culture - encyclopedia on early childhood development

46
Culture Last update: Octobre 2012 ©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 1

Upload: others

Post on 14-Nov-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

CultureLast update: Octobre 2012

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 1

Page 2: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Table of contentSynthesis   4

     

Culture and Early Childhood Learning   81MICHAEL COLE, PHD, 2PENTTI HAKKARAINEN, PHD, 2MILDA BREDIKYTE, PHD, FÉVRIER 2010

     

Culture and Early Socio-Emotional Development   14XINYIN CHEN, PHD, JUIN 2009

     

Culture and Social Development   20KENNETH H. RUBIN, PHD, MELISSA MENZER, BA, JANVIER 2010

     

Culture and Early Childhood Education   29JESSICA BALL, PHD, SEPTEMBRE 2010

     

Bridging Cultures in Early Childhood Education   361PATRICIA M. GREENFIELD, PHD, 2CARRIE ROTHSTEIN-FISCH, PHD, 3ELISE TRUMBULL, ED.D., 4BLANCA QUIROZ, PHD, OCTOBRE 2012

     

Culture and Policy in Early Childhood Development   41SARA HARKNESS, PHD, CHARLES M. SUPER, PHD, JUILLET 2010

     

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 2

Page 3: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Synthesis

How Important Is It?

With the emergence of globalization and the growth in multicultural nations, it has becomeimperative to study the link between child development and culture. Culture broadly refers to agroup’s shared attitudes, traditions, beliefs and practices that are transmitted across generations.Cultures shape children’s experiences, and cross-cultural work attempts to study the processesby which these influences occur.

Two distinct frameworks have been used to explore the connection between culture and childdevelopment. The most commonly used is the etic framework, where a method used to measurea concept, such as social competence, is assumed to be pertinent to all cultures. One potentialrisk associated with this framework is a failure to explore other more culturally-relevantdefinitions of a concept. For instance, ren or patient endurance is used to define socialcompetence in China, and is observed when children detach themselves from confrontation toencourage the opponent to show self-control. Although this construct is different thanconceptualizations of social competence in western countries, the distinction may not beacknowledged through an etic framework. This framework can also be disruptive for thedissemination of local knowledge, practice and traditions that are used to educate children. Incontrast, the emic framework considers the meaning of a given concept, practice and principle formembers of a cultural group, and is a more unbiased approach to understand how cultureinfluences child development.

Broad cultural trends have been categorized through the dimensions of individualism andcollectivism. Individualism is related to Western ideologies about independence andcompetitiveness, whereas collectivism refers to Southern and Eastern dogma aboutinterdependence and group harmony. Although these dimensions coexist within nations, it isassumed that some cultures are more individualistic, or more collectivistic, than others. Thisallows researchers to understand why the same behaviour or practice carries different meaningsacross cultures. 

What do we know?

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 3

Page 4: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture influences several spheres of child development. A child’s learning experiences in aculture without an official education system are shaped through their participation with orobservation of adults engaging in culturally relevant activities (e.g., girls learning how to weavefrom their mothers in the traditional Mayan peasant culture). Culture also plays a role in socio-emotional development by either promoting or discouraging particular behaviours. Although mostof the existing knowledge about socio-emotional development comes from studies with North-American children, there is evidence for cultural variability. For instance, pretend play is far lesscommon in children from Eastern countries, such as Korea, than in Western children. When thisform of play does occur in Eastern cultures, children often impersonate a family member butrarely pretend to be a fairy tale character.

Differences in temperament also exist between cultures. Preschool children from Korea and Chinatend be more anxious, inhibited and withdrawn, and less sociable than their Western-Europeancounterparts. Whereas a child’s inhibited conduct is associated with a risk of troubled peerrelationships and internalizing problems (e.g., loneliness and depression) in Western cultures,these difficulties are much less common in inhibited children from Eastern cultures, though newresearch shows that children from India and urban China are equally at risk. These differencescan be explained through the meaning assigned to these behaviours. In East-Asian cultureswhere group harmony is valued, an inhibited child is viewed as socially-competent, obedient andpolite. In contrast, an inhibited child in Western cultures is perceived as apprehensive and lackingin social skills.

Children from cultures emphasizing interdependence tend to act less aggressively and moreprosocially than children from nations where independence and competitiveness is valued.Compared to European-American mothers, more Chinese mothers believe that their child shouldact prosocially to conform with group norms (e.g., fitting in) and emphasize self-control as achildrearing practice. Regardless of the culture, one universal trend is that children who areprosocial and nonaggressive are liked by other children.

When families immigrate from a culture emphasizing interdependence into a culture emphasizingindependence, children can receive conflicting socialization messages at home and at school. TheBridging Cultures Project was designed to alleviate this cross-cultural value conflict by trainingteachers to understand both cultural orientations and make a bridge between them in theclassroom.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 4

Page 5: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

The meaning of friendship also differs across cultures. In nations such as Cuba and Korea wherefriendships are an index of success, school-age children report closer relationships with peerscompared to North-American children. Children in Eastern cultures also use detachment toresolve conflicts with friends while Western children prefer to negotiate with their peers.

Every nation shares a desire to promote child development, but cultures differ in their beliefsabout which child outcomes should be prioritized. An illustration of this is seen in the differentnational versions of Sesame Street. While the original American television program wasdeveloped to promote learning of reading and counting, an adapted version in Northern Irelanddedicates equal portions of the program to the learning of prosocial actions, and in the IsraeliRechov Sumsum, the content of the program emphasizes mutual respect and understanding.

What can be done?

The importance of culture requires practitioners and policy makers to be knowledgeable aboutculture and child development. This is a pressing issue in host nations, where addressing theneeds of a diverse population of immigrant children who differ in acculturation (changes resultingfrom the meeting of cultures) extends beyond a linguistic issue. Immigrant families should also beinformed about how different ideologies can contribute to their child’s difficulty with peers in thehost country. One way to achieve optimal adaptation for these children is to form positive andproductive alliances with families and communities. Identifying what works best in one particularenvironment should be carefully validated, and must reflect a community’s needs.

Training field workers to be culturally sensitive is essential to understand the meaning of a child’sconduct and allow them to identify it as normal or problematic. The Bridging Cultures Project wasdesigned to train teachers at the preschool and elementary level to become aware of thecollectivistic or familistic cultural orientation that children from Latino immigrant families withroots in Mexico or Central America bring with them from home into their school environments.Bridging Cultures professional development also educates teachers to become aware of theindividualistic value orientation engrained in the school environment and of the value conflictsbetween these two cultural orientations. Teachers trained in the Bridging Cultures paradigm havedeveloped many techniques that make a bridge between the familistic collectivism of a Latinoimmigrant home and the individualism of a U.S. school.

In some cases, professionals require a completely unique approach that includes goals and

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 5

Page 6: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

conditions that are tailored to local beliefs and traditions. These changes are also likely toencourage the involvement of cultural community members in the education of their youngchildren. For example, Aboriginal communities in Canada advocate for a formal educationcurriculum that teaches children about their history, descendants and cultural roles. They alsoargue that children’s learning can be enhanced through a self-esteem boost, emphasizingstrengths rather than deficits.

Just like mental and physical health can enhance learning and development in young children, socan cultural fortification. Thus, intervention programs in early childhood should always allowflexibility for cultural adjustments, and enable families to incorporate services smoothly into theirlife.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 6

Page 7: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture and Early Childhood Learning1Michael Cole, PhD, 2Pentti Hakkarainen, PhD, 2Milda Bredikyte, PhD1University of California, USA, 2University of Oulu, FINLANDFévrier 2010

Introduction

During the period from birth to 5 years of age, children undergo massive transformations in size,biological organization, behavioral capacities, and the social organization of experience thatgreatly complicate our understanding of the relation between culture and their learningprocesses. 

Examination of this complex topic requires provisional definitions of our basic terms. We adopt the following definitions:

Culture consists of the historically accumulated knowledge, tools and attitudes that pervade the child's proximal ecology, including the cultural “practices” of nuclear family members and other kin. These enculturated members of society are themselves subject to a variety of forces in both the natural ecology and society as they carry out their roles, such as care giving and earning a living.

Learning is understood as a relatively permanent change in behavior and understanding brought about by the child's experience.

Development entails qualitative changes in the functional organization of children’s intra-individual brain, body and behavior and in accompanying changes in the relationship between children and their socio-culturally organized experiences.

Subject

Culture plays an essential role in how children make sense of the world. A decisive differencebetween children’s learning and any intelligent technical system is that technical systems canrecognize and organize information, but cannot grasp its meaning. Development of significationand adoption of the appropriate cultural tools (symbols, meanings, scripts, goals etc.) of humanactivity are basic challenges of early learning.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 7

Page 8: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Problems

Research context

Learning mediated by culture requires consideration of a cultural context that cannot be reducedto laboratory conditions. “Natural experiments” are often-employed research settings usingfollow-up studies in one culture or collecting comparative data from several cultures. Michael Cole2,3 has elaborated a specially-designed form of activity, called the “Fifth Dimension” environmentas a sustainable subculture for learning. Its principles are used for the research of culturallearning in play settings.4,5

Key research questions

Recent research results

Universal features of culture

It is necessary to remember that children do not make sense of the world consciously andanalytically at this age. Meanings are grounded in bodily connections with things and areconstantly bound up with the process of acting.6 Nonetheless, from birth or shortly thereafter,children are extremely sensitive to contingencies among all kinds of events in their environment.These range from learning characteristic patterns of activity, to the differential responses of

1. How are enculturation and individuation related in early learning? Each cultural context has unifying tendencies, but individuals are unique. What are the universal and the specific niches of learning in each culture?

2. What is the unit of learning? Early stages of human development demonstrate dependence of the child on adults and the reverse influence of infant on adults. Mother–child dyads are important units. How are dyads replaced as units of learning?

3. How does the role of culture in learning change during early childhood?1

What kinds of environmental organization promote children’s learning of their cultural heritage? 

How do different cultural traditions shape children’s learning?

How are different modes of learning related in different cultural circumstances?

Are there “qualitative leaps” in early childhood related to culturally-related changes in modes of learning?

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 8

Page 9: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

people in their environment, to the contingencies among the phonemes in the language theyhear that will form the basis of the grammar of their native language.7 Children are born alreadyknowing the characteristic “tune” of their native language, learning that is displayed whendifferent attention is given to vocalizations in that language.8 

From birth onwards, children’s learning of a variety of universal concepts in such “privileged domains” as arithmetic, physics and  psychology  are present in a “skeletal” form that subsequent, culturally-mediated learning builds upon according to local circumstances.3

For example, infants appear to recognize basic physical concepts associated with such phenomena as gravity (they are surprised if an object appears to fall through a solid barrier) and mathematical concepts such as 1+1=2 (they are surprised if two objects are hidden behind a screen and when the screen is removed, only one object is to be seen), and are able to distinguish between intentional and mechanical causation, providing the scaffolding for learning the distinction between animate and inanimate objects.

Children also start to create their own “cultures” by about nine months and before the age of five the need for, and organization of, adult and peer cooperation radically change.9,10,11 Early in development, children are incapable of regulating the social organization of their interaction, but as middle childhood approaches, greater autonomy of child groups becomes possible.

Cultural constraints

Many psychologists believe that children from different cultural groups learn a basic “cognitivestyle” characterized in somewhat different, but overlapping terms depending upon differentscholarly traditions. One such “cognitive style” is said to privilege an initial attention to thecontext in which events occur followed by attention to the objects that participate in the event; asimilar formulation is between cultures that foster individualism or collectivism.12 It has beendemonstrated, for example, that Japanese mothers asked to engage their 5-month-old child in aninteraction involving an object, systematically orient the child to themselves first and to theobject secondarily, whereas American mothers orient the child to the object first and themselvessecondarily. At 5 months there is no difference discernable in the behavior of the children, butseveral months later, the children orient in the manner that has been shaped by repeated(differently-oriented) interactions with their parents in a wide variety of everyday events.13

Cultural practices

Different forms of play (object play, symbolic play, pretend role play) create different kinds of

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 9

Page 10: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

cultural environments for learning. However, there are wide cultural variations in the extent towhich adults sanction different forms of play during early childhood.14 In societies where play is avalued cultural practice at this age, Poddiakov15 demonstrated how children carry out socialexperimentation with other persons in play and everyday life. Vygotsky16 and other playresearchers17,18,19 emphasize the importance of mutuality and transcending the present situation inplay by creating other (imaginative) worlds. Vygotsky argued that distorting reality in playparadoxically reinforces learning applied to real life by changing children’s understanding of therelation between objects and meanings.

Greenfield and her colleagues have documented a pattern of learning among traditional Mayan peasants girls learning to weave, in which mothers organize the girls’ learning by having them participate in changing roles from very early childhood to middle childhood and beyond.20

Such learning involves very little verbal interaction. Similarly, Barbara Rogoff and her colleagues have shown that children from societies where schooling is either absent or very brief learn through a process of intent observation.21 

Research gaps

Pragmatism and cultural constraints guide decisions on what problems are central in the study ofculture and early learning. For example, a recent review on the effects of creative drama onlanguage learning states: “In this era of accountability and high-stakes testing, educators andadministrators need tangible proof of drama’s benefits, and only the highest quality research canprovide this type of evidence.”22 From this viewpoint, high quality does not include culturalrelevance. However, research based on strict isolation of causal variables obscures theexperiential essence of learning and impedes combining research results in different domains(e.g. cognitive, affective and psychomotor). “A whole child approach” to the study of culture andearly learning is a continuing challenge. 

Conclusions

The study of culture and early learning involves the interweaving of biological and culturalfactors. Active development of subcultures aiming at expanding and enhancing learning is onepromising current approach. But there is disagreement about what such subcultures should be.For example, play is only lately regaining the status of a legitimate form of learning activity.23,24,25

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 10

Page 11: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Very important is the sheer exposure to the material to be learned that is afforded by different cultural practices. It is a routine finding in research across many content domains that when children are asked to learn or solve problems based upon materials with which they are familiar, or in ways that make “human sense”26 they learn more rapidly.

These relations between culture and learning do not fade away, but become even more pronounced as children move from early into middle childhood and adolescence. Consequently, those concerned with leveraging the power of culture to promote learning should take care to pay as much attention to the cultural enrichment of children as to their health and physical well-being, all of which play an especially important role during this period of extraordinarily rapid developmental change.

Implications

Misunderstanding the cultural character of early childhood learning has resulted in a situationwhere effective forms of learning and sense making that take place in a play context areeliminated from children’s life. When learning is defined in terms of analytic understanding,children’s own subcultures and play forms are denied. A negative consequence of this view maybe diminished impact of learning on child development.27,28 

References

1. Vygotsky LS. . Cole M, John-Steiner V, Schribner S, Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes

Souberman E, eds. Cambridge, UK: Harvard University Press; 1978. 

2. Cole M. . Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of  Harvard University Press; Cultural psychology. A once and future discipline

1996. 

3. Cole M, the Distributed Literacy Consortium. . New York, NY: Russel Sage; 2006. The fifth dimension

4. Bredikyte M. Play as an educational tool in early childhood. 2008;14(3):10-11. Every Child

5. Hakkarainen P. Narrative learning in the fifth dimension. 2004;1:5-20. Outlines

6. Johnson M. . Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press; The meaning of the body. Aesthetics of human understanding

2007. 

7. Jusczyk PW. How infants adapt speech-processing capacities to native language structure. 2002;11(1):15-18. 

Current Directions in Psychological Science

8. Kuhl PK. A new view of language acquisition. In: Luria H, Seymour DM, Smoke T, eds. . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2006:29-42. 

Language and linguistics in context: Readings and applications for teachers

9. Lisina MI. [Child, adults, peers: patterns of communication]. Judelson K, trans. Moscow, Rebenok, vzroslye, sverstniki

Russia: Progress Publishers; 1985. 

10. Lobok AM. [The anthropology of myth]. Yekaterinburg, Russia: Bank Kul’turnoi Informatsii; 1997. Antropologiia mifa

11. Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ. Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory and clinical applications. 2001;42(1):3-48. 

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 11

Page 12: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

12. Kitayama S, Cohen D, eds. . New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2007. Handbook of cultural psychology

13. Bornstein MH, Tal J, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Parenting in cross-cultural perspective: The United States, France, and Japan. In: Bornstein MH, ed. . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991:69-90. Cultural approaches to parenting

14. Göncü A, Gaskins S, eds. . Mahwah, NJ: Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2007.  

15. Podd’iakov NN. [Specific characteristics of psychological Osobennosti psikhicheskogo razvitiia detei doshkol’nogo vozrasta

development of preschool children] Moscow, Russia: Pedagogika; 1996. 

16. Vygotsky LS. Imagination and creativity in childhood. 2004;42(1):7-97. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology

17. Fein G. Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. 1981;52(4):1095-1118.Child Development

18. Garvey C. . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1977. Play

19. Sutton-Smith B. . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1977. The Ambiguity of play

20. Greenfield P. . Santa Fe: School of American Weaving generations together: Evolving creativity in the Maya of Chiapas

Research Press; 2004. 

21. Mejía-Arauz R, Rogoff B, Paradise R. Cultural variation in children's observation during a demonstration. 2005;29(4):282-291. 

International Journal of Behavioral Development

22. Mages WK. Does creative drama promote language development in early childhood? A review of the methods and measures employed in empirical literature. 2008;78(1):124-152. Review of Educational Research

23. Singer D, Golinkoff R, Hirsh-Pasek K, eds. . New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. 

Play = learning. How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth

24. Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Berk LE, Singer D. . New Mandate for playful learning in preschool. Presenting the evidence

York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2009. 

25. Hakkarainen P. Learning and development in play. In: Einarsdottir J, Wagner J, eds. . Greenwich, CT: IAP-

Nordic childhoods and early education: philosophy, research, policy, and practice in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway, and Sweden

Information Age Pub; 2006:183-222. 

26. Donaldson M. . London, UK: Penguin; 1993. Human minds: an exploration

27. Hakkarainen P, Bredikyte M. The zone of proximal development in play and learning. 2008;4:2-11. 

Cultural - historical Psychology

28. Zuckerman G. Child-adult interaction that creates a zone of proximal development. 2007;45(3):38-64.

Journal of Russian and East European Psychology

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 12

Page 13: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture and Early Socio-Emotional DevelopmentXinyin Chen, PhD

University of Western Ontario, CanadaJuin 2009

Introduction

There are considerable individual differences in children’s early dispositional characteristics, suchas how they react to challenging situations and their ability to regulate behavioral and emotionalreactions.1 These early characteristics serve as a basis for socio-emotional development inchildhood and adolescence. It has been found that early dispositional characteristics and socio-emotional functioning have an extensive and prolonged impact on social, school andpsychological adjustment. In Western societies, for example, positive emotionality and sociabilityare predictive of peer acceptance, school achievement and psychological well-being. In contrast,defiance and aggression are associated with later peer rejection, school problems, and otheradjustment problems. Finally, social anxiety and behavioral inhibition in infancy and earlychildhood may contribute to difficulties in peer relationships and adjustment problems of aninternalizing nature such as loneliness and depression.2,3,4

Subject

Socio-emotional development is likely to be affected by cultural contexts. Developmentaltheorists and researchers have long recognized the comprehensive role of culture in children’ssocial development in the early years.5 Culture may promote or constrain the exhibition ofspecific aspects of socio-emotional functioning through facilitation or suppression processes.Moreover, cultural norms and values may provide guidance for the interpretation and evaluationof social behaviors and thus impart meanings to the behaviours.6 These arguments have beensupported by findings from a number of studies in the past two decades.

Problems

Despite the importance of culture for human development, research on socio-emotionalfunctioning has been conducted mostly with Western, particularly North American, children.Consequently, little is known about how children behave and perform in social situations in other

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 13

Page 14: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

societies. Our understanding of social behaviours, relationships and psychological adjustment islimited to Euro-American cultures.

Research Context

Over the past 20 years, there has been an increased interest in exploring children’s socio-emotional functioning in different regions of the worlds, particularly Asia, Europe and SouthAmerica. A number of studies have been conducted in diverse societies using both qualitative(e.g., interviewing, ethnographic, observation) and quantitative (e.g., large scale surveys,standardized questionnaires) methods. A major challenge in the cross-cultural study of socio-emotional functioning is the understanding of its cultural meaning. Two strategies to achieve thisunderstanding are (1) to examine how socio-emotional functioning is associated with socialinteractions and relationships, and (2) to examine how socio-emotional functioning develops inthe culture (e.g., what developmental outcomes it leads to).6 These strategies can be used in bothwithin-cultural and cross-cultural studies. An examination of the social interaction context and thedevelopmental pattern of socio-emotional functioning from the within-cultural perspective is thefirst step toward understanding its meaning and significance and provides a critical andnecessary foundation for cross-cultural comparisons on children’s socio-emotional functioning.

Key Research Questions

Recent Research Results

Children across cultures may display similar as well as different socio-emotional characteristics in

1. Are there cross-cultural differences in the exhibition of specific aspects of socio-emotionalfunctioning?

2. Are there cross-cultural differences in the antecedents, concomitants and consequences ofspecific aspects of socio-emotional functioning?

3. Are the developmental processes and patterns of socio-emotional functioning similar ordifferent across cultures?

4. What cultural beliefs and values are associated with socio-emotional functioning anddevelopment?

5. What are the processes in which cultural beliefs and values affect socio-emotionalfunctioning and development?

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 14

Page 15: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

early childhood. Whereas similarity emerges in pervasive aspects, the distinct patterns of socio-emotional functioning have been revealed in cross-cultural research on children in differentsocieties. For example, Chinese and Korean toddlers exhibited higher fearful, vigilant and anxiousreactions than Australian, Canadian and Italian toddlers in novel stressful situations.7,8 Chinesechildren also displayed more committed and internalized control or self-regulation on complianceand delay tasks than North American children in the early years.9,10,11 Similarly, Cameroonian Nsotoddlers displayed more regulated behaviors than Costa Rican toddlers who in turn were moreregulated than Greek toddlers, as indicated by their compliance with maternal requests andprohibitions.12

Cross-cultural differences in early characteristics may be associated with parental socializationexpectations, attitudes and practices. Chen et al.7 found that whereas children’s wary andreactive behavior was associated with parental disappointment and rejection in Canada, thisbehavior was associated with warm and accepting parental attitudes in China. Compared withEuro-American parents, Chinese and Korean parents were also more likely to emphasizebehavioral control in childrearing.9 In addition, according to Keller et al.,12 rural Cameroonian Nsomothers were more likely than Costa Rican mothers, who in turn were more likely than middle-class Greek mothers, to use a proximal parenting style (body contact, body stimulation) whichwas believed to facilitate child obedience and regulation.

Socio-emotional characteristics in the early years may have implications for the development ofsocial behaviors. Edwards13 found that children in relatively open communities (e.g., Taira inOkinawa, one of Japan’s southern prefectures, and Orchard Town in the U.S.) where peerinteractions were encouraged had significantly higher scores on overall social engagement thanchildren in more “close” and agricultural communities (e.g., Nyansongo in Kenya and Khalapur inIndia). Relatively low social interaction was also found in Chinese and Indonesian children,compared with their North American counterparts.14,15

Cross-cultural differences exist not only in overall social engagement but also in the quality ofsocial interaction. A particular form of peer interaction which varies across cultures is socio-dramatic activity in children’s play. Western children tend to engage in more socio-dramaticbehaviors than children in many other, particularly group-oriented, cultures. Farver, Kim and Lee16

found that Korean American preschool children displayed less social and pretend play than Anglo-American children. Moreover, when Korean children engaged in pretend play, it contained moreeveryday and family role activities and less fantastic themes (e.g., actions related to legend or

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 15

Page 16: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

fairy tale characters that do not exist). Gosso Lima, Morais and Otta17 found that rural children inBrazil displayed less pretend or socio-dramatic behaviors than urban children. Furthermore, theurban children’s socio-dramatic activities involved more fantastic characters or themes thanthose of rural children. Also, the prevalent characters in the pretend play of seashore childrenwere domestic animals (dogs and horses), which, according to Gosso et al.17, was due to thefrequent contact of these children with them in daily life.

Children in societies where extended families live together in traditional styles tend to displaymore prosocial-cooperative behavior than children in economically complex societies with classstructures and occupational division of labour.13 Early socialization of responsibility is associatedwith the development of prosocial-cooperative behavior. Cultures that value competitiveness andthe pursuit of personal goals seem to allow for more coercive and aggressive behavior thancultures that emphasize group harmony. Researchers have reported that North American childrentended to exhibit higher levels of aggressive and externalizing behavior than their counterparts insome Asian countries such as China, Korea, Japan and Thailand, in Australia and in someEuropean nations such as Sweden and the Netherlands.18,19,20,21

Research Gaps

Several major gaps exist in the study of culture and socio-emotional development. First, there arefew systematic cross-cultural longitudinal research programs. As a result, little is known about thedevelopmental processes of socio-emotional functioning in a cultural context. Second, theexisting research has relied mostly on cross-cultural comparisons. Although the findings areimportant in revealing cultural similarities and differences, they provide limited information aboutwhat specific cultural beliefs and values are associated with children’s social behaviours andemotions and their development. Third, researchers have paid little attention to the processes inwhich cultural norms and values are involved in socio-emotional development. Chen, Chung andHsiao22 have recently proposed a contextual-developmental perspective that emphasizes the roleof the social evaluation and response processes in mediating the links between culture and socio-emotional development. According to this perspective, during social interactions, peers evaluateand respond to individual characteristics in manners that are consistent with cultural beliefsystems in the society and express corresponding attitudes (e.g., acceptance, rejection) towardchildren who display the characteristics. Culturally-directed social evaluations and responses, inturn, regulate children’s behaviours and ultimately their developmental patterns. How the peerinteraction processes serve to transmit and construct cultures and to regulate children’s social

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 16

Page 17: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

functioning and development need to be examined thoroughly in future research.

Conclusions

Cross-cultural research has indicated the involvement of cultural factors in virtually all aspects ofchildren’s socio-emotional functioning. Cultural norms and values may affect the display andsignificance of children’s socio-emotional functioning. The impact of cultural context on socio-emotional development is likely to occur through parental socialization practices and, in the lateryears, through peer interactions. Future research should explore the processes in which culturalfactors are involved in children’s social behaviors and emotions and their development.

Implications

Cross-cultural research helps us understand the role of social and cultural conditions in thedevelopment of social competence and problems. The findings also have implications forestablishing appropriate policies related to families and children in Canada who have differentcultural backgrounds. Moreover, the information about cross-cultural differences in children’ssocio-emotional characteristics and interaction styles are helpful for professionals to designculturally-sensitive and relevant programs in the community and the school for children ofdifferent backgrounds who have social and psychological problems.

References

1. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Lerner RM. . Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley &Sons;  2006: 99-166.  Eisenberg N, ed. Vol. 3.

Handbook of child psychologySocial, emotional, and personality development.

2. Dodge KA, Coie JD, Lynam D. Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth.  In: Damon W, Lerner RM. . Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons;  2006: 719-788.  Eisenberg N, ed.

Vol. 3.

Handbook of child psychology Social, emotional, and personality development.

3. Coplan RJ, Prakash K, O'Neil K, Armer M. Do you 'want' to play? Distinguishing between conflicted-shyness and socialdisinterest in early childhood. 2004;40(2):244-258.Developmental Psychology

4. Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Coplan RJ. Social withdrawal and shyness. In: Smith PK, Hart CH, eds. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers; 2002:330-352.

Blackwell handbook of childhood social development.

5. Hinde RA. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1987.Individuals, relationships and culture.

6. Chen X, French D. Children’s social competence in cultural context. 2008;59:591–616.Annual Review of Psychology

7. Chen X, Hastings P, Rubin KH, Chen H, Cen G, Stewart SL. Childrearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese andCanadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. 1998;34(4):677-686.Developmental Psychology

8. Rubin KH, Hemphill SA, Chen X, Hastings P, Sanson A, LoCoco A, Zappulla C, Chung O, Park SY, Do HS, Chen H, Sun L, YoonCH, Cui L. A cross-cultural study of behavioral inhibition in toddlers: East-west-north-south.

2006;30(3):219-226.International Journal of

Behavioral Development

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 17

Page 18: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

9. Chen X, Rubin KH, Liu M, Chen H, Wang L, Li D, Gao X, Cen G, Gu H, Li B. Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers. 2003;27(5):428-436.International Journal of Behavioral Development

10. Gartstein MA, Gonzalez C, Carranza JA, Ahadi SA, Ye R, Rothbart MK, Yang SW. Studying cross-cultural differences in thedevelopment of infant temperament: People's Republic of China, the United States of America, and Spain.

2006;37:145-161.Child Psychiatry

& Human Development

11. Sabbagh MA, Xu F, Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Lee K. The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: Acomparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. 2006;17(1):74-81.Psychological Science

12. Keller H, Yovsi R, Borke J, Kartner J, Jensen H, Papaligoura Z. Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences:Self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. 2004; (6):1745-1760.Child Development 75

13. Edwards CP. Children’s play in cross-cultural perspective: A new look at the Six Culture Study. 2000;34(3):318-338.

Cross-Cultural Research

14. Chen X, DeSouza A, Chen H, Wang L. Reticent behavior and experiences in peer interactions in Canadian and Chinesechildren. 2006;42(4):656-665.Developmental Psychology

15. Farver JM, Howes C. Cross-cultural differences in social interaction: A comparison of American and Indonesian children. 1988;19(2):203–315.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

16. Farver JM, Kim YK, Lee Y. Cultural differences in Korean- and Anglo-American preschoolers’ social interaction and playbehaviors. 1995;66(4):1088–1099.Child Development

17. Gosso Y, Lima MD, Morais SE, Otta E. Pretend play of Brazilian children: A window into different cutlural worlds. 2007;38(5):539-558.

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

18. Bergeron N, Schneider BH. Explaining cross-national differences in peer-directed aggression: A quantitative synthesis. 2005;31(2):116-137.Aggressive Behavior

19. Russell A, Hart CH, Robinson CC, Olsen SF. Children’s sociable and aggressive behavior with peers: A comparison of the USand Australia, and contributions of temperament and parenting styles. 2003;27(1):74-86.

International Journal of Behavioral Development

20. Weisz JR, Suwanlert S, Chaiyasit W, Weiss B, Walter BR, Anderson WW. Thai and American perspectives on over-andundercontrolled child behavior problems: Exploring the threshold model among parents, teachers, and psychologists.

1988;56(4):601-609.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

21. Zahn-Waxler C, Friedman RJ, Cole PM, Mizuta I, Hiruma N. Japanese and United States preschool children’s responses toconflict and distress. 1966;67(5):2462-2477.Child Development

22. Chen X, Chung J, Hsiao C. Peer interactions, relationships and groups from a cross-cultural perspective. In: Rubin KH,Bukowski W, Laursen B, eds. New York, NY: Guilford Press;2008:432-454.

Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 18

Page 19: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture and Social DevelopmentKenneth H. Rubin, PhD, Melissa Menzer, BA

University of Maryland, USAJanvier 2010

Introduction

Culture can be defined as “the set of attitudes, values, beliefs, and behaviors shared by a groupof people, communicated from one generation to the next.”1 Given that the majority of theworld’s children do not reside in Westernized countries, and that culture influences development,cross-cultural research on child development requires special attention.

Subject and problems

The focus of this essay is on the role of culture on children’s social development. Importantly, the form that behaviors take may appear identical across cultures. Yet, given that cultures vary intheir customs and beliefs, the same behavior may be interpreted differently across cultures.2 It islikely that any behavior that is viewed, within a culture, as adaptive will lead to itsencouragement by significant others including parents and peers; in contrast, if a behavior isperceived to be maladaptive, it will be discouraged. Moreover, the means by which the givenbehavior is encouraged or discouraged may be culturally determined and defined.

Most cross-cultural work on children’s social development has been dominated by an etic

framework, which assumes that the constructs measured have relevance across all cultures.3 Onthe other hand, an emic framework refers to the specific ideas, behaviors, and values that areviewed as meaningful by members of a particular culture. The etic perspective may causeresearchers to operationally define (and thus assess) constructs in the same ways (with the samemethods and measures) across cultures. Thus, the etic approach may result in overlookingculturally-specific definitions of given constructs. For example, researchers may assume thatsocial competence, as a construct, is universally relevant and that it can be measured byassessments created in, for example, North American laboratories. This etic assumption may beentirely correct; however one would clearly need to empirically test this assumption. It is likelythat, to some extent, the study of social competence would require an emic belief requiringwithin-culture conceptualization and measurement. Some aspects of competence may be

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 19

Page 20: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

universally held and others not.

Research Context

In addition to culture, other significant constructs need to be addressed. For example, broadly,researchers typically discuss two cultural phenomena: 1) independent, individualistic, or Westerncultures, and 2) interdependent, collectivistic, or Eastern and Southern (e.g., Central and SouthAmerican) cultures. Western cultures are often described as those for whom members valueassertiveness, expressiveness and competitiveness; whereas Eastern and Southern cultures areoften described as those for whom members value group harmony and cooperation. Morerecently, there has been agreement that most countries are a fine mix of both of theseconstructs, with some being relatively more individualistic and others relatively morecollectivistic. Significantly, in the research area reviewed herein, there is relatively little known ofSouthern cultures (or differences between Northern and Southern cultures); thus, the review isfocused mainly on comparisons between Western and Eastern cultures.

Key Research Questions

Recent Research Results

Individual Characteristics and Interactions

Temperament. Generally defined, temperament is the biological basis of personality.4 Researchon the topic of temperamentally-based socially wary, reticent and inhibited behavior has reporteddifferences in prevalence of this construct between East Asian (e.g., China, South Korea) andWestern children and youth (e.g., Western Europe, Canada and the United States); the formergroup has demonstrated a higher prevalence of wary, inhibited behavior than the latter.5,6,7,8,9 InWestern cultures, which value independence and assertiveness, socially-inhibited and reticentbehavior is viewed as reflecting shyness, fearfulness and social incompetence; in East Asiancultures, which are dominated historically by Confucian and Taoist philosophies, socially wary and

1. What defines social competence in Western, Eastern, Northern and Southern cultures?

2. How do peers react to children and adolescents who fail to conform to cultural norms ofsocial competence?

3. How do individual characteristics, social interactions and relationships, groups and cultureinteract to influence social development?

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 20

Page 21: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

inhibited behavior is viewed as reflecting compliance, obedience, being well-mannered, and thus,social maturity and accomplishment.10

Prosocial behavior. In general, prosocial behaviors (helping, sharing, caring, politeness) increase during the course of childhood, although the development and prevalence of prosocial behaviors varies across cultures.11 For example, researchers find that prosocial behavior, as observed among peers and in parent-child interaction is more prevalent among young East Asian children than among Western children. Researchers suggest that this difference results from the collectivist ideologies prevalent in East Asian cultures. In support of this contention, researchers have reported that Chinese mothers of preschoolers are more likely than European American mothers to believe that their preschool children should share and help other children for social conventional reasons (e.g., to fit in with the group and function well in Chinese society).12

Cooperation/competition. Whereas competition can damage group harmony, cooperation isnecessary in relationship maintenance.13 Children from interdependent communities are morecooperative and less competitive than those from Westernized cultures. However, competitionand cooperation appear to co-exist regardless of culture. For example, in East Asian nations,children are more cooperative with friends and family, but more competitive in educationalcontexts.14 Further, generational differences appear to exist within cultures. For example, third-generation Mexican Americans are more competitive than their second-generation counterparts.15

Aggression. Physical, verbal and relational aggression have been identified as distinct entities inmany cultures and countries.16,17,18,19,20 Typically, physical aggression is viewed as unacceptable byparents and is associated with peer rejection in most countries.21,22,16,23,24,25,8,26 Nevertheless, meta-analyses have demonstrated that cultures characterized by collectivistic and Confucian valuesgenerally show lower levels of aggression, regardless of type, towards peers than their Westerncounterparts.27

Social withdrawal. There is increasing evidence that fearful, wary, inhibited behavior amongtoddlers predicts early childhood social reticence and anxiety.9 Although inhibited toddlers inNorth America and East Asia are at increased risk for social reticence as preschoolers, theprevalence of reticent behavior is higher among East Asian than Western children.28 Relatedly,young Western children are more sociable (i.e., friendly and outgoing) than their East Asiancounterparts.

Peer relationships: Friendships

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 21

Page 22: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Friendship is often referred to as a close, mutual and voluntary dyadic relationship. The voluntarynature of friendships means that children are able to initiate, maintain and relinquish friendshipsthat meet their expectations and/or needs. However, the notion that friendship is a voluntary,freely-chosen relationship may not be the case in all cultures.29 In some cultures, children rarelyengage in non-familial friendships. For example, children in traditional Yucatec Mayancommunities spend most of their time with their immediate and extended family.30

From a Western perspective, researchers have argued that friendship serves different functionsfor children at different points in development.31 For example, young children’s friendships serveto maximize excitement and amusement during play and to aid in the organization of behavior.Little is known, however, about the developmental course of the functions of friendship acrosscultures. Moreover, the functions and nature of friendship appear to vary across cultures. Incultures within which friendships are considered one of very few relationships guaranteeingsocietal success, both intimacy and exclusivity should be regarded as the most important aspectsof a friendship.32 Reflecting this idea, researchers have found that intimacy is more important inthe friendships of children in Korea and Cuba than in those of North American children.33,34

It is also the case that across cultures, friends spend more time together than non-friends; oneoutcome is that friends are often observed to engage in more conflict than unfamiliar peers ormere acquaintances.35 If appropriately resolved, conflict can positively affect developmentalgrowth.36 However, conflict is resolved differently across cultures. Researchers have reported thatnegotiation is often used to resolve conflict among Western children; whereas disengagementappears to be favored among Eastern cultures.37

From an early age, most children form friendships with those who are similar to themselves inobservablecharacteristics, such as age, sex, ethnicity, and behavioral proclivities. Even childrenof preschool age are more likely to choose play partners who are similar to them in age, sex,ethnicity and behavior.38,39

The group: Peer acceptance and rejection

Young, socially-accepted children are typically skilled at initiating and maintaining positiverelationships, and are viewed by peers and teachers as cooperative, sociable and sensitive. Thesefindings cut across cultures: friendly children tend to be accepted by peers across cultures; on theother hand, researchers have found that across cultures, immature, socially unskilled and

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 22

Page 23: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

aggressive preschoolers are rejected by their peers.21,40,41,42,22,16,23,24,25,8,20,26

In Westernized contexts, social withdrawal has been linked to peer rejection.40,22,43,44 But recent

findings have revealed that social withdrawal is also associated with rejection among children inIndia and industrialized China.45,8 Thus, the correlates of peer acceptance and rejection acrosscultures appear to be similar. Both aggressiveness and withdrawal are associated with rejection,whereas prosocial behavior is linked with acceptance.

Research Gaps

As aforementioned, a salient problem in cross-cultural work is the belief that an etic approach issuperior to an emic approach. In many respects, such a belief may result from the accompanyingbelief that measures created in Western countries can be “parachuted,” in valid and reliableways, into different countries and cultures. To demonstrate the fallacy of this argument, we referto a social competence construct specific to China: Ren or forbearance. Ren is a construct thatencourages group harmony. When young Chinese children use ren in response to peer animosity,they disengage from, rather than do battle with, their peers.46 This strategy is unlike problem-focused avoidance because it does not reflect the goal to escape or avoid the social situation.Instead, the goal of ren is to elicit restraint and tolerance from the peers with whom they areinteracting. Western researchers may well overlook the social convention of ren and thus, mayinaccurately construe and assess the construct of social competence in Chinese culture.Therefore, it would behoove researchers to consider their cultures of interest, and to collaboratewith members of those cultures to conceptualize and operationally define social competence.Along the way, investigators should consider how the given construct may be defined at differentdevelopmental periods and how it evolves both in the short and long term.

Another consideration is the study of ethnic subpopulations within multicultural societies. Forexample, in the United States, the East Asian American and the Latino American populations arecontinually rising in numbers. There is some indication that immigrant populations in thesecountries hold similar values to their Asian and Latin mainland counterparts.47,15 Yet, for someyouth, there appear to be generational and acculturation effects, whereby later generations aremore acculturated to mainstream Westernized culture than previous generations. It would benefitresearchers to examine the effects of acculturation in their assessments of cross-cultural or cross-ethnic variability.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 23

Page 24: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Conclusion

In summary, Western researchers who have interests in cross-cultural studies of young children’ssocial development (and development in general) would do well to incorporate into their researchprograms the expertise of collaborators from other cultures. Only through conversations withtheir collaborators will they develop a better understanding of the constructs that truly matter inthe lives of children and their peers.

Implications for Parents, Services and Policy

Given that the majority of the world's inhabitants do not reside in culturally “Westernized”countries, cross-cultural work on the study of social development bears careful note. From ourexample of social inhibition or reticence, one can begin to understand that behaviors, whenexhibited across cultural settings may take the same form; however, the function of thesebehaviors varies from culture-to-culture. Within any culture, children are shaped by the physicaland social settings within which they live; culturally-regulated customs and childrearing practices;and culturally-based belief systems. The bottom line is that the psychological “meaning”attributed to any given social behavior is, in large part, a function of the ecological niche withinwhich it is produced. All-in-all then, it would appear most sensible for the international communityof child development researchers to not generalize to other cultures, their own culture-specifictheories of normal and abnormal social development.

These statements are also relevant insofar as policy and “translation” are concerned.Practitioners, such as psychologists, social workers and teachers must begin to understand thatnormalcy is culturally defined. Criteria for psychiatric and psychological diagnoses must begin totake into account different cultural values. If criteria are not culturally sensitive, then a child whois reinforced to behave in X-manner by his or her immigrant parents, when X is viewed, within thelarger cultural community as inappropriate or reflective of abnormality, all manner of difficultymay arise. Thus, policy makers and practitioners must be educated to understand thesignificance of cultural norms when interpreting the meanings of social behavior. Further, anunderstanding that social development is influenced by culture may aid host communities todevelop sources of information (and possibly intervention) for parents (and children) whose beliefsystems may place children at risk for rejection, exclusion, discrimination, and victimization bymembers of the host community or country.

References

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 24

Page 25: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

1. Matsumoto D. . Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co; 1997:5.Culture and modern life

2. Whiting J, Child IL. . New York, NY: Yale University Press; 1953.Child training and personality: a cross-cultural study

3. Bornstein M. . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991.Cultural approaches to parenting

4. Goldsmith H, Buss A, Plomin R, Rothbart MK, Thomas A, Chess S, Hinde RA, McCall RB. Roundtable: What is temperament?Four approaches. 1987;58(2):505-529.Child Development

5. Broberg A, Lamb M, Hwang P. Inhibition: Its stability and correlates in sixteen- to forty-month-old children. 1990;61:1153-1163.

Child Development

6. Chen X, Hastings PD, Rubin KH, Chen H, Cen G, Stewart SL. Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese andCanadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. 1998;34(4):677-686.Developmental Psychology

7. Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, Schmidt LA. Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition andexuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. 2001;72(1):1-21.

Child Development

8. Prakash K, Coplan RJ. Socioemotional characteristics and school adjustment of socially withdrawn children in India. 2007;31(2):123-132.International Journal of Behavioral Development

9. Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Hastings PD. Stability and social-behavioral consequences of toddlers' inhibited temperament andparenting behaviors. 2002;73(2):483-495.Child Development

10. King AYC, Bond MH. The Confucian paradigm of man: A sociological view.  In: Tseng W, Wu DYH, eds. . Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985:29-46.

Chinese culture and mental health

11. Benenson JF, Markovits H, Roy R, Denko P. Behavioural rules underlying learning to share: Effects of development andcontext. 2003;27(2):116-121.International Journal of Behavioral Development

12. Cheah CSL, Rubin KH. European American and mainland Chinese mothers’ socialization beliefs regarding preschoolers'social skills. 2003;3(1):1-21.Parenting: Science and Practice

13. Schneider BH, Woodburn S, del Toro MPD, Udvari SJ. Cultural and gender differences in the implications of competition forearly adolescent friendship. 2005;51(2):163-191.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly

14. Kim U, Triandis H, Kâgitçibasi Ç, Choi S, Yoon G, eds. .Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1994.

Individualism and collectivism:Theory, method, and applications

15. Knight G, Kagan S. Acculturation of prosocial and competitive behaviors among second- and third-generation Mexican-American children. 1977;8(3):273-284.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

16. French D, Jansen E, Pidada S. United States and Indonesian children's and adolescents’ reports of relational aggression bydisliked peers. 2002;73(4):1143-1150.Child Development

17. Lagerspetz K, Björkqvist K, Peltonen T. Is indirect aggression typical of females? Gender differences in aggressiveness in11- to 12-year-old children. 1988;14(6):403-414.Aggressive Behavior

18. Nelson DA, Nelson LJ, Hart CH, Yang C, Jin S. Parenting and Peer-Group Behavior in Cultural Context. In: Chen X, French DC,Schneider BH, eds. . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2006:213-246.Peer relationships in cultural context

19. Owens LD. Sticks and stones and sugar and spice: Girls’ and boys’ aggression in schools. 1996;6:45-55.

Australian Journal of Guidance and Counseling

20. Tomada G, Schneider BH. Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability overtime, and concordance among informants. 1997;33(4):601-609.Developmental Psychology

21. Attili G, Vermigli P, Schneider BH. Peer acceptance and friendship patterns among Italian schoolchildren within a cross-cultural perspective. 1997;21(2):277-288.International Journal of Behavioral Development

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 25

Page 26: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

22. Cillessen AH, Van IJzendoorn HW, Van Lieshout CF, Hartup WW. Heterogeneity among peer-rejected boys: Subtypes andstabilities. 1992;63(4):893-905.Child Development

23. Hart C, Yang C, Nelson L, Robinson CC, Olsen JA, Nelson DA, Porter CL, Jin S, Olsen SF, Wu P. Peer acceptance in earlychildhood and subtypes of socially withdrawn behaviour in China, Russia and the United States.

2000;24(1):73-81.International Journal of

Behavioral Development

24. Hatzichristou C, Hopf D. A multiperspective comparison of peer sociometric status groups in childhood and adolescence. 1996;67(3):1085-1102.Child Development

25. Kerestes G, Milanovic A. Relations between different types of children's aggressive behavior and sociometric status amongpeers of the same and opposite gender. 2006;47(6):477-483.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology

26. Xu Y, Farver JM, Schwartz D, Chang L. Social networks and aggressive behavior in Chinese children. 2004;28(5):401-410.

International Journal of Behavioral Development

27. Bergeron N, Schneider B. Explaining cross-national differences in peer-directed aggression: A quantitative synthesis. 2005;31(5):116-137.Aggressive Behavior

28. Chen X, Rubin KH, Li B, Li D. Adolescent outcomes of social functioning in Chinese children. 1999;23(1):199-223.

International Journal of Behavioral Development

29. Krappmann L. Amicitia, drujba, shin-yu, philia, Freundschaft, friendship: On the cultural diversity of a human relationship.In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, eds. . NewYork, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1996:19-40.

The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence

30. Gaskins S. The cultural organization of Yucatec Mayan children’s social interactions. In: Chen X, French DC, Schneider BH,eds. . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2006:283-309.Peer relationships in cultural context

31. Parker JG, Gottman JM. Social and emotional development in a relational context: Friendship interaction from earlychildhood to adolescence. In: Bernt TJ, Ladd WG, eds. . Oxford England: John Wiley& Sons; 1989:95-131.

Peer relationships in child development

32. Triandis H, Bontempo R, Villareal MJ, Asai M, Lucca N. Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relationships. 1988;54(2):323-338.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

33. French DC, Lee O, Pidada S. Friendships of Indonesian, South Korean, and U.S. Youth: Exclusivity, Intimacy, Enhancementof Worth, and Conflict. In: Chen X, French DC, Schneider BH, eds. . New York, NY:Cambridge University Press; 2006:379-402.

Peer relationships in cultural context

34. González YS, Moreno DS, Schneider BH. Friendship Expectations of Early Adolescents in Cuba and Canada. 2004;35(4):436-445.

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

35. Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. Children's friendship relations: A meta-analytic review. 1995;117(2):306-347.

Psychological Bulletin

36. Hartup WW, Laursen B. Conflict and context in peer relations. In: Hart C, ed. . Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1993:44-84.

Children on playgrounds: Research perspectives and applications

37. French DC, Pidada S, Denoma J, McDonald K, Lawton A. Reported Peer Conflicts of Children in the United States andIndonesia. 2005;14(3):458-472.Social Development

38. Howes C, Wu F. Peer interactions and friendships in an ethnically diverse school setting. 1990;61(2):537-541.

Child Development

39. Kao G, Joyner K. Do Race and Ethnicity Matter among Friends? Activities among Interracial, Interethnic, and IntraethnicAdolescent Friends. 2004;45(3):557-573.Sociological Quarterly

40. Casiglia A, LoCoco A, Zappulla C. Aspects of social reputation and peer relationships in Italian children: A cross-culturalperspective. 1998;34(4):723-730.Developmental Psychology

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 26

Page 27: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

41. Chang L. Variable effects of children's aggression, social withdrawal, and prosocial leadership as functions of teacherbeliefs and behaviors. 2003;74(2):535-548.Child Development

42. Chen X, Liu M, Rubin KH, Cen G, Gao X, Li D. Sociability and prosocial orientation as predictors of youth adjustment: Aseven-year longitudinal study in a Chinese sample. 2002;26(2):128-136.International Journal of Behavioral Development

43. Rubin KH, Chen X, Hymel S. Socioemotional characteristics of withdrawn and aggressive children. 1993;39(4):518-534.

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly

44. Schaughency E, Vannatta K, Langhinrichsen J, Lally C, Seeley J. Correlates of sociometric status in school children inBuenos Aires. . 1992;20(3):317-326.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

45. Chen X, Cen G, Li D, He Y. Social Functioning and Adjustment in Chinese Children: The Imprint of Historical Time. 2005;76(1):182-195.Child Development

46. Xu Y, Farver JM, Chang L, Yu L, Zhang Z. Culture, Family Contexts, and Children's Coping Strategies in Peer Interactions. In:Chen X, French DC, Schneider BH, eds. . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press;2006:264-280.

Peer relationships in cultural context

47. Chen X, Tse, HC. Social functioning and adjustment in Canadian-born children with Chinese and European backgrounds. 2008;44(4):1184-1189.Developmental Psychology

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 27

Page 28: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture and Early Childhood EducationJessica Ball, PhD

University of Victoria, CanadaSeptembre 2010

Introduction

“Cultural sensitivity” is common advice in the field of early childhood learning and development,and few would argue with it.1 But are we willing to take this advice to the point of yielding toculturally-based understandings of how children learn and how to promote optimaldevelopmental outcomes? On the contrary, there is a great deal more rhetoric about respondingto cultural diversity than evidence that we really mean it when we say, as most developmentalpsychologists and many educators do, that culture is embodied in the ways that children areraised and the environments where they grow and develop.2,3,4,5,6,7 Many educators, researchersand international development specialists acknowledge the geographic and cultural limitations ofthe research base that informs current child development theory, learning assessment tools, andprogram models. However, this recognition has not prevented the proliferation of brand-nameprograms touted as “best practices” based on the authority of Euro-western science or simply onpersuasive marketing of training, toys, tools and teaching techniques.8,9 Standardized tools, suchas the Early Childhood Environment Ratings Scales,10 used to characterize the adequacy of earlylearning environments, and the Early Development Inventory,11 used to characterize the schoolreadiness of groups of children, are playing an increasingly instrumental role to set governmentagendas, plan policy, and justify the transfer of early learning program goals and models frommore to less developed countries.12,13 Expediency, along with assumptions that theory andresearch on child development developed from Euro-western perspectives are universally valid,tends to be used to justify the transport of “best practices.” It is common to hear that wherethere are no readily available, locally developed tools or programs, there is no need to “re-inventthe wheel” when an existing tool or program can be imported. While there are manycommonalities across cultures in goals for children’s early learning, researchers and educatorsmust work to identify cultural distinctiveness in developmental trajectories and expectations.14

Research Context

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 28

Page 29: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

The concept of “best practices” may once have been meaningful, designating early learningmeasurement approaches or program models identified through experimental and quasi-experimental research as capable of delivering, comparatively, the best outcomes with respect toa particular aspect of development within a particular population of children. Today, however,declaring an approach a “best practice” often signifies little more than that a measurement toolor program model is favoured by a particular stakeholder group, such as the originators of thetool or program, and that a government agency, program advisory board or funding/donoragency would like to promote the practice based on its intuitive, theoretical, or financial appeal,or the fact that the practice worked well in one particular setting. All too often there is a lack ofpeer-reviewed research reports substantiating the claim of “best” through comparative studiesthat have established the predictive validity of standardized early learning measurement tools orthe effectiveness of curricula for culturally diverse young children.

Key Questions

What developmental norms and goals for children’s learning and development and whose culturalvalues and methods for socializing children and transmitting knowledge drive the creation andchoice of curricula for early learning programs exported from a (usually Western) source countryto a receiver country or cultural setting?15And what is at stake?

Exporting early learning measurement tools and programs created in Euro-western countrieswhere European-heritage norms and approaches to development predominate can interrupt thetransmission of locally-valued cultural knowledge and practices and undermine the diversity ofvoices, knowledge sources, ways of life and supports for raising children in local conditions inreceiver countries and communities.16,17,18 Cultural knowledge and positive parenting practicesconstitute the very resources that community development programs such as those operated bymany non-governmental organizations aim to preserve and capitalize in order to promotecommunity-based, culturally resonant supports for children’s learning and development.19

Programs built on these local assets are likely to garner high demand and participation fromparents, grandparents, and local leaders and are most likely to be adapted to local conditions andsustainable.8

Recent Research Results

Four examples from the author’s program of research, Early Childhood Development Intercultural

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 29

Page 30: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Partnerships,20 illustrate the usefulness of “re-inventing the wheel” to ensure an approach tailoredto local conditions and destinations for children’s early learning. In a study of the views ofIndigenous parents, Elders and early childhood practitioners about assessing young children’scognitive development and readiness for school, participants emphasized the importance ofbuilding self-esteem as a foundation for learning.21 In addition to opportunities to enhance orallanguage, emergent literacy and numeracy, they described key curriculum content focusing oncommunity history (how children are related to the land), genealogy (who children are related to),and cultural participation (preparing for roles in ceremonies and sustenance using naturalresources). They disagreed with mainstream definitions and standardized measures of schoolreadiness promoted by public schools, arguing that schools need to be ready to receive childrenwho have a rich understanding of who they are and their cultural identity, even if they are notacculturated to the forms of teaching and learning emphasized in dominant culture classrooms.

In a second study exploring early identification of learning difficulties, Indigenous parents andElders asked why standardized and globally disseminated early learning tools such as the EarlyDevelopment Inventory22 and screening tools such as the Nipissing District Developmental Screen23 do not assess young children’s strengths, but seem more focused on identifying deficits.24 OneElder in the study commented: “They don’t ask whether children know their Indigenous languageor what children know about how to behave in different social settings or in ceremony. Schoolsaren’t interested in children learning their culture so they don’t ask about it.” A First Nationscommunity leader in the study asked: “Has anything changed since the government firstdesigned their education systems to take the Indian out of the child?”25

In a third study focused on roles for speech language pathologists, 49 out of 70 speech languagepathologists who had worked with First Nations children for two or more years reported that theirstandardized measurement tools did not yield valid or useful information and their best practicesfor early intervention were not helpful in their practice. They overwhelmingly called for “analtogether different approach” – one that is responsive to local goals and conditions for youngchildren speech-language development and that actively involves parents and other caregiversas primary supports for children’s early learning.26

A fourth study found that many Indigenous parents and some non-Indigenous teachers wereconcerned that standardized tools for measuring speech and language development and schoolreadiness may lead to misinterpretations of speech and language differences such as FirstNations English dialects or vernaculars as evidence of deficits. Low scores on tools assumed to be

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 30

Page 31: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

universally valid likely contribute to the alarmingly high rates of diagnosis of First Nationschildren as cognitively and linguistically delayed or impaired.27 There is ongoing debate about thepossibility of developing standardized tests for First Nations languages and for Indigenous childdevelopment overall. The extreme diversity among First Nations and other Indigenous children,families, and communities in Canada, with over 60 language groups and over 600 culturallydistinct, registered First Nations,  has been raised repeatedly as an obstacle to creating tools thatwould be valid or meaningful across more than a handful of communities. 

Rather than relying on standardized tools, the most useful and culturally appropriate approachmay be for educators and other practitioners to rely upon members of cultural communities todescribe and explain optimal and normative development and developmental supports and toidentify indicators and exemplars of development that represent deviations from normativeexpectations within the child’s cultural context.28 These within-community standards can bediscussed with reference to developmental norms based on research, and decisions about thegoals for early learning programs and interventions can be guided through a negotiation ofculturally based reference points and by external considerations, including considerations of thetask demands that children will face in the school they will attend, as well as child rights.29

Research Gaps

Examples of the kind of co-generated, culturally situated approach described above would bevaluable contributions to the literature on early learning research and practice. In particular,research is needed to develop and test measures of early learning and program effectivenessthat are culturally relevant but that also are not entirely idiosyncratic and reliant upon unwieldyphenomenological or public opinion, survey type research. The past decade has seen a growingrecognition of the value of collaborative approaches to research whereby investigators, policymakers, and program designers can compensate for their cultural blinders by collaborating atevery step with skilled members of cultural communities to develop the research base forculturally appropriate policies, tools, and interventions (e.g., Community Based Research Canada,30 Community Campus Partnerships for Health,31 Living Knowledge Network,32 Society forParticipatory Research in Asia33). In Canada, for example, a federally funded research projectinvolved more than 20 community-university partnerships over five years to examineenvironmental impacts on young children’s development.34

Conclusion

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 31

Page 32: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

This article calls for us to curb our enthusiasm for promoting uniform methodologies forinternational comparisons and exporting so-called “best practices” to cultural and nationalcontexts that are fundamentally different from their source. Bi-cultural, co-constructedinterpretation of development and early learning action plans has the potential to avoid theimposition of a singular, dominant cultural lens and insistence upon unidirectional assimilationthat has been the hallmark of colonialism.

Implications

What roles can we play in supporting children’s development in ways that protect and build uponculturally based assets and goals? Governments should ensure quality early learningopportunities for all children whose caregivers seek support, but funding need not be tied to one-size-fits-all curricula or learning goals.35 In Canada, the federal government’s investment inAboriginal Head Start is a powerful example of a program mandated to stimulate children’sdevelopment across six domains, including culture and home language, using methods andcurriculum content that are chosen, elaborated and delivered by each host community.36

Open-ended, dialogical engagement with communities can illuminate how to bring knowledgeand tools from research together with local knowledge and approaches to address culturallydefined goals for children’s early learning and development. There are many examples ofparticipatory, co-scripted approaches to early learning program development.37,38,39,40,41 In additionto supporting early learning and preparation for success in school, these programs are working toprotect cultural heterogeneity in the face of the overwhelmingly homogenizing forces ofglobalization.

Cultures are always changing: goals and approaches to children’s early learning and how wemeasure it embody these changes over time. As investigators, policy makers and practitioners,we need to leave room for culturally diverse families to re-invent themselves in their own imageand not, through the absence of choice, in the image of English-speaking North American middleclass cultural constructions of the child.42,43

References

1. Gonzalez-Mena J, Eyer DW. . 6thed. New York, NY: McGraw Hill; 2004.

Infants, toddlers, and caregivers: A curriculum of respectful, responsive care and education

2. Cole M. Culture in development. In: Woodhead M, Faulkner D, Littleton K, eds. . London,UK: Open University Press; 1998: 11-33.

Cultural worlds of early childhood

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 32

Page 33: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

3. Goncu A, ed. . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press;1999.

Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives

4. Greenfield PM, Suzuki LK. Culture and human development: Implications for parenting, education, pediatrics, and mentalhealth. In: Sigel IE, Rennigner KA, vol.eds. . 5th ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 1998. Damon W,general ed. ; vol. 4.

Child psychology in practiceHandbook of child psychology

5. LeVine RA, New RS, eds. . New York, NY: Blackwell; 2008.Anthropology and children development: A cross-cultural reader

6. Rogoff B. . New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003.The cultural nature of human development

7. Super CM, Harkness S. The cultural structuring of child development. In: Berry JW, Dasen PR, Saraswathi TS, eds. . Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1997: 1-39.

; vol.2.Basic processes and human development Handbook of cross-cultural psychology

8. Fleer M. Early childhood education as an evolving ‘community of practice’ or as lived ‘social reproduction’: Researching the‘taken-for-granted’. 2003;4(1):64-79.Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood

9. Kincheloe JL. Certifying the damage: Mainstream educational psychology and the oppression of children. In: Soto LD, ed. . New York, NY: Peter Lang Publishing; 2000: 75-84.The politics of early childhood education

10. Harms T, Clifford RM, Cryer D. . Carrboro, NC: Child Development Institute; 2005.Early childhood environment rating scales

11. Janus M, Offord D. Readiness to learn at school. 2000;1(2):71-75.ISUMA

12. Goldfeld S, Sayers M, Brinkman S, Silburn S, Oberklaid F. The process and policy challenges of adapting and implementingthe Early Development Instrument in Australia. 2009;20(6):978-991.Early Education and Development

13. Guhn M, Janus M, Hertzman C. The Early Development Instrument: Translating school readiness assessment intocommunity actions and policy planning. 2007;18(3):369-374.Early Education and Development

14. Lubeck S. Is developmentally appropriate practice for everyone? 1998;74(5):283-292.Childhood Education

15. Viruru R. . New Delhi, India: Sage; 2001.Early childhood education: Postcolonial perspectives from India

16. Stairs AH, Bernhard JK. Considerations for evaluating ‘good care’ in Canadian Aboriginal early childhood settings. 2002;37(3):309-330.

McGill Journal of Education

17. Canella GS, Viruru R. . New York, NY: Routledge Falmer; 2004.Childhood and postcolonization

18. Soto LD, Swadener BB. Toward liberatory early childhood theory, research and praxis: Decolonizing a field. 2002;3(1):38-66.

Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood

19. Nsamenang AB. (Mis)understanding ECD in Africa: The force of local and imposed motives. In: Garcia M, Pence A, Evans J,eds. . Washington, DC: TheWorld Bank; 2008.

Africa’s future, Africa’s challenge: Early childhood care and development in Sub-Saharan Africa

20. Early Childhood Development Intercultural Partnerships Web site. Available at: http://www.ecdip.org/. Accessed August 10,2010.

21. Ball J, Janyst P. Enacting research ethics in partnerships with Indigenous communities in Canada: “Do it in a good way.” 2008;3(2):33-52.Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics

22. Janus M, Offord DR. Development and psychometric properties of the Early Development Inventory (EDI): A measure ofchildren’s school readiness. 2007;39(1):1-22.Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science

23. Nipissing District Developmental Screen. North Bay, ON: Nipissing District Development Screen Inc.; 1993.

24. Ball J. Developmental monitoring, screening and assessment of Aboriginal young children: Findings of a community-university research partnership. Paper presented at: The Aboriginal Supported Child Care conference. November 23, 2006.Vancouver, BC.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 33

Page 34: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

25. Ball J, LeMare L. You’ll never believe what happened”…is always a good way to start”: Lessons from community-universitypartnerships with First Nations. In: Goelman H, Pivik J, Guhn M, eds.

. Palgrave Macmillan. In press.New approaches to research in early child

development: Rules, rituals, and realities

26. Ball J, Lewis M. Aboriginal parents’ goals for children’s language: What is our role? 2006;46:11-16.IMPrint

27. Ball J, Bernhardt BM. First Nations English dialects in Canada: Implications for speech-language pathology. 2008;22(8):570-588.

Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics

28. McCall RB, Green BL. Beyond the methodological gold standards of behavioral research: Considerations for practice andpolicy. 2004;18(2):3-19.Social Policy Report

29. Ball J, Pence A. . Vancouver, BC: UBCPress; 2005.

Supporting Indigenous children’s development: Community-university partnerships

30. Community Based Research Canada Web site. Available at: http://www.communityresearchcanada.ca. Accessed August10, 2010.

31. Community Campus Partnerships for Health Web site. Available at: http://www.ccph.info/. Accessed August 10, 2010.

32. Living Knowledge: The International Science Shop Network. Available at: http://www.scienceshops.org. Accessed August10, 2010.

33. Society for Participatory Research in Asia (PRIA) Web site. Available at: http://www.pria.org/en/home. Accessed August 10,2010.

34. Goelman H, Pivik J, Guhn M, eds.  New approaches to research in early child development: Rules, rituals, and realities.Palgrave Macmillan. In press.

35. Fuller B. . Palo Alto: Stanford UniversityPress; 2007.

Standardized childhood: The political and cultural struggle over early education

36. Ball J. Promoting equity and dignity for Aboriginal children in Canada. 2008;14(7):1-30.IRPP Choices

37. May H, Carr M. Empowering children to learn and grow – Te Whariki: The New Zealand early childhood national curriculum.In: Hayden J, ed.

. New York, NY: Peter Lang; 2000: 153-169.Landscapes in early childhood education: Cross-national perspectives on empowerment – A guide for the

new millennium

38. Reeders E. The collaborative construction of knowledge in a traditional context. In: Simpson J, Wigglesworth G, eds. . London, UK: Continuum; 2008.Children’s language and multilingualism: Indigenous language use at home and school

39. Rinaldi C. Reggio Emilia: The image of the child and the child’s environment as a fundamental principle. In: Gandini L,Edwards CP, eds .  New York, NY: Teachers College Press; 2001: 49-54.. Bambini: The Italian approach to infant/toddler care

40. Tagataga Inc, Inuit Tapiriit Kanatami (Organization), Summit on Inuit Education. . Ottawa, ON: Inuit Tapiriit Kanatami; 2007.

Inuit early childhood education and care: Present successes, promising directions

41. Wilson WH, Kamana K. “Mai loko mai O ka ‘I’ini: Proceeding from a dream” – the ‘aha punana leo connection in Hawaiianlanguage revitalization. In: Hinton L, Hale K, eds. . San Diego, CA:Academic Press; 2001: 147-176.

The green book of language revitalization in practice

42. Cannella GS. New York, NY: Peter Lang; 1997.Deconstructing early childhood education: Social justice and revolution.

43. Kessen W. The American child and other cultural inventions. 1979;34(10):815-820.American Psychologist

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 34

Page 35: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Bridging Cultures in Early Childhood Education1Patricia M. Greenfield, PhD, 2Carrie Rothstein-Fisch, PhD, 3Elise Trumbull, Ed.D., 4

Blanca Quiroz, PhD1FPR-UCLA Center for Culture, Brain, and Development, University of California, USA, 2California State University, USA, 3Educational Consultant, USA, 4Southwest EducationalDevelopment Laboratory, USAOctobre 2012

Introduction

In this entry, we describe the relevance of the value systems of individualism and collectivism.The articulation of these two value systems led to research demonstrating that the home cultureof collectivistic children often opposes the individualistic culture of schools in the United States.1,2

The roots of this cross-cultural value conflict lie in the poverty and lack of opportunity for formaleducation experienced by new immigrant parents  from Mexico and Central America – asociodemographic environment in which collectivism is functional.3 Because of the conflictbetween the collectivism of the home environment and the individualism of the school, thissituation creates the need for educational intervention. The Bridging Cultures Project™ is justsuch an intervention. It was designed to alleviate the cross-cultural value conflict experienced bymost immigrant families from Mexico and Central America when they send their children to U.S.schools. The project can also be applied to other minority cultures with familistic or collectivisticcultural roots. 

Research context: Applying the two cultural pathways of development to formal education

Empirical research documents cross-cultural value conflict between the more collectivisticpathway of development assumed by Latino immigrant families and the individualistic pathwaytaken for granted by the schools.1,2 These differences in cultural values coalesced into five majorthemes; in each case, the teacher assumed the relatively greater importance of the first elementin the dichotomy, while the Latino immigrant parent assumed the importance of the secondelement: 1) individual versus family accomplishment; 2) praise versus criticism; 3) cognitiveversus social skills; 4) verbal expression versus respectful communication with authority; and 5)

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 35

Page 36: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

parent’s role in teaching versus socializing the child.

Key Research Question

Based on ethnographic observation and other research,1,2 four researchers (Patricia M. Greenfield,Blanca Quiroz, Carrie Rothstein-Fisch and Elise Trumbull) posed the question: How mightknowledge of the cultural pathways of individualism and collectivism impact teachers’ ability towork with collectivistic students and families?

Our research for the Bridging Cultures Project was based on Latino immigrant families and theirschool experience, and thus we recruited seven bilingual (Spanish-English) elementary schoolteachers serving large numbers of immigrant Latino students from the greater Los Angeles area.4

Four of the teachers were Latino, three were European-American; six were female, one was male;all grade levels of elementary school were represented, kindergarten through fifth grade. In a pre-test, the teachers’ responses to home-school scenarios revealed that they were stronglyindividualistic in their problem solving strategies (86% individualistic). After three workshops, weconducted a post-test that showed the teachers had shifted to a more balanced perspective (57%collectivistic, 21% individualistic and 21% both individualistic and collectivistic). Thereafter weconducted classroom observations, interviews and semi-monthly meetings, documenting howteachers developed, implemented and evaluated new strategies for their collectivistic students.

How Bridging Cultures changed classroom practices and home-school relations

The teachers’ experimentation in their own classrooms and schools has proven the framework ofindividualism and collectivism to be more generative than we ever dreamed possible, resulting invast numbers of innovations to support student learning. We have identified many of theseclassroom practices elsewhere.5,6,7 The innovations we highlight in this entry include parentrelations, language arts and classroom management.

Changed practices with families

Bridging Cultures teachers almost immediately initiated changes in how they communicated withand supported families.8 One teacher substituted small-group parent conferences for the usualindividual conferences, a change that resulted in a new parent-teacher dynamic and a groupvoice for the parents rather than individual voices. After one hour, parents could sign up for aprivate conference or ask a few questions privately.9 (p. 69) As a result of this teacher’s success, her

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 36

Page 37: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

principal asked her to conduct a workshop at her school to encourage other teachers to try thegroup conference approach. Other Bridging Cultures soon began experimenting with the sameapproach.

Another teacher decided she would try to increase the number of parent volunteers. Although theteacher is Latina and her first language is Spanish, she reported, “Both the parents and I haddifficulty approaching each other for help. Most parents had little formal education and probablydid not know they could actually assist in the classroom: only a few had attended junior high orhigh school. I had to conduct my own informal ethnographic research about my families andbegan to build relationships with parents in the process… Although I was now averaging fiveparent volunteers a week, I still felt like there was something missing. Many parents would staybut were uncomfortable [interrupting me] while I was teaching a lesson to ask what they coulddo…”5

To support parents, the teacher compiled a notebook with an introductory paragraph describingwhy parental help would be so useful in the class. She also described an array of different waysparents could help in the classroom, from preparing materials to helping with reading. She alsoindicated that younger siblings were welcome in the class and that their presence might actuallyhelp them transition to school later on. Ultimately, the teacher more than doubled her volunteersfrom 5 to 12, including 10 who worked over 100 volunteer hours during the school year.8

Cultural knowledge facilitates learning in language arts

Bridging Cultures teachers were also successful in promoting language development, particularlyimportant because many of their students are English learners. Frequent use of choral readingactivities allowed students to practice their burgeoning English skills without fear of errorsbecause their voices could blend in with the group.6 In other cases, children normally very fearfulof speaking in front of the whole class or not wanting to be isolated from the group, wereassigned to groups who came to the front of the class together as each one took turns describingan object brought from home. In this way, learning to “speak out,” an individualistic goal of theschool, was shaped by the teacher’s knowledge that initial group support would assist students tomeet that goal.

Classroom management

One constellation of issues that seemed to be affected early on and most dramatically were

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 37

Page 38: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

related to classroom management, such as classroom rules, monitors and rewards.8 The physicalaspects of the classroom and the way it was arranged also came to reflect a collectivisticperspective in the classes participating in Bridging Cultures. In the early-elementary grades,children sat very close to each other during rug time, not on individual carpet squares as is oftenthe case in classroom practice. The children were observed touching each other’s hair or shoes ina completely non-disruptive way, much as they might do in their homes with siblings or cousins.Because the teachers now understood this to be natural and nondisruptive, they did not have totake away from lesson time to say, “Keep your hands to yourself,” but let the children behave asthey would naturally – allowing them to stay comfortable and focused, rather than potentiallymiserable and befuddled by a rule counter to their own inclinations.6

It is critical to point out that all of the teachers’ innovations were their own construction.Energized by their new understanding of the differences between the individualistic andcollectivistic value perspective, teachers made changes in their classroom practices in an effort tosolve problems as they encountered them.

Conclusions

There is no recommended ratio of individualism to collectivism in a given classroom, althoughmost of the innovations in Bridging Cultures classrooms have, quite naturally, been in thedirection of making uniformly individualistic classrooms more collectivistic. However, it is equallyimportant to note that the Bridging Cultures teachers have not rejected individualism. From thebeginning, they were very aware that eventually their students would have to learn how tosucceed in an individualistic world – likely beginning with their next classroom. For them, thebridge to individualism was as important as the introduction of more collectivistic practices.

We want to emphasize that the Bridging Cultures method is nonprescriptive. We provide theparadigm; the teachers use the paradigm to generate their own ideas, which vary greatly notonly from teacher to teacher, but also from grade level to grade level, and from school to school.

The outcomes of the Bridging Cultures Project are causes for optimism about the potential foreducational change. Note that it is highly unusual for relatively short professional developmentefforts to have a documented long-term impact on teacher practice beyond the training itself. Incontrast, we have documented an enduring transformation in the Bridging Cultures teachers’ability to learn from and build successful strategies for their students and their families.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 38

Page 39: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Acknowledgments

This chapter has been adapted from two earlier chapters, Cultural conceptions of learning and development (Greenfield, Trumbull,Keller, Rothstein-Fisch, Suzuki & Quiroz, 2006) in P.A. Alexander and P.H. Winne, Handbook of Educational Psychology (2nd ed.). pp.675-692 and Uncovering the role of culture in learning, development, and education (Rothstein-Fisch, Greenfield, Trumbull, Keller,& Quiroz, 2010) in D.D. Preiss & R.J. Sternberg (Eds.) Innovations in Educational Psychology: Perspectives on Learning, Teaching,and Human Development (pp. 269-294). New York: Springer. The Bridging Cultures Project was initially supported by a grant fromthe U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement to WestEd, the regional educational laboratoryfor the Western United States. Other support has come from the Michael D. Eisner College of Education at California StateUniversity, Northridge (and it’s Center for Teaching and Learning), the Foundation for Child Development, the A.L. MailmanFoundation, and the Russell Sage Foundation.

WestEd holds the Bridging Cultures™ trademark and has licensed its use to the four core Bridging Cultures researchers: Patricia M.Greenfield, Blanca Quiroz, Carrie Rothstein-Fisch, and Elise Trumbull.

References

1. Greenfield, P. M., Quiroz, B., & Raeff, C. (2000). Cross-cultural conflict and harmony in the social construction of the child.In S. Harkness, C. Raeff, & C. M. Super (Eds.), (pp. 93-108). New Directionsin Child and Adolescent Development. No. 87. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Variability in the social construction of the child

2. Raeff, C., Greenfield, P.M., & Quiroz, B. (2000). Conceptualizing interpersonal relationships in the cultural contexts ofindividualism and collectivism. In S. Harkness, C. Raeff, & C. Super, (Eds.), .(pp. 59-74). New Directions in Child Development. No. 87. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Variability in the social construction of the child

3. Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: Shifting pathways of human development. , 45, 401-418.Developmental Psychology

4. Trumbull, E., Diaz-Meza, R., Hasan, A. & Rothstein-Fisch, C. (2001) Five Year – Report of the Bridging Cultures Project. SanFrancisco: WestEd.

5. Trumbull, E., Rothstein-Fisch, C., Greenfield, P. M., & Quiroz, B. (2001). . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bridging cultures between home and school: A guide for teachers

6. Rothstein-Fisch, C. & Trumbull, E. (2008). .Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Managing diverse classrooms: How to build on students’ cultural strengths

7. Trumbull, E., Rothstein-Fisch, C., and Greenfield, P. M. (2000). . San Francisco: WestEd.

Bridging cultures in our schools: New approaches that work. Knowledge Brief

8. Rothstein-Fisch, C., Trumbull, E., Isaac, A., Daley, C., & Pérez, A. (2003). When “helping someone else” is the right answer. , 3, 123-140.Journal of Latinos and Education

9. Quiroz, B., Greenfield, P. M., & Altchech, M. (1999) Bridging cultures with a parent- teacher conference. . 56, 68-70.

Educational Leadership

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 39

Page 40: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Culture and Policy in Early Childhood DevelopmentSara Harkness, PhD, Charles M. Super, PhD

University of Connecticut, USAJuillet 2010

Introduction

Policies are cultural products. They are generated using concepts shared by members of acultural group and implemented through culturally-based institutions. Their effects play out in thenatural laboratory of everyday life in a particular cultural place. The relationship between cultureand policy in early childhood development is therefore intimate, complex and multi-faceted.Understanding the ways in which culture and policy reflect and influence each other should bepart of the theoretical toolkit of educators, health care providers and policy makers; but in fact,culture and policy are rarely considered in the same context. Examining the cultural context ofpolicy is of particular importance in the current era of rapid culture change and globalization.

Subject

Cultural effects on early childhood development are the focus of a burgeoning research literature;using either culture-specific “emic” constructs or proposed “etic” universal typologies, cross-cultural researchers have sought to describe and understand the ways in which children’s dailyexperiences are culturally shaped. A separate literature has addressed the effects of particularpolicies on children and their families. Nevertheless, there are several ways in which culture andpolicy intersect. Like cultures, policies exist at many levels, from national and internationalorganizations to local groups. Similar to cultural beliefs (“cultural models” or “ethnotheories”),policies also vary in terms of how formalized they are: some can be found in handbooks orlegislation, whereas others are simply shared understandings of what is expected of individuals inparticular circumstances. Policies usually reflect shared values, and in that sense they are part ofa culture - or more particularly the dominant culture in any given social entity. Policies can alsobe instigators of culture change, in which case they may be controversial. Policies are expressedthrough specific programs, just as cultural beliefs are instantiated in practices. Finally, whenpolicies are not consistent with the culture of families or individuals affected by them, they oftendo not work as intended.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 40

Page 41: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

Problems

The most general issue arising from the intersection of these two broad-ranging concepts, as in“culture and policy in early childhood development,” concerns how the actions that follow from aparticular policy fit into and shape – or fail to shape – family decision making and the daily lives ofaffected children in particular cultural contexts. Research on the effects of policies on childoutcomes is typically carried out in a single culture with little attention to mediating mechanisms– that is, to the child and family behaviours that connect the policy actions to developmentalprocesses. These mechanisms, however, involve culturally-organized beliefs, values and customs,leaving the key to policy success in the unexamined “black box” of culture.

Research Context

Ecological frameworks are helpful for understanding the influence of policy on children’sdevelopment. In Bronfenbrenner’s1 classical formulation, the child’s environment consists of aseries of nested “systems” from the most proximal “microsystems” through the intermediary“mesosystems” and “exosystems,” to the overarching “macrosystem.” As Garbarino andcolleagues suggest, recognition that multiple systems link the individual to society isfundamental, because “it focuses attention on the crucial role of policy in stimulating, guiding andenhancing these intermediary systems [the meso- and exosystems] on behalf of more effectiveparenting.2

Weisner’s3 concept of the “ecocultural niche” also considers the child and family as they areaffected by social institutions such as welfare, schools and provisions for the care of children.4,5

This model highlights the central issue of family adaptation, including the family’s ability to buildand sustain culturally-meaningful daily routines. The “developmental niche” frameworkelaborated by Super and Harkness6,7 conceptualizes the child’s environment of daily life asconsisting of three subsystems: the physical and social settings of the child’s daily life; customsand practices of care; and the psychology of the caretakers, especially parental ethnotheoriesconcerning children’s development, parenting, and the family.8 The subsystems interact witheach other, and with the wider culture and characteristics of the individual child. Both the Weisner3 and the Super and Harkness’6,7 frameworks lend themselves readily to the analysis of howpolicies affect the everyday settings of children’s lives and the practices of care they experience.

Key Research Questions

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 41

Page 42: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

From the perspective of these ecological frameworks, four key research questions can be askedin relation to any given policy:

Recent Research Results

Policy-oriented research on young children in the U.S. often describes cultural patterns in theenvironments of young children, but they tend not to be recognized as such. For example, arecent report on “the family dinner table” documents the brevity and infrequency of familymeals, and urges that “Communities should... launch public information campaigns to promotethe importance of family mealtime and work with schools to promote the idea of at least onenight a week when families eat together.”9 An ecological approach would lead one to considersuch questions as how family dinnertime fits into the child’s daily routines, what the importanceof family dinnertime may be for parents, or how features of the larger environment – includingchildren’s extra-curricular activities, parental work schedules and other social priorities – mayaffect family dinnertime as a cultural practice. Another recent policy-oriented report shows thathome visiting programs seem to be more effective for Latino than non-Latino families.10 Thisinteresting finding could be further explored using an ecological approach to learn more aboutthe ways that home visiting may be differentially perceived by various cultural groups.

The growing cultural diversity of children living in the U.S. is frequently cited as a reason forculturally-competent policies and service delivery,11 and there is a growing literature on the needfor early childhood education and care among immigrant groups. Nevertheless, recommendationsoften focus on linguistic rather than cultural obstacles to fuller integration of immigrant children,and their families, into successful partnerships.12 In contrast, Garcia and Jensen13 have proposed a

1. What is the socio-cultural background of the policy? What cultural beliefs – explicit orimplicit – does the policy reflect?

2. Through which specific pathways does a policy influence the family ecology or the child’sdevelopmental niche? Which aspects of family routines and of the niche are affected by newprograms?

3. How can knowledge about the family ecology or the child’s developmental niche be used toassess the likely impact of a new policy across diverse populations?

4. After a policy has been implemented, how can understanding the cultural context of itsapplication help to understand why it has succeeded or failed?

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 42

Page 43: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

multi-dimensional model including culture as well as socio-cultural institutions for understandingLatino children’s involvement in child welfare.

Duncan and colleagues4 provide an example of integrating culture and policy in their study of theimpact of Project Hope, an experimental intervention to help poor working families transition tobetter employment and improved quality of life. The project included providing a package ofbenefits – tailored to each family’s own needs –intended to fill the gaps in family support left bystandard social welfare systems. Using the classic anthropological method of ethnography, theresearchers traced the ups and downs of families over the course of the three-year intervention inorder to understand the pathways leading to success or failure of the program for particularfamilies. They found that it was the families who were neither relatively well-off at the beginningof the project nor truly overwhelmed by multiple challenges - that is, the families in the middle -who benefitted the most from the program. They concluded that these families were successfulbecause they were able to integrate the new services into their existing daily routines. Similar tothis work, some “action research” projects integrate attention to cultural issues withprogrammatic interventions.14,15

Research on children and policies in other countries sheds light on the relationships betweenculture and policy. International variation in childrearing practices and policies can provide awider array of possible options for study than are available in the U.S. (or any other singlenation). For example, policies related to childbirth in the Netherlands include universal availabilityof a postpartum care provider who spends a total of about 80 hours at the home of the newmother during the immediate post-partum period in order to carry out basic health checks onmother and baby, coach the mother in basic infant care, and help with whatever else is neededaround the house. Research on the possible influences of this policy on maternal and child healthand well-being could lead to consideration of similar policies in the U.S.16

Research in other countries has also documented the importance of cultural adaptations of earlyintervention programs.17 For example, the Home Intervention Program for Preschool Youngsters(HIPPY)17 was originally developed in Israel for improving the school readiness of children from low-income families. In Turkey, Kağitçibaşi17 added to the relatively structured cognitive curriculum a“mother enrichment” component that reinforced traditional Turkish values of “relatedness,” andalso introduced a focus on “autonomy,” believed by the authors to be essential for success in therapidly changing modern environment. After four years, the child results replicated advancesnoted in Israel, and the mothers increased their competence and confidence. On the other hand,

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 43

Page 44: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

a Dutch implementation of the program, not so finely tuned to the multiple minority-culturegroups involved, had no overall effects on children and mothers.18

Close attention to the mechanisms of change underlines the importance of cultural sophisticationin instituting policies and programs. For example, a three-year Colombian adaptation of theoriginal Perry Preschool cognitive development curriculum was unexpectedly found to be nearlyas powerful as nutritional supplementation over the same period in reducing long-term growthstunting.19 Post-hoc analysis of this effect, which was not part of the original U.S. project, points toaspects of the developmental niche that mediated the physical growth result. More recently, aSenegalese program successfully improved the school readiness of three-year-olds bydeliberately drawing on local parent beliefs and practices about early learning to promoteparticularly relevant skills.20

These examples illustrate two key points. First, success in early childhood programs is criticallydependent on adapting content and policies to local needs and practices. Second, specificpathways of influence, culturally mediated as they are, may vary in unexpected ways from groupto group.

A final example illustrates a further point: As programs and policies are cultural products, not onlytheir outward gloss, but even their ultimate purpose may be transformed. The television programSesame Street was developed with the specific goal of boosting school readiness among childrenfrom economically deprived households in the U.S. Following its well-documented success,21,22 ithas been replicated in nearly 20 other countries. Most of the adaptations involve obvious changesin language, names, and characters; but as the work went farther afield, especially to high-conflict areas, the focus and orientation of the programs were adapted as well. The Northern IrishSesame Tree, for example, emphasizes cooperation and sharing as much as counting andreading, and the Israeli Rechov Sumsum aims to foster inter-group respect and understanding.

Research Gaps

The importance of “culture,” “cultural competence” or “cultural sensitivity” is often invoked inpolicy discussions without further elaboration on how a cultural perspective could be integratedinto research or policy development. This stems at least in part from the fact that psychologists,who carry out much of the research, are trained to work at the individual level. As Granger notes,“We give an almost automatic nod to the ecology of development, but our models, measurement,

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 44

Page 45: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

and research are uniformly weak at the level of social settings. Because policies are usuallyassumed to influence individuals in ways mediated by settings, this is a major limitation”.23

Conclusions

Policies are cultural productions from their conceptualization through implementation andevaluation, yet this is not commonly recognized in research or public discourse. Globalization andthe increasing cultural diversity of many societies have raised concerns about how to adaptpolicies to a variety of client populations. Ecological frameworks for the study of the child’sCulturally-constructed environment can inform efforts to understand why and how policiessucceed or fail in particular instances. The use of a cultural lens for looking at policies can alsohelp in sorting out distinctions between universally positive aspects of child development, andthose that are simply the current focus in a given society. Likewise, cross-cultural research onpolicies and their effects on child development and families can point to a wider array of policyoptions than are available in one’s own society.

Implications for Parents, Services, and Policy

Parents’ ideas and practices related to child care and development are naturally shaped byculturally constituted “received wisdom.” These assumptions are further embodied in publicpolicies and practices across a wide array of institutions including health, social services andeducation. A greater awareness of cultural variability in parenting practices and developmentalagendas may be liberating for parents within the dominant culture of a society, as well as forimmigrants. Service providers will benefit from cultural awareness that goes beyond learning afew often inaccurate generalizations, to becoming ethnographers of the families they encounter.24

Finally, research on policy related to children should integrate several disciplinary perspectives inorder to match expertise on individual development with knowledge about culture and how tostudy it.

References

1. Bronfenbrenner U. . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1979.The ecology of human development

2. Garbarino J, Vorrasi JA, Kostelny K. Parenting and public policy. In: Bornstein MH, ed. . 2nd ed.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 2002: 504. , vol 5.

Practical issues in parentingHandbook of parenting

3. Weisner TS. Ecocultural understanding of children's developmental pathways. 2002;45(4):275-281.Human Development

4. Duncan GJ, Huston AC, Weisner TS. New York, NY: RussellSage; 2007.

Higher ground: New hope for the working poor and their children.

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 45

Page 46: Culture - Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development

5. Yoshikawa H, Weisner TS, Lowe ED. . New York,NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 2006.

Making it work: Low-wage employment, family life, and child development

6. Super CM, Harkness S. The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. 1986;9:545-569.

InternationalJournal of Behavioral Development

7. Super CM, Harkness S. Culture structures the environment for development. 2002;45(4):270-274.Human Development

8. Harkness S, Super CM, eds. . New York:Guilford; 1996.

Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences

9. The Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD). The Family Dinner Table: Implications for Children’s Health andWellbeing. 2008;22(4):2.Social Policy Report Brief

10. Astuto J, Allen L. Home visitation and young children:  An approach worth investing in?  2009;23(4):1-21.

Social Policy Report

11. Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA. . Washington, DC:National Academy Press; 2000.

From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development

12. Hernandez DJ, Denton NA, Macartney SE. Children in immigrant families:  Looking to America's future. 2008;22(3):1-22.

Social Policy Report

13. Garcia E, Jensen B. Early educational opportunities for children of Hispanic origins. 2009;23(2):1-19.Social Policy Report

14. Cooper CR, Brown J, Azmitia M, Chavira G. Including Latino immigrant families, schools, and community programs asresearch partners on the good path of life (el buen camino de la vida). In: Weisner TS, ed.

. Chicago, IL: University of ChicagoPress; 2005: 359-385.

Discovering successful pathways in children's development: Mixed methods in the study of childhood and family life

15. Harkness S, Hughes M, Muller B, Super CM. Entering the developmental niche: Mixed methods in an intervention programfor inner-city children. In: Weisner TS, ed.

. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2005: 329-358.Discovering successful pathways in children's development: Mixed methods in

the study of childhood and family life

16. Cheng CY, Fowles ER, Walker LO. Postpartum maternal health care in the United States: A critical review. 2006;15(3):34-42.

Journal of Perinatal Education

17. Kağitçibaşi Ç. . 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum; 2007.

Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and Applications

18. Eldering L, Vedder P. The Dutch experience with the Home Intervention Program for Preschool Youngsters (HIPPY). In:Eldering L, Leseman PPM, eds. . New York, NY: Falmer Press; 1999:259-287.

Effective early education: cross-cultural perspectives

19. Super CM, Herrera MG, Mora JO. Long-term effects of food supplementation and psychosocial intervention on the physicalgrowth of Colombian infants at risk of malnutrition. 1990;61(1):29-49.Child Development

20. Super CM, Harkness S, Barry O, Zeitlin M. Think locally, act globally: Contributions of African research to childdevelopment. . In press.Child Development Perspectives

21. Cole CF. What difference does it make?: Insights from research on the impact of international co-productions of SesameStreet. 2009;7:157-177.NHK Broadcasting Studies

22. Fisch S, Truglio R, eds. . Mahwah, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Associates, 2001.

‘G’ is for Growing: Thirty years of research on children and Sesame Street

23. Granger RC. Commentary. 2005;19(4):9.Social Policy Report

24. Harkness S, Keefer CH, Super CM. Culture and ethnicity. In: Levine MD, Carey WB, Crocker AC, eds. . 4th ed. New York, NY: W. B. Saunders; 2009: 182-191.

Developmental-behavioral pediatrics

©2009-2021 CEDJE | CULTURE 46