Problems in Defining SpeechILanguage lmpairments

The range of the observed prevalence rates of speech and language disorders is

quite large (3%-25%). It is at least partially due to the fact that there is Iittle or no

agreement as to how exactly to define and operationalize these irnpairments. The criteria

provided by the DSM-IIIR are suffkiently vague that they can only be used as a guide for

investigatorç who choose to define the disorders on their own. For exarnple, the DSM's

diagnostic criteria for Developrnental Receptive Language Disorder States that a child

"should score substantially lower in a standardized test of language reception than in a

standardized test of intellectual ability" (p. 48). The DSM does not, however, state what

is considered "substantially fower" nor does it state which standardized tests are

acceptable.

Page 21: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Because of these reasons, investigators have been left with much fiexibility in

how they regard speech and language irnpaiments. This is refiected in the literature by

the many different types of language tests in use, the varying methodology in which

these tests are utilized, and the variation in the constructs that are actually measured.

For example, some investigations lump together children with receptive disorders and

children with expressive disorders into a single group. The authors of other studies have

constructed their own nosologies by using terms like "speech retardation" and

"articulation deviance". Although intuition guides us in their interpretation, they are an

example and representation of the difficulties in the speech/language Iiterature. As a

result, the responsibility of defining and operationatizing impairrnents is left up to the

investigator.

Stevenson and Richman (1976) suggested that "a decision about what

constitutes abnormality would be strengthened if prognostic validation data were

available" (p. 431). Since then, a number of longitudinal studies of speech and language

disorder have been perforrned and have highlighted the longer-terni course and

significance of these disorders.

2.2 Longitudinal Oütcome of SpeechlLanguage lmpainnent

The longitudinal outcomes of speech/language impaired children that have been

studied generally fall into three domains: linguistic outcorne, which may include changes

in speechllanguage deficits; acadernic ouicornes, which may include changes in school

performance and reading ability; and psychological outcomes, which may include

changes in the rates of emotional, behavioural or psychiatric problems. Because this

investigation is concerned pnmarily with psychiatnc outcorne, a summary of literature

only on the emotional, behavioural or psychiatric outcomes will be provided.

2.2.1 Behavioural Findings

The most common late childhood behavioural outcorne among speechllanguage

impaired children are increased rates of extemalizing behaviours of inattention and

hyperactivity. However, internalizing behaviour such as anxiety and socially withdrawn

behaviours have also been reported. Ammg clinic-based samples, Baker and Cantwell

(1 987) reported on a foliow-up study of children first seen at a speech and language

clinic. The age range of the children was 2-1 5.9 years (with a mean age of 5.i years).

AI1 children were initially diagnosed as having a speech-only impairment, a language-only

7

Page 22: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

impairment or a combined impairment. There were no controls. The overall prevalence of

psychiatric disorders increased frorn 44% initially to 60% at follow-up five years later - a

significant change. The major diagnoses showing changes were attention-deficit

disorder (ADD) which more than doubled in prevalence (from 16% to 37%) and anxiety

disorder which alrnost doubled in prevalence (from 8% to 14%). The third rnost prevalent

diagnosis was oppositional/conduct disorder.

The above study, however, had a number of weaknesses. For example, in

addition to the absence of controls, only 300 of the 600 subjects initially assessed were

followed-up. Furthemore, the initial age range of the subjects was large and the authors

assumed that the effects of speechnanguage impainnent on outcome were uniform over

this range which may not necessarily be true.

Another clinic-based study (Benasich, Curtiss, & Talial, 1993), which did use

control groups repcirted similar findings. This study did not use psychiatric diagnoses as

the outcornes measures, but instead used parent-assessed behavioural rneasures. The

investigators followed a group of language impaired and control children from age 4 to

age 8. At age 8, the language irnpaired group had a significantly higher total behaviour

problem score as well as a significantly higher nurnber of parent-endorsed behavioural

problem items than the control group. With respect to particular types of behavioural

problerns, both language impaired boys and girls scored significantly higher levels of

hyperactivity than the controls. However, language impaired girls also scored

significantly higher levels of social withdrawal symptoms than controls. These findings

are similar to those of Baker and Cantwell (1987) which show increased rates of both

intemalizing and externalizing behavioural symptoms among language impaired children

compared to control children followed over time.

A third clinic-based study (Cohen at al., 1993) investigsted the occurrence of

unsuspected language impairments in a sarnple of 4-12 year old psychiatnc outpatients.

The study also examined a group of psychiatric outpatients who did have a previously

identified language impairment. The authors showed that of 288 children referred solely

for psychiatric disorders, 99 (34.4%) had more subtle language irnpairments than did the

other children with previously recognized impaiments. This study used parent- and

teacher-assessed scores of behaviour problems. As in the other two clinic-based

studies reviewed above, children in both groups in this case had symptoms associated

with attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). As well, children with

unsuspeded language impaiments had the rnost senous externalizing behaviour

problems while children with previously identified language impaiments had more

Page 23: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

internalizing problems. Again, because these studies were based on clinic samples, their

generalizability is limited.

Among the comrnunity-based investigations, the same four major studies that

assessed the prevalence of speechtlanguage impairments have also followed these

children over tirne to assess their behavioural outcomes. They were the Dunedin study

(Silva, Williams, & McGee, 1987) which followed children from age 3 to age Il, the

Waltham Forest study (Stevenson, Richman, & Graham, 1985) which followed children

from age 3 to age 8, the Newcastle study (Fundudis et al., 1979) which also followed

children from age 3 to age 8 and the Ottawa Language Study (OLS) (Beitchman et al.,

1993) which followed children from age 5 to age 12.5. The Dunedin study showed that

children with delayed verbal comprehension and general Ianguage delay at three years

of age were most at risk for later behaviour problems. The type of behaviour problern,

however, was not reported. The Newcastle study found the children with language

delays to be more Iikely to show rnarked introversion and withdrawal than the control

group at follow-up five years later. The Waltham Forest study, however, reported that

early problems with language structure were specifically associated with later problems

in neurotic deviance, rather than with antisocial deviance, even after controlling for initiai

behaviour problems.

Rutter and Mawhood (1991) reviewed the literature on the Iink between language

problems in early childhood and later psychopathology. They concluded that: "As judged

by al1 the available data, the main increase in psychopathology seems to be in the domain

of anxiety, social relationships and attention-deficit problems rather than in conduct

disturbance or antisocial behaviour" (p. 121). Beitchman et al. (1 993) in the OLS,

however, did not show such specificity of early speech/language impairments on later

internalizing problerns. Although they did corroborate others' finding that report high rates

of inattention and hyperactivity among impaired children as well as intemalizing symptorns

such as anxiety, they also reported conduct disorders. Furthemore, overalt measures of

externalizing symptoms were significant among the impaired groups.

Unfortunately, almost al1 longitudinal investigations of speech~language impaired

children that have studied behavioural outcomes have only followed their subjects to late

childhood or adolescence and it is not known to what extent early speechllanguage

impairment is a precursor of young adulthood behavioural deviance.

Page 24: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2.3 Relevance

Speechnanguage impaired children have been observed to shaw increased rates

of intemalizing and extemalking behaviours. It may be argued ?an an epidemiotogical

and clinical point of view, however, that the extemalizing problerns represent the more

significant outcome as they are more stable than intemalizing behaviours and cany (except in the instances of severe inhibition or depression) a worse prognosis as well as

resistance to most forms of intervention (Robins, 1979).

Antisocial activity represents one type of extemalizing behaviour that is found to

develop arnong speech and ;anguage impaired children and although the studies

reviewed above have only equivocally pointed towards this outcome, this fact should not

deter its exploration among thzs2 children at a much later point in their lives - young

adulthood. There are four main reasons for this. First, the young adulthood period in

one's Iife is an important developmental stage. Theorists have emphasized it as a pivotal

transition point from adolescence to adulthood. Young men and women gain significant

freedoms and are forced to make important decisions that will have long-term

consequences. A successful transition secures an adult role, identity and opportunities

for social advancement, while failure rnay tead into pathways that have negative social

and health consequences (Beitchrnan, 1995; Rutter & Giller, 1983).

Second, young adulthood represents the ages during which antisocial behaviour

reaches a peak (Robins & Reiger, 1991) and antisocial behaviour is predorninantly a male

phenornenon (Henggeler, 1989; Robins, 1986). The recent Ontario Health Survey-Mental

Health Supplement (mord et al., 1996) and other epidemiological psychiatrie studies

such as the Epiderniologic Catchment Area Study (Robins & Reiger, 1991), the National

Comorbidity Study (Kessler et al., 1994) and the Edmonton Psychiatrie Epidemiology Study

(Biand, Newman, & Om, 1988; Bland, Om, & Newman, 1988) have al1 reported the

highest rates of antisocial behaviour in the 18-24 year-old age group as well as

significantly greater prevalence rates for males than for fernales. As there have been no

controlled prospective studies which have followed speech and language impaired

children to adulthood, a relationship between earfy childhood speechnanguage impairment

and the peak in prevalence in antisocial behaviour during the young adulthood years

seems worthy of investigation. F~irthemare, the fact that these high prevalence rates

represent prirnarily male antisocial behaviour and that males are at greater nsk for speech

and language impairment (Silva, 1987) substantiates the need to investigate the long-term

outcorne of these domains.

Page 25: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Third, antisocial behaviour is a critical social concem demanding attention. At a time

when antisocial behaviour in young people seerns to be increasingly significant, it is of

great importance that interventions be designed to modify such behaviours at the earliest

possible age (Maughan & McCarthy, 1997). In the case of children with speechnanguage

impairment, should these impairment prove to be an underiying cause of antisocial

behaviour in adulthood, interventions through speech or language therapy in childhood

may prove to be a more desired treatment strategy rather than attempting to intervene

many years later in young adulthood when the rnany other long-term effects of

speechAanguage impaiment have already taken their toll on the individual.

Finally, there are two main areas of research that have alluded to this association.

The first is the literature that suggests a possible antecedent role of speechllanguage

impaiment in the relationship between academic underachievernent and extemalizing

behaviour; and the second is the literature that suggests a mediating role of hyperactive

and inattentive symptoms for the indirect effect of speechnanguage impairment on young

adulthood antisocial behaviour.

2.4 Evidence that Suggests an Association Between SpeechlLanguage

Impairnient and Antisocial Behaviour

2.4.1 Speechllanguage Impairment as an Antecedent Variable

Much interest in the role of speechllanguage impairment and its relationship to

acting-out behaviour cornes from the large number of studies which have explored the

association between extemalizing behaviour problerns and academic underachievement.

A variety of causal models have been tested: some studies support the model that

unde rachievement leads to extemalizing behaviour, others support the opposite model

that extemalizing behaviour leads to academic underachievernent, while others support a

bi-directional or reciprocal mode1 (Lynam, Moffit, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1993). In his

review of the literature, however, Hinshaw's (1 992) overarching conclusion was that

research of unidirectional rnodels to explain the association between academic

underachievement and externalizing behaviour were iriconclusive and as a whole,

research in the field pointed toward the action of an antecedent variable.

This model, that an antecedent or third variable explains the association requires

that the antecedent variable precedes the association of interest and that the effect of

the antecedent variable should not be explainable by the other background variables.

Early childhood speecManguage problerns are an excellent candidate as an underlying

Page 26: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

variable as they correlate with acadernic and extemalizing behaviour problems and

because they predate the emergence of the association. However, because such a test

of this model requires a prospective longitudinal evaluation that includes sensitive

rneasures of al1 the hypothesized variables at multiple time points, only two such studies

have foflowed children beyond the elementary school years.

In the first study (Olweus, 1983). the author made an explicit attempt to establish

the credibility of unidirectional, reciprocal and antecedent variable models. The study

used peer-rated rneasures of aggression and averaged school grades at both the sixth

and ninth grades in a sarnple of Swedish boys. Using structura1 equation rnodeling, when

background variables of social class, parents' ages, divorce, birth out of wedlock, and

birth order were controlled for, the unidirectior;al and reciprocal rnodels were not

supported, leading to the conclusion that antecedent variables accounted for the

association between poor grades and aggressive status.

In the second study, Schonfeld et al. (1988) used path analyses and provided

some evidence for a link between cognitive deficits in childhood and conduct disorder 10

years later. More s?ecifically, the investigators found an indirect path from age 7 Verbal

IQ and severïty ratings of conduct disorder at age 17 - rnediated by adolescent IQ

scores. This finding held up even when: i) early aggressive behaviour and ii)

background variables of environmental disadvantage, neurological soft signs and

parental psychopathology were controlled. Although methodological problems cloud the

viability of their findings (the sarne psychologists measured both initial behaviour ratings

and the initial cognitive ability and were, therefore, not blinded), this study is interesting

because it: i) performed measlirements of each dornain at multiple time points, ii)

assessed relevant antecedent variables, and most importantly, iii) found a Iink between

childhood Verbal IQ - a variable highly correlated with speechllanguage ability - and

antisocial behaviour in late adolescence.

As mentioned above, a sizable literature has emerged on the relationship between

early language problems and the development of both behavioural difficulties and

academic deficits. In the case of academic achievement, there is a consensus in the

literature of studies of speechnanguage impairment showing higher rates of academic

underachievement than controls (Schacter, 1996). Underachievement has k e n

measured in many different ways including reading ability, high school completion,

grades, parent and teacher reports on studying and hornework habits, standardized

tests, the presence of learning disabilities as well as placement in special education

classes. Though al1 the studies described in Schacter's recent review (1 996) obsewed

an increase in rate among speechnanguage impaired children, methodoiogical problems

Page 27: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

rnay lirnit their extemal validity. For example, most did not control for potentially

confounding variables such as social class or intelligence. Nevertheless, in those

studies that did statisticafly control for the effect of these variables, two of them

continued ta observe an independent effed of speechnanguage status on achievement

(Beitchman et al., 1993; Menyuk et al., 1991) while one did not (Ararn, Ekelman, & Nation,

1 984).

The close relationship between speechnanguage impairment and academic

underachievement sheds new light on previous findings between academic achievement

and extemalizing behaviour. Thus outcornes once thought to be related to academic

underachievement may now be seen within a new, although indirect, causal context of

speech/language ability.

2.4.2 Speechllanguage Impairment, ADHD and Antisocial Behaviour

Upon examining externalizing behaviours, mild-to-moderate language problems

seem to be associated with several types of difficulties and, as rnentioned above,

attention and hyperactivity problems are at the top of the list (Hinshaw, 1992).

Specifically, a developmental trend in the nature of the behavioural difficulties among

underachieving children has been noted. In the middle to late childhood yean academic

deficits seem most correlated with inattention and hyperactivity while in adolescence,

academic problems become most strongly correlated with problems of conduct and

antisocial behaviour (Maughan, Pickles, Hagell, Rutter, & Yulc, 1996).

Finally, inattention and hyperactivity are well established risk factors for conduct

disorder (Taylor, 1994) and a number of studies have found increased rates of antisocial

disorders in adulthood arnong individuals with ADHD in childhood (Gittelman, ~annuzza,

Shenker, & Bonagura, 1985; Greenfield, Hechtman, & Weiss, 1988; Weiss & Hechtrnan,

1986). For example, Loney, Kramer, & Milich (1981) found that young hyperactive adults

when compared to their nonhyperactive brothers were found to have a greater incidence

of antisocial personality disorder (45% vs. 18%) and more frequent episodes of

incarcerations (41 % vs. 5%). A controlled prospective follow-up study (Hechtman &

Weiss, 1986) examined 64 patients with a rnean age of 25 from a group of 104 who had

been assessed as hyperactive 15 years earlier. The patients in the study had more court

referrals than controls and 23% also met DSM-III criteria for antisocial personality disorder

while only 2.3% of controls did. The two fields of research, that is, the one which

studies the relationship between inattentionlhyperactivity and subsequent antisocial

behaviour and the second which studies academic underachievernent and its

Page 28: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

subsequent association with externalizing behaviour rnay overlap considerably and have

speechnanguage impairment in cornmon as a latent variable.

2.5 From Early Childhood Impairment to Young Adulthood Outcorne:

Possible Mechanisms

Over time, the proportion of speechAanguage irnpaired individuals with a

psychiatric problem increases (Baker & Cantwell, 1987). White the persistence of the

speecManguage impairment may decrease over time, the clinic-based and community-

based studies reviewed above suggest that sorne longer-terni vulnerability remains that

increases the chances of developing a psychiatric problem even after the impairment

diminishes. Cuvent evidence supports this contention only up until late childhood or early

adolescence. This longer-term vulnerability rnay exercise its effects by irnpeding the

ability to communicate. That is, children who have trouble communicating rnay be at risk

for considerable frustration and a poor self-esteem. This may lead to dysfunctional

parent-child relationships andlor child-peer relationships both of which could precipitate

or maintain externalizing behaviour problerns such as antisocial behaviour (Howlin &

Rutter, 1987). Becaase of the importance of communication in a social world, it seems

likely that children with speechnanguage impairments would be at risk for behavioural

problems.

lt is also possible that early speech/language impairment does not play a causal

role in the emergence of later antisocial behaviour. The association between these two

variables may be due to an antecedent variable that leads to both. Beitchman (1985)

suggested that neurodeveloprnental immaturity could be such a variable and that

speechllanguage impairment may in fact be a marker for it. However, even if

neurodevelopmental immatunty could account for both speechAanguage impaiments and

later antisocial behaviour, the above rnentioned difkulties in communication would, most

likely, also play a role in the developrnent of behaviour problerns. Thus, the action of a

neurodevelopmental immaturity and diffÏculties in communicating would most likely not be

mutually exclusive in causing later antisocial problems.

Another possible mechanism alluded to above involves the relationship between

speechnanguage impairment and subsequent symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity.

Symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity are a known risk factor for subsequent

conduct disorder (Taylor, 1994). Furthemore, the rnost comrnon behavioural outcome in

late childhood among speechnanguage impaired children is attention-deficit and

Page 29: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Hinshaw, 1992). It is possible that this outcome rnay act

as a subsequent exposure van'able for later antisocial behaviour.

Finally, it is possible that variables related to speechnanguage impairment may

fully or partially account for its association with later antisocial behaviour. That is,

antisocial behaviour arnong speecNlanguage impaired children may be a function of

covariates such as a low intelligence, childhood adversity or other factors that are both

associated with speechllanguage impairment and risk factors for antisocial behaviour.

The rote of these variables in explai~ing antisocial outcome is very likely because not al1

speechnançvage impaired children rnay become antisocial and also because many non-

speechAangliage impaired children do becorne antisocial. If this is the case, when these

covariates are accounted for statistically, the association between speechilanguage

impairment and later antisocial behaviour would be attenuated.

2.6 The Present Study

2.6.1 Data Source and Conceptual Framework

This study makes use of the most recent wave of data from the Ottawa

Language Study (OLS) (Dr. J.H. Beitchman, Principle Investigator), a comrnunity-based

three-wave panel study. The OLS was initiated in 1982 (time-1) when subjects were 5

years of age, followed-up approximately 7 years later in 1989 (tirne-2) when the subjects

were 12.5 years of age and most recently followed-up for a third time in 1996 (tirne-3)

when the subjects were 19 years of age. Predictor variables have been measured at

both tirne-1 and tirne-2 in a number of dornains including a) speech and language

competence; b) intellectual functioning competence; c) academic achievement ; d)

psychiatric disorders; e) psychological distress and impairment; e) psychological

adjustment; and f) data pertaining to family and parental mental health. Subject treatment

history was also recorded.

ln the present study, only time-1 measures are utilized to assess their association

with antisocial outcornes. The choice of variables that are controlled in this investigation

was driven by the Iiterature. Once specific risk factors for antisocial behaviour are

identified and entered into a predictive model, it is important to determine whether

speechnanguage impairment expIains an additional significant amount of variance in the

occurrence of antisocial behaviour. If it does, then the knowledge of speechllanguage

impairment identified at as early an age as 5 years provides investigators in the field (and

subsequently parents of such children and their physicians) knowledge that the

impairment puts the child at significant risk for later antisocial behaviour. If it does not,

15

Page 30: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

then the knowledge of such irnpairments would not be considered to provide any

information on the risk of subsequent antisocial outcorne and that the variables controlled

may explain rnost of the antisocial outcorne variance.

Figure 2.1, below, illustrates the follow-up and attrition of subjects in the OLS.

Although no tirne-2 data is used, it is represented in the figure to provide a complete

depiction of the study. The figure aiso shows the number of subjects that were

categorized as speechllanguage impaired and as controls at each wave of the study.

Finally, the figure displays that 90.1 % of the subjects at time-1 provided at least some

data at time-3, while 85.2% of the subjects at üme-1 provided complete data at time-3.

Differences between subjects with and without missing data are described in section 3.6

and in appendicies A and B.

Page 31: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons
Page 32: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2.6.2 Antisocial Behaviour: Conceptualizations

In the field of psychopathology, behavioural deviance can be characterized in

several ways. For example, to convey a dimension of antisocial behaviour, investigators

can sum or aveage data gathered from rating scales or behaviour observations, yielding

a quantitative score. Through the application of cutoff scores or multivariate clustering

strategies, the same instruments rnay yield a category - that is, a subgroup of children

with common characteristics #en investigators use clinical judgments, inclusionary

markers, and exclusionary criteria, they can obtain a diagnosis. Finally, alternative

definitions, such as the legal entity of delinquency with respect to antisocial activities c m

also be employed. This study intends to measure antisocial behaviour in each of these

three ways: First, as a diagnosis; second, as a legat entity; and third, as a dimension-

Generafly, antisocial behaviour involves the violation of the nghts of others, 3 lack

of conformity to social noms and is usually present in variety of settings including farnily,

work and interpersonal contexts. Antisocial individuals tend to be aggressive and

impulsive and some are thought to lack normal capacities for love, guilt and cooperation

with authority figures. Many, but not alII antisocial individuals come into contact with the

law and the notion of equating antisocial behaviour with cn'minality has been discussed

(Robins & Reiger, 1991). For exarnple, the psychiatric nosology has been criticized as an

attempt to "medicalize" bad behaviour and that diagnoses of Antisocial Personality

Disorder (ASPD) express the alienation of the disadvantaged and are representative of a

"sick" society rather than of a sick patient (Robins & Reiger, 1991). Robins (1 979),

however, argued four main reasons as to why antisocial behaviour can be considered a

psychiatric illness.

First, its syrnptoms are highly intercorrelated, making it a coherent syndrome and

not just an assemblage of various types of deviance. Second, it has been shown to

have a heritable cornponent (Rutter, 1 996). Third, it occurs in and is recognized by every

society, no matter what its econornic system and throughout history, showing that it is

not purely an indication of a modem "sick" society. Although its prevalence varies with

time and place, the same can be said with almost every psychiatric and nonpsychiatric

disorder. Finally, if the criticism were warranted that the diagnosis of ASPD was simply

a medicalization of criminality, then one would expect that most cnminals would be

diagnosed with ASPD and that most indivi&als with ASPD would also be criminals.

Robins and Reiger (1991) argued that this is not the case. In their large investigation of

psychiatric disorders, The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study (Robins 8 Regier, 1991),

these authors reported that less than half (47%) of those with ASPD had a significant

Page 33: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

record of arrest (which they defined as more than one arrest other than for a rnoving

violation or any felony conviction).

One could argue that the criteria used by Robins and Regier was strict and that a

much a larger proportion of those diagnosed with ASPD may have had a significant

history of arrest that would have still distinguished thern from the non-ASPD sample.

Thus, the overlap between these h o domains depends rnuch on the way in which

"cn'rninality" is defined. To avoid this problem, this study addresses its research objective

by measuring antisocial behaviour as a diagnosis, as a legal entity and as a dimension,

each separately. Perfomiing separate analyses for each measure of antisocial

behaviour improves the extemal validity of the study by allowing for differences in not

only the sources of the antisocial behaviour data, but also in the ways in which antisocial

behaviour can be conceptualized.

2.6.3 Risk Factors to be Controlled

Intelligence

Low intelligence is a factor that has been shown to be both associated with early

childhood speechllanguage impairment and a risk factor for subsequent antisocial

behaviour. In order for intelligence to be considered a confounding variable by the

classical definition (see Last, 4995) it must satisfy three criteria: i) it must be a variable

that can cause or prevent the outcorne of interest; ii) it rnust be associated with the factor

under investigation; and iii) it must not be an intemediate variable. Evidence thus far

suggests that intelligence, as measured by IQ, satisfies the first two criteria. There is

however, no information pertaining to the third cnteria. The reasons for this are related to

the early developmental association between thought and language which are

considered to develop in a parallel and reciprocal fashion. Because thought and

language develop so closely to each other and at such a young age, it is very difficult to

parse out their effect on each other (Silva, 1987). There is, however, research showing

a clear association between speech/language impairment and later IQ. The four main

longitudinal studies of speechllanguage impairment have al1 shown later significant

deficits in intelligence which were not initially present (Beitchman et al., 1994; Fundudis et

al., 1979; Silva, McGee, & Williams, 1 983; Richman, Stevenson, & Graham, 1982).

An association between low IQ and delinquency has also long been noted.

Furthemore, this association is stronger for verbal tests than for performance or full

scale tests (Lynam et al, 1993). There were three general ways in which IQ was

thought to be related to antisocial behaviour: a delinquent lifestyle was thought to lead to

a low IQ; a low IQ was thought to lead to delinquency; or a third (or antecedent) variable

Page 34: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

waç thought to explain the association between delinquency and low IQ, in which case

the relationship between delinquency and low IQ was considered to be only spun'ous.

Two "antecedent" hypotheses were put forward. The first was the differential

detection hypothesis (Moffitt & Silva, 1988; Murchison, 1926; Stark, 1975; Sutherland,

i 931) which suggested that the population of delinquents and nondelinquents were not

distinguished by their IQ1s, but rather, it was only the lower IQ delinquents who were

detected by the police and were thus more likely to be represented in research samples

(Feldrnan, 1977; Hirschi and Hindelberg, 1977). Moffitt and Silva (1 988) tested this

hypothesis and found no significant difference in IQ between a group of self-reported

delinquents and a group that had been detected by the police. The second "antecedent"

hypothesis was that some third variable lead to both IQ and delinqtiency. The most often

tested candidates were race and social class (Lynam et al,, 1993). However, many

studies found the low IQ-delinquency relation to be maintained even when race or social

class were controlled (Hirschi, 1969; Reiss & Rhodes, 1961 ; Short & Strodtbeck, 1965;

Toby & Toby, 1961 ; West & Famngton, 1973). Lynam et al. (1 993) also controlled for

both at the same time and reported that the relationship still held.

The hypothesis that delinquency leads to low IQ has also been tested. A number

of authors have suggested that a delinquent lifestyle results in lower intellectual

functioning (Hare, 1984; Shanok & Lewis, .198l). One expianation describes that

delinquents score more poorly on IQ tests because they are not interested in doing well

(Tamopo!, 1970). Thus, the lower IQ scores do not reflect impaired cognitive functioning,

but rather that they are personally opposed to test taking and do not value it. However,

Lynam et al. (1993) found that IQ differences between delinquents and nondelinquents

r2mained significant even after controlling for the boys' observed level of effort while

taking the 1Q test. Furthemore, prospective studies of juvenile delinquents indicate that

lower IQ scores are present well before the initiation of delinquent activities (Denno,

1990; Moffitt, Gabrieli, Mednick, & Schulsinger, 1981 ; West & Farrington, 1973).

The rernaining hypothesis that low IQ leads to delinquency has received the most

support. The specific explanations generally faIl into one of hrvo categories: the direct-

effect formulation and the indirect-effect formulation (Moffitt, 1990). In the case of the

direct-effect formulation, many interpret the 1Q as a broad index of neuropsychological

health and deficits in neuropsychologicai abilities, referred to "executive functions,"

interfere with a person's ability to monitor or control his or her own behaviour. In

addition, the most popular tests of executive functions al1 share significant proportions of

variance with tQ scores and to this extent IQ should have direct effects on delinquency.

For example, some low IQ children are more impulsive, less attentive and less adept at

Page 35: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

seIf-control, thus setting the stage for delinquency and antisocial behaviour. Using path

analyses, Lynam et al. (i993), however, found that onty 17-25% of the effect of low IQ

on delinquency operated indirectly through rneasures of impulsivity while most of the

effect acted frorn low IQ directly to delinquency.

Altematively, many authors have suggested that low IQ leads to delinquency

indirectly through school failure (Hirschi, 1969; Hirschi & Hindelang, 1977). Children who

experience failure in school will be more Iikely to become delinquents than those who do

not. In this model, IQ acts as an index of ability to succeed in school. Again, Lynam et ai.

(1 993) tested this hypothesis and did not find a simple answer. They found that this

hypothesis proved correct only for A f r h n Amencan subjects but not for Caucasians. In either case, there is considerable evidence in the literature for the risk factor status of IQ

on subsequent antisocial behaviour thaï warrants its control when detennining the risk

factor status of early chiidhood speechtlanguage impairment on young adulthood

antisocial behaviour. As such, IQ was controlled in this investigation.

Socioeconomic Status

Many studies report an association between low socioeconomic status (SES) and

poor Ianguage ability. In the Dunedin study the average language score of the lowest

SES group was about a year below that of the children in the highest SES group (Silva,

McGee, Thomas & Williams. 1982b). Similar SES gradients relating to language ability

have also been found by others (Lawton, 1968). For example, Wooster (1 970) found

social class differences in the ability to produce and understand language. As well,

Beitchman et al. (1 994) found similar SES differences between a speech/language

irnpaired group and a control group and Fundudis et al. (1 979) also found an association

between low SES and language delay.

In an attempt to determine whether social class variables accounted for the

relationship between speechllanguage impairment and psychiatrie problems, Beitchman,

Peterson and Clegg (1988) measured family income, occupation, parental marital status,

educational attainment, family size and birth order. Although occupation, marital status

and matemal education distinguished psychiatrically disturbed children from normal

children, none of the variables studied distinguished these children within the

speechllanguage impaired group. The authors concluded that these variables did not

appear to mediate the relationship in It should be noted that this study was

based on cross-sectional data and its findings do not preclude the possibility that social

class variables rnight act as confounding variables in explaining the relationship between

Page 36: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

speecManguage impairment and later antisocial behaviour. As such, socioeconomic

status was controlied in this investigation.

Childhood Adversity

The relationship between speechnanguage ability and low socioeconornic status

as been discussed above. What rernains unclear is why this association exists. SES or

poverty are useful measures, but Bloom (1974) pointed out that they are too general and

too static to serve as the environmental measure for such a specific human

characteristic as language ability. Bloom argued that it is what parents and others in the

environment do in their interaction with the child that is iikely to influence the development

of a specific characteristic rather than the more static or status attributes o f the parent.

Maughan and McCarthy (1 997) argue that the effects of childhood poverty and broader

social variables like SES on various constructs Iike cognition and language abilrty seem

largely mediated through their impact on various aspects of family fundioning (Sampson

& Laub, 1993).

Research on early and middle childhood environmental corre:ates of disruptive

behaviour problems have been identified with impressive consistency. A meta-analysis

(Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986) identified four main paradigms of childhood

adversity: i) a neglect paradigrn, refiecting poor parent-child relationships and a lack of

appropriate parental supervision; ii) a conflict paradigm, reflecting hostile, erratic or

threatening discipline and rejecting relationships; iii) a deviant parent paradigm, reflecting

deviant parent behaviours and attitudes, including parent criminality; and iv) a disruption

paradigm, including marital discord between parents and family breakdown. Findings

from two long-term British studies illustrate these patterns and the risk factor status of

childhood adversity on antisocial behaviour. The Cambridge Study of Delinquent

Development (Farrington and Hawkins, 1991) reported that the nsk of offending by age

21 doubled for males with a convicted parent and that early onset of delinquency had a

strong association with poor parental childrearing and harsh or erratic discipline. The

Newcastle Tho~tsand Family Study (Kolvin, Miller, & Fleeting, 1988) reported that the risk

of offending by age 33 was tripled for those whose parents' marnages were unstable as

well as for those males who experienced poor early mothering or physical care. Fer

fernale offenders, very high rates of childhood adversity were reported. Overall for

males, multiple early stressors were associated with the highest rates of n'sk.

Because childhood adversity seems to have a crucial effect on increasing the nsk

of later antisocial behaviour there is reason to control for this construct when detemining

Page 37: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

the relationship between eariy childhood speechnanguage impairment and subsequent

young adulthood antisocial behaviour.

Esrly Antisocial Behaviour - Among the most frequently cited predictors of antisocial behaviour is early

aggressive behaviour (Huesman, Eron, Lefkowitz, & Walder, 1984; McCord, 1983; Rutter

& Giller, 1983). For example, Pakiz, Reinherz and Giaconia (1997) found that early

aggression was strongly associated with delinquent behaviour ai age 15. As well,

Farrington (1 991) showed that early aggression and violent behaviour showed

significant continuity and predicted chronic offending in young adulthood in a sample of

males. In Maughan and McCarthy1s (1997) review of the Merature, these authors

observed that severe antisocial disorders in adulthood are almost invanably preceded by

overt antisocial behaviour in childhood and aggressive and disruptive behaviours show

high levels of persistence across development.

As to why antisocial behaviours persist into adult life, previous research

suggested that the continuity of antisocial behaviour reflected the direct unfolding of the

pathogenic process over time where the course of chronic delinquency follows a

succession of anticipated paths (Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989). including a

progression from Iess serious to more serious acts (Loeber, 1991). Other evidence,

however, (Quinton, Pickles, & Maughan, 1993) suggests a somewhat different model.

Here, persistence of antisocial behaviour appears to depend at least in part on an

individual's continued exposure to other later risks and eariy behaviour probfems to be of

significance in increasing exposure to those risks. For example, eariy disruptive

behaviour is known to be associated with a range of "real world" social handicaps such

as acadernic underachievernent, unempIoyment, early family formation, deviance and a

lack of support in partners and relationship breakdown (Maughan 8 McCarthy, 1997).

These consequences may be particulariy important at key transition points in

development, such as puberty or adolescence, increasing the chance that individuals will

be exposed to later risks for antisocial behaviour (Scarr, 1992). By the same token,

reduction or absence of such later risks can allow for tuming points out of dysfunctional

trajectories (Pickles & Rutter, 1991).

With respect to the association between eariy childhood speechAanguage

impairment and early antisocial, oppositional or conduct problems, studies which have

followed speechdanguage impaired children to late childhood do not report that these

dornains are related with a high degree of specificity. Nevertheless, conduct problems

are present at rates greater than controls. For example, Beitchman, Wilson, Brownlie,

Page 38: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Walters and Lancee (1996) reported significant differences between low overall and

high overall language functioning groups in self-reported conduct problems.

Nevertheless, enough of an association exists between early speechflanguage

impairment and concurrent or later antisocial problems to warrant the control of this

constnict. Moreover, early childhood conduct problems are one of the strongest

predictors of adult antisocial behaviour (Pakiz , Reinherr, & Gianconia, 1997). The

purpose of this study is to determine whether knowledge of speechllanguage impairment

at as early as 5 years of age provides us with additional information on risk beyond that

provided by the other known risk factors for young adulthood antisocial behaviour. If not,

and al1 eise being equal, then parents and dinicians rnay have to wait until later factors,

such as antisocial behaviour in rniddle or late childhood, emerge before solid predictions

about risk for future antisocial behaviour can be made.

Inattention and Hyperactivity

Mild-to-moderate language problems are associated with severa! types of

behavioural difficulties and symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity are at or near the

top of the list. For example, Beitchman, Hood and Inglis (1990) reported that the rates for

both boys and girls of DSM-III Axis-l diagnosis of Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) were

not only significantly greater for the speech/language groups than for the control groups,

but were also the most cornmon DSM-Ill diagnosis made. Sirnilariy, in a ciinic-based study,

Baker and Cantwell (1 987) reported that of the 50% of the 628 children in their sarnple

who were assigned a DSM-III Axis-l diagnosis, the most cornmon diagnosis was Attention

Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity (ADHD). There was, however, no controt group in the

study. Nevertheless, other more methodologically sound cornrnunrty-based studies have

also shown this. For example, Beitchman et al. (1989a) used cluster analysis to classify

speechllanguage impairment in a sampie of 347 five year old children using scores on a

variety of speech and language tests. Four linguistic profiles were identified: high

overall, low overall, poor auditory cornprehension and poor articulation. The authors

reported that increased risk of behaviour disturbance was related to the child's linguistic

profile. Specifically, the rate of behavioural disturbance was greatest when the

language involved both comprehension and articulation problems. When they did, the

major outcome was hyperactivity or ADHD.

In addition, speech/language impaired children are ai increased risk for later

inattention and hyperactivity. For exarnple, Baker and Cantwell (1 987), although still not

using a control group, showed that those who were initially free of any psychiatrie

disorders but who were language disordered were at an increased flsk for subsequently

Page 39: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

acquiring ADD. Beitchman et al. (1996) also followed up their sample of children from

age 5 to age 12-5 and found that even after controlling for initial behavioural status, that

the early childhood language profile was still associated with an increase in teacher-

rated hyperactivity. Thus, there appears to be an association between speechAanguage

impairment and later emergence of inattention and hyperactivity symptorns.

The risk factor status of these symptoms, however, is less clear but the following

will show that these symptoms still warrant control. Weiss and Hechtrnan (1 986) studied

the adolescent and adult antisocial outcome of hyperactive children. They also reported

their own long-tem prospective follow-up data results of hyperactive children and

concluded that the concern that hyperactive children grow up to become antisocial is

exaggerated and that, in fact, few hyperactive children grow up to become serious adult

offenders. They elaborated, however, by stating that although hyperactive adults are not

significantly different from controls on measures of antisocial behaviour, one can still see

trends of greater antisocial involvernent in individuak with a history of hyperactivrty. On

the other hand, two studies (Gittefman et al., 1985; Greenfield, Hechtrnan, & Weiss, 1988)

published at approxirnately the same time reported that among those diagnosed as ADHD

in childhood, the greatest risk factor for the development of antisocial behaviour in

adulthood is the maintenance of ADHD symptoms. Gittelman et al. (1 985) conducted a 15

year follow up of subjects, with a final age-range of 22-29 years, who were either

diagnosed as hyperactive during childhood or were controls. They identified two

subgroups of the hyperactive population: those with moderate or severe continuing

symptoms and those with none or only rnild continuing symptoms. The former group was

characterized by significant emotional difficulties, alcohol use and antisocial behaviour,

while the latter group did not have dificulties in psychosocial functioning and were, in

addition, similar to the control group in many respects. Because of the evidence

supporting the risk factor status of symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity on later

antisocial behaviour, as well as the association between these symptoms and chitdhood

speechllanguage impairment, they were controlled in the analysis.

Gender

A relationship exists between speechflanguage problems and gender. For

example, gender differences in language development and developmental language delay

favoring girls have been found in a number studies. The Dunedin studies (Silva, 1980;

Silva, McGee, 8 Williams, 1982a, 1983) found that girls began to ta!k, on average, a rnonth

earlier than the boys and they gained significantly higher scores on al1 language tests.

AIso, about twice as rnany boys as girls are usually found to have a language delay

Page 40: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

(Morley, 1965; Stevenson & Richman, 1976; Fundudis et al., 1979; Silva, 1980). Although

most studies find more speechAanguage problems among boys, this is not always

reported. Beitchman et al. (1 986a), for example, found that the rates of speech-only and

language-only impaiment were slightly greater for girls than for boys, while the rate of

combined speechflanguage impairment was more than double for giris than for boys. It

should also be noted that the instruments used to assess these rates were gender-

referenced and gender-specific (Beitchman et al., 1986a).

As rnentioned earlier, there is also an association between gender and

delinquency. Male predominance in antisocial behaviour has been well documented

across studies (Henggeler, 1989; Robins, 1986). Male rates of antisocial behaviour

personality disorder greatly exceed female rates for every age and ethnic group (Robins

& Reiger, 1991). In addition, research suggests that antisocial behaviour develops within

different pathways for each gender. For example, in their review, Offord and Bennett

(1 994) note that the predictive power of earlier conduct problems on adult antisocial

behaviour is stronger in men than in women and that earlier factors rnay explain different

amounts of later antisocial variance. As well, early conduct problems predict different

types of later disorders for each gender. For example, in females, conduct problems

predict later intemalizing disorderç more strongly than they do antisocial behaviours,

while the opposite is true for males (Robins, 1986). Finally, the specific consequences of

antisocial behaviour have been reported as different for each gender. For example,

Robins and Reiger (1 991) showed that while the overall symptom patterns for antisocial

males and females were similar, men exceeded women in rates of traffic and nontraffic

arrests (as they do in the population at large), while wornen exceeded men in reporting

marital difficulties.

Almost al1 research on antisocial behaviour is based on male-only samples and

there is a paucity of data on antisocial outcorne in girls. This is noteworthy as the

authors of the Ottawa Language Study found that ratings by both a psychiatrist and the

child's mother indicated that girls with speechllanguage impaiment were at greater risk

than boys for a psychiatric disorder (Beitchman, Hood, & Inglis, l99O). The reasons for

this, however, are not known and had not been previously reported in the Iiterature. The

authors suggested that the gender with the lower prevalence rate, when affected, tend

to be more severely affected and since speechllanguage disorders are generally less

frequent in females they may be more severe and consequently contribute to a greater

vulnerability to other problems as well. Nevertheless, as the present study intends to

draw on the data from the OLS, it has the opportunity to contribute knowledge to this

issue. In summîry, because of gender differences in antisocial behaviour and

Page 41: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

speecManguage ability, in addition to the difference in pathways leading to antisocial

behaviour, gender was controlled in assessing the relationship between eariy childhood

speecManguage impairment and young adulthood antisocial behaviour.

SpeechLang uage Treatment

The occurrence of treatment for speechllanguage irnpairments also needs to be

controlled. It is associated with the exposure variable of interest and may be related to

antisocial outcorne. At tirne-3, parents reported that 33%, 62% and 12% of the language

impaired, speech-only impaired and control subjects, respectively received sorne form of

speech andior ianguage therapy some tirne between ages 5 and 19. Thus, subjects who

had some forrn of impairment at time-1 were more Iikely to have received some fom of

treatment before time-3 than the subjects who were normal at tirne-1. There is no data,

however, to detemine whether speechAanguage therapy is effective in preventing

youog adulthood antisocial behaviour. if speechnanguage irnpainnent does lead to an

increase in risk for such behaviour, then conceptually, therapy for such irnpairments

might prove helpful in that it would be expected to increase the child's ability to relate to

others without resorting to aggressive or delinquent behaviours.

Evidence as to the effectiveness of such therapy is not positive. Beitchman et al.

(1 994) failed to demonstrate an effect of speechllanguage treatment. However, because

that investigation was not intended as an evaluation study (for example, the subjects

were not randomly assigned to receive treatrnent) no reliable conclusions can be drawn.

As well, despite the meta-analysis by Nye, Foster and Seaman (1987), tfiere is Iittte

documented evidence of the effectiveness of speechllanguage therapy. Beitchman et al.

(1994) pointed out that the above meta-analysis based its conclusions on studies with

serious methodologicstl problems such as the lack of control groups and random

assignments as well as non-blind ratings, al1 of which cast doubt on the conclusions

drawn by Nye, Foster and Seaman ('l987).

Despite the lack of data supporting the effectiveness of speechllanguage

therapy, as well as data supporting its relationship to later antisocial behaviour, the

occurrence of therapy is still highly associated with the present study's exposure group.

Therefore, it still proves worthwhiie to account for this variable when determining the

relationship between early childhood speechnanguage impairment and young adulthood

antisocial behaviour.

Page 42: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

2.6.4 Hypothesis

The most common behavioural outcome in late childhood among speecManguage

impaired children are symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity (Hinshaw, 1992). These

symptorns are also a known risk factor for subsequent conduct and antisocial problerns

(Taylor, 1994). It seems Iikely, therefore, that individuals with earIy childhood

speecManguage irnpairrnents rnay be at risk for antisocial problems in young adulthood.

The truth of this hypothesis, however, rnay depend on the type of antisocial outcorne

measure used. For example, it rnay most Iikely be true for diagnoses of ASPD which

depend on the occurrence of conduct problems before age 15 and which are also

present in speechllanguage impaired children in late childhood. it rnay not be true, however, for measures of crirninal behaviour. For example, even if there is a strong

association between speechnanguage impairment and syrnptoms of inattention and

hyperactivity, it is still unclear as to whether such children grow up to becorne serious

adult offenders (Weiss & Hechtrnan, 1986). The dimensional measures of antisocial

behaviour are useful in their ability to report on the more subtle tendencies associated

with antisocial behaviour that rnay not be of a severity such that these individuals would

exhibit crÏminaI activity. Furtherrnore, these subtle differences rnay prove to be

statistically significant and clinically relevant.

Page 43: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3. Methods

3.1 The Ottawa Language Study

The present study comprises a secondary analysis that makes use of data from

the Ottawa Language Study (OLS). The present author did not take part in the collection

of the OLS data at any of the tirne points, but did contnbute in constnicting a nurnber of

the variables presently used. Specifically, independent variable data was obtained from

the first wave of the study (time-1 , 1982) when the subjects were 5 years of age and

dependent variable data was obtained from the most recent, third wave of the study

(time-3, 1996) when the subjects were 18 years of age. A description of the relevant

data acquired frarn the OLS and how it was utilized in the present study now follows.

3.2 lndependent Variable of Prirnary Interest: SpeechlLanguage Status

One of the purposes of the OLS during tirne-l was to determine the prevalence of

speech and language disorders in kindergarten children (5 years of age) attending

English-tanguage schools in the Ottawa-Carlton region. A one-in-three sample of al1 5

year old children (for whom English was their mother tongue) was administered the first

stage of a three-stage screening procedure, each stage consisting of standardized test

batteries. This original study reported that the Ottawa-Carlton population was

represented well by the studyls sample (Beitchrnan et al., 1986b).

Ali standardized tests were completed within one academic year and were

administered to the students on an individual basis. Stage I consisted of a 30 minute

speech and language screening. All children scoring below the Stage 1 cutoffs, as welt

as a small randorn sample of those scoring above the cutoffs (the purpose of which was

to estimate the rate of false-negatives) were asked to participate in a more intensive

testing session at Stage II. Those children falling below the Stage Il cutoff points were

identified as either speech-only impaired, languageanly impaired or cornbined speech

and language impaired (for a more detailed description of the screening procedure and

the test batteries used, see Beitchman et al., 1986b).

These speech andfor fanguage irnpaired children and a control sarnple (matched

forage and sex, but scoring above the Stage II cutofis) comprised the current

investigation's study sample. This information was also used to comtruct the

independent variable of pnrnary interest in this investigation: time-1 speechflanguage

status. Because, however, there are few, if any, notable differences in psychiatnc and

Page 44: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

developmental disorders that distinguish children with language-only impaiments and

children with speech and language impaiments (e-g. see Baker & Cantwell, 1987a; and

Cantwell & Baker, 1987a), these two groups were cornbined into a single "Ianguage

impaired" group. This was also performed because the speech and language impaired

group had the largest proportion of missing values compared to any of the others (see

sections 3.6.2). Merging these two groups helped reduced possible bias. Thus, the

independent variable of primary interest was composed of three categories: a control

group, a speech-only impaired group and a language impaired group.

At tirne-1, the speecManguage impaired and control subjects were also asked to

participate in Stage II1 testing which was designed to ascertain a variety of background,

developrnental and farnily information (as previously described). The relevant information

from this final stage of testing waç the source of the following covariate data.

3.3 Covariates

As described in chapter 2, the çelection of additional risk factors for young

adulthood antisocial behaviour that needed to be controlled was guided by the literature.

These Rsk factors incfuded: intelligence, socioeconomic status (SES), childhood

adversity, being reared in a single-parent family, eariy conduct problerns, inattention,

hyperactivity, gender and speech/language therapy. Except for the latterrnost covariate,

each one of the risk factors was assessed as part of the Stage III procedures dunng

tirne-?. The occurrence of speechllanguage therapy was assessed at time-2 and tirne-3.

A description of the instruments used and the operationalization of each covanate is

provided below.

3.3.1 Intelligence

At time-1, the Wechsler Primary Preschool Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) was used

to assess IQ (Wechsler, 1987). Silverstein (1 985) reports very good psychometric

properties for this instrument. The WPPSI provides three scores: a full-scale IQ, a verbal

IQ and a performance IQ.

The risk factor status of low intelligence on subsequent antisocial behaviour has

been discussed. Although this relationship has been shown to be stronger for measures

of verbal IQ than for either performance or full-sale 1Q (Prentice & Kelly, 1963; West &

Famngton, 1973), the use of verbal IQ as a covariate poses a problern. Because of the

strong association between language and thought, measures of verbal intelligence and

measures of language ability are very likely to be collinear, such that controlling for verbal

30

Page 45: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

IQ would simultaneously control for speechnanguage ability. A similar problem would

exist if full-sale IQ, which represents a measure of both verbal and nonverbal

intelligence, was used. For Our purposes, it is preferable to have a rneasure of

intelligence that is most independent of language ability. Using performance IQ achieves

this and allows for a more complete separation of language abilities and intelligence.

Therefore, measures of performance IQ were used as a covariate when assessing the

risk factor status of eariy childhood speechnanguage impairment on later antisocial

behaviour.

3.3.2 Socioeconornic Status (SES)

Part of the time-l Stage Ill testing involved a parent questionnaire which assessed

total family income. Respondents were asked to report in what category out of a

possible 13 their income fell. The lowest category was less than $5,000 peryear, the

largest was greater than $60,000 per year and each intermediate category encompassed

a $5,000 per year range. Although SES was coded as a 14-level categorical variable (1 3

income categories and 1 missing observation category), it was treated as an ordinal

variable in the analyses.

3.3.3 Childhood Adversity

Research on eariy childhood environmental correlates of disniptive behaviour has

been discussed and the concept of childhood adversity described. Although no specific

measures of this construct were provided at time-1 , a similar construct, parent adversity,

was assessed. This variable was originatly intended to provide a composite measure of

parental mental health that included information from mothers and fathers. However,

dunng the collection of time-l data, almost al1 respondents (97%) were only the children's

mothers. As a result, this variable was re-termed "rnother's adversity".

The mother's adversity variable is a composite score based on two main

subfactors: 1) parent psychopathology, as represented by scores on the Center for

EpidemioIogic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977) and scores fom the

Global Symptorn Index Subscale of the Behavioural Symptom lnventory (BSI) (Derogatis,

1975); and 2) scores on marital adjustment and discord from the Marital Adjustment Scale

(MAS) (Locke 8 Wallace, 1959). The CES-D is a self-administered scale which assesses

depression in adults. lts psychometric properties are good and it has been shown to

have high correlations with other self-report measures of depression and with clinical

ratings of depression (Radloff, 1977); The BSI is self-report questionnaire that measures

general psychopathology. Derogatis (1 983) reported that this instrument had good

Page 46: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

psychometric properties; and the MAS is a widely used self-report of marital satisfaction

and it too has been shown to have good psychometric properties (Cohen, 1988).

For two-parent families, each score (CES-D, BSI and MAS) was given a weight of

0.333 to create the composite. For single-parent families only the CES-D and the BSI

were available and each was given a weight of 0.5 for the composite. The analysis in

the curent study did not distinguish between mother and father respondents as there

were relatively few of the latter (only 3%). Thus, mother's adversity is used as a proxy

for childhood adversity. This was deemed appropriate for three reasons: First, rnother's

adversity was the closest approximation to childhood adversity available for use in this

study. Second, mother's adversity was previously shown to be related to the children's

psychiatrie outcome at time-2 (Beitchman et al., 1993); and third, it was assumed that

because the mother traditionally plays a significant role in child rearing, especially during

the preschool years, the adversity experienced by the mother would correlate well with

the adversity experienced by the child if such a variable were available. The limitations,

however, of using the mother's adversity construct should be noted. Such a variable

might not accurately represent the adversity experienced by the child if the parents are

separated or divorced and the child spent a rnajority of the time with the parent not

interviewed in the OLS. As weli, in farnilies in which there are a large nurnber of children,

the older ones might be delegated a large amount of responsibility that rnay increase their

adversity relative to their mother as well as to their younger siblings. The assumption that

the adversity experienced by the mother of the subject during the subject's childhood is

related to the subsequent adult functioning of the subject was tested in the course of the

analysis.

It was not assumed, however, that mothers (or children) from single-parent

homes would experience the same amounts of adversity as those from two-parent

homes. In fact, 30% of mother respondents indicated that they were single parents. (An

anonyrnous 4 mother respondents reported that they had partners but were single

parents. For these data, the MAS was not used in the calculation of mother's adversity

and were thus treated as single-parent families). To account for this effect associated

with adversity and which may be related to later antisocial outcorne, a dichotornous

variable indicating whether a child was reared in a single- or two-parent home was

considered in the analyses. Conceptually, child rearing in single-parent homes are

associated with the neglect and disruption paradigrns descnbed by Loeber & Stouthamer-

Loeber (1 986). For example, Henggeler (1 989) identified a modest association between

broken homes and antisocial acts. This categorical variable was operationalized,

however, with three levels: a two-parent family category, a single-parent family category

Page 47: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

and a missing data category. The lattermost group had only 6 observations (see Missing

Data, below).

3.3.4 Inattention, Hyperactivity and Conduct Problems

Tirne-1 measures of inattention, hyperactivity and conduct problems were

assessed using the respective subscale percent scores from the Revised Conners

Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) (Goyette, Conners, & Ulrich, 1978). This instrument

includes items that are rated by the child's teacher as not at al1 present, just a little

present, pretty much present or very much present (scored as 0-3, respective1 y). The

above subscales are ernpirically derived factors. This scale has good psychometnc

properties and has also repeatedly been shown to be sensitive to treatment effects in

hyperactive children (Miller, Koplewicz, & Klein, 1997). A potential limitation in the use of

this instrument in the operationalkation of these risk factors lies in the fact that it reIies on

a single teacher source for the information. Previous research shows that the stability of

early behaviour problerns is particulariy high for preschoolers who exhibit problems both

at home and in preschool settings. ldeally, had parent-rated data been available, it would

have been used in conjunction with the teacher-rated data. Previous work, however,

has also shown that teacher-rated information is more sensitive in assessing the

significance and impact that children's disruptive behaviour will have on Iater functioning

(Miller et al., 1997; Egeland, Kalkoske, Gottesman, & Erikson, IWO).

3.3.5 SpeechILanguage Treatment

Data from the parent and subject time-3 interviews was used to assess the

occurrence of speechllanguage therapy during the period between times-1 and 3.

Specifically, the respondents were asked whether the subject had ever received

treatment for a speech andlor language impairment. Both parent- and subject-assessed

data were used in the analyses and both were treated as dichotomous variables.

3.4 Dependent Variables

3.4.1 Antisocial Behaviour as a Diagnosis

The time-3 measure of antisocial behaviour conceptualized as a diagnosis utilized

the Composite lntemational Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (World Health Organization, 1990).

The CIDI is a comprehensive interview scheduie designed for the assessment of major

diagnostic categories according to the criteria in the lntemational Classification of

Diseases (ICD-1 O)-Diagnostic Criten'a for Research (World Health Organization, 1991 a, b)

33

Page 48: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

and the DSM-IIIR (Amerkm Psychiatnc Association, 1987). It is a highly stnictured

instrument intended for use by trained lay interviewers and in epidemiological studies of

mental disorders in general populations. CID1 questions are fully spelled-out and positive

answers are further explored with a strictly specified probing system. A symptom

assessed by the CID1 is considered clinically significant if it led to professional

consultation, treatment with medication or was believed by the respondent to have

interfered significantly with Iife activities. This latter criterion is assessed with the Global

Assessrnent of Functioning (GAF) s a l e which is listed in the DSM-IIIR as an Axis V

diagnostic criterion test (American Psychiatric Association, 4 987).

The GAF is also an interviewer-rated scale and provides a rating from 1 to 90 of

an individual's overall psychological, social and occupational functioning. In the present

study, a GAF score of 69 or less was required (in addition to endorsement of the

relevant itemslbehaviourç on the CIDI) for a subject to be "diagnosedn with an antisocial

personality disorder (ASPD). Ali "layn interviewers were research assistants working

with the OLS and took part in a 2day intensive training course for the administration of

these instruments. The training was sirnilar to that employed by the investigators of the

Ontario Health Survey-Mental Health Supplement (mord et al., 1996). Finaily, the CIDI

has been shown to have good psychometric properties (Kessler et al., 1994) and the

GAF has also been shown to be adequate (Hall, 1995; Piersma & Boes, 1997).

3.4.2 Antisocial Behaviour as a Legal Entity

Antisocial behaviour was also assessed with respect to the law. Specifically, an

atternpt was made to detennine whether or not antisocial behaviour elicited by the

subjects was of a nature (or perhaps, severity) such that they would have had

increased contact with the police andior courts. To address this problem, data from one

of the time-3 subject interviews on contact with the law was used. From this

questionnaire, a simple dichotomous variable called criminal behaviour was created.

Criminal behaviour was considered present if a subject reported at least one of any of

the following: an arrest, a court trial for having been accused of something, a conviction,

tirne on probation, time in a juvenile detention center or time in a police station or jail.

3.4.3 Antisocial Behaviour as a Dimension Measured Continuously

Antisocial behaviour conceptualized as a dimension was assessed using three

different instruments, each of which relied on a different informant: parents (mother or

prirnary parent if mother is unavailable), teachers (subject-identified teacher likely to

know thern best) and the subjects themselves. The corresponding instruments used

Page 49: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

were the Achenbach Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, 1991 a), Teacher

Report Fom (TRF) (Achenbach, 1991b) and Youth Self Report (YSR) (Achenbach,

1991 c). Each test is a standardized and reliable checklist that has been shown to

differentiate between children referred for psychiatrie treatment and normal children. As

well, the psychometric properties for each are reported to be good (Achenbach, 1991d).

Each checklist contains a large nurnber of problem items which the respondents

rate as not true, somewhat or sometimes true or very true or oiten true (scored 0-2,

respectively). The problems items are used in the construction of a variety of syndrome-

specific scales. ln this study, the syndrome-specific scores that were used to measure

antisocial behaviour are the Aggressive and Delinquent Behaviour Scales. Both of these

scales are produced by al1 three instruments. In other words, the constructs of

aggression and delinquency are each as assessed by different informants from their

respective contexts or environments. For each instrument, there are two types of

syndrome-specific scales: those that rely only on the problem items common to alt three

instruments (the cross-informant syndrome scales) and those that rely on the common

problems items plus the items specific to a particular instrument (instrument-specit7c

syndrome scales). Only the latter scales were used in this study.

The reason for this is that there are behaviours for which some types of

informants can report on while other types of informants cannot. For example, teachers

may be better assessors of how a subject interacts with other subjects in a controlled

environment (such as a classroom) than a parent. For this reason, the TRF contains the

problern items such as "disnipts classn or "disturbs othersn while the CBCL does not.

One reason for using multiple sources is that different types of informants are likely to

observe different samples of subject behaviour. Using the instrument-specific syndrome

scales rather than the cross-informant syndrome scales allows for this study to capture

better the different ways in which antisocial behaviour is manifested by the subjects

instead of how different informers report on the same subject behaviour.

For each syndrome-specific scale, a raw score is produced which is converted

to a norrnalized T score. In this study, each instrument's Aggressive Behaviour T score

and Delinquent Behaviour T score was combined to form a mean "antisocialn T score

which was used in the study's analyses. This was done for four main reasons: First,

although problem items in difFerent syndrome scales have subtle conceptual differences

that warranted the grouping of them separately in the original Achenbach scales, ail of

the items in both scales can still be considered antisocial in nature. Second, considering

al1 problems items together as a single score yields a measure of antisocial behaviour

which is most similar to the notion of antisocial behaviour as conceptualized in the

Page 50: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

diagnosis of an ASPD. Third, combining these two T scores is reasonable in that it yields

a useful and easily interpretable summary score that does not involve a loss of

information. Finally, the T scores and raw scores for each of the syndrome-specific

scales correlated highly (see Table 3.1, below).

ssion and Delinquency Subscale strurnents, respectively.

Table 3.1 : Correlations (and their p-values) of the Aggr T scores and raw scores for the CBCL, TRF and YSR

Finally, because of a large proportion of rnissing values arnong the teacher-rated

(36%) and parent-rated antisocial behaviour T scores (23%), these two measures were

merged into a single variable conceptualized as "non-subject rneasured" antisocial

behaviour (see below for a more complete discussion of missing values and how they

were dealt with). This merging invoived using the mean of both rneasures if both data

were present and using either datum if the other was rnissing. Although cornbining

parent and teacher data resulted in a substantial loss of information by losing the ability to

distinguish behnreen parent and teacher scores (for example, they only share

approximately 20% of their variance), it was the only way to salvage and make good use

of the data available. As well, useful distinctions were still made between subject-

measured and non-subject rneasured antisocial behaviour. Furthemore, combining these

two rneasures reduced the proportion of values missing to 16.5% frorn the higher values

of 23% and 36%. Table 3.2 shows how well parent- and teacher-rated antisocial

behaviour rneasures correlated for T scores and raw scores, respectively.

YSR

Aggression &

Delinquency 0.45408

(p=0.0001)

TRF Aggression

& Delinquency 0.621 76

(p=0.000 1) T score

CBCL Aggression

& Delinquency

0,73004 (p=0.0001)

Page 51: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

?ir p-values) of the parent-rated (from the CBCL) and iean antisocial measures for the T scores and raw scores,

Table 3.2: Correlations (and th teacher-rated (from the TRF) i respectively.

To bnefiy summarize:

T score

raw score

antisocial behaviour as a dimension was operationalized by using scores from

parents, teachers and su bjects

due to missing values, parent and teacher measures were merged into a single score

referred to as "non-subject measures" of antisocial behaviour

a separate analysis was performed for subject and non-subject measures of

antisocial behaviour

within each analysis, the actual score used was the mean of the Aggressive

Behaviour and Delinquent Behaviour T scores from the respective instruments'

Parent- & Teacher. Measured Antisocial

Behaviour 0.43776

(p=O.O001)

0.43307 (p=0.0001)

3.5 Statistical Analysis

3.5.1 Data Exploration

The analyses used to explore the data have been partially described. In order to

make decisions about how the data would be treated a variety of simple analyses were

performed. For categoncal variables, frequency calculations were perfomed and

missing value categories were created. For continuous variables, distributions were

analyzed and issues conceming whether assumptions underlying the various analyses

would be violated were addressed. Decisions about the ways in which variables were

treâted are discussed in more detait in sections 3.3. 3.6 and 3.9.

' ln the case of non-subject measured antisocial behaviour with both parent and teacher data available, the T score used was a mean of 4 T scores: parent- rated and teacher-rated aggressive behaviour T scores and parent-rated and teacher-rated delinquent behaviour T scores.

Page 52: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3.5.2 Logistic Regression

Young adulthood antisocial behaviour was first conceptualized as an illness and

was operationalized as a diagnosis of antisocial personaIity disorder being either present

or absent at time-3. This tirne-3 outcome was therefore, dichotornous and the logistic

model was used to obtain odds ratio estimates (ps) for a time-1 speech-only impairment

and a tirne-1 language impainnent (each of which was defined relative to the control

group). The same set of analyses of the model building procedure was used for the

second conceptualization, that of antisocial behaviour as a legal entity. As discussed

above, the outcome variable far this conceptualization was termed cnminal behaviour and

was also operationalized as either present or absent at tirne-3 and was, therefore,

dichotomous, Thus, the logistic model was used a second time to obtain a second set of

the above mentioned odds ratio estirnates.

Model Building

Because this investigation studied the relationship between an independent

variable of primary interest and an outcome for which a number of potential confounders

required control, a combination of Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) and model building

techniques were employed. For the logistic analyses, this was accomplished via the

following steps:

Step 1 : ANCOVA

A set of ANCOVA procedures were performed involving the independent variable

of interest and each covariate to assess whether the latter confounded the relationship

between speechAanguage impairment and risk of an antisocial outcome. This involved

two substeps: First, an assessment of an interaction between speech~language status

and the covariate was perforrned (it should be noted, however, that there were no a

prion hypotheses of any interactions); and second, if no interaction existed, an

assessment of the confounding status of the covanate was performed. A covariate

was defined as a confounder if the adjusted ps differed by greater than 10% from the

unadjusted (i.e. cade) iys. This definition of confounding has been shown to perform

acceptab!~ and the 10% difference between adjusted and unadjusted p s to be an

acceptable cut-point (Maldonado & Greenland, 7 993).

Page 53: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Step 2: Variable Selection

At least one of the two following criteria were required for a covariate to be

considered for inclusion in the model building process: 1) the adjusted and unadjusted Ws

differed by 10% or more; and/or 2) the covariate's Wald x2 statistic was significant at the

0.1 O level in the wrresponding ANCOVA performed in Step 1.

Step 3: Mode1 Building

A backward-deletion model building strategy similar to that suggested by Hosmer

and Lemeshow (1 989) and Rothrnan and Greenland (1 998) was employed. The

advantage of a backward-deletion approach (as opposed to a fonnrard-addition

approach) is the fact that it takes into consideration the possibility that a collection of

variables, each of which may be weakly associated with the outcome in a multivariate

model, can strongly effect outcome when taken together (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989).

A fonnrard-addition process does not account for this because it considers the impact of

the addition of only one variable in the model at a time.

The backwarddeletion process first involves the fitting of the largest multivariate

model containing the independent variable of primary interest (time-l speechnanguage

status) and the covariates identified in step 2. The corresponding p s of

speechllanguage status are determined and the probability of each covariate's Wald X2

statistic is also noted. The covariate with the least statistically significant value is deleted

from the model and a new model is fit. The p s for speechlianguage status are then

recalculated and campared to the p s from the largest multivariate rnodel. If at least one

pair of @s differ by 10% or more, the deleted covariate is retained, the model is

considered final and the speechAanguage @s are reported. If the iys do not differ by

10% or more, the deleted variable is eliminated from consideration and the process is

repeated using the new, reduced model.

Step 4: Iterations

This process of deleting covariates, refitting reduced models and cornparing the

new p s to iys from the fullest multivariate rnodel is repeated up until at least one pair of

corresponding Ys differ by 10%. This is the final model.

Page 54: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3.5.3 Linear Regression

Essentially the same ANCOVA-rnodel building steps that were used for the logistic

regression analyses were also used for the linear regression analyses; the major

difference being that now, the mean antisocial T scores for the speech-only impaired

group, the language irnpaired group and the control group were the estirnates of interest

rather than odds ratios. Regression diagnostics were also performed to assess the

validity of the assumptions involved in linear regression analyses. As well, an analysis

of multicollinearity arnong the independent variables in the final Iinear and logistic rnodels

was assessed with painvise correlations and multiple R ~ S . The results from these

analyses appear in Appendix C.

3.6 Missing Data

3.6.1 Independent Variables

OnIy four of the nine independent variables had missing data and arnong each

one, a relatively small proportion was rnissing: performance IQ (1.8%), rnother's adversity

(7.7%), single-parent family (2.1 %) and SES (8.5%). For each of these, missing values

were substituted with the variable's respective mean. For the covariates of rnother's

adversity and SES, those with rnissing data at time-1 were most Iikely to be those most

impaired. Because those most irnpaired tend to have the poorest tevels for the

covanates, such missing covanate data might bias the results toward rejecting the nul1

hypothesis. See Appendicies A and B for a more complete discussion of missing data.

3.6.2 Dependent Variables

Missing data among dependent variables was more senous: parent-rated

antisocial behaviour, 22.5%; teacher-rated antisocial behaviour, 36.3%; (as mentioned

a bove), subject-rated antisocial behaviour, 1 0.6%; diagnosis of ASPD, 14. A %; and

criminal behaviour, 1 1 -6%. Observations with rnissing dependent variables data were

not included in any of the main analyses. However, cornparisons of subjects with

missing data to those without missing data for each dependent variable were made on all

the variables in the study. This was intended to expose any patterns in missing data that

rnay be of use in when interpreting results. As well, cornparisons of the proportions of

subjects with missing dependent variable data among each tirne-? speechllanguage

status group were also made. The results for these analyses appear in Appendix A. Al1

of this information was used to assess the reliability of the results.

Page 55: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3.7 Descriptive Staüstics

A set of descriptive statistics was calculated for each time-1 speechnanguage

group on each independent variable. Because this study was concemed oniy with

correlates that were also risk factors for antisocial behaviour, these data are only a

partial representation of the time-1 study sample characteristics.

3.8 Power Calculations

Before the start of time-3 data collection, power calculations were perfonned

based on a participation rate estirnated to be equivalent to the tirne-2 participation rate.

Specifically, 210 subjects were expected to take part and sample sizes of 225, 200 and

175, respectiveiy, were used to estimate the srnallest standardized effect size (Beta)

detectable. These calculations were also carried out with respect to: number of

independent variables in final regression models, overall explained variance ( R ~ ) and

tolerance levels among the independent variables (Greenland, 1985). All calculations

were performed setting ~ ~ ~ û . 0 5 and fk0.20 (power=0.80). Calculations were performed

for models containing 5 and 7 independent variables, respectively. As well. time-3 R~

values considered were 0.50, 0.40 and 0.25 respectively. Tolerance levels of 0.60 and

0.80 were also considered.

For a 5-independent variable regression model, the Beta ranged from the smallest

value of 0.1640 (for an n=225, an ~'=0.40 and tolerance of 0.80) to the largest value of

0.2410 (for an n-175, an ~~=0.25 and a tolerance of 0.60). For a 7-independent variable

regression rnodel, the Beta ranged from the smallest value ~f 0.1 504 (for an n=225, an

~*=0.50 and tolerance of 0.80) to the largest value of 0.2168 (for an n=175 an ~~=0.40

and a tolerance of 0.60). These analyses deemed that even with a smaller than

anticipated sarnple size of 175, the detectable effect sizes (the targest of which was

0.241 0) remained, from a dinical point of view, adequately small.

A preliminary check of the data for the curent study showed that the model with

the smallest number of observations exceeds the value of 225 used in the above

calculations. It was not assumed, however, that this study would find as large

proportions of explained variance, or as high levels of tolerance among the independent

variables which may exist in particular final models. Because of this, the above

mentioned a priori power calculations can only be used as a guide in the attempt to

explain any non-significant results that rnay be found.

Page 56: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

3.9 Tests of Reproducibility of the Results

The variables in this study can be operationalized in rnany ways. To detemine

whether variations in covariate definition affects the results, three variable modifications

were considered: first, the use of the Conners measures of inattention, hyperactivity and

conduct as ordinal vs. continuous; second, the use of subject- vs. parent-assessed data

on whether the subject had ever received speechiianguage therapy; and third, the use of

raw scores vs. T scores in the definitions of the dependent variables used in the linear

regression analyses.

3.9.1 Independent Variables

Conners Measures of Inattention, Hyperactivity and Conduct

These percentage scores were found to be positively skewed. That these

covariates need to be normally distributed is not an assumption of Iinear or logistic

regression. There was concem, however, that the few observations at the far right of

each distribution were influential values. An attempt to solve this problem while retaining

these observations was made by forming groups of deciles, nurnbering each group and

then treating these nurnbers as continuous in the ANCOVA and model building processes

(Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989). Although the new variables are actually transformed

continuous linear terms, for simplicity of argument, these new variables are referred to

as "ordinaln- A caveat should be noted: Because of the preponderance of certain values

(e-g. 39% of the subjects had a value of 0.00 for the Conduct variable), in none of the

Conners variables did the decile groups have an equivalent 10% of the total number of

subjects. This also caused the Conduct and Inattention variables not to have 10 decile

groups, but instead to have only 7 and 6 approximate "decilen groups, respectively. The

percentage scores at which the approximate decile groups were defined for each new

ordinal Conners variable appear below in Table 3.3.

Page 57: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons
Page 58: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Subject-assessed Versus Parent-assessed SpeechILanguage Treatment Information

As described in section 3.3.5, both the subjects and their parents were

interviewed conceming whether the subject had ever received any speecManguage

therapy. To account for any possible bias in either of these rneasures, both were

assessed in the analyses.

3.9.2 Dependent Variables

Dimensional Measures of Antisocial Behaviour

There are two reasons to consider the Achenbach raw scores versus the

normalized T scores. First, Achenbach (1 991 6) suggested that for purposes of

statistical analysis, it is preferable to use the raw scores from the syndrome-specific

scales rather than the T scores in the order to capitalize on the full range of variation in

the raw data. This is because the transformation of raw scores into T scores often

involves truncation where the assignment of a single T score to multiple raw scores may

eliminate variations that are important to detect. Second, the subject- and non-subject-

measured mean antisocial T scores described above were found to be very positively

skewed. Although norrnality of the dependent variable is not an assumption in Iinear

regression, it was found that corresponding distributions of the raw scores were not as

severely skewed.

A disadvantage of using raw scores over T scores is the resulting difficulty in

their interpretation. The main function of the T scores is to facilitate cornparisons of the

degree of deviance indicated by a child's standing on different scales and different

instruments. However, because this study does not intend to make such comparisons (it

only intends to make comparisons among different subjects using the same scale and

then comparing the resulting pattern of results to those frorn the use of a different

instrument), it was still of use to compare results from analyses using raw scores and

analyses using T scores.

3.9.3 Reproducibility of the Results: Summary

This study also determines whether different operationalizations of particular

covariates changes the conclusions drawn from the results. As such, for the analysis

of each antisocial outcome measure (diagnoses of ASPD, criminal behaviour, non-subject

dimensional measures and subject dimensional measures), there were four difierent sets

of covariates that were considered when detemining the relationship between early

childhood speechnanguage impairment and young adulthood antisocial behaviouc

Page 59: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Set 1:

Set 2:

Set 3:

Set 4:

Conners variables, as measured continuously, subject-assessed

speechllanguage treatrnent variable and the rernaining covariates (Le. P-IQ,

mother's adversity, SES and gender).

Conners variables (continuous), parent-assessed speechllanguage

treatment variable and the remaining covariates.

Conners variables, as measured ordinally, subject-assessed speechllanguage

treatment variable and the remaining covariates.

Conners variables (ordinal), parent-assessed speechflanguage treatment variable

and the remaining covariates.

For the dimensional measures of antisocial behaviour, there were actually 8 different

sets of models; four rnodels using the above sets of covariates while using the

Achenbach T scores as the dependent variable and another four models using the same

four sets of covariates but using the Achenbach raw scores as the dependent variable.

However, because the raw scores do not allow for easy interpretation and

generalization, only the results from the analyses using the T scores are reported. Thus,

for the analyses of non-subject and subject measures of antisocial behaviour, sets 1

through 4 refer to the modets that used the Achenbach T scores as the dependent

variable.

Page 60: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

4. Results I: Primary Analyses

Table 4.1 provides descriptive statistics for each time-1 speechllanguage group.

The pattern of results is such that the ianguage impaired children were generally worse

off than the speech-only impaired and control children. For example, ianguage impaired

children had significantly lower (pc0.05) performance-la (1 00.58) than both the speech-

only impaired group (1 13.72) and controls (1 17.96). They also had significantly higher

(pc0.05) rates of inattention (25.43%), hyperactivity (32.72%) and conduct problerns

(1 7-20 %) than speech-only impaired children (1 6-31 %, 24.75% and 1 1.57%,

respedively) and controls (1 4.06%, 18.25% and 10.31 %, respectively). Furthermore, a

larger proportion (23.23%) of language impaired children had been raised in single-parent

families than speech-only impaired children (10.26%) or controls (12.86%), however, the

differences in these proportions were only nearly significant (p=0.056). As well, a

significantly lower level of family income was reported for the language impaired group

(mean income level 6.02) than for the speech-only irnpaired (7.92) and control groups

(7.45), corresponding to less than $1 0,000 per year.

Page 61: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Tirne-1 Sarnple Size (n)

Performance IQ

Mother's Adversity

Inattention

Hyperactivity

Conduct

E S

Single-Parent Familyo

Gendet

Treatment (subject)O

Treatment (parent)d

Table 4.1 : Descriptive statistics (and standard deviations) for the speechllanguage groups on time-1 sample sbe, each of the time-l covariates, and time-3 speechllanguage treatment variables.

F(2,281)=63,38, p=0.0001; C S , S>L, C>L, al1 p<O.05

F(2,281)=2,16, p=0.1171

F(2,28l)=lO.2OI p=0.0001; L A , L>C, bolh p<0.05

F(2,281)=13,26, p=0.0001; L>S, LIC, both pc0.05

F(2,281)=4.45, p=0.0125; L>C, pc0.05

F(2,281)=8.68, p=0.0002; C>L, S>L, both p<0.05

~2(2df)=5.78, p=0,056

;~2(2df)=2,16, p=0.339

22(241)=40.487, p=0,001

~2(2df)=47.416, p=0.001

Tirne-1 sample size, covariates and tirne-3

treatment data

Note: aproportion from single-parent families. b proportion male. Csubject-reported proportion having received any speech or language therapy. d parent-reported proportion having received any speech or language therapy. "for post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language impaired group.

Control Speech-only Language lmpaired Group ~lfferences' Group lmpaired Group Group

Page 62: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

A sirnilar, but less dramatic, pattern of results was observed for the speech-only

impaired group. For example, they had significantly lower performance-IQs than controls,

and had higher, although not significantly higher, rates of inattention, hyperactivity and

conduct problems.

4.2 Conceptualization 1: Diagnoses of ASPD

4.2.1 Unadjusted Results

Table 4.2 shows the unadjusted time-3 ASPD results. The language impaired

group had a significantly higher proportion with diagnoses than the controi group

(19.48% vs. 7.75%, z=2.61, pc0.05), but not significantly higher than the speech-only

impaired group (13.16%). f i e proportions of speech-only impaired children and controls

with diagnoses were also not significantly difFerent from each other- Table 4.2 also

shows the unadjusted ~2 comparing the proportions with a diagnosis of ASPD arnong the

speechllanguage groups.

Table 4.2: Unadjusted proportions with diagnoses of ASPD for each speechllanguage

I~iagnosis of ASPD 1 1 O 7.75 1 5 13.161 1 5 19.481 p=0.045 1

Control

n 70

The corresponding unadjusted +s and their 95% confidence Iimits (CLs) for the speech-

only irnpaired and language impaired groups appear in Table 4.3, below. Table 4.3 also

shows the likelihood ratio ~2 value for the crude mode1 which contains only the tirne-1

+ \

Speech-only

n %

speechllanguage variable.

Table 4.3: Odds ratios ( p) and their 95% CLs for the diagnosis of an ASPD for the

Language

n Yt

Group Differences

~ 2 ( 2 d f ) = 6 . 1 8 2 ,

speech-only impaired and language irnpaired groups unadiusted for any covariates.

95%CL(Y)

(0 .576-5 .641 ) ( 1 -222-6.783)

' LRTa: ~2(2df)=6.027, p-0.0491

Speech-only Lang uage

Note: aLRT=likelihood ratio test.

tjis defined with respect to the control group.

Pb 1.803 2.879

Page 63: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

4.2.2 Adjusted Results

Thirteen separate univariate analyses were perfomed (for nine covan'ates, with

the Conners variables of Inattention, Hyperactivity and Conduct having two definitions

each, continuous and ordinai, and with the speecManguage treatment variables having

two definitions - subject- and parent-assessed). An equal number of ANCOVAs were

also perfomed. The results from each of these analyses appear in Table 4.4, below.

Page 64: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.4: Results from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate and for

Performance lQ

Mother's Adversity

Inattention (continuous).

Hyperactivity (continuous)

Conduct (continuous)

SES

Single-Parent

Gender*

rreatment (subject).

covariate for tC. Covariate

Jnivariate Analysis

! diagnosis of an ASPD. ANCOVA

Covariate 1 1 Speechnanguage Status

Language 0.0323 3.002 (1 .097-8.212)

Language 0.021 O 2.763 (1 -1 66-6.548) 0.01 O8 0.2501 Speech-only 0.3330 1.759 (0.561 -5.597)

Language 0.0336 2.589' (1 -097-6.226) 0.00741 0.3927 Speech-only 0.3603 1 -710 (0.542-5.344)

Language 0.031 8 2.630 (1 -088-6.357) -0.00526 0.6453 Speech-only 0.3053 1-81 7 (0.580-5.689)

Language 0.01 41 2.948 (1 -244-6.985)

Language 0.01 99 2.809 (1 -1 78-6.702)

0.3073 0.5704 Speech-only 0.2957 1,840 (0.587-5.768)

Language 0.01 98 2.786 (1.177-6.595) 1 -8857 0.0029* Speech-only 0.3995 1 -646 (0.51 7-5.243)

Language 0.0073 3 . 3 5 ~ ~ (1 -385-8.1 25)

Language 0.01 12 3.089 (1 -293-7.381 ) L

Alternate Definitions for the Conners and SpeechILanciuaqe Treatment Variables - - Inattention (ordinal). ) 0.21 94 0.0594 1 0.1 637 0.1799 Speech-only 0.3563 1,714 (0.546-5.390)

I I Language 0.041 2 2.505' (1 -037-6.047) dyperactivity (ordinal)' 0.1 454 0.0453 0.1 1 15 0.1 417 Speech-only 0.4905 1 -633 (0.51 6-5.1 70)

I I Language 0.8973 2.453' (1 -01 6-5.925)

Sonduct (ordinal) 1 0.0718 0.4290 1 0.0493 0.5970 Speech-only 0.3144 1.797 (0.574-5.625)

1 1 Language 0.01 88 2.809 (1 -1 86-6.650) rreatment (parent), 1 0.5596 0.2173 1 0.1 163 0.8174 Speech-only 0.1 541 2.636' (0.695-9.995)

I I Languaqe 0.01 39 3.647* (1 -300-1 0.230)

Note: a f i =parameter estimate. bp=pro ba bility. " p=adjusted odds ratio estimate. d95%C~( @)=confidence limits of tjk '=adjusteci and unadjusted odds ratios differ by 10% or more. '=parameter estimate is significant at the p=O.l O level in its ANCOVA. *=covariate entered into the model building process (Le. was significant at the p=0.10

level in its ANCOVA (*) andfor changed 4 by 10% or more (')).

As described in section 3.5.1, only those covariates which were either significant

at the p=0.10 level in their ANCOVA andior changed at least one of the speechllanguage

odds ratio estimates by 10% or more were considered to be entered into a model building

process. The satisfaction of this criterion for the particular covariates for each covariate

50

Page 65: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

set (described in section 3.9.3) was assessed. Of the four covariate sets, set 1 and 3

yielded identical models and sets 2 and 4 yielded identical models. Thus, the four

covan'ate sets yielded only two distinct final models. These were:

From covariate sets 1 and 3:

logit[P(Dx ASPD=present)]=a+Pr(Speech-only)+B2(Language)+~~(Gender)+~,(Treatment-S)

From covariate sets 2 and 4:

logit[P(Dx ASPD=present)]=a+Bl(Speech-only)+~2(Language)+P3(Gender)+P,ITreatment-P)

Because each of these two final models are almost the sarne and because each

produced speech/language odds ratios that were virtually identical (differed only at the

third decimal place), only the final rnodel from covariate sets 1 and 3 will be reported.

This final model is henceforth referred to as 'Mode1 1". This rnodel and its liklihood ratio X2

value appear below. The associated odds ratios for each of the variables in Model 1

appear in Table 4.5.

Model 1 : Liklihood Ratio Test: k(3df)=l9.?'84. p=0.0006 (n=241). logit[P(Dx ASPD=present)]=a+~l(Speech-only)+~~(Language)+~3(Gender)+p~(Treatment-S)

Table 4.5 shows that a time-1 language impairment was associated with a

significant odds ratio of 3.662 for a tirne-3 diagnosis of an ASPD, while a time-1 speech-

only impairment was associated with a non-significant odds ratio of 1.973. Only Gender

and Subject-assessed speechllanguage treatment were controlled as none of the eleven

other covariates proved to be confounding variables and were, therefore, not included in

the above final model. It is also worth noting that the male gender had a highly significant

Table 4.5: Relevant odds ratios and their 95% CLs for the variables in Model 1. 95%CL(I9)

(0.575-6.767) (1 -488-9.01 2) (1 -907-1 2.31 0) (0.247- 1 -784)

Speech-onfya Languagea Gender"

Treatment (S)= Note: aodds ratio defined with respect to the control group. bodds ratio for males defined wRh respect to females. 'odds ratio for having received treatment (subject-assessed) defined with respect to not

having received treatment.

Y 1.973 3.662 6.636 0.664

Page 66: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

odds ratio of 6.591 defined with respect to fernales, but that the treatment odds ratio of

0.664, which identifies a protective effect. was not signifiwnt at the p=0.05 level. As

well, that covariate sets 1 through 4 could al1 be surnrnarized by a single rnodel (Mode1 1)

shows that varying the operationalizations of the Conners and speechnanguage

treatment covariates did not affect the conclusions drawn from these results.

4.2.3 Summary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the Analysis of ASPD

When companng the adjusted to the unadjusted speechAanguage odds ratios. it

can be seen that only their magnitude increased white their significance at the p=0.05

level did not. After accounting for the variables of gender and speechnanguage

treatment, the adjusted odds ratios can be considered as more precise estimates of the

risk for a diagnosis of ASPD. A comparison of the adjusted and unadjusted odds ratios

for the speechflanguage groups appears below in Table 4.6.

Table 4.6: Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted speechllanguage odds ratio - - estirnates and their 95%CLs for the analysis of ASPD.

-

Note: "LRT=likelihood ratio test. bodds ratio defined with respect to the control group.

Unadjusted Resuits

speech-mly Language

4.3 Conceptualization II: Criminal Activity

4.3.1 Unadjusted Results

When antisocial outcome was operationalized in ternis of illegal activity, both the

language impaired (28.92%) and speech-only impaired (27.03%) groups had significantly

higher (z=2.37 and ~2.29, respectively, ~ ~ 0 . 0 5 for both) proportions of subjects who

reported some criminal behaviour than the controls did (15.27%) (see Table 4.7). The

speech-only impaired and language impaired groups did not differ from each other. Table

4.7 also shows the likelihood ratio 5 value for the crude model which contains only the

time-1 speechAanguage variable.

LM: ~2(2df)=6.027, ~=0.0491

Adjusted Results I

LRT~: x2(4df)=1 9.784, 0=0-0006 qb

1.803 2.879

95%CL(*) yb (0 -576-5 -641 ) (1 -222-6.783)

9S%CL(@)

(0.575-6.767) (1 -488-9.01 2)

Speech-only Language

1 -973 3.662

Page 67: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.7: Unadjusted proportions of subjects who reported sorne form of criminal

- - -- - - - - -

Note: 'LRT=likelihood ratio test.

behaviour for each speechnanguage group.

The corresponding unadjusted ys and their 95% CLs for the speech-only impaired and

language impaired group appear, in Table 4.8, below:

Table 4.8: Odds ratios ( q ) and their 95% CLs for the occurrence of criminal behaviour for the speech-only impaired and ianguage irnpaired groups unadjusted for any

Criminal Behaviour

covariates -

Control

n YO 2 0 15.27

Speech-only

n %

10 27.03

Note: ' 4 s defined with respect to the control group.

Language

n YO 24 28.92

Speech-only Language

4.3.2 Adjusted Results

A similar set of univariate and ANCOVA analyses were perfomed with criminal

behaviour as the outcome variable. The results from these analyses are summarized in

Table 4.9, below:

Group Differences

~2(2df )=6 .386 ,

p=0.041

>

2,056 2.258

(0.863-4.896) (1 -1 53-4.422)

Page 68: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.9: Resutts from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate for the occurrence of cnminal bt

I Performance lQ

Mother's Adversity

Inattention (continuous)*

Hyperactivity (continuous)

Conduct (continuous)

SES*

Single-Parent.

llnattention (ordinal)

Hyperactivity (ordinal)

Conduct (ordinal)

Treatment (parent)

~aviour. Covanate

Jnivan'ate Analysis

I

Definitions for the

Covariate 1 1 SpeechKanguage Status

Language 0.0475 2.247 (1 -009-5.003)

0.01 23 0.3937 Speech-only 0.1 018 2.066 (0.866-4.927)

Language 0.0221 2.201 (1 -1 20-4.325)

0.01 57 0-0380* Speech-only 0.1 250 1.982 (0.827-4-749)

Language 0.0660 1,918' (0.958-3.840) 0.01 83 0.0080* Speech-only 0.1 950 1.797' (0.741 -4.361 )

Language 0.1089 1.7~7' (0.880-3.587)

0.01 41 0.0743* Speech-only 0.1 163 2.01 3 (0.841 -4.81 9)

Language 0-0360 2.077 (1 -049-4.1 12)

-0.1 360 0.01 Speech-0nly 0.0661 2.294' (0.946-5.561)

Language 0.0529 1 -972' (0.992-3.920)

1.3979 0-0005* Speech-only 0.0756 2.250 (0.920-5-506)

Language 0.031 6 2.1 50 (1 -070-4.323)

0.8877 0-01 51 * Speech-only 0.1 546 1.892 (0.786-4.555)

Language 0.01 24 2.429 (1 -207-4.742)

-0.4587 0.2427 Speech-oniy 0.0448 2-637' (1.023-6.800)

Language 0.01 60 2.350 (1.1 72-4.71 0)

Conners and SpeechILanguage Treatrnent Variables

Note: ' =parameter estimate. 'p=probability. ' p=adjusted odds ratio estimate. d95% CL( @)=confidence limits of p.

0.2372 O.O1 51 * Speech-only 0.1 472 1.914 (0.796-4.606)

Language 0.0893 1 -83zt (0.91 1-3.683)

0.1 788 0-0030* Speech-only 0.21 29 1.760' (0.723-4.286)

Language 0.1 282 1.724~ (0.855-3.478)

0.1 753 0.01 53* Speech-only 0.1 1 10 2.044 (0.849-4.924)

Language 0.0398 2.048 (1 -034-4.057)

-0.5401 0.21 46 Speech-only 0.061 3 2.729' (0.954-7.808)

Language 0.0050 3.082' (1 -405-6-761 )

'=adjusted and unadjusted odds ratios differ by 10% or more. '=parameter estimate is significant at the p=O. 10 level in its ANCOVA. *=covanate entered into the model building process (Le. was significant at the p=0.10

level in its ANCOVA (') andlor changed g by 10% or more (3).

Page 69: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

For the analysis of criminal behaviour, covariate sets 1 and 2 yielded the same

final model, This mode1 is henceforth referred to as "Mode1 2an. This model and its

likiihood ratio x2 value appear below. The associated odds ratios for each of the

variables in Model2a appear in Table 4.1 0.

Model 2a: LikIihood Ratio Test: x2(3df)=1 3.396, p=0.0039 (n=251). logit[P(Crim.Beh.=present)]=a+~~(Speech-only)+Pz(language)+$~(Hyperactivity) 1

Table 4.1 0: Odds ratios and their 95% CLs for the variables in Model Sa. I w I 95C/oCL(tp)

Note: aodds ratio defined with respect to the control group. bodds ratio defined by a 10% increase in Hyperactivity.

Similady, covanate sets 3 and 4 yielded the sarne final model. This model is henceforth

referred to as "Model 2bn. This model and its liklihood ratio X2 value appear below. The

associated odds ratios for each of the variables in Model2b appear in Table 4.1 1.

Model2b: Liklihood Ratio Test: X2(3df)=26.842, p=0.0001 (n=251).

1 logit[P(Crim.Beh~=present)]=a+~~(Speech-only)+~~(Language)+ 1

Table 4.1 1 : Relevant odds ratios and their 95% CLs 1 ? 1 9 5 % ~ ~ ( y )

Speech-onlya Languagea

Hyperactivity- ORD SES"

GendeP Note:

or the variables in Model2b.

aodds ratio defined with respect to the control group. bodds ratio defined by a decile increase in Hyperactivity. 'odds ratio defined by a $5,000 increase in total yearfy family income. dodds ratio for males defined with respect to females.

Page 70: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Although Model2b contained more variables than Model 2a, both yielded sirnilar odds

ratios for each of the speechllanguage groups. For Model 2a, the odds ratios for a

speech-only impairment and a language impairment (1.797 and 1.777, respectively) were

both non-significant at the p=0.05 level (see Table 4.10). For Model2b, the

corresponding odds ratios were also non-significant (1 -887 and 1.660, respectively) (see

Table 4.1 A ) . The four covariate sets were surnman'zed by only two separate models (2a

and 2b) and these two models produced very similar odds ratios for the risk of criminal

behaviour for each speechllanguage group. This suggests that differences in the

operationalizations of the Conners and speechnanguage treatment covariates do not alter

greatly the odds ratios of prirnary interest-

Both Models 2a and 2b controlled for Hyperactivity (defined continuously in the

former model and ordinally in the latter model). Each definition of Hyperactivity was

associated with a significant odds ratio of 1 .O18 and 1.141, respectively. Model2b,

however, controlled for the additional variables of SES and Gender. SES had a

significant odds ratio of 0.857 indicating a protective effect against the occurrence of

criminal activity as socioeconomic status increased (as measured by total family incorne).

As well, Gender had a significant odds ratio of 2.498 indicating a higher risk for males

than for females. As covariates are deleted from the final model during the model building

process there is a Ioss in the precision of the estimates. It should also be pointed out,

therefore, that even though few covan'ates rernained in the final Models (2a and 2b), the

resulting loss in precision did not prevent a significant odds ratio for either

speechllanguage group from being observed. Even in the fullest muftivariate case for

each Model (2a and 2b) the speechllanguage odds ratios were still not significant at the

p=0.05 level.

4.3.3 Summary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the Analysis of

Criminal Activity

It can be seen that the effect of adjustment on the speechllanguage odds ratios

was to decrease their magnitude. Furthemore, afier adjustment, the odds ratio for the

language impaired group was no longer significant at the p=0.05 level. This was tme

when referring to either Model 2a or 2b. In the latter, when the additional variables of

SES and gender were controlled, this odds ratio decreased even further. A comparison

of the adjusted and unadjusted odds ratios for the speechnanguage groups appears

below in Table 4.12.

Page 71: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

4.4 Conceptualization III: Antisocial Behaviour as a Dimension

Table 4.12: Cornpanson of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model 2a) speecManguage odds ratio estimates and their 95%CLs for the analysis of criminal activity.

4.4.1 Non-subject Measures

Unadjusted Results

Unaditrsted Results

Table 4.1 3 shows the unadjusted parent andfoi teacher measured ratings for

each speechllanguage group. Differences were reported with the language impaired

children (rnean antisocial T score=56.89) scoring significantl y higher than both the

speech-only impaired children (54.49) and the controls (53.48). These latter two groups

were not significantly different from each other. Table 4.1 3 also shows the overall F-

statistic for the crude mode1 which contains only the tirne-l speechllanguage variable.

Note: "LRT=likelihood ratio test. odds ratio defined with respect to the control group.

-

Adjusted Results (Mode1 2a)

Table 4.1 3: Mean non-subject measured antisocial T ratings for each speechnanguage

95% CL(@)

(0.863-4.896) (1 -1 53 -4 .422 ) -

B

LW: ~2(2df)=6.027, p=0.0491

C

Speech-only Language

95% CL(@)

(0.741-4.361) (0.880-3.587)

LR?: x2(4df)=1 9.784, p=0.0006

Speech-only Language

Note: afor post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language impaired group.

@b

2.056 2.258

qb 1.797 1.777

impaired group, unadjusted for any covariates.

Adjusted Results

A set of Iinear univanate and ANCOVA analyses were performed with parent

Control Speech-only

Languaqe

andlor teacher measures of subject antisocial behaviour as the outcome. These results

are summarized in Table 4.14, below:

- X

53.478 54.486 56.892

F(2,234)=7.98, p=0.0004

vs C, p=0.3605 L vs S, p=0.0432 L vs C, p=0.0001

Page 72: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

1 able 4.14: Results Rom the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate and for each altemately definec

Performance IQ

Mothefs Adversity

Inattention (continuous)*

Hyperactivity (continuous)

Conduct (continuous)

SES*

Single-Parent*

Sender

Treatment (subject)

Covanate

Univanate Analysis Covariate 1 1 Speechnanguage Status

" S ] b P

b P 1 R 1 d ~ - ~ differencs

covariate for the non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour. ANCOVA

- m - F

- - -0.0215 0.4952 Controls 53.602

Language 0.0023 56,668

0.051 4 0.1 598 Controls 53.517

Language 0.0002 56.797 0.0388 0-05 17* Controls 53.628

Speech-only 0.4267 54.500 S vs L, p=0.07d

Language 0.0007 56.628

0.0682 0-0002* Controls 53.833

Speech-only 0.6769 54.285 S vs L, p=0.07(

Language 0.0033 56.381

0.0713 0-001 6* Controls 53.639

Speech-only 0.4461 54.464 S vs L, p=0.06!

Language 0.0005 56.625

-0.2073 0 -0955~ ControIs 53.541

Speech-only

Language

0.7523 0,5120 Controls

Speech-only

Language

0.9683 0.2247 Controls

Speech-onty

Language

0.2334 0.8083 Controls

Speech-only

Lanauaae 0.0001 57.03

nattention (ordinal)

4yperactivity (ordinal)

Zonduct (ordinal) *

rreatment (parent)

Alternate - - 05862 0.01 62* Controls 53.689

J - - Definitions for the Conners and S~eech/Lanauaae Treatment Variables

Speech-only 0.4853 54.453 S vs L, p=O.O77

Language 0.001 2 56.546

0.4939 0-0004* Controls 53.81 4

Speech-only 0.6422 54.317 S vs L, p=0.074

Language 0.0030 56.399

0.671 1 0 . 0 ~ 0 2 ~ Controls 53.656

Speech-only 0.4740 54.531 S vs L, p=0.080

Language 0.0006 56.565

0.21 05 0.8305 Controls 53.22

Speech-only 0.3525 54.38 S vs L, p=0.068

Language 0.0003 56.65

Page 73: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

From Table 4-14, above: d Note: "B =parameter estirnate; bp=probability; X=adjusted mean estimate; S-L differences=p-values

cornparing the rneans of the speech-only group and the language group; '=adjusted and unadjusted means differ by 10% or more; *=parameter estimate is significant at the p=û.10 level in its ANCOVA; .=covariate

entered into the model building process (Le. was significant at the p=0.10 level in its ANCOVA (*) and/or t changed by 10% or more ( )).

For the analysis of non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour, covariate sets

1 and 2 yielded the same final model. This model is henceforth referred to as 'Model 3a".

This mode1 and the overall F-test for the mean parent andor teacher rneasured antisocial

ratings appear below. The associated means for each speechiianguage group for Model

3a appear in Table 4.15.

Model 3a: Overall F-test: F(4,231)=8.36, p=0.0001 ( ~ 2 3 7 ) . ASBNon-suwea = a+~l(Speech-oniy)+~zfLanguage)+~~(Hyperactivi~)+~4(Single-Parent) 1

"for post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language

Table 4.1 5: Adjusted means for each speechllanguage group from Model3a.

impaired group.

Similarly, covariate sets 3 and 4 yielded the same final model. This model is henceforth

referred to as "Model 3b". This model and the overall F-test for the mean non-subject

measured antisocial ratings appear below. The associated means for each

speechflanguage group for Model3b appear in Table 4.1 6.

aPost hoc Tests 7

S vs C, p=0.6544 L vs S, p=O.llO3 L vs Cl p=0.0071

Control Speech-only bnguage Note:

T

56.895 57.372 59.200

Page 74: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Models 3a and 3b yielded very similar results. For Model 3a, the mean antisocial T score

for the language impaired group (X=59.200) was significantly higher (p=0.0071) than the

control group ( X=56.895), but not significantly higher (p=0.1103) than the speech-only

impaired group (F=57.372) while the control and speech-only irnpaired groups were not

significantly different from each other (p=0.6544) (see Table 4.1 5). The pattern of

results from Model 3b (Table 4.16) was essentially the sarne as both Models controlled

for similar confounding variables. Thus, differences in the operationalizations of the

Conners and speechllanguage treatment covariates did not appear to alter greatly the

mean parent andior teacher measures of subject antisocial behaviour.

Finally, the exact same patîern of results was found when the raw scores

instead of the T scores were used as the dependent variable. This was true for each of

the above analyses (Models 3a and 3b). Because the raw scores do not allow for easy

interpretation and generalization, the raw score models and means are not reported.

Table 4.16: Adjusted means for each speech/lan$uage group from Model3b.

Comparison of the Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the Non-subject Measures

After the results were adjusted for the potential confounders, the pattern of

significance among the speechJlanguage groups did not change. The magnitude of the

means, however, increased by approxirnately 3 T points for each group. Thus,

increasing the precision of the estimated means by accounting for both hyperactivity and

being reared in a single-parent family adjusted the level of antisocial behaviour across al1

of the speech/language groups. A cornparison of the adjusted and unadjusted estirnated

rneans for the speechAanguage and control groups appears below in Table 4.1 7.

Control Speech-only Language Nofe: afor post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language

impaired group.

X 1 'Pest hoc Tests

56.916 57.460 59.267

S vs C, p=0.6106 L vs S, p=O.ll65 L vs C, p=0.0064

Page 75: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

4.4.2 Subject Measures

Table 4.1 7: Cornparison of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model3a) speechlianguage mean antisocial behaviour estimates for the analysis of non-subject rneasures.

Unadjusted Results

Table 4.1 8 shows that when subjects rated their own antisocial behaviour, there

Unadjusted Results

were no differences in the unadjusted mean antisocial T ratings among the tirne-1

Note: aOverall F-test, bSpeech/language group means. 'for post hoc tests: C=control group. S=speech-only impaired group and L=language

irnpaired group.

a~(2.234)=7.92. p=0.0004

Controls Speech-only

Languaqe

Adjusted Results (Model 3a)

speechflanguage groups. Table 4.1 8 also shows the overall F-statistic for the crude

'~(5.231)=8.36, p=0.0001

Controls Speech-only

Language

model which contains only the time-1 speechnanguage variable.

XI

53.478 54.486 56.892

Table 4.1 8: Mean subject rneasured antisocial T ratings for each speechnanguage

Post hoc Tests

S vs C, p=0.3605 L vs S, p=0.0432 L vs C. p=0.0001

x

56.895 57.372 59.200

Post hoc Tests

" S v s C , p=0.6544 L vs S, p=O.llO3 LvsC,p=0.0071

Note: afor post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speechsnly impaired group and L=language

impaired group.

- - impaired group, unadjusted for any covariates.

Adjusted Results

Control Speech-only

A similar set of linear univariate and ANCOVA analyses were perforrned with

subject-measured antisocial behaviour as the outcome variable. The results frorn these

Lanquaqe 55.177 L vs C, p=0.4330 L

z 55.791 5 5.1 84

analyses are surnrnarized in Table 4.1 9, below.

F(2,251)=0.38, p=0.6836 vs C, p=0.5544

L vs S, p=0.9946

Page 76: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.19: Results from the univariate and ANCOVA analyses for each covariate and for 3ach altemately defin€

3erformance IQ

ulother's Adversity*

nattention (continuous)

fyperactivity (continuous)

2onduct (continuous)

SES*

single-Parent*

Sender

'reatment (subject)

covariate for tt Covariate

Jnivariate Analysis

! subject measures of antisocial behaviour. ANCOVA

Covariate 1 1 SpeecNtanguage Status

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-onIy

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Controls

Speech-only

Language

Alternate Definitions for the Conners and Speech/Language Treatment Variables - nattention (ordinal)

iyperactivity (ordinal).

:onduct (ordinal)

'reatment (parent)

- -

0.3094 0.1 656 Controls 55.90

Speech-only 0,4827 55.1 8 S vs 1. p=0-861

Language 0.2639 55.00

0.1 982 0.1 263 Controls ~ 4 . 9 8 ~

Speech-only 0-4376 55.13 S vs L, p=0.89;

Language 0.2448 ~ 5 . 9 3 ~

0.1 143 0.4949 Controls - 55.82

Speech-only 0.5491 55.20 S vs L, p=0.95[

Language 0.3865 55.13

-0.4688 0.6228 Controls 55.73

Speech-only 0.9655 55.69 S vs L, p=0.37(

Language 0.2486 54.70

Page 77: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

From Table 4.19, above: Note: a =parameter estimate: bp,probability; X =adjusted mean estimate; d ~ - ~ differences=p-values comparing the means of the speech-only group and the language group; '=adjusted and unadjusted means

differ by 10% or more; *=parameter estimate is significant at the p=0.10 levei in its ANCOVA; *=covariate

entered into the modei building process (i.e. was significant at the p=0.10 level in its ANCOVA (*) andlor t

changed by 10% or more ( )).

Each one of the four covaBate sets described in section 3.9.3 yielded the same final

model. This final mode1 is henceforth referred to as "Model4". The modei, and the overall

F-test for the subject measured antisocial scores appears below, The mean subject

measured antisocial scores for each speecMlanguage group appear in Table 4.20.

Model4: Overa Il F-test: F(4,249)=1.48, p=0.2079 (n=254). ASBçubjea = a+~1(Speech-only)+~2(Language)+Pî(Single-Parent)

group from Model4.

Control

Note: "for post hoc tests: C=conirol group, S=speech-only impairec! group and L=language

impaired grou p.

The results from the subject-measured analyses did not yield significant differences at

the p=0.05 level. The language impaired group ( %=56.62) was not significantly different

from either the speech-only impaired group ( X =S6.83, p=0.8518) or from the control

group (X=57.36, p=0.3438). The speech-only impaired and control groups were also not

significantly different (p=0.5998). Once again, that al1 four covariate sets yielded the

same final model suggests that variations in the operationalization of the Conners and

speechllanguage treatment covariates does not alter the conclusions drawn from these

results.

Finally, the exact same pattern of results was found when the raw scores

instead of the T scores were used as the dependent variable. Because the raw scores

do not allow for easy interpretation and generalization, the raw score models and means

are not reported.

Page 78: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Comparïson of the Unadjusted and Adjusted Results for the Subject Measures

In this analysis, there were no significant differences found among the

speechllanguage groups before or after adjustment. After adjusting for the variable of

single-parent rearing, the increase in the precision of estimated means was exhibited by

an increase in their magnitude. This increase of approximately 2 T points, however, is

small.

4.5 Summary of Unadjusted and Adjusted Results The first part of this section summarizes the results when they were not adjusted

Table 4.21 : CompaRson of the unadjusted and the adjusted (Model3a) speechllanguage mean antisocial behaviour estimates for the analysis of subject measures.

by any of the covariates used in this investigation. Among the dichotomous measures of

antisocial behaviour, a language impairment at time-1 was associated with a significant

risk for a diagnosis of an ASPD ( @=2.879, 95%CL: 1 -222-6-783) as well as for the

Unadjusted Results

occurrence of cnminal behaviour at time-3 ( tj7 =2.258, 95%CL: 1.1 53-4.422). A speech-

Note: "Overall F-test. bSpeecManguage group means.

Adjusted ResuIts

only impairment at time-?, however, was not associated with a significant risk for either

of these outcornes at time-3. Among the dimensional measures of antisocial behaviour,

non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour rated those subjects who had been

language impaired at time-1 as significantly more antisocial ( Z=56.9) than either those

who had been speech-only impaired ( F=54.5) or controls ( Z=53.5) at time-1 (p=0.0001

and p=0.04, respectively). Subject measures of antisocial behaviour, however, did not

yield any differences at time-3 among the speechAanguage groups. The unadjusted

results are summarized in Table 4.22, below.

Post hoc Tests

S vs C, p=0.5544 L vs S, p=0.9946 L v s C,p=0.4330

aF(z,251)=o-38, p=0.6836

Controls Speech-only

Language

'F(4,249)=1.48, p=0.2079

Controls Speech-only

- Language

X

5 S . 7 9 1 55.184 55.177

K

57.36 56.83 56.62

Post hoc Tests

S vs C, p=0.5998 L vs S, p=0.8518 L vs C, p=0.3438

Page 79: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 4.22: Summary table of the results unadjusted for any of the covariates for the dichotomous outcomes (diagnoses of ASPD and occurrence of criminal behaviour) and dimensional outcomes (non-subject and subject measures), respectively.

Dichotomous Outcomes 1 Diagnoses of ASPD I Criminal Behaviour I

I Dimensional Outcomes I

Speech-only

Language

1 Non-Subject Measures 1 Subject Measures 1

"w 1 -803

2.879

--

Note: aodds ratios defined with respect to the control group. b95% confidence limits for the #S.

I m

'unadjusted mean antisocial behaviour score. d for post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language

impaired group.

~S%CL(W>

(0.576-5.641 )

(1.222-6.783)

The adjusted results differ somewhat from the unadjusted results. In the latter,

Controls

S~eech-on l~

Language

when analyzing cnminal behaviour, the language impaired group had a significant

associated odds ratio of 2.258 (95%CL:I -1 534.422). However, when the variable of

Speech-only

Language

cx 53,478

5 4.48 6

56.892

hyperactivity was controlled (as in Model 2a), the corresponding odds ratio of 1.777 was

no longer significant (95%CL:0.880-3.587). A similar change between adjusted and

unadjusted results was observed when the variables of hyperactivtiy, SES and gender

F(2,251)=0.38, p=0.6836

dS vs C, p=0.5544

L vs SI p=0.9946

L vs CI p=0.4330

F(2,234)=6.82, p=0.0004

dS vs CI p=0.3605

L vs SI p=0.0432

L vs CI p=0.0001

were controlled, as in Model2b. The associated odds ratio for criminal behaviour for the

"@ 2 - 0 5 6

2.258

speech-only impaired group, however, was non-significant before and after adjusting for

9 5 8 ~ ~ ( ~ )

(O .863-4.896)

( 1 - 1 53-4.422)

C~ntrols

S ~ e e c h - o n l ~

Language

covariates. In addition to the pattern of significance changing, it should also be noted that

"X

55.791

5 5.1 8 4

55.177

when this relationship was controlled, the magnitude of the odds ratios decreased from

2.056 and 2.258 to 1.797 and 1.777, for the speech-only and language impaired groups,

respectively.

For the analysis of ASPD, the pattern of results for the associated odds ratios for

the speech-only impaired and language impaired groups did not change. Both before and

after adjustment, the odds ratio for the speech-onty impaired group was non-significant.

As well, the odds ratio for the language impaired group was significant before adjustment

(2.879, 95%CL:1.222-6.783) and after adjustment (3.662, 95%CL: 1 -448-9.01 2). Although

Page 80: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

the pattern of significance did not change, the magnitude of the odds ratios did change,

increasing from 1.803 and 2.879 to 1.973 and 3.662. for the speech-only impaired and

language impaired groups, respedively.

For the dimensional measures of antisocial behaviour, the pattern of results did

not change greatly. Both before and after the results were controlled, parent andlor

teacher measures of the antisocial behaviour of the subjects, rated those who had been

language impaired the most antisocial. Before adjustment. this group was considered

significantly more antisocial than both the control (p=0.0001) and speech-only impaired

(p=0.0432) groups (see Table 4.8). However, after controlling for the covariates. the

antisocial behaviour of the language impaired group ( F=59.2) and speech-only impaired

group (%=57.4) were not rated as significantly different from each other (p=0.1103).

Among the non-subjed measures of antisocial behaviour. the pattern of results did not

change. Both before and after control, subjects did not rate themselves any more or less

antisocial based on their time-1 speechnanguage status. These adjusted results are

summarked in Table 4.23.

Table 4.23: Sumrnary table of the results adjusted for the covariates for the dichotomous outcornes (diagnoses of ASPD and occurrence of criminal behaviour) and dimensional

Diagnoses of ASPD (Mode1 1) 1 Criminal Behaviour (Mode1 2a) 1

I Dimensional Outcornes I I Non-Subject Measures (Model 3a) I Subject Measures (Model 4)

Speech-only

Language

Controls s

56.895 S vs C, p=0.6544 Contmls 57.36 d S vs C, p=0.5998

Speech-only

Language

L vs C, p=0.0071 Language 56.62 L vs C, p=0.3438

Note: Only the results from Models 1, 2a, 3a and 4 are repoited in this table.

a @

1 -973

3.662

'odds ratios defined with respect to the control group. -

b95% confidence lirnits for the Ys. 'adjusteci mean antisocial behaviour score.

for post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language impaired group.

b 95% CL($)

(0.575-6.767)

(1.488-9.01 2)

a$

1 -797

1.777

95% CL(@)

(0.741 -4.361 ) (0.880-3.587)

Page 81: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

5. Results II: Supplementary Analyses The dependent variables of antisocial behaviour were operationalized in two main

ways, as a dichotornous outcome and as a dimensional outcorne. For the former type,

significant differences were found arnong the speechnanguage groups when antisocial

behaviour was defined as a diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (ASPD),

however, non-significant differences were found when the dependent variable was

defined as the presence or absence of criminal behaviour. For the latter type, significant

differences were found when antisocial behaviour was measured by parents and/or

teachers, however, non-significant differences were found when subjects measured

their own antisocial behaviour. The purpose of the present section was to explore the

apparent inconsistency between subject and non-subject measured results. The

discrepancy between the ASPD and criminal behaviour results was not explored in this

study (see Discussion). Although differences between subject and non-subject ratings

are not uncornmon in the psychiatrie and psychological literature, four main

supplernentary analyses were performed in an effort to more fully understand the

differences observed,

5.1 The Composite Nature of the Dimensionai Measure of Antisocial

Behaviour

As mentioned in section 3.4.2, the dimensional measure of antisocial behaviour is

the mean of two separate subscale scores; the Delinquency subscale and the

Aggression subscale. Table 2.1 showed, however, that the correlation between these

scores was the lowest (r=0.45, p=0.0001) for the subject-measured data. It was

hypothesized that one of the subscaIes, rnight in fact, be more sensitive in the ability to

detect differences among the speech/language groups. Furthemore, that these two

subscale scores were not highly correlated, using the mean of the subscales might

inadvertently be obscunng the ability of the composite variabte to detect any differences.

To test this hypothesis, a separate analysis was performed for each subscale. If this

hypothesis was correct, the two analyses would yield different results, one of which

would resemble the non-subject results. A paraflel analysis was also performed using

the non-subject measures. Finally, these analyses were also performed with the

corresponding raw scores. A surnmary of the findings from the analyses of the subject-

measures appear in Table 5.1 and the summary of the findings from the non-subject

measures appear in Table 5.2 (the corresponding final models are not shown).

Page 82: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table 5.1 : SpeechJlanguage group mean T scores and raw scores for the subject- rneasured Delinquency and Aggression Subscales.

1 Delinquency Aggression F(4,249)=2.04, p=0.0891 F(4.249)=0.65, p=0.6286

1

F Post hoc Tests % Post hoc Tests Control 59.206 S vs C, p=0.9501 55.51 9 S vs C, pd.3619

T scores Speech-only 59.1 25 L vs S, pa.6858 54.530 L vs S. pd.8920 Language 58.558 L vs C. p=0.5169 54.687 L vs C. pd.3166

r Post hoc Tests x Post hoc Tests Control 4.964 S vs C, pd.8046 9.408 S vs C. p=û.6021

Raw scores Speech-only 5.093 L vs S. p=0.5594 9.560 L vs S. ~ 4 . 8 7 8 1 Language 4.766 L vs C, pa.6212 t 0.037 L vs C, pa.3704

Note: 'for post hoc tests: C=control group. S=speechsnly irnpaired group and L=language

impaired group.

Table 5.2: Speechllanguage group rnean T scores and raw scores for the non-subject measured Delinquency and Aggression Subscales.

Control

I -

Control Raw scores Speech-on1 y

1 Language

Note: 'for post hoc tests: C=control group, S=speech-only impaired group and L=language

impaired group.

The Table 5.1 results show that when the Aggression subscale was used, no

Delinquency

F(4.232)=5.84, p=0.0002

significant differences were found among the speechllanguage groups (for either the T

x 58.1 78 59.260 61.606

Aggression

F(4,232)=4.23, p=0.0025

scores or the raw scores) when subject-measured scores were analyzed. However,

Post hoc Tests S vs C. pd.3895 L vs S. p=0.0847 L vs C, p=0.0005

X 54.773 55.843 57.781

x 3.538 4.067 4.969

near significant differences were found when the Delinquency subscale was used

F(4,232)=6.02, p=0.0001

Post hoc Tests S vs C, pd.3349 L vs S. p=0.1061 L vs C. p=0.0006

(overali F-tests: p=0.0891 and p=0.0990, for the T scores and raw scores, respectively).

F(4.232)=4.03, p=0.0035

Post hoc Tests S vs C, pd.3293 L vs S, p=0.1240 L vs C, p=0.0008

Table 5.2 shows that a sirnilar pattern was found when the non-subject measured

scores were analyzed: both scales found significant differences among the

- X

5.676 6.276 8.577

speechllanguage groups, but the ~elinquency scale yielded far more significant results

Post hoc Tests S vs C, p=0.581 O L vs S, p=0.0508 L vs C. p4.0007

(p=0.0002 and p=0.0001 for the T and raw scores, respectiveiy) than did the Aggression

subscale (p=0.0025 and p=0.0035 for the T raw scores, respectively).

Page 83: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

The above hypothesis appears to be only partly correct: the separate analyses

did produce different results, however, the pattern of differences among the

speechnanguage scores was not significant at the pc0.05 level and the pattern of

differences was dissimilar tu that of the non-subject results. For example, the language

impaired group had the lowest scores (58.558 and 54.687 for the Delinquency and

Aggression subscales, respectively) for the subject-measured data, while the same

group had the highest scores (61.606 and 57.781 for the Delinquency and Aggression

subscales, respectively) for the non-subject measured data. Because the standard

errors of the means for the subject-measured data are approximately the same as for the

equivalent means from the non-subject scores, it would be unreasonable to assume that

with an increase in power, the pattern of speechllanguage scores would change. The

above hypothesis, therefore, does not appear to explain the discrepancy between the

subject and non-subject measured scores.

5.2 The Composite Nature of the Non-subject Measure of Antisocial

Behaviour

As mentioned in section 3.4.2, due to the large proportion of missing values

among the teacher ratings, the teacher and parent ratings were combined to form a "non-

subject" measure of antisocial behaviour. The possibility that the pattern of means for the

speechllanguage groups as measured by the parent or teacher ratings was similar to the

pattern from the subject ratings and that the composite rating was obscuring this was

also considered. It was hypothesized that if the pattern of differences among the

speechllanguage groups for parent and teacher measures were grossly dissirnilar

where either was similar to the subject-measured pattern, then this would detract from

the validity of the non-subject measures and allow for re-consideration of the differences

between the subject and non-subject results. The results form this analysis are

presented in Figure 5.1, below:

Page 84: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Controls Speech- Language only

Time-1 Speech/Language Status Group

+ Subject-measured data -i- Non-subject measured data -+- Parent-measured data --

* Teacher-measured data

:igure 5.1 : Adjusted subject and non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour by time-1

This figure shows the adjusted mean antisocial T score for the subject and non-

subject rneasures, as welt as the individual parent and teacher measures, for each

speechllanguage group. Three points about this figure and the data it illustrates can be

made: First, by cornpanng the non-subject and parent measures, it can be seen that the

non-subject means closely resemble the parent-measured means. This is due to the fact

that a majority of the non-subject measures were composed of parent-rated data as a

much greater proportion of teacher-rated data was missing (36% vs 23% for teachers

and parents, respectively). Second, parent- and teacher-measured data are similar with

respect to the pattern of differences among the speechllanguage groups in that both

perceived the language impaired group to be very different from the speech-only irnpaired

and control groups and the speech-only irnpaired group not to be very different frorn the

control group. This corresponds to the correlation of parent- and teacher-measured data

that appeared in Table 3.2 ( ~0 .43 , p=0.0001). Third, that when al1 four rneasures of

antisocial behaviour are plotted together the pattern of the subject-rneasured means are

clearly different from the patterns observed for the other three measures. This analysis,

Page 85: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

therefore, lends credence to the non-subject measures of antisocial behaviour, but does

not help explain the discrepancy between the subject and non-subject measured results.

5.3 Group Differences in Subject-perceived Social Support

In an atternpt to understand why the subject and non-subject ratings differed rnost

for the language impaired group, differences among the speechAanguage groups in the

ways in which the subjects believed they were perceived by others was assessed.

To rneasure this, the Social Support Appraisal (SSA) scale2 was used. No significant

difierences were found, however, on any of the individual ratings or summary scores

(al1 p>0.17). In a second atternpt to address this issue, the sarne set of analyses were

perfomed only on those subjects who had been diagnosed with an ASPD. Similarly, no

significant differences (or near significant differences) among the speechllanguage

groups were found (al1 p>0.11). Because this set of analyses only yielded negative

findings, they do not irnprove Our understanding of the differences between the subject

and non-su bject results.

5.4 ASPD and Subject Measures of Antisocial Behaviour

Using a diagnosis of an ASPD as a gold-standard, subjects with and without a

diagnosis were compared as to how they rated their own antisocial behaviour. The

same analysis was also performed for non-subject measures. The results for this

analyses are presented in Table 5.3, below:

Table 5.3: Subiect-measured mean antisocial behaviour T scores for those with and without a diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD).

Subject Measures 1 Non-subject Measures

ASPDAbsent 154.79 1 p=O.OOOl 1 1 p=O-0001

ASPD Present 6 1 -35

This table shows that subjects with an ASPD do indeed perceive themsetves as more

antisocial than those without an ASPD (p=0.0001). The same was true with non-subject

'The SSA is a 23 item instrument that taps the extent to which the individual believes that he or she is loved by, esteemed by, and involved with family. friends and others. The SSA has been reported to have very good psychometric properties (Vaux, 1 986).

Page 86: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

measures, that is, subjects with an ASPD are rated independently by parents andlor

teachers as more antisocial than those without an ASPD (p=0.0001). ln this respect,

there appears to be agreement between diagnoses of ASPD and self-rated scores of

antisocial behaviour. It was hypothesized that having a speechnanguage impairment

might be associated with a poorer tendency for self-rated scores to agree with the gold-

standard diagnosis of an ASPD. If this were true, then rneasures o f agreement would be

srnallest for the language impaired group than for the speech-only irnpaired or control

groups. To detennine this, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated for each

speecManguage group to measure the agreement between ASPD and subject-

rneasures. Table 5.4 shows these correlations as well as the subject rneasured scores

for those with and without an ASPD for each speechflanguage group.

Table 5.4: Subject measures of antisocial behaviour for those with and without an ASPD

1 Pearson Correlation 1 0.3536. p =0.0001 1 0.6845. p=O.OOO1 1 0.3454. p=0.0021 1

and the associated Pearson correlation coefficients for each speech/language group.

These data show that the self-reports frorn the language impaired subjects tend to agree

the least (r=0.345) with the gold-standard of a diagnosis of an ASPD, however, for

reasons unknown, the control group also had a very low level of agreement r=0.354).

The correlation coefficients for both the control and language impaired groups were not

significantly different from each other, but each was significantly different ( ~ ~ 0 . 0 5 ) from

the correlation coefficient for the speech-only impaired group. To formalize the

relationship between tevel of agreement and speechAanguage group, the interaction

between subject rneasures of antisocial behaviour and speechAanguage group was

tested in the following model:

ASPD Absent

lagit [P(Dx ASPD=present))=a+fl~(subject measures)+&(speechilanguage group)+b(Gender)+ &(subject masures X speedinanguage group)

The likelihood ratio test for the interaction t e n approached significance k2(2df)=5.1 194,

p=O.O773).

A similar model was used to test for the interaction between non-subject rneasures of

antisocial behaviour and speechnanguage group:

Control

55.30 1 p=O.OOOI S~eech-onlv

53.73 1 p=0.0001 Lanuuaqe

54.39 1 p = 0 . 0 0 2 1

Page 87: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

logit [P(Dx ASPD=present)~+~~(non-subjed measures)+&(speeManguage group)+&(Gender)+

&(non-subjed measures X speecManguage group)

The liklihood ratio test for the interaction term in this model was non-significant

(X2(2d9=1 -4929, p=O.4?4O).

Thus, these results show that when subjects rate their own antisocial behaviour, the

degree to which the subject measures distinguish those with and without an ASPD may

depend on the speechAanguage group king measured- But, when non-subjects rate the

antisocial behaviour, the degree to which non-subject measures agree with a diagnosis

of an ASPD does not depend on the speechllanguage group being rneasured. Although

the former interaction only approached significance as well as the fact that the control

group appeared to have a low level of agreement, these findings appear to partially

confirm the above hypothesis and warrant the further study of subject measure

reliability.

Page 88: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

6. Discussion

6.1 Ovewiew of Findings

The purpose of this study was to determine whether speechllanguage

impairments identified in children as young as five years of age were associated with

antisocial behaviour in young adulthood. Antisocial behaviour was measured among

three groups of 19 year olds which had been fonned by the age 5 speechjlanguage

status of their members. These three groups that were studied comprised a language

impaired group (some of the subjects of which may have had an additional speech

impairment), a speech-only irnpaired group and a control group. In summary, the main

findings of this study were:

Early childhood speechllanguage impairments were associated with subsequent

antisocial behaviour. For the outcome of antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), the

risk was approximately triple for individuals with a history of language impairment

than for those who were controls.

A Ianguage impairment at 5 years of age was associated with an increased risk (as

compared to controls) for being diagnosed with an ASPD at age 19, while a speech-

only impairment at 5 years of age was not.

Neither a language impairment nor a speech-only impairment at 5 years of age was

associated with an increased risk for exhibiting criminal behaviour by age 19 when

adjusted for the covariates.

When parents andlor teachers rated the antisocial behaviours of the subjects at age

19, those who had been language impaired at age 5 were considered more antisocial

than those who had been controls, but not more antisocial than those who had been

speech-only irnpaired. Those who had been speech-only impaired were not

considered different from those who had been controls.

Although the language impaired group was rated by parents and/or teachers as more

antisocial than controls, whether the level of antisocial behaviour of the language

impaired group is of dinical significance is unclear.

The differences in age 19 antisocial behaviour among the age 5 speechllanguage

groups still held after controlling for the confounders of speechllanguage treatment,

hyperactivity, SES, gender and single-parent reanng.

When the subjects measured dirnensionally their own antisocial behaviour at age 19,

no differences were found arnong the groups as defined by age 5 speechllanguage

status.

Page 89: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

This study appears to be the first to have evaluated in a prospective and

longitudinal design a comrnunity sample of speechnanguage impaired and control children

over 14 years, from kindergarten to young adulthood, in assessing the long-terrn

psychiatnc implications of eariy childhood speechnanguage impairments. As it is known

that speechnanguage impairments are associated with concurrent and subsequent

symptorns of inattention and hyperactivity, this study explored the hypothesis that eariy

childhood speechlianguage impairments would be associated with young adulthood

antisocial behaviour. The hypothesis proved to be partly true: depending on how

antisocial behaviour is rneasured in young adulthood early childhood speechJlanguage

impairments did predict subsequent antisocial behaviour in the sample of individuals

studied. When it was operationalized dichotomously in ternis of diagnoses of ASPD or

dimensionally in ternis of parents and/or teacher reports, antisocial behaviour was found

to be associated with speecManguage impairments. When operationalized

dichotomously in terms of criminal activity or when operationalized dimensionally in ternis

of subject self-reports, when adjusted for covariates, no association was found. This

study reports important long-term prospective findings that have substantial implications

for speech/language impaired children and the parents and health care providers who

care for thern. The main features of this study's findings that warrant discussion are

sumrnarized as follows:

Language impaiments were found to be associated with antisocial behaviour while

speech-only irnpairments were not. This is consistent with the Iiterature that reports

poorer outcome arnong language impaired children versus speech-only impaired

children.

Early childhood speechllanguage impairments have antisocial behavioural

consequences, but they were not associated with increased rates of criminal activity

after controlling for the covariates.

When the subjects reported their own antisocial behaviour, their age 5

speechJlanguage status was unrelated to their scores. However, when parents

andfor teachers measured the subject's antisocial behaviour, the scores were related

to the subjects' age 5 speechJlanguage status.

The inff uence of several covariates was controlled. Only five of them, however,

proved to be confounding variables: hyperactivity, single-parent rearing, SES, gender

and having received speechJlanguage treatment. As well, variations in the definitions

of some covariates did not affect the results.

Page 90: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

6.2 Poorer outcome among those language versus speech-only impaired

Children who had been Ianguage impaired had consistently worse outcomes than

those who had been speech-only irnpaired when differences in the long-tem outcornes

of the age 5 speechflanguage impaired groups (ASPD and non-subject rated analyses)

were shown. Although no other studies have examined specifically the long-term

psychiatnc outcornes among speechnanguage irnpaired children in the young adulthood

years, this pattern of outcome is consistent with studies that have followed such children

over shorter periods of time. Cantwell& Baker (1980) and Baker & CantwelI (1982a)

followed children aged 2-15.9 years (with a mean age of 5.1 years) for 5 years and

reported that children with "pure language" disorders were more likely than those with

'pure articulation" disorders to have later psychiatric and psychosocial problems. The

Newcastle study (Fundudis et al., 1979) which followed children from age 3 to age 8

reported that children with language delays were more likely than children with "speech

retardationn to show marked introversion and withdrawal. The present study extends

such behavioural findings into the young adulthood years by showing that children with a

history of language impairments continue to be more affected than those with a history of

speech-only impairments.

This pattern of outcome is also consistent with studies that have followed

speech/language irnpaired children into the young adulthood years but which did not

assess behavioural or psychiatric outcomes (Felsenfeld, Broen & McGue, 1992.1 994;

Hall & Tomblin, 1978; King, Jones & Lasky, 1982; Lewis & f reebairn, 1992; Tomblin,

Freese & Records, 1992). Johnson recently reported a 14-year follow-up study on the

speech/language stability of the same OLS sarnple studied in the present investigation

(Johnson, 1998, submitted for publication). This author reports three related long-terni

findings. First, those with initia1 language irnpairments fared worse than those with initial

speech-only impaiments, showing lower rates of recovery. Second, at age 19, those

who had been speech-only impaired at âge 5 were comparable to controls on most

measures of language, cognition and academic performance, while those who had

language impairment performed significantly worse than those who had been speech-

only impaired or controls. Third, considerable long-terni stability of individual differences

in language ability were also apparent and adult levels of language skills were well

predicted by eariy language measures. The present investigation is significant because it

adds antisocial behaviour to the list of long-terni potential problems among those initially

language, but not speech, impaired.

The issue as to why those with language impairment fare worse than those with

speech-only impaiments is likely related to the degree to which early childhood language,

76

Page 91: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

as opposed to speech, impaiments represent a cognitive and developmental obstacle to

the child. A chiid with only a speech articulation problem faiis to make the correct

articulation of speech sounds at the developmentally appropriate age and may also have

a problem with phonology and discriminating certain sounds, Speech articulation,

however, is only one aspect of language ability. Problerns with articulation are mild

compared to and are in sharp contrast with the linguistic features of language

impaiments which may be of an expressive or receptive nature. Children with such

language impairments tend to exhibit unusual word orden, vocabulary errors or difficulty

understanding spatial ternis or complex "if-thenn statements (APA, 1987). The

development of these fundamental language skills, especially dun'ng earfy childhood, are

closely related to the development of cognition and the maturation of the brain (Crittenden,

1996). Since the c l a m of speech articulation is only one aspect of language ability, it

seems less likely that speech-only impaiments rather than language impaiments would

have such long-term effects.

The findings from studies of hearing impaired children supports this idea. Many

such children struggle with speech impairments secondary to sensorineural deafness

(Howlin & Rutter, l987), but nevertheless develop fundamental language skills by

acquiring a non-spoken mode of communication such as Amencan Sign Language (ASL).

Hearing impaired children who are immersed in ASL-based education earîy in childhood

acquire greater overall language skills and report fewer psychosocial problems later in

life (Paul & Jackson, 1993). Crittenden (1 996) argues that the "pragmaticsn or practical

and functional aspects of language are crucial in the developing structure and function of

the minds of young children. Because of the importance of effective communication in a

social worid, language impaiments seern more detrimental than speech-only impaiments

in impeding the practical aspects of communication and socialization.

The notion that language but not speech-only irnpairments lead specifically to

problems of antisocial behaviour remains unclear and requires the investigation of other

long-tem psychiatric outcomes. lt does seem unlikely, given that some of the major

psychiatric outcomes of speechnanguage impaired individuals in late childhood include

problems in the domains of anxiety and social relationships (Rutter & Mawhood, 1991).

That these problems continue or perhaps lead to other non-antisocial problems in the

young adulthood years remains to be shown.

6.3 Evidence of antisocial personality disorder versus criminal behaviour

Adult antisocial behaviour is defined and studied several ways. Criminologists

tend to focus on persistent offending as they study crirninal careers. Clinical behavioural

Page 92: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

scientists tend to focus on antisocial personality disorder, the officia1 diagnostic category

of the DSM-III-R (APA, 1987). Personality psychologists rnay focus on the psychopath,

an antisocial designation that depends as much on personality style as it does on illegal

behaviour (Moffitt & Lynarn, 1994). Although the three designations have partially

overlapping critena sets, they are conceptually distinct (Robins 8 Reiger, 1991). This

study addressed antisocial behaviour using the first two definitions in addition to a

dimensional conceptualization. That each of the definitions used is conceptually distinct

seems to have been supported by this study's findings. Perhaps the antisocial behaviour

exhibited by the language impaired group is of neither the severity nor the nature such

that these individuals would have increased rates of contact with the police or courts.

Perhaps this present study failed to measure ctiminal activity in a way that was

sufficiently sensitive. This is possible as many contacts with the law may have

occurred, but remained unofficia1 (police may have not pressed charges but rather given

a waming). The interview which collected this data did not assess such information.

Whether the language impaired subjects in this present study will go on to develop

more severe antisocial behaviour is uncertain. However, it suggests that the antisocial

behaviour exhibited by the language irnpaired subjects is of a different nature from the

antisocial behaviour observed in persistent cnminals. The finding that antisocial

behaviour in adults is almost invariably associated with a history of such behaviour in

childhood usually describes a subset of individuals whose antisocial behaviour has

remained severe and stable throughout their Iives (Moffitt & Lynarn, 1994). The language

irnpaired subjects in the present study do not match that description with respect to the

severity or the development of their antisocial behaviour. For example, when parents

andlor teachers rated the antisocial behaviour of the subjects, the highest mean score,

given to the language impaired group, was only 59.2. This mean T score is not very large

and only corresponds to approximately the 84th percentile in the population. As well, this

score does not meet the Achenbach designated clinical rating of 70. Furthermore, 1

(0.7%), O (0%) and 5 (4.9%) subjects scored above the clinical cutoff in the control,

speech-only impaired and language impaired groups, respectively. These findings seem

to suggest that the majority of children with language impairments do have a statistically

significant, although clinically rnild level of antisocial behaviour, but that a small subgroup

(5%) have clinically significant antisocial behaviour.

Although this study atternpted to assess criminal activity as one measure of

antisocial behaviour, it does not assume that crirninal behaviour represents the rnost

severe form of antisocial behaviour. The inclusion of criminal activity as an outcome was

perfomed in an effort to identify the possible ways in which antisocial behaviour might

Page 93: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

express itself in individuals with a history of speechllanguage impairment. This study

also does not assume that the pathways leading to crirninal activity might necessarily

involve a history of speechnanguage impairment. It is possible, however, that

speechllanguage impairments might predispose individuals to other risk factors that are

more closely related to subsequent increased contacts with the law.

Although this study reports odds ratios for the outcomes of ASPD and criminal

behaviour, they rnay be thought of as relative risks as the study was perfomed

prospectively and because these outcomes can be considered "raren (7.75% and

15.27% arnong the controls for ASP D and criminal behaviour, respectively) (Rothman €4

Greenland, 1998). Although the odds ratio for the language irnpaired group was 3.66 for

ASPD, odds ratios are known to slightly overestimate the relative risk. The unadjusted

risks were 7.75% and 19.48% for the control and language irnpaired groups,

respectively. This corresponds to an unadjusted relative risk of 2.51 while the

unadjusted odds ratio was 2.88. Thus, the adjusted odds ratio of 3.66 is a slight

overestimate of the adjusted relative risk. Therefore, it can be assumed that true relative

rÎsk is approximately three. Equivalently, those with a history of language impaiments

were approximately 3 tirnes more Iikely than controls to be diagnosed with an ASPD.

6.4 Subject versus non-subject measures

That the present study found confiicting results between subject and non-subject

(parent andlor teacher) measures of antisocial behaviour was not unexpected. Lack of

agreement among multiple sources is a cornmon finding in the psychiatrie and

psychological literature (Achenbach, McConaughy & Howell, i987). The purpose of

using subject, in addition to parent and teacher, measures of antisocial behaviour was to

detemine whether earfy childhood speechllanguage impairments affected the subjects'

perception and reports of their own behaviour. At 19 years of age, the subjects did not

report differences in their levels of antisocial behaviour based on their age 5

speechIlanguage impairment status. However, non-subject measures did report

differences, scoring those children who had been language impaired as significantly

more antisocial than controls. Subjects who had been speechlonly impaired at tirne-1,

were not considered to be any more antisocial than controls. These parent andlor

teacher findings are similar ta those for the analysis of ASPD. That is, those with a

history of speech-only impairment were not considered to be at greater risk than controls

for antisocial behaviour, while those with a history of language impaiments were at

greater risk than controls.

79

Page 94: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

A number of analyses were performed in an effort to understand why such

differences in the results would have occurred between the two main rater types. It

was first hypothesized that subjects really did report differences in antisocial behaviour

based on their tirne-1 speechiianguage status, but that this was being obscured by the

composite nature of the dependent variable. Subsequent analyses were performed on

the individual delinquency and aggression subscales; significant differences were still

not found. In a second attempt, it was hypothesized that the merging of parent and

teacher measures obscured the fact that one of the rater cornponents had actually rated

the subjects in the sarne way the subjects rated themselves. This was also proven false

as separate parent and teacher analyses showed that they both perceived language

impaired subjects as being significantly more antisocial than controls and speech-only

impaired subjects as not being different from controls. A third hypothesis, that possible

differences existed among the speech/language groups in how they believed their

families, friends and others perceived them also did not prove to be revealing. All three

speechnanguage groups reported similar scores on al1 such rneasures.

A fourth attempt was made by considering the reliability of the subject measures.

It was hypothesized that the subjects were not perceiving andlor reporting their true

levels of antisocial behaviour. This was assessed by first determining whether or not

those subjects who had been diagnosed with an ASPD considered themselves any more

antisocial than those who had not been diagnosed. In this respect, a diagnosis of ASPD

was thought of as a "gold-standardn. This analysis was useful. Those who had been

diagnosed did see themselves as significantly more antisocial than those who had not. A

parallel analysis with non-subject measures also provided the same result. Thus, it

appeared as though both subject and non-subjects ratings could be used to distinguish

between those who were truly antisocial and those who were not. It was then

considered that this rnay not be the case across each of the time-1 speechllanguage

groups. That is, for any one of the groups, subject rneasures rnay be less reliable than

the others in identifying those who were tmly antisocial. This appeared to be sornewhat

true as an analysis of the interaction between speechllanguage status and subject-

measured scores was performed: Speechllanguage status did alter the way subjects

rated their own behaviour, with those who had been language impaired and diagnosed

with ASPD giving themselves the lowest antisocial ratings. A parallel analysis using the

non-subject measures did not yield an equivalent interaction between speechAanguage

status and non-subject measured scores. This suggests that self-reports of antisocial

behaviour may be under-rated from subjects with a history of language impairment and

may, therefore, not be as accurate as the reports from non-language impaired subjects.

Page 95: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

It should also be noted that another possible explanation is that the parents

andfor the teachers of the language impaired subjects rnay have been influenced by the

knowledge of the subject's impairment. For example, although the parents were blinded

to the tirne-î speechnanguage status of their children, the disabilities of those who were

most impaired likely Ied to the disclosure of the irnpaitment to the parents. The handicaps

of some children rnay have been such that the parents sought out assessrnent and

treatment for the child. Either way, the parent's knowledge of such impairments rnay

have biased the antisocial ratings of their children, incorredly leading them to believe that

because their child was language impaired, he or she must, therefore, be somewhat

antisocial. It is unclear, however, whether this bias is specific to the domain of antisocial

behaviour or whether it includes other behaviours, as well.

As well, the non-language impaired subjects rnay be more able to hide or disguise

their antisociaI behaviour from their parents andlor teachers while those language

impaired subjects rnay be iess able. The ability to hide such behaviours rnay be indexed

by their intelligence. The summary statistics would support such a hypothesis as the

controls had the highest levels of performance IQ while those language impaired had the

lowest. If such an explanation were correct, the parent andlor teacher ratings of

language impaired subjects would, in fact, be the most accurate rather than the least.

The finding, however, that language impaired subjects diagnosed with an ASPD assign

themselves the lowest antisocial ratings compared to non-language impaired subjects

with an ASPD does not make this hypothesis seem likely.

It is possible that the differences between the subject and non-subject rneasures

are at least partly due to the differences in the type of behaviours that are actually

scored arnong the subject, parent and teacher instruments (YSR, CBCL and TRF,

respectively). For example, the TRF assesses behaviours that are not assessed by the

YSR and would, therefore, not be endorsed by the subjects. These behaviour include

among others: disturbs classrnates, talks out of turn, is explosive and disrupts class.

Unfortunately, inspection of the individual behaviour items that were endorsed by each

rater was not possible, but might prove illustrative in future studies to reveal specifically

which behaviaurs are most readily endorsed by each rater type as well as for each

speechnanguage group.

What seems likely in explaining the differences between subject and non-subjed

findings is situational specificity (Pervin 1985). The situations, environments or contexts

in which parents and/or teachers observe the subjects rnay have allowed for

differences in antisocial tendencies among the speecWlanguage groups to be

rnanifested. While subject-measures, on the other hand, are more akin to the subjects'

Page 96: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

perception of themseives across situations which rnay, in fact, not differ arnong

speechAanguage groups.

6.5 Confounding

M i l e examining the relationship between early childhood speechhanguage

impairment and young adulthood antisocial behaviour, this study also assessed the

influence of nine covariates. The covariates were: intelligence, mother's adversity,

hyperactivity, inattention, conduct problems, single-parent rearing, SES, gender and

having received speechnanguage treatment. All of these covariates were either known

or suspected risk factors for antisocial behaviour. Few of thern, however, proved to

confound the relationship under study. The use of the multiple regression and model

building techniques used in this study needs to be descnbed and emphasized to ensure

the correct interpretation of the various final rnodels as well as the estimates of antisocial

behaviour that they provide.

Wth an independent variable of pnmary interest (in this study, speechnanguage

status), multiple regression techniques allow the investigator to estimate odds ratios (in

the case of logistic regression) or rneans (in the case of linear regression) that take into

account the other independent variables which appear in the regression equation. These

other independent variables rnay be risk factors andfor confounding variables. Although

any number of independent variables rnay appear in a regression equation, the larger the

number, the larger the standard errors of the parameters (and therefore, the odds ratio or

rnean estimates) becorne. This inevitably increases the chance of cornmitting a type II

erro r.

Because the purpose of this study was to investigate the association between

early childhood speechllanguage impairment and young adulthood antisocial behaviour,

this necessarily involved the estimation of the "best" rneasures of this association. The

"bestn measures are those estimates that balance precision with stability. Covariates

which proved to be risk factors, but did not prove to be confounding variables were not

included in the various final models as they had no effect on the odds ratio or mean

estimates of antisocial behaviour. If they had been included in the final models, the

estimates of antisocial behaviour would have been less stable and the chance of not

finding an effect of speechllanguage impairment when one really existed (Le. type II

error) would have increased substantially. Whether or not a particuiar covariate appears

in a final model is not an indication as to whether it has k e n controlled. Al1 the

82

Page 97: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

covariates in this study have been controlled. Only covariates that proved to confound

the speechhanguage-antisocial behaviour relationship appear in the various final models.

6.5.1 The Covariates as Confounding Variables

Every covariate in this study, except for intelligence and speechllanguage

treatrnent (see section 6.5.2 Ancillary Findings, below) proved to be a rïsk factor in at

least one of the four major analyses of antisocial behaviour. Only the covariates of

speechnanguage treatment, hyperactivity, SES, gender and single-parent rearing proved

to be confounding variables. That these covariates proved to be confounders is

important in that this reiterates the necessity for their control that similar studies may have

failed to perforrn. Despite the fact that these variables were controlled, significant

differences were found among the speechllanguage groups on later antisocial behaviour

thereby lending strength to this study's findings. For the control of hyperactivity this is

especially relevant. MoffÏtt's (1 993) hypothesis of life-course persistent antisocial

behaviour, suggests that syrnptoms of inattention and hyperactivity in childhood are

continuous with antisocial behaviour problems in adulthood. This study suggests that

eariy childhood language impairrnents, independently of hyperactivity problems, lead to

the subsequent emergence of antisocial behaviour many years later.

Among the dichotomous analyses of antisocial behaviour, gender also proved to

be a confounding variable as well as a significant predictor of antisocial behaviour. This

is consistent with the body of literature that shows males being at a much higher n'sk for

antisocial behaviour than are fernales. In the dimensional analyses of antisocial

behaviour, gender was neither a confounding variable nor a significant predictor of

antisocial behaviour. This was surprising, especially for the parent andfor teacher

ratings. Most antisocial behaviour in general occurs among males. Thus, it was

unexpected that gender was not used by these raters to distinguish levels of antisocial

behaviour. While gender was an important variable in the dichotomous analyses but not

in the dimensional analyses, being reared in a single-parent family was important in the

dimensional analyses but not the dichotornous analyses. This may be related to the

observation that single parents may have been biased in their perception of their child's

antisocial behaviour. This hypothesis seems to be supported by the results, as single-

parent rearing was a significant predictor of antisocial behaviour when parents andlor

teachers rated the antisocial behaviour of the subjects, but not when subjects rated their

own antisocial behaviour.

Variations in the precise methods in which some of the covariates were defined

did not affect the results. The Conners variables of Inattention, Hyperactivity and

Page 98: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Conduct were defined as continuous and ordinal and the variables used to represent

speechllanguage treatment were both parent- and subject-assessed. Although different

covanate definitions caused sorne of the analyses to yietd more than one final model,

these different final rnodels did not yield large differences in the odds ratios (in the

analysis of criminal behaviour) or means (in the analysis of non-subject measures). That

differences in the operationalization of these covariates did not affect the results

appears to add weight to this study's conclusions.

One caveat should be noted. The coverage of the risk factors and possible

confounders in this study is tilted towards the psychopathological approach. This should

be desired as this study was conceived for a psychiatric/epidemiologic audience and

intended to make use of clinical psychiatric constructs in present use. It should be noted,

however, that a criminologist's approach might not weigh as heavily the variables on

which this study has placed rnuch emphasis. For exarnple, Andrews and Bonta (1 994)

argue that, for the forensic psychologist, the clinical constructs of low self-esteern and

hyperactivity play a very minor rote in explaining risk for criminal conduct within the

general paradigm of personality and social psychology. Nevertheless, the criminologist's

approach is not as different from the present psychopathological approach as it rnay

seern. For example, the other major risk factors Andrews and Bonta (1 994) report on

were constructs that were captured in this study. These include among others: history

of antisocial behaviour evident from a young age, familylneighborhood adversity as well

low levels of verbal intelligence. That such constructs which have relevance across

disciplines which study antisocial behaviour and which were accounted for in the

present study strengthens the external validity of this study's findings.

6.5.2 Ancillary Findings

Although not part of this study's objective, a number of additional findings related

to the covariates and relevant to the speechllanguage and antisocial literature. have been

described below.

Performance lQ

In section 2.6.3, it was noted that a common neuropsychological correlate of

delinquency is a deficit in IQ and that this elationship is stronger for verbal tests of 1Q

than for performance or full-scale measures (Lynam et al., 1993). In this study,

performance rather than verbal IQ was controlled. The justification for this approach

was that verbal IQ would be highly collinear with the independent variable of primary

interest: time-? speechllanguage status. In none of this study's analyses did

84

Page 99: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

performance IQ prove to be either a confounding variable or a significant predictor of

antisocial behaviour. This finding is important for two reasons. First, it contrasts with

much research that shows that low IQ, as a broad index of neuropsychological

functioning, predicts delinquency. Second, if the speechllanguage status variable is twly

collinear with verbal IQ, it suggests that when verbai and non-verbal components are

separated, the non-verbal component fails to predict antisocial behaviour.

The mechanism by which low IQ leads to delinquency is still unclear. Some

investigators have suggested that the effect of Iow IQ on antisocial behaviour is mediated

indirectly by school failure (Hirschi, 1969; Hirschi & Hindelang, 1977). That is, children

who experience failure in school will be more likely to becorne delinquents than those

who do not. Thus, in this model, IQ acts as an index of ability to succeed acadernically. It

is possible that performance IQ does in fact lead to antisocial behaviour, but that the

subjects in this study have not experienced such failure. Hence, these subjects would

have avoided later exhibition of antisocial behaviour as a result of low performance IQ.

History of speechnanguage treatment

A second, ancillary finding worthy of mention was the overall lack of an effect of

the speechllanguage treatment variable on the study results. This observation should be

interpreted with caution. The inclusion of this variable was perforrned merely as a "poor

man's tooln to control for any possible effect on the speechllanguage-antisocial behaviour

relationship that speechnanguage treatment may have had. This variable changed the

odds ratios for the risk of an antisocial outcome only in the analyses of ASPD for both the

speech-only impaired and language impaired groups, respectively. The variable did not

change the significance of the odds ratios, but rather, increased their magnitude slightly.

It is possible that this was the result of the fact that those who rernained irnpaired tended

to be those who were most Iikely to receive treatment (Beitchrnan et al., 1994) and these

subjects are usually the most impaired. Although this implies that speechllanguage

treatment had the effect of attenuating the relationship between speechAanguage

impairment and antisocial behaviour, it must be remembered that such an interpretation

should not be made. The effect of such treatment on speechllanguage ability cannot be

tested as the subjects were not randomly assigned to treatrnent groups. Furthemiore, as

mentioned in section 2.6.3, the effectiveness of such treatment for speechnanguage

impainent remains to be established (Beitchman et al, 1994).

Page 100: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

6.6 Implications

What are the potential implications to be derived from this study? First, the study

results imply that language-based problerns predispose individuals for the development of

antisocial behaviour. By drawing this implication, this study would appear to support a

large body of research which proposes that neuropsychological deficits underlie this

vulnerability. Second, because conduct and antisocial problerns do not respond well to

treatrnent, this study irnplicates the importance of prevention and therapy for those

children most at risk. Each of these implications will be discussed in turn followed by an

assessment of the study's limitations and recommendations for future research.

6.6.1 Language-based Problems and Vulnerability for Antisocial Behaviour

As mentioned in section 2.5, there are many ways in which language-based

impairments might lead to psychiatric disorders in generaI and antisocial behaviour in particular. That this study found such a long-term association between antisocial

behaviour in young adulthood and language impaiments identified in children as young as

5 years of age suggests something very fundamental about language abilities and their

relationship to aggression and delinquency. Given the role early childhood language

abilities have on the development of thought, it is not suprising that they would be

associated with psychiatric disorders. This study, however, is not the first to suggest

the importance of the relationship between language and antisocial behaviour and

supports the studies which link neuropsychological deficits with externalizing problems.

Neuropsychological theories may be useful in explaining the association behveen

speechnanguage irnpairments and subsequent antisocial behaviour. The belief that

neuropsychological factors are among the causes of antisocial behaviour is long

established. Benjamin Rush (1 81 2, cited in Elliott, 1 W8), for example, referred to the

"total perversion of the moral facultiesn in people who displayed "innate pretematural

moral depravityn. He also proposed that "there is an original defective organization in

those parts of the body which are occupied by the moral faculties of the mindn. Indeed,

almost 150 years later, Luria (1961) outlined a comprehensive theory of the importance of

normal language for the acquisition of morals and for the self-control of behaviour (see

also Wilson 8 Henstein, 1985; Eysenck, 1977; and Savitsky & Czyzewski, 1978).

The most commonly found neuropsychological deficits associated with antisocial

behaviour involve language-based verbal skills and self-control or "executive functionsn

(Moff~tt & Lynam, 1994). Because children with speechnanguage impairments often

show or acquire symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity and such symptoms are often

associated with neuropsychological deficits, it is ternpting to suggest an association

86

Page 101: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

between speechnanguage impainents and neuropsychological impaiments.

Such an argument for the relationship between eariy childhood speechilanguage

impairments and young adulthood antisocial behaviour should must be made cautiously as

these associations are only partially adequate in explaining this study's results. Fint,

studies involving the relationship behnreen neuropsychological deficits and antisocial

behaviour usually involve individuals with a history of severe and stable antisocial

behaviour throughout their lives. The subjects in this study may or rnay not ffi that

description as the evidence of their antisocial behaviour is neither particularly severe nor

stable throughout their lives. For example, although many of the subjects who had been

Ianguage irnpaired were diagnosed with an ASPD at age 19, there was Iittle other

corroborating evidence from the other antisocial behaviour measures.

Second, these theories tend to provide only proximal explanations for syrnptoms

of antisocial behaviour. It has been said that children with verbal deficits will rely more

on physical modes of selfexpression: resorting to hitting rather than discussion, when

disagreements anse. These proximal descriptions do not explain why the subjects in the

OLS appear to have developed antisocial behaviour over time; they apply as well to a 19-

year old as they do to a 5-year old. To aid our understanding of how and why such

behaviour is acquired, such explanations must take a developmental perspective. If eariy

childhood language impaiments. such as those in this study, are markers for ianguage-

based verbal and executive function deficits, then it is possible that they wreak their

effects early in the life course. A more realistic description of how they would effect

later emergence of antisocial behaviour would involve their interaction with subsequent

exposures,

For example, Moffit and Lynam (1994) illustrate how the effects of early childhood

neuropsychological deficits might be amplified over time as children interact with their

environments, to culminate later in antisocial behaviour. They describe a preschooler

who may have a neuropsychological vulnerability related to language and therefore

resists his mother's efforts to read to hirn. This causes the child to have delays in school

readiness. Poor academic performance, therefore results, especially arnong schools in

low SES areas that do not have the resources for his special needs. Repeated years of

school failure may lead to peer rejection andfor exposure to remedial classes containing

other students who have behavioural problems and learning disabilîties. Familiarity with

delinquent behaviours inevitably follows as the child attempts to win peer acceptance. A

small neuropsychological deficit, therefore, might be multiplied insidiously over time

culminating in an antisocial personality style. This explanation embodies the notion that the

Page 102: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

long-terni ernergence of antisocial behaviour should by studied by inctuding the influence

of many contributing factors each of which continue to put the child at risk.

Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber (1 998) asserted that explanations of antisocial

behaviour that ernerge in adolescence are unlikely to be found only among the preschool

causes. These authon suggest that explanatory models have to deal with later emerging

risk factors that, together with earlier causes in a cumulative fashion, can help to explain

the later onset of aggression in adolescence. A good example of such a developmental

approach is Loeber and colleagues' (Loeber et al., 1993; Loeber, Keenan, & Zhang,

1997) triple-pathway model that integrates bath predelinquent behaviour nsk factors and

subsequent delinquent acts. The model is airned at descnbing which youth are at highest

nsk of becoming chronic offenders. The first pathway concems overt antisocial

behaviour which starts with minor aggression (e.g. bultying, annoying others), followed

by physical fighting which is followed by serious violence (e.g. rape, assault, strong-

aming). The second pathway concems covert antisocial behaviour which first consists

of minor covert behaviours (e.g. shoplifting, frequent lying), followed by property damage

(e-g. fire setting, vandalism) which is followed by moderate to serious forrns of

delinquency (e-g. fraud, burglary, serious theft). The third pathway concems individuals

whose antisocial behaviour is of a nature such that they exhibit conflict with authority.

This pathway consists of those who first exhibit stubbom, defiant or oppositional

behaviour, followed by more serious acts of disobedience which is followed by acts of

authority avoidance (e.g. truancy, running away, staying out late at night).

It would appear as though the antisocial behaviour exhibited by those with a

history of language impainent is more of a covert or authority defiant nature rather than

of a covert nature as it is unclear whether they are involved in violence and physical

aggression. They were not at significantly increased risk for exhibiting crÎminal

behaviour, but were ai risk for diagnoses of ASPD. These diagnoses incorporates many

behaviours which may not be violent or criminal in their manifestation, but are still

antisocial and were captured by the diagnosis. Within the context of Loeber et a l 3 triple-

pathway model, more detailed analyses of the exact nature of these subjects' antisocial

behaviour might prove useful in elucidating the mechanisms by which this behaviour

developed over tirne.

6.6.2 Implications for Prevention

The ability to make long-terni behavioural predictions based on early childhood

attributes bestows a powerful diagnostic and preventative capacity on the child's health

care provider. This study shows that young children with language impairments deserve

Page 103: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

special attention as they are at significant risk for antisocial behaviour during the young

adulthood years. There is IittIe evidence for the effectiveness of interventions for

conduct and antisocial problems in adolescence and especially in young adulthood

(ûfford & Bennett, 1994). A compelling argument in favour of an increased emphasis on

primary prevention thus evolves. It seems logical that children with language impairrnents

who are at risk for antisocial behaviour should undergo speechnanguage therapy. An

important caveat for the role of prevention relates to the underlying causes of the

speechnanguage impairments themselves. For example, Beitchrnan (1 985) has

suggested that the association between speech/language impairrnents and antisocial

behaviour may be due to an antecedent variable that leads to both. He suggested that a

gene or group of genes might influence or cantrol neurodevelopmental maturation and

that speechnanguage irnpairments rnight be one of the more obvious manifestations of

delayed neurodeveiopment. Psychiatnc problems also can then be anticipated. If the

language impairment observed in this study were the result of such neurodevelopmental

delays, then prevention of antisocial behaviour in the form of speechllanguage therapy

might not prove useful.

Another form in which prevention might take place is in the assessrnent of

individual differences and protective factors. The outcome among language impaired

children is not uniformly negative and many do not develop antisocial behaviour problems.

Wth respect to psychiatnc disorders in general, Rae-Grant, Thomas, Offord and Boyle

(1989) reported that the developrnent of non-school skills (e.g. sports and music)

reduces the risk of emotional disorder arnong children and adolescents. Whether these

variables woufd serve as protective factors in young adults who had been identified as

speechllanguage impaired in childhood is unknown. The search for other "non-clinicaln

modes of prevention andfor protective factors might prove to be useful in the prevention

of antisocial behaviour.

6.7 Study Limitations

6.7.1 Missing Data

One significant limitation to the implications to be drawn from this study is the

absence of time-3 antisocial outcome data. This was especially true among teacher

(36.3% missing) and parent (22.5% missing) assessed antisocial data The analysis of

subjects with and without each measure of outcome data showed that those without

data at tirne3 tended to be worse off at time-1 showing, among others, lower levels of

Page 104: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

IQ, a greater proportion of diagnoses of ASPD and occurrences of criminal behaviour, a

greater likelihood of coming from a single-parent family and having a lower level of family

incorne. Most of the missing information at time-3 occurred for the subjects who had

been language impaired at tirne-1. Missing data such as these would tend to bias the

results towards the nul1 hypothesis (Rothman & Greenland, 1998). Furthemore, that

such large proportions of data were missing for the parent and teacher dimensional

scores limits the generalizability of those findings. Undoubtedly, the reported results

would have been strengthened had there been a more complete set of data.

6.7.2 lntegrated Analysis of the Data for Both Males and Females

Compared to other longitudinal studies of speecManguage impainnent, the OLS

had a relatively small sample size. The largest regression model included 254 subjects

while the Dunedin Study involved almost 1,000 subjects. Because of sample size

limitations, separate analyses could not be performed for males and females. This was

unfortunate as research has suggested that the pathways to and adult consequences of

antisocial behaviour may be different for males and females (ûfford & Bennett, 1994).

lntegrated analysis of both genders may have resulted in the loss of useful information.

6.7.3 Merging of Parent and Teacher Measures

A potential weakness of the study is that parent and teacher data had to be

combined into a single scale because of a large amount of missing teacher data.

Although useful information was still acquired by comparing subject and non-subject

measures, this may have also resulted in the loss of useful information pertaining to the

specificity of the behaviour exhibited by the subjects in different contexts.

6.8 Recommendations

Recommendations can be made for future research projects. The relationship

between neuropsychological deficits such as those involving language-based mediation

and executive functions (MoffÏtt & Lynam, 1994) and specific language irnpainnents

should prove illustrative. More information concerning the effectiveness of

speechnanguage treatment and its effect on the later emergence of antisocial problems

might yield important preventative information. A replication of this study on each gender

individually would shed further Iight on the strength of the relationship between

speechnanguage irnpairments in eariy childhood and antisocial behaviour in young

adulthood.

90

Page 105: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

6.9 Summary

The purpose of this study was to detemine whether eariy childhood

speecManguage impaiments identified in children as young as 5 years of age were

associated with antisocial behaviour in young adulthood. This was confirmed for the

situation in which antisocial behaviour was measured in tems of a diagnosis of antisocial

personality disorder (ASPD) and when non-subject raters (parents andlor teachers)

measured dimensionally the level of antisocial behaviour in the subjects. When antisocial

behaviour was measured in terms of contact with the law and when subjects measured

dirnensionally their own antisocial behaviour the association was not observed. When

differences among the groups were found, those subjects who had been language

irnpaired at age 5 were rated more antisocial than those who had been controls, while

those who had been speech-only impaired were not different than controls. With

respect to the analyses which did not yield differences arnong the speechllanguage

groups at age 79, the measures of criminal behaviour may not have been sufFiciently

sensitive to detect the true rates of contact with the law that the subjects may have had.

Subsequent research needs to study further the issue as to whether the antisocial

behaviour exhibited by individuals with a history of language impaiments is of the nature

or severity such that they would have increased rates of criminal behaviour.

Furthermore, supplernentary analyses suggested that the self-reports from individuals

with a history of language impairnent may be less accurate than those from individuals

without such a history of impairment. This was suggested as a possible explanation as

to why differences among subject measures of antisocial behaviour were not found.

Subsequent research might usefully address the neuropsychological features of those

with speechAanguage impairments and how such features relate to the developmental

pathways these individuals travel as they mature.

Page 106: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

References Achenbach, T.M. (1 991 a). Manual for the child behaviour checklist and 1991 profile.

Buriington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T.M. (1 991 b). Manual for the teacher's report form and 1991 profile. Burlington VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T.M. (1 991 c). Manual for the youth self-report and the 1991 profile- Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T.M. (1 991 d). lntegrative guide for the 1991 CBLCl4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T.M., McConaughy, S. H., Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioural and emotionat problems: Implications of cross-informant correiations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, - 101 (Z) , 21 3-232.

American Psychiatric Association. (1 987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.. Rev.). Washinaton. DC: Author.

Andrews, K., & Bonta, J. (1994). A general personality and social psychology of criminal condud. in ~ona ld ~kckburn (Ed.), ~ h e psychology-of criminai conduct: Theory, research and pradice. Chichester; New York: Wiley.

Aram, D.M., Ekelrnan, B.L., & Nation, J.E. (1984). Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, - 27,232-244.

Baker, L., & Cantwell, D.P. (1982a). Developrnental, social and behavioural characteristics of speech and language disordered children. Child Psychiat~ and Human Development, 12, 1 13-1 26.

Baker, L., & Cantwell, D.P. (1 987). A prospective psychiatric follow-up of children with speech/language disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 546-553.

Beitchman, J.H. (1985). Speech and language impairment and psychiatric nsk: Toward a mode1 of neurodevelopmental irnmatuhty. Psychiatrie Clin& of North Arnerica, - 8, 721 -735.

Beitchman, J.H. (1 995). Transitions to adulthood: Developmental pathways from preschool to young adult linguistic, academic and psychosocial outcornes. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto at Toronto, Ontario.

Beitchman, J.H., Brownlie, E.B., lnglis, A., Wild, J., Mathews, R., Schachter, D., Kroll, R., Martin, S., Ferguson, B., & Lancee, W. (1 994). Seven-year follow-up of speechnanguage stability and outcorne. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, s, 1322-1 330.

Page 107: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Beitchman, J.H., Ferguson, B., Schachter, D-, Brownlie, E.B., Inglis, A., Wild, J., Mathews, R., Lancee, W., Kroll, R., Brunshaw, J., Walters, H., & Martin, S. (1993). A Seven Year Follow-Up Study of Speechltanguage Impaired and Control Children. Final Report, Grant No. 6606-381 242, Health and Welfare Canada, Ottawa, Canada.

Beitchman, J.H., Hood, J., & Inglis, A. ('l990). Psychiatrie risk in children with speech and language disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychoio~y I, 18, 283-296.

Beitchman, J.H., Hood, Je, Rochon, J., & Peterson, M. (1989a). Empirical classification of speech/language impairment in children II. Behavioural characteristics. Journal of the Amencan Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, - 28(1), 1 18-1 23.

Beitchman, J.H., Kmidenier, B., Clegg, J., Hood, J., & Corradini, A. (1989b). Diagnostic interviewing with children: The use and reliability of the diagnostic coding fom. Canadian Joumal of Psychiatry, 2, 283-290.

Beitchrnan, J.H., Nair,R., Clegg, M., Ferguson, B., & Patel, P.G. (1986b). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in children with speech and language disorders. Joumal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, BI 528-535.

Beitchman, J.H., Nair, R., Clegg, M., & Patel, P.G. (1986a). Prevalence of speech and language disorders in 5-year old kindergarten children in the Ottawa-Carlton region. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, - 51, 98-1 10.

Beitchrnan, J.H., Peterson, M., & Clegg, M. (1 988). Speech and language impairnent and psychiatnc disorder: The relevance of famil y demographic variables. Child Psychiatry and Human Developrnent, 18, 191-207.

Beitchrnan, .J.H., Wilson, B., Srownlie, E.B., Walters, H., & Lancee, W. (1 996). Long-terni consistency in speech/language profiles: 1. Developmental and academic outcomes. Joumal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 804-814. -

Benasich, A.A., Curtis, S., & Tallal, P. (1993). Language, leaming and behavioural disturbances in childhood: A longitudinal perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, - 32, 585-593.

Berry, W.D., & Feldman, S. (1985). Multiple regression in practice. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, series no. 07-050, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Bland, R.C., Newman, SC., & Om, H. (1988). Period prevalence of psychiatrie disorders in Edmonton. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavia, (Suppl. 338), 33-42.

Bland, R.C., Om, H., & Newman, S.C. (1988). LÏfetime prevalence of psychiatric disorders in Edmonton. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavia, (Suppl. 338), 24-31.

Bloom, B.S. (1 974). Preface. In K. Ma joribanks (Ed.). Environments for Learning. Windsor. NF1 Company.

Page 108: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Bondy, S.J. (1995). Patterns of drinking and the acute consequences of alcohol use: Ontario, 1994. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Canada.

Cantwell, D.P., & Baker, L. (1 980). Academic failures in children with communication disorders. Joumal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 3, 579-591.

Cohen, N.J., Devine, M., Horodezky, N., Lipsett, L., & lssacson, L. (1993). Unsuspected language impairment in psychiatricdly disturbed children: ~revalence and - language and behavioural characteristics. Joumal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, - 32, 595-603,

Cohen, P.M. (1 988). Locke Marital Adjustment Scale and the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. American Joumal of Family Therapy, 13, 66-71.

Cnttenden, P.M. (1 996). Language and psychopathology: An attachment perspective. In J.H. Beitchman, N. Cohen, M. Konstantareas & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, leaming, and behaviour disorders: Developmental, biolo~ical and clinical perspectives (pp. 59-75). Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

Denno, D.W. (1990). Biology and violence: From birth to adulthood. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Derogatis, L.R. (1975). The Brief Symptom Inventory. Baltimore, MD: Clinical Psychometrics.

Derogatis, L.R. (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory report. Psycholoqical Medicine, 12 (3), 595-605.

Egeland, B., Kalkoske, M., Gottesman, N., & Erikson, M.F. (1990). Preschool behaviour problems: Stability and factors accounting for change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 891 -909.

Elliot, F.A. (1978). Neurological aspect of antisocial behaviour. In W.H. Reid (Ed.), The psychopath (pp. 146-1 89). New York: Bniner/Mazel.

Eysenck, H.J. (1977). Crime and personality. London, England: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Famngton, D .P. (1 991). Longitudinal research strategies: Advantages, problems and prospects. Joumal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 3, 369-374.

Farington, D.F., & Hawkins, J.D. (1 991). Predicting participation, early onset and later persistence in officially recorded offending. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 1. 1-33.

Feldman, M.P. (1977). Criminal behaviour: A psychological analysis. New York: Wiley.

Fundudis, T., Kolvin, I., & Garside, R.F. (Eds.). (1979). Speech retarded and deaf children: Their psychological development. London: Academic Press.

Page 109: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Felsonfeld, S., Broen, P., & McGue, M. (1992). A 28-year follow-up of adults with a history of rnoderate phonological disorder. Linguistic and personality results. Joumal of Speech and Hearing Research, g, 11 14-1 125.

Felsonfeld, S., Broen, P., & McGue, M. (1994). A 28-year follow-up of adults with a history of moderate phonological disorder: Educational and occupational results. Joumal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37,1341-1 353.

Gittelrnan, R., Mannuna, S., Shenker, R., & Bonagura, N. (1985). Hyperactive boys almost grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 937-947.

Goyette, CH., Conners, C.K., & Ulrich, R.F. (1978). Normative data on revised Conners Parent and Teacher Rating Scales. Journal of Abnomal Psychology, 6, 221-236.

Greenfield, B., Hechtman, L., & Weiss, G. (1 988). Two subgroups of hyperactives as adults: Correlations of outcorne. Canadian Joumal of Psychiatry, 33, 505-508.

Greenland, S. (1985). Power, sample size, and smallest detectable effect determination for multivariate studies. Statistical Medicine, 4, 1 1 7-1 27.

Hall, R.C. (1 995). Global Assessrnent of Functioning: A modified scale. Psychosornatics, 36,267-275. -

Hall, P., & Tomblin, J.B. (1978). A follow-up study of children with articulation and language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing research, - 43, 227-241.

Hamrnill, D.D., Brown, V.L., Larsen, S C . , & Wiederholt, J.L. (1 994). Test of Adolescent Language-3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Hare, R.D. (1984). Performance of psychopaths on cognitive tests related to frontal lobe function. Journal of Abnomal Psychology, 93, 1 33-1 40.

Hechtman, L., & Weiss, G. (1 986). Controlled prospective fifteen-year fotlow-up of hyperactives as adults: Non medical drug and alcohol use and antisocial behaviour. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 31 (6), 557-567.

Henggeler, S.W. (1989). Delinquency in adolescence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Hinshaw, S. P. (1 992). Extemalizing behaviour problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 111 ( l) , 127-1 55.

Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Hirschi, T., & Hindeiang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist review. American Sociological Review, 42,- 571 -587.

Hosmer Jr., D.W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied Logistic Regression. New York: Wiley.

Page 110: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (1 987)- The consequences of language delay for other aspects of developrnent. ln W. Yule & M. Rutter (Eds.), Language development and disorders. London: MacKeith Press (pp. 271 -294).

Huesman, R.L-, Eron, L.D.. Lefkowitr. M.M., & Walder, L.O. (1984). Stability of aggression over time and generations. Developrnental Psychology, 6, 1 120-1 134.

Johnson, C. (1998). Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speecManguaqe impaiments: Speechllanguage stability and outcornes. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Kessler, R.C., McGonagle, A., Zhao, S., Nelson. C.B., Hughs, M., Esfileman, S., Wittchen, H-U., & Kendler K.S. (1 994). Lifetirne and 12-month prevalence of DSM-Ill-R psychiatnc disorders in the United States, Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8- - 19,

King, R., Jones, C., & Lasky, E. (1982). In retrospect: A fifteen-year follow-up report of . - speech-language disordered children. Language, speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 13, 24-32.

Kolvin, F.J., Miller, LW., & Fleeting, M. (1 988). Social and parenting factors affecting criminal-offense rates: Findings from the Newcastle thousand family study (1 947- 1980). British Joumal of Psychiatry, 152, 80-90.

Last, J.M. (Ed.). (1995). A dictionary of epidemiology (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Lawton, D. (1 968). Social class, language, and education. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Lewis-Beck, M.S. (1 990). Applied regression: An introduction. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, 07-022. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Lewis,

Locke,

B., & Freebairn, L. (1992). Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in grade school, adolescence, and adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35,819-831.

H.J., & Wallace, K.M. (1959). Short marital adjustrnent and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Mamage and Family Living, 21, 251 -255.

Loeber, R. (1 9%). Antisocial behaviour: More enduring than changeable? Joumal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30,393-397.

Loeber, R., Keenan, K., & Zhang, Q. (1997). Boys' experimentation and persistence in developmental pathways toward serious delinquency. Joumal of Child and Family Studies, 6, 321 -357.

Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinqüency. In M. Tonry & N. Moms (Eds.), Ctirne and justice: An annual review of research, (Vol. 7, pp. 29-149). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Page 111: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1998)- Devetopment of juvenile aggression and violence: Some cornmon misconceptions and controversies. ~ t d c a n Psychologist, 53 (2), 242-259.

Loeber, R., Wung, P., Keenan, K., Giroux, B., Stoutharner-Loeber, M., Van Karnmen, W.B.,

Loney,

& Maughan, B. (1993). Developmentai pathways in dismptive child behaviour. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 101 -1 32.

J., Kramer, J., & Milich, R. (1 981). The hyperkinetic child gmws up: Predictors of syrnptoms, delinquency, and achievement at follow-up. In K. Gadow & J. Loney (Eds.) , Psychosocial aspects of drug treatment for hyperactivity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Luria, A.R. (1 961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour. New York: Basic Books.

Lynam,D., Moffi, T-, & Southamer-Loeber, M. (1 993). Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 9 - 102 (2), 187-1 96.

MacKeith, R.C., & Rutter, M. (1972). A note on the prevalence of speech and language disorders. In M. Rutter & M.A. Martin (Eds.), The child with delayed speech: Clinics in developmental medicine No. 43. London: S.I.M.P. with Heinemann:

McCord, J. (1983). A longitudinal study of aggression and antisocial behaviour. In K. T. Van Duesen & S. A. Madnick (Eds.), Prospective studies of crime and delinquency (pp. 269-275). Boston: Kluwer-Nijhoff Publishing.

Maldonado, G., & Greenland, S. (1993). Simulation study of confounder-selection strategies. Amencan Journal of Epidemiology '- 138, 923-935.

Maughan, B., & McCarthy, G. (1 997). Childhood adversities and psychological disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 2 (l), 156-1 69.

Maughan, B., Pickles, A., Hagell, A., Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1 996). Reading problerns and antisocial behaviour: Developmental trends in comorbidity. Journal of Child PsychoIogy and Psychiatry, 37 (4), 405-41 8.

Menyuk, P., Chesnick, M., Liebergott, J.W., Komgold, B., DIAgnostino, R., & Belanger, A. (1991). Predicting reading problerns in at-risk children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, g, 893-903.

Miller, L.S., Koplewicz, H.S., Klein, R.G. (1 997). Teacher ratings of hyperactivw, inattention, and conduct problerns in preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25 (2), 1 A 3-1 19.

Moffitt, T.E. (1990). The neuropsychology of delinquency: A critical review of theory and research. In N. Moms & M. Tonry (Eds.), Cn'me and justice: An annual review of research, - 12, (pp. 99-169). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Page 112: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Moffitt, TE. (1 993). Adotescence-limited and life-course persistent anti-social behaviour. A developrnental taxonomy. Psychological Review, - 100, 674-701.

Moffrtt, T.E., Gabrielli, W.F., Mednick, S.A., & Schulsinger, F. (1 981). Socioeconornic status, IQ, and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90.152-1 56.

Moffitt, TE., & Lynman Jr., L.D. (1994). The neuropsychology of conduct disorder and delinquency: Implications for understanding antisocial behaviour. Progress in Experimental PersortaMy of Psychopathology Research, 233-262.

Moffitt, T.E. & Silva, P.A. (1988). IQ and delinquency: A direct test of differential detection hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 (3), 330-333.

Morley, E.M. (1 965)- The developrnent and disorders of speech in childhaod. Edinburgh: E. & S. Livingstone.

Murchison, C. (1 926). Criminal intelligence. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

Newwmer, P L , & Hamrnill, D.D. (1 977). The test of lanquage development (TOLD). Austin, TX: Empiric Press.

Nye, C., Foster, S.H., & Searnan, D. (1 987). Effectiveness of language intewention with the language/learning disabIed. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 348-357.

Offord, D.R., & Bennett, K.J. (1994). Condud disorder: Long-terni outcomes and intervention effectiveness. Journal of the Arnerican Acaderny of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33 (8), 1 069-1 078.

mord, D.R., Boyle, M.H., Campbell, D-, Goering, P., Lin, E., Wong, M., & Racine, Y.A. (1 996). One-year prevalence of psychiatric disorder in Ontarians 15 to 64 years of age- The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, - 41, 559-563.

Olweus, 0. (1983). Low school achievement and aggressive behaviour in adolescent boys. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), Human development: An interactional perspective (pp. 353-365). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Pakiz, B., Reinherz, H Z , & Giaconia, R.M. (1 997). Eariy risk factors for serious antisocial behaviour at age 21 : A longitudinal cornmunity study. Arnerican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 67 (l), 92-1 01.

Patterson, GR., DeBaryshe, B.D., & Rarnsey, E. (1989). A developrnental perspective on antisocial behaviour. Amencan Psychologist, - 2, 329-335.

Paul, P.V., & Jackson, D.W. (1993). Toward a psycnology of deafness: Theoretical and empirical perspectives, Needham Heights, Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon.

Pervin, L- A. (1 985). Personality: Current controversies, issues and directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 83-1 14.

Page 113: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (1991). Statistical and conceptual models of turning points in developmental processes. In D. Magusson, L. Bergman, G. Rudinger 8 B. Torestad (Eds.), Problerns and methods in longitudinal research: Stability and chanqe (pp. 133-1 65). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Piersma, H.L., & Boes, J.L. (? 997). The GAF and psyct~iatric outcorne: A descriptive report. Community Mental Heatth Journal, 33 (l), 3541.

Pinker, S. (1995). The language instinct. New York: HarperPerennial.

Prentice, N.M., 8 Kelly, F.J. (1963). Intelligence and deiinquency: A reconsideration. Journal of Social Psycholos y, 60, 327-337.

Quinton, D., Pickles, A,, & Maughan, B. (1993). Partnets, peers, and pathways: Assortative pairing and continuities in conduct disorder. Developrnental Psychopatholoqy, - 5, 763-783.

RadIoff, LS . (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.

Rae-Grant, N., Thomas, B., H., Offord, D., R., & Boyle, M., H. (1989). Risk, protective factors, and the prevalence of behavioural and emotional disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28 (Z ) , 262-268.

Reiss, A.J., & Rhodes, A.L. (1961). The distribution of juvenile delinquency in the social class structure. American Sociological Review, -. 26, 720-732.

Richrnan, N., Stevenson, J.E., & Graham. (1982). Preschool to School: A Behavioural Study. London: Academic Press.

Robins, L.N. (1979). Follow-up studies. In H.C. Quay & JS. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of childhood (2nd ed., pp. 483-51 3). New York: Wiley.

Robins, L.N. (1 986). The consequences of conduct disorders in girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behaviour: Research, theories and issues (pp. 385478). New York: Academic Press.

Robins, LN., & Regier, D.A. (Eds.). (1991). Psychiatrie disorders in America: The epidemiological catchment area study. New York: Free Press.

Rothrnan, K.J., & Greenland, S. (1998). Modem epidemiology (2nd ed.). New York: Lippincott.

Rutter, Sir M. (1996). Introduction: Concepts of antisocial behaviour, of cause and of genetic influences. ln GR. Bock, J.A. Goode (Eds.), Ciba Foundation Symposium: Genetics of Cnminal and Antisocial Behaviour, (pp. 1-20). Chichester, England: Wiley & Sons.

Page 114: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Rutter, M., & Giller, H. (1983). Juvenile delinquency: Trends and perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.

Rutter, M., & Mawhood, L. (1991). The long tem sequelae of specific development disorders of speech and language. In M- Rutter & P. Casaer (Eds.), Bioloqical risk factors in childhood and psych patholoay . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Samson, R., & Laub, J. (1993). Crime in the making: Pathways and tuming points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. -

Savitsky, J-C., & Czyzewski, D. (1 978). The reaction of adolescent offenders and nonoffenders to nonverbal ernotional displays. Journal of Abnomal Child Psychology, 6, 89-96.

Scarr, S. (1 992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Chiiâ Development, - 63, 1 -1 9.

Schachter, D. C. (9 996). Academic performance in children with speech and language impairment: A review of follow-up research. ln J.H. Beitchman, N. Cohen, M. Konstantareas & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, leaming. and behaviour disorders: Developmental, biological and clinical perspectives (pp. 51 5529). Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

Schonfeld, I.S., Shaffer, D., O'Connor, P., & Portnoy, S. (1988). Condud disorder and cognitive functioning: Testing three causal hypotheses. Child Development, 59, 993-1 007.

Shanok, S.S., & Lewis, D.O. (1981). Medical histones of female delinquents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 21 1-21 3.

Short, J.F., Jr., & Strodtbeck, F.L. (1965). Group process and gang delinquency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Silva, P.A. (1 980). The prevalence, stability and significance of developrnental language delays in preschool children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 2, 768-777.

Silva, P.A. (1 987). Epidemiology, longitudinal course and some associated factors: An update. In W. Yuie & M. Rutter (Eds.), Language development and disorders. London: MacKeith Press (pp. 1-1 5).

Silva, P.A., Bradshaw, J., & Spears, G.F. (1978). A study of the concurrent and predictive validity of the Reynell developrnental lanquage scales: Report from the Dunedin rnultidisciplinary child development study. Windsor: N.F.E.R.

Silva, P.A., McGee, R.O., Thomas, J., & Williams, S.M. (1 982b). A descriptive study of socio-economic status and child development in Dunedin five year olds: A report from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development Study. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 17, 21 -32.

Page 115: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Silva, P.A., McGee, R.O., & Williams, S.M. (1982a). The predictive significance of slow walking and talking. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 17, 133-139.

Silva, P.A., McGee, R.O., & Williams, S.M. (1983). Developmental language delay from three to seven years and its significance for low intelligence and reading difficulties at age seven. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 783- 793.

Silva, P A , Justin, C., McGee, R., Williams, S.M. (1984). Some developmentaf and behavioural characteristics of seven year old children with delayed speech development, British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 17, 133-1 39.

Silva, PA., Williams, S.M., & McGee, R.O. (1987). A longitudinal study of children with developmental language delay at age three: Later intelligence, reading and behaviour problems, Development and Medicine in Child Neurology, 29, 630-640.

Silverstein, AB. (1985). Two- and four- subtest short forms of the WAIS-R: A closer look at validity and reliabiiity. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 408-409.

Stark, R. (1975). Social problems. New York: Random House.

Stevenson, J., & Richman, N. (1 976). The prevalence of language delay in a population of three year old children and its association with general retardation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology '- 18, 431 -441.

Stevenson, J., Richman, N., & Graham, P. (1985). Behaviour problems and language abilities at three years and behavioural deviance at eight years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,z, 21 5-230.

Sutherland, E.H. (1931). Mental deficiency and crime. In K. Young (Ed.), Social attitudes (pp. 357-375). Philadelphia: Winston.

Tamopol, L. (1970. Delinquency and minimal brain dysfunction. Journal of Leaming Disabilities, 3, 200-207.

Tayior, E. (1994). Syndromes of attention deficit and overactivity. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modem approaches (3rd ed., pp. 285-307). Oxford: Biackwell Scientific Publications.

Toby, J., & Toby, M.L. (1961). Low school status as a predisposing factor in subcultural delinquency. New Brunswick, N J: Rutgers University Mimeo.

Tomblin, J.B. (1996). The big picture of SLI: Epidemiology of SU. Paper presented at the symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, W.

Tomblin, J.B., Freese, P., 8 Records, N. (1992). Diagnosing specific language impairment in adults for the purpose of pedigree analysis. Journal of Speech and Heanng Research, 35, 832-843.

Tuomi, S., & Ivanoff, P. (1 977). Incidence of speech and hearing disorders among kindergarten and grade one children. Special Education in Canada, - 51, 5-8.

Page 116: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Vaux, A. (1986). The Social Support Appraisals (SS-A) Scale: Studies of reliability and validity. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14, (2), 195-219.

Huesman, R.L., Eron L.D., Lefkowitz, M.M., & Walder, 1.0. (1 984). Stability of aggression over time and generations. Developmental Psychology,s, 1 1 20-1 134.

WechsIer, D. (1 974). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children - revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (1981). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children - revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Weiss, G., & Hechtman, 1- (1986). Hyperactive Children Grown Up. New York: The Guilford Press.

West, D. J., 8 Famngton, D. P. (1 973). Who becomes deiinquent? London: Heinemann.

Wilson, J.Q., & Herrnstein, R.J- (1985). Crime and human nature. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Wolfgang, M., Figlio, R.M., Tracey, P.E., & Singer, F.I. (1 985). The national survey of crime severity. Washington, DC: US. Govemment Printing Office.

Wodd Health Organization. (1 990). Composite international Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). Version 1 .O. Geneva, Switzerland: Worid Health Organization.

Worid HeaIth Organization. (1 991 a). ICD-1 O, Chapter VI Mental and Behaviour Disorders (including disorders of psychological deveiopment). Diagnostic Criteria for Research, Draft for Field Trials. Geneva: Worid Health Organization.

World Health Organization. (1991 b). Tenth Revision of the International Classifications of Diseases Chapter V (F): Mental and Behaviour Disorders (including disorders of psychological development). Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: Worid Health Organization.

Page 117: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

APPENDlX A Missing Data.

Tables A l and A2.

Page 118: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Missing Observations

As was mentioned in sedion 3.6.2, most of the missing dependent variable data

occurred among the subjects who were categorized as having a cornbined speech and

language impairment. This can be seen in Table A l , below, which shows the proportion

of missing dependent variable data for each speechllanguage group and for each

dependent variable used.

Table A l : Proportion of missing values for each speecManguage group on each

When the combined speech and language impaired group was merged with the

language-only impaired group, the severity of the missing data was slightly diminished,

although there were still significant differences in the proportions of missing values

among the newly-defined speechllanguage groups. This can be seen in Table 3.29,

below.

Outcome Variable

DX ASPD

Criminal Behaviour

Non-subject measures

Subject rneasures

Table A 2 Proportion of missing values for the newly speechllanguage groups on each dependent variable.

I Outcorne Variable Controls I Group Differences I

Controls

9.1 5

7-75

10.25

5.63

Subjects with and without missing dependent variable data were aiso compared on al1

the other variables in the study. The results of these analyses appear in Tables B I to B5.

As a surnrnary description for al1 of these tables, the rnost conspicuous observation is

that subjects with missing dependent variable data tend to be worse off than those

without rnissing data. For example, Table B I shows that subjects with missing parent-

measured antisocial behaviour data have significantly higher (p=0.045) teacher rneasured

Speech-onIy

2.56

5.13

7.69

2.55

C Dx ASPD

Criminal 8ehaviour

Non-subject measures

Subject measures

Language-only

19.35

14.52

29.03

16.1 3

x2(3df)=1 7.727, p=0.001

x2(3df)=9.773 p=0.008

x2(3df)=1 5.946, p=0.001

x2(3df)=1 6.81 8, p=0.001

9.1 5

7.75

10.25

5.63

Speech&Language

34.1 5

26.83

26.83

26.83

Group Diff erences

x2(3df)=22.1 89, p=0.001

z2(3df)=1 3.41 7 p=0.004

x2(3df)=1 6.032. p=O.OOl

x2(3df)=1 9.809. p=0.001

2.56

5.1 3

7.69

2.56

25.24

19.42

28.1 6

20.39

Page 119: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

antisocial behaviour ratings. significantly lower (p=0.0036) performance IQ scores,

significantly higher rates of hyperactivity and conduct problems (p=0.0095 and p=0.0406,

respectively), have a significantly larger proportion of subject eliciting criminal behaviour

(p=0.047), have a significantly larger @=O-001) proportion of subjects who corne from single-parent families and have a significantly lower family income (p=0.009). A similar

pattern is repeated for each of the dependent variables used in this study.

Page 120: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Cornpansons of subjects with and without missing tirne-3 antisocial outcome data. Tables B I -B5.

Page 121: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Variable Teacher-measured dimensional ASB

Subject-measured dimensional ASB

Performance IQ

Childhood Adversity

Inattention

Hy peractivi ty

Early conduct problems

Table B 1: Cornparisons of subjects with and without rnissing data on parent-rated antisocial behaviour for dl variables. Parent-rated data is rnissing for 64 subjects (22.5%).

I I 7

I

-

- - - "

- I

- I

-

1

Single parent status a

n= Mean/Proportion Test p-value present=I 64 54.1 3 t( 179df)=-2.0285 0.045

missing4 1 31.15%

present=207 P U P 9 ~ 2 ( I2df)=26.538 0.009

missing=53 p u p s 2 & 5

presen t=220 65.00% x2(ldf)=.135 0.7 13 missing=64 62.50%

b % missing in combined speech & language impaired group, language-only impaired

group, speech-only im aired group and controls, respectively. '9=4045K$, 2=5- I OK$, 5=28-2X$. d% male.

Page 122: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table B2: Cornparison of subjects with and without missing data on teacher-rated antisocial behaviour for al1 variables. Teacher-rated data is missing for 103 subjects

le of interest.

Variable 'arent-measured Iimensional ASB

iubject-measured

limensional ASB

'erformance 1Q

:hildhood Adversity

nattention

i yperactivity

M y conduct problems

)X of ASPD'

lriminal I3ehavioura

peecManguage rtaturb

Iingie parent status "

b% missing in combined speech & language impaired group, language-only impaired

n= MeanIProportion Test p-value present=164 54.17 t(74.5df)=- 1.1589 0.25û2 missing=56 55.58

present=l80 55.2 t(252df)=-1.3387 0.1819

missing=74 56.23

present= 1 79 11251 t(277df)=2.2 186 0.0273

missing= 100 1085

present=l7 1 50.56 t(260df)=0.064 1 0.9489

missing=9 1 50.47

present=18 1 1?.1 t(282df)=- 1.5402 O. 1246

rnissing= 1 03 20.95

present=l8 1 23.17 t(282df)=- 1.2039 0.2296 missing=103 26.53

present=18 1 12.32 t(182.8df+0.7638 0.446

niissing==l03 14.14

present=I 76 9.66% x2( 1 df) =4.069 0.044

missing=68 19.12%

present=l77 18.08% ~ 2 ( ldf)=4.195 O.# 1 rnissing=74 29.73%

pesent=' 81 41.46%. 45.16% ~2(3df)=5.727 0.126 missing=l03 23.08%, 34.5 1 %

present=i 79 1329% x2( ldf')=5.625 0.0 18

speech-only im aired group and controls, respectively. '9=40-4K$, 2=5- 10K$, 8=3!-40~$. d 5% male.

ncomer

;enderd

missing=99 23.238

present= 170 P U P 9 ~ 2 ( 12df)=28.13 1 0.005 rnissing=90 groups 2 & 8

present= 1 8 1 6 1.88% ~ 2 ( 1 df)=I .425 0.233 missing= IO3 68.93%

Page 123: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Variable 'arent-measured limensional ASB

éacher-measured limensional ASB

'erformance 1Q

Ihildhood Adversity

nattention

Iyperactivity

larly conduct probIerns

b peecManguage status

ingle parent status"

n= MeanIProportion Test p-value present=2 1 8 54.57 t(217df)=10.1331 0.000 1

missing=2 50

present=180 54.4 I t( 179df)=.3358 0.7374

missing= 1 52-5

present=250 11 1.97 t(277df)=3.06 14 0.0024 missing=29 1 03 34

present=239 50.42 t(260df)=--50 14 0.6 165

rnissing=Z 51.6

present=254 17.63 t(282df)=-2.10 15 0.0365

missing=30 25.83

present=254 23 -43 t(282df)=-2.0827 0.0332 missing=30 32.48

presen t=254 12-33 t(33.3dî+1.4320 0.1615 missing-30 18-46

present=244 12.30% x2=da n/a

missin@ nia present=249 2 1.69% x2( 1 df)=.553 0.457

missing=2 0.00%

present=254 26.83% 16.13% ~2(3df)= 19.809 0.00 1

missing=30 2.56%. 5.63%

present=25 1 13.55% ~ 2 ( ldf)=13.289 0.00 1

rnissing=27 40.74%

present=238 groups 7.8 & 9 ~ 2 ( 1 2df)=36.263 0.0 1

rnissing~22 P U P 2 present=254 63.39% ~2(ldf)=1.159 0.282

rnissing=30 73.33%

b% missing in combined speech & languitge impaired group, language-only impaired group, speech-oniy impaired group and controls, respectiveIy.

'7~30-35K$, 8=35-40K$, 9~40-45K$. d % male.

Page 124: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table B4: Compansons of subjects with and without missing data on diagnoses of ASPD for al1 variables.

8

Continuous Variables

L

Categorical VariabIes

peecManguage s t n d

ingle parent status"

ncomer

1

iubject-rneasured ) present=244 55.6 t(252df)= 1.3662 0.1731

Diagnoses of ASPD data is missing for 40 subjects (14.1 %).

F C m

d - C

d - F

L

- 1

- k

- E

C

- S

- s -

Variable n= Mean/Proportion Test p-val u c

rnissing=ll 9.09%

present=254 34.15%. 19.35% ~2(3df)=22.189 0.00 1

missing=30 2.56R.9.158

present=24 1 13.288 ~ 2 ( 1 d e 1 1.295 0.00 1

rnissing=37 35.149

present=228 F O U P 9 x2( 1 2df)=27.122 0.007

l - l "'33 with outcome of interest-

b 55 missing in combined speech & language impaired group, language-only impaired

group, speech-only impaired group and controis, respectively. '940-45K$, S=5- 1 0K$. d% male.

'arent-measured

limerisional ASB

reacher-rneasured

Iirnensional ASB

present=212 54.37 t(2 18df)=,4464 0.6557

missing=8 565

present=176 54.34 t(179df)=-3757 03824

missi ng=5 56.6

limensional ASB

'erformance IQ

:hildhood Adversity

nattention

missing= 1 0 53.15

present=242 1 12.26 t(47.9df)=3.563 0.0005

missing=37 10332

present-229 50.48 t(260df)=.2065 0.8366 missing=33 50.89

present=24 1736 t(282df)=-2.3395 0.02 missing== 25.42

brly conduct problems present=2# 12.09 t(282df)=-2.0264 0.0437 missing4.4 18.4

Page 125: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table B5: Cornparisons of subjects with and without missing data on criminal behaviour ES. Criminal behaviour data is missing for 33 sibjects (1 1.6%).

Variable IF MeadProportion Test p-value

interest.

'arerit-measured

Iimensionai ASB

reacher-measured

Limensional ASB

iubject-rneasured

lirnensiond AS B

'erformance 1Q

Zhildhood Adversity

nattention

i yperactivi ty

3rly conduct problems

l x of ASPD a

:peecManguage statusb

ingle parent status"

ncomee

ienderd .

b 5% missing in combined speech & language irnpaired group, language-only impaired

group, speech-only im aired group and controls. respectively. '8=35-40K%, 9=45-50K$, 2=!- 1 OK$. d% male.

present=2 16 54.62 t(218df)=1.3788 0.1694

rnissing--4 50

presenr= 177 54.4 t( 179df)--.O778 0.938 1

missing4 54.63

present=249 55.54 t(252df)=.7307 0.4656 missing=5 53.7

present=247 11 1.85 t(277df)=2.5 1 57 0.01 24

rnissing=32 1 05 .O3

present=236 50.6 1 t(260df)=.3796 0.7045 rnissing=26 49.77

present=23 1 17-87 t(282df)z- 1.4252 0.1552 missing=33 23 -23

present=25 1 23.7 t(282df)z- 1.422 1 0.156 1

missing=33 29.65

present=25 1 12.63 t(282df)=-0.9039 0.3668 missing=3 1 15.69

present=240 1 2.08% ~ 2 ( 1 df)=0.609 0.43 5

missin@ 25.W'

26.83%. 14.52% x2(3df)= 13.4 17 0.004

missing=30 5.13%,7*75%

present=248 13.31% ~ 2 ( 1 df)= 14.056 0.00 1

missing=30 40.00%

presen t=234 groups 8 & 9 ~ 2 ( 12df)=25.638 0.012

missing=26 group 2

present=25 1 63.759 ~ 2 ( 1 df)=.35 1 0.502

missing=33 69.70%

Page 126: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

APPENDIX C Multicollinearity and Diagnostics.

Tables Cl-C6.

Page 127: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Multicollinearity and Diagnostics

The purpose of this section is to address two important statisticâl issues present

in this investigation: multicollinearity and linear regreçsion diagnostics.

Because this study involved the construction of various models that contained a

number of independent variables, the degree of rnulticotlinearity among these variables

needed to be addressed. Multicollinearity in regression analysis exists when the

independent variables are highly correlated with one another, or more fomally, when one

independent variable is regressed on another and the R' approaches 1 .O (Berry &

Feldman, 1985; Lewis-Beck, 1990). There is always sorne degree of multicollinearity, but

high multicollinearity creates a problem with estimation. In general, as the degree of

correlation among the independent variables increases, the standard errors of the siope

coefficients increase thus decreasing the chance of rejecting the nuIl hypothesis that the

dope coefficients are zero. A number of indicators may be used to detect high

rnulticollinearity. Observed pairwise correlations between independent variables larger

than 0.8 rnay be one sign, but multicollinearity rnay still be a problern when bivariate

correlations are lower or not a problem when they are higher (Bondy, 1995). A

somewhat better test is to examine the joint influence of the independent variables on

each other by examining the multiple R~ values obtained. However, there is no cntical

value of R' which signifies the presence of absence of multicollinearity.

Tables Cl through C5 show the pairwise correlations and multiple R~ for Models

14, respectively. Generally, these results did not identify any multicollinearity of

concem, however, some important points will be made. For example, the highest

pairwise correlation found was -0.3405 (in Table Cl) which represented the association

between having received sorne form of treatment and having been categorized as either

Speech-only impaired or Language impaired at time-7. Although this pairwise correlation

is quite high it should be noted that it does not alter the conclusions that would have been

made from the model that contained this variable. This is explained by two related facts.

First, the standard errors of the speech-only and language impaired groups before and

after the treatment variable was included differed very little. Specifically, the standard

error of speech-only variable increased by only 3.7% and the language irnpaired group

increased by only 1.8%; and second, the ,significance of the odds ratios associated with

each of these speechtlanguage variables did not change when the treatment variable

was added. (It should be noted that the sarne was tme with the model (not shown) that

included the parent-reported treatrnent variable).

Page 128: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Another large paiwise correlation coefficient was 0.2781 (in Table C2) which

represented the association between Hyperactivity (continuous measure) and

membership in the Language impaired group and appeared in Model2a. This high level of

multicollinearity would also not have altered the conclusions drawn from that model. The

standard errors of the Speech-only and Language impaired variables increased by only

2.1 % and 1.6%. respectively. It should also be noted that the largest multiple R* found

was 0.1779 which existed for the Hyperactivity (ordinal) variable when it was regressed

onto speechllanguage status, Gender and SES. Thus, while some multicollinearity was

found, it would not be considered so high as to preclude the inclusion of the independent

variables in their respective models in which the multicollinearity occurred.

Linear regression diagnostics were also performed to assess the assumptions

involved in the linear regression analyses. Four types of graphs were plotted for each

multiple linear regression rnodel (Models 3a & b and 4): i) a plot of the distribution of the

studentized residuals; ii) a plot of the studentized residuals versus each of the

independent variables that were included in the respective final modeis; iii) a plot of the

studentized residuals versus the predicted antisocial behaviour T score values; and iv) a

plot the extemally studentized residuals versus the predicted antisocial behaviour T

score values. The purpose of the latter most graph type was to highlight outlying values

by determining how they affected the estimates of the regression parameters. Ideally,

the studentized and externally studentized plots (types iii and iv) should be sirnilar.

For each model. the distribution plots of the residuals were roughly normal. As

well, the plots of the studentized residuals versus each independent variables and the

plots of the studentized residuals versus the predicted values were very scattered, each

with no apparent pattern. Finally, the plots of the externally studentized residuals versus

the predicted values were almost identical to the plots of the studentized residuals

versus the predicted values indicating that in none of the Models were there any outlying

values the largely effected the respective parameter estimates.

In summary, based on the results from al1 of the above mentioned plots, none of

the assurnptions involved in multiple linear regression appeared to be violated.

Page 129: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

TableCl:Pairwise--andmultipk~%f~~Modd 1. -

1 -cInebtiots I I Gender 1 (~ribject) ( M- R~

Gender ID I I ml31 Treatment (Subject) 0-0296ûQ~4I~64ûû) 1 If] [ 0.1607

ControI -0.27 16 (p=O.ûûO 1 ) Speec WLanguage Speech-onty 0.006 1 (pû.9 1 87) 11) OB86 1

Status Lariguage 0.2787 (p-d.0001)

TabieC3: PaiFwiçe amdations and mulople R ~ S for Model2.b.

I

Gender ID 1

SES 0-1 805 (p=û-0023) i

t Statu5 . . I R2 Hyperactmty ID 1 0.1338 Single-Parent 0251 2(p=û~ûûûl) 11) 0.0695

Contrd -0.27 1 6 (p=û.Oûûl) -0.08ô7 (pû- 1 446) S~eech/Language ' Speechnfy 0.006Tcp.O.9187) -0.067 7 (p0.3052) 1.0 0.0887

S t a h b L a m * 0-2781 (p=û.ûûû1) 0-1 340 (p=O.0240) }

I 1 P a i i C~r~etatiors I

Status [ , 0-1 243

t ?Co 1 I t O-? t O7

m 1 1 I I

p e m z N i (wt%~tl) 1 0 . 2 5 7 3 ( ~ . ~ l ) ' 4-1 597 1 1 .O I 1 0,1779

Gender SES i

Speech/ bwuage

Status

I HY- - - speaW (ordinal) ~anguage

~1452CpS).U.C2t4î) : bf 1837 (m0463)

C m d 1 -CLûû74(@.9ols)

, Mdt.i~AeR~

412719{p--Op--00001) : iaO?3C)(W-827 3)

1

, 113 , only

Language -02360 (p=û.ûûûl)

l

0-1 1 42 . 0-0827(@-1646)

-0,051 55 @=O3867) 02734(p=0-0001) , rn

Page 130: corner - University of Toronto T-Space · his advice and explanations on statistical matters. I also acknowledge my indebtedness ... Tables A1 and A2 ..... 103 APPENDW B Comparisons

Table C5: Pairwise correlations and multiple R ~ S for Model3b. 1

1 1 Hyperactivity

Hyperactivity

- Pairwise Correlations

Single-Parent 1 SpeechlLanguage Multiple (ordinal)

1 .O I

- -

Status 1 FP

0.2279 (p=O.OOOl) -0.271 9 (p=O.O.OOOl)

0.01 30 (pd.8273) 0.2734 (p=O.OOOl)

Single-Parent

Table C6: Pairwise correlations and multiple R2s for Model4. 1 Pairwîse Conelations

SpeechlLanguage Status

1 ( Single-Parent 1 Speech/Language

Control Speech-only

Language

Multipfe Fe

0.01 84 Sinqle-Parent 1 .O

-0.0868 (p=0.1446) -0.061 1 (p=0.3052) 0.1 340 (p=0.0240)

SpeechILanguage Status

1 .O Control

Speech-only Language