department of psychology the relationship between …

132
DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER FEATURES IN CHILDREN: THE ROLE OF ATTACHMENT AND EMOTIONAL AVAILABILITY DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DISSERTATION FILIA-ANNA CHRISTODOULOU 2016 Filia-Anna Christodoulou

Upload: others

Post on 01-Mar-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER FEATURES IN CHILDREN: THE

ROLE OF ATTACHMENT AND EMOTIONAL AVAILABILITY

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DISSERTATION

FILIA-ANNA CHRISTODOULOU

2016

Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 2: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER FEATURES IN CHILDREN: THE

ROLE OF ATTACHMENT AND EMOTIONAL AVAILABILITY

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DISSERTATION

FILIA-ANNA CHRISTODOULOU

A Dissertation Submitted to the University of Cyprus in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

May, 2016

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 3: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Doctoral Candidate: Filia-Anna Christodoulou

Doctoral Thesis Title: The relationship between trait emotional intelligence and

borderline personality disorder features in children: The role of attachment and emotional

availability.

The Present Doctoral Dissertation was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy at the Department of Psychology and was

approved on the 22nd of April, 2016, by the members of the Examination Committee.

Examination Committee:

Research

Supervisor:________________________________________________________

Committee

Member:_________________________________________________________

Committee

Member:_________________________________________________________

Committee

Member:_________________________________________________________

Committee

Member:_________________________________________________________

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 4: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

DECLARATION OF DOCTORAL CANDIDATE

The present doctoral dissertation was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of the University of Cyprus. It is a product of

original work of my own, unless otherwise mentioned through references, notes, or any

other statements.

Filia-Anna Christodoulou

…………………………………..

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 5: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Table of Contents

Abstract in Greek language………………………………………...…………………….....7

Abstract in an international language……..…………………………...………………........8

Chapter 1: Introduction to the Study……………………………………………………......9

Chapter 2: Literature Review…………….………………………………………………..11

Chapter 3: The Present Study……………………………………………………………...37

Chapter 4: Results…………………………………………………………………………45

Chapter 5: Discussion………………………………………………………………….......55

References…………………………………………………………………………………63

Appendix A:……………………………………………………………………………….88

Appendix B:……………………………………………………………………………….92

Appendix C:……………………………………………………………………………….93

Appendix D:……………………………………………………………………………….99

Appendix E:………………………………………………………………………………100

Appendix F:………………………………………………………………………………105

Appendix G:……………………………………………………………………………...107

Appendix H:……………………………………………………………………………...109

Appendix I:………………………………………………………………………………113

Appendix J……………………………………………………………………………….114

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 6: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix K………………………………………………………………………………115

Appendix L……………………………………………………………………………….116

Appendix M………………………………………………………………………………117

Appendix N………………………………………………………………………………118

Appendix O………………………………………………………………………………119

Appendix P……………………………………………………………………………….120

Appendix Q………………………………………………………………………………121

Appendix R………………………………………………………………………………122

Appendix S………………………………………………………………………………123

Appendix T………………………………………………………………………………124

Appendix U………………………………………………………………………………125

Appendix V………………………………………………………………………………126

Appendix W……………………………………………………………………………...127

Appendix X………………………………………………………………………………128

Appendix Y………………………………………………………………………………129

Appendix Z………………………………………………………………………………130

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 7: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Abstract in Greek Language

Η μεταιχμιακή διαταραχή προσωπικότητας (ΜΔΠ) έχει χαρακτηριστεί ως διαταραχή

συναισθηματικής ρύθμισης και η σύγχρονη βιβλιογραφία υπογραμμίζει την ανάγκη για

έγκαιρο εντοπισμό και προώρη παρέμβαση. Η παρούσα έρευνα διερεύνησε τη σχέση

μεταξύ της συναισθηματική νοημοσύνης ως γνώρισμα και των αναδυόμενων

χαρακτηριστικών ΜΔΠ σε μαθητές Γυμνασίου και Λυκείου, καθώς και την προσκόλληση

και αντιλαμβανόμενη συναισθηματική διαθεσιμότητα ως μεσολαβητή και διαμεσολαβητή

αντίστοιχα. Τα αποτελέσματα υποδεικνύουν πως η συναισθηματική έκφραση προέβλεπε

τα χαρακτηριστικά ΜΔΠ μόνο για τους συμμετέχοντες που φοιτούν σε Λύκεια.

Αναφορικά με διαφορές φύλου, η αντίληψη συναισθημάτων ήταν σημαντικός

προβλεπτικός παράγοντας για τα χαρακτηριστικά ΜΔΠ κορίτσια, ενώ στα αγόρια μόνο η

διεκδικητικότητα προέβλεπε αυτά τα χαρακτηριστικά. Διαφάνηκε επίσης ότι ο αγχώδης-

αμφίθυμος τύπος προσκόλλησης ήταν σημαντικός προβλεπτικός παράγοντας και στα δύο

επίπεδα φοίτησης. Περαιτέρω, διαφάνηκε ότι η αντιλαμβανόμενη συναισθηματική

διαθεσιμότητα για πατέρες προέβλεπε τα χαρακτηριστικά ΜΔΠ αλλά μόνο για μαθητές

Γυμνασίου.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 8: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Abstract in an International Language

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) has been labeled a disorder of emotion

dysregulation and contemporary research underscores the need for earlier detection and

intervention. The current study investigated the relationship between trait emotional

intelligence (EI) and emerging borderline personality disorder features in adolescents in

junior high and high school, and the moderating and mediating effect of attachment and

perceived emotional availability, respectively. The results revealed that emotion expression

predicted BPD features only for high school participants. With regards to gender

differences, emotion perception was a significant predictor of BPD features in girls, while

for boys only assertiveness predicted these features. It was also revealed that an anxious

ambivalent attachment style significantly predicted BPD features in both education levels.

Furthermore, the study found that perceived emotional availability of fathers also

significantly predicted BPD features, but only for junior high participants.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 9: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Chapter 1: Introduction to the Study

Background

The debate regarding whether or not persons younger than 18 years old should be

diagnosed with a personality disorder (PD) is well-known and ongoing. However, the more

important question is which features of these disorders evident in children could elevate

the risk of them receiving a later diagnosis? Evidence from research investigating aspects

of Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), indicates that emotion has a central role in the

development of the disorder (Crowell, Beauchaine & Linehan, 2009; Farrell & Shaw,

1994) yet no study to date has looked at the possible link of Trait Emotional Intelligence

(TEI) and BPD in children. Furthermore, research regarding attachment styles in

individuals with BPD, has produced equivocal results, with some studies pointing to an

anxious/ambivalent style and others finding associations with an avoidant attachment style.

Additionally, the concept of emotional availability which involves important aspects of

dyadic relationships, predominantly those concerning parents and children has not been

investigated in relation to BPD, despite its theoretical relevance. Thus, by including the

latter as a possible mediator, this investigation could potentially shed more light on the

development of the disorder and, at the same time, guide future research and early

intervention efforts. The current study aims to bridge the gap in the literature, by

investigating the role of trait Emotional Intelligence (trait EI) in relation to BPD and

whether specific attachment styles can act as a moderator for the presence of BPD features

in children.

BPD is one of the most commonly diagnosed PDs (Leichsenring, Leibing, Kruse,

New & Leweke, 2011), as 30-60% of patients with a PD are diagnosed with BPD (Wenar

& Kerig, 2005). Prevalence rates of BPD in adults in the US have been found to be

between 0.5-5.9 percent in the general population, while in many clinical psychiatric

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 10: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

settings BPD is diagnosed in as high as 20% of patients (Kernberg & Michels, 2009). BPD

is also associated with a high degree of suffering for the patients themselves but also their

families and friends. It has been reported that between 60 and 70% of individuals with

BPD attempt suicide at least once in their lifetime, with self-injurious behavior ranking

much higher in frequency (Oldham, 2006). Self-injurious behavior in patients with BPD

has often been attributed to the intense suffering which they report experiencing, as they

indicate such behaviors provide a sense of temporary relief (Perroud, Dieben, Nicastro,

Muscionico & Huguelet, 2012). Moreover, research indicates that for almost a third of

BPD patients who self-harm, onset began prior to the age of 12 (Zanarini, Frankenburg,

Ridolfi, Jager-Hyman, Hennen & Gunderson, 2006).

Diagnoses of PDs have traditionally been given to individuals over the age of 18,

when traits and behaviour patterns are believed to be stable and not falling within the range

of developmental norms (i.e. for adolescence). However, there is growing concern that this

view may not allow for early intervention and treatment efforts. While the DSM-IV (APA,

2000), and more recently the DSM-5 (APA, 2013), allow for the diagnosis of BPD, and

other PDs, prior to age 18, providing there is an enduring pattern of symptoms for at least a

year, there is evidence that clinicians are reluctant to utilize this. In a survey of child and

adolescent psychiatrists (Griffiths, 2011), only 37% of participants indicated that they

believed BPD to be a valid diagnosis for adolescents, and only 23% reported using the

diagnosis on a regular basis within their practice. Evidence such as the latter, also raises

concerns regarding receiving an accurate diagnosis, as well as access to appropriate

treatment, for individuals presenting with symptoms of BPD in adolescence. The need for

diagnosis and timely treatment of adolescents with BPD is also highlighted in research that

indicates that utilization of healthcare, including hospitalizations, psychiatric medication Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 11: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

and treatment participation, is similar in adolescents when compared to adult patients

(Cailhol, Jeannot, Rodgers, Guelfi, Perez-Diaz et al. 2013).

Based on the above, the current study aims to investigate the relationship between

TEI and BPD in adolescence, while exploring the possible role of attachment and

emotional availability as a moderator and mediator respectively. The study’s rationale is

grounded in the relevant theory and research which indicates that the construct of trait EI

may be of significant relevance to BPD, despite being largely overlooked in the literature.

Meanwhile, in light of mounting evidence pointing to the need to address emerging

features of BPD in children and adolescents, it is necessary to have a sound theoretical

model which also has practical utility. Identifying children at risk for later diagnosis of

BPD is imperative and further research is needed to inform clinicians, educators and policy

makers on how to accomplish this without the stigma associated with being labeled as

having a PD.

Chapter 2: Literature Review

Borderline Personality Disorder

Within the context of mental health, the term borderline was initially coined by

Adolph Stern in 1938 (Stern, 1938) in order to describe patients who appeared to be on the

‘border’ so to speak, between neurosis and psychosis. While the conceptualization of BPD

has changed dramatically since then, this misleading term remains despite a more accurate

understanding of the complexities of this pervasive disorder. According to the Diagnostic

and Statistical Manual for mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000), BPD is

characterized by a pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image

and affects and marked impulsivity. The disorder is further defined by marked disturbances

in numerous aspects of functioning such as affect, behavior, cognition, attention and

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 12: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

relationship skills. It is considered a cluster B PD, a subgroup associated with dramatic,

erratic and emotional behaviors. In the newest version of the manual (DSM-5; APA, 2013)

criteria include those specified for all PDs in addition to the disorder specific criteria as

follows:

A pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, and

affects, and marked impulsivity, beginning by early adulthood and present in a variety of

contexts, as indicated by five (or more) of the following:

1. Frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment. (Note: Do not include suicidal or

self-mutilating behavior covered in Criterion 5.)

2. A pattern of unstable and intense interpersonal relationships characterized by alternating

between extremes of idealization and devaluation.

3. Identity disturbance: markedly and persistently unstable self-image or sense of self.

4. Impulsivity in at least two areas that are potentially self-damaging (e.g., spending, sex,

substance abuse, reckless driving, binge eating). (Note: Do not include suicidal or self-

mutilating behavior covered in Criterion 5.)

5. Recurrent suicidal behavior, gestures, or threats, or self-mutilating behavior.

6. Affective instability due to a marked reactivity of mood (e.g., intense episodic

dysphoria, irritability, or anxiety usually lasting a few hours and only rarely more than a

few days).

7. Chronic feelings of emptiness.

8. Inappropriate, intense anger or difficulty controlling anger (e.g., frequent displays of

temper, constant anger, recurrent physical fights).

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 13: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

9. Transient, stress-related paranoid ideation or severe dissociative symptoms.

While the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) continues to adopt a categorical view of PDs as part

of the main manual, it also recognizes and presents an alternative model. The latter model,

views PDs as extreme patterns of maladaptive traits, within the continuum of normal

personality. The dimensional viewpoint also argues that both Axis I and Axis II disorders

(APA, 2000) reflect similar mechanisms which contribute to the development of more

transient forms of psychopathology and the more enduring patterns seen in PDs (Krueger

& Tackett, 2003; Tillfors, Furmark, Ekselius & Fredrikson, 2004). More specifically, it has

been suggested in the literature, that adopting a dimensional view of BPD in adolescents

may in fact be more appropriate as it could best reflect the variability and heterogeneity

that is present in this age group (Miller, Muehlenkamp & Jacobson, 2008). Also, a review

of research indicates that dimensional conceptualizations have been favored for certain

disorders regardless of age, and this includes BPD (Haslam, 2003; Rothschild, Cleland,

Haslam & Zimmerman, 2003; Trull, Widiger & Guthrie, 1990). It is worth noting that,

despite the shift to the latest diagnostic manual the core aspects of the disorder remain

unchanged.

Theoretical Models of BPD

While the aetiology of BPD has yet to be fully agreed upon, there are several

models in the literature which attempt to explain the emergence of BPD and are derived

from different types of theoretical orientations within psychology. Despite the latter, the

models appear to include similar factors, albeit described in different terminology in

accordance with their respective theoretical backgrounds. Three models for the

development of BPD are discussed below, as well as the implications for moving towards

early intervention.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 14: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

In perhaps the most cited model for the disorder, Linehan (1993a) suggested that

BPD develops from the pairing of emotional vulnerability and an invalidating childhood

environment. According to this biosocial model, the invalidating environment occurs when

those close to the child constantly criticize the child’s expressions of thoughts and

emotions and attribute them to undesirable traits, which in turn, interacts with a biological

tendency for affective instability (heightened emotional sensitivity, high emotional

intensity and slow return to emotional baseline). An important aspect of BPD is a severe

dysfunction regarding affect which has played an important part in the biosocial (Linehan,

1993a) model, and which includes, as one of the six core characteristics, that of emotional

vulnerability. The researcher defines emotional vulnerability as a pattern of pervasive

difficulties in regulating negative emotions, including high sensitivity to negative

emotional stimuli, high emotional intensity, and slow return to emotional baseline, as well

as awareness and experience of emotional vulnerability (as cited in Wenar & Kerig, 2005).

In fact, it was proposed that BPD is primarily a disorder of emotion dysregulation, which

stems, at least in part, from an invalidating childhood environment in which the child is not

able to learn how to understand, label and cope with emotions. In an extension of the 1993

model, emotion continues to play a central role in the conceptualization of BPD, where

emotion dysregulation, along with impulsivity, are thought to contribute significantly to

the emergence of BPD within a developmental psychopathology framework (Crowell,

Beauchaine & Linehan, 2009).

Research has provided some support for Linehan’s (1993a) model, in which

negative affect intensity has been significantly correlated to BPD traits (e.g. Rosenthal,

Cheavens, Lejuez, and Lynch, 2005; Yen, Zlotnick, and Costello, 2002). A range of

articles support several aspects of the emotion dysregulation component of the theory (e.g.

Chapman, Leung & Lynch, 2008; Glen & Klonsky, 2009). In the Chapman et al. (2008)

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 15: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

study, results revealed that individuals with a higher number of BPD traits reported a

greater number of difficulties in aspects of emotion dysregulation, such as impulse control

and acceptance of emotions, when under conditions of emotional distress. However, a

discrepancy between laboratory and self-report results emerged in the latter study which

limits the conclusions which can be drawn from the outcomes. A recent study, (Gill &

Warburton, 2014) found partial support for the biosocial theory, where only emotional

dysregulation predicted BPD traits. The study also found that while emotional vulnerability

and invalidating parenting independently predicted emotional dysregulation, there was a

limited interaction between the two. Meanwhile, given that the bulk of research relied on

self-report measures, a recent review article (Cavazzi & Becerra, 2014) examined

psychophysiological research with regards to the biosocial theory. It was discovered that,

in contrast to self-report data and the biosocial theory, individuals with BPD evidenced a

lower baseline arousal (heart-rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia and blood pressure) than

controls. Furthermore, with regards to skin conduct response, the literature indicates that

the results are mixed, with some studies indicating a higher response in those with BPD

and others failing to find any significant differences between BPD patients and controls.

Mixed results were also found for reactivity to emotionally and non-emotionally valenced

stimuli (see Cavazzi & Becerra, 2014).

Another core model in BPD literature has been suggested by Fonagy et al. (Fonagy,

Target, Gergely, Allen & Bateman, 2003), often referred to as the mentalization-based or

mentalizing model for BPD. In this model the disorder is conceptualized in terms of

impairment of the capacity for stress regulation, attentional control and mentalization

abilities which are believed to have developed within the context of attachment

relationships. Mentalization, or reflective functioning, is defined as the individual’s ability

to understand one’s own and others behavior in mental state terms (Fonagy, 2000).

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 16: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Meanwhile, attentional control is closely associated to impulsivity given that it refers to the

ability to inhibit a dominant response in order to perform a subdominant response, thus

modulating impulsivity (Fonagy et al., 2003). Stress regulation, further defined as affect

representation and regulation, is a core feature of BPD and the mentalization-based model

emphasizes the development of these abilities from early infancy through adulthood.

Furthermore, the researchers clarify that these abilities are acquired via the mirroring

processes provided by the attachment figure (Fonagy & Bateman, 2007). The attention

difficulties suggested in the model are also believed to be linked to difficulties in

controlled processing of information, as illustrated by patients’ erratic and impulsive

behavior. It is also suggested that deficits in attention may also contribute to difficulties in

directing attention appropriately to interpersonal and social contexts (Fonagy & Bateman,

2008).

The model suggests that abnormalities in parenting and/or genetic factors, such as

attention deficits, may limit the capacity for mentalization. It is also suggested that a high

reflective capacity in parents may promote secure attachments, while at the same time a

secure attachment style may precede a strong reflective capacity. Moreover, evidence from

research indicates that absence of marked contingent mirroring may be associated with the

later development of a disorganized attachment which is in turn linked to difficulties with

affect regulation (Fonagy & Bateman, 2007). In a more recent presentation of the model

(Fonagy, Luyten & Strathearn, 2011), the researchers suggest that genetics and early

environmental factors such as neglect or trauma, may undermine the development of

mentalized activities, the second order representation of emotional states. This leads to

limitations in the infant’s affect regulation which may undermine the development of

effortful control and understanding of others as motivated by neutral mental states. In

summary, the mentalization-based model for BPD is a transactional model which supports

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 17: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

that affect regulation, attentional control, mentalizing and attachment may interact in

multiple and complex ways in the development of BPD. Although the researchers who

developed the model have produced research in support of this model, this is in fact limited

and to the author’s knowledge there have been no attempts by others to test its validity and

reliability. Instead, research available focuses solely on the treatment derived from this

model, namely mentalization-based therapy (for a review see Haskayne, Hirschfield &

Larkin, 2014).

A third theoretical model, conceptualizes the disorder from a psychoanalytic

perspective (Kernberg, 1967; Clarkin, Lenzenweger, Yeomans, Levy & Kernberg, 2007).

According to Kernberg, personality can be understood along two continuous dimensions,

one being personality organization and the other a dimension of extroversion and

introversion. With regards to personality organization, there are three levels; the neurotic

level being the healthiest, as opposed to the psychotic level which reflects severely

disorganized personalities. Borderline Personality Organization (BPO) falls in the middle

of the aforementioned levels, and includes personalities which have, an intact sense of

reality on the one hand, and a fragmented sense of self (and others), on the other hand. The

BPO model claims to include, but go beyond the DSM-IV (2000) based BPD

conceptualization and as mentioned, is a dimensional understanding of this type of

personality, rather than a categorical approach. The BPO model is deeply rooted in object

relations theory which emphasizes the role of internalization of dyadic object relations in

understanding the development of personality (Clarkin et al., 2007). In order to fully

comprehend the proposed model, familiarity with the basic premises of object relations

theory in general is necessary.

Object relations theory proposes that internalization of object relation dyads is

strongly influenced by early mother-infant interactions. Moreover, it claims that there are

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 18: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

two positions which can emerge from the above, namely the paranoid-schizoid position

and the depressive position. In early development, it has been proposed that individuals go

through a phase of separation-individuation, during which a differentiation takes place

between good and bad, for both the self, and significant others. Ideally, this phase is

followed by the development of integrated representations of the self and others, thus

acknowledging that the self and others can be both ‘good’ and ‘bad’. With regards to

personality in particular, the theory conceptualizes psychic structure as composed of a

representation of self, a representation of other in relation to self, and an affect linking the

two (Clarkin et al., 2007) which is referred to as an object relation dyad. These object

relation dyads are what organize both motivation and behavior. Individuals with typical

personalities have three characteristics: an integrated concept of self and other, a broad

spectrum of affective experience and the presence of an internalized value system (Clarkin

et al., 2007). Normal identity requires integration of the self and the ability to combine

positive and negative emotions, as opposed to experiencing polarized affect. Meanwhile,

failure to develop the latter, results in, what is referred to as identity diffusion. Normal

personality structure also includes the component of affect regulation, whereby the

individual is able to experience a full range of complex emotions, while retaining impulse

control. Finally, the structure of normal personality within the framework of this model

requires an integrated system of internalized values, one that is influenced by parental

values but not inflexibly so, and can therefore exist independently.

With regards to BPO, and severe PDs in general, the premise is that there is lack of

integration, or identity diffusion (Clarkin et al., 2007). Thus, the symptoms observed in

BPD are believed to be a manifestation of pathology in the very structure of personality.

More specifically, BPO emerges due to a lack of integration between the positive and

negative parts which is rooted in early mother-infant relationships. This lack of integration

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 19: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

is believed to be responsible for the clinical presentation of emotional lability, anger,

interpersonal chaos etc. in individuals diagnosed with BPD. Further, it is proposed that

individuals with BPO are more dependent on primitive defense mechanisms, as opposed to

mature ones which allow the individual to interact in a flexible way. The model proposes

the distinction between high and low-level BPO on the basis of four dimensional variables,

comprising of the following: Introversion-Extraversion, Object-Relations, Moral values

and Negative Affect. The Introversion-Extraversion variable essentially refers to

temperament with regards to gravitating towards others or avoiding them. In BPO, Object-

Relations, or relationships with others, are characterized by significant deficits. These

include a lack of empathy in understanding others and inconsistencies in perceptions of

others, whereby individuals with BPO alternate between idealization and devaluation and

rejection. The moral values variable refers to an internalized set of values. According to the

model, this internalized value system differs among individuals with BPD, with some

achieving internalization of some values, and low level borderlines lacking this

internalized system. Finally, borderlines are believed to be dominated by Negative Affect

and difficulties with aggressive affects and behavior.

Some features of the BPO model appear to be supported in the literature (see

Clarkin et al, 2007 for a review). Furthermore, studies utilizing measures for assessing

BPO in adults, such as the Borderline Personality Inventory (Leichsenring, 1999a), have

yielded positive results with regards to construct validity (Leichsenring, 1999b, 1999c;

Leichsenring & Sachsse, 2002) and reliability (e.g. Chabrol et al., 2004). Meanwhile,

examination of core components of the theory through newly developed self-report

measures also show promising results (Goth, Foelsch, Schlüter-Müller, et al., 2012).

Despite this evidence, research on BPO and adolescents is essentially nonexistent,

whereby, to the author’s knowledge, only a single study investigated BPO in terms of

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 20: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

comorbidity with psychopathic traits in a non-clinical group of adolescents (Chabrol &

Leichsenring, 2006). Given this lack of research, the utility of this model, at least in terms

of early identification of BPD features in adolescents, is put into question.

Despite marked differences between theories, there are also certain common

threads, yet this is rarely recognized in light of the varying theoretical background from

which these models have been developed. It is important to recognize these and utilize this

common ground to develop theories and models which are comprehensive and non-

redundant but also hold practical significance. The above models appear to share a focus

on impaired emotional functioning and the early origins of this in the context of attachment

relationships. In the mentalizing model (Fonagy et al., 2003), environmental and genetic

factors are believed to contribute to a diminished ability for affect regulation and

subsequently the development of effortful control. Meanwhile, in the psychoanalytic model

of BPO, negative affect is an important variable. In the biosocial model (Crowell,

Beauchaine & Linehan, 2009) emotion is believed to be at the core of the disorder. Based

on the above, it is important for the purpose of this study to use terminology that

recognizes the common ground of the aforementioned models. Thus, the term emotion

dysregulation will be adopted to refer to the difficulties in emotion regulation which have

discussed using slightly different terms in the two of the three models – affect regulation

(Kernberg, 1967), affect representation and regulation (Fonagy et al., 2003). Attachment

and emotional availability in this study are terms believed to encompass the invalidating

environment in the biosocial model (Crowell, Beauchaine & Linehan, 2009), attachment

and mirroring discussed in the mentalization-based model (Fonagy et al., 2003) and

internalization of object relation dyads in the BPO model (Kernberg, 1967). Utilizing the

same terms to refer to concepts with varying theoretical background is not an attempt to

minimize the significance of underlying theories nor is it meant to oversimplify complex

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 21: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

processes. Instead, the premise here is that, when it comes to prevention in children and

adolescents, we need to have a common language, which can potentially strengthen efforts

for early detection of risk factors in non-clinical populations.

While it is reasonable to allow for a number of theoretical models for a particular

disorder, given the varying psychological approaches, the practical utility of this with

regards to the research-practice gap can be questioned. Kazdin (1997) has referred to the

lack of plan for progressing from research to effective interventions, particularly when it

comes to children and adolescents. He proposes a model which begins with a

conceptualization of the dysfunction, concurrent correlates, antecedents and causal factors,

in order to understand onset and course of clinical dysfunction (Kazdin, 1997).

Additionally, it is proposed that cross-sectional and longitudinal studies should be used to

test models of clinical dysfunction and developmental pathways or trajectories, in order to

uncover subtypes, moderators and other important factors relating to the risk profile. These

steps will enable a more effective transition to treatment development and research on

treatment processes and effectiveness. Meanwhile, others have also referred to the often

competing spheres of research and treatment and the need to merge the two (Lebow, 2006).

While the above models have given rise to relatively effective treatments for BPD patients,

these have been originally designed to cater to adults (Bateman & Fonagy, 2004; Clarkin,

Yeomans & Kernberg, 2006; Linehan, 1993a; Linehan, 1993b). The treatments in question

have been adapted for adolescents (see Ensink, Biberdzic, Normandin & Clarkin, 2015;

Fonagy, Rossouw, Sharp, Bateman, Allison et al., 2014; Miller, Rathus & Linehan, 2006;

Rathus & Miller, 2014; Normandin, Ensink & Kernberg, 2015; Rossouw, 2013) however,

it appears we have yet to find a model which would allow for identifying individuals at risk

for developing BPD in non-clinical adolescents, a vital step when developing early

intervention programmes. Within this context, the current study has adopted a cross-

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 22: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

sectional design to identify risk factors and moderators for the disorder as per Kazdin’s

(1997) suggestions.

BPD Features in Childhood

When diagnoses of PDs are made around the age of 18, maladaptive patterns of

behaviour and cognition are by definition enduring and inflexible. At the same time,

models for the development of PDs focus on the contribution of genetics and the early

environment in establishing these maladaptive patterns. This, however, is done mainly in

retrospective studies with individuals who have been diagnosed with PDs in adulthood.

While it is undoubtedly valuable to do such research, it does not inform us on how the

features of the disorder appear prior to the individual meeting the full criteria of the

personality disorder. Consequently, there is insufficient focus on identifying and defining

features or precursors of PDs in childhood and adolescence, which could potentially lead to

the development of early intervention programmes. PDs are known to be difficult to treat,

with BPD being a particularly strong example of this, thus it is imperative that prevention

and early intervention become prime research targets.

More recently, research has begun to address the above issues. There has been

growing interest in researching trait-related symptoms of PDs in children by creating a

developmental taxonomy of the latter (Clercq, Fruyt, Van Leeuwen & Mervielde, 2006).

Such taxonomies emerged in the last two decades for adults (e.g. Clark, 1986, 1993;

Livesley, 1990) and aimed at providing a comprehensive pool of items that address

maladaptive personality traits. In the last decade this has also been extended to children

(Clercq et al., 2006). Similarly, an important issue to consider in research surrounding

BPD is whether certain features of the disorder are present through childhood and the

stability of these characteristics, in order to identify individuals at risk for later diagnosis.

Important to this endeavour is the concept of clusters of symptoms as a precursor to a PD,

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 23: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

with the most pertinent example of this being the relationship between conduct disorder

(CD) and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD). The strong relationship between CD and

ASPD led to the requirement that individuals diagnosed with the latter need to have

exhibited symptoms of conduct disorder prior to the age of 15 (APA, 2000). While it is

clear that not all children diagnosed with CD will go on to receive a diagnosis of ASPD,

the severity of symptoms seems to elevate the risk and this provides the opportunity to

intervene early on, potentially lowering the associated risk. Meanwhile, ADHD has also

been found to significantly increase risk for ASPD (Loeber, Green & Lahey, 2003).

Similarly, clusters of internalizing and externalizing symptoms have been

associated with BPD and researchers have suggested they may lead to an elevated risk for

developing BPD (e.g. Crick, Murray-Close & Woods, 2005). In fact, Borderline Pathology

of Childhood (BPC) is a term that has been coined to describe a certain type of complex

and severe behavioral pathology which is characterized by affective, impulsive, and

cognitive symptoms (see Bemporad & Cicchetti, 1982; Paris, 2000). Research indicates

that children and adolescents with BPC are at a higher risk for future psychopathology

(Zelkowitz, Paris, Guzder, Feldman, et al., 2007), although more research is needed to

further investigate the relationship between BPC and BPD. In line with this viewpoint is

emerging research regarding the possible link between childhood disorders and BPD. A

recent study by Stepp and colleagues (2012) looked at the relationship between

oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and

BPD given the overlap in deficits and other symptomatology in these disorders. The

researchers found that ADHD and ODD at age eight predicted BPD symptoms at age 14,

even after controlling for symptoms of the other two disorders. Similarly, van Dijk and

colleagues (2011) found that all adult patients with BPD in their study had some symptoms

of ADHD in childhood and adulthood, although the specific study included only females in

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 24: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

its sample. These findings indicate that children presenting with certain symptoms may be

at risk for developing BPD features either in childhood or early adolescence.

Meanwhile, there is accumulating evidence that BPD features present in a similar

manner as in adults and may predict later diagnosis. In a research review, Miller and

colleagues (Miller et al., 2008) found that the prevalence, reliability and validity of BPD in

adolescence is both adequate and comparable to the disorder in adults. Further, research

shows that PDs in adolescents are not merely extreme, maladaptive traits of normal

personality but instead are best explained by dimensions of abnormal personality (Tromp

& Koot, 2010). Research such as the above further emphasizes the need to recognize that

features of BPD in adolescents are a valid occurrence and at the same time work on

defining and reliably assessing these early symptoms. Furthermore, these features appear

to be stable over time. In a large sample of 6-8 year old girls, impulsivity, negative

affectivity and interpersonal aggression, three of the core features of BPD were found to be

stable over time (Stepp, Pilkonis, Hipwell, Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2010).

Additional studies also point to the stability of BPD symptoms during adolescence

(Bornovalova, Hicks, Iacono & McGue, 2009; Stepp, Whalen, Scott, Zalewski, Loeber &

Hipwell, 2014); in one study involving female twin sets (Bornovalova, Hicks, Iacono &

McGue, 2013), exploring developmental trends revealed that BPD traits remained

relatively stable from age 14 to age 18. There also appears to be preliminary evidence that

this may be the case in at least one non-Western culture in preadolescence (Kawabada,

Youngblood & Hamaguchi, 2014), where BPD features were found to be relatively stable

in 9-11 year old Japanese children.

Moreover, it has been found that some personality characteristics, some of which

represent symptoms of BPD, are heritable (Torgersen, 2000), while brain scans have also

indicated deviant brain maturation processes in adolescent girls with BPD symptomatology

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 25: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

(Houston, Ceballos, Hesselbrock, & Bauer, 2005). A longitudinal study (Belsky et al.,

2012) using a large cohort of twins followed from age five to 12 years, found that 66% of

variance in Borderline Personality Related Characteristics (BPRC) could be accounted for

by genetic factors. Twin studies place the concordance rate between 35-37% (Kendler,

Aggen, Czajkowski, Roysamb, Tambs, et al., 2008; Torgersen, Czajkowski, Jacobson,

Reichborn-Kjennerud, Roysamb, et al. 2008; Reichborn-Kjennerud, Czajkowski,

Roysamb, Orstavik, Neale et al., 2010). Meanwhile, a large genetic study found an

association between a specific allele of a variant in the dopa decarboxylase gene which

catalyzes the synthesis of serotonin and dopamine (Mobascher, Bohus, Dahmen, Dietl,

Giegling et al., 2014), neurotransmitters that are heavily associated with features of BPD

such as mood regulation and impulsivity. Additionally, a study looking at DNA

methylation in BPD patients with severe childhood adversity and participants with major

depressive disorder without adversity, found that a specific site was associated with both

adversity and BPD and may represent a potential epigenetic pathway to the disorder

(Prados, Stenz, Courtet, Prada, Nicastro et al., 2015).

Interestingly, a recent review and meta-analysis of the genetic modulation of BPD

found insufficient evidence to support a direct relationship of the latter and several

neurotransmitter systems (Calati, Gressier, Balestri & Serretti, 2013). An additional review

also confirmed these findings and suggests a conceptual shift where research efforts should

focus on genetic effects on vulnerability to environmental causes, rather the genetic effects

on the disorder itself (Amad, Ramoz, Thomas, Jardri & Gorwood, 2014). However,

positive evidence was uncovered to support single BPD symptoms and neurobiological

systems, a finding which would make sense given the heterogeneity of the disorder (Calati

et al., 2013). Collectively, this growing body of research indicates that the genesis of BPD

is largely influenced by genetics, and more specifically gene and environment interactions

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 26: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

and correlations (see Amad et al., 2014, for a review). Thus, it is reasonable to assert that

BPD features are a reality in children, rendering childhood as the critical period for

purposes of intervention and treatment.

Attachment in BPD

As is evident from the discussion of the theoretical models on the development of

BPD, attachment issues are implicated in all three models discussed, and are believed to

play a significant role in the interpersonal difficulties faced by individuals diagnosed with

the disorder. While some research on attachment and BPD has been conducted over the

years, it has produced rather equivocal results and has yet to clarify the relationship

between the two. Thus, it is important to continue to include attachment in research

looking to uncover the pathways which lead to the development of BPD.

The basis for any discussion on this concept however, must refer to the theories of

Bowlby (1969, 1988). Bowlby postulated that through the interactions of mother and child,

the dyad develops an affective bond, namely attachment. He theorized further that the

caregiver’s responses serve as a working model for future relationships that is believed to

endure throughout childhood and into adulthood. Early research using a controlled

experiment to assess attachment styles gave way to the formulation of the following

categories: Secure attachment, Anxious/avoidant attachment and Anxious/resistant

attachment (Ainsworth et al., 1971; Ainsworth et al., 1978). These were later supplemented

with a fourth category (Main & Solomon, 1990) labeled disoriented-disorganized, after it

became apparent that not all attachment behaviours could fit into the aforementioned

categories. Since its conception, a plethora of research has been conducted revealing the

extensive effects of attachment style on a wide range of outcomes. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 27: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

It is widely accepted that attachment style has an important role with regards to

psychological well-being. Insecure attachment styles have been associated with anxiety in

children; with an ambivalent attachment style revealing the strongest relationship between

the two (see Colonessi, Draijer, Stams, Van der Bruggen, Bögels et al., 2011 for a review).

Anxious and avoidant styles of attachment have also been associated with emotion

regulation difficulties such as dysregulation and suppression of emotions (Brenning,

Soenens, Braet & Bosmans, 2012). Insecure attachment with both parents assessed in

infancy also appears to be associated with teacher-rated externalizing behaviour in later

childhood (Kochanska & Kim, 2013). Additionally, secure attachment is associated with

multiple factors of school adjustment (i.e. academic, emotional, social and behavioral),

with children with avoidant and disorganized attachment evidencing the poorest

adjustment (Granot & Mayseless, 2001).

Overall, attachment has figured prominently in wellbeing research where secure

attachments have been linked to higher levels of wellbeing (see Mikulincer & Shaver,

2007). A study investigating the relationship between attachment and wellbeing revealed

that a preoccupied (the equivalent of an anxious-ambivalent style in children) attachment

style in adulthood is related to the most adverse outcome (Karreman & Vingerhoets, 2012),

as it negatively predicted well-being. Overall, the evidence available currently indicates

that an anxious ambivalent (or disorganized) style of attachment is of most concern for

later functioning (Lyons-Ruth, 1996; O’Connor, Bureau, McCartney & Lyons-Ruth, 2011).

Research also indicates that individuals with disorganized attachment styles are at a higher

risk for both externalizing and internalizing disorders (Lecompte & Moss, 2014; Lyons-

Ruth & Jacobvitz, 2008; Zeanah, Keyes & Settles, 2003).

In relation to BPD, relevant literature on attachment most frequently implicates a

preoccupied pattern of attachment with borderline personality features, often referred to as

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 28: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

anxious-preoccupied with regards to adults and anxious-ambivalent where children are

concerned. A study conducted by Fonagy et al. (1996) found that the majority of

individuals with borderline features were found to have a preoccupied attachment with

parents, while almost half of patients with BPD based on the DSM criteria were also

fearfully preoccupied with traumatic events. Findings have also suggested that adolescents

with an anxious ambivalent attachment are at a greater risk for a number of affective

disorders, including BPD (Rosenstein & Horrowitz, 1996). Other studies both in

nonclinical and clinical populations have also associated borderline personality features

with attachment anxiety and preoccupied attachment patterns (e.g. Meyer et al., 2004;

Nickell, Waudby & Trull, 2002). Additionally, studies have also associated BPD features

with both preoccupied and fearful attachments (Deborde, Milkjovitch, Roy, Dugré- Le

Bigre, Pham-Scottez, Speranza & Corcos, 2012).

Meanwhile, a study by Aaronson, Bender, Skodol and Gunderson (2006) found that

patients with BPD, in comparison to those with obsessive compulsive personality disorder,

had higher mean total scores for patterns of angry withdrawal and compulsive care-

seeking, indicative of an anxious-ambivalent attachment pattern. This study is consistent

with earlier research revealing similar patterns (e.g. West & Sheldon-Keller, 1992). In a

2004 review of attachment studies regarding BPD, Agrawal and colleagues found that all

studies revealed an association between BPD diagnosis and insecure forms of attachment.

However, there was great variability in the relationships that were targeted, as well as in

the assessment measures (Agrawal et al., 2004). More recent literature, however, reveals a

much more complex pattern of relationships between attachment styles and BPD. In one of

the more recent studies (Barone, Fossati & Guiducci, 2011) the findings indicate that there

may be differences in attachment styles among BPD patients depending on differences in

comorbidity of Axis I disorders. The researchers included four comorbidity groups in the

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 29: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

study: anxiety/mood, substance use, alcohol use and eating disorders. The first group was

believed to relate to internalized emotion regulation difficulties, while the other three were

linked to externalization difficulties. It was revealed that a preoccupied attachment style

was found more frequently in adults with a BPD diagnosis who were also diagnosed with

either an anxiety or a mood disorder. Different attachment patterns also emerged based on

the other comorbidity groups. Similarly, in a recent study with a clinical sample of

adolescents with BPD (Ramos, Canta, de Castro & Leal, 2014), researchers found that in

the internalizing subgroup there were higher rates of preoccupied/ambivalent insecure and

avoidant insecure attachment style perceptions, while in the externalizing subgroup secure

attachment style perception was significantly more prevalent. Thus, the relationship

between BPD and attachment may be more complex than previously understood,

especially when there is comorbidity. As is evident from the above, there are some

discrepancies regarding whether or not there is a single insecure attachment style is

consistently associated with BPD. Thus, further research into attachment and BPD is

warranted, while keeping in mind the broad range of difficulties which may be included in

the disorder. It is important to investigate whether there are variations in the attachment

styles associated with BPD features in relation to non-clinical populations of adolescents, a

primary aim of the current study. Establishing this trajectory in adolescents will contribute

to the understanding the more complex relationships between BPD and attachment in

clinical populations of both adolescents and adults with the disorder.

Emotional Availability

Emotional availability is a concept that emerged within the domain of

developmental psychopathology and is rooted in both attachment theory and systemic

approaches to development, with a focus on emotional perspectives (Biringen &

Easterbrooks, 2012). Emotional availability refers to the emotional connection between a

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 30: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

dyad and their ability to enjoy a mutually fulfilling and healthy relationship. Although

there have been varying conceptualizations in the literature, emotional availability has

predominantly been associated with the level of parental responsiveness, sensitivity, and

emotional involvement (Biringen & Robinson, 1991; Lee & Gotlib, 1991). While deeply

rooted in attachment theory, Biringen and Robinson (1991) have argued that the emotional

availability describes an interdependent quality of relation between a parent and child. For

instance, EA focuses more on positive emotions (while including negative emotions) when

compared to work on attachment (Biringen & Easterbrooks, 2012). Support for the

construct was initially derived from the development of an observational system to

measure the emotional availability (EA scales) present during caregiver-child interactions

(Biringen, Robinson and Emde, 1990, 1993, 1998; Easterbrooks & Biringen, 2005). EA

includes dimensions of the construct such as parental sensitivity, parental structuring,

parental nonintrusiveness, and parental nonhostility, as well as child responsiveness to the

parent and child involvement of the parent in interaction. Subsequently, a self-report

questionnaire was developed in order to assess perceived EA in older children and includes

the dimensions of parental responsiveness, parental sensitivity and emotional involvement

(Lum, 1999).

Research investigating observed emotional availability reveals that higher levels of

parental emotional availability have been associated with secure infant–parent attachment

(Bretherton, 2000) as well as a higher level of infants’ attention toward their parent

(Volling, McElwain, Notaro, & Herrera, 2002). Moreover, research shows that children of

sensitive mothers are more often securely attached and children of less sensitive mothers

are more often insecurely attached (Sagi, Koren-Karie, Gini, Ziv, & Joels, 2002; Ziv,

Aviezer, Gini, Sagi, & Koren-Karie, 2000). Research with mothers of 4 year olds also

revealed that parental stress was related to less favorable EA scores during their

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 31: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

interactions with their children (McMahon & Meins, 2012). Results from a longitudinal

study (Easterbrooks, Bureau & Lyons-Ruth, 2012), revealed that specific aspects of

emotional availability are related to emotional and behavior difficulties in children. More

specifically, externalizing behaviors and total behavior problem scores, as rated by

teachers, were associated with EA insensitivity and passive/withdrawn behavior.

Meanwhile, child depressive symptoms at age eight were associated with maternal

insensitivity and maternal hostility. Results from an additional study also confirm that

more positive perceptions of parental emotional availability are associated with lower

levels of externalizing behaviours (White & Renk, 2012).

Given the theoretical background of BPD, which emphasizes emotional aspects of

the parent-child relationship, such as an emotionally invalidating environment, the

construct of EA seems particularly pertinent. Despite the relevance of EA to theories on

the development of BPD, there appears to be no research investigating the relationship

between perceived parental EA and increased risk for BPD. One small study, however,

looked at maternal BPD with regards to observed EA towards their pre-school aged

children (Macfie, Coens, Fitzpatrick, Frankel, McCollum et al. (2007). The results,

although limited by the sample size, indicate that mothers with BPD are less emotionally

available. It is reasonable to hypothesize that parental difficulties in EA, irrespective of

their cause, may lead to emotion regulation difficulties in their offspring and thus a higher

risk of developing BPD. Research does in fact reveal preliminary support for the

connection between EA and emotion regulation – the core difficulty in BPD. A study

which looked at emotion regulation and reactivity in 12-month olds, indicated that aspects

of EA (i.e. maternal hostility) are related to emotion dysregulation, above and beyond

temperamental factors (Little & Carter, 2005). More importantly, due to possible

interaction effects of temperamental and environmental factors on the development of

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 32: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

BPD, looking at perceived EA in older children may offer new insight. As studies

involving perceived parental EA are scarce, further research is needed to investigate its

involvement in psychological wellbeing, particularly regarding emerging BPD features in

late childhood and adolescence.

Emotional intelligence

The roots of emotional intelligence (EI) can be traced to as early as 1920 when

Thorndike conceptualized what he referred to as social intelligence. This work was later

expanded by Gardner (1983, p.8) who spoke of multiple, relatively autonomous

intelligences, which included personal intelligence, comprised of interpersonal and

intrapersonal intelligences. These early theories, however, lacked sufficient empirical

support and adopted a very broad view of EI. In fact, the first definition and model of EI

was not introduced until 1990 (Mayer, Di Paolo & Salovey, 1990) and was later

popularized by Goleman (1995). The researchers defined emotional intelligence as “[..] a

subset of social intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and others’

feelings and emotions, to discriminate among them and to use this information to guide

one’s thinking and actions’ (Salovey & Mayer, 1990, p.189). Further, it was suggested that

there are distinct mental processes concerning emotions which include the following: 1.

Appraisal and expression of emotions, 2. Emotion regulation, 3. Utilization of emotions.

Their later revised model (Mayer & Salovey, 1997) placed greater emphasis on thinking

about emotions and asserted that EI should be considered a set of abilities grounded in

cognition. The latest model also resulted in the development of a measure for the cognitive

based EI (Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, MSCEIT; Mayer, Salovey

& Caruso, 2002). Similarly, Goleman (1995) proposed a model of EI which he defined as

“[…] a meta-ability, determining how well we can use whatever other skills we have,

including raw intellect” (Goleman, 1995, p.36). While he recognizes the influence of traits

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 33: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

such as temperament, Goleman emphasizes that skills included within EI can and should

be taught.

A major departure from the traditional view of EI came in the form of Bar-On’s

research which viewed EI as a disposition or affect rather than conceptualizing it from a

cognitive perspective (1997). Instead, EI was defined as “[…] an array of non-cognitive

capabilities, competences and skills that influence one’s ability to succeed in coping with

environmental demands and pressures” (Bar-On, 1997, p.14). Specifically, the model is

believed to reflect the potential for performance or achievement rather than performance

itself (Bar-On, 1997; Bar-On, Brown, Kirkcaldy & Thome, 2000). Meanwhile, the

researcher was also the first to develop what he claimed to be a non-cognitive measure of

EI, namely the Emotional Quotient Inventory (Bar-On, 1997).

Given the growing trend in research involving EI, an increasing number of

measures were developed to assess the construct. However, these attempts largely

overlooked the difference between typical and maximum performance (e.g. Ackerman &

Heggestad, 1997; Hofstee, 2001). As Cronbach first distinguished in 1949, typical

performance, such as personality, is primarily measured through self-report questionnaires,

while maximum performance tests, which claim to measure ability, should include correct

and incorrect responses to items and tasks. In light of this, Petrides and Furnham (2000)

argued for the operationalization of two distinct constructs, trait EI and ability EI on the

basis that measurement is central to the operationalization of a construct (Perez, Petrides &

Furnham, 2005). Consequently, trait EI is a construct measured via self-report and

encompasses self-perceptions regarding facets of emotion, while ability EI, measured via

maximum performance tests assesses emotion-related cognitive abilities (Petrides, 2011).

The distinction between the two has consistently found support in research (e.g.

Freudenthaler & Neubauer, 2007; Martins, Ramalho & Morin, 2010).

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 34: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Research investigating EI, both ability and trait, has flourished in recent decades.

Regarding trait EI, there is accumulating evidence that the construct has incremental

validity over personality traits and other factors and it affects a range of domains and

important outcomes (e.g. Petrides & Furnham, 2003; Schutte, et al. 2001; Van der Zee,

Schakel & Thijs, 2002; Wong & Law; 2002). One study (Petrides, Gonzalez-Perez &

Furnham, 2007) found that high trait EI was a predictor of coping styles and the inverse

was significantly related to dysfunctional attitudes. High trait EI has also consistently been

associated with romantic relationship satisfaction (for a review see Malouff, Schutte &

Thorsteinsson, 2014) and academic performance (Perera & DiGiacomo, 2013). Moreover,

low trait EI scores have been associated with higher levels of neuroticism (Dawda & Hart,

2000) and found to be a significant predictor of both internalizing and externalizing

problems (Reker & Parker, 1999). Research also reveals a relationship between ability EI,

mental health and other measures of adjustment (see Brackett, Rivers & Salovey, 2011 for

a review). In a study investigating academic achievement, ability EI scores were positively

associated with academic achievement indices, and added to the incremental variance in

explaining academic success (Lanciano & Curci, 2014). In a study conducted with

university students, lower scores on both trait and ability EI measures were associated with

more binge drinking episodes and alcohol related problems, while interestingly trait EI

mediated the relationship between the level of the latter and ability EI (Schutte, Malouff &

Hine, 2011).

Research investigating EI and BPD specifically is surprisingly sparse, despite

evidence to suggest that the relationship between the two would be meaningful. Emotional

dysregulation has been found to have a central role in most forms of psychopathology (e.g.

Philippot & Feldman, Eds, 2004), and is at the core of BPD (e.g. Crowell, Beauchaine &

Linehan, 2009, Farell & Shaw, 1994). Kuo & Linehan (2009) found that individuals

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 35: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

diagnosed with BPD had a higher baseline emotion intensity based on electrophysiological

measurements as well as self-report measures. In addition, individuals suffering from BPD

seem to have poorer emotion perception but also increased sensitivity to emotional stimuli

(e.g. Wolff, Stiglmayr, Bretz, Lammers & Auckenthaler, 2007). Others have suggested that

emotional regulation difficulties, a core aspect of trait EI, may be one pathway through

which traumatic or stressful experiences, increase the risk of developing BPD symptoms

(Fernando, Beblo, Schlosser, Terfehr, Otte et al., 2014).

Given the above, there have been several studies which have looked at BPD and EI

in recent years. A study which utilized the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence

Test (MSCEIT; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2000), an ability EI measure, found that female

BPD patients scored lower on overall EI, and lower on emotional regulation and

understanding emotional information (Hertel, Schutz & Lammers, 2009). In an

investigation of emotion facets in children, using the affective dysfunction subscale of the

Coolidge Personality and Neuropsychological Inventory for Children (CPNI; Coolidge,

2005) and Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997), Gratz and

colleagues (2009) found that affective dysfunction and disinhibition were significantly

associated with BPD features in children aged 9-13. Meanwhile, research on trait EI and

BPD features is also limited, with only a handful of studies investigating the relationship

between the two. A study by Sinclair and Feigenbaum (2012) found that trait EI was low in

those with a BPD diagnosis and was the strongest predictor of BPD symptomatology.

Research conducted by Gardner and Qualter (2009) found that in non-clinical adults, BPD

symptoms were associated with trait EI and particularly affective instability and identity

disturbance as measured by the borderline (BOR) Scale of the Personality Assessment

Inventory (PAI-BOR; Morey, 1991), and the DSM-IV (APA, 2000) criterion of emptiness.

Meanwhile, trait EI has also been found to be inversely related to BPD symptoms in a non-

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 36: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

clinical sample of college students (Gaher, Hofman, Simons & Hunsaker 2013). While not

directly related to BPD, in a sample of non-clinical adolescents, high trait EI was found to

be negatively associated with self-harm, a relationship mediated by choice of emotional

coping strategies (Mikolajczak, Petrides & Hurry, 2009). Moreover, a study investigating

the relationship between trait EI and symptoms of nine PDs in a nonclinical sample, found

that trait EI was a significant negative predictor of BPD symptomatology (Petrides, Pérez-

Gonzalez & Furnham, 2007). The results of a study by Leible and Snell (2004), which

investigated the link between aspects of EI and PDs, using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale

(TMMS; Salovey, Mayer & Goldman, 1995) found that BPD features in a non-clinical

sample were associated with poor emotional clarity and emotional repair.

Preliminary research available on EI and BPD indicates that further research is

warranted in this area, particularly as it may aid in providing a basis for identifying

individuals at risk. While there is preliminary evidence associating both trait EI and ability

EI and elements of BPD, there is also sufficient evidence to suggest that the former holds

more promise. An earlier review article by Schutte and colleagues (2007) indicates that

trait EI is more strongly associated with mental health than ability EI. Since then, trait EI

has consistently been found to have a more robust relationship with psychopathology than

ability EI (Davis & Humphrey, 2012a; Davis & Humphrey, 2012b; Gardner & Qualter,

2010; Williams, Daley, Burnside & Hammond, 2009). Meanwhile, ability EI and its

measurement have received a fair amount of criticism (see Fiori, Antonietti, Mikolajczak,

Luminet, Hansenne et al., 2014; Siegling, Saklofske & Petrides, 2015). More importantly,

the construct of trait EI is more relevant to an investigation of BPD, as well as PDs in

general, in that the interest lies not in the cognitive abilities related to emotion but the more

enduring self-perceptions. According to Petrides et al (2007) trait EI is a constellation of

emotion-related self-perceptions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies, and

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 37: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

is thus more pertinent to personality psychopathology. As others have suggested (see

Gardner, Qualter & Whiteley, 2011) self-perceptions are likely to be influenced strongly

by temperament, the latter being implicated in theories of the development of BPD (i.e.

emotional vulnerability; Linehan, 1993). Additionally, Gardner et al (2011) found that

temperament characteristics are related to trait but not EI ability. Based on the above

research, this study will investigate the relationship between trait EI and BPD.

Chapter 3: The Present Study

Introduction

Given the role of emotion in BPD and preliminary evidence linking trait EI and

BPD, further research is warranted regarding the relationship between the two. To the

author’s knowledge there are only a couple of studies in which trait EI was investigated

with regards to BPD specifically (Gardner & Qualter, 2009; Leible & Snell, 2004). Both

studies however, utilized trait EI measures that do not fully encompass the trait EI domain

(see Gardner & Qualter, 2009; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). A third study however,

indicates that there is strong preliminary evidence that trait EI may be useful in research

with PDs (Petrides, Pérez-Gonzalez & Furnham, 2007). While all three studies show

encouraging results, none of these have looked into trait EI and BPD features in children or

adolescents. Assessing trait EI in children and adolescents may be an efficient manner in

which to identify difficulties and provide timely intervention without the stigma associated

with other assessments related to psychopathology. Additionally, such investigations may

shed light on the complexities of the emotional deficits encountered in individuals with

BPD. With regards to attachment and BPD, it is clear from the research to date, that the

findings are equivocal (see West & Sheldon-Keller, 1992; Barone, Fossati & Guiducci,

2011). Thus, continuing research into attachment styles and BPD and trying to elucidate

the relationship between the two is an important task which will help inform future risk

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 38: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

assessment and intervention efforts. Finally, emotional availability is a concept which

appears to be relevant to difficulties associated with BPD, yet research in this area is

severely limited. Perceived emotional availability of parents seems particularly pertinent

when one considers the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) criterion of ‘Frantic efforts to avoid real or

imagined abandonment’, as well as aspects of the theories for the development of BPD

(Fonagy et al, 2011; Kernberg, 1967; Linehan, 1993). Further investigating the emotional

availability of parents from the perspective of adolescents with regards to BPD features

seems a worthwhile endeavor.

The present study aims to address the above discrepancies and research gaps by

looking into trait EI, attachment and emotional availability in relation to features of BPD in

adolescents. To fully investigate the relationship between trait EI and BPD features the

current study will explore the full domain of the former. This will enable a closer look at

specific facets of trait EI as they relate to BPD features. To the author’s knowledge, this

will also be the first study to research this with a sample of adolescents. Moreover, this

study will add to research looking at attachment patterns and BPD characteristics in

adolescents, while also extending previous research by investigating whether there are any

moderation effects of attachment regarding the potential relationship between trait EI and

BPD features. Furthermore, the relationship between perceived emotional availability of

parents with regards to BPD has yet to be explored and the inclusion of this construct in

the current study may lead to further research in this area. Additionally, the study employs

a cross-sectional design which will enable a closer look at how the relationships between

the constructs mentioned above may present at different points in adolescence.

To this end, the present study will examine several hypotheses which include trait

EI, attachment, perceived emotional availability and BPD features. Hypothesis 1 will look

at the relationship between global trait EI and BPD features. It is hypothesized that low

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 39: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

trait EI will be associated with higher scores on BPD features. Further, hypothesis 2, states

that child trait EI indicators (factors) will predict BPD features in both age groups, with the

relationship being stronger for 15 year olds. Additionally, it is hypothesized that low

parental trait EI will be associated with low child trait EI (hypothesis 3). Hypothesis 4 will

investigate specific facets of trait EI in relation to BPD features. It is hypothesized that the

trait EI facets of emotion management, emotion regulation, impulsiveness (low),

relationships and stress management will have a stronger relationship to BPD features.

Hypothesis 5 involves the relationship between attachment and BPD features. It is

hypothesized that BPD features will be higher for insecure attachment styles, with the

strongest relationship between anxious-ambivalent attachment and BPD features.

Hypothesis 6 posits that the relationship between trait EI and BPD features will be

moderated by attachment. Hypothesis 7 will investigate the relationship between perceived

emotional availability and BPD features. It is hypothesized that low perceived emotional

availability will be associated with higher scores for BPD features. Further, hypothesis 8

posits that perceived emotional availability will act as a mediator between trait EI and BPD

features. It is also hypothesized that lower parent trait EI will be associated with lower

child trait EI and lower EA (hypothesis 9).

Method

Procedure

Following approval by the Cyprus Bioethics Committee, permission to approach

schools for pupil participation was obtained by the Ministry of Education. Schools were

selected randomly from major districts in Cyprus, after they had been separated into rural

and urban in order to ensure that the selection was representative with regards to other

factors. Letters describing the study were sent to parents in envelopes given to the children

(Appendix A). Aside from a clear description of the study, the content emphasizes the

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 40: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

voluntary nature of participation as well as the right to drop out of the study at any time.

Parents were also assured of the confidentiality and anonymity of the information

provided.

Participants

The current study included a sample of 780 adolescents, 291 in junior high and 489

in high school and their parents (N=266). Mean age and gender for each level of education

can be found in Table 1 (Appendix B). Adolescent participants were recruited from 12

schools in Cyprus, seven junior high schools (two rural) and five high schools. All

adolescent participants were freshman in either junior high or high school. In the complete

case sample, parents of junior high participants (N=90) had a mean age of 42.80 (SD=

6.12), of which 40 were college educated. Parents of high school participants (N=96) had a

mean age of 45.50, and 47 parents reported they received a college education.

Measures

Trait EI. Trait EI in parents was measured using the TEIQue (v.1.50) (Appendix C). The

TEIQue is a self-administered measure which includes a total of 153 items, and assesses

‘emotional self-perceptions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies’ (Petrides,

Pita & Kokkinaki, 2007). The Likert-type questionnaire takes 25 minutes to complete and

covers a total of 15 trait EI facets (see Table 2, Appendix D). Meanwhile, there are four

trait EI factors: well-being, self-control, emotionality and sociability. When scored, the

TEIQue (v.1.50) yields a single global score, four factor scores on 15 scores – one for each

subscale.

The TEIQue questionnaire (full form), has been used extensively in research over

the past years and has demonstrated good reliability and validity (Petrides, 2006; Petrides,

2009b). In independent research which has been conducted (Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 41: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

& Roy, 2007), the majority of the instrument’s subscales demonstrated good reliability,

with Cronbach’s a ranging from acceptable to excellent (α = .71 - .91) among both men

and women, while the remainder of the subscales evidenced acceptable to modest internal

consistencies. Internal consistencies for global scores for both males and females (α = .94,

α = .95 respectively) and at the factor levels were found to be excellent. Meanwhile,

evidence of the four factor structure of the instrument, as well as reliability data, has also

been replicated in other countries (Freudenthaler, Neubauer, Gabler, & Sherl, 2008;

Mikolajczak et al., 2007). Research has also confirmed that trait EI is a personality

construct, as opposed to a form of intelligence, and thus is associated with personality and

not measures of intelligence (Petrides & Furhnam, 2001, 2003; Van Rooy, Viswesvaran, &

Pluta, 2005). There is also accumulating evidence of the incremental validity of the

measure (Mikolajczak et al., 2007; Petrides, Pérez-González, & Furnham, 2007; Petrides,

Pita et al., 2007) and its overall predictive utility (see Andrei, Mancini, Baldaro, Trombini

& Agnoli, 2014 for a recent review).

The TEIQue (v.1.50) was also used to measure trait EI in adolescents. While the

researcher has also developed a modified version, with regards to wording and syntactic

complexity, for use with adolescents – the TEIQue-Adolescent Full Form (TEIQue-AFF;

Petrides, 2009a) - the full measure has not yet been translated into the Greek language.

Although the TEIQue Adolescent Short Form is currently available in Greek, it provides

only a global trait EI score which would not be sufficient for the scope of this study.

During communication with the main author of the instrument (Petrides, K.V.), it has been

indicated that simplifying the TEIQue (v.150) for use with adolescents is a straightforward

process. Thus, necessary changes have been made to the adult version for use with the

adolescent participants of the study (Appendix E). Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 42: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Permission to use the TEIQue instruments was obtained from the primary author

(Petrides, K.V.). The TEIQue is scored by the London Psychometric Lab at University

College London, after which reports are sent to the researcher.

Attachment. Attachment styles (ATT) were measured using the Attachment Styles

Classification Questionnaire (ASCQ; Finzi et al., 1996) which is an adaptation for children

of the Hebrew version (Mikulincer et al.,1990) of the Attachment Questionnaire for adults

(Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The questionnaire is suitable for use with children 7-14 years old,

but does not however, exclude the use with slightly older children (in this case 15-16 year

olds). The ASCQ is comprised of 15 statements which tap into three attachment patterns:

Secure, Anxious/Ambivalent and Avoidant (see Appendix F). This self-report

questionnaire includes items such as “I make friends with other children easily”,

“Sometimes I am afraid that other kids won’t want to be with me” and responses are

recorded on a five point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (All wrong) to 5 (Very right).

Children are assigned an attachment style based on which category they score the highest

in. The ASCQ takes approximately 5 minutes to complete. The measure was translated and

back-translated by two bilingual individuals (the author and a Clinical Psychology Phd

student) for use in this study.

Data on the psychometric qualities of the instrument have proven thus far to be

satisfactory. The original article, which is in Hebrew, reports some psychometric properties

(taken from the abstract and future articles), including the internal consistency for the

scales which ranged from .69-.81. Furthermore, two-week test-retest reliability was

reported as ranging from .87-.95. Meanwhile, a later study (Al-Yagon & Mikulincer, 2004)

reported internal consistency values ranging from .64-.73 for the three factors.

Emotional availability. To assess the perceived emotional availability (EA) of parents, the

Lum Emotional Availability of Parents (LEAP; Lum, 1999) was administered to

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 43: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

adolescents in the study (Appendix G). LEAP is a single-domain, 15-item instrument

which was designed to assess children and adolescents’ perceptions of the emotional

availability of their parents or caregivers. This subjective construct encompasses

perceptions regarding parental responsiveness, sensitivity and emotional involvement. The

instrument is appropriate for use which children and adolescents (4th grade – 12th grade)

and young adults (college students) and was designed to measure the emotional availability

of both parents (LEAPm and LEAPf).

The LEAP is a self-report instrument containing 15 items and takes approximately

2 minutes to complete. Items are scored on a 6 point Likert-type scale ranging from 1

(never) to 6 (always). Available research regarding reliability and validity has produced

positive results. Internal consistency was reported for young adults (n=220; mothers a=.98;

fathers a=.98), a nonclinical sample of children and adolescents (n=635; mothers a=.96;

fathers a=.97), a clinical sample of children and adolescents (n=110; mothers a=.93; fathers

a=.95) (Lum & Phares, 2005). An additional study which aimed to validate the measure in

an Italian sample (Babore, Picconi, Candelori & Trumello, 2014) also report satisfactory

data for the internal consistency of the measure, for both reports on mother (a = .93) and

father reports (a = .95) and all item-correlations. Similar findings are also reported in a

study by White & Renk (2012). The study by Babore et al. (2014) also reports a significant

positive correlation of LEAPm with the Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment (IPPA)

for mothers (r = .85, p < .001) and LEAPp with IPPA for fathers (r = .82, p <.001),

indicative of the measure’s convergent validity. Short term temporal stability was also

reported for the nonclinical sample (r=.81, p<.001 for reports for mothers and r=.76,

p<.001 for reports for fathers) (Lum & Phares, 2005). Meanwhile, a principal component

analysis (varimax rotation) using data from a sample of older adolescents (n=155) resulted

in a one-factor solution, where reports from mothers and fathers were analyzed separately.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 44: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

The one-factor solution accounted for 95% and 94% of the explained variance (Lum &

Phares, 2005). The one factor solution was also supported in the Babore et al study (2014).

Additional iterated principal axis factor analysis was performed with three other samples

which resulted in the same one-factor solution accounting for 96%, 93% and 95% of the

explained variance for the mother’s perceived emotional availability and 97%, 94% and

97% of the explained variance for the father’s emotional availability (Lum & Phares,

2005). Finally, there is evidence of both convergent and divergent validity of the measure

after correlations were found in the predicted direction between LEAP scores and other

instruments measuring both similar and dissimilar constructs.

To date, the LEAP has been translated and validated in a study with an Italian

population (Babore et al., 2014) which pointed to the validity of the instrument. It is a brief

instrument that uses simple language and does not appear to include items which are

heavily influenced by cultural considerations. The instrument has been translated into

Greek and then back-translated for use in this study by two bilingual Clinical Psychology

Phd students.

Borderline personality disorder features. Borderline Personality Disorder Features

(BPDF) were assessed using the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children

(BPFSC; Crick, Murray-Close & Woods, 2005). The BPFSC is a self-report instrument

that assesses borderline personality features among children and adolescents aged 9 and

older (Crick et al., 2005) and is based on the PAI-BOR (Morey, 1991). The BPFSC

assesses the four domains found in the PAI, specifically affective instability, identity

problems, interpersonal problems and self-harm, using age appropriate terms. It is a 24

item instrument where items are responded to on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not

at all true) to 5 (always true). Children report on their affective instability (six items; e.g.,

‘My feelings are very strong. For instance, when I get mad, I get really, really mad. When I

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 45: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

get happy, I get really, really happy’), identity problems (six items; e.g., ‘I feel that there is

something important missing about me, but I don’t know what it is’), negative

relationships (six items; e.g., ‘I’ve picked friends who have treated me badly’), and self-

harm (six items; e.g., ‘I get into trouble because I do things without thinking’). Items are

summed up to yield a global score – i.e. the scales are not recommended for use as stand-

alone constructs, according to the developers of the scale. As the BPFSC is not currently

available in Greek, the measure was translated from English to Greek and then back-

translated by two bilingual Clinical Psychology Phd students, for use in this study

(Appendix H).

Research on the BPFSC supports that the measure has good internal consistency

(Cronbach α > .76) (Crick et al., 2005; Hawes, Helyer, Herlianto & Willing, 2013), as well

as construct validity (Crick et al., 2005). A study by Chang and colleagues (2011) found

similar results, including good overall internal consistency (Cronbach α = .89) as well as

moderate to high levels of internal consistency for the measure’s subscales (Self-Harm α =

.86; Affective Instability α =.72; Negative Relationships α = .65). The BPFSC has also

demonstrated high accuracy in discriminating adolescents with a diagnosis of BPD in an

inpatient population, providing preliminary support for the criterion validity of the measure

(Chang, Sharp & Ha, 2011).

Chapter 4: Results

Preliminary analyses

Missing data

The initial sample consisted of 780 adolescents, 291 in junior high (mean age =

12.65) and 489 in high school (mean age = 15.47). Participants with more than 15%

missing data were deleted from the sample (N=118). Additionally, respondents who had

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 46: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

completed the measures in a non-serious manner, such as responding to items randomly i.e.

circling one answer throughout, were also removed from the sample (N=40) . This resulted

in a sample of 622 adolescents. To investigate the type of missing data in the sample,

missing Values Analysis (MVA; SPSS, version 20) was performed on the data, including

Little’s MCAR test (Little, 1988).

Little's MCAR test was performed on the sample (N = 622) and proved to be

significant for all measures: BPFSC, χ2 (364, N=622) = 442.03, p < .01; ASCQ, χ2 (326,

N=622) = 739.33, p < .001; LEAPm, χ2 (137, N= 622) = 239.00, p <.001, and for the

LEAPF, χ2 (233, N=622) = 483.01. Obtaining significant results for Little’s MCAR test,

indicates that the data were not missing completely at random (Little, 1988), and thus

multiple imputation is recommended as a suitable method to replace missing values, while

minimizing bias introduced when using other methods. Multiple imputation was thus

performed on the data via SPSS. This resulted in six datasets; the original dataset and five

imputation sets. Missing data analysis was performed separately for the TEIQue (v.1.50) as

scoring for the measure is performed by the London Psychometric Laboratory at

University College London. Participants with more than 15% missing data were deleted

from the sample prior to scoring. Missing data for the TEIQue (v.1.50) were then replaced

with the median of nearby points. This resulted in a final, complete case, sample of 555

adolescents and 248 parents.

Multivariate normality

Prior to addressing the main hypotheses of the study, data was screened for

multivariate normality. Regarding multivariate normality, an analysis of standard residuals

was carried out, which showed the data contained no outliers (Std. Residual Min = -3.16,

Std. Residual Max = 3.03). Multicollinearity was also not an issue in the sample, indicated

by the VIF values for all of the main variables and the trait EI factors which were all well

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 47: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

below 3 (see table 3, Appendix I). Meanwhile, for the TEIQue subscales, multicollinearity

was also not an issue, as all VIF values were found to be well below 3. The data also met

the assumption of independence of errors (Durbin-Watson value = 2.075).

Descriptives

Mean scores, as well as minimum and maximum scores, for the BPFSC, and

LEAPm, LEAPf and global trait EI, for junior high and high school participants, can be

found in Tables 4 and 5 (Appendix J) respectively. Mean scores for the BPFSC were

slightly higher than those reported in prior research, but comparable (see Crick et al, 2005;

Hawes, Helyer, Herlianto & Willing, 2013), particularly since the mean age in this study

was higher than others. Mean scores and standard deviations for the TEIQue subscales for

junior high participants (N = 196) and high school participants (N = 354) can be found in

Table 6 (Appendix K) and Table 7 (Appendix L), respectively. Mean scores and standard

deviations for the TEIQue subscales for parents can be found in Table 8 (Appendix M).

Reliability

Cronbach’s alpha was computed in order to investigate the internal consistency of

the measures. For the BPFSC, reliability for the scale was moderately high, Cronbach’s α =

.80 (N = 622). As previously noted, the researchers (Crick et al., 2005) recommend using

only global scores, despite the fact that the measure is based on the PAI-BOR and its four

subscales: Affective instability, Identity problems, Negative relationships and Self-harm.

Although the researchers have not reported specific reasons for the latter, others have noted

that the subscales have not yet been validated (Chang et al., 2011). Nonetheless,

Cronbach’s alpha was computed for the subscales and found to be low for all four:

Affective instability, α = .45; Identity problems, α = .55; Negative relationships, α = .54;

Self-harm, α = .66.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 48: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

For the ASCQ, reliability was low for the subscales Secure, Cronbach’s α = .43 and

Avoidant, α = .36. However, for the subscale Anxious-Ambivalent Cronbach’s alpha was

acceptable, α = .70. Regarding the LEAP measure, Cronbach’s alpha was excellent for

both mothers, α = .95, and fathers, α = .96. For the TEIQue, Cronbach’s alpha for the

complete scale in the sample was excellent, α = .90 and for the subscales Cronbach’s alpha

ranged from .53 - .84 (see table 9, Appendix N).

Factor Analysis

For the purpose of investigating the factor structure of the ASCQ, a principal

component analysis (PCA) was conducted on the 15 items of the measure, with oblique

rotation (direct oblimin). The factor solution (N=622) indicated that there were five factors,

based on Kaiser’s criterion of eigenvalues above 1. Upon examining the correlations

between factors, it was revealed that the highest correlation was .22, well below the

recommended threshold for orthogonal rotation (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Thus, it was

decided to proceed with PCA with orthogonal rotation.

PCA was conducted with orthogonal rotation (varimax). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin

measure verified the sampling adequacy for the analysis, KMO = .77 and with the

exception of one item, all KMO values for individual items were above the acceptable

criterion of .5. Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2 (105) = 1362.06, p < .001 indicated that

correlations between items were sufficiently large for PCA. Five components had

eigenvalues above Kaiser’s criterion of 1; however, one component only marginally

reached that value. Parallel analysis (Horn, 1965) was performed using SPSS syntax

(O’Connor, 2000) to decide the number of factors to retain. According to the results, only

four factors were retained. Based on the reliability analyses and the PCA results, it was

decided that only one of the factors would be retained for further analyses. Factor loadings

for all items of the ASCQ can be found in Table 10 (Appendix O). The factor was

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 49: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

composed of all items identified by the authors (Finzi et al., 1996) as reflecting an anxious

ambivalent attachment style, with the exception of two additional items: item 2, which

prior research on the instrument has indicated that it loads on the factor identified as

avoidant and item 3 which typically loads on the factor identified as secure.

Principal axis factoring (PAF) with Direct Oblimin (oblique) rotation for the 15

items of the LEAPm was conducted on the sample (N=622). An examination of the Kaiser-

Meyer Olkin measure of sampling adequacy suggested that the sample was factorable

(KMO=.96). Factor loadings can be found in Table 11 (Appendix P). The results revealed

a single factor, which explained 55.20% of the variance. For the LEAPf, KMO=.97, the

factor solution also revealed one factor which explained 62.40% of the variance. Prior

research using the LEAP with both PAF and principal components analysis (Lum &

Phares, 2005) has also indicated a one factor solution for the LEAP, for both mothers and

fathers.

PAF with Direct Oblimin (oblique) rotation was also performed on the 24 items of

the BPFSC. The Kaiser-Meyer Olkin measure verified the sampling adequacy for the

analysis (KMO=.86). The results of the PAF revealed a seven factor solution (for factor

loadings see Table 12, Appendix Q). Parallel analysis via SPSS syntax (O’Connor, 2000)

also indicated that seven factors should be retained. Based on the reliability results and the

factor solution in this sample, in addition to the recommendations of the authors (Crick et

al., 2005), it was decided that only global BPFS-C scores would be used in this study.

Correlations

Correlations between the main variables in the study and the four trait EI factors

can be found for junior high participants in Table 13 (Appendix R) and high school

participants in Table 14 (Appendix S).

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 50: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Main results

Hypothesis 1

To examine the first hypothesis of the study, that low trait EI would be associated

with higher scores on the BPDF, a Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was

computed to assess the relationship between trait EI global and BPDF scores separately for

each education level. There was a non-significant correlation of r = -.06 (p = n.s.) between

trait EI and BPDF scores in the junior high sample (N=196), and for the high school

participants a non-significant correlation of r = .01 (p=n.s.). Thus, hypothesis 1 was not

supported.

Hypothesis 2

To test the hypothesis that trait EI factors would predict BPD features, standard

multiple regression was performed with BPDF as the criterion variable and the four trait EI

factors as predictors, separately for each education level. For junior high participants, none

of the four trait EI factors were significant predictors of BPDF scores: wellbeing, Β = -.11,

n.s.; self-control, Β = .03, n.s.; emotionality, Β = -.02, n.s. and sociability, Β = .04, n.s. The

overall fit for the model was R2 =.01 (Adjusted R2 = -.01). The same was also true for

high school participants: wellbeing, Β = .05, n.s.; self-control, Β = .05, n.s.; emotionality,

Β = -.10, n.s. and sociability, Β = .02, n.s. The overall fit for the model was R2 = .08

(Adjusted R2 = .01). Therefore, hypothesis 2 was unsupported.

Hypothesis 3

In order to investigate the hypothesis that low parental trait EI will be associated

with low child trait EI, a Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was computed to

assess the relationship between the two. The complete sample of children’s scores matched

with parental scores was utilized (N=193). There was a non-significant correlation of r

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 51: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

=.12 (p= n.s.) between parental trait EI global scores and child trait EI global scores for all

participants (regardless of education level). Correlation analyses were also repeated based

on education level. For junior high participants, the correlation was also non-significant, r

= .13 (p= n.s.), N= 93. For high school participants, the correlation was also non-

significant, r = .09 (p=n.s.), N=100. Thus, this hypothesis was not met.

Hypothesis 4

The fourth hypothesis stated that certain trait EI facets, namely those of emotion

management, emotion regulation, impulsiveness (low), relationships and stress

management would be stronger predictors of BPDF scores, than other subscales. To

explore this hypothesis, standard multiple regression was performed with BPDF as the

dependent variable and the TEIQue subscales as predictors, separately for each education

level. The samples were deemed to be sufficient in size based on Tabachnick and Fidell’s

(2007) recommendations for testing individual predictors with multiple regression.

Correlation coefficients for the relationship between TEIQue subscales and BPDF for

junior high and high school participants can be found in Table 15 (Appendix T) and Table

16 (Appendix U). For junior high participants (N=196), none of the subscales were

significant predictors of BPDF (see table 17, Appendix V). For high school participants

(N=359), only emotion expression was a significant predictor of BPDF, where the

regression coefficient was -.1.96 (95% CI = -3.46 - -.46). Regression coefficients for all

subscales can be found in Table 17 (Appendix V) Therefore, although one of the subscales

was a significant predictor of BPDF scores, the results did not support hypothesis 4.

Hypothesis 5

Hypothesis 5 predicted that BPD features would be higher for insecure attachment

styles, with the strongest relationship between anxious-ambivalent attachment and BPD

features. Given the poor reliability of two out of three attachment style scores explored in

the study, as well as the factor analysis results, it was not possible to fully explore this

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 52: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

model. However, anxious ambivalent attachment scores were explored in relation to BPDF

in additional analyses that were performed on the sample.

Hypothesis 6

Hypothesis 6, postulated that the relationship between trait EI and BPD features

would be moderated by attachment. Since the relationship between trait EI and BPD was

not supported in the previous models, with the exception of the emotion expression

subscale in high school participants, the possibility of any moderation effects was

excluded. Thus, hypothesis 6 was not explored.

Hypothesis 7

It was hypothesized that low perceived emotional availability would be associated

with higher scores for BPDF – hypothesis 7. In order to investigate this hypothesis, a

Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was computed to assess the relationship

between perceived emotional availability and BPDF scores, based on education level. For

junior high participants (N= 196), there was a non-significant correlation between LEAPM

and BPDF, r = -.12 (p= n.s), however, there was a significant negative correlation between

LEAPF and BPDF, r = -.18, p < .05. For high school participants (N=359), there was a

significant negative correlation between LEAPM and BPDF, r = -.19, p < .05, and a

significant negative correlation between LEAPF and BPDF, r = -.17, p < .05. Thus,

hypothesis 7 was supported for high school participants and partially supported for junior

high participants.

Hypothesis 8

An additional hypothesis stated that perceived emotional availability would act as a

mediator between trait EI and BPD features. As the latter relationship was not supported in

the study, any mediation effects were excluded as possibilities. Thus, it was not possible to

explore this hypothesis.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 53: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Hypotheis 9

The final hypothesis of the study predicted that lower parent trait EI would be associated

with lower child trait EI and lower EA. A Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient

was computed to assess the relationships between parental trait EI (global), child trait EI

(global) and perceived EA for mothers and fathers (N=193). Non-significant correlations

were found for the relationship between parental trait EI and child trait EI, r = .12 (p= n.s.),

parental trait EI and LEAPM, r = .08 (p= n.s.) and parental trait EI and LEAPF, r = .04

(p=n.s.). Thus, hypothesis 9 was unsupported.

Secondary analyses

Given that only one of the trait EI subscales was found to be a significant predictor

of BPD features, in one age group, it was decided that the other variables in the study

would be explored as independent predictors - as moderation and mediation relationships

were no longer possibilities. Thus, multiple regression was performed, by education level,

including the following variables: Anxious ambivalent attachment, LEAPm and LEAPf.

For high school participants (N=359), standard multiple regression was performed

with emotion expression, Anxious Ambivalent attachment, LEAPm, and LEAPf as

predictors. Regression coefficients for the above variables can be found in Table 18

(Appendix W). The association between the criterion and explanatory variables was

moderate (Multiple R = 0.55). Only Anxious Ambivalent attachment style was positively

related to BPD features, accounting for 30% of the variation (Adjusted R2). The regression

coefficient for Anxious Ambivalent attachment style was 0.55 (95% CI = 5.15 – 7.25).

For junior high participants (N=196), standard multiple regression was performed

on LEAPm, LEAPf, and Anxious Ambivalent attachment style (Table 18, Appendix W).

The association between the criterion and explanatory variables was moderate (Multiple R

= .43). Anxious Ambivalent attachment style was a significant predictor of BPD features,

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 54: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

where the regression coefficient was 0.38 (95% CI = 2.93 – 5.86). LEAPf was also found

to be a significant predictor of BPDF, with a regression coefficient of -.17 (95% CI = -.21 -

-.01). The predictors accounted for 17% of the variation (Adjusted R2).

Given that none of the TEIQue subscales were found to be significant predictors of

BPDF in the two education level groups (with the exception of emotion expression in high

school participants), the relationship between trait EI subscales and BPDF was explored as

a function of gender. Due to restrictions with sample size in each education level, the

samples of girls and boys included participants from both junior high and high school.

Standard multiple regression was performed on girls (N=253) with the trait EI subscales as

the predictors and BPDF as the criterion variable. The results indicated that emotion

perception was a significant predictor of BPDF for female adolescent participants, with a

regression coefficient of 2.82 (95% CI=.42 -5.23), p < .05. For boys (N=215), only

assertiveness was found to be a significant predictor of BPDF, where the regression

coefficient was 2.65 (95% CI = .25 -5.06), p < .05. Unstandardized and standardized

coefficient values for both girls and boys can be found in Table 19 (Appendix X).

Moderation effects were then explored via SPSS PROCESS (Hayes, 2013). Due to

the fact that PROCESS cannot handle imputation files, the analyses were run on only one

of the imputation sets. Attachment did not moderate the relationship between emotion

perception and BPDF for girls (N= 249) where the regression coefficient was 1.14, p > .05

(R2 = .01, F(1, 249) = 1.39, p > .05). The possibility of a moderation effect of attachment

on the relationship between assertiveness and BPDF for boys was also explored and found

to be non-significant, with a regression coefficient of 1.26, p > .05 (R2 = .01, F(1, 208) =

2.76, p > .05).

Given the unexpected results, the possibility of parental scores on the TEIQue

predicting BPDF was also explored further. Standard multiple regression was performed

with the measure’s subscales as predictors and BPDF as the criterion variable, with

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 55: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

complete cases of adolescents matched with parental scores (N=193). Correlations between

the subscales and BPDF can be found in Table 20 (Appendix Y). None of the subscales

were found to be significant predictors of BPDF (for regression coefficients see Table 21,

Appendix Z).

Moreover, to investigate potential differences between participants who scored

higher on the BPD measure and those who scored lower, adolescent participants were

divided into high and low BPD groups. Individuals placed in the high BPD group (N=

143), scored 69 or higher on the BPFSC, while adolescents in the low BPD group (N=

413), scored below 69. It should be noted that the value of 69 was selected as a cut-off

point for the two groups as it represented scores in the 75th percentile. Subsequently, a one-

way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to explore whether there is a

statistically significant difference between high and low BPD group on trait EI (global),

controlling for LEAPm and LEAPf. The results of the ANCOVA revealed no main effects

of BPD group status F(1, 548) = .02, p > .05, ηp2 < .001, and no interaction between high

and low groups and LEAPm, F(1, 548) = .01, p > .05, ηp2 < .001 or LEAPf, F(1, 548) =

.05, p > .05, ηp2 < .001.

Additionally, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed with

the four trait EI factors as dependent variables and BPD groups (high vs. low) as

independent variables. There was a non-significant difference in trait EI factor scores,

based on which BPD group participants belonged to (high vs. low), F(4, 549) = .77, p >

.05. It should also be noted that Box’s M value of 26.08 was found to be significant, p <

.005, indicating that the covariance matrices between the groups were unequal. Based on

the latter, the results of the MANOVA need to be interpreted with caution.

Chapter 5: Discussion

The present study aimed at investigating trait EI in relation to BPD features in

adolescents and the possible influence of perceived emotional availability and attachment

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 56: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

on that relationship. The results of the study found that anxious ambivalent attachment

scores significantly predicted BPDF scores in both junior high and high school

participants. Further, it was revealed that the perceived emotional availability of fathers,

significantly predicted BPDF scores in junior high participants only. Additionally, the

emotion expression subscale of the TEIQue was found to be a significant predictor of

BPDF in high school participants. Moreover, the study revealed significant gender

differences with regards to trait EI and BPD features. Specifically, the trait EI facet of

emotion perception significantly predicted scores on the BPDF measure in girls only, while

in the sample of boys, only assertiveness significantly predicted these features.

Interestingly however, hypotheses relating to child trait EI and BPDF scores were

not supported in this study. Specifically, it was hypothesized that low trait EI (global)

would be related to higher scores on BPD features and that trait EI factors would

significantly predict BPDF scores at both education levels. Moreover, it was hypothesized

that specific trait EI facets (i.e. emotion management, emotion regulation, impulsiveness -

low, relationships and stress management) would also significantly predict features of

BPD. Suprisingly, trait EI (global) was unrelated to BPDF scores in this study. The limited

prior research available, conducted mainly with adults, has linked trait EI to BPD

symptomatology (e.g. Gardner & Qualter, 2009; Sinclair & Feigenbaum, 2012), while

there is also some preliminary evidence that points to an association between BPD related

symptomatology, such as self-harm, and trait EI in adolescents (Mikolajczak et al., 2009).

There could be several explanations as to why the results of the present study are not fully

in line with prior research. Specifically, in the Sinclair and Feigenbaum (2012) study, trait

EI global, assessed via the TEIQue, was the only measure to significantly predict BPD

diagnosis. However, the sample size in the study was severely restricted, as it included just

33 participants in the BPD group and 39 participants in the non-clinical group, thus

limiting the generalization of their results. Additionally, as indicated by others, the use of

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 57: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

only global trait EI may obscure the significance of individual EI facets and reduces the

explanatory power (Gardner & Qualter, 2009).

In the Gardner and Qualter (2009) study, negative correlations were found between

trait EI facets, assessed via the Schutte Emotional Intelligence Scale (SEIS; Schutte et al.,

1998) and BPD symptomatology, in a sample of 523 non-clinical adults. Specifically,

substantial correlations were found between global trait EI and global BPD (aggregated

scores) and PAI-BOR affective instability, PAI-BOR identity disturbance and the DSM-IV

criterion of emptiness. While the current study utilized a measure based on the PAI-BOR,

the developers of the questionnaire (Crick et al., 2005) caution against using the subscales

as stand-alone constructs, and the reliability of these scores were low in the sample. Thus it

was not possible to investigate whether trait EI was related to specific aspects of BPD

features. Additionally, in the Gardner and Qualter study several measures were used to

assess BPD and scores were aggregated to obtain global BPD scores, which may be

particularly suited to non-clinical samples, and has been said to be a more stable estimator

than using a single measure (Rushton, Brainerd & Pressley, 1983). However, the study

only investigated correlations between variables and thus cannot address whether or not

trait EI facets could predict BPD features.

In contrast to the above research, to the author’s knowledge, this is the only study

to investigate trait EI and BPD features in a sample of adolescents. Based on the latter, it is

possible that alternate constructs are more pertinent to emerging features of BPD during

the developmental period of adolescence. For instance, recent research indicates that

constructs such as somatization symptoms and experiencing peer victimization within the

context of a self-reported reactive temperament significantly predict emerging BPD

features in adolescents (Haltigan & Vaillancourt, 2016). An alternative explanation may be

that since BPD affects an estimated 0.5-5.9 percent of the population, BPD features in this

non-clinical sample of adolescents were not sufficiently prevalent to reveal robust

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 58: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

relationships between trait EI and emerging BPD features. Nonetheless, the study did

reveal that specific trait EI facets, when comparing genders, were predictive of BPD

features, namely, assertiveness in boys and emotion perception in girls. Gender differences

in the way BPD presents in adolescence have been a focus of past research. In fact, a

recent study (Michonski, Sharp, Steinberg & Zanarini, 2013) revealed that the predominant

feature of BPD in adolescence was paranoid ideation for boys and identity disturbance in

girls. As much needed research in this area emerges, it will likely shed more light on the

multifaceted presentation of BPD in youth.

Regarding attachment styles and BPD, this study revealed that in line with the

study’s hypothesis, an anxious ambivalent attachment style predicted features of the

disorder in adolescents in junior high and high school. This finding is consistent with the

bulk of the research on attachment and BPD (e.g. Fonagy et al., 1996; Meyer et al., 2004;

Nickell, Waudby & Trull, 2002). However, it must be noted that, due to the low reliability

and the factor structure of the measure utilized in the study, it is not possible to exclude the

possibility of another attachment style (i.e. avoidant) also predicting BPD features in the

current sample.

The current study is likely the first to investigate the construct of perceived

emotional availability and BPD in adolescents. It was hypothesized that lower scores

relating to perceived emotional availability of parents would be associated with higher

BPDF scores. This hypothesis was fully supported for high school participants, while in

junior high participants only perceived emotional availability of fathers was related to

higher BPDF scores. The results of additional analyses also indicated that low perceived

emotional availability in fathers significantly predicted BPD features, but this held true

only for junior high participants. There could be several explanations for the finding

relating to perceived emotional availability of fathers. One possible explanation could be

that during early adolescence, perceiving a father as more emotionally available is of

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 59: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

higher importance, in comparison to later years. Meanwhile, the absence of this finding for

mothers may be due to cultural reasons, such as the close familial bonds in Cyprus and the

fact that it has been traditionally viewed as child centered society which may have

influenced the way in which participants view and respond to questions regarding parental

EA and especially maternal EA. Research with regards to perceived emotional availability

may be warranted to further investigate the ways in which it may relate to BPD features,

and this may be particularly worthwhile in other cultures.

It is also necessary to consider the findings of this study within the context of

developing a practical model that will help identify features of BPD in adolescents and aid

early intervention efforts. The prevalent theoretical models for the development of the

disorder focus on specific constructs, such as attachment, emotion dysregulation and

emotional availability, as discussed previously (Crowell et al, 2009; Fonagy, et al., 2003;

Kernberg, 1967). The results of this study, confirm the importance of attachment in

relation to BPD features - a construct which was implicated in all three theoretical models.

Meanwhile, the findings regarding perceived emotional availability are also in alignment

with the equivalent constructs identified within the models. A review article (Keinänen,

Johnson, Richards & Courtney, 2012) has reported that parenting behaviors, and

particularly low parental warmth, are important psychosocial factors which seem to

contribute to the development of BPD. Further research is needed though to replicate and

further elaborate on these findings.

Meanwhile, facets of trait EI, such as emotion regulation, were not found to have a

significant association with the BPD features in the present study. Constructs such as

emotion regulation, and impulsivity have been considered a core part of explanations

pertaining to the development of BPD. Therefore, the findings of this study could have

both theoretical and practical implications to the way in which we conceptualize BPD and

move forward with research and intervention efforts. First, it should be mentioned that

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 60: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

prior research has often found mixed results with regards to emotion regulation, which

encompasses both physiological aspects and individuals’ self-perceptions. For instance,

prior research has associated BPD with behavioral disinhibition, even after controlling for

other personality disorder characteristics (Nigg, Silk, & Stavro, 2005). However, other

studies report contradictory findings. A study by Chapman et al. (2008) looked at the

effects of negative emotional states on physiological measures of impulsivity and self-

report aspects of emotion regulation. The researchers found that while high-BPD

individuals made a larger number of impulsive responses than low-BPD individuals,

negative emotions were in fact related to behavioral inhibition – findings which are in

contrast with the biosocial model (Crowell et al., 2009). Recent research has also

contradicted other aspects of the biosocial theory (see Cavazzi & Becerra, 2014). It is

therefore possible, that emotion dysregulation does not operate in the ways previously

thought of when it comes to individuals with BPD. Moreover, it is possible that,

individuals who have BPD features, and particularly adolescents, are not fully cognizant of

their difficulties in perceiving, expressing and regulating their emotions. Further research

needs to explore the aspects of emotion dysregulation more extensively, both in the

laboratory and with respect to the self-perceptions of individuals with BPD. This research

also needs to focus on adolescents, and be longitudinal in design, in order to further

understand the role of emotion dysregulation in the development of the disorder.

Meanwhile, it is important to note that there are several limitations to this study.

First, the study relied entirely on self-report measures which could have inadvertently

introduced bias. Using multiple informants for adolescents’ BPD features and other traits

may have enhanced the validity of the findings or revealed alternative relationships

between the constructs. An additional related limitation concerns the time which was

required to complete the questionnaires, specifically for adolescent participants. The fact

that the study employed the full form of the TEIQue, while useful for investigating the 15

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 61: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

facets of the construct, may have been tiresome for some of the participants, particularly

junior high pupils. The latter, also likely led to the sample size reduction in the study due

to missing data. Furthermore, the fact that this study adopted a cross-sectional design,

rather than a longitudinal one also constitutes a limitation, as the latter would be able to

follow specific patterns or clusters of features over time. Finally, the choice to perform

multiple regression on the data of this study, as opposed to multi-level analysis, is also

considered a limitation, as data was nested (i.e. collected from a number of classes within

the same schools).

In closing, the validity of BPD features in adolescence and the need to address

these has become the focus of contemporary research (see Winsper, Lereya, Marwaha,

Thompson, Eyden et al., 2016 for a review). Particular emphasis has been placed on the

validity of the diagnosis of the disorder or its emerging features in adolescents (Winsper,

Marwaha, Lereya, Thompson, Eyden et al., 2015). Studies such as the present one, can aid

in guiding research of BPD in adolescents, while emphasizing that exploring the disorder’s

features in individuals under the age of 18 is not only a valid endeavor but also a necessary

one. The current study is the first to attempt to understand the relationship between BPD

features and facets of trait EI, making an important contribution to current literature. The

need for specificity with regards to risk factors of BPD in adolescence has recently been

pointed out (Sharp & Fonagy, 2015), with regards to guiding universal and selective

prevention efforts. The present study took a step in that direction and attempted to

investigate trait EI as a possible risk factor. Further research in trait EI and BPD may be of

value in clinical populations of adolescents, as this may add to the understanding of the

relationship between the two. At the same time, it is also of paramount importance that

future research investigates the psychosocial risk factors for BPD in prospective research.

The need for this is two-fold. First, future research should focus on aspects of the

prevailing theoretical models in order to ascertain whether there is sufficient evidence to

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 62: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

support the respective constructs which have previously been associated with the

development of the disorder. Finally, there is a pressing need to develop a comprehensive

model for the development of BPD which can be utilized not only in treatment efforts but

also early intervention planning.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 63: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

References

Aaronson, J.C., Bender, S.D., Skodol, E.A. & Gunderson, G.J. (2006). Comparison of

attachment styles in borderline personality disorder and obsessive-compulsive

personality disorder. Psychiatric Quarterly, 77 (1), 69-80.

Ackerman, P.L. & Heggestad, E.D. (1997). Intelligence, personality and interests:

Evidence of overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219-245.

Agrawal, R.H., Gunderson, J., Holmes, M.B. & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2004). Attachment

studies with Borderline patients: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12 (2),

94-104.

Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love.

Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1982). Attachment: retrospect and prospect. In C. M. Parkes & J.

Stevenson- Hinde (eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior. London:

Tavistock (p. 3-30).

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. (1971). Individual differences in Strange

Situation behavior of one-year-olds. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), The origins of human

social relations (17-57). London: Academic Press.

Ainsworth, D.M., Blehar, C.M.., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A

psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Al-Yagon, M. & Mikulincer, M. (2004). Socioemotional and academic adjustment among

children with learning disorders: The mediational role of attachment-based factors.

Journal of Special Education, 38, 111-123. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 64: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait

emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and

Individual Differences, 47, 758-762.

Amad, A., Ramoz, N., Thomas, P., Jardri, R. & Gorwood, P. (2014). Genetics of

borderline personality disorder: Systematic review and proposal of an integrative

model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 40, 6-19.

American Psychological Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (4th ed. text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychological Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Andrei, F., Mancini, G., Baldaro, B., Trombini, E., & Agnoli, S. (2014). A systematic

review on the predictive utility of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire

(TEIQue). Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 271(62), 2-29.

Austin, E. J., Dore, T. C. P., & O’Donovan, K. M. (2008). Associations of personality and

emotional intelligence with display rule perceptions and emotional labour.

Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 679-688.

Babore, A., Picconi, L., Candelori, C & Trumello, C. (2014) The emotional relationship

with parents: A validation study of the LEAP among Italian adolescents, European

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(6), 728-739.

Bar-On, R. (1997). Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory: Technical manual. Toronto:

Mutli-Health Systems.

Barone, L., Fossati, A. & Guiducci, V. (2011). Attachment mental states and inferred

pathways of development in borderline personality disorder: A study using the

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 65: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Adult Attachment Interview. Attachment and Human Development, 13 (5), 451-

469.

Bateman, A.W., Fonagy, P. (2004). Mentalization-based treatment of BPD. Journal of

personality disorders, 18, 36-51.

Belsky, W.D., Caspi, A., Arseneault, A., Bleidorn, W., Fonagy, P., Goodman, M., et al.

(2012). Etiological features of borderline personality related characteristics in a

birth cohort of 12-year-old children. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 251-

265.

Bemporad, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1982). Borderline syndromes in childhood: Criteria for

diagnosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 596–601.

Biringen, Z. & Easterbrooks, A.E. (2012) Emotional availability: A developmental

psychopathology perspective. Development & Psychopathology. Vol. 24 (Edited

volume).

Biringen, Z., & Robinson, J. (1991). Emotional availability in mother–child interactions: A

reconceptualization for research. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 258–

271.

Biringen, Z., Robinson, J. & Emde, R.N. (1990). The Emotional Availability Scales.

Unpublished manual. Department of Human Development and Family Studies,

Colorado State University, Fort Collins.

Biringen, Z., Robinson, J.L., & Emde, R.N. (1993). The Emotional Availability scales (2nd

ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center,

Denver. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 66: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Biringen, Z., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. N. (1998). The Emotional Availability Scale (3rd

ed.). Fort Collins, CO: Colorado State University, Department of Human

Development and Family Studies.

Bornovalova, A.M., Hicks, M.B., Iacono, G.W. & McGue, M. (2013). Longitudinal Twin

Study of Borderline Personality Disorder Traits and Substance Use in Adolescence:

Developmental Change, Reciprocal Effects, and Genetic and Environmental

Influences. Personality Disorders, 4(1), 23-32.

Bornovalova, A.M., Hicks, M.B., Iacono, G.W. & McGue, M. (2009). Stability, change,

and heritability of borderline personality disorder traits from adolescence to

adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Development and Psychopathology, 21,

1335-1353.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. Attachment and Loss, 1 (2nd ed.). New York: Basic

Books.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory. Routledge.

London.

Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., & Salovey, P. (2011). Emotional intelligence: Implications

for personal, social, academic, and workplace success. Social and Personality

Psychology Compass, 5, 88-103

Brenning, K., Soenens, B., Braet, C., & Bosmans, G. (2012). Attachment and depressive

symptoms in middle childhood and early adolescence: Testing the validity of the

emotion regulation model of attachment. Personal Relationships, 19, 445-464.

Bretherton, I. (2000). Emotional availability: An attachment perspective. Attachment and

Human Development, 2 (2), 233-241.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 67: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Cailhol, L., Jeannot, M., Rodgers, R., Guelfi, D.J., Perez-Diaz, F., Pham-Scottez, A.,

Corcos, M. & Speranza, M. (2013). Borderline personality disorder and mental

healthcare service use among adolescents. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27(2),

252-259.

Calati, R., Gressier, F., Balestri, M. & Serretti, A. (2013). Genetic modulation of

borderline personality disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of

Psychiatric Research, 47, 1275-1287.

Cavazzi, T. & Becerra, R. (2014). Psychophysiological research of borderline personality

disorder: Review and implications for biosocial theory. Europe's Journal of

Psychology, 10(1), 185–203

Chabrol, H. & Leichsenring, F. (2006). Borderline personality organization and

psychopathic traits in nonclinical adolescents: Relationships of identity diffusion,

primitive defense mechanisms and reality testing with callousness and impulsivity

traits. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 70(2), 160-170.

Chabrol, H., Montovany, A., Ducongé, E., Kallmeyer, A., Mullet, E., & Leichsenring, F.

(2004). Factor structure of the Borderline Personality Inventory. European Journal

of Psychological Assessment, 20, 59–6

Chang, B., Sharp, C. & Ha, C. (2011). The criterion validity of the Borderline Personality

Features Scale for Children in an adolescent inpatient setting. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 25(4), 492-503.

Chapman, A. L., Leung, D. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2008). Impulsivity and emotion

dysregulation in Borderline Personality Disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders,

22(2), 148-164

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 68: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Clark, L. A. (1993). Manual for the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality

(SNAP). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Clarkin, J. F., Yeomans, F., Kernberg, O. F. (2006). Psychotherapy for borderline

personality: Focusing on object relations. New York: Wiley.

Clarkin, J.F., Yeomans, F.E., Lenzenweger, M.F., Levy, K.L. & Kernberg, O.F. (2007). An

Object Relations Model of Borderline Pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders,

21(5), 474-499.

Clercq, D.B., Fruyt, D.F., Leeuwen, V.K. & Mervielde, I. (2006). The structure of

maladaptive personality traits in childhood: A step towards an integrative

developmental perspective for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4),

639-657.

Colonnesi C., Draijer E. M., Stams G. J. J. M., Van der Bruggen C. O., Bögels S. M.,

Noom M. J. (2011). The relation between attachment and child anxiety: A meta-

analytic review. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 630–645.

Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., Stewart, S. E., & Segal, D. L. (2002). The Coolidge

Personality and Neuropsychological Inventory for Children (CPNI): Preliminary

psychometric characteristics. Behavior Modification, 26, 550–566.

Crick N.R., Murray-Close, D. & Woods, K. (2005). Borderline personality features in

childhood: a short-term longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 17,

1051-1070.

Cronbach, L. J. (1949). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper and Row Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 69: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Crowell, E. S., Beauchaine, P.T. & Linehan, M. M. (2009). A Biosocial Developmental

Model of Borderline Personality Disorder: Elaborating and Extending Linehan’s

Model. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 495-510.

Davis, S.K. & Humphrey, N. (2012a). Emotional intelligence predicts adolescent mental

health beyond personality and cognitive ability. Personality and Individual

Differences, 52(2), 144–149.

Davis, S.K. & Humphrey, N. (2012b). The influence of emotional intelligence (EI) on

coping and mental health in adolescence: Divergent roles for trait and ability EI.

Journal of Adolescence, 35(5), 1369-1379.

Dawda, D. & Hart, S.D. (2000). Assessing emotional intelligence: Reliability and validity

of the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) in university students.

Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 797-812.

Deborde, SA., Milkjovitch, R., Roy, C., Dugré- Le Bigre, C., Pham-Scottez, A., Speranza,

M. & Corcos, M. (2012). Journal of Personality Disorders, 26(5), 676-688.

Easterbrooks, M.A. & Biringen, Z. (2005). The Emotional Availability Scales:

Methodological refinements of the construct and clinical implications related to

gender and at risk interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26(4), 291-294.

Easterbrooks, M.A., Bureau, F.J. & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2012). Developmental correlates and

predictors of emotional availability in mother-child interaction: A longitudinal

study from infancy to middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 24,

65-78. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 70: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Ensink, K., Biberdzic, M., Normandin, L. & Clarkin, J. (2015). A developmental

psychopathology and neurobiological model of borderline personality disorder in

adolescence. Journal of Infant, Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 14, 46-69.

Farrell, J. M., & Shaw, I. A. (1994). Emotional awareness training: A prerequisite to

effective cognitive–behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder.

Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 1, 71–91.

Fernando, C.S., Beblo, T., Schlosser, N., Terfehr, K., Otte, C., Löwe, B., Bernd, L., Wolf,

T.O., Spitzer, C., Driessen, M. & Wingenfeld, K. (2014). The impact of self-

reported childhood trauma on emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder

and major depression. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation 15, 384-401.

Finzi, R., Har-Even, D., Weizman, A., Tyano S. & Shnit, D. (1996). The adaptation of the

attachment style questionnaire for latency-aged children. [Hebrew]. Psychology, 5,

167– 177.

Finzi, R., Cohen, O., Sapir, Y., Weizman, A. (2000) Attachment styles in maltreated

children: A comparative study. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 31, 113-

128.

Fiori, M., Antonietti J.P., Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Hansenne, M. & Rossier, J.

(2014). What is the ability emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT) good for? An

evaluation using item response theory. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e98827.

Fonagy, P. (2000). Attachment and borderline personality disorder. Journal of the

American Psychoanalytic Association, 48, 1129-1146.

Fonagy, P. & Bateman, W. A. (2007). Mentalizing and borderline personality disorder.

Journal of Mental Health, 16(1), 83-101.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 71: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Fonagy, P. & Bateman, W.A. (2008). The development of borderline personality disorder –

a mentalizing model. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22(1), 4–21

Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., Target, M. &

Gerber, A. (1996). The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and

response to psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(1),

22-31.

Fonagy, P., Luyten, P. & Strathearn, L. (2011). Borderline personality disorder,

mentalisation and the neurobiology of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal,

32(1), 47-69.

Fonagy, P., Rossouw, T., Sharp, C., Bateman, A., Allison, L. & Farrar, C. (2014).

Mentalization-based treatment for adolescents with borderline traits. In Sharp, C. &

Tackett, L.J. (Eds). Handbook of borderline personality disorder in children and

adolescents. New York: Springer.

Fonagy, P., Target, M., Gergely, G., Allen, J.G. & Bateman, W.A. (2003). The

developmental roots of borderline personality disorder in early attachment

relationships: A theory and some evidence. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 23, 412-459.

Freudenthaler, H.H. & Neubauer, A. (2007). Measuring emotional management abilities:

Further evidence of the importance to distinguish between typical and maximum

performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1561-1572.

Freudenthaler, H.H., Neubauer, C.A., Gabler, P. & Sherl, G.W. (2008). Testing and

validating the trait emotional intelligence questionnaires (TEIQue) in a German-

speaking sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 673-678. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 72: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Gaher, M.R., Hofman, N.L., Simons, S.J. & Hunsaker, R. (2013). Emotion regulation

deficits as mediators between trauma exposure and borderline symptoms. Cognitive

Therapy and Research, 37, 466-475.

Gardner, H. (1993). Mutliple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, K. & Qualter, P. (2009). Emotional intelligence and borderline personality

disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 94-98.

Gardner, K. & Qualter, P. (2010). Concurrent and incremental validity of three trait

emotional intelligence measures. Australian Journal of Psychology, 62(1), 5-13.

Gill, D. & Warburton, W. (2014). An investigation of the biosocial model of borderline

personality disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70(9), 866-873.

Glenn, R.C. & Klonsky, E. D. (2009). Emotion dysregulation as a core feature of

borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(1), 20–28.

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam.

Goth, K., Foelsch, P., Schlüter-Müller, S., Birkhölzer, M., Jung, E., Pick, O. & Schmeck,

K. (2012). Assessment of identity development and identity diffusion in

adolescence - Theoretical basis and psychometric properties of the self-report

questionnaire AIDA. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 6(1), 1-

16.

Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and adjustment to school in

middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 530–541.

Gratz, L.K., Tull, T. M., Reynolds, K. E., Bagge, L.C., Latzman, D.R., Daughters, B.S. &

Lejuez, C.W. (2009) Extending extant models of the pathogenesis of borderline

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 73: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

personality disorder to childhood borderline personality symptoms: The roles of

affective dysfunction, disinhibition, and self- and emotion-regulation deficits.

Development and Psychopathology 21, 1263–1291.

Griffiths, M. (2011). Validity, utility and acceptability of borderline personality disorder in

childhood and adolescence: Survey of psychiatrists. The Psychiatrist, 25, 19-22.

Haltigan, D.J. & Vaillancourt, T. (2016). Identifying trajectories of borderline personality

features in adolescence: Antecedents and interactive risk factors. The Canadian

Journal of Psychiatry, 61(3), 166-175.

Haskayne, D., Hirschfeld, R. & Larkin, M. (2014). The outcome of psychodynamic

psychotherapies with individuals diagnosed with personality disorders: a systematic

review, Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 28(2), 115-138.

Haslam, N. (2003). Categorical vs. dimensional models of mental disorder: the taxometric

evidence. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 37(6), 696-704.

Hawes, J.D., Helyer, R., Herlianto, C.E. & Willing, J. (2013). Borderline personality

features, and implicit shame-prone self-concept in middle childhood and early

adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42(3), 302-308.

Hayes, A. F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation and Conditional Process

Analysis. New York, New York: Guilford Press.

Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511-524.

Haltigan, D.J. & Vaillancourt, T. (2016). Identifying trajectories of borderline personality

features in adolescence: Antecedent and interactive risk factors. The Canadian

Journal of Psychiatry, 61(3), 166-175.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 74: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Hertel, J., Schütz, A. and Lammers, C.-H. (2009), Emotional intelligence and mental

disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 942–954.

Hofstee , W. K. B.( 2001 ). Personality and intelligence: Do they mix? In J.M. Collis & S.

Messick (Eds.), Intelligence and personality: Bridging the gap in theory and

measurement (pp. 43 – 60). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Houston, R. J., Ceballos, N. A., Hesselbrock, V. M., & Bauer, L. O. (2005). Borderline

personality disorder features in adolescent girls: P300 evidence of altered brain

maturation. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 1424−1432.

Jellesma, F.C., Rieffe, C., Meerum Terwogt, M., & Westenberg, P.M. (2011). Children’s

sense of coherence and trait emotional intelligence: A longitudinal study exploring

the development of somatic complaints. Psychology & Health, 26(3), 307-320.

Karreman, A., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2012). Attachment and well-being: The

mediating role of emotion regulation and resilience. Personality and Individual

Differences, 53, 821-826.

Kawabada, Y., Youngblood, J. & Hamaguchi, Y. (2014). Preadolescents’ borderline

personality features in a non-Western urban context: Concurrent and longitudinal

associations with physical and relational aggression, friendship exclusivity and peer

victimization. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 219-228.

Kazdin, E.A. (1997). A model for developing effective treatments: Progression and

interplay of theory, research, and practice. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,

26(2), 114-129. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 75: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Keinänen, T.M., Johnson, G.J., Richards, S.E. & Courtney, A.E. (2012). A systematic

review of the evidence-based psychosocial risk factors for understanding of

borderline personality disorder. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 26(1), 65-91.

Kendler, K.S., Aggen, S.H., Czajkowski, N., Roysamb, E., Tambs, K., Torgersen, S.,

Neale,M.C. & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., (2008). The structure of genetic and

environmentalrisk factors for DSM-IV personality disorders: a multivariate twin

study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 1438–1446.

Kernberg, O. (1967). Borderline personality organization. Journal of American

Psychoanalytic Association, 15, 641-685.

Kernberg, F.O. & Michels, R. (2009). Borderline Personality Disorder. American Journal

of Psychiatry, 166, 505-508.

Krueger, R. F., & Tackett, J. L. (2003). Personality and psychopathology: Working toward

the bigger picture. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 109–128.

Kochanska, G. & Kim, S. (2013). Early attachment organization with both parents and

future behavior problems: From infancy to middle childhood. Child Development,

84(1), 283-296.

Kuo, R.J. & Linehan, M.M. (2009) Disentangling emotion processes in borderline

personality disorder: Physiological and self-reported assessment of biological

vulnerability, baseline intensity and reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(3), 531-544.

Lanciano, T. & Curci, A. (2014). Incremental validity of emotional intelligence ability in

predicting academic achievement. American Journal of Psychology, 127(4), 447-

461.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 76: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Lebow, J. (2006). Research for the psychotherapist: From science to practice. NY:

Routledge.

Lee, C. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (1991). Family disruption, parental availability and child

adjustment. Advances in Behavioral Assessment of Children and Families, 5, 171–

199.

Leible, T. L., & Snell, W. E., Jr. (2004). Borderline personality disorder and multiple

aspects of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 393–

404.

Leichsenring, F. (1999a). Development and first results of the Borderline Personality

Inventory: a self-report instrument for assessing borderline personality

organization. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73(1), 45-63.

Leichsenring, F. (1999b). Splitting: An empirical study. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic,

63, 520–537.

Leichsenring, F. (1999c). Primitive defense mechanisms in schizophrenics and borderline

patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 229–236

Leichsenring, F., Leibing, E., Kruse, J, New, S.A. & Leweke, F. (2011). Borderline

Personality Disorder. The Lancet, 377, 74-84.

Leichsenring, F., & Sachsse, U. (2002). Emotions as wishes and beliefs. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 79, 257–273

Linehan, M.M. (1993a). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality

disorder. New York: The Guildford Press. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 77: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Linehan, M.M. (1993b). Skills training manual for treating Borderline Personality

disorder. New York: The Guilford Press.

Little, C. & Carter, S. A. (2005). Negative emotional reactivity and regulation in 12-

month-olds following emotional challenge: Contributions of maternal-infant

emotional availability in a low-income sample. Infant Mental Health Journal,

26(4), 354-368.

Livesley, W. J. (1986). Traits and behavioral prototypes of personality disorder. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 728–732.

Livesley, W. J. (1990). Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology—Basic

Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia,

Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.

Loeber, R., Green, S.M. & Lahey, B.B. (2003) Risk factors for antisocial personality. In:

Farrington D.P. & Coid, J.W. (Eds). Early Prevention of Adult Antisocial

Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 79–108.

Lum, J.J. (1999). Assessing the emotional availability of parents: Child, adolescent, and

parent perceptions. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, (3-B).

Lum, J.J. & Phares, V. (2005). Assessing the emotional availability of parents. Journal of

Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 27(3), 211-226.

Lyons-Ruth, K., Jacobvitz, D. (2008). Disorganized attachment: Genetic factors, parenting

contexts, and developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood. In: Cassidy,

J. & Shaver, P. (Eds). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical

applications. 2nd Edition. New York: Guilford, 666-697. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 78: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Macfie, J., Coens, L., Fitzpatrick, K., Frankel, M., McCollum, K., Trupe, R. & Watkins, C.

D. (2007, April). Effects of maternal borderline personality disorder on emotional

availability in mother-child interactions. Presented at the biennial meeting of the

Society for Research in Child Development.

Mayer, J.D., DiPaolo, M.T. & Salovey, P. (1990). Perceiving affective content in

ambiguous visual stimuli: A component of emotional intelligence. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 54, 772-781.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P. & Caruso, D.R. (2002). The Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional

Intelligence Test (MSCEIT): User’s manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

McMahon, A.C. & Meins, E. (2012). Mind-mindedness, parenting stress, and emotional

availability in mothers of preschoolers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27,

245-252.

Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as

disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M.T.

Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E.M. Cummings, Attachment during the preschool

years: Theory, research and intervention. 121–160. Chicago: University of

Chicago Press.

Malouff, M.J., Schutte, S.N. & Thorsteinsson, B.E. (2014). Trait emotional intelligence

and romantic relationship satisfaction: A meta-analysis. The American Journal of

Family Therapy, 42, 53-66.

Martins, A., Ramalho, N., & Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the

relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual

Differences, 49, 554-564.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 79: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Mavroveli, S. Petrides. K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence,

psychological well-being, and peer-rated social competence in adolescence. British

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263-275.

Mayer, D.J., Salovey, P. & Caruso, R.D. (2000). The Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso

Emotional Intelligence Test: Technical manual. Toronto: MHS.

Meyer, B., Pilkonis, A.P. & Beevers, G.C. (2004). What’s in a (neutral) face? Personality

disorders, attachment styles and the appraisal of ambiguous social cues. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 18 (4), 320-336.

Michonski, J.D., Sharp, C., Steinberg, L., & Zanarini, M. (2013). Borderline personality

disorder criteria in a population-based sample of 11 to 12-year-old children: An

item-level analysis. Houston, TX: University of Houston.

Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Leroy, C., & Roy, E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the

trait emotional intelligence questionnaire: Factor structure, reliability, construct,

and incremental validity in a French-speaking population. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 88, 338–353.

Mikolajczak, M., Petrides, K. V., & Hurry, J. (2009). Adolescents choosing self-harm as

an emotion regulation strategy: The protective role of trait emotional intelligence.

British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 181-193.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Boosting attachment security to promote mental

health, prosocial values, and inter-group tolerance. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 139-

156. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 80: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Miller, L.A., Muehlenkamp, J.J., Jacobson, M.C. (2008). Fact or fiction: Diagnosing

borderline personality disorder in adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 28,

969-981.

Miller, L.A., Rathus, H.J. & Linehan, M.M. (2006). Dialectical Behavior Therapy with

suicidal adolescents. NY: Guilford Press.

Mobascher, A., Bohus, M., Dahmen, N., Dietl, L., Giegling I., Jungkunz M.,…Lieb, K

(2014). Association between dopa decarboxylase gene variants and borderline

personality disorder. Psychiatry Research. 219(3), 693-695.

Morey, L.C. (1991). Personality Assessment Inventory - Professional Manual. Florida,

USA: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Nickell, A. D., Waudby. C. J. & Trull, T. J. (2002). Attachment, parental bonding and

borderline personality disorder features in young adults. Journal of Personality

Disorders, 16, 148-159.

Nigg, J. T., Silk, K. R., & Stavro, G. (2005). Disinhibition and borderline personality

disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 1129-1149.

Normandin, L., Ensink, K. & Kernberg, F.O. (2015). Transference-focused psychotherapy

for borderline adolescents: A neurobiologically informed psychodynamic

psychotherapy. Journal of Infant, Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 14, 98-110.

O’Connor, E., Bureau, F.J., McCartney, K. & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2011). Risks and outcomes

associated with disorganized/controlling patterns of attachment at age three years in

the national institute of child health and human development study of early child

care and youth development. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32(4), 450-472. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 81: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Oldham, M.J. (2006). Borderline personality disorder and suicidality. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 163, 20-26.

Paris, J. (2000). Childhood precursors of borderline personality disorder. The Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 23, 77–88.

Pérez, J. C., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring trait emotional intelligence.

In R. Schulze & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), International handbook of emotional

intelligence. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber.

Perera, N.H. & DiGiacomo, M. (2013). The relationship of trait emotional intelligence

with academic performance: A meta-analytic review. Learning and Individual

Differences, 28, 20-33.

Perroud, N., Dieben, K., Nicastro, R., Muscionico, M.& Huguelet, P. (2012). Functions

and time-scale of self-cutting in participants suffering from borderline personality

disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26(2), 267-279.

Petrides, K. V. (2001). A psychometric investigation into the construct of emotional

intelligence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University College London.

Petrides, K. V. (Spring, 2006). Internal consistency data for the TEIQue and TEIQue-SF

(v. 1.50). http://www.ioe.ac.uk/schools/phd/kpetrides/teique1.htm, Retrieved

23/6/2013.

Petrides, K.V. (2009a). TEIQue:AF: Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire:

Adolescent Form (1st Ed., 4th print) [Self-Report Measurement]. London, England:

Psychometric Lab. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 82: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Petrides, K. V. (2009b). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire. In C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske, and J. D. Parker, Advances in the

assessment of emotional intelligence. New York: Springer.

Petrides, K. V. (2011). Ability and trait emotional intelligence. In Chamorro-Premuzic, T.,

Furnham, A., & von Stumm, S. (Eds.), The Blackwell-Wiley Handbook of

Individual Differences. New York: Wiley.

Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence. Psychometric

investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of

Personality, 15, 425–448.

Petrides, K.V. & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural validation

in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European

Journal of Personality, 17, 39-57.

Petrides, K.V., Pérez-Gonzalez, J.C. & Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and

incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 26-

55.

Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional

intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 273–

289.

Philippot, P. & Feldman, R.S. (2004) (Eds.). The regulation of emotion. New York:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Prados, J., Stenz, L., Courtet, P., Prada, P., Nicastro, R., Adouan, W., Guillaume, S., Olié,

E., Aubry, J.M., Dayer, A. & Perroud, N. (2015). Borderline personality disorder Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 83: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

and childhood maltreatment: a genome-wide methylation analysis. Genes, Brain &

Behavior, 14, 177-188.

Ramos, V., Canta, G., de Castro, F. & Leal, I. (2014). Discrete subgroups of adolescents

diagnosed with borderline personality disorder: A latent class analysis of

personality features. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(4), 463-482.

Rathus, H.J. & Miller, L.A. (2014). DBT skills manual for adolescents. NY: Guilford

Press.

Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Czajkowski, N., Røysamb, E., Ørstavik, R.E., Neale,

M.C.,Torgersen, S. & Kendler, K.S. (2010). Major depression and dimensional

representations of DSM-IV personality disorders: a population-based twin study.

Psychological Medicine, 40, 1475–1484.

Reker, D. & Parker, J.D. (1999). Relationships between emotional intelligence, mood, and

problem behaviors in children and adolescence. Poster presented at the annual

meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA.

Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. A. (1996). Adolescent attachment and psychopathology.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 244.

Rosenthal, M. Z., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2005). Thought

suppression mediates the relationship between negative affect and borderline

personality disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1173-1185.

Rossouw, T. (2013). Mentalization-Based Treatment: Can It be translated into practice in

clinical settings and teams? Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 220-222. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 84: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Rushton, J.P., Brainerd, J.C. & Pressley, M. (1983). Behavioral development and construct

validity: The principle of aggregation. Psychological Bulletin, 94(1), 18-38.

Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y., & Joels, T. (2002). Shedding further light on

the effects of various types and quality of early child care on infant–mother

attachment relationship: The Haifa study of early child care. Child Development,

73, 1166– 1186.

Salovey, P. & Mayer, J.D. (1990). Emotional Intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and

Personality, 9, 185-211.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional

attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the trait meta-

mood scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (125–154).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M., Bobik, C., Coston, T.D., Greeson, C., K., Jedlicka, C. et al.

(2001). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. The Journal of Social

Psychology, 141, 523-536.

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et

al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence.

Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167–177

Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M. & Hine, W.D. (2011). The association of ability and trait

emotional intelligence with alcohol problems. Addiction Research and Theory,

19(3), 260-265. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 85: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M., Thorsteinsson, B.E., Bhullar, N. & Rooke, E.S. (2007). A

meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and

health. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 921-933.

Sharp, C. & Fonagy, P. (2015). Practitioner review: Borderline personality disorder in

adolescence – recent conceptualization, intervention, and implications for clinical

practice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12), 1266-1288.

Shields, A. M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation in school-age children: The

development of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906–

916.

Siegling, B.A., Saklofske, H.D. & Petrides, K.V. (2015). Measures of ability and trait

emotional intelligence. In Boyle, J.B., Saklofske, H.D. & Matthews, G. (Eds),

Measures of personality and social psychological constructs. MA: Academic Press.

Sinclair, H. & Feigenbaum, J. (2012). Trait Emotional Intelligence and Borderline

Personality Disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(6), 674-679

Spearman, C. (1904). General Intelligence, Objectively Determined and Measured. The

American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201–292.

Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. NY: Macmillan.

Stepp, D.S., Burke, D.J., Hipwell, E.A. & Loeber, R. (2012). Trajectories of Attention

Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Oppositional Defiant Disorder symptoms as

precursors of Borderline Personality Disorder symptoms in adolescent girls.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 7-20. Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 86: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Stepp, D.S., Pilkonis, A.P., Hipwell, E.A., Loeber, R. & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2010).

Stability of borderline personality disorder features in girls. Journal of Personality

Disorders, 24(4), 460-472.

Stepp, D.S., Whalen, J.D., Scott, N.L., Zalewski, M., Loeber, R. & Hipwell, E.A. (2014).

Reciprocal effects of parenting and borderline personality disorder symptoms in

adolescent girls. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 361-378.

Stern, A. (1938). Psychoanalytic investigation of and therapy in the border line group of

neuroses. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 7, 467-489.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston:

Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., Ekselius, L., & Fredrikson, M. (2004). Social phobia and

avoidant personality disorder: One spectrum disorder? Nordic Journal of

Psychiatry, 58, 147–152.

Torgersen, S. (2000). Genetics of patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 23, 1−9.

Torgersen, S., Czajkowski, N., Jacobson, K., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Roysamb, E.,

Neale, M.C. & Kendler, K.S., (2008). Dimensional representations of DSM-IV

cluster B personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a

multivariate study. Psychological Medicine, 38, 1617–1625.

Tromp, B. N. & Koot, M. H. (2010). Dimensions of Normal and Abnormal Personality:

Elucidating DSM-IV Personality Disorder Symptoms in Adolescents. Journal of

Personality, 78(3), 839-864. Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 87: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Van der Zee, K., Schakel, M. & Thijs, L. (2002). The relationship of emotional intelligence

with academic intelligence and the Big Five. European Journal of Personality, 16

(2), 103–125.

van Dijk, F., Lappenschaar, M., Cornelis, K., Verkes, J.R. & Buitelaar, J. (2011). Lifespan

attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and borderline personality disorder

symptoms in female patients: A latent class approach. Psychiatry Research, 190,

327-334.

Van Rooy, D. L., Viswesvaran, C., & Pluta, P. (2005). A meta-analytic evaluation of

construct validity: What is this thing called emotional intelligence? Human

Performance, 18, 445-462

Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. (2002). Parents’ emotional

availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent–infant

attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 447–

465.

Wenar, C. & Kerig, P. (2005). Developmental Psychopathology: From Infancy to

Adolescence. NY: McGraw and Hill.

West, M., Sheldon-Keller, A. (1992). The assessment of dimensions relevant to adult

reciprocal attachment. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 37, 600–606.

White, R. & Renk, K. (2012). Externalizing behavior problems in adolescence: An

ecological perspective. Journal of Child and Family studies, 21, 158-171.

Williams, C., Daley, D., Burnside, E., & Hammond-Rowley, S. (2010). Can trait

Emotional Intelligence and objective measures of emotional ability predict Filia

-Ann

a Chri

stodo

ulou

Page 88: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

psychopathology across the transition to secondary school? Personality and

Individual Differences, 48, 161-165.

Winsper, C., Marwaha, S., Lereya, T.S., Thompson, A., Eyden et al. & Singh, P.S. (2015).

Clinical and psychosocial outcomes of borderline personality disorder in childhood

and adolescence: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 45, 2237-2251.

Winsper, C., Lereya, T.S., Marwaha, S., Thompson, A., Eyden, J. & Singh, P.S. (2016).

The aetiological and psychopathological validity of borderline personality disorder

in youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 44,

13-24.

Wolff, S., Stiglmayr, C., Bretz, H., Lammers, C. & Auckenthaler, A. (2007) Emotion

identification and tension in female patients with borderline personality disorder.

British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(3), 347-360.

Wong, C.S. & Law, K.S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence

on performance and attitudes: An exploratory study. Leadership Quarterly, 13,

243-274.

Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., & Costello, E. (2002). Affect regulation in women with borderline

personality disorder traits. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 693–696.

Zanarini, C.M., Frankenburg, R.F., Ridolfi, E.M., Jager-Hyman, S., Hennen, J. &

Gunderson, G.J. (2006). Reported childhood onset of self-mutilation among

borderline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20(1), 9-15.

Zeanah, C.H., Keyes, A., and Settles, L. (2003). Attachment relationship experiences and

childhood psychopathology. In J.A. King, C.F. Ferris, and I.I. Lederhendler (Eds.)

Roots of mental illness in children. NY: New York Academy of Sciences.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 89: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Zelkowitz, P., Paris, J., Guzder, J., Feldman, R., Roy, C. & Rosval, L. (2007). A five year

follow-up of patients with borderline pathology of childhood. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 21(6), 664-674.

Ziv, U., Aviezer, O., Sagi, A., & Koren-Karie, N. (2000). Emotional availability in the

mother–child dyad as related to the quality of infant–mother attachment

relationship. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 149–169.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 90: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix A

ΕΝΤΥΠΑ ΣΥΓΚΑΤΑΘΕΣΗΣ

για συμμετοχή σε πρόγραμμα έρευνας

(Τα έντυπα αποτελούνται συνολικά από 4 σελίδες)

Αγαπητοί γονείς/κηδεμόνες,

Καλείστε να συμμετάσχετε σε ένα ερευνητικό πρόγραμμα του Τμήματος Ψυχολογίας,

του Πανεπιστημίου Κύπρου. Πιο κάτω (βλ. «Πληροφορίες για Εθελοντές») θα σας δοθούν

εξηγήσεις σε απλή γλώσσα σχετικά με το τι θα ζητηθεί από εσάς και το παιδί σας εάν

συμφωνήσετε να συμμετάσχετε στο πρόγραμμα. Θα σας περιγραφούν οποιοιδήποτε

κίνδυνοι μπορεί να υπάρξουν ή ταλαιπωρία που τυχόν θα υποστείτε από τη δική σας

συμμετοχή και αυτή του παιδιού σας στο πρόγραμμα. Θα σας επεξηγηθεί με κάθε

λεπτομέρεια τι θα ζητηθεί από εσάς και ποιος ή ποιοι θα έχουν πρόσβαση στις πληροφορίες

ή/και άλλο υλικό που εθελοντικά θα δώσετε για το πρόγραμμα. Θα σας δοθεί η χρονική

περίοδος για την οποία οι υπεύθυνοι του προγράμματος θα έχουν πρόσβαση στις

πληροφορίες που θα δώσετε. Θα σας επεξηγηθεί τι ελπίζουμε να μάθουμε από το

πρόγραμμα σαν αποτέλεσμα και της δικής σας συμμετοχής. Επίσης, θα σας δοθεί μία

εκτίμηση για το όφελος που μπορεί να υπάρξει για τους ερευνητές ή/και χρηματοδότες αυτού

του προγράμματος. Δεν πρέπει να συμμετάσχετε, εάν δεν επιθυμείτε ή εάν έχετε

οποιουσδήποτε ενδοιασμούς που αφορούν την συμμετοχή σας στο πρόγραμμα. Εάν

αποφασίσετε να συμμετάσχετε, πρέπει να αναφέρετε εάν είχατε συμμετάσχει σε οποιοδήποτε

άλλο πρόγραμμα έρευνας μέσα στους τελευταίους 12 μήνες. Είστε ελεύθεροι να αποσύρετε

οποιαδήποτε στιγμή εσείς επιθυμείτε την συγκατάθεση για την συμμετοχή σας στο

πρόγραμμα. Έχετε το δικαίωμα να υποβάλετε τυχόν παράπονα ή καταγγελίες, που αφορούν

το πρόγραμμα στο οποίο συμμετέχετε, προς την Επιτροπή Βιοηθικής που ενέκρινε το

πρόγραμμα ή ακόμη και στην Εθνική Επιτροπή Βιοηθικής Κύπρου.

Πρέπει όλες οι σελίδες των εντύπων συγκατάθεσης να φέρουν το ονοματεπώνυμο και

την υπογραφή σας.

Σύντομος Τίτλος του Προγράμματος στο οποίο καλείστε να συμμετάσχετε

Κατανόηση και Ρύθμιση Συναισθημάτων

Υπεύθυνος του Προγράμματος στο οποίο καλείστε να συμμετάσχετε

Δρ. Ειρήνη-Άννα Διακίδου, Αναπληρώτρια Καθηγήτρια, Τμήμα Ψυχολογίας,

Πανεπιστήμιο Κύπρου

Φίλια-Άννα Χριστοδούλου, Διδακτορική Φοιτήτρια Κλινικής Ψυχολογίας, Τμήμα

Ψυχολογίας, Πανεπιστήμιο Κύπρου

Επίθετο

Μητέρας:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα: ………………………………………..

Υπογραφή: ……………………………………………….………

Ημερομηνία:

.......................................

Επίθετο

Πατέρα:

……………………………………………….……… Όνομα:

……………………………………….

Υπογραφή: Ημερομηνία:

Επίθετο

Κηδεμόνα:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα:

………………………………………..

Υπογραφή:

……………………………………………….……….

Ημερομηνία: ………………………………………..

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 91: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

ΕΝΤΥΠΑ ΣΥΓΚΑΤΑΘΕΣΗΣ

για συμμετοχή σε πρόγραμμα έρευνας

(Τα έντυπα αποτελούνται συνολικά από 4 σελίδες)

Σύντομος Τίτλος του Προγράμματος στο οποίο καλείστε να συμμετάσχετε

Κατανόηση και Ρύθμιση Συναισθημάτων

Δίδετε συγκατάθεση για τον εαυτό σας ή/και για κάποιο άλλο

άτομο;

Εάν πιο πάνω απαντήσατε για κάποιον άλλο, τότε δώσετε λεπτομέρειες και το όνομα του.

Ερώτηση ΝΑΙ ή

ΟΧΙ

Συμπληρώσατε τα έντυπα συγκατάθεσης εσείς προσωπικά;

Τους τελευταίους 12 μήνες έχετε συμμετάσχει σε οποιοδήποτε άλλο ερευνητικό

πρόγραμμα;

Διαβάσατε και καταλάβατε τις πληροφορίες για ασθενείς ή/και εθελοντές;

Είχατε την ευκαιρία να ρωτήσετε ερωτήσεις και να συζητήσετε το Πρόγραμμα;

Δόθηκαν ικανοποιητικές απαντήσεις και εξηγήσεις στα τυχόν ερωτήματά σας;

Καταλαβαίνετε ότι μπορείτε να αποσυρθείτε από το πρόγραμμα, όποτε θέλετε;

Καταλαβαίνετε ότι, εάν αποσυρθείτε, δεν είναι αναγκαίο να δώσετε

οποιεσδήποτε εξηγήσεις για την απόφαση που πήρατε;

Συμφωνείτε να συμμετάσχετε στην έρευνα;

Συμφωνείτε να συμμετάσχει το παιδί σας στην έρευνα;

Με ποιόν υπεύθυνο μιλήσατε;

Επίθετο

Μητέρας:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα: ………………………………………..

Υπογραφή: ……………………………………………….………

Ημερομηνία:

.......................................

Επίθετο

Πατέρα:

……………………………………………….……… Όνομα:

……………………………………….

Υπογραφή: Ημερομηνία:

Επίθετο

Κηδεμόνα:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα:

………………………………………..

Υπογραφή:

……………………………………………….……….

Ημερομηνία: ………………………………………..

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 92: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

ΕΝΤΥΠΑ ΣΥΓΚΑΤΑΘΕΣΗΣ

για συμμετοχή σε πρόγραμμα έρευνας

(Τα έντυπα αποτελούνται συνολικά από 4 σελίδες)

Σύντομος Τίτλος του Προγράμματος στο οποίο καλείστε να συμμετάσχετε

Κατανόηση και Ρύθμιση Συναισθημάτων

Πληροφορίες για Εθελοντές:

Αγαπητέ γονέα/κηδεμόνα,

Η παρούσα έρευνα, η οποία πραγματοποιείται στα πλαίσια διδακτορικής διατριβής στο

Τμήμα Ψυχολογίας του Πανεπιστημίου Κύπρου, πρόκειται να μελετήσει ψυχοκοινωνικούς

παράγοντες που εμπλέκονται στην κατανόηση και ρύθμιση των συναισθημάτων σε έφηβους.

Η αντίληψη και ρύθμιση των συναισθημάτων είναι σημαντικό μέρος της ψυχολογικής υγείας

και έχει προστατευτικό ρόλο όσον αφορά εύρος δυσκολιών που αντιμετωπίζουν τόσο οι

έφηβοι όσο και οι ενήλικες. Ως εκ τούτου, είναι ιδιαίτερα σημαντικό να μελετήσουμε

περαιτέρω τέτοιους παράγοντες ώστε να αναπτυχθούν τα αναγκαία προγράμματα πρόληψης

και θεραπείας.

Η έρευνα αυτή θα διεξαχθεί σε 10 σχολεία από διάφορες επαρχίες της Κύπρου και το

σχολείο του παιδιού σας επιλέχθηκε με τυχαίο τρόπο από μια ολοκληρωμένη λίστα όλων των

σχολείων Μέση Εκπαίδευσης τα οποία λειτουργούν στην Κύπρο.

Η παρούσα έρευνα περιλαμβάνει τη συμπλήρωση ερωτηματολογίων από εσάς τους

γονείς/κηδεμόνες και το παιδί σας. Το ερωτηματολόγιο που θα σας ζητηθεί να

συμπληρώσετε θα δοθεί στο παιδί σας στο σχολείο, το οποίο θα το παραδώσει με τη σειρά

του σε εσάς. Το ερωτηματολόγιο αυτό περιέχει ερωτήσεις για την κατανόηση και ρύθμιση

συναισθημάτων. Παράλληλα, τα παιδιά που θα συμμετέχουν στην έρευνα θα συμπληρώσουν

ερωτηματολόγια που θα τους δοθούν από κάποιον από τους ερευνητές στην τάξη τους. Τα

ερωτηματολόγια διερευνούν την κατανόηση και ρύθμιση συναισθημάτων και τις σχέσεις

τους με τους φίλους και την οικογένεια τους. Όλα τα ερωτηματολόγια περιέχουν ερωτήσεις

που είναι κατάλληλες για την συγκεκριμένη ηλικία και στάδιο ανάπτυξης, τόσο όσον αφορά

το λεξιλόγιο που χρησιμοποιείται αλλά και γενικώς το περιεχόμενο. Δεν υπάρχουν ερωτήσεις

οι οποίες είναι ακατάλληλες ή θα μπορούσαν να βλάψουν το παιδί σας. Ο χρόνος

συμπλήρωσης των ερωτηματολογίων εκτιμάται ότι θα είναι 60 λεπτά για τα παιδιά και 25

λεπτά για εσάς τους γονείς/κηδεμόνες.

Επίθετο

Μητέρας:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα: ………………………………………..

Υπογραφή: ……………………………………………….………

Ημερομηνία:

.......................................

Επίθετο

Πατέρα:

……………………………………………….……… Όνομα:

……………………………………….

Υπογραφή: Ημερομηνία:

Επίθετο

Κηδεμόνα:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα:

………………………………………..

Υπογραφή:

……………………………………………….………. Ημερομηνία:

………………………………………..

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 93: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Τα ερωτηματολόγια θα δοθούν σε σφραγισμένο φάκελο σε κάποιον από τους ερευνητές και

για καταχώρηση των πληροφοριών. Τα συμπληρωμένα ερωτηματολόγια θα κωδικοποιηθούν,

συνεπώς δεν θα αναγράφονται οι προσωπικές πληροφορίες των συμμετεχόντων. Οι

πληροφορίες που θα δοθούν από τους συμμετέχοντες θα αξιοποιηθούν για άντληση

πληροφοριών για το σύνολο των συμμετεχόντων και όχι μεμονωμένα ή σε ατομικό επίπεδο.

Τα ερωτηματολόγια και όλα τα στοιχεία από την έρευνα θα φυλάσσονται σε χώρο στον

οποίο θα έχουν πρόσβαση μόνο οι ερευνητές και θα χρησιμοποιηθούν αποκλειστικά για την

παρούσα έρευνα. Τα στοιχεία θα καταστραφούν μετά την πάροδο 5 ετών από έναν εκ των

ερευνητών.

Τόσο η δική σας συμμετοχή όσο και του παιδιού σας είναι εθελοντική και είστε ελεύθερος/-ή

να αποφασίσετε κατά πόσο θα θέλατε να λάβετε μέρος στην έρευνα. Εάν σε οποιαδήποτε

στιγμή θελήσετε να διακόψετε τη συμμετοχή σας ή αυτή του παιδιού σας, είστε ελεύθεροι να

το κάνετε χωρίς να δώσετε οποιεσδήποτε εξηγήσεις για την απόφαση σας. Όλες οι

πληροφορίες που θα μας παρέχετε είναι εντελώς εμπιστευτικές και δεν θα αποκαλυφθούν σε

κανένα.

Σας ευχαριστούμε εκ των προτέρων για την πολύτιμη συμβολή σας στην έρευνα αυτή, τα

ευρήματα της οποίας ελπίζουμε πως θα είναι σημαντικά για την ψυχική υγεία των εφήβων

και την ποιότητα των σχέσεων μεταξύ γονέων/κηδεμόνων και παιδιών.

Σε περίπτωση που υπάρχουν απορίες ή διευκρινήσεις οι ερευνητές είναι στη διάθεση σας

(κα. Φίλια-Άννα Χριστοδούλου και Δρ. Ειρήνη-Άννα Διακίδου, ερευνητικός επόπτης). Για

οποιαδήποτε παράπονα παρακαλώ επικοινωνήστε με τον Δρ. Γρηγόρη Μακρίδη (Διευθυντή

Έρευνας, Διεθνών Σχέσεων και Δημοσίων Σχέσεων του Πανεπιστημίου Κύπρου) στο

τηλέφωνο 22894287. Σε περίπτωση που επιθυμείτε να λάβετε τα γενικά αποτελέσματα για

την έρευνα παρακαλώ σημειώστε με √ στο κουτάκι στο τέλος της σελίδας και καταγράψετε

την ηλεκτρονική σας διεύθυνση.

Επίθετο

Μητέρας:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα: ………………………………………..

Υπογραφή: ……………………………………………….………

Ημερομηνία:

.......................................

Επίθετο

Πατέρα:

……………………………………………….……… Όνομα:

……………………………………….

Υπογραφή: Ημερομηνία:

Επίθετο

Κηδεμόνα:

……………………………………………….……….

Όνομα:

………………………………………..

Υπογραφή:

……………………………………………….……….

Ημερομηνία: ………………………………………..

ΕΠΙΘΥΜΩ ΝΑ ΛΑΒΩ ΠΕΡΙΛΗΨΗ ΤΩΝ ΓΕΝΙΚΩΝ ΑΠΟΤΕΛΕΣΜΑΤΩΝ ΤΗΣ

ΕΡΕΥΝΑΣ

Ηλεκτρονική διεύθυνση: ……………………………………………………

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 94: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix B

Table 1. Age and gender by education level

___________________________________________________________________________

Education level

_______________________________________

Junior High High school

___________________________________________________________________________

N 196 359

Age 12.70 (.47) 15.49 (.43)

Male 72 143

Female 91 162

___________________________________________________________________________

Note. Gender information was missing for 88 participants.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 95: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix C

Oδηγίες: Σας παρακαλούμε να σημειώσετε με ένα κύκλο τον αριθμό που αντανακλά καλύτερα το βαθμό

συμφωνίας ή διαφωνίας σας με κάθε μια από τις προτάσεις που ακολουθούν. Όσο πιο πολύ διαφωνείτε με μια

πρόταση, τόσο η απάντηση σας θα πλησιάζει το «1». Αντίθετα, όσο πιο πολύ συμφωνείτε, τόσο η απάντηση

σας θα πλησιάζει το «7». Μη σκέφτεστε πολύ ώρα για την ακριβή σημασία των προτάσεων. Δουλέψτε γρήγορα

και προσπαθήστε να απαντήσετε όσο το δυνατόν με μεγαλύτερη ακρίβεια. Σας υπενθυμίζουμε ότι δεν

υπάρχουν σωστές ή λάθος απαντήσεις.

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

1. Συνήθως μπορώ να ελέγχω αρκετά καλά τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να κατανοήσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3. Όταν μαθαίνω καλά νέα, δυσκολεύομαι να ηρεμήσω γρήγορα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4. Έχω τη τάση να βλέπω δυσκολίες σε κάθε ευκαιρία παρά ευκαιρίες σε κάθε δυσκολία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5. Γενικά είμαι απαισιόδοξος άνθρωπος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6. Δεν έχω πολλές χαρούμενες αναμνήσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7. Δεν είναι πρόβλημα για μένα το να κατανοώ τις ανάγκες και τις επιθυμίες των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8. Πιστεύω ότι γενικά τα πράγματα θα εξελιχθούν καλά στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

9. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να καταλάβω ποιο ακριβώς συναίσθημα νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

10. Είμαι «κοινωνικά αδέξιος». 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

11. Ακόμα και αν το θέλω πολύ, δυσκολεύομαι να πω στους άλλους ότι τους αγαπώ. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

12. Οι άλλοι με θαυμάζουν γιατί είμαι «άνετος». 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

13. Σπάνια σκέφτομαι παλιούς φίλους από το παρελθόν. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

14. Όταν το θέλω, μου είναι εύκολο να πω στους άλλους πόσο πραγματικά σημαντικοί μου

είναι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

15. Γενικά για να δουλέψω σκληρά πρέπει να βρίσκομαι υπό πίεση. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

16. Συχνά ανακατεύομαι σε καταστάσεις και αργότερα το μετανιώνω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

17. Μπορώ να «διαβάζω» τα συναισθήματα των περισσότερων ανθρώπων σαν ανοιχτό

βιβλίο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

18. Συνήθως μπορώ να επηρεάσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

19. Μου είναι δύσκολο να ηρεμήσω κάποιον όταν είναι θυμωμένος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

20. Υπάρχουν πολλές οικογενειακές καταστάσεις που δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

21. Γενικά ελπίζω για το καλύτερο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

22. Οι άλλοι μου λένε ότι με θαυμαζουν για την ακεραιότητα και την τιμιότητά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

23. Δε μου αρέσει καθόλου ν’ ακούω τα προβλήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

24. Συνήθως μπορώ να «μπω στη θέση του άλλου» και να καταλάβω τα συναισθήματά

του. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

25. Νομίζω πως είμαι γεμάτος από προσωπικές αδυναμίες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

26. Μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τις συνήθειές μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

27. Όταν θέλω πάντα βρίσκω τρόπους για να εκφράσω στοργή και τρυφερότητα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

28. Πιστεύω πως έχω πολλά χαρίσματα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

29. Συχνά κάνω πράγματα χωρίς τον παραμικρό σχεδιασμό. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 96: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

30. Δυσκολεύομαι να μιλήσω για τα ενδόμυχα συναισθήματά μου ακόμη και στους πιο

κοντινούς μου φίλους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

31. Οι περισσότεροι άνθρωποι τα καταφέρνουν καλύτερα από μένα στη ζωή τους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

32. Ποτέ δεν είμαι πραγματικά σίγουρος για τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

33. Συνήθως είμαι ικανός να εκφράζω τα συναισθήματά μου όταν το θέλω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

34. Όταν διαφωνώ με κάποιον, συνήθως μου είναι εύκολο να το εκφράσω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

35. Δεν έχω αρκετά κίνητρα στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

36. Ξέρω πως να ελέγξω αρνητικές σκέψεις και συναισθήματα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

37. Γενικά δυσκολεύομαι να περιγράψω τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

38. Μου είναι δύσκολο να μη στενοχωρηθώ όταν κάποιος μου αναφέρει κάτι δυσάρεστο

που του συνέβη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

39. Όταν κάτι με ξαφνιάσει, δεν μπορώ να το βγάλω εύκολα από το μυαλό μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

40. Συχνά, σταματώ αυτό που κάνω και συγκεντρώνομαι σε αυτό που νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

41. Τείνω να βλέπω το ποτήρι μισο-άδειο παρά μισο-γεμάτο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

42. Συχνά το βρίσκω δύσκολο να δω τα πράγματα από την οπτική γωνία των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

43. Γενικά προτιμώ να ακολουθώ άλλους παρά να τους καθοδηγώ. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

44. Οι κοντινοί μου άνθρωποι παραπονιούνται ότι δεν τους συμπεριφέρομαι σωστά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

45. Πολλές φορές δεν μπορώ να κατανοήσω τι συναισθήματα νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

46. Δε θα μπορούσα να επηρεάσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ακόμη και αν το ήθελα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

47. Όταν ζηλεύω κάποιον μου είναι δύσκολο να μην του συμπεριφερθώ άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

48. Συχνά αγχώνομαι από καταστάσεις στις οποίες οι άλλοι νιώθουν άνετα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

49. Μου είναι δύσκολο να νιώσω συμπόνοια για τα προβλήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

50. Kάποιες φορές στο παρελθόν πιστώθηκα λαθραία τις επιτυχίες άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

51. Γενικά είμαι καλός στο να αντιμετωπίζω αλλαγές στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

52. Πιστεύω πως δεν έχω καθόλου επιρροή στα συναισθήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

53. Πάντα έχω λόγους να επιμένω σ’ αυτό που κάνω και να μην τα παρατάω εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

54. Μου αρέσει να προσπαθώ ακόμη και για πράγματα που δεν είναι πραγματικά

σημαντικά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

55. Όταν κάνω κάτι λάθος, πάντα αναλαμβάνω τις ευθύνες μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

56. Έχω την τάση να αλλάζω γνώμη συχνά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

57. Όταν διαφωνώ με κάποιον, μπορώ να δω μόνο τη δική μου άποψη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

58. Στο τέλος τα πράγματα παίρνουν το σωστό δρόμο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

59. Όταν διαφωνώ έντονα με κάποιον, προτιμώ να παραμένω σιωπηλός παρά να κάνω

σκηνή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

60. Εάν το ήθελα, θα μου ήταν εύκολο να κάνω κάποιον να αισθανθεί άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

61. Θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως ήρεμο άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

62. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να δείχνω στοργή στους κοντινούς μου ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

63. Υπάρχουν πολλοί λόγοι που με κάνουν να περιμένω το χειρότερο στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

64. Συνήθως το βρίσκω δύσκολο να εκφράσω τις σκέψεις μου ξεκάθαρα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

65. Δεν με πειράζει να αλλάζω συχνά την καθημερινή μου ρουτίνα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

66. Οι περισσότεροι άνθρωποι είναι πιο συμπαθητικοί από μένα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

67. Οι κοντινοί μου άνθρωποι σπάνια παραπονιούνται για το πώς τους συμπεριφέρομαι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 97: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

68. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να εκφράσω τα συναισθήματά μου με τον τρόπο που θα ήθελα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

69. Γενικά, μπορώ να προσαρμόζομαι σε καινούρια περιβάλλοντα και καταστάσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

70. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να προσαρμόζω τη ζωή μου ανάλογα με τις περιστάσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

71. Θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως καλό διαπραγματευτή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

72. Μπορώ να χειριστώ αποτελεσματικά τους άλλους ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

73. Γενικά είμαι ένα ιδιαίτερα δραστήριο άτομο με στόχους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

74. Όταν ήμουνα μικρό παιδί, έκλεψα πράγματα που ανήκαν σε άλλους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

75. Σε γενικές γραμμές, είμαι ευχαριστημένος από τη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

76. Δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω τον εαυτό μου όταν είμαι υπερβολικά χαρούμενος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

77. Κάποιες φορές νιώθω πως παράγω πολύ καλή δουλειά χωρίς καθόλου προσπάθεια. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

78. Όταν αποφασίζω κάτι, είμαι πάντα σίγουρος ότι η απόφασή μου είναι σωστή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

79. Εάν έβγαινα ραντεβού στα τυφλά, το άλλο άτομο θα απογοητευόταν από την εμφάνισή

μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

80. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να προσαρμόσω τη συμπεριφορά μου ανάλογα με τους

ανθρώπους τριγύρω μου . 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

81. Μου είναι εύκολο να ταυτιστώ με κάποιον άλλο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

82. Προσπαθώ να ρυθμίζω το πρόγραμμα μου προκειμένου να ελέγχω το άγχος μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

83. Δε νομίζω πως είμαι άχρηστος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

84. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να ελέγξω τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

85. Αντιμετωπίζω τις δυσκολίες στη ζωή μου με ηρεμία και ψυχραιμία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

86. Εάν ήθελα, θα μου ήταν εύκολο να κάνω κάποιον να θυμώσει. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

87. Σε γενικές γραμμές, ο εαυτός μου μ’αρέσει. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

88. Πιστεύω πως έχω πολλά προτερήματα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

89. Γενικά δε βρίσκω τη ζωή διασκεδαστική. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

90. Όταν κάποιος με εξοργίσει, συνήθως μπορώ να ηρεμήσω γρήγορα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

91. Μπορώ να παραμείνω ήρεμος ακόμη και όταν νιώθω υπερβολικά χαρούμενος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

92. Γενικά, δεν είμαι καλός στο να παρηγορώ τους άλλους όταν νιώθουν άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

93. Συνήθως μπορώ να επιλύω διαφορές και αντιδικίες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

94. Πάντα βάζω πρώτα τη δουλειά και μετά τη διασκέδασή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

95. Δε δυσκολεύομαι καθόλου να φανταστώ τον εαυτό μου στη θέση κάποιου άλλου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

96. Χρειάζομαι ιδιαίτερο αυτοέλεγχο για να μένω μακριά από μπλεξίματα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

97. Μου είναι εύκολο να βρίσκω τις κατάλληλες λέξεις για να περιγράφω τα

συναισθήματά που νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

98. Προσδοκώ πως, στο μεγαλύτερο μέρος της, η ζωή μου θα είναι απολαυστική. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

99. Είμαι ένας συνηθισμένος άνθρωπος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

100. Έχω την τάση να παρασύρομαι εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

101. Συνήθως προσπαθώ να αντιστέκομαι σε δυσάρεστες σκέψεις και να σκέφτομαι θετικές

εναλλακτικές λύσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

102. Δεν μου αρέσει να κάνω σχέδια για το μέλλον. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

103. Μόνο και μόνο κοιτώντας κάποιον μπορώ να καταλάβω πώς αισθάνεται. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

104. Η ζωή είναι ωραία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

105. Συνήθως το βρίσκω εύκολο να ηρεμήσω αφού τρομάξω από κάτι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 98: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

106. Θέλω πάντα να ελέγχω τα πράγματα και τις εξελίξεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

107. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να αλλάζω τη γνώμη των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

108. Είμαι γενικά καλός στιv κοινωνική κουβεντούλα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

109. Δε μου είναι μεγάλο πρόβλημα να ελέγξω τον παρορμητισμό μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

110. Η εμφάνισή μου δεν μου αρέσει καθόλου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

111. Μιλώ με τρόπο σωστό και ξεκάθαρο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

112. Σε γενικές γραμμές δεν είμαι ικανοποιημένος από το πώς αντιμετωπίζω το άγχος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

113. Συνήθως, τα συναισθήματα που νιώθω μου είναι ξεκάθαρα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

114. Δυσκολεύομαι να ηρεμήσω μετά από κάτι που μου προκάλεσε μεγάλη έκπληξη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

115. Σε γενικές γραμμές, θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως δυναμικό άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

116. Γενικά δεν είμαι χαρούμενο άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

117. Όταν κάποιος με προσβάλλει, συνήθως παραμένω ήρεμος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

118. Τα περισσότερα πράγματα που καταφέρνω να κάνω καλά απαιτούν μεγάλη

προσπάθεια εκ μέρους μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

119. Ποτέ μου δεν έχω πει ψέματα για να αποφύγω να πληγώσω κάποιον. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

120. Δυσκολεύομαι να δεθώ πολύ ακόμη και με όσους βρίσκονται πολύ κοντά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

121. Πριν πάρω μια απόφαση σκέφτομαι προσεκτικά όλα τα πλεονεκτήματα και τα

μειονεκτήματα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

122. Δεν ξέρω πώς να κάνω τους άλλους να νιώσουν καλύτερα όταν το έχουν ανάγκη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

123. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τις απόψεις μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

124. Οι άλλοι μου λένε ότι σπάνια μιλάω για τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

125. Γενικά, είμαι ικανοποιημένος από τις στενές μου σχέσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

126. Μπορώ να αναγνωρίσω ένα συναίσθημα από τη στιγμή που αναπτύσσεται μέσα μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

127. Μου αρέσει να βάζω τα συμφέροντα των άλλων πάνω από τα δικά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

128. Τις περισσότερες μέρες αισθάνομαι υπέροχα που είμαι ζωντανός. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

129. Ευχαριστιέμαι πάρα πολύ όταν κάνω κάτι καλά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

130. Είναι πολύ σημαντικό για εμένα να τα πηγαίνω καλά με τους στενούς φίλους μου και

την οικογένεια μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

131. Kάνω ευχάριστες σκέψεις συχνά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

132. Έχω πολύ έντονους καυγάδες με τους κοντινούς μου ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

133. Δε δυσκολεύομαι καθόλου να εκφράσω τα συναισθήματά μου με λόγια. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

134. Μου είναι δύσκολο να ευχαριστηθώ τη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

135. Συνήθως έχω την ικανότητα να επηρεάζω τους άλλους ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

136. Συχνά όταν είμαι αγχωμένος χάνω την ψυχραιμία μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

137. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τη συμπεριφορά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

138. Οι άλλοι με έχουν ως πρότυπο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

139. Οι άλλοι μου λένε πως αγχώνομαι πολύ εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

140. Συνήθως μπορώ να βρω τρόπους να ελέγξω τα συναισθήματά μου όταν το θέλω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

141. Θεωρώ πως θα γινόμουν καλός πωλητής. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

142. Χάνω πολύ εύκολα το ενδιαφέρον μου για πράγματα που μπορώ να κάνω εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

143. Έχω πολλές συνήθειες και ρουτίνες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 99: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

144. Πάντα θα υπεράσπιζα τις απόψεις μου ακόμη και εάν αυτό σήμαινε το να μαλώσω με

σημαντικούς ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

145. Πιστεύω πως προσαρμόζομαι εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

146. Γενικά, χρειάζομαι πολλά κίνητρα για να προσπαθήσω να δώσω τον καλύτερο εαυτό

μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

147. Ακόμη και όταν αντιπαρατίθεμαι με κάποιον, συνήθως είμαι σε θέση να καταλάβω τη

δική του οπτική γωνία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

148. Γενικά, είμαι ικανός να αντιμετωπίσω το άγχος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

149. Προσπαθώ να αποφεύγω τους ανθρώπους που με αγχώνουν. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

150. Συχνά κάνω πράγματα χωρίς να σκεφτώ καλά τις συνέπειες τους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

151. Έχω την τάση να υποχωρώ ακόμη και όταν γνωρίζω πως έχω δίκιο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

152. Υπάρχουν πολλές καταστάσεις στη δουλειά που δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

153. Κάποιες απαντήσεις που έδωσα στο παραπάνω ερωτηματολόγιο δεν είναι 100%

ειλικρινείς. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 100: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Οδηγίες: Σας παρακαλούμε απαντήστε στις παρακάτω ερωτήσεις, οι οποίες έχουν συμπεριληφθεί

στο ερωτηματολόγιο προκειμένου να διευκολύνουν τη στατιστική επεξεργασία των δεδομένων. Σας

θυμίζουμε ότι οι απαντήσεις σας είναι εντελώς ανώνυμες. Εάν σας είναι πολύ δύσκολο να

απαντήσετε κάποια ερώτηση, παρακαλούμε αφήστε την κενή.

1. ΦΥΛΟ (σημειώστε το κατάλληλο κουτάκι) ΑΝΔΡΑΣ ΓΥΝΑΙΚΑ

2. ΕΤΟΣ ΓΕΝΝΗΣΗΣ __________

3. ΕΧΕΤΕ ΠΤΥΧΙΟ; ΝΑΙ ΟΧΙ

ΕΑΝ ΝΑΙ, ΤΙ ΠΤΥΧΙΟ ΕΧΕΤΕ; ΑΕΙ ΤΕΙ

4. ΣΗΜΕΙΩΣΤΕ ΤΗΝ ΚΑΤΗΓΟΡΙΑ ΠΟΥ ΑΝΤΙΣΤΟΙΧΕΙ ΣΤΟ ΕΤΗΣΙΟ ΚΑΘΑΡΟ ΟΙΚΟΓΕΝΕΙΑΚΟ

ΣΑΣ ΕΙΣΟΔΗΜΑ.

500 – 1.000 € 1.000 – 3.000 € 3.000 – 5.000 €

5.000 – 10.000 € 10.000 – 20.000 € 20.000 – 30.000 € 30.000 + €

5. ΑΠΟ ΠΟΣΑ ΜΕΛΗ ΑΠΟΤΕΛΕΙΤΑΙ Η ΟΙΚΟΓΕΝΕΙΑ ΣΑΣ;

1 2 3 4 5 6 7+

6. ΠΟΙΑ ΕΙΝΑΙ Η ΟΙΚΟΓΕΝΕΙΑΚΗ ΣΑΣ ΚΑΤΑΣΤΑΣΗ;

ΑΓΑΜΟΣ ΠΑΝΤΡΕΜΕΝΟΣ ΧΗΡΟΣ

ΔΙΑΖΕΥΓΜΕΝΟΣ ΣΥΖΩ ΜΕ ΚΑΠΟΙΟΝ

7. ΠΟΣΑ ΠΑΙΔΙΑ ΕΧΕΤΕ;

0 1 2 3 4 5 6+

8 . ΠΟΣΑ ΑΔΕΛΦΙΑ ΕΧΕΤΕ (ΑΔΕΛΦΟΥΣ ΚΑΙ ΑΔΕΛΦΕΣ);

0 1 2 3 4 5 6+

9. ΠΟΣΟ ΘΡΗΣΚΟΣ ΕΙΣΤΕ; (βάλτε κύκλο γύρω από τον κατάλληλο αριθμό)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

ΚΑΘΟΛΟΥ ΠΑΡΑ ΠΟΛΥ

10. ΟΙ ΠΟΛΙΤΙΚΕΣ ΣΑΣ ΠΕΠΟΙΘΗΣΕΙΣ ΤΕΙΝΟΥΝ ΓΕΝΙΚΩΣ ΠΡΟΣ ΤΗΝ:

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

ΑΡΙΣΤΕΡΑ ΔΕΞΙΑ

Σας ευχαριστούμε πολύ για τη συμμετοχή σας.

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 101: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix D

Table 2. Sampling domain of trait EI

Facets High scorers perceive themselves as…

Adaptability Flexible and willing to adapt to new

conditions

Assertiveness Forthright, frank, and willing to stand up for

their rights

Emotion expression Capable of communicating their feelings to

others

Emotion management (others) Capable of influencing other people’s

feelings

Emotion perception (self and others) Clear about their own and other people’s

feelings

Emotion regulation Capable of controlling their emotions

Impulsiveness (low) Reflective and less likely to give in to their

urges

Relationships Capable of having fulfilling personal

relationships

Self-esteem Successful and self-confident

Self-motivation Driven and unlikely to give up in the face of

adversity

Social awareness Accomplished networkers with excellent

social skills

Stress management Capable of withstanding pressure and

regulating stress

Trait empathy Capable of taking someone else’s

perspective

Trait happiness Cheerful and satisfied with their lives

Trait optimism Confident and likely to ‘look on the bright

side’ of life

Reproduced from Petrides (2011) Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 102: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix E

Oδηγίες: Σας παρακαλούμε να σημειώσετε με ένα κύκλο τον αριθμό που αντανακλά καλύτερα το βαθμό

συμφωνίας ή διαφωνίας σας με κάθε μια από τις προτάσεις που ακολουθούν. Όσο πιο πολύ διαφωνείτε με μια

πρόταση, τόσο η απάντηση σας θα πλησιάζει το «1». Αντίθετα, όσο πιο πολύ συμφωνείτε, τόσο η απάντηση

σας θα πλησιάζει το «7». Μη σκέφτεστε πολύ ώρα για την ακριβή σημασία των προτάσεων. Δουλέψτε γρήγορα

και προσπαθήστε να απαντήσετε όσο το δυνατόν με μεγαλύτερη ακρίβεια. Σας υπενθυμίζουμε ότι δεν

υπάρχουν σωστές ή λάθος απαντήσεις.

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

1. Συνήθως μπορώ να ελέγχω αρκετά καλά τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να κατανοήσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3. Όταν μαθαίνω καλά νέα, δυσκολεύομαι να ηρεμήσω γρήγορα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4. Τείνω να βλέπω δυσκολίες στα πάντα 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5. Γενικά έχω αρνητική εικόνα για τα περισσότερα πράγματα 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6. Δεν έχω πολλές χαρούμενες αναμνήσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7. Δεν είναι πρόβλημα για μένα το να κατανοώ τις ανάγκες και τις επιθυμίες των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8. Πιστεύω ότι γενικά τα πράγματα θα εξελιχθούν καλά στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

9. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να καταλάβω ποιο ακριβώς συναίσθημα νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

10. Δεν έχω κοινωνικές δεξιότητες 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

11. Ακόμα και αν το θέλω πολύ, δυσκολεύομαι να πω στους άλλους ότι τους αγαπώ. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

12. Οι άλλοι με θαυμάζουν γιατί είμαι «άνετος». 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

13. Σπάνια σκέφτομαι παλιούς φίλους από το παρελθόν. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

14. Όταν το θέλω, μου είναι εύκολο να πω στους άλλους πόσο πραγματικά σημαντικοί μου

είναι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

15. Γενικά για να δουλέψω σκληρά πρέπει να βρίσκομαι υπό πίεση. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

16. Συχνά ανακατεύομαι σε καταστάσεις και αργότερα το μετανιώνω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

17. Μπορώ να «διαβάζω» τα συναισθήματα των περισσότερων ανθρώπων σαν ανοιχτό

βιβλίο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

18. Συνήθως μπορώ να επηρεάσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

19. Μου είναι δύσκολο να ηρεμήσω κάποιον όταν είναι θυμωμένος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

20. Υπάρχουν πολλές οικογενειακές καταστάσεις που δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

21. Γενικά ελπίζω για το καλύτερο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

22. Οι άλλοι μου λένε ότι με θαυμάζουν για την ειλικρίνεια μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

23. Δε μου αρέσει καθόλου ν’ ακούω τα προβλήματα των φίλων μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

24. Συνήθως μπορώ να «μπω στη θέση του άλλου» και να καταλάβω τα συναισθήματά

του. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

25. Νομίζω πως είμαι γεμάτος από προσωπικές αδυναμίες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

26. Μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τα πράγματα που έχω συνηθίσει και μου αρέσουν. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

27. Όταν θέλω πάντα βρίσκω τρόπους για να εκφράσω στοργή και τρυφερότητα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 103: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

28. Πιστεύω πως έχω θετικά στοιχεία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

29. Συχνά κάνω πράγματα βιαστικά χωρίς τον παραμικρό σχεδιασμό. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

30. Δυσκολεύομαι να μιλήσω για τα προσωπικά συναισθήματά μου ακόμη και στους πιο

κοντινούς μου φίλους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

31. Δεν μπορώ να κάνω πράγματα όσο καλά μπορούν άλλοι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

32. Ποτέ δεν είμαι πραγματικά σίγουρος για τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

33. Συνήθως είμαι ικανός να εκφράζω τα συναισθήματά μου όταν το θέλω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

34. Όταν διαφωνώ με κάποιον, συνήθως μου είναι εύκολο να το εκφράσω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

35. Συνήθως το βρίσκω δύσκολο να κρατήσω τον εαυτό μου κινητοποιημένο 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

36. Ξέρω πως να ελέγξω την αρνητική μου διάθεση 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

37. Γενικά δυσκολεύομαι να περιγράψω τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

38. Μου είναι δύσκολο να μη στενοχωρηθώ όταν κάποιος μου αναφέρει κάτι δυσάρεστο

που του συνέβη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

39. Όταν κάτι με ξαφνιάσει, δεν μπορώ να το βγάλω εύκολα από το μυαλό μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

40. Συχνά, σταματώ αυτό που κάνω και συγκεντρώνομαι σε αυτό που νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

41. Τείνω να βλέπω το ποτήρι μισο-άδειο παρά μισο-γεμάτο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

42. Συχνά το βρίσκω δύσκολο να δω τα πράγματα από την πλευρά των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

43. Γενικά προτιμώ να ακολουθώ άλλους παρά να τους καθοδηγώ. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

44. Οι κοντινοί μου άνθρωποι παραπονιούνται ότι δεν τους συμπεριφέρομαι σωστά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

45. Πολλές φορές δεν μπορώ να κατανοήσω τι συναισθήματα νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

46. Δε θα μπορούσα να επηρεάσω τα συναισθήματα των άλλων ακόμη και αν το ήθελα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

47. Όταν ζηλεύω κάποιον μου είναι δύσκολο να μην του συμπεριφερθώ άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

48. Συχνά αγχώνομαι από καταστάσεις στις οποίες οι άλλοι νιώθουν άνετα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

49. Μου είναι δύσκολο να νιώσω συμπόνοια για τα προβλήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

50. Kάποιες φορές στο παρελθόν, έχω αποδεχθεί την επιβράβευση για τις ιδέες άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

51. Γενικά είμαι καλός στο να αντιμετωπίζω αλλαγές στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

52. Πιστεύω πως δεν μπορώ να επηρεάσω καθόλου τα συναισθήματα των άλλων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

53. Πάντα έχω λόγους να επιμένω σ’ αυτό που κάνω και να μην τα παρατάω εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

54. Μου αρέσει να προσπαθώ ακόμη και για πράγματα που δεν είναι πραγματικά

σημαντικά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

55. Όταν κάνω κάτι λάθος, πάντα αναλαμβάνω τις ευθύνες μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

56. Έχω την τάση να αλλάζω γνώμη συχνά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

57. Όταν διαφωνώ με κάποιον, μπορώ να δω μόνο τη δική μου άποψη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

58. Στο τέλος τα πράγματα παίρνουν το σωστό δρόμο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

59. Όταν διαφωνώ έντονα με κάποιον, προτιμώ να παραμένω σιωπηλός παρά να κάνω

σκηνή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

60. Εάν το ήθελα, θα μου ήταν εύκολο να κάνω κάποιον να αισθανθεί άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

61. Θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως ήρεμο άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

62. Συχνά δυσκολεύομαι να δείχνω στοργή στους κοντινούς μου ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

63. Υπάρχουν πολλοί λόγοι που με κάνουν να περιμένω το χειρότερο στη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

64. Συνήθως το βρίσκω δύσκολο να εκφράσω τις σκέψεις μου ξεκάθαρα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

65. Δεν με πειράζει να αλλάζω συχνά την καθημερινή μου ρουτίνα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 104: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

66. Οι περισσότεροι άνθρωποι είναι πιο συμπαθητικοί από μένα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

67. Οι κοντινοί μου άνθρωποι σπάνια παραπονιούνται για το πώς τους συμπεριφέρομαι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

68. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να εκφράσω τα συναισθήματά μου με τον τρόπο που θα ήθελα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

69. Γενικά, μπορώ να προσαρμόζομαι σε καινούρια περιβάλλοντα και καταστάσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

70. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να προσαρμόζω τη ζωή μου ανάλογα με το τι συμβαίνει. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

71. Θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως καλό διαπραγματευτή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

72. Μπορώ να χειριστώ αποτελεσματικά τους άλλους ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

73. Γενικά είμαι ένα άτομο με δυνατά κίνητρα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

74. Στο παρελθόν, έχω κλέψει πράγματα που ανήκαν σε άλλους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

75. Σε γενικές γραμμές, είμαι ευχαριστημένος από τη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

76. Δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω τον εαυτό μου όταν είμαι υπερβολικά χαρούμενος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

77. Κάποιες φορές νιώθω πως παράγω πολύ καλή δουλειά χωρίς καθόλου προσπάθεια. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

78. Όταν αποφασίζω κάτι, είμαι πάντα σίγουρος ότι η απόφασή μου είναι σωστή. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

79. Εάν έβγαινα ραντεβού στα τυφλά, το άλλο άτομο θα απογοητευόταν από την εμφάνισή

μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

80. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να προσαρμόσω τη συμπεριφορά μου ανάλογα με τους

ανθρώπους τριγύρω μου . 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

81. Μου είναι εύκολο να ταυτιστώ με κάποιον άλλο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

82. Προσπαθώ να ελέγχω τα επίπεδα του άγχους μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

83. Δε νομίζω πως είμαι άχρηστος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

84. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να ισορροπήσω τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

85. Αντιμετωπίζω τις δυσκολίες στη ζωή μου με ηρεμία και ψυχραιμία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

86. Εάν ήθελα, θα μου ήταν εύκολο να κάνω κάποιον να θυμώσει. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

87. Σε γενικές γραμμές, ο εαυτός μου μ’αρέσει. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

88. Πιστεύω πως έχω πολλά προτερήματα/δυνατότητες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

89. Γενικά δε βρίσκω τη ζωή διασκεδαστική. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

90. Όταν κάποιος με θυμώσει, συνήθως μπορώ να ηρεμήσω γρήγορα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

91. Μπορώ να παραμείνω ήρεμος ακόμη και όταν νιώθω υπερβολικά χαρούμενος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

92. Γενικά, δεν είμαι καλός στο να παρηγορώ τους άλλους όταν νιώθουν άσχημα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

93. Συνήθως μπορώ να επιλύω διαφορές και διαφωνίες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

94. Πάντα βάζω πρώτα τη δουλειά και μετά τη διασκέδασή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

95. Δε δυσκολεύομαι καθόλου να φανταστώ τον εαυτό μου στη θέση κάποιου άλλου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

96. Χρειάζομαι ιδιαίτερο αυτοέλεγχο για να μένω μακριά από μπελάδες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

97. Μου είναι εύκολο να βρίσκω τις κατάλληλες λέξεις για να περιγράφω τα

συναισθήματά που νιώθω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

98. Αναμένω πως, στο μεγαλύτερο μέρος της, η ζωή μου θα είναι απολαυστική. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

99. Είμαι ένας συνηθισμένος άνθρωπος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

100. Έχω την τάση να παρασύρομαι εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

101. Συνήθως προσπαθώ να αντιστέκομαι σε αρνητικές σκέψεις και να σκέφτομαι θετικές

εναλλακτικές λύσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

102. Δεν μου αρέσει να κάνω σχέδια για το μέλλον. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

103. Μόνο και μόνο κοιτώντας κάποιον μπορώ να καταλάβω πώς αισθάνεται. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 105: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

104. Η ζωή είναι ωραία. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

105. Συνήθως το βρίσκω εύκολο να ηρεμήσω αφού τρομάξω από κάτι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

106. Θέλω πάντα να ελέγχω τα πράγματα και τις εξελίξεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

107. Συνήθως δυσκολεύομαι να αλλάζω τη γνώμη των άλλων ανθρώπων. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

108. Είμαι γενικά καλός στηv κοινωνική κουβεντούλα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

109. Δε μου είναι μεγάλο πρόβλημα να ελέγξω τον εαυτό/παρορμητισμό μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

110. Η εμφάνισή μου δεν μου αρέσει καθόλου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

111. Μιλώ με τρόπο σωστό και ξεκάθαρο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

112. Σε γενικές γραμμές δεν είμαι ικανοποιημένος από το πώς αντιμετωπίζω το άγχος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

113. Συνήθως, ξέρω ακριβώς γιατί αισθάνομαι έτσι όπως αισθάνομαι. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

114. Δυσκολεύομαι να ηρεμήσω μετά από κάτι που μου προκάλεσε μεγάλη έκπληξη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

115. Σε γενικές γραμμές, θα περιέγραφα τον εαυτό μου ως δυναμικό άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

116. Γενικά δεν είμαι χαρούμενο άτομο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

117. Όταν κάποιος με προσβάλλει, συνήθως παραμένω ήρεμος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

118. Τα περισσότερα πράγματα που καταφέρνω να κάνω καλά απαιτούν μεγάλη

προσπάθεια εκ μέρους μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

119. Ποτέ μου δεν έχω πει ψέματα για να αποφύγω να πληγώσω κάποιον. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

120. Δυσκολεύομαι να δεθώ πολύ ακόμη και με όσους βρίσκονται πολύ κοντά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

121. Πριν πάρω μια απόφαση σκέφτομαι προσεκτικά όλα τα πλεονεκτήματα (υπέρ) και τα

μειονεκτήματα (κατά). 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

122. Δεν ξέρω πώς να κάνω τους άλλους να νιώσουν καλύτερα όταν το έχουν ανάγκη. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

123. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τη στάση και τις απόψεις μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

124. Οι άλλοι μου λένε ότι σπάνια μιλάω για τα συναισθήματά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

125. Γενικά, είμαι ικανοποιημένος από τις στενές μου σχέσεις. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

126. Μπορώ να αναγνωρίσω ένα συναίσθημα από τη στιγμή που αναπτύσσεται μέσα μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

127. Μου αρέσει να βάζω τα συμφέροντα των άλλων πάνω από τα δικά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

128. Τις περισσότερες μέρες αισθάνομαι υπέροχα που είμαι ζωντανός. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

129. Ευχαριστιέμαι πάρα πολύ όταν κάνω κάτι καλά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

130. Είναι πολύ σημαντικό για εμένα να τα πηγαίνω καλά με τους στενούς φίλους μου και

την οικογένεια μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

131. Kάνω ευχάριστες σκέψεις συχνά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

132. Έχω πολύ έντονους καυγάδες με τους κοντινούς μου ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

133. Δε δυσκολεύομαι καθόλου να εκφράσω τα συναισθήματά μου με λόγια. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

134. Μου είναι δύσκολο να ευχαριστηθώ τη ζωή μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

135. Συνήθως έχω την ικανότητα να επηρεάζω τους άλλους ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

136. Συχνά όταν είμαι αγχωμένος χάνω την ψυχραιμία μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

137. Συνήθως μου είναι δύσκολο να αλλάξω τη συμπεριφορά μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

138. Οι άλλοι με έχουν ως πρότυπο (με θαυμάζουν). 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

139. Οι άλλοι μου λένε πως αγχώνομαι πολύ εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

140. Συνήθως μπορώ να βρω τρόπους να ελέγξω τα συναισθήματά μου όταν το θέλω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

141. Θεωρώ πως θα γινόμουν καλός πωλητής. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 106: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Διαφωνώ Απόλυτα 1. . . . . . . 2 . . . . . . . 3 . . . . . . . 4 . . . . . . . 5 . . . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . 7 Συμφωνώ Απόλυτα

142. Χάνω πολύ εύκολα το ενδιαφέρον μου για πράγματα που μπορώ να κάνω εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

143. Έχω πολλές συνήθειες και ρουτίνες. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

144. Πάντα θα υπεράσπιζα τις απόψεις μου ακόμη και εάν αυτό σήμαινε το να μαλώσω με

σημαντικούς ανθρώπους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

145. Πιστεύω πως προσαρμόζομαι εύκολα. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

146. Γενικά, χρειάζομαι αρκετή ενθάρρυνση για να προσπαθήσω να δώσω τον καλύτερο

εαυτό μου. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

147. Ακόμη και όταν καυγαδίζω με κάποιον, συνήθως είμαι σε θέση να καταλάβω τη δική

του πλευρά. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

148. Γενικά, είμαι ικανός να αντιμετωπίσω το άγχος. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

149. Προσπαθώ να αποφεύγω τους ανθρώπους που με αγχώνουν. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

150. Συχνά κάνω πράγματα χωρίς να σκεφτώ καλά τις συνέπειες τους. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

151. Έχω την τάση να υποχωρώ ακόμη και όταν γνωρίζω πως έχω δίκιο. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

152. Υπάρχουν πολλές καταστάσεις στο σχολείο που δυσκολεύομαι να ελέγξω. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

153. Κάποιες απαντήσεις που έδωσα σε αυτό το ερωτηματολόγιο δεν είναι 100%

ειλικρινείς. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 107: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix F

Πιο κάτω βρίσκονται 15 προτάσεις. Πόσο αληθεύει η κάθε πρόταση για σένα; Ο καθένας έχει την δική του απάντηση. Προσπάθησε να

απαντήσεις τι νιώθεις. Αυτό δεν είναι τεστ και δεν υπάρχουν σωστές ή λάθος απαντήσεις. Διάβασε την κάθε πρόταση προσεκτικά. Μετέπειτα,

διάλεξε μία από τις 5 απαντήσεις στο κουτί πιο κάτω. Η κάθε απάντηση έχει ένα αριθμό. Κύκλωσε τον αριθμό της απάντησης που σε

περιγράφει καλύτερα.

1 2 3 4 5

εντελώς λανθασμένο λανθασμένο μερικώς λανθασμένο/μερικώς σωστό σωστό πολύ σωστό

1. Κάνω εύκολα φιλίες με άλλα παιδιά 1 2 3 4 5

2. Δεν νιώθω άνετα προσπαθώντας να κάνω φίλους 1 2 3 4 5

3. Εύκολα εξαρτώμαι από τους άλλους, εάν μου είναι καλοί φίλοι 1 2 3 4 5

4. Κάποιες φορές οι άλλοι είναι υπερβολικά φιλικοί και κοντά σε εμένα 1 2 3 4 5

5. Κάποιες φορές φοβάμαι ότι τα άλλα παιδιά δεν θα θέλουν να είναι μαζί μου 1 2 3 4 5

6. Θα ήθελα να είμαι πραγματικά κοντά σε κάποια παιδιά και να είμαι συνεχώς μαζί τους 1 2 3 4 5

7. Είναι εντάξει μαζί μου εάν καλοί φίλοι με εμπιστεύονται και εξαρτώνται από μένα 1 2 3 4 5

8. Είναι δύσκολο για μένα να εμπιστευτώ τους άλλους εντελώς 1 2 3 4 5

9. Κάποιες φορές νιώθω ότι οι άλλοι δεν θέλουν να είναι καλοί φίλοι μαζί μου όσο εγώ μαζί τους 1 2 3 4 5

10. Συνήθως πιστεύω ότι τα άτομα που είναι κοντά σε εμένα δεν θα με αφήσουν 1 2 3 4 5

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 108: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

11. Κάποιες φορές φοβάμαι ότι κανένας δεν με αγαπά πραγματικά 1 2 3 4 5

12. Το βρίσκω άβολο και ενοχλούμαι όταν κάποιος προσπαθεί να έρθει πολύ κοντά μου 1 2 3 4 5

13. Μου είναι δύσκολο να εμπιστευτώ πραγματικά τους άλλους, ακόμα και όταν μου είναι καλοί φίλοι 1 2 3 4 5

14. Κάποιες φορές τα άλλα παιδιά με αποφεύγουν όταν θέλω να έρθω κοντά τους

και να είμαι καλός/ή τους φίλος/η 1 2 3 4 5

15. Συνήθως, όταν κάποιος προσπαθεί να έρθει πολύ κοντά μου δεν με ενοχλεί 1 2 3 4 5

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 109: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix G

Οδηγίες:

Σε αυτό το ερωτηματολόγιο θα διαβάσεις ερωτήσεις οι οποίες σχετίζονται με τους γονείς σου. Θα σου ζητηθεί να βαθμολογήσεις τη

συμπεριφορά της ΜΗΤΕΡΑΣ και του ΠΑΤΕΡΑ σου. Για κάθε ερώτηση κύκλωσε τη δήλωση που αντιστοιχεί στο πώς ο γονέας σου

συμπεριφέρεται απέναντι σου. Αν αυτή τη στιγμή δε διαμένεις με τους βιολογικούς σου γονείς, παρακαλώ βαθμολόγησε τη συμπεριφορά του

ατόμου που θεωρείς εσύ ότι είναι η μητέρα ή ο πατέρας σου (π.χ. θετός γονέας, μητρυιά, πατριός).

Ποτέ Σπάνια Μερικές Φορές Συχνά Πολύ Συχνά Πάντα

1 2 3 4 5 6

Παρακαλώ βαθμολόγησε τη συμπεριφορά της ΜΗΤΕΡΑΣ και του ΠΑΤΕΡΑ σου κυκλώνοντας την απάντηση σου.

Η ΜΗΤΕΡΑ ΜΟΥ Ο ΠΑΤΕΡΑΣ ΜΟΥ

1. Με στηρίζει 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

2. Με παρηγορεί όταν είμαι αναστατωμένος/η (παράδειγμα, μου δίνει σημασία και δείχνει ενδιαφέρον για μένα)

1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

3. Μου δείχνει ότι νοιάζεται για μένα 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

4. Δείχνει γνήσιο ενδιαφέρον σε εμένα (παράδειγμα: μου δίνει σημασία και δείχνει να ενδιαφέρεται για μένα)

1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 110: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Η ΜΗΤΕΡΑ ΜΟΥ Ο ΠΑΤΕΡΑΣ ΜΟΥ

5. Θυμάται πράγματα τα οποία είναι σημαντικά για μένα 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

6. Είναι διαθέσιμος/η για να μιλήσει σε οποιαδήποτε στιγμή 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

7. Ρωτά ερωτήσεις με τρόπο που δείχνει ότι νοιάζεται 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

8. Περνά επιπλέον χρόνο μαζί μου απλά γιατί θέλει να γνωρίσει τα ενδιαφέροντα μου

1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

9. Είναι πρόθυμος/η να συζητήσει τα προβλήματα μου ` 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

10. Επιδιώκει να συζητήσει τα ενδιαφέροντα μου (παράδειγμα: προσπαθεί να μου μιλήσει για πράγματα που μου αρέσουν)

1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

11. Εκτιμά τις απόψεις μου (παράδειγμα: νοιάζεται για τις ιδέες μου) 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

12. Συναισθηματικά είναι διαθέσιμη όταν τον/την χρειαστώ 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

13. Με κάνει να νιώθω επιθυμητός/ή 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

14. Με παινεύει (παράδειγμα: μου λέει θετικά πράγματα για μένα) 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

15. Είναι κατανοητικός/η 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 111: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix H

Πως αισθάνομαι για τον Εαυτό μου και για Άλλους

Οδηγίες: Πιο κάτω υπάρχουν κάποιες δηλώσεις για το πώς αισθάνεσαι για τον εαυτό σου και

για άλλους ανθρώπους. Σημείωσε με Χ στο κουτί που λέει πόσο αλήθεια είναι η κάθε

δήλωση για σένα.

1. Είμαι αρκετά χαρούμενος άνθρωπος

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

2. Νιώθω πολλή μοναξιά.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

3. Αναστατώνομαι όταν οι γονείς μου ή οι φίλοι μου φεύγουν εκτός πόλης για λίγες μέρες.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

4. Κάνω πράγματα που οι άλλοι θεωρούν τρελά ή εκτός ελέγχου.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

5. Αισθάνομαι ένα περίπου το ίδιο συνέχεια. Τα αισθήματα μου δεν αλλάζουν πολύ συχνά.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

6. Θέλω να ξέρουν κάποιοι άνθρωποι πόσο με έχουν πληγώσει.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 112: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

7. Κάνω πράγματα χωρίς να σκέφτομαι.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

8. Τα συναισθήματα μου είναι πολύ έντονα. Για παράδειγμα, όταν θυμώνω, θυμώνω πάρα

πάρα πολύ, και όταν χαίρομαι, χαίρομαι πάρα πάρα πολύ.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

9. Αισθάνομαι πως υπάρχει κάτι σημαντικό που λείπει σε μένα αλλά δεν ξέρω τι είναι.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

10. Έχω διαλέξει φίλους/φίλες που μου έχουν συμπεριφερθεί άσχημα.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

11. . Είμαι απερίσκεπτος/η με πράγματα που είναι σημαντικά για μένα.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

12. . Αλλάζω γνώμη σχεδόν κάθε μέρα για το τι να κάνω όταν μεγαλώσω.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

13. Άνθρωποι που ήταν κοντά μου με έχουν απογοητεύσει.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 113: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

14. Πηγαίνω μπρος και πίσω μεταξύ διάφορων συναισθημάτων, όπως το να είμαι

θυμωμένος/η ή λυπημένος/η ή χαρούμενος/η.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

15. Μπλέκω σε μπελάδες επειδή κάνω πράγματα χωρίς να σκέφτομαι.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

16. Ανησυχώ ότι οι άνθρωποι για τους οποίους νοιάζομαι θα φύγουν και δεν θα ξανάρθουν.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

17. . Όταν θυμώνω δεν μπορώ να ελέγξω τι κάνω.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

18. Το πως αισθάνομαι για τον εαυτό μου αλλάζει πολύ.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

19. . Όταν αναστατώνομαι, κάνω πράγματα που δεν είναι καλά για μένα.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

20. Πολλές φορές, οι φίλοι μου και εγώ είμαστε πολύ κακοί μεταξύ μας.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 114: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

21. Θυμώνω τόσο πολύ που δεν μπορώ να αφήσω όλο μου το θυμό να βγει.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

22. Βαριέμαι πολύ εύκολα.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

23. . Προσέχω πολύ τα πράγματα που είναι δικά μου.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

24. Όταν κάποιος γίνει φίλος/η μου, μένουμε φίλοι.

Καθόλου

Αλήθεια

Σχεδόν Καθόλου

Αλήθεια (Σπάνια

Αλήθεια)

Κάποτε

Αλήθεια

Συχνά Αλήθεια

Πάντα Αλήθεια

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 115: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix I

Table 3. Multicollinearity statistics for the main variables

Tolerance VIF

LEAPm .674 1.483

LEAPf .674 1.484

Anxious/Ambivalent .998 1.002

Trati EI (Global) .993 1.007

Self-control .756 1.323

Emotionality .518 1.931

Sociability .607 1.646

Wellbeing .608 1.645

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 116: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix J

Table 4. Means and standard deviations on main measures for junior high participants

Min Max Mode Mean SD

BPFSC 28 96 59 61.36 10.93

LEAPm 15 90 90 80.20 12.45

LEAPf 15 90 90 74.94 16.41

Trait EI

(global)

3.25 6.16 3.25 4.61 .53

Table 5. Means and standard deviations on main measures for high school participants

Min Max Mode Mean SD

BPFSC 32 90 63 60.90 11.30

LEAPm 22 90 90 81.15 11.09

LEAPf 15 90 90 78.23 13.75

Trait EI

(global)

2.82 6.34 2.82 4.74 .57

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 117: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix K

Table 6. Mean scores and standard deviations for the TEIQue subscales for junior high

participants

___________________________________________________________________________

Variable Mean SD

___________________________________________________________________________

Self esteem 4.80 .83

Emotion expression 4.53 .93

Self motivation 4.68 .69

Emotion regulation 4.10 .78

Happiness 5.42 1.04

Empathy 4.70 .79

Social awareness 4.62 .82

Impulsivity (low) 4.42 .94

Emotion perception 4.48 .82

Stress management 4.16 .82

Emotion management 4.41 .85

Optimism 5.26 .87

Relationships 5.02 .88

Adaptability 4.02 .70

Assertiveness 4.51 .80

___________________________________________________________________________

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 118: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix L

Table 7. Mean scores and standard deviations for the TEIQue subscales for high school

participants

___________________________________________________________________________

Variable Mean SD

___________________________________________________________________________

Self esteem 4.90 .94

Emotion expression 4.50 1.04

Self motivation 4.73 .69

Emotion regulation 4.15 .87

Happiness 5.49 1.15

Empathy 4.90 .82

Social awareness 4.78 .81

Impulsivity (low) 4.64 .92

Emotion perception 4.58 .79

Stress management 4.20 .97

Emotion management 4.73 .89

Optimism 5.29 .97

Relationships 5.33 .89

Adaptability 4.08 .80

Assertiveness 4.79 .86

___________________________________________________________________________

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 119: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix M

Table 8. Mean scores and standard deviations for the TEIQue subscales for parents

___________________________________________________________________________

Variable Mean SD

___________________________________________________________________________

Self esteem 4.93 .81

Emotion expression 4.96 1.00

Self motivation 5.04 .81

Emotion regulation 4.49 .80

Happiness 5.55 1.03

Empathy 5.08 .75

Social awareness 4.98 .83

Impulsivity (low) 4.91 .91

Emotion perception 5.01 .84

Stress management 4.28 .90

Emotion management 4.57 .84

Optimism 5.27 .97

Relationships 5.40 .77

Adaptability 4.39 .86

Assertiveness 4.64 .77

___________________________________________________________________________

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 120: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix N

Table 9. Reliability values for the 15 subscales and four factors of the TEIQue

Cronbach’s alpha

Self-esteem .73

Emotion expression .74

Self-motivation .53

Emotion regulation .69

Happiness .81

Empathy .58

Social awareness .72

Impulsivity (low) .68

Emotion perception .64

Stress management .66

Emotion management .63

Optimism .73

Relationships .64

Adaptability .57

Assertiveness .56

Well-being .84

Self-control .70

Emotionality .76

Sociability .77

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 121: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix O

Table 10. Factor loadings for the 15 items of the ASCQ

___________________________________________________________________________

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4

___________________________________________________________________________

Item 1 .62

Item 2 .46 -.43

Item 3 .59

Item 4 .68

Item 5 .73

Item 6 .37 .53

Item 7 .58

Item 8 .80

Item 9 .65

Item 10 .60

Item 11 .62

Item 12 -.69

Item 13 .71

Item 14 .69

Item 15 .70

___________________________________________________________________________

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 122: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix P

Table 11. Factor matrix for the LEAPm and LEAPf

LEAPm LEAPf

Supports me .69 .79

Consoles me .78 .83

Shows she cares .74 .81

Genuine interest .75 .84

Remembers things .73 .78

Available to talk .75 .79

Asks questions .79 .83

Spends extra time .66 .70

Wants to talk about my troubles .77 .82

Wants to talk about my interests .69 .77

Values my input .78 .81

Emotionally available .80 .83

Makes me feel wanted .76 .80

Praises me .70 .76

Is understanding .74 .78

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 123: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix Q

Table 12. Factor loadings for the BPFS-C

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 6 Factor 7

1. ‘I’m a pretty happy person’ .38

2. ‘I feel very lonely’ -.51

3. ‘I get upset when people leave town for a few days’ .59

4. ‘I do things that are wild’ .61

5. ‘I feel the same way all the time’

6. ‘I want people to know they’ve hurt me’ .43

7. ‘I do things without thinking’ .65

8. ‘My feelings are very strong’ -.58

9. ‘I feel there’s something missing in me’

10. ‘I’ve picked friends who’ve treated me badly’ -.35

11. ‘I’m careless about things that are important to me’ .31

12. ‘I change my mind about what I should do when I grow up’ -.37

13. ‘People who were close to me have let me down’

14. ‘I go back forth between different feelings’ -.40

15. ‘I get into trouble for doing things without thinking’ .54

16. ‘I worry that people will leave and not come back’ .32

17. ‘When I’m mad I can’t control what I do’ -.72

18. ‘How I feel about myself changes a lot’ -.72

19. ‘When I get upset, I do things that aren’t good for me’ -.38

20. ‘Lots of times, my friends and I, are mean to each other’ -.55

21. ‘I get so mad, I can’t let all my anger out’ -.37

22. ‘I get bored very easily’ -.45

23. ‘I take good care of things that are mine’ .37

24. ‘Once someone is my friend, we stay friends .47

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 124: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix R

Table 13. Correlations between main variables and the trait EI factors for junior high participants

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. BPFSC __

2. LEAPm -.12 __

3. LEAPf -.18** .50** __

4. Anxious/Ambivalent .38** .01 .01 __

5. Global trait EI -.06 -.08 -.02 .08 __

6. Well-being -.09 -.01 .04 .08 .85** __

7. Self-control -.01 -.15* -.03 .10 .71** .44** __

8. Emotionality -.06 -.08 -.04 .03 .90** .73** .54** __

9. Sociability -.03 -.01 -.00 .05 .77** .60** .36** .62** __

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 125: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

Appendix S

Table 14. Correlations between main variables and the trait EI factors for high school participants

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. BPFSC __

2. LEAPm -.19** __

3. LEAPf -.17** .62** __

4. Anxious/Ambivalent .53** -.09 -.08 __

5. Global trait EI .01 -.01 .06 .00 __

6. Well-being .02 -.02 .01 .02 .83** __

7. Self-control .03 .03 .09 .03 .70** .46** __

8. Emotionality -.04 -.02 .04 -.05 .85** .59** .47** __

9. Sociability .00 -.01 .08 .01 .76** .54** .31** .62** __

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 126: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

120

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix T

Table 15. Correlations between BPDF and TEIQue subscales (Junior High)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

1. Self-esteem _

2. Emotion

expression

.51** _

3. Self-motivation .39** .39** __

4. Emotion

regulation

.26** .35** .19** __

5. Happiness .52** .37** .47** .17* __

6. Empathy .27** .40** .38** 20** .36** __

7. Social

awareness

.51** .50** .41** .36** ..52** 42** __

8. Impulsivity

(low)

.32** .37** .31** .45** .27** .31** .29** __

9. Emotion

perception

.53** .51** .36** .34** .44** .31** .57** .36** __

10. Stress

management

.35**

.

.41** .23** .43** .17* .06 .24** .32** .35** __

11. Emotion

management

.39** .31** .35** .11 .28** .32** .57** .17* .46** .21** __

12. Optimism .58** .53** .44** .37** .61** .40** .55** .48** .47** .23** .28** __

13. Relationships .40** .37** .46** .28** .65** .49** .47** .43** .40** .20** .25** .54** __

14. Adaptability .24** .39** .25** .44** .18* .15* .30** .37** .43** . 41** .12 .30* .22** __

15. Assertiveness .50** .37** .24** .15* .32** .18** .46** .28** .40** .23** .42** .38** .28** .16*

__

16. BPDF -.02 -.02 -.04 -.05 -.09 .00 -.04 .04 -.05 -.03 -.06 -.09 -.10 -.04 .03 __

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 127: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

121

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix U

Table 16. Correlations between BPDF and TEIQue subscales (High School)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

1. Self-esteem _

2. Emotion

expression

.47** _

3. Self-motivation .56** .44** _

4. Emotion

regulation

.16** .17** .23** _

5. Happiness .61** .47** .54** .27** _

6. Empathy .14** .28** .37** .18** .21** __

7. Social

awareness

.59** .54** .50** .28** .50** .42** __

8. Impulsivity

(low)

.35** .31** .44** .46** .43** .29** .43** __

9. Emotion

perception

.47** .55** .48** .32** .38** .34** .60** .38** __

10. Stress

management

.29** .25** .36** .60** .30** .21** .28** .32** .28** __

11. Emotion

management

.34** .24** .31** .05 .19** .30** .57** .12* .45** .10** __

12. Optimism .59** .41** .55** .29** .74** .20** .45** .42** .40** .35** .13* __

13. Relationships .39** .42** .53** .22** .55** .48** .48** .48** .37** .27** .16*

*

.45** __

14. Adaptability .30** .21** .32** .41** .28** .17** .30** .30** .25** .40** .13* .33* .26** __

15. Assertiveness .58** .44** .50** .15** .46** .20** .62** .32** .51** .19** .48*

*

.42** .37** .17** __

16. BPDF -.01 -.08 .03 -.02 -.01 -.03 -.02 .04 .04 .06 -.01 .06 -.02 .02 .03 __

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 128: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

122

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix V

Table 17. Regression coefficients for the 15 trait EI facets, junior high and high school

participants

Junior High High School

Variable B SE B B SE B

Self-esteem .63 .05 -.37 -.03

Emotion expression .15 .01 -1.94 -.18

Self-motivation .33 .02 .10 .01

Emotion regulation -.94 -.07 -1.88 -.15

Happiness -.38 -.04 -.52 -.05

Empathy 1.05 .08 -.42 -.03

Social awareness .87 .07 -.56 -.04

Impulsivity (low) 1.80 .15 .82 .07

Emotion perception -.26 -.02 2.15 .15

Stress management -.18 -.01 1.40 .12

Emotion

management

-1.37 -.11 -.49 -.04

Optimism -1.74 -.14 1.30 .11

Relationships -1.63 -.13 -.49 -.04

Adaptability -.51 -.03 .26 .02

Assertiveness 1.12 .08 .83 .06

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 129: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

123

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix W

Table 18. Regression coefficients for the main variables as predictors of BPDF

Junior High High School

Variable B SE B B SE B

LEAPm -.02 -.03 -.11 -.11

LEAPf -.12 -.17 -.06 -.07

Anxious/Ambivalent 4.36 .38 6.14 .51

Emotion expression -.50 -.05

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 130: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

124

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix X

Table 19. Regression coefficients for the trait EI subscales as predictors of BPDF as a

function of gender

Girls Boys

Variable B SE B B SE B

Self-esteem -.19 -.02 -.64 -.05

Emotion expression -1.40 -.13 -1.58 -.13

Self-motivation .30 .02 1.92 .12

Emotion regulation -1.16 -.09 -2.26 -.16

Happiness .45 .04 -1.19 -.12

Empathy -.30 -.02 1.31 .09

Social awareness -1.24 -.09 .13 .01

Impulsivity (low) .50 .04 1.60 .13

Emotion perception 2.82 .20 -.66 -.05

Stress management .20 .02 2.23 .18

Emotion

management

.05 .00 -1.34 -.10

Optimism -.90 -.07 1.90 .16

Relationships .18 .02 -2.64 -.21

Adaptability 1.62 .11 -1.17 -.08

Assertiveness -.05 -.00 2.66 .19

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 131: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

125

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix Y

Table 20. Correlations between parental and child trait EI subscales and BPDF 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

1. Self-esteem __

2. Emotion

expression

.55** __

3. Self-

motivation

.59** .57** __

4. Emotion

regulation

.40** .38** .50** __

5. Happiness .67** .52** .63** .45** __

6. Empathy .43** .47** .41** .35** .43** __

7. Social

awareness

.62** .67**

.60** .40** .53** .50** __

8. Impulsivity

(low)

.46** .38** .61** .45** .46** .33** .46** __

9. Emotion

perception

.54** .64** .45 .34** .46** .46** .63** .27** __

10. Stress

management

.51** .46** .46** .51** .52** .29** .46** .39** .36** __

11. Emotion

management

.56** .54** .48** .32** .41** .47** .61** .30** .59** .40** __

12. Optimism .59** .46** .54** .44** .70** .34** .45** .40** .38** .50** .37** __

13.

Relationships

.55** .48** .50** .30** .62** .45** .51** .53** .51** .35** .42** .48** __

14.

Adaptability

.30** .37** .37 .37** .34** .20** .33** .22** .17 .41** .29** .32** .22** __

15.

Assertiveness

.48** .43** .48** .26** .29** .29** .57** .39** .38** .39** .47** .31** .40** .27** __

16. BPDF -.00 -.07 -.06 .05 -.01 -.02 -.07 -.01 -.08 .06 -.12 .02 .02 .02 -.01 __

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u

Page 132: DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN …

126

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT EI AND BPD FEATURES

Appendix Z

Table 21. Regression coefficients for parental trait EI subscales and adolescent BPDF scores

Variable B SE B

Self-esteem 1.23 .09

Emotion expression -.71 -.06

Self-motivation -1.46 -.10

Emotion regulation .96 .07

Happiness -.86 -.08

Empathy .54 .04

Social awareness -.97 -.07

Impulsivity (low) -.43 -.04

Emotion perception -.46 -.03

Stress management .99 .08

Emotion management -2.35 -.18

Optimism .44 .04

Relationships 1.81 .12

Adaptability 1.80 .14

Assertiveness .61 .04

Filia-A

nna C

hristo

doulo

u