differential course of executive control changes during normal aging

Upload: feergarcia

Post on 09-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    1/26

    Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14: 370393, 2007

    http://www.psypress.com/anc

    ISSN: 1382-5585/05 print; 1744-4128 online

    DOI: 10.1080/13825580600678442

    NANC1382-5585/051744-4128Aging,Neuropsychology, and Cognition,Vol. 00, No.0,June 2006: pp.147Aging,Neuropsychology, and Cognition

    Differential Course of Executive ControlChanges During Normal AgingExecutive ControlChanges During Normal AgingFriederikeH.Treitz etal.

    FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZ, KATRIN HEYDERAND IRENE DAUM

    Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr University of Bochum, Germany

    ABSTRACT

    Normal aging has been associated with executive control deficits, but it is as yet

    unclear whether different executive subprocesses are differentially affected during the

    course of aging. The present study aimed to investigate age effects on a range of exec-

    utive control subcomponents. Four consecutive age groups (2030 years, 3145 years,

    4660 years, 6175 years), matched on present state IQ and mood, were compared on

    tasks of strategic memory processing, verbal fluency, reasoning, inhibition, task man-

    agement, and self-rating of executive abilities. Deficits concerning the suppression of

    habitual and experimentally induced prepotent response tendencies and the ability to

    efficiently divide attention were observed in subjects over 60 years of age compared to

    the younger groups, while memory, verbal fluency, and reasoning were largely unaf-

    fected. Results suggest a sharp decline of executive function after age 60 and a differ-

    ential course of different executive subcomponents across aging, adding further

    support to a multi-dimensional model of executive function.

    The cognitive decline associated with normal aging is generally discussed in

    relation to mild neurodegenerative changes, including neuronal shrinkage,

    loss of dendritic and synaptic density, or alterations in neurotransmitter sys-

    tems (Backman et al., 2000; Jernigan et al., 2001; Kaasinen et al., 2000;

    Resnick et al., 2003; Tisserand & Jolles, 2003; Wang et al., 1995, 1998).Disproportionate volume loss occurs within the frontal lobes and the hippoc-

    ampal region, but changes in the thalamus and the mamillary bodies are also

    observed (Guttmann et al., 1998; Jernigan et al., 2001; Raz et al., 1992,

    2004; Resnick et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2001; Van Der Werf et al., 2001;

    Woodruff-Pak, 1997). With respect to the prefrontal cortex, grey matter vol-

    ume loss was reported to reach 8.9% per decade in subjects over 65 years of

    Address correspondence to: Friederike H. Treitz, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of

    Neuropsychology, Ruhr-University of Bochum, 44780 Bochum, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    2/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 371

    age (Van Petten et al., 2004). In addition, functional neuroimaging studies

    reported evidence of reduced activations within the prefrontal cortex in older

    compared to younger adults (Logan et al., 2002).

    Given the frequently reported neuropathological changes in the pre-

    frontal cortex (PFC) relative to other cortical areas (Head et al., 2004; Jernigan

    et al., 2001; Raz et al., 2004; Resnick et al., 2003), older age has been con-

    sidered a model of diffuse mild prefrontal dysfunction and associated execu-

    tive control impairments (West, 1996). This view is supported by parallels in

    the cognitive profile of healthy elderly subjects and young individuals with

    acquired PFC lesions (Daum et al., 1996; Daum & Mayes, 2000; Daum &

    Schugens, 1999), characterized by deficits in source memory, memory for

    temporal order, or impaired use of cognitive strategies (Daum et al., 1995;

    Daum & Schugens, 1999; Glisky et al., 2001; Mayes & Daum, 1997).Executive control has recently been described as a heterogeneous con-

    cept, involving five major subcomponents (Smith & Jonides, 1999):

    a.focusing attention on relevant information and inhibiting irrelevant

    information;

    b.task management, including switching attention between tasks;

    c.planning a sequence of subtasks to accomplish a goal;

    d.updating working memory contents to determine the next step in a

    sequential task;

    e.coding context of representations in working memory.

    Following Shallice and Burgess (1991), the prefrontal cortex is associ-

    ated with the supervisory attentional system (SAS), which is responsible for

    strategic control of mental processes, such as the use of strategies or rule-

    guided retrieval from long-term memory. In novel and unfamiliar situations,

    the SAS is responsible for strategy formation, planning, and problem-

    solving to achieve goals. Aging has been reported to adversely affect most of

    the executive control subcomponents defined by Smith and Jonides (1999),

    as well as executive processes describes by Shallice and Burgess (1991).

    Meta-analyses by Verhaeghen and Cerella (2002) yielded a consistent age-

    related decline of task management abilities beyond the effect of general

    slowing. Set-shifting deficits in older adults have been related to mild frontal

    dysfunction (Keys & White, 2000; Kramer et al., 1999; Kray & Lindenberger,

    2000; Meiran et al., 2001). Age effects have been reported for the use of orga-

    nizational strategies in memory, such as self-generated use of semantic catego-

    ries (Daum et al., 1996).

    Inhibition of habitual responses, as assessed by the Stroop Test (e.g.,

    Wecker et al., 2000), as well as inhibition of newly learned prepotentresponses, were also found to be affected by age (McDowd, 1997; Verhaeghen

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    3/26

    372 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    & De Meersman, 1998a). Whether these inhibition deficits can be attributed to

    a general slowing of information processing or whether they represent a spe-

    cific age-related executive impairment, is a matter of an ongoing debate

    (Gamboz et al., 2002; Grant & Dagenbach, 2000; Schelstraete & Hupet, 2002;

    Shilling et al., 2002; Uttl & Graf, 1997; Van der Linden, 2000; Verhaeghen &

    De Meersman, 1998b; Wecker et al., 2000; West & Alain, 2000).

    In spite of the wealth of empirical data, the pattern and course of age

    effects on executive function is as yet inconclusive. The available studies

    were mainly based on extreme group comparisons where a number of vari-

    ables other than age may influence group differences (e.g., Brink &

    McDowd, 1999; Grant & Dagenbach, 2000; Shilling et al., 2002; Van der

    Linden, 2000). Longitudinal studies often reported an accelerated decline in

    later life (e.g. Schaie, 1996), while cross-sectional studies suggest a graduallinear decline across adulthood (Park et al., 1996; Salthouse, 2003). By using

    a cross-sectional design with four consecutive age groups, the present study

    aimed to assess the course of executive function changes during adulthood.

    The investigation of consecutive age groups is useful to economically assess

    linear vs. critical threshold changes with respect to different subprocesses of

    executive control, even though cohort effects cannot be completely elimi-

    nated. The data should contribute to the issue of whether age-related changes

    manifest themselves gradually or whether deficits occur once a critical

    threshold has been reached, leading to a nonlinear change. In addition, dif-

    ferent executive control subprocesses may be associated with independentneuronal substrates (see Heyder et al., 2004; Smith & Jonides, 1999), and

    may therefore follow a differential course during aging. It has been sug-

    gested that processes related to the dorsolateral PFC are particularly affected

    by aging (MacPherson et al., 2002), given that there is evidence of relative

    sparing of the orbitofrontal region (Salat et al., 2001), although comparable

    effects on dorsolateral and orbital PFC regions have also been reported (Raz

    et al., 1997; Tisserand et al., 2002). These conflicting results may relate to

    the methods of volume measurement (Tisserand et al., 2002).

    In summary, the present study aimed to further elucidate the effect of

    normal aging on the course of executive control changes, focusing on task

    management and inhibition as the most elementary executive processes (see

    Smith & Jonides, 1999).

    METHODS

    Subjects

    Sixty-two healthy subjects (34 men and 28 women) were selected from

    a large subject pool to form four consecutive age groups matched on generalintellectual abilities. The first group comprised the 2030 year age range, the

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    4/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 373

    second group the 3145 year range, the third group the 4660 year range,

    and the oldest group the 6175 year range. The four groups were closely

    matched on present state IQ, as assessed by the Similarities and Picture

    Completion subtests of the short German version from the Wechsler Adult

    Intelligence Scale (Dahl, 1972). The groups were also matched on sex ratio

    and present-state mood (Visual Analogue Scales; Bond & Lader, 1974). For

    an overview of background data see Table 1.

    Subjects were recruited by advertisements in the local press. They were

    screened in an interview for health problems. Based on the interview, sub-

    jects were excluded from participation if they had suffered from psychiatric

    or neurological disease in the past or present or from diseases potentially

    effecting the central nervous system. Subjects gave written informed consent

    and received a 20 euro reimbursement.Neuropsychological Assessment

    Strategic Memory Processing

    To assess the self-generated use of memory strategies, verbal memory

    was assessed by word list recall (Daum & Mayes, 2000). Three lists consist-

    ing of 16 items each were read to the subject at a one word per second pre-

    sentation rate. The first list consisted of four items of each of four categories

    (metals, animals, landscape formations, vegetables), which were presented

    in order of category membership (consecutive categories list; CC). The sec-

    ond list also contained four items of each of four categories (items of cloth-

    ing, fruit, furniture, and weather conditions), which were presented in

    randomized order (randomized categories list; RC). Encoding and retrieval

    of the RC list can be improved by self-generated categorization of the list

    according to semantic categories. The third list was uncategorized (RR), i.e.,

    the 16 items were unrelated. List order was randomized across subjects. Sub-

    jects were asked to reproduce each list immediately after presentation.

    Delayed free recall was assessed after a 30-min delay. The number of cor-

    rectly reproduced items per list during both delays (immediate, delayed) as

    well as retention rates (correct items delayed recall/immediate recall) wereanalyzed.

    TABLE 1. Group Size, Mean (SDs) Age, IQ Estimates, and Scores in the VAS (Visual Analogue

    Scales) for the Consecutive Age Groups

    2030 yrs. 3145 yrs. 4660 yrs. 6175 yrs. p

    Mean age in yrs. 25.4 (3.3) 38.8 (4.3) 52.4 (3.9) 67.5 (4.4)

    N 16 16 13 17

    IQ estimate 111.4 (4.8) 114.4 (4.6) 112.0 (5.7) 112.0 (7.4) 0.45

    VAS 29.9 (11.9) 24.4 (9.3) 25.4 (11.4) 20.7 (13.3) 0.17

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    5/26

    374 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    Verbal Fluency/Cognitive Flexibility

    To assess efficiency of rule-guided retrieval from long-term memory, a

    verbal fluency task with three conditions was administered (see Daum et al.,1994). This procedure involved rule-guided search strategies, strategic

    retrieval from semantic long-term memory, as well as cognitive flexibility.

    The number of correctly produced exemplars was assessed in three fluency

    conditions: a semantic category condition (countries), a phonemic cate-

    gory condition (nouns beginning with B), and a switching condition (alter-

    nate vegetables and male first names). The switching condition was

    added as a measure of task management, which requires the switching of

    attention between tasks. Subjects were asked to produce as many exemplars

    as possible within 1 minute for each condition.

    Reasoning

    A German adaptation of the Cognitive Estimates Test (CET; see Shallice

    & Evans (1978) and Daum & Mayes (2000) was administered to assess the

    ability to draw plausible conclusions and to give realistic estimates based on

    the subjects knowledge and reasoning. Subjects were asked to give esti-

    mates for 10 problems such as How tall is the Cologne Cathedral? Based

    on criteria described by Hodges (1996), the scoring ranged from 0 (response

    within the normal range) to 3 (large deviation from the normal range).

    Everyday Behavioral Correlates of Executive Impairment

    To evaluate the everyday consequences of reduced executive control,

    the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) from the Behavioral Assessment of

    the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) Test battery (Wilson et al., 1996) was

    administered. The 20-item questionnaire addresses a range of problems

    associated with the Dysexecutive Syndrome including emotional or person-

    ality changes, motivational changes, or behavioral problems. Each item was

    scored on a 5-point scale ranging from never to very often. Sum scores

    were obtained for the self-rating and the independent rating, respectively. In

    the independent rating, the DEX was also completed by a close relative orfriend.

    Inhibition

    To assess the ability to focus attention on relevant information and to

    inhibit irrelevant responses, an adaptation of the Stroop Test (Bumler,

    1985) and a version of the AX-Continuous Performance Test (AX-CPT) pre-

    viously described by Braver et al. (1999) were administered.

    In the Stroop Test, subjects had to read out the names of colors printed

    in black (reading color words (RCW)), name the color of colored patches(NCP), and they also had to name the print color of color words, with print

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    6/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 375

    color and color names being incongruent (interference (INT)). To account for

    reading speed and color-naming speed, reaction times (RTs) in the interfer-

    ence condition were analyzed using RTs in the RCW and NCP conditions as

    covariates. The number of errors in the interference condition was also recorded.

    In the AX-CPT task, letters were presented sequentially on a computer

    screen. Letter size was 3.8 3.8 visual angle at a distance of 40 cm. In

    70% of the trials, the letter X followed the letter A and subjects had to

    press a target key to the A-X sequence. The remaining trial types were A

    followed by the letter Y (the inhibition trial type), B-X and B-Y trial types

    (Y represents all letters except X, and B represents all letters except

    A) with a frequency of 10% each. To these combinations, subjects had to

    press a nontarget key on the keyboard. Subjects used the index and middle

    finger of their dominant hand. The high frequency of the A-X combinationinduced a prepotent response tendency to the target key after presentation of

    the letter, A which had to be suppressed for the rare A-Y sequence (which

    requires the nontarget key).

    The task was administered in a short- and a long-delay condition (100

    trials each), with order being randomized across subjects. Each trial started

    with a hyphen presented for 200 ms in the center of the screen for fixation,

    followed by the first letter (200 ms). The interstimulus interval (ISI) was

    1000 ms (short-delay condition) or 4000 ms (long-delay condition), thereby

    varying working memory load. Then the second letter appeared for 200 ms,

    to which the subject had to respond.Median RTs and errors were recorded for each trial type. Based on the

    procedure used by Braver et al. (1999) and Carter et al. (1998), difference

    scores representing inhibition and use of context entered analysis:

    1. Context cost: RTs A-Y minus RTs B-Y: Degree of response slow-

    ing in nontarget trials where the prepotent response tendency has to

    be suppressed.

    2. Inhibition cost: RTs A-Y minus RTs A-X. Index of inhibition,

    where the prepotent response to the target key induced by A needs

    to be inhibited if Y occurs.

    3. Context use: RTs B-X minus RTs B-Y. Degree of response slowing

    in nontarget trials on the ambiguous X stimulus. Small differences

    between both trial types indicate benefit from use of context.

    Task Management

    Task management in stimulus processing was assessed by two tasks,

    the Divided Attention subtest of a German Attention Test Battery

    (Zimmermann & Fimm, 1993) and by a dual-task paradigm described byStablum et al. (2000). In the Divided Attention task, subjects had to

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    7/26

    376 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    process two sensory channels in parallel, while motor output did not differ.

    In the Dual Task, subjects had to coordinate between information from

    two sensory channels and two motor responses.

    In the Divided Attention task, subjects were asked to press a key as

    fast as possible to particular stimulus configurations presented in two sen-

    sory domains. In the visual domain, subjects had to respond if crosses

    appearing randomly on a computer screen formed a square. Subjects had to

    simultaneously attend to a sequence of high and low pitched tones. If two

    consecutive tones were of the same pitch, the response key had to be

    pressed. Reaction times and errors were recorded.

    The Dual Task procedure comprised a single- and a dual-task part

    and assessed the ability to process two stimulus features and to coordinate

    two motor responses. During both subtests, two letters (3.8 9.5

    visual

    angle at a distance of 40 cm) appeared in a vertical arrangement on a com-

    puter screen (17), either to the right or to the left of a central fixation point.

    The letters were either the same or different (50% each) and were presented

    for 150 msec. In the initial single task part, subjects were instructed to press

    a left or a right key, depending on the location relative to the central fixation

    point. In the dual task part, subjects also had to make the location decision

    by pressing the respective keys, but they additionally had to indicate ver-

    bally whether the letters were the same or different. Eighty trials were pre-

    sented in each condition; RTs and errors were recorded. In addition, dual

    task costs were assessed by calculating a dual task variable (DTC: (RTs dualtask RTs single task)/RTs single task).

    General Procedure

    All subjects completed the test battery in one session. Each session

    lasted 1.5 to 2 hours. Tasks were administered in the following order: Verbal

    Fluency, Cognitive Estimation Test, Word List Recall (immediately),

    Divided Attention, AX-CPT, Word List Recall (delayed), Stroop-Test, Dual-

    Task, DEX. AX-CPT test administration was followed by a rest interval.

    The list order for word list recall was randomized between subjects.

    Data Analysis

    Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 11.0 (SPSS

    Inc.). Group differences were evaluated by nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis-H or

    Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), where appropriate. If significant group dif-

    ferences were found, pairwise comparisons were performed (Mann-Whitney-U

    and Bonferroni, respectively). For nonparametric post-hoc comparisons, the

    significance level was set to p < .008. Repeated measure ANOVAs were

    performed with Bonferroni adjusted post-hoc testing. When Analysis of

    Covariance (ANCOVA) was performed, the significance level for post-hocpairwise ANCOVAS was set to p < .008. For Pearson correlations, the

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    8/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 377

    significance level was set top < .05. In addition, eta2 effect sizes were calcu-

    lated in case the demands for ANOVA were met.

    RESULTS

    Strategic Memory Processing

    The results for word list recall are presented in Table 2. Repeated mea-

    sures ANOVA with Group, List (CC, RC, RR) and Delay (immediate vs.

    delayed free recall) as factors did not yield a significant group effect (p =

    .19) or significant interactions involving the group factor (allp > .59). Effect

    size was eta2 = 0.3. A significant delay effect [F1,58 = 450.808; p < .001]

    indicated better recall at immediate relative to delayed recall. A significant

    list effect [F2,116 = 51.146; p < .001], followed by a paired t-test indicated

    best recall of the consecutive categories and randomized categories list and

    poorest recall of the uncategorized list (allp < .001).

    Analysis of retention rates with group and list as factors indicated a

    significant main effect for List [F2,116 = .511; p < .001], but no significant

    effects involving the group factor (allp > .79). For the CC-list, effect size

    was eta2 = 0.04, for the RC list it was eta2 = 0.01 and eta2 = 0.06 for the

    RR-list.

    Verbal Fluency/Cognitive Flexibility

    The results for the three fluency subtests are presented in Table 2. The

    four age groups did not differ significantly with respect to the number of

    correctly produced items in the semantic (p = .18; eta2 = 0.08), phonemic

    (p = .60; eta2 = 0.03), or switching condition (p = .25; eta2 = 0.07).

    Reasoning

    The results for the Cognitive Estimation Test are presented in Table 2.

    Nonparametric group comparisons did not reveal significant age group

    differences (Hdf = 3.58 = 2.265;p = .52).

    Everyday Consequences of Executive Impairment

    The results for the DEX questionnaire are presented in Table 2.

    Repeated measure ANOVA with group and condition (self-rating vs. inde-

    pendent rating of dysexecutive behavior) as factors did not yield a signifi-

    cant group effect (p = .56, eta2 = 0.04) or a significant interaction (p = .12,

    eta2 = 0.11).

    Separate group comparisons of subjects DEX self-rating and relatives

    independent rating of dysexecutive behavior did not reveal significant group

    differences [(F3,58 = .241; p = .87; eta2

    = 0.01) and (F5,50 = 1.392; p = .26;eta2 = 0.07), respectively].

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    9/26

    378 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    Inhibition

    Stroop Test

    Results for the Stroop Test are presented in Figure 1. ANCOVA with

    RTs in the RCW and NCP conditions as covariates indicated a significantgroup effect for RTs in the INT condition (F3,56 = 6.302;p = .001, eta

    2 = 0.25).

    TABLE 2. Means (SDs) for Cognitive Performance in the Four Age Groups

    2030 Yrs. 3145 Yrs. 4660 Yrs. 6175 Yrs.

    Strategic Memory Processing

    Word list recall immediately (no. correct)

    consecutive categories 10.6 (2.0) 10.9 (2.6) 9.5 (3.6) 9.7 (2.1)

    randomized categories 9.1 (2.3) 8.4 (3.5) 7.5 (2.3) 8.2 (2.3)

    uncategorized 7.6 (2.2) 7.7 (2.6) 6.3 (1.8) 6.4 (1.7)

    Word list recall delayed (no. correct)

    consecutive categories 6.8 (2.2) 6.4 (2.8) 5.7 (2.9) 5.1 (2.4)

    randomized categories 6.1 (3.2) 5.3 (3.8) 4.9 (2.6) 5.3 (2.5)

    uncategorized 2.6 (1.6) 3.4 (2.8) 2.6 (2.0) 1.8 (1.5)

    Verbal Fluency/Cognitive Flexibility (no. correct)

    semantic 29.9 (9.5) 28.1 (6.5) 25.7 (7.4) 24.5 (5.9)

    phonemic 13.1 (3.8) 12.9 (2.6) 13.2 (3.2) 11.8 (3.8)switching 16.3 (4.0) 17.1 (2.5) 14.7 (2.7) 15.9 (3.0)

    Reasoning

    Cognitive Estimation Test 4.5 (2.5) 3.7 (3.1) 3.4 (2.5) 3.5 (2.4)

    Subjective Rating

    DEX self-rating 22.1 (6.0) 22.7 (11.7) 22.3 (7.6) 20.4 (7.0)

    DEX independent rating 14.9 (8.0) 17.4 (9.7) 21.7 (10.7) 23.7 (17.8)

    Inhibition

    Stroop Test

    RTs RCW 25.9 (2.1) 28.6 (5.0) 30.0 (5.8) 33.8 (3.8)

    RTs NCP 44.1 (6.9) 45.0 (8.4) 43.2 (9.5) 48.7 (6.7)

    RTs INT 67.4 (10.2) 70.9 (14.6) 80.2 (18.8) 91.5 (14.6)

    INT no. corrected errors 2.9 (2.3) 3.0 (2.7) 2.5 (2.9) 4.0 (4.1)

    INT no. uncorrected errors 0.8 (0.7) 0.6 (1.0) 2.6 (2.8) 2.5 (2.4)

    AX-CPT

    context use 10.8 (35.7) 6.4 (34.8) 16.4 (60.3) 13.5 (38.7)

    context costs 220 (53) 189 (85) 200 (57) 341 (117)

    inhibition costs 145 (43) 147 (59) 137 (64) 211 (93)

    Multi-tasking

    Divided Attention

    RTs in msec 597 (67) 641 (55) 694 (51) 707 (74)

    no. errors 2.4 (2.5) 2.4 (2.2) 5.2 (3.4) 4.4 (3.4)

    Dual task

    RTs single condition 298 (33) 345 (45) 364 (65) 397 (44)

    single condition no. errors 0.7 (0.9) 0.4 (0.6) 0.4 (0.7) 0.4 (0.7)

    RTs dual condition 386 (48) 431 (116) 454 (66) 675 (138)dual condition no. errors 2.2 (2.0) 1.3 (1.5) 2.1 (3.2) 5.6 (4.2)

    DEX-Dysexecutive Questionnaire; AX-CPT-AX-Continuos Performance Test.

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    10/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 379

    After Bonferroni correction, subsequent paired group ANCOVAs revealed

    significant group effects for comparisons of the oldest and the two youngest

    groups (allp < .008), indicating a significant slowing of the 61+ group in the

    interference condition. The results for errors in the interference condition are

    presented in Table 2. The four age groups did not differ with respect to cor-

    rected errors (Hdf = 3 = 1.344;p = .72), but there was a significant group dif-

    ference for uncorrected errors (Hdf = 3 = 12.092; p < .01), with the oldest

    group making more errors than the youngest group (p = .001).

    Continuous Performance Task

    Because repeated measures ANOVA of RTs with Group, Condition,

    and Delay as factors did not yield any significant interactions involving the

    delay factor (allp > .38), further analyses were performed with RTs pooled

    for the two delays. Analysis of inhibition costs (RTs A-Y minus A-X, see

    Figure 2) and context costs (RTs A-Y minus B-Y, see Figure 3) yielded sig-

    nificant age group differences [(F3.58 = 4.159,p < .01, eta2 = 0.18) and (F3,58 =

    11.418,p < .001, eta2 = 0.37), respectively], with higher inhibition cost for

    the oldest compared to the youngest and second oldest group (all p < .05)and higher context cost (all p .001) for the oldest group compared to all

    FIGURE 1. Mean interference scores and SDs of the four age groups in the Stroop Test.

    INT-interference; RCW-read color words; NCP-name color pictures.

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    11/26

    380 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    other groups. Analysis of context use (RTs B-X minus B-Y, see Table 2) did

    not yield significant group differences (F3,58 = 1.594,p = .20, eta2 = 0.08).

    In further analyses, group differences were explored using RTs in theB-Y condition as covariate. ANCOVA yielded highly significant age group

    FIGURE 2. Means and SDs of inhibition costs (RTs A-Y minus A-X) in the AX-CPT task for the

    four age groups.

    FIGURE 3. Means and SDs of context costs (RTs A-Y minus B-Y) in the AX-CPT task for the

    four age groups.

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    12/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 381

    differences for inhibition cost (p < .01) and context cost (p < .001), and the

    respective paired age group comparisons also remained significant for

    context costs (all p .001). For inhibition costs, paired group compari-

    sons revealed significant differences between the oldest and youngest

    group (p < .004).

    Task Management

    Divided Attention

    Results for the Divided Attention task are presented in Figure 4. There

    was a significant age group difference for RTs (F3,58 = 10.193;p < .001, eta2 =

    0.35), with the oldest group showing longer RTs than the youngest (p < .001)

    and the second youngest group (p < .02). The second oldest group was also

    slower than the youngest group (p = .001). There was a significant age group

    difference for errors (Hdf = 3 = 10.655;p < .01). Post-hoc comparisons did not

    yield significant differences for error rates (allp > .008).

    Dual Task

    The results for the dual task are illustrated in Table 2 and Figures 5 and 6.

    RT analysis with Group and Task as factors yielded a significant interaction

    (F3,57 = 24.482; p < .001, eta2 = 0.55). Post-hoc paired group comparisons

    revealed significant Group Task interactions (allp < .001) in all compari-

    sons involving the oldest group, suggesting that the oldest group showedlarger dual task costs than any of the other groups.

    FIGURE 4. Means and SDs of RTs and number of errors in the divided attention task for the

    four age groups.

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    13/26

    382 FRIEDERIKE H. TREITZETAL.

    FIGURE 5. Scattergram of mean RTs in the single and dual condition of the dual task.

    FIGURE 6. Mean RTs and SDs in the single and dual condition of the dual task for the four age groups.

  • 8/8/2019 Differential Course of Executive Control Changes During Normal Aging

    14/26

    EXECUTIVE CONTROL CHANGES DURING NORMAL AGING 383

    Comparable results emerged for the dual task costs variable (DTC:

    (RTs dual task RTs single task)/RTs single task). The groups differed sig-

    nificantly on DTC [F3,57

    = 18.112;p < .001, eta2 = 0.49]. Post-hoc compari-

    sons revealed significant differences between the oldest group and all other

    groups (allp < .001).

    Repeated measures ANOVA of errors with Group and Task as factors

    yielded a significant interaction (F3,57 = 7.205;p < .001, eta2 = 0.28). Post-

    hoc analyses yielded significant Group Task interactions (all p < .003)

    when the oldest group was compared to the two youngest groups. Compari-

    sons including the oldest group, indicating a disproportional increase in

    errors from the single to the dual task condition in the oldest group.

    After statistical control of response speed by using RTs of the single

    task as covariate, ANCOVA yielded a highly significant age group differ-ence for RTs in the dual task (F3,56 = 14.823; p < .001, eta

    2 = 0.44), which

    was due to slower RTs in the oldest compared to the second youngest and

    second oldest group (all p .001).

    Correlations

    To supplement the group comparisons, cognitive test performance was

    correlated with age. Significant correlations were found for found for a

    range of executive function variables of the Stroop Test (INT-RCW, r = .50,

    p < .001; INT-NCP, r = .63,p < .001; uncorrected errors, r = .45,p < .001),

    the AX-CPT (inhibition cost, r = .34, p < .01; context cost, r = .32, p = .01;context benefit, r = .26, p = .05), divided attention (RTs, r = .58, p < .001;

    errors, r = .36,p = .004) and the dual task (RTs: single condition, r = .65,p