david bashwiner, university of chicago · david bashwiner, university of chicago it is common to...

21
Friday, May 15 9:15–10:45 SYNTAX,AFFECT, AND COGNITION Chair: Lawrence Zbikowski What is Musical Syntax? An Evolutionary Perspective David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term defined. In its linguistic sense, “syntax” invokes the notion of lexical category (e.g., noun, verb, adjective). Musicologists generally agree that there is no musical parallel to lexical category. Yet “order” (taxis) is clearly important in music—causing the same C major chord, for instance, to sound as a dominant in F, a subdominant in G, and a flat sixth in E. Music is literally, then, “with order.” It is this intuition that leads theorists to claim—correctly, I believe—that musical utterances are syntactic. Nevertheless, without a proper understanding of what “syntax” is, describing music as such carries little theoretical weight. The comparison with language is problematic largely because of the fact that, in both music and language, syntax is already speciated. What is needed is a general theory against which to compare both. I argue that such a general understanding can be arrived at by means of (a) a comparison of the syntactic behaviors used communicatively throughout the animal kingdom, and (b) a consideration of how syntacticity functions within those communication systems. Much communication, human or otherwise, is nonsyntactic (e.g., crying, cooing, gesturing, laughing). Syntacticity affords a certain set of benefits above and beyond those afforded by nonsyntactic communication, but evolutionary pressures would presumably be required to bring them about. Evolutionary theory thus offers one way of accounting for what musical syntacticity is, how it functions, and why it emerged. Theorizing Musical Affect: Arvo Pärt’s Passio (1982) Arnie Cox, Oberlin Conservatory This paper theorizes the role of mimetic motor imagery (MMI) in the generation of musical affect. MMI is imagery related to muscle activity performed in imitation of some seen and/or heard action. It occurs most often in contexts of observing other humans in real life, athletics, and the performing arts. While it occasionally manifests in overt mimetic behavior, as when we sing along with or dance to music, MMI most often remains covert and nonconscious. MMI is a form of physical empathy that contributes a physical feeling to musical experience. This feeling takes the form of partly reinstated body states, which theoretically involve not only activation of skeletal‐motor systems but also associated systems that contribute to affect. Excerpts from Pärt’s Passio demonstrate the relation between the acoustic fact (objective/structural features), mimetic engagement, and affective (subjective) experience.

Upload: others

Post on 13-Mar-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

Friday,May159:15–10:45 SYNTAX,AFFECT,ANDCOGNITION

Chair:LawrenceZbikowski

WhatisMusicalSyntax?AnEvolutionaryPerspectiveDavidBashwiner,UniversityofChicago

Itiscommontospeakoftherebeinga“syntax”inmusic,butmuchless

commonlyisthistermdefined.Initslinguisticsense,“syntax”invokesthenotionoflexicalcategory(e.g.,noun,verb,adjective).Musicologistsgenerallyagreethatthereisnomusicalparalleltolexicalcategory.Yet“order”(taxis)isclearlyimportantinmusic—causingthesameCmajorchord,forinstance,tosoundasadominantinF,asubdominantinG,andaflatsixthinE.Musicisliterally,then,“withorder.”Itisthisintuitionthatleadstheoriststoclaim—correctly,Ibelieve—thatmusicalutterancesaresyntactic.Nevertheless,withoutaproperunderstandingofwhat“syntax”is,describingmusicassuchcarrieslittletheoreticalweight.

Thecomparisonwithlanguageisproblematiclargelybecauseofthefactthat,inbothmusicandlanguage,syntaxisalreadyspeciated.Whatisneededisageneraltheoryagainstwhichtocompareboth.Iarguethatsuchageneralunderstandingcanbearrivedatbymeansof(a)acomparisonofthesyntacticbehaviorsusedcommunicativelythroughouttheanimalkingdom,and(b)aconsiderationofhowsyntacticityfunctionswithinthosecommunicationsystems.Muchcommunication,humanorotherwise,isnonsyntactic(e.g.,crying,cooing,gesturing,laughing).Syntacticityaffordsacertainsetofbenefitsaboveandbeyondthoseaffordedbynonsyntacticcommunication,butevolutionarypressureswouldpresumablyberequiredtobringthemabout.Evolutionarytheorythusoffersonewayofaccountingforwhatmusicalsyntacticityis,howitfunctions,andwhyitemerged.

TheorizingMusicalAffect:ArvoPärt’sPassio(1982)ArnieCox,OberlinConservatory

Thispapertheorizestheroleofmimeticmotorimagery(MMI)inthegenerationofmusicalaffect.MMIisimageryrelatedtomuscleactivityperformedinimitationofsomeseenand/orheardaction.Itoccursmostoftenincontextsofobservingotherhumansinreallife,athletics,andtheperformingarts.Whileitoccasionallymanifestsinovertmimeticbehavior,aswhenwesingalongwithordancetomusic,MMImostoftenremainscovertandnonconscious.MMIisaformofphysicalempathythatcontributesaphysicalfeelingtomusicalexperience.Thisfeelingtakestheformofpartlyreinstatedbodystates,whichtheoreticallyinvolvenotonlyactivationofskeletal‐motorsystemsbutalsoassociatedsystemsthatcontributetoaffect.ExcerptsfromPärt’sPassiodemonstratetherelationbetweentheacousticfact(objective/structuralfeatures),mimeticengagement,andaffective(subjective)experience.

Page 2: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

Theanalyticalapproachtakesmusicalmeaningtoemergeinsubjectiveandintersubjectiveresponsestoobjectivefeatures,wheremeaningismotivatedbyandultimatelygroundedinembodiedexperience.MMIispresentedhereasoneofeightfactorscontributingtomusicalaffect.Amongtheimplications,thisapproachfleshesouttherolesofexpectationandanticipationbycharacterizinganticipatedeventsasanticipatedactions,performedbyperformersandmimeticallyperformedbylisteners.

TemporalSegmentationandPrototypicalPhraseModelsMitchOhriner,IndianaUniversity

Theliteratureoncategorizationpresentstwoviewsofcategoryformation,

theclassicalandtheprototypical.Intheclassicalview,elementsbelongtocategoriesbyvirtueofmeetingasetofnecessaryandsufficientconditions.Intheprototypicalconception,objectscanresembletheprototypicalinstanceofacategory,orevenmultiplecategories,andhencetheboundariesofcategoriesarefuzzy.

IntheworkofSchoenbergandCaplin,musicalphrasesarecategorizedusingtheclassicalmodel.Yetmanyphrasesdonotfittheconditionsoftraditionalcategories.Inaprototypicalconceptionofphrasestructure,allphrasescouldbeprobabilisticallyplacedinallcategories,butinorderforsuchaconceptiontobeadoptedthecriteriausedincategoryformationmustbeuniformandmeasurable.Thispaperproposesamethodofusingexpressivetiminginperformancetodeterminea“temporalpartitioning”ofapiecethatcanthenbefittoasmallnumberofphrasecategories.Thegoodnessoffitbetweenanumberofphrasesandasinglefittingmodelcreatesaspectrumofassociationanalogoustoaprototypicalconceptionofphrasecategorization.

Oftentheresultsofthemethodmatchourintuitionsofphrasestructure,butjustasoftentheydonot.InademonstrationusingtwoofChopin’sMazurkas,associationsbetweenphrasesdissimilarinmanyrespectsarerevealed.Inthelargercontextofmusicalscholarship,themethodhastheadvantageofprioritizinginterpretationinanalysisandwherepossiblegivingthehighestprioritytothemostwidelyheldinterpretationsofmusicalstructure.

Page 3: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

9:15–10:45 MOTIVEANDPROCESS Chair:HaliFeldman(yettoconfirm)

MiddlegroundMotivesinBeethoven’sLastQuartet(Opus135)JasonBritton,LutherCollege

MostofthepublishedanalyticalcommentariesonBeethoven’slastcomplete

composition,theStringQuartetinFmajor,op.135,approachtheworkmoreorlessexclusivelyfromamotivicstandpoint.Thisisnotsurprising,giventhecuriousmotivicepigraphBeethovenattachedtothequartet’sfinalmovement.ArnoldSchoenberg(1941),RudolfRéti(1951),DeryckCooke(1963),andChristopherReynolds(1988)haveallpraisedop.135foritshighlyunifiedmotivicstructure.Butwhatarewetodowhenthedetailsofonemotivicreadingopposesanother?Orwhatifaparticularreadingcontradictsthewaywemightunderstandapassageaurally(aswhentheproposedreadingconflictsthemusic’sharmonic‐contrapuntalstructure)?Whatcriteriashouldweusetoevaluateamotivicanalysis?Clearly,weneedastrategythatwillhelpanswerthesequestionsandadvanceusbeyondmereintuition.ThepositiontakenhereechoesJohnRothgeb’sargumentthat“proposedthematicrelationshipsmustbearscrutinyinthelightoftheSchenkeriantheoryofstructuralstrata”(1983,42).ASchenkerianapproachisadoptedtohelpassessexistingmotivicreadingsofop.135withintherequisitecontextsofharmonyandvoiceleading.Themethodisthenusedtogenerateamethodologicallyconsistent,hearableanalysisthatrevealsmotivicrelationshipsthatresideatdeeper,hiddenlevelsofstructure.TheMythofModulation:MotivicUnityAcrossSurface‐LevelKeysinEinHeldenleben

SooHyunJeong,UniversityofMinnesota–TwinCities

RichardStrauss’sEinHeldenlebenpresentsadramaticmusicalprogramwhoseexcitementrestsinnosmallpartonrapid,chromatickeychanges.Despitethefrequentandvariedtypesofmodulation,wecanstilldetectanunderlyingmonotonallogictothisdizzyingarrayofkeys.Behindtheseeminglyunpredictablekeychangesliesasenseofcontinuitygrantedbyanunderlying ^5–^6–^5or̂5–ß^6–^5skeletalmelodicgesture.TheusageofthisgestureallowedStrausstoachievecoherenceinEinHeldenlebenbyrepeatingthesameharmonicidiomsonboththemusicalsurfaceandonhierarchicallydeeperlevels,atechniquehealsoappliedwithsomesuccessinlaterworks.Analytically,ourrecognitionofthisrecurringgesturehelpsustoexplainhowStraussexpressivelyunitesavarietyofharmonicandthematicelementsthroughoutonecomposition.

Tosupportthisclaim,mystudycomprisestheanalysisofavarietyofbothharmonicprogressionsandmodulations.Itakeintoconsiderationsurface‐levelharmonicprogressionsaswellasdeeper‐levelrelationshipsbetweenlocalkeyareas,showinghowamonotonalinterpretationofthemelodicscaledegreesprovidesasenseofunity.MyanalyticalapproachisinfluencedheavilybyDaniel

Page 4: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

Harrison’sthoughtsonmodulation.Harrison’sanalysiscreatesachronologicalkeytravelogueofEinHeldenleben,usingtheRiemannianTonnetzasasortoftonalmap.Thestrengthofthisanalysisisthatonemayclearlyrecognizeboththevarietyoftheharmonicmotionsandtheentireconceptualgroupofkeysappliedthroughoutthepiece.MyapproachdiffersfromHarrison’s,however,inthatratherthanrelyingonRiemannianconceptionsoftonalityandmodulation,IuseSchenkerianconceptsandmethodology,anapproachmoreappropriatefordistinguishinghierarchicallevelsoftonalstructure.ThiskindofanalysiswilleffectivelyshowthatnewkeyrelationsinEinHeldenlebenfollowfromtherecursivecounterpointofthecentral̂5–(ß)̂6–^5gesture.Moreover,myanalysesillustratehowtheskeletalthematicmotionscontinuouslyseepupfromthebackground,eventuallysaturatingtheforeground.

VoiceLeadingandEmergingConsciousnessinaSchoenbergPassageChristopherM.Barry,UniversityofWisconsin–Madison

AremarkablepassageinthesixthsongofArnoldSchoenberg’sDasBuchderhängendenGärten,Op.15(1908–09),“Jedemwerkebinichfürdertot,”formsanelegantparallelofemerginghumanconsciousnessthroughthesingulardetailsofitsvoiceleading.Thepassagecrystallizesinafleetingmomentcoincidingwiththesinger’senunciationoftheverb“weinen”(toweep).Theharmonicsimultaneityofthismomentisstrikingincomparisontoitspredecessors.ThoughitputativelysimulatesanEmajortriad,thereisnothing“E‐ful”abouttheWeinenklang:itisagroundless,harmonicallyimpoverishedproductofadistinctlynon‐tonalvoice‐leadingprocess.

Thispaperexpandsonandformalizesthevoice‐leadingprocessinthisextraordinarypassagewithreferencetoremarksfromSchoenberg’sHarmonielehre(1911)andRoeder’s(1989)commentaryonthoseremarks.Itheninterpretthevoice‐leadingprocesswithintheframeworkofahumansubjectemergingintoconsciousness,aframeworkbasedonthemusic‐theoreticalworkofMichaelCherlin(1993)andtheneuroscientificworkofAntonioDamasio(1999).Forthissong,aspecificallyFreudianinterpretivemodelparallelstheprocessiveformationofatotalistic,consolidatedsonoritythroughvoiceleading.TheWeinenklangdisruptsthepoeticmeterandsensualproclamationsofsexualdesire—actionsconsistentwithanegoistic,repressiveagent.Thecoincidenceofthevoice‐leadingprocess’stermination,theremarkableWeinenklang,textualdisruption,andtheactofweepingisnothappenstance:togethertheyilluminatethehumansubjectofthesong,aconsciousnessemergingthroughemotion,throughwhatDamasiomightcallthe“feelingofafeeling.”

Page 5: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

11:00–12:30 BRAHMSANDBEACH Chair:FrankSamarotto(yettoconfirm)

BrahmsoderDietrich?AnAnalyticPerspectiveonthePianoTrioinAMajorRyanMcClelland,UniversityofToronto

DebatecontinuesovertheauthenticityofapianotrioinAmajordiscoveredin1924byErnstBückenandpublishedasaworkofBrahmsin1938byBreitkopf&Härtel.Thescore’sprefaceclarifiesthattheattributionrestedentirelyon“internalstylisticevidence”;themanuscriptwasinanunknownhand(presumablyacopyist’s),thetitle‐pagewasmissing,andpaperstudiesgavenoprofitableinformation.Themanuscripthassincevanished.

SeveralauthorshaveofferedopinionsonwhetherBrahmscomposedthistrioandwhenhemighthavedoneso.WhileearlywriterssuchasBücken,FriedrichBrand,andRichardFellinger,supportedaBrahmsattribution(c.1853–54),nearlyallrecentwriters—includingGeorgeBozarth,DavidBrodbeck,andMichaelMusgrave—havequestionedthis.Mostoffernoalternative,butBrodbeckproposesAlbertDietrich.TheonlyrecentauthorfullyinaccordwiththeBrahmsattributionisMalcolmMacDonald,buthesuggestsadateof1856. Thispaperviewsthetriofromananalyticperspective,somethingthatmanyoftheaforementionedwritershavesignaledasnecessarytoadvancetheauthorshipdebate.SinceDietrich’snamehasrepeatedlysurfacedindiscussionsofthistrio,thisinvestigationinvolvesbothearlyworksofBrahmsandtheinstrumentalmusicofDietrich.WhilenoanalyticinquirycanprovidecompletelyconclusiveproofofBrahms’sauthorship,thispaperdemonstratesthatthetrioisveryconsistentwithBrahms’searlystyle(c.1852–53)andinconsistentwithDietrich’scompositionallanguage.“ShallIFindAughtNew?”TheExpressivePotentialofModifiedStrophicForminthe

SongsofAmyBeachVictoriaMalawey,KenyonCollege

DespitetheseeminglyuncomplicatedsurfaceofsomeofAmyBeach’ssongs,

manycomprisesophisticatedharmonicandformalstructures.ComposedinthelateRomantictonalstyleoverherentirecareer,manyofhersongsfeatureunusualformaldesignssituatedsomewherebetweenmodifiedstrophicandthrough‐composedform.ThisstudyexaminesformalmodificationsinfivesongsbyBeachpublishedoveraspanof25years,andseekstoanswerthefollowingquestions:Inwhatwaysdostrophicmodificationsaffectlisteners’perceptionsofformandtextualparallels?Towhatextentdoesthedegreeofmodificationimpactperceptionsofnewmaterialorvariationofpreviousmaterial(restatement)?Finally,howdoBeach’sformalmodificationsimpacttherangeofexpressivityinthesongtexts?

Thefocalexampleofthispresentation,“Ah,Love,butaDay”(op.44,no.2),

Page 6: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

demonstratesacomplexmanifestationofstrophicprinciplesbecausetheelementsthatrecallsimilaritycontinuallyshift.Differentlisteningstrategiescanallowlistenerstoexperiencethesongaseithermodifiedstrophicorthroughcomposed.Moststrophicqualitiesemergeintheareaofmelody,whereacombinationofscale‐degreefunctionandabsolutepitchesarekeptintactbetweenstrophes.

ComparisonsofBeach’sscoreswithanalyticre‐compositionsillustratetheproductivegapbetweenstrictlystrophicdesignsinthelatterandheavilymodifiedstrophicformsintheformer.Beachusessuchmodifieddesignsfordramaticeffect,oftenenhancingemotiveaspectsofthesongtexts.Differentcombinationsofelementsdelineatingchangecorrespondwithdifferentemotiveeffectsinthesongs.

“YouKissMeasWePart”:UnifyingTechniquesinTwoBrahmsSongPairingsDanielB.Stevens,UniversityofDelaware

Whilethematicconnectionsareoftenunderstoodasbindingmusical

passagestogether,couldtheyalsothreatentocollapsethedistinctionbetweentwoseparatepieces?OneofthemostunusualfeaturesfoundinBrahms’s“Liederstrauβe”(song‐bouquets)aretheinstancesofadjacentsongsthatemployvirtuallyidenticalthematicmaterial.Unlikesongcyclesinwhichthematicrecallsoccurafterinterveninglieder(suchasBeethoven’sAndieFerneGeliebteorSchumann’sDichterliebe),thesesongpairingsoccursequentially,andtheirthematicrepetitionblurstheboundariesofthesesongs,effectivelyerasingthesilencebetweenthem.Whilerecentcommentatorshaveacknowledgedthecomplementarynatureofthesepairings,thesesongsalsoraiseissuesinvolvingmusicalunity,therelationoftextandmusic,andthecontributionofperformancetotheidentificationofmusicalworksaswholes. Focusingontwosongpairsfromtheop.19and85collections,IemploySchenkerian,formal,andtextualanalysistosuggestthatthesesongs,farfromexhibitingmundanerepetitions,ratherrepresenthighlycalculatedattemptsbyBrahmstorethinkhowmusicandtextmaybeinterrelatedacrossthedoublebar.Bycontrastingtheunifyingtechniquesemployedinthesetwopairs,IarguenotonlythatBrahmswascreativelyengagedintheproblem—andpotential—ofthematicrepetitionbutalsothatthesignificanceofhiscompositionalsolutionshingesupontheiractualizationthroughperformance.

Page 7: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

11:00–12:30 ELUSIVEASPECTSOFPITCH:TIMBREANDSPRECHSTIMME Chair:SherstenJohnson

OppositionsandMetamorphosis:TimbreinSaariaho’sChâteaudel'âmeCrystalPeebles,FloridaStateUniversity

Inwritingaboutherworks,KaijaSaariahohasemphasizedtheroleoftimbre

asastructuraldeterminant.Shemapscertaintimbresontothecommonoppositionbetweenconsonanceanddissonance,withastatedgoalofcreatingahierarchyoftimbre.ThroughthelensofthisviewoftimbreandSaariaho’sideasaboutinterpolation,ahierarchyoftimbralgesturesarisesinherChâteaudel'âme,apieceforsolosoprano,woman’schorus,andorchestra.Timbreisusednotonlytodefinepitch‐specificgestures,buttocreateanoveralltrajectoryofthefirstmovementofthiswork.Throughaninterpretivegraphthatchartsthechangeoftimbralconsonancethroughtime,thistalkexplorestextualmeaninganditsinteractionswithtimbre.

FunctionalandDescriptiveAccountsofTimbreWilliamO’Hara,UniversityofWisconsin–Madison

InthediscussionofKlangfarbenmelodiethatcloseshisHarmonielehre

(1911),ArnoldSchoenbergwaxespoeticaboutthesublimitythatharnessingandsystematizingtimbrewouldbringtomusic,sayingthatitcould“bringusclosertotheillusorystuffofourdreams.”However,explorationsoftimbre’sorganizationalandexpressivepotentialarerareinsubsequentmusicalliterature.Thiscould,perhaps,beattributedtothelackofasystematictheorywithwhichtodescribethephenomenonoftimbreinmusic.Whileitisdifficulttodenytimbre’sexpressivepower,itisevenmoredifficulttoquantifyandexplainthemodesthroughwhichthispoweroperates.Severalobstacleshamperourunderstandingoftimbre.Itisimpreciselydefinedandeludesmeasurementonanyexistingspectrum,makingcomparisonsdifficult.Thegreatdiversityofpossibletimbresandtheextra‐musicalassociationstheyoftencarrypresentadauntingmountainofdatathroughwhichtosift.

Thispaperarguesthatexistingconceptionsoftimbrecanbesplitintotwocategories:descriptiveandfunctional.Descriptiveaccountsarefoundinmusicalwritingsofallkinds,fromorchestrationtreatisestomusicreviews.Functionalaccounts,suchasthoseundertakenbyWessel(1979)andLerdahl(1987),seektodescribetimbreasanelementthatcanbevariedinmusicallysignificantways,throughoperationssuchasintensificationandcombination.Bothhavetheirplaceinmusicologicalscholarship.Thispaperpositsthatadvancingourunderstandingoftimbrewillrequireresearcherstocombinethesetwoapproaches,andsuggestspossibleavenuesforsuchcombination.

Page 8: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

RapandSprechstimme:AnalyzingthePitchContentofHip‐HopChristopherSegall,CityUniversityofNewYork

Thereisalongstandingassumptionthatpitchplaysnoroleinrapmusic.

Existingscholarlytranscriptionsrenderrappedlinesinrhythmicnotationonly,implyingthatanypitchvariationismerelyincidental.Bycontrast,Icontendthatthewayrappedlinesarepitchedconstitutesanimportantcomponentofmanysongs.Furthermore,Iproposeusingapitch‐sensitivenotation,basedonArnoldSchoenberg’sSprechstimme,inraptranscription.

Existingtranscriptionshaveusedoneoftwomethodsfornotatingrapping.Inthefirst,Westernclassicalnotationconveysrhythmsonanunheightedstaff;thesecond,developedbyAdamKrims,conveysrhythmswithXsplacedinametricalgrid.Neithermethodaccountsfortheroleplayedbypitch.Everysyllableofspeech,whetherspokenorrapped,hassomepitch,evenifitisnotsustainedasinsinging.Sprechstimmenotation,designedforvocalistswhomustpitchtheirspokenvoices,canapproximatethepitchesofrappedsyllablesintranscription.

Inthispaper,Iillustratehowpitchfunctionsinrapmusic.Ontheonehand,pitchcontributestotheformalorganizationofsongs,forinstanceallowingrepetitionsofchorusestosoundalike.Ontheother,pitchaccentplaysasignificantroleinwhatKrimscalls“percussion‐effusiveflow,”astyleofdeliverycharacterizedbyinternalrhymeandpolyrhythm.Pitchisusedtoreinforcetheserhythms.Ultimately,Iconcludethatpitchplaysagreaterroleinrapmusicthanpreviouslythought.

Page 9: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

2:00–5:15 POSTERSESSION:PEDAGOGY IncorporatingKeyboard‐BasedMasteryLearningintotheMusicTheoryCurriculum:

TwoStudiesLeighVanHandel,MichiganStateUniversity

Thisposterreportstheresultsoftwostudiesthatimplementedamastery‐

learningmodelofkeyboardinstructionintoafreshman‐levelmusictheorycoursetodeterminewhetherthekinestheticactivityofproducingharmonicprogressionsatthekeyboardhelpsstudentsimprovetheirwrittenmusictheoryskills.Thehypothesiswasthatifthekinestheticreinforcementofthekeyboardmoduleshadapositiveeffectonparticipants’understandingofthematerial,theparticipantsmayhaveseenanincreaseinperformanceonregularcourseassessmentswhencomparedtothecontrolgroupmembers.

Apilotstudyindicatedthatstudentswhowereatriskinthecourse(thoseperformingat75%orbelow)benefitedthemostfromtheinclusionofthekeyboardmodules.Infact,theperformanceofthelow‐achievingparticipantsbecamestatisticallyindistinguishablefromtheperformanceofthehigherachievingcontrolgroupmembers.ThisfollowsthefindingsofKulik,Kulik,andBangert‐Drowns(1990),whofoundthatthelowestlevelofstudentisthemostlikelytobenefitfromamastery‐learningapproach.

Theexperimentdesignalsotestedwhetherstudentsretainedthebenefitfromthemastery‐learningkeyboardmodules;resultsindicatethattheat‐riskparticipantgroupcontinuedtooutperformtheircontrolgroupcounterpartsevenafterthetwounitsthatincludedthemodules.

Theposterwillpresentresultsfromthepilotstudyandasecondstudy,runduringSpringsemester2009,andwillprovidesuggestionsforincorporatingkeyboard‐orientedlearningintoexistingcurriculathatdonothaveanintegratedkeyboardskillscourse.

BarbershopQuartetsintheSightSingingClassBruceTaggart,MichiganStateUniversity

Teachingsightsingingisanimportantassignmentgiventothe

undergraduateauralskillsteacher.Musiciansshouldbeabletosingcomfortablyatsightanymelodytheyencounterinaperformance,teaching,oracademicsetting.Butaswithallskills,learningtosingatsightrequirestimeandextensivepractice.

Thisposterpresentstheuseofbarbershopquartetsasateachingtoolinthesecond‐yearauralskillsclass.Forthepastfiveyears,thefourthsemesterclassinafour‐semestersequenceintheCollegeofMusicatMichiganStateUniversityhasusedthequartetcollectionBarbershopJubilee(arr.CharlesBoutelle)asarequiredtext.

Barbershopsingingoffersanincreasedawarenessofsingingmechanics,afirstopportunityformanytosinginachoralsetting,lightlychromaticharmonies

Page 10: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

thatprovideopportunitiestodrillbasicchordlabelingskillsandapplyconceptsfromthewrittentheoryclass,and,notleast,ahugelyenjoyablesingingactivitythatcanserveasagreatmotivatorinwhatcanbeafrustratingandlessthanexcitingclass.

Severalquartetsfromthecollectionarediscussed,lookingatproblemsinperformance,andofferingwaysofmakingconnectionswithconceptsfromwrittentheory,includingchromaticharmony,harmonicprolongation,modulation,andthephrasemodel.

Theuseofbarbershopquartetscanbeaneffectiveteachingtool,improvingbasicsingingandsightreadingskills,strengtheninganalyticalskills,andgivingstudentsachancetosinginachoralsetting,perhapsforthefirsttime.2:00–4:00 JAZZPLUS

Chair:RamonSatyendra

EllingtontheImproviser:GroupInteractionintheMoneyJungleRecordingsGarrettMichaelsen,IndianaUniversity

ThispaperwillfocusonDukeEllingtonasajazzimproviserthroughthe

investigationofoneparticularlyintriguingrecordingsession.His1962MoneyJunglerecordingdatebroughttogetherthediverseandindividualistictalentsofEllington,CharlesMingus,andMaxRoachforasessionthatwasatturnsbrilliantandbewildering.Ellingtonwrotethatrecordinghiscomposition“FleuretteAfricaine”was“wasoneofthosemysticmomentswhenourmuseswereoneandthesame.”However,othercutssuchasthetitletrack“MoneyJungle”displaypalpablemusicalfrictionbetweentheperformers.Throughdetailedtranscriptionsofthesetworecordedperformances,Iwilldrawintotheanalyticalpicturetheelementofmusicalinteractionthatissocompellinginthissession.IdevelopaninteractiveanalyticalframeworkthatdrawsontheworkofscholarssuchasRobertHodson,IngridMonson,JeffPressing,EdSarath,R.KeithSawyer,andPaulSteinbeck.ThroughthisanalyticalframeworkIcharacterizeinteractiveconnectionsaseitherassociativeorinterruptivecontinuationsofprecedingimprovisedutterances.Additionally,Idescribeaspectrumofinteractiveconvergenceanddivergence,wherebothpolesfunctionasunstableboundariesbetweenwhichmostimprovisedinteractionoccurs.Theanalysesof“Fleurette”and“MoneyJungle”willidentifyconvergentanddivergentelementsofboth,paintingthetotalinteractivepicturenotinblackorwhitebutinshadesofgray.

Page 11: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

MetricalDissonanceinBillEvans’s“AlltheThingsYouAre”StefanLove,EastmanSchoolofMusic

BillEvans’ssolorecordingsrevealakeenabilitytodisruptthemeter.He

frequentlysuperimposesconflictingmetricallayers(groupingdissonance)orshiftstheapparentdownbeat(displacementdissonance).Thelackofensemblesupportmakestheseeffectsallthemoredramatic.Inthispresentation,IdiscussEvans’versionof“AlltheThingsYouAre”fromthealbumAlone,withtheaimofansweringthefollowingtwoquestions:1)WhattypesofdissonancedoesEvansemploy?2)Howdoesheprepare,sustain,andresolvethesedissonances?

Whileseveraltheoristshaveanalyzedmetricaldissonanceinjazz(e.g.Larson2006,Downs2002,Waters1997),priordiscussionofdisplacementhasbeenlimitedtothedisplacementofmotives.Yet“AlltheThingsYouAre”includessomeastonishingexamplesofphraseandsubphrasedisplacement.Toclassifytheseandothertechniques,IborrowHaraldKrebs’snotationformetricaldissonance,aspresentedinhisbookFantasyPieces(1999).Usinganadaptationofhisapproach,IdescribeEvans’techniquesindetail.Thisdescriptionopensavenuesforfurtherresearchonmetricaldissonanceinjazz.

OrderedStepMotivesinJazzCompositionKeithSalley,ShenandoahConservatory

Whenanalyzingjazzcompositionsthatareeitherharmonicallycapriciousor

tonallyambiguous,thetaskofdeterminingpointsofarrivalanddepartureandtracingthelinearpatternsthatconnectthemcanbecomedifficult.Thisstudyinvestigatestheextentstowhichsuchcapriciousorambiguousaspectsofjazzcompositionsaremelodicallydriven.WhileIdonotwishtoinvokeintentionality,Idowishtoexplorehowmelodiesmakesenseofsomeunusualturnsofharmony,tonality,andevenform.

Thefocusofthisstudyisthemelodicmotive.ItusesatoolcalledtheOrderedStepMotive(OSM)toinvestigatethewaylinearmotivesgiveshapetojazzcompositionsthathavefrequentlychangingtonalcenters,nonfunctionalchordconnections,noclearglobaltonics,orstructurallyopen,circularforms.ThisstudyfeaturesanalysesofDaveBrubeck’s“It’saRaggyWaltz,”and“InYourOwnSweetWay,”DizzieGillespie’s“ConAlma,”BennyGolson’s“Stablemates,”HoraceSilver’s“Peace,”andWayneShorter’s“Nefertiti.”Thisstudyinvitesfurtherresearchintotherelationshipbetweencommonriffsandunderlyingstructureinjazzcomposition,whichmayrevealcrucialdifferencesbetweenstandardswrittenbyBroadwayandTin‐Pan‐Alleycomposersandthosewrittenbypracticingjazzmusicians.

Page 12: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

A"New"LydianTheoryforFrankZappa'sDiatonicMusicBrettClement,UniversityofCincinnatiCollege­ConservatoryofMusic

ThoughmuchofFrankZappa’smusichasbeencalled“tonal”duetoitsfundamentaldiatonicism,reconcilingthisrepertoirewithfamiliartheoriesoftonalityisfraughtwithdifficulties.Inthispaper,IwillarguethatanunderstandingofZappa’sdiatonicmusicisbestachievedinreferencetotheLydianscale.ThetheorypresentedislooselyadaptedfromGeorgeRussell’sinfluentialjazztheory“TheLydianChromaticConcept.”ItintroducestheconceptofaLydiansystem,containingalimitedgroupofdiatonicmodesgeneratedfromacommonLydianscale.Withinthesystem,theLydianmode,duetoitsspecialstaticproperties,functionsasa“tonic”state,representativeofmajortonality.

TheremainingmodesoftheLydiansystemwillbeshowntointeractwiththeLydiantonicinvariouswaysinZappa’smusic.First,Ishalldefinethreetexturallystratified“zonesofmusicalactivity”intheseworks—melodic,chordal,andpedal—andwilldemonstratehowthesethreezonesplayimportantrolesinachievingmodalarticulation,whilealsoremainingindebtedtotheLydiantonicintheirpitchstructures.Second,IwillexamineZappa’spracticeofpedalsubstitution,aprocesswherebythesamediatonicmelodyisset,atvarioustimesinapiece,todifferentpedalsoftheLydiansystem.Third,IwilldescribecertainprogressionsthatmayoccurwithinaLydiansystem.Thepresentationwillconcludewithananalysisofthesong“Andy,”whichwilldemonstratetheintegrationofseveralcloselyrelatedLydiansystemsacrossacompletework.3:00–4:00 MODESANDTRANSFORMATIONSINBRITISHMUSIC Chair:JulianHook

TheModalOrganizationofVaughanWilliams’WorksIanBates,YaleUniversity

Thispaperinvestigatesthelarge‐scalemodalorganizationofVaughan

Williams’works.Itbeginsbyclassifyingthevarioustypesofmodalrelationshipsfoundinhisworksanddevisesastandardvocabularyfordiscussingtheserelationships.Itintroducestheconceptofthemodaltrajectory,adirectedrelationshipbetweentwoormoremodesthatunfoldschronologicallyoveramusicalspan.Modaltrajectoriesarethendividedintotwobroadcategories:thoseinwhichthevalueofonemodaldomain(pccontent,modetype,orcentricity)remainsfixedandthoseinwhichthevaluesofallthreedomainsarevaried.Thesearedubbedfixed­andvariable­domaintrajectories,respectively.Atthesametime,itdevelopsamethodofgraphicallyrepresentingawork’smodalorganizationforanalyticalpurposes.Avisualrepresentationofdomainrelationsamongallthediatonicmodesisdevised,andthisTableofModalRelationsisthenusedtorelatemodaltrajectoriestooneanotherviashareddomainconflictstoformtrajectory

Page 13: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

systems.Finally,thepaperproposesinterrelatedtrajectorysystemsasthestandardmodelformodalorganizationinthecomposer’sworksandshowshowapiece’strajectorystructuretypicallyrelatestoitsformalstructure.

FormandTransformationinthe"Nocturne"fromBritten'sSerenadeforTenor,

Horn,andStringsMichaelBaker,UniversityofKentucky

OfthemanywritingsonthemusicofBenjaminBritten(1913–1976),most

areconcernedwithhisnumerousoperas,includingPeterGrimes,BillyBudd,DeathinVenice,andTheTurnoftheScrew.WhilediscussionofBritten’smusicfocusesheavilyontheoperas,comparativelylittleiswrittenontheanalysisandinterpretationofthesongcyclesforvoiceandorchestra,whichincludestheSerenadeforTenor,HornandStrings,oneofBritten’smostbelovedandoftenperformedworks.

The“Nocturne”fromBenjaminBritten’sSerenadeforTenor,HornandStringspresentsanumberofinterestingmelodicandmotivicdevicesthatcanbeeffectivelymodeledbyaspectsofdiatonictransformationaltheories.Afterreviewingapairofimportanttransformationaloperations,Iwillpresentananalysisofthe“Nocturne”thatdrawsuponbothtraditionalandrecentdevelopmentsintransformationaltheory.Iwillthenexaminethedynamicrelationshipoftheseoperationswithimportantaspectsofthetextforthissong,illustratingthatthetransformationalpathBrittentraversedinthisworkechoestheoverallmeaningofTennyson’spoem.4:15–5:15 BALKANMETERS

Chair:RonaldRodman

ProblemswithUnequalBeats:ADiscussionofNon‐IsochronousMeterinBalkanBrassBandMusic

DanielGoldberg,UniversityofBritishColumbia

Innon‐isochronousmeter,categoricallydifferentdurationsbetweensuccessivebeatscoexistwithineachmeasure,allowingforahierarchicalmetricalframeworktoaccommodatetheaksak(“irregular”)rhythmicperiodsthatoccurinmanynon‐Westerncontexts.OnesuchcontextistherepertoryofatypeofbrassbandpopularintheBalkanregionofsoutheasternEurope,inwhichtheaccentualpatternsofaccompanimentalostinatosgrouprepeatingseriesofpulsesintounequalunits,suchasthe2‐2‐2‐3groupingofanine‐pulseperiod.ThispresentationexaminestranscriptionsofostinatosfromrecordingsofBalkanbrassbandsandtheirAmerican“Balkanite”counterpartsinordertoexplorehownon‐isochronousmetermightbetheorizedinrelationtotraditionalWesternmeter.

Page 14: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

Threespecialtheoreticalproblemsoftherepertoryareaddressed.First,thechanginginteronsetintervalsbetweenbeatsinnon‐isochronousmeterseeminglyworkagainstmetricalstability,andcharacteristicsoftheaccompanimentalpatternslikefasttemposandcompletematerializationofthepulsecanbeseentocounteractthisproblem.Second,comparisonofnon‐isochronousmetricalcycleswithsimilarisochronouscyclesillustratesadistinctionbetweenpulse‐andbeat‐orientedapproachestodefiningandclassifyingnon‐isochronousmeter.Finally,intheinterpretationofanostinatofromarecordingofthepiece“Nasredsela,”acontradictionbetweenthemeterindicatedbygestaltprinciplesfortheperceptionofaccentandthemeterthatacculturatedlistenershearsuggeststhatculturalcontextcanaffecthowweprocessauditoryinputtoagreaterextentthancurrentgenerativemetricaltheorytakesintoaccount.

Four‐beatAksakMeterBeyondtheBalkans

AleksandraVojcic,UniversityofMichigan–AnnArbor

Thispaperdetailstheproblemsinreadingandunderstandingcompoundmetersfromthetop‐down,ratherthanbottom‐up.Iexaminecompositemetersasrelatedtocompoundmeters,butcomprisingnon‐isochronousbeats—e.g.,afour‐beat“9/8”metercommontomuchfolkmusicoftheBalkansthatalsofrequentlyappearsin20th‐centuryscoresinvariednotation.MusicalexamplesillustratetherelationshipbetweencustomaryfolkidiomssuchastheSerbianpopular‐musicadaptationsbythebrassband“ZlatneUste”andthecontemporaryscoresbyBartokandCrawford.Thediscussionofnon‐isochronousfour‐beatmeterisextendedtoincludecompositeaksakmeterinLigeti’sCapriccio,a<5/8,5/8,7/8,8/8>sequence,andillustratestheimportanceofasymmetricalperiodicityofcompositemeterinphrasestructureanalysis.4:15–5:15 PERSPECTIVESONCOPLAND’SQUIETCITY

Chair:NadineHubbs(yettoconfirm)

TheStructureandGenesisofCopland’sQuietCityStanleyV.Kleppinger,UniversityofNebraska–Lincoln

AaronCopland’sQuietCity(1940),aone‐movementworkfortrumpet,

Englishhorn,andstrings,derivesfromincidentalmusicthecomposerwroteforanunsuccessfulandnow‐forgottenIrwinShawplay.ThispaperwillexplorethedetailsofQuietCity’spitchstructure,suggestingdramaticparallelsbetweenthemusicandwhatisknownofShaw’splay.

Thepiece’soutersectionshingeuponthesameanhemitonicpentatoniccollection.Thiscollectionisthesourceofsignificantpitchcentersforthewhole

Page 15: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

composition—thatis,themost‐stressedpitchclassesofeachsectiontogetherreplicatethecollectiongoverningthemusic’sfirstandlastmeasures.Thisprinciple,andthemusic’sexceptionstoit(especiallyatthework’sclimax),generatesaremarkablecorrespondencetotheinternalstrugglesofShaw’sprotagonist,GabrielMellon.

Inaddition,QuietCityoffersadistinctiveopportunitytoobservethecomposer’sassemblyofaunifiedtonalstructure.Copland’ssketchesforthework,nowarchivedattheLibraryofCongress,shedlightontheprocessbywhichhecreatedatonallycoherentmusicalworkfromasetofcuesoriginallymeanttounderscoreaplay.Studyofthesesketcheswillallowustoobservethecomposeralteringhisoriginalmaterialtoreinforcetonalconnectionsacrossthespanofthepiece.

ATonalRevolutioninFifthsandSemitones:AaronCopland’sQuietCityDavidHeetderks,UniversityofMichigan–AnnArbor

TonalambiguityinAaronCopland’smusicisatopicthatrecursoccasionally

incriticalcommentary.Thispaperarguesthatthisambiguityissymptomaticofaradicalreorganizationoftheconstituentelementsoftonality,anddemonstratesthisthroughananalysisofQuietCity(1940)thatusesmodelsofharmonicrelationdevelopedbyRobertMorris,StephenBrown,andJosephStraus.

ThepitchrelationsusedintheworkcanbemodeledthroughaTonnetzthatisgeneratedbytheintervalsofaperfectfifthandsemitone,andwhichhasCasaprivilegedmember.Intheopeningsection,patterncompletion,avoice‐leadingtechniqueinwhichthemissingnoteofacollectionestablishedasnormativeconspicuouslysoundsinordertocompletethecollection,articulatespointsofarrival.ThepitchC,whichistheaxisofinversionoftheopeningcollection,doesnotfigureprominentlyinthebasspart,butratheractsasthemelodiccenterofthetrumpetmelody.Themotiontowardtheclimax,whichmakesadramaticshiftbetweentwodiatoniccollections,showsalarge‐scaleinversionalrelationshiparoundthecentralpitch,C,mirroringtheinversionalsymmetryaroundCintheoriginalpentatoniccollection.

Thisanalysisshowshowthisworktakesconstituentelementsoftonality,includingdiatonicandpentatoniccollections,theconceptofprivilegedregister,andthemodelofthecadence,andtransformstheirinnerrelations.InOurNewMusic(1941)Coplandavowedtotheaestheticgoaloftappingnewpotentialwithinthetonalsystem;QuietCityprovidesastrikingexample.

Page 16: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

Saturday,May169:00–10:30 INSEARCHOFMETER

Chair:KyleAdams(yettoconfirm)

ProjectiveMeterinGesualdo’sTenebraeResponsoriesTimothyChenette,IndianaUniversity

WhileCarloGesualdo’smusicisoftendisjunct,hisTenebraeResponsoriesforMaundyThursdayareinacontinuouslypolyphonicstyle.Thispaperwillsuggestanapproachtocontinuityandstructureinthispieceastheyarefacilitatedbyrhythmandmeter.Itwillfocusonthecontextual,cognitiveprocessoflisteningusingconceptsofprojectiondevelopedbyChristopherHasty,thoughitwillalsodrawonmorescore‐basedandretrospectivemodesofhearing. Seenfromaprojectivestandpoint,Gesualdo’sTenebraeResponsoriesforMaundyThursdayareremarkablysimilarinconstructionandshowahighdegreeofcontinuityandcleardramaticstructure.Almostallbeginwitharelativelyextendedsectionofpolyphonythatcarefullyestablishesapulseand,often,higherlevelsofmeter.Whileeachopeningsectiondoesthisinauniquewaythathasimplicationsfortheaffectanddramaticstructureofthepiece,theprocessesbywhichitisdonearesimilarineachcase.Thispolyphonicsectionistheninterruptedandenrichedbyvariousexpressivemetricalstates.AfteradetailedexaminationoftheopeningpolyphonyofResponsoriesIandIIIfromGesualdo’sTenebraeResponsoriesforMaundyThursday,thispaperwillanalyzeResponsoryIImorethoroughly.Finally,itwillpresentanoverviewofGesualdo’smetricstrategiesastheyrelatetostructureandcontinuity.

ParsingTimewithHarmonySaraJ.Bakker,IndianaUniversity

TheunmeasuredpreludesoftheFrenchBaroquearenotoriouslyenigmatic

pieces.Asguidestoimprovisation,thesepiecesuseincompletenotation;theycontainneitherbarlinesnorfixeddurations,andinsteaduselongandshortnotevaluestorepresentharmonicandmelodiceventsrespectively.Thus,manydetailsofthemusicalsurfaceareleftuptotheperformer.MusicwithsuchinherentperformativeflexibilitynecessarilychallengesourtraditionalmodesofunderstandingtimeinWesternartmusic—notionsofrhythm,meterandhypermeterbecomeimpossible.Thereisnoinherentperiodicity,noupbeatsandnodownbeats.

Despitethesecomplications,Iassertthatpitchcontent,theonlyfixedelementsavailableinthisrepertoire,canobtainamorepertinentandusefuldescriptionoftime.FourclearphrasesemergeinthepreludetoElisabeth‐ClaudeJacquetdelaGuerre’sSuiteIIIinAMinor(1687).EachofthesearticulatesthefunctionsT,PD,D,andTdifferentlyandenablesoneofthreeperceptionsoftime.

Page 17: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

WeexperienceFocusedTimewhentheharmonicgoalaswellasthemotiontowarditisdirect.InDiffuseTime,however,weareconfrontedwithbothtonicizationsandphraseexpansionsthatobscuretherelationshipbetweentheharmoniesandphrasefunctions—therelationshipisnolongerone‐to‐oneandonto.Finally,weexperienceUnendingTimewhennosustainablesenseofharmonicgoalispossible.Mypaperexploresthesedifferentperceptionsoftimeandsuggestsnewlisteningstrategiesforaharmonicallybasedtemporalityoftonalmusic.

DurationalIdealismandMessiaen’sLivred'orgue

AntonVishio,SteinhardtSchool,NewYorkUniversity Difficultiesininterpretingeventsinthe“ametric”frameworkofsomestylesoftwentieth‐centurymusicarewellknown,andinparticularwhatwemighttermtheproblemof“durationalidealism,”aphenomenonJoelLesterhasdescribedasthelackof“one‐to‐onerelationshipbetweennotatedsymbolandperceivedduration.”Messiaen’sLivred’orgue(1951–52),whichisakindofcompendiumofdurationalexplorations,posestheprobleminextremis. Canwegainaccesstotheexperienceofthesetimespans?Fortunately,thecomposer’sparticulartemporalcreativity—theveryprocessesbywhich,asheputittoAlmutRößler,he“chop[s]upTimehereandthere,andcanevenputittogetherinthereverseorder,alittleasthoughheweregoingforawalkthroughdifferentpointsoftime”—suggestsawayin.Forinstance,inthethirdmovement,LesMainsdel’Abîme,Messiaenusesvarioustransformationsoftālastoallowproportionalrelationshipsandcharacteristicrhythmicpatternstoemergefromtheirjuxtaposition.Aclosereadingofthisandotherexampleswillsuggesthowwemightclimboutoftheabyssofdurationalcomplexity—andgainaheightenedappreciationofthosemomentswhichremainbeyondourtemporalgrasp. 10:45–12:15 ANALYSISANDPERFORMANCE

Chair:MiguelRoig‐Francoli(yettoconfirm)

TheActofInterpretingJ.S.Bach'sAllemandeforSoloFluteSamanthaInman,UniversityofCincinnatiCollege­ConservatoryofMusic

Analyticalinquirycoupledwiththestudyofpastperformancesprovidesthe

performerwithawealthofinformationthatcanbeusedtoguidethecreationofameaningfulinterpretation.UsingthefirstmovementofJ.S.Bach’sPartitaforSoloFlute,BWV1013asacasestudy,thispaperproposesandillustratesafive‐stepprocesstomusicalinterpretationthatbalancesbothdramaticandanalyticconcerns.Thismethodbuildsonearlierattemptstoilluminatethepotentialforinteractionbetweenperformanceandanalysis,particularlythoseofJanetSchmalfeldt(1985)

Page 18: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

andTimHowell(1992).Mymethoddiffersfromthesebyemphasizingtheperformer’sactivecontributiontotheinterpretiveprocess.

Thefivestepsare1)InitialReadings;2)GeneralListening;3)Analysis;4)FocusedListening;and5)FormationofInterpretation.Possessionoftheknowledgegleanedfromthisprocessdoesnotrestricttheperformerfromseekingnewinterpretationsofafamiliarwork;insightsgainedfromthisprocesssupplementratherthanreplaceoriginalideascontributedbytheperformer.Thiscombinationfreestheperformertochoosefromthewidestpossiblerangeofoptionswhenmakingbothplannedandspontaneousperformancedecisions,fosteringatrulycreativeexpressionofthework’sessence.

OnTheRelationshipBetweenAnalysisandPerformanceinAtonalMusicTimothyC.Best,IndianaUniversity

Intheforty‐fiveyearssincecomposerArthurBergercalledfora“newbranch

ofmusictheory”toaddressthelanguageofpost‐tonalrepertoires,theproliferationofanalyticaltoolsforexaminingthestructureofsuchworkshasbeenconsiderable.Thereremains,however,aconsiderablegapbetweentheanalyticalresultsprovidedbysuchtoolsandtheirrelevancetomusicalperformance—agapthatIcontendisfarnarrowerintonalanalysis.Inthispaper,Iproposethatthecomprehensionofeventhemostbasicatonalstructuresislargelyunhelpfultoamusicianindevelopinganinformedandmeaningfulperformance.Tofillthisgap,Iproposeamultifacetedanalyticalapproachgroundedinthefieldofmusicalmeaning,anapproachthatutilizesrecenttheoriesofmusicalgesture,embodiment,andintertextuality.

Defendinghistwelve‐tonemethodin1936,ArnoldSchoenbergwrotethatheinstructedhisstudentsto,“…usethesamekindofformorexpression,thesamethemes,melodies,sounds,rhythms,asyoudidbefore.”ThispapertakesSchoenberg’scompositionaladviceasanalyticalimperative.Focusingononeofthemostover‐analyzedworksofatonalmusicintherepertoire,thesecondmovementofWebern’sPianoVariations,op.27,thisdiscussionavoidsanymentionofrowforms,invertedcanons,pitch‐classsymmetries,orfixed‐registraldyads.Rather,usingPeterStadlen’s1936performanceeditionasastartingpoint,Isuggestspecificgesturalandtopicalprototypes,establishingthemovement’sconnectiontothepastthroughvariousrecompositions.Myaimistodemonstratethatthemovementderivesitsmeaningfromtheexpressionisticdistortionoftheseprototypes.ConveyingProportionandOtherPerformanceIssuesinJohnAdams’sChinaGates

KyleFyr,IndianaUniversity

Inthestudyoftheinteractionsbetweenmusicalanalysisandperformance,minimalistandpost‐minimalistmusichavelargelyescapedscholarlyattention.JohnAdams’sChinaGates(writtenin1977forsolopiano)providesaninterestingcasestudy,however,ofhowcloselyanalysiscanintertwinewithperformanceinsuch

Page 19: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

music,especiallywithregardtoproportion.ChinaGatesfeaturesaconstantlyarticulatedeighth‐notepulsestreamatasteadytempothroughoutthepiece,andisformallyproportionalbothatlocalandgloballevels.ThispaperassertsthatproportionisnotonlyausefullineofanalyticalinquiryforChinaGates,buttheconveyanceofitsinherentproportionalityalsodirectlyaffectsmanyimportantperformancedecisions.Inturn,Iillustratemethodsofpianisticattack,pedaling,andsubtledynamiccontrastthathelpcommunicatetherelationshipsbetweenlocalpairedsectionswhilealsoconveyingthelargerproportionaldesigninperformance.Ialsoexplorethewaysinwhichtheperformermayinterpretthevisualrepresentationofthepiece’s“gating”thatAdamsprovidesontheinsidecoverofthescore.Thirdly,IillustratehowproportionalanalysiscanguidetheperformerintheinterpretationofAdams’sscoreindications.Finally,IdiscusstheperceptibilityofproportioninmusicalformanditsusefulnessasananalyticconstructbyrelatingmyworktotheworkofJonathanKramer.2:00–4:00 HISTORIESANDDISCOURSESOFTHEORY

Chair:ThomasChristensenHeinrichChristophKoch’sDescriptionoftheAndanteinJosephHaydn’sSymphony

No.42asaResponsetoRecentSonataTheoriesGregoryHellenbrand,St.Paul,MN

InElementsofSonataTheory,HepokoskiandDarcyproposetheconceptsof

themedialcaesura,two‐partexposition,andcontinuousexpositiontodescribecertainworksofJosephHaydnandothereighteenth‐centurycomposers.However,thisapproachdoesnotsufficientlyreflectthemusicalthinkingofeighteenth‐centurytheorists.Forexample,thenotionthatanexpositioncanbeclassifiedaseithertwo‐partorcontinuous,dependingonthelocationofamedialcaesura,isnotcompatiblewithKoch'sconceptofthefirstmainperiod.HepokoskiandDarcyoverlookthevitalroleofthe“subsidiaryperiod”or“clarifyingperiod,”asatypeof“appendix”(Anhang),whichKochdescribesasafurther“explanation”afteraclosingphrase.

ThemainperiodofJosephHaydn'sAndanteconsistsofsixfour‐measurephrases,repeatedandvaried,followedbyanexpansiveclosingphrase.Inadditiontoanewcantabilepassage,tooshorttobeconsideredacompletephrase,unequalsegmentsmakeuptwoclarifyingperiodsasappendices.Theapplicationofsonata‐theoryprinciplestothiscomplexofphrasesisproblematic.IfHaydn’sexpositionwereconsideredas“two‐part,”the“medialcaesura”(V/V)wouldnotfunctionasexpected,sinceitisfollowedbyaclosingphrasewithalready‐familiarmelodiccontent,notacontrastingsecondtheme.NeithercanHaydn’sexpositionbefruitfullyconstruedas“continuous”becauseoftheearlyarrivalofaclosingphrasewithtwoextensivesubsidiaryperiods.Bycontrast,Koch’sanalysisprovidesamorepreciseformulationbasedprimarilyonharmonicallydefinedphrasesratherthan

Page 20: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

themes.

ReflectiveEquilibriumandDavidLewin'sAnalyticalMethodologiesPeterShultz,UniversityofChicago

DavidLewin'sfamousessayonStockhausen'sKlavierstückIIIpresentsnot

onlyananalysisofthepiece,butalsotheprocessbywhichLewinconstructsthatanalysis.Beginningwithsomeintuitiveobservationsaboutthepiece,heproposesananalyticalsystem.Thenoverthecourseoftheessayheprogressivelyrefinesboththesystemandhisintuitivejudgments,inordertobringthemintomutualharmony.

Inthispaper,IarguethatthisprocessisadistinctlyLewinianmethodologyakinto“reflectiveequilibrium”(RE),aprocessofmoralreasoningfirstdescribedbyNelsonGoodmanandpopularizedbyJohnRawls.REinvolvesasimilarprogressiveharmonizationof“consideredjudgments”andsystematicbeliefs;whatLewincalls“intuitions”taketheplaceofRawls's“consideredjudgments.”Iproposethatintuitioncolorseachstepofhisanalyticalprocess,andthattheresultinganalysesaresatisfactory(toLewin)notmerelybecauseoftheirself‐consistency,butalsobecauseoftheiraccordancewithintuitive,pre‐theoreticaljudgments.

ThemostprofoundsymptomofREinLewin'sthoughtishisrejectionofbothnaturallaw(àlaRameauorSchenker)andmereconsistencyasjustificationsfortheoriesaboutmusic.Selectionsfromhiswritingsshowthatalthoughheregardsconsistencyasinsufficient,heputsahighvalueonthe/search/forconsistency;andthroughouthisownanalysesitremainshisonlyepistemologicalappeal.

TheUniversalismofMarinMersenne'sHarmonieuniverselleJohnReef,IndianaUniversity

ThefourthtreatiseofMarinMersenne’sHarmonieuniverselle(1636)

containstwocuriousexamplesinapropositiononthediatonicgenus:oneisofaCanadiandancesong;theotherisofsongsoftheBrazilianTupinambaIndians.Incitingtheseexamples,MersennetakesastanceonaquestionthatoccupiedFrenchthinkersinthedecadesprecedinghispublication:whethercertainuniversalmoralandaestheticvaluesunderwritehumanexperience,orwhetherthemoralandaestheticpracticesofacultureareproductsofcustomalone.Mersenne’sintentistoprovethatthediatonicgenusismorenaturalthanthechromaticandenharmonicgenerathatsomeonebroughtupina“primitive”culture,unschooledinEuropeanmusic,wouldnaturallysinginthediatonicgenus.Ifthediatonicgenus’snaturaloriginweretrue,thenthegenuscouldberegardedasanelementofhumankind’suniversalmusicality.Nevertheless,Mersennecannotescapecounterclaimsthatmusicalpreferencesarediverse,anddonotreflectanyuniversalstandard.ThisantagonismbetweenuniversalismanddiversityinformsMersenne’sdialecticalprosethroughoutHarmonieuniverselle.

Inthispaper,IcontextualizeMersenne’snegotiationofuniversalismand

Page 21: David Bashwiner, University of Chicago · David Bashwiner, University of Chicago It is common to speak of there being a “syntax” in music, but much less commonly is this term

diversitywithinhisintellectualclimate,andwithinthefascinationfortheexoticandprimitivethatpervadedcontemporarythought;specifically,IadduceMontaigne’sessay,“OnCustom”asafoiltoMersenne’suniversalizingdiscourse.Inaddition,IcompareanothersortofuniversalismthatpervadesMersenne’swriting,onedeterminednotinnature,butthroughthecontemplationofheavenlyperfection.ThisunderstandingisthebasisofMersenne’sdiscourseontheunison.

SympatheticResonance:GyörgyLigeti’s“Cordesàvide”andtheDiscoursesofTheory

AugustSheehy,UniversityofWisconsin–Madison

IfGyörgyLigeti’smusicisdifficulttoclassify,hislateworksareespeciallyso,atleastinpartbecauseofconspicuous“backward‐looking”structuringdevicessuchasintervalsandtriadicsonorities.Inthecaseoftriads,EricDrotthasconvincinglyarguedthattheiruseinworkssuchastheHornTrioandÉtudespourpianorepresentacompositionalcounterargumenttoassertionsmadebyAdornoandBoulezthatthesesoundsaretoohistoricallyburdenedtobeusefulinmodernmusic.RatherthaninterpretingLigeti’slatemusicasageneralresponsetoparticularmodernistarguments,Iaskwhatspecificrolesuch“restorativegestures”mightplayinonework,Étudepourpiano,No.2,“Cordesàvide.”

Ifindthat“Cordesàvide”respondsto—indeedresonateswith—apanoplyofdiscoursesdistributedacrossthehistoryofWesterntheory.Whatappearsatfirsttobeacompositionalstudyonthepotentialofperfectfifthsturnsouttobesomethingratherdifferent:ademonstrationofthephenomenologicalincommensurabilityamongdifferenttheoreticalconceptsoftensupposedtonamethe“samething,”e.g.,perfectfifth,[07],or2:3.“Cordesàvide”canthusbereadasasonorouscritiqueofasystematizingimpulsethreadedthroughthehistoryofmusictheory.Morebroadly,thispapergesturestowardsamethodologythatreconcilesformalanalysiswithhistoriographicconcernsinlatetwentieth‐centurymusicbysuggestingthat,ratherfixingthemeaningofaworkthroughanalysis,themusicbeallowedtoresonatepluralisticallywithdiscoursesinprocessesofmutualcritique.