dynamic touch of effective golf swing

1
DYNAMIC TOUCH OF EFFECTIVE GOLF SWING Wangdo Kim, Antonio Veloso, Maria Machado, Veronica Vleck, Liliana Aguiar, Silvia Cabral and Filomena Vieira Biomechanics Laboratory Faculty of Human Kinetics Technical University of Lisbon Estrada da Costa, Cruz Quebrada, Portugal Introduction We are revisiting the classic doctrine of “the significance of left-hand extension during a golfer’s downswing (Kim 2007).” Recent studies have suggested that the inertia tensor, a physical property whose values are time- and coordinate- independent, may be an important informational invariant that is used by the proprioceptive system to control the movement of limbs. We use this ‘dynamic touch paradigm’ to explain why much of traditional golf instruction has stressed the importance of a neutral left wrist position, classically called the ‘flat left wrist. We found that a skilled golfer may achieve a stronger relationship between perception and action in terms of their correlation. The implication of our preliminary findings about wobbling inertia with respect to the functioning of the ‘flat left wrist’ are discussed. Methods We chose the club’s Instantaneous Screw Axes (ISA)(Kiat Teu and Kim 2006; Kim and Sean 2011) and principal axes of inertia (e 3 ) (Pagano and Turvey 1995) and used their instantaneous successive correspondence during swing as generating manifolds for the perception to action diagram. The test conducted to correlate perceptual learning to skill level was presented as a so-called ‘perception to action’ spatiotemporal diagram that is based on the two ruled surfaces generated in terms of 3 e the perceptive information and ISA – the corresponding actions of players. The 3 e traced out a one-parameter family of ruled surface as the segment displaces. Results The profile of e 3 , indicative of the correct swing plane, showed different forms in the expert and non-expert player: Player A produced a perturbation in e 3 whereas player B produced an e 3 shape more uniformly aligned to the path of ISA (Figure 1 and 2). Greater congruency was observed in the alignment of the two surfaces in Player B compared to player A. Moreover, by virtue of positioning the ISAs alongside the e 3 of the club, Player B achieved such a skilled motion that the ISAs could be somewhat connected with the principal inertia axes 3 e . Figure 1: Player A: lesser skilled player. Figure 2: Player B: Expert player. Discussion The perception to action diagram could measure the disturbing patterns of player-specific swing as a consequence of a club’s wobbling inertia. Basically this means that our computational framework offers a more accurate player-specific model over a generic model. References Kim W. 2007. Response to letter to the editor. Journal of Biomechanics 40(1):225-225 Kiat Teu K, and Kim W. 2006. J Biomech 39(15):2857-2862. Kim W, and Sean SK. 2011. Comput Methods Biomech Biomed Engin in press. Pagano CC, and Turvey MT. 1995. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(5):1070-1087. Presentation 1018 − Topic 41. Sport biomechanics S621 ESB2012: 18th Congress of the European Society of Biomechanics Journal of Biomechanics 45(S1)

Upload: wangdo-kim

Post on 04-Nov-2016

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: DYNAMIC TOUCH OF EFFECTIVE GOLF SWING

DYNAMIC TOUCH OF EFFECTIVE GOLF SWING

Wangdo Kim, Antonio Veloso, Maria Machado, Veronica Vleck, Liliana Aguiar, Silvia Cabral

and Filomena Vieira

Biomechanics Laboratory

Faculty of Human Kinetics

Technical University of Lisbon

Estrada da Costa, Cruz Quebrada, Portugal

Introduction

We are revisiting the classic doctrine of “the

significance of left-hand extension during a golfer’s

downswing (Kim 2007).” Recent studies have

suggested that the inertia tensor, a physical property

whose values are time- and coordinate-

independent, may be an important informational

invariant that is used by the proprioceptive system

to control the movement of limbs. We use this

‘dynamic touch paradigm’ to explain why much of

traditional golf instruction has stressed the

importance of a neutral left wrist position,

classically called the ‘flat left wrist. We found that

a skilled golfer may achieve a stronger relationship

between perception and action in terms of their

correlation. The implication of our preliminary

findings about wobbling inertia with respect to the

functioning of the ‘flat left wrist’ are discussed.

Methods

We chose the club’s Instantaneous Screw Axes

(ISA)(Kiat Teu and Kim 2006; Kim and Sean

2011) and principal axes of inertia (e3) (Pagano and

Turvey 1995) and used their instantaneous

successive correspondence during swing as

generating manifolds for the perception to action

diagram. The test conducted to correlate perceptual

learning to skill level was presented as a so-called

‘perception to action’ spatiotemporal diagram that

is based on the two ruled surfaces generated in

terms of 3e � the perceptive information and ISA –

the corresponding actions of players. The 3e traced

out a one-parameter family of ruled surface as the

segment displaces.

Results

The profile of e3, indicative of the correct swing

plane, showed different forms in the expert and

non-expert player: Player A produced a

perturbation in e3 whereas player B produced an e3

shape more uniformly aligned to the path of ISA

(Figure 1 and 2). Greater congruency was observed

in the alignment of the two surfaces in Player B

compared to player A. Moreover, by virtue of

positioning the ISAs alongside the e3 of the club,

Player B achieved such a skilled motion that the

ISAs could be somewhat connected with the

principal inertia axes 3e .

Figure 1: Player A: lesser skilled player.

Figure 2: Player B: Expert player.

Discussion

The perception to action diagram could measure the

disturbing patterns of player-specific swing as a

consequence of a club’s wobbling inertia. Basically

this means that our computational framework offers

a more accurate player-specific model over a

generic model.

References

Kim W. 2007. Response to letter to the editor.

Journal of Biomechanics 40(1):225-225

Kiat Teu K, and Kim W. 2006. J Biomech

39(15):2857-2862.

Kim W, and Sean SK. 2011. Comput Methods

Biomech Biomed Engin in press.

Pagano CC, and Turvey MT. 1995. J Exp Psychol

Hum Percept Perform 21(5):1070-1087.

Presentation 1018 − Topic 41. Sport biomechanics S621

ESB2012: 18th Congress of the European Society of Biomechanics Journal of Biomechanics 45(S1)