emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders ......vi recognition in young children with...

145
Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders: Effects of Age, Subtype, and Gender By Trevor Changgun Lee A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Graduate Department of Medical Sciences University of Toronto © Copyright by Trevor Changgun Lee, 2013

Upload: others

Post on 17-Mar-2021

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders:

Effects of Age, Subtype, and Gender

By

Trevor Changgun Lee

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

for the degree of Master of Science

Graduate Department of Medical Sciences

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Trevor Changgun Lee, 2013

Page 2: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

ii

EMOTION RECOGNITION IN CHILDREN

WITH ANXIETY DISORDERS:

EFFECTS OF AGE, SUBTYPE, AND GENDER

Master of Science, 2013

Trevor Changgun Lee

Institute of Medical Science

University of Toronto

Abstract

Objectives: It is unclear whether anxiety disorders are associated with children’s ability to

recognize emotions. To elucidate this relationship, the effects of age, subtype, and gender

were examined, which have been neglected in past studies. Methods: Sixty-three anxious

children and 59 non-anxious children identified various emotions displayed by an animated

character. Children also completed questionnaires measuring state anxiety and

depressive/anxiety symptoms. Results: Anxious children generally did not have difficulty

identifying emotions compared with non-anxious children. However, children with

separation anxiety disorder (SAD) and young children with generalized anxiety disorder

(GAD) showed difficulty. Gender played a minimal role in emotion recognition, but anxious

girls were less accurate in recognizing disgust when compared with anxious boys and non-

anxious girls. Conclusion: Anxious children as a group may not exhibit difficulty in

emotion recognition. When age and subtype factors are considered, however, children with

SAD and young children with GAD exhibit some deficits.

Page 3: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

iii

Acknowledgments

First of all, I would like to express my deepest appreciation to my thesis supervisor,

Dr. Katharina Manassis, for providing me an opportunity to conduct my first graduate

research at Sick Kids. Without her masterly guidance, time, and patience, my thesis and

publication would have been impossible. Katharina’s words of support and wisdom, during

our Tuesday meetings, helped me stay motivated in my project, and fostered my passion for

research. I sincerely thank her for everything for the last two years of my life, one of the

happiest moments in my life. My marvelous experience as her student will be forever

unforgettable.

I am indebted to Dr. Rosemary Tannock and Dr. Paul Arnold who gave me

constructive advice and suggestions throughout my project. I am absolutely convinced that

taking every piece of their valuable advice has led to timely dissemination of my research.

To Rosemary, I am grateful for being my committee member despite her scheduled

retirement this year. Also, I wish to greatly thank Paul for letting me observe his full-day of

assessments and follow-ups for my graduate module and for my better understanding of

childhood OCD. It was a very fascinating and eye-opening experience for me.

Words fail me to express my appreciation to Dr. Judy Wiener for her insightful

advice for my project and for her continual support throughout my undergraduate and

graduate years. She was always beside me during happy and difficult times to push me and

motivate me. Her compassion for students and teachings will always stay with me.

This study would not have been successful without the help of Mr. David Avery, Ms.

Emily Jones, Dr. Monique Herbert, Dr. Annie Dupuis, and Ms. Carly Guberman. I also

Page 4: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

iv

thank the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and The Hospital for

Sick Children Research Institute for financial support.

I wish to share the credit of my work with my family and friends, who I love from my

heart. With their emotional support and encouragement, I have been able to effectively

manage my stress at times of difficulty. Lastly, I wish to thank my grandparents, who are no

longer among us, for their love and discipline during my childhood in Korea. I wish that, if

they can look down from the above, they would be proud of me thriving to live my dreams.

Page 5: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

v

Table of Contents

Page

Abstract ii

Acknowledgements iii

Table of Contents v

List of Tables viii

List of Figures ix

Chapter 1 General Introduction 1

General Organization of Thesis

Introduction

Non-pathological Anxiety in Children

Pathological Anxiety in Children

Types of Childhood Anxiety Disorders

o Separation Anxiety Disorder (SAD)

o Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD)

o Social Phobia (SP)

o Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD)

o Post-traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

o Specific Phobia

o Panic Disorder (PD)

Changes in Classification and Diagnostic Criteria for

Childhood Anxiety Disorders in DSM-5

Consequences of Childhood Anxiety Disorders

Evidence-based Treatments for Childhood Anxiety

Disorders

o What is Cognitive Behavioural Therapy?

o Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Childhood

Anxiety Disorders and Its Efficacy

o Emotion-focused Cognitive Behavioural

Therapy

Emotion Understanding

Emotion Recognition

o Neural Mechanisms of Emotion Recognition

o Social Correlates and Psychopathologies Linked

to Emotion Recognition

o Previous Findings on Emotion Recognition in

Children with Anxiety Disorders

What Are the Factors Potentially Contributing to

Inconsistent Results of Past Studies?

o Measurement Limitations in Assessing Emotion

2

4

5

6

7

8

9

9

10

11

12

12

13

14

15

15

16

18

19

20

21

22

24

25

Page 6: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

vi

Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety

Disorders

o Other Limitations of Previous Studies

Merits of the Present Study and General Aims

Hypotheses

o Objective #1: Emotion Recognition Accuracy in

Children with and without Anxiety Disorder

o Objective #2: Effect of Age on Emotion

Recognition Accuracy in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder

o Objective #3: Effects of Anxiety Subtype on

Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with

Anxiety Disorders

o Objective #4: Effect of Gender on Emotion

Recognition Accuracy in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder

25

27

28

30

30

31

31

32

Chapter 2 The Effects of Age and Subtype on Emotion Recognition in

Children with Anxiety Disorders

33

Abstract

Introduction

Methods

Results

Discussion

34

35

38

44

48

Chapter 3 The Effect of Gender on Emotion Recognition in Children

with and without Anxiety Disorders

59

Abstract

Introduction

Methods

Results

Discussion

60

61

62

64

66

Chapter 4 Additional Analysis: The Effects of State Anxiety, Task

Completion Time, Depressive Symptoms, and Anxiety

Symptoms on Emotion Recognition in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorders

Abstract

Introduction

Methods

Results

Discussion

70

71

72

75

77

82

Chapter 5 General Discussion 91

Page 7: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

vii

Chapter 6 Future Directions

References

Appendix A

104

108

134

Page 8: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

viii

List of Tables

Page

Chapter 2

1. Characteristics of the anxiety group and the control group

2. Ordinal regression analyses of anxiety diagnosis and separation and anxiety

disorder predicting emotion recognition accuracy in comparison with the

control group

3. Ordinal regression analyses of age predicting emotion recognition accuracy for

children with and without anxiety disorder

52

53

56

Chapter 3

1. Ordinal regression analyses of gender and clinical status predicting emotion

recognition accuracy on specific emotions

68

Chapter 4

1. Ordinal regression analyses of state anxiety predicting recognition accuracy on

specific emotions in children with and without anxiety disorder

2. Ordinal regression analyses of CDI-T score predicting recognition accuracy on

specific emotions in children with and without anxiety disorder

3. Ordinal regression analyses of SCARED T-score predicting recognition

accuracy on specific emotions in children with and without anxiety disorder

85

87

89

Page 9: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

ix

List of Figures

Page

Chapter 2

1. Total accuracy score on MAAC by subtypes: SAD, GAD, SP, and control

groups

2. Regression liens for age and total accuracy score on MAAC in the SAD and

GAD groups, comparison with the control group

57

58

Page 10: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

1

Chapter One

General Introduction

Page 11: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

2

General Organization of Thesis

My thesis begins with a chapter of general introduction (Chapter 1). In this chapter,

published scholarly articles and textbook chapters are reviewed to frame my thesis with

reference to the history of the present research questions, while explaining important

terminology, concepts, and gaps in knowledge in this area of research. This chapter further

describes the importance of investigating the ability to correctly recognize others’ emotional

states in children with anxiety disorders, and addresses some of the methodological issues in

measuring emotion recognition skill in young children. Lastly, the chapter ends with the

main study aims and hypotheses.

Chapters 2 and 3 are the main studies of my thesis, and are presented as scientific

journal manuscripts for submission. Chapter 2 is based on my first journal manuscript

“Effects of Age and Subtype on Emotional Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders”,

which was accepted in November of 2012 for publication in Canadian Journal of Psychiatry.

This chapter examines if children with anxiety disorders are impaired in their ability to

identify others’ emotional states, and if children with certain types of primary anxiety

disorder are more impaired than children with other types of anxiety disorder or without

anxiety disorder.

Chapter 3 is my second journal manuscript “Effect of Gender on Emotion

Recognition Accuracy in Children with Anxiety Disorders: Disgust Recognition Implicated”,

which was prepared as a brief report. This chapter examines if gender significantly predicts

emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders.

Page 12: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

3

Chapter 4 contains additional exploratory analyses that delve into the potential effects

of state anxiety, task completion time, depressive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms on

emotion recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

Chapter 5 is a general discussion section that summarizes significant findings of the

present study, while comparing them to the original hypotheses. Then, the chapter discusses

clinical implications and the limitations of the present study. A general conclusion is briefly

stated, and finally, my thesis ends with Chapter 6, a description of future directions for this

research.

Because my thesis work is organized as a set of self-contained chapters that all

examine emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders using common

methods, some degree of repetition was inevitable. Tables and figures are displayed at the

end of each chapter, and all references are placed at the end of this thesis.

Page 13: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

4

Introduction

The ability to correctly identify the emotional states of others plays a pivotal role in

interpersonal relationships. Proficiency in this emotional skill may have also served as an

adaptive trait in human evolution. For example, accurate recognition of others’ anger or

disgust would allow the observer to avoid confrontation or poisons/contaminants, leading to

better chance of survival. On the other hand, impairment in this skill may diminish

experiences in everyday lives. Not surprisingly, impaired recognition of emotions is reported

in various mental disorders.

Children with anxiety disorders have difficulties in social and emotional functioning.

These observations raise the question of whether these children are impaired in the ability to

recognize the emotional states of others. However, there is a paucity of research on this

issue, and the available past studies have shown inconsistent results. Answering this

question may allow scientists to better understand the nature of anxious children’s emotional

development and social difficulties.

Further, investigating emotion recognition in anxious children may have implications

for current psychotherapies for childhood anxiety disorders. For example, the traditional

anxiety-focused psychotherapy focuses on the reduction of anxiety symptoms via changing

thinking and behavioural patterns, and regulation of emotions. However, efficacy of the

psychotherapy might improve if equal emphasis were placed on improving emotion-related

skills in children with anxiety disorders. For this reason, researchers developed a new

emotion-focused psychotherapy which involves more sessions to discuss emotions that

anxious children may have difficulty understanding and regulating. Examining emotion

Page 14: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

5

recognition in anxious children would inform psychotherapy and elucidate when and whether

emotion-focused psychotherapy may be more beneficial.

The present study aims to elucidate the relationship between anxiety disorders and

children’s ability to recognize others’ emotions. In this thesis, I highlight the effects of age,

subtype, and gender on emotion recognition in anxious children. Paying attention to these

factors may lead to novel findings in this field of research, relevant to better understanding

and treating anxious children’s emotional development.

Non-pathological Anxiety in Children

Anxiety or worry is characterized by a diffuse sense of apprehension and unpleasant

thoughts (e.g. anticipation of harm) that are accompanied by autonomic symptoms (e.g. heart

palpitations, muscle tension, stomach pain) and avoidance behaviour (Barlow, 1988; Lang,

1968). Transient anxiety is a normal response to threats or dangerous situations, and this

fleeting condition effectively triggers defensive fight-or-flight responses (Ohman, 1993).

These defensive reactions help individuals protect themselves from the negative emotional

states or physical dangers associated with the acute stressors (Ohman, 1993). Further, this

passing, non-pathological anxiety also motivates individuals to be vigilant about potential

threats and to avoid confrontation of these aversive stimuli by anticipating them (Barlow,

2000).

According to social-cognitive theory, the state of anxiety transpires when a person’s

perception of threat or danger outweighs self-efficacy of coping such that lower perceived

self-efficacy compared with harmful aspects of an aversive event will result in anxiety

arousal (Barlow, 2000; Bandura, 1986; 1988, Marks, 1977). In this sense, the same

Page 15: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

6

potentially harmful stimulus or event may cause anxiety arousal in some individuals, but may

not in others based on their perception of self-efficacy of coping.

Non-pathological anxiety or worry is very common in school-age children. For

example, about 70% of school-age children are mildly anxious or worried about everyday

life (Muris, Meesters, Merckelbach, Sermon, & Zwakhalen, 1998; Silverman et al., 1995).

Mothers of approximately 43% of school-age children report that their children undergo

seven or more mild worries and fears, often including separation concerns (Bell-Dolan, Last,

& Strauss, 1990; Lapouse & Monk, 1964).

Although the theme of anxiety or worry in school-age children often changes during

the course of development (Albano, Chorpita, & Barlow, 2003; Bauer, 1976), the most

common themes of anxiety in children relate to academic performance, physical health,

dying, and interpersonal relationships (Muris et al., 1998). Such anxiety-generating thoughts

can occur as early as five years, and these thoughts tend to be more frequent and more

complex in content at 8 years or above (Vasey, 1993). The age-related increase in the

frequency and complexity of anxious thoughts in school-age children may reflect their

development in cognitive skills, such as their ability to anticipate threatening outcomes and

to elaborate negative implications from these outcomes (Magnusson, 1985; Vasey, Crnic, &

Carter, 1994). In most cases, anxiety or worry in children is seen as non-pathological

because their anxious feelings do not lead to significant impairment and distress and subside

when the anxiety-provoking stimulus is no longer present or out of sight. Further, the

amount of anxiety children experience is reasonable to the circumstances and in proportion

to the actual threat (Wagner, 2002, pp. 22).

Pathological Anxiety in Children

Page 16: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

7

Non-pathological anxiety or worry can become pathological and maladaptive when a

child’s anxiety or worry is excessive (i.e. out of proportion to the actual threats), pervasive,

recurrent or chronic, and if anxiety interferes with normal functioning, learning and

concentration. In such case, a child may be diagnosed with an anxiety disorder (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000). The onset of anxiety disorder may be sudden, sometimes

within a few hours, or gradual, often taking weeks, months, or years (Angst & Vollrath,

1991).

Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent psychiatric disorders of childhood

and adolescence (Anderson, 1994; Bernstein & Borchardt, 1991). However, the exact

prevalence rate of childhood anxiety disorders is unclear as the number varies significantly

across different epidemiologic studies (Cartwright-Hatton, McNicol, & Doubleday, 2006). It

is estimated that approximately 5-15% of the general population has some type of anxiety

disorder during childhood (Klein & Pine, 2002). The most conservative point prevalence in

the literature indicates that at least 3% of children and adolescents are affected (Ford,

Goodman, & Meltzer, 2003), attesting to the high prevalence of childhood anxiety disorders.

Pathological symptoms of anxiety disorders can be a salient feature in a multitude of

psychiatric and mood disorders in children. For example, about 16% to 62% of children and

adolescents with depression have comorbid anxiety disorders (Brady & Kendall, 1992).

Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents also tend to be highly comorbid with each

other (Craske & Waters, 2005), implying that a child diagnosed with one type of anxiety

disorder is at increased risk for another type of anxiety disorder.

Types of Childhood Anxiety Disorders

Page 17: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

8

Anxiety disorder in children and adolescents, similar to those in adults, may manifest

in a number of distinct forms, including separation anxiety disorder (SAD), generalized

anxiety disorder (GAD), social phobia (SP), specific phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder

(OCD), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and panic disorder (PD) (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000). Among these different types of anxiety disorders in children,

SAD is the only type that is specific to childhood (Ollendick & Schroeder, 2003, pp. 34).

Separation anxiety disorder (SAD).

SAD is among the most common types of anxiety disorders of childhood, affecting

approximately 4.1% of school-age children (Shear, Jin, Ruscio, Walters, & Kessler, 2006).

Children diagnosed with SAD are abnormally reactive to real or imagined separation from

their parents or other attachment figures, and their distress is severe enough to interfere with

normal functioning and development (Masi, Mucci, & Millepied, 2001). Childhood SAD is

strongly associated with school-refusal behavior (Last & Strauss, 1990), which in turn

significantly predicts inadequate relationships with peers and poor academic performance

(Berg, Marks, McGuire & Lipsedge, 1974; Hersov, 1972). The onset of school-refusal in

children diagnosed with SAD can begin as early as 8-9 years, and their age at psychiatric

assessment is around 11-12 years (Last & Strauss, 1990).

Childhood SAD has been proposed as a major risk factor for the development of

major depression (Lewinsohn et al., 2008) and certain types of anxiety disorders in adults,

such as panic disorder (Battaglia et al., 1995; Lewinsohn et al., 2008) and agoraphobia in

women (Zitrin & Ross, 1988). However, some recent longitudinal studies have argued

against the link between childhood SAD and panic disorder in adults (Aschenbrand, Kendall,

Webb, Safford, & Flannery-Schroeder, 2003), warranting further investigation in this regard.

Page 18: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

9

Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD).

According to DSM-IV, a diagnosis of GAD can be made when a child is excessively

anxious for more than six months, but the focus of anxiety is not on a specific event or

stimulus (2000). These children tend to worry about a variety of issues (e.g. relationships,

physical health, keeping schedules), and often exaggerate their concerns and the risks of

situations (Hudson, Deveney, & Taylor, 2005). Symptomatology of GAD in children

includes feelings of tension, apprehension, need for reassurance, irritability, negative self-

perception (Masi, Mucci, Favilla, Romano, & Poli, 1999), and one or more chronic

physiological symptoms (e.g. stomach ache, sleep disturbance, restless, impaired

concentration on tasks) (Kendall & Pimentel, 2003). Children with GAD present a more

frequent need for reassurance than adolescents with GAD, whereas adolescents with GAD

tend to brood more frequently than children with GAD (Masi et al., 1999). The prevalence

rate of GAD in school-age children is estimated to be 5% (Shear et al., 2006), and the onset

is about 9 to 10 years (Last, Perrin, Hersen, & Kazdin, 1992). GAD in children is usually

comorbid with other type(s) of anxiety disorders or mood disorders, and this comorbidity

often limits our understanding of pure childhood GAD (Hudson et al., 2005).

Social phobia (SP).

Children living with SP (or social anxiety disorder) experience persistent and extreme

fear of being judged negatively in social situations, and are often concerned that others will

notice their anxiety symptoms in social settings (American Psychiatric Assocation, 2000;

Heckelman & Schneier, 1995, pp. 3). As a result, SP presents as the second most frequent

anxiety subtype in children and adolescents who refuse to go to school (Last & Strauss,

Page 19: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

10

1990), and is also often comorbid with other types of anxiety disorder, such as GAD

(Francis, Last, & Strauss, 1992).

The SP syndrome can be diagnosed in school-age children (Biedel & Turner, 1998),

and symptoms resemble those in adults (e.g. fear of speaking/reading/writing in public,

informal social activities) (Beidel, Turner, & Morris, 1999). Psychopathologies of SP in

children manifest as heightened emotional responsiveness, social inhibition or shyness,

dysphoria, loneliness, distress, and maladaptive coping (Beidel et al., 1999).

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).

The concept of OCD involves both the cognitive feature of obsessions and the

behavioural feature of compulsions. An obsession is “an intrusive, repetitive thought, image,

or impulse that is unacceptable or unwanted and gives rise to subjective resistance”

(Rachman & Shafran, 1998, pp. 51), whereas a compulsion is “a repetitive, stereotyped,

intentional act” with an “experienced sense of pressure to act, and the attribution of this

pressure to internal sources” (Rachman & Shafran, 1998, pp. 53-54). Compulsive

behaviours and cognition are thought to help temporarily relieve or suppress obsessions, and

these behaviours can be either overt (e.g. washing) or covert (e.g. counting, praying,

neutralizing) in nature (March & Friesen, 1998, pp. 5).

The diagnostic criteria for OCD include obsessions or compulsions that cause

significant distress, time consumption (spending more than an hour per day), or marked

interference with the individual’s functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

The most common themes of obsessions in children are fear of contamination, fear of

harming self or others, and an excessive urge for symmetry or exactness, whereas the most

common compulsions are excessive washing, checking, counting, repeating, touching and

Page 20: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

11

straightening (March & Friesen, 1998, pp. 6; Swedo et al., 1989). The prevalence rates of

OCD in children and adolescents have been found to be approximately 2-3% (Valleni-Basile,

Garrison, Jackson, & Waller, 1994). About a half of adult OCD patients acquired their OCD

during childhood (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1990)

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

When a child directly experiences or observes a catastrophic event, which is life-

threatening or involves serious injury, he/she may develop ongoing emotional and physical

difficulties known as PTSD (American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 2010).

Children experience PTSD through (1) repeated, persistent re-experiencing of the traumatic

event (e.g. distressing memories, dreams, flashbacks), (2) emotional numbing and

detachment (e.g. avoidance of thoughts and feelings about the traumatic event), and (3)

hypervigilance and/or chronic arousal (e.g. always being on guard for the event to recur)

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Additionally, children may express post-

traumatic stress through hyperactivity, excessive fear, helplessness, horror, disorganized or

agitated behaviour for more than one month (Fletcher, 2003, pp. 332; Kaminer, Seedat, &

Stein, 2005). If these symptoms last for less than four weeks in duration, however, the child

may be diagnosed with acute stress disorder instead of PTSD (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2007, pp.

193).

Due to the unpredictability of catastrophic or traumatic events, it is difficult to

determine the range of prevalence of PTSD. In Canada, the lifetime prevalence of PTSD and

current (one month) PTSD are estimated to be about 9.2% and 2.4%, respectively (Van

Ameringen, Mancini, Patterson, & Boyle, 2008); however, there is a lack of systematic

epidemiologic review in childhood PTSD. It is generally agreed that school-age children are

Page 21: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

12

more susceptible to PTSD than adults exposed to a traumatic event as about 36% of

traumatized children are diagnosed with PTSD, whereas about 24% of adults are diagnosed

with PTSD following such events (Fletcher, 2003, pp. 332). Also, it seems evident that

certain familial factors contribute to the risk of developing PTSD in children. For example,

there is a strong correlation between parental PTSD and the likelihood of PTSD in offspring

(Yehuda, Halligan, & Bierer, 2001).

Specific phobia.

Specific phobia refers to a pathological fear of specific objects or situations that are

unrelated to social phobia or panic disorder (Albano et al., 2003, pp. 289-290). The DSM-IV

lists four distinct subtypes of specific phobia: blood-injection-injury type (e.g. seeing blood),

animal type (e.g. spiders), natural environmental type (e.g. heights), and situational type (e.g.

closed spaces) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). In childhood specific phobia,

children’s anxiety symptoms are present for at least six months, and their phobic reactions

are expressed as tantrums, crying, freezing, and clinging (Muris, Schmidt, & Merckelbach,

1999). Specific phobias in children are as common as 5% (lifetime prevalence), making

them the most prevalent type of anxiety disorders in children (Costello & Angold, 1995, pp.

115).

Panic disorder (PD).

It is highly controversial whether panic attacks or panic disorder (PD) exist in

children (Nelles & Barlow, 1988). One study used retrospective reports of adults diagnosed

with panic disorder, and found a few cases of childhood PD (Klein, Mannuzza, Chapman, &

Fyer, 1992). However, Abelson and Alessi (1992) questioned whether retrospectively

Page 22: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

13

reviewed psychiatric status should be regarded as valid evidence for childhood PD (Abelson

& Alessi, 1992), and the controversy seems to continue in the literature.

PD becomes more common during adolescence. According to a longitudinal

epidemiologic study of psychiatric disorders within a representative sample of children and

adolescents (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003), prevalence of PD begins

to increase at a moderate rate during adolescence and the disorder eventually becomes one of

the most prevalent anxiety problems by middle adolescence.

Changes in Classification and Diagnostic Criteria for Childhood Anxiety Disorders in

DSM-5

According to recent revisions in DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2012),

one of the major changes in the classification of anxiety disorders is that OCD has been

removed from the anxiety disorders category because the key feature of OCD is not often

anxiety but rather the repertoire of intrusive obsessions and compulsive rituals (Stein et al.,

2010). Alternatively, it is unknown to what extent anxiety is linked to obsessions and

compulsions, and some researchers therefore argue that OCD should be included in a new

category of OC-spectrum disorders or obsessive-compulsive-related disorders (OCRD),

consistent with the classification system of the International Classification of Mental

Disorders (ICD) (Hollander, Kim, & Zohar, 2007). It is expected that the new specification

may allow clinicians to make more targeted diagnoses and screening.

There are also other minor changes within the category of anxiety disorders in DSM-

5. First, the age of onset requirement has been dropped for the diagnosis of SAD,

recognizing that SAD may occur in adulthood (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and

making the disorder no longer specific to childhood. In fact, an epidemiologic study (Shear,

Page 23: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

14

Jin, Ruscio, Walters, & Kessler, 2006) showed that SAD in adults is very common condition,

and that the majority of them have their first onsets in adulthood. Second, the ‘generalized’

specifier for social anxiety disorder (or SP) has disappeared, whereas the ‘performance only’

specifier has remained (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Third selective mutism

has been included in the anxiety disorders category because “a large majority of children

with selective mutism are anxious” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Scrutiny is mandated in the future for the effects of revised diagnostic criteria of

GAD in DSM-5. For example, the test-retest reliability (i.e. extent to which clinicians agree

on the same diagnosis) of the GAD diagnosis in DSM-5 is in the questionable range. This

reliability has not improved from the field trial results based on the DSM-IV criteria for

GAD (Regier et al., 2013).

Consequences of Childhood Anxiety Disorders

Anxiety disorders of childhood can cause various debilitating social and academic

problems for affected children and their families (Donovan & Spence, 2000). Evidence

shows that children with anxiety disorders have difficulty with social adjustment (Wood,

2006), peer relationships (Ginsberg, La Greca, & Silverman, 1998), academic performance

(Van Ameringen, Mancini, & Farvolden, 2003; Wood, 2006), and school attendance (i.e.

school refusal) (Atkinson, Quarrington, Cyr, & Atkinson, 1989; Last & Strauss, 1990).

These mental disorders of childhood often persist into later life, resulting in adult

diagnoses of anxiety disorder (Last, Philips, & Statfield, 1987; Shear et al., 2006). The

persistent pathological symptoms of anxiety disorders in adolescence or adulthood

significantly predict a range of social and psychological/psychiatric long-term complications,

including comorbid depression (Kovacs, Gatsonis, Paulauskas, & Richards, 1989),

Page 24: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

15

vocational impairment (Koran, 2000), marital problems (McLeod, 1994), and substance

abuse (Kushner, Sher, & Beitman, 1990; Woodward & Fergusson, 2001).

Given the high prevalence, a significantly increased risk for comorbid psychiatric

disorders or conditions, and their emergence as a significant public health concern, early

intervention for anxiety disorders in children is essential.

Evidence-based Treatments for Childhood Anxiety Disorders

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and serotonin-specific medication are the

evidence- based treatment options for anxiety disorders in children. In terms of clinical

outcomes, a combination of CBT with medication therapy is significantly superior to CBT or

medication alone; however, each type of monotherapy is significantly more effective than

placebo (Walkup et al., 2008). CBT is equally effective as medication therapy for reducing

the anxiety symptoms, but generally causes significantly fewer adverse side effects than

medication therapy in clinically anxious school-age children (Walkup et al., 2008).

Consequently, many clinicians prefer CBT to medication as the first-line treatment in

children with anxiety disorders.

What is cognitive behavioural therapy?.

CBT is an evidence-informed psychotherapeutic approach that is used to treat

symptoms of a range of mental health disorders by reducing dysfunctional emotions and

behaviour through modifying cognitive patterns and contents (Sheldon, 1995, pp. 3). The

primary goals of CBT for anxiety disorders in children are suppression and management of

anxiety symptoms. However, recent clinical applications have broadened, emphasizing the

need of improving their social and emotional skills (Mash, 2006, pp. 12).

Page 25: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

16

The systematic procedures implicated in CBT integrate two distinct models of

psychological/psychiatric disorders: the cognitive model and the behavioural model. These

models are based on the shared assumption that prior learning may cause maladaptive

consequences, and that intervention reduces dysfunctional emotions and behaviour by

reversing the prior learning (Brewin, 1996). The cognitive model assumes that we react to

life events via a combination of cognitive, emotional, motivational, and behavioural

responses (Brewin, 1996; Corsini & Wedding, 2000, pp. 276), and a cognitive therapist

would evaluate an individual’s irrational thinking patterns of causal attribution and treat them

as the main target of treatment (Beck, 1976). On the other hand, behavioural modification

focuses on extinguishing an undesirable behaviour by replacing with a desirable behaviour

through contingency learning (Brewin, 1996), visualized or real exposure to anxiety-

generating objects or situations (i.e. gradual desensitization), and relaxation training (Suveg,

Kendall, Comer, & Robin, 2006).

Cognitive behavioural therapy for childhood anxiety disorders and its efficacy.

CBT is effective in the treatment of all types of anxiety disorders in adults (Hoffman

& Smits, 2008). Although CBT is also effective for anxiety disorders in children aged 6

years and older when compared with wait-list controls, reviews of CBT for specific anxiety

disorders in children (e.g. SAD, SP) are absent in the literature (Cartwright-Hatton, Roberts,

Chitsabesan, Fothergill, & Harrington, 2004). Most trials grouped children with specific

anxiety disorders into a unitary anxiety group due to high comorbidity, without assessing the

relative effect of CBT on each type of anxiety disorders. Such specification in research,

however, may help optimize clinical outcomes of CBT for anxious children, as evident in the

Page 26: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

17

treatment of specific anxiety disorders in adults that benefit from different approaches

(Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004).

Kendall’s ‘Coping Cat’ (2006) is unquestionably among the most-widely-used,

evidence-based CBT protocols for treating anxiety disorders of childhood, especially for

SAD, GAD or SP (Macklem, 2011, pp. 34). Some strategies applied in Coping Cat, however,

can also be applied in the treatment of childhood PTSD, OCD, and specific phobia (Kendall,

Furr, & Podell, 2003, pp. 45). This manualized CBT program was first developed at the

Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders Clinic at Temple University (Philadelphia, USA).

The program requires the therapist to follow the therapist’s treatment manual (Kendall &

Hedtke, 2006a) and child to use his or her workbook during treatment (Kendall & Hedtke,

2006b). The Coping Cat protocol has been adapted internationally and modified for

populations in different countries: Coping Bear in Canada (Scapillato & Mendlowitz, 1993)

and Coping Koala in Australia (Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee, 1991).

The main goal of the Coping Cat CBT is to help children with anxiety disorders

recognize their anxiety-related physiological, cognitive, and behavioural symptoms in order

to effectively apply coping strategies in anxiety-generating situations (Velting et al., 2004;

Wood, 2006). This protocol is composed of sixteen sessions in which the first eight sessions

are devoted to psychoeducation (e.g. learning how to recognize cues for their anxiety, cues

for feelings of one’s own or others, coping skills), and the second eight sessions focus on

behavioural desensitization and modification (e.g. facing their fears in a graded hierarchy)

(Beidas, Benjamin, Puleo, Edmunds, & Kendall, 2010). During the psychoeducation period,

emotion-related concepts are addressed to some extent with more emphasis on emotion

Page 27: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

18

regulation than emotion understanding. However, the largest focus of this traditional CBT is

on anxiety symptoms (Suveg, et al., 2006).

In the first randomized controlled trial (RCT) by the developer of Coping Cat, the

anxiety symptoms were markedly reduced at post-treatment in about two thirds of anxious

children with primary diagnoses of SAD, GAD, and SP; however, the remaining one third

still met the full criteria for anxiety disorders following the treatment (Kendall, 1994). The

second RCT also replicated the initial findings of Kendall (1994), and nearly half of treated

children with anxiety disorders no longer met the diagnostic criteria for any anxiety disorder

at post-treatment (Kendall et al., 1997). Many further RCT’s of Coping Cat or programs

very similar to it have been done internationally with similar results. Finally, a recent meta-

analysis on the efficacy of CBT on anxiety disorders in children and adolescents strongly

supported the effectiveness of anxiety-focused CBT and suggested that its effects could be

maintained for up to two years (Ishikawa, Okajima, Matsuoka, & Sakano, 2007).

Despite the evidence for short-term and long-term efficacy of CBT for children with

anxiety disorders, at least one third of treated children remain unresponsive to CBT, thereby

categorizing the treatment as a “probably efficacious” (Silverman, Pina, & Viswesvaran,

2008). Given this clinical limitation of anxiety-focused CBT, researchers must further

examine potential areas of difficulty beyond cognitive or behavioral domains in children with

anxiety disorders. One promising area of research is emotion.

Emotion-focused cognitive behavioural therapy.

There has been growing emphasis in CBT on assessing emotion-related difficulties

that children with anxiety disorders may experience, especially in those unresponsive to

traditional CBT. One example of such an effort is emotion-focused CBT (ECBT), which

Page 28: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

19

closely follows the standard procedures of traditional anxiety-focused CBT. Although ECBT

is consistent with anxiety-focused CBT, it places equal weight on ameliorating emotional

deficits and anxiety symptoms (Jablonka, Sarubbi, Rapp, & Albano, 2012, pp. 546; Suveg et

al., 2006). Specifically, whereas the traditional anxiety-focused CBT spends only 1-2

session(s) on emotion recognition or understanding, ECBT integrates the same emotional

content throughout the entire eight sessions of psychoeducation (Suveg et al., 2006). In each

session of ECBT, both clinician and child work as a team to identify emotion(s) that the child

has difficulty regulating and understanding (Suveg et al., 2006). However, the emotion

understanding component in ECBT focuses on understanding or recognizing one’s own

emotional experiences, with less emphasis on others’ emotional states (Beidas et al., 2010;

Suveg, Sood, Comer, & Kendall, 2009; Rynn, Vidair, & Blackford, 2012, pp. 546).

Emotion Understanding

Emotion understanding refers to the “conscious knowledge about emotion” and

related processes (Southam-Gerow & Kendall, 2002, pp. 200; Thompson, 1990). In

research, the construct of emotion understanding comprises emotion recognition and other

emotional knowledge (e.g. causes of emotion, multiple emotions, emotion display or hiding,

knowledge about emotion regulation) (Southam-Gerow & Kendall, 2002). Emotion

understanding facilitates a child’s social functioning, (Hubbard & Coie, 1994), and

impairment in this conscious knowledge is associated with some types of psychopathologies

of childhood. For example, impaired emotion understanding is reported in school-age

children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Da Fonseca, Seguier, Santos,

Poinso, & Deruelle, 2009).

Page 29: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

20

Research on emotion understanding in children with anxiety disorders is very scant in

the literature. Only one study by Southam-Gerow and Kendall (2000) examined four distinct

facets of the emotion understanding construct in anxious children: knowledge about

emotional cues, multiple or simultaneous emotions, changing or hiding emotions, and

knowledge about emotion regulation and coping. The study found that children with anxiety

disorders are less able to hide or change their emotions than non-anxious children; however,

the study did not assess the children’s ability to recognize emotions, although emotion

recognition is another crucial element of emotion understanding.

Emotion Recognition

Emotion recognition refers to the perceptual and cognitive capacity to identify others’

or one’s own emotional states through facial, postural, and contextual cues (Buitelaar, Van

der Wees, Swaab-Barnesveld, & Van der Gaag, 1999). The ability to recognize others’

emotions may manifest as early as in newborns and infants, albeit in immature form, and

infants begin to heed facial expressions for appraisal of emotions (Field, Woodson,

Greenberg, & Cohen, 1983). According to the theory of natural selection by Darwin (1898),

this biological predisposition in humans to pay attention to facial expressions and to correctly

discriminate emotions may have been a favoured trait for survival (Fridlund, 1997, pp. 109-

111). More specifically, accurate recognition of others’ emotional states allows receivers to

appropriately change their behaviour based on the sender’s emotional expressions (e.g.

acting submissively after seeing an angry face), and such adaptive behaviour may facilitate

cooperation and prevent aggression and energy expenditure (Elfenbein, March, & Ambady,

2002, pp. 38-39). Furthermore, proficiency in emotion recognition may allow individuals to

Page 30: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

21

effectively manage their emotional states (e.g. hiding their feelings or deceiving others) in

the presence of others (Ekman, 1973).

There is another theory relating to emotion recognition and its potential importance

for survival, the ‘subordination hypothesis’ (Henley, 1973). According to this hypothesis,

women are generally more accurate than men in emotion recognition based on the

historically lower social status of women relative to men. This subordinate or oppressed

status made emotion recognition an important skill for women’s survival. However, some

researchers have opposed this view with empirical evidence that men can recognize certain

emotions, such as anger, more accurately than women (Elfenbein et al., 2002; Mandal &

Palchoudhury, 1985; Wagner, MacDonald, & Manstead, 1986).

Neural mechanisms of emotion recognition.

In terms of neuroanatomical correlates of emotion recognition, the process takes

place predominantly in the right hemisphere in most people when judging negative or

positive emotions (DeKosky, Heilman, Bowers, & Valenstein, 1990; Ley & Bryden, 1979).

However, this tendency seems less apparent when judging neutral or mild emotions (Ley &

Bryden, 1979). Further, some lesion studies (Blonder, Bowers, & Heilman, 1991) also

provide evidence for the right hemisphere playing a major role in emotion recognition,

especially when appraising emotions through facial and bodily expressions. On the other

hand, some variations seem to exist across studies in locating discrete sectors for processing

emotions. For example, one study (Adolphs, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1996) found that

the right inferior parietal cortex and mesial anterior infracalcarine cortex are the main

cortical systems for the processes involved in emotion recognition, whereas other studies

Page 31: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

22

have located this function in the right inferior frontal cortex (Nakamura et al., 2000) or

exclusively in the right temporoparietal cortex (Bowers, Bauer, Coslett, & Heilman, 1985).

There is converging evidence, however, that such differential activation of the

discrete cortical areas may reflect emotion-specific processes during emotion recognition

(Adolphs et al., 1996). For example, although perceiving some negative basic emotions

(anger, disgust, and fear) all equally activates Brodmann area 47 in the left inferior-frontal

cortex, ‘anger’ seems to distinctively activate right gyrus cinguli, whereas ‘disgust’ and ‘fear’

may predominantly activate right putamen and amygdala, respectively (Adolphs et al., 1996).

Following this perceptual processing, amygdala and orbitofrontal cortices communicate with

other parts of the neocortex and hippocampal formation in order to retrieve emotional

knowledge. Then, the conscious recognition of the emotional states of others ensues

(Adolphs, 2002).

Social correlates and psychopathologies.

The ability to correctly recognize others’ emotional states is an important emotional

skill in social communication and interpersonal relationships. For example, emotion

recognition is significantly associated with children’s social competence (Mueser et al.,

1996), which helps children to adapt successfully in various social settings (Semrud-

Clikeman, 2007). Further, emotion recognition is one of the most reliably validated elements

of emotional intelligence (Elfenbein et al., 2002, pp. 45), which in turn predicts leadership

competencies (George, 2000) as well as satisfaction with interpersonal relationships (Lopes,

Salovey, & Straus, 2003). By the same logic, however, any deficit in emotion recognition

may presage social difficulties in everyday life. In fact, impairment in emotion recognition

Page 32: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

23

seems to be significantly related to various types of psychopathologies (Collin, Bindra, Raju,

Gillberg, & Minnis, 2013) that cause social and behavioural problems in affected individuals

Impaired ability to recognize others’ emotional states has been reported in adults and

children suffering from a wide range of mental health disorders. For example, a deficit in

this ability is seen in adults with alexithymia (Lane, Sechrest, Reidel, Weldon, Kaszniak, &

Schwartz, 1996), dementia (Keane, Calder, Hodges, & Young, 2002), bipolar disorder (Getz,

Shear, & Strakowski, 2003), depression (Demenescu, Kortekaas, Den Boer, & Aleman,

2010), eating disorders (Kucharska-Pietura, Nikolaou, Masiak, & Treasure, 2003), mania

(Lembke & Ketter, 2002), panic disorder (Kessler, Roth, von Wietersheim, Deighton, &

Traue, 2006), and schizophrenia (Johnston, Stojanov, Devir, & Schall, 2005; Kohler et al,

2003; Mandal, Jain, Haque-Nizamie, Weiss, & Schneider, 1999; Namiki et al., 2007; Sachs,

Steger-Wuchse, Kryspin-Exner, Gur, & Katsching, 2004). Impaired emotion recognition has

also been found in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (Kuusikko et al.,

2009), ADHD (Singh, Ellis, Winton, Singh, Leung, & Oswald, 1998), bipolar disorder

(McClure, Pope, Hoberman, Pine, & Leibenluft, 2003), and abuse and neglect (Camras,

Grow, & Ribordy, 1983; Pollak, Cicchetti, Hornung, & Reed, 2000).

Despite the strong evidence that children with anxiety disorders have a broad range of

social deficits (e.g. social shyness/withdrawal, inappropriate social skills, social

maladjustment) (Strauss, Lease, Kazdin, Dulcan, & Last, 1989), current treatments, including

both CBT and ECBT, for anxiety disorders of childhood are mostly focused on the child’s

ability to recognize his/her own emotions. They do not carefully address and treat the child’s

potential difficulty in recognizing others’ emotions. This is unfortunate because a deficit in

decoding others’ emotional information is an important correlate of interpersonal problems

Page 33: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

24

(Kornreich et al., 2002), and such a deficit, if it exists, could potentially contribute to

exacerbation of anxiety symptoms during social interactions in children with anxiety

disorders.

Previous findings on emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders.

Research on the relationship between anxiety disorders and emotion recognition in

children is scarce and inconsistent. One study by Easter et al. (2005) found that children

with anxiety disorders are more impaired in recognizing emotions than children without

anxiety disorder. In this study, researchers compared emotion recognition accuracy in a

relatively small number of child participants, 15 clinically anxious children and adolescents

and 11 non-anxious controls, using a set of posed pictures of facial expressions. By

convention, SAD, GAD, and SP types of anxiety disorders were considered as a single

experimental group in this study. This is commonly done because these types are highly

comorbid, but neglects the distinct clinical features associated with each of the anxiety types.

Furthermore, the effect of other highly comorbid conditions, such as conduct or oppositional

disorder, has not been examined in children with anxiety disorders.

With the same conventional research design and with the same set of facial emotion

cues, however, other studies produced results that are different from the finding by Easter et

al. (2005). For example, Manassis and Young (2000) found that children with anxiety

disorders can identify others’ facial emotions as accurately as children without anxiety

disorder. Only children with learning disabilities without anxiety were significantly impaired

in this study. Similarly, McClure et al. (2003) found that children and adolescents with

anxiety disorders could recognize others’ emotional states as accurately as their non-anxious

counterparts, but only children and adolescents with bipolar disorder showed a deficit in

Page 34: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

25

emotion recognition. Lastly, Guyer et al. (2007) compared emotion recognition accuracy

among children and adolescents from various clinical groups, and reported that children with

anxiety disorders and children with depression as a group were not impaired in emotion

recognition. In this particular study, however, different pediatric mood and psychiatric

disorders were arbitrarily combined into single groups (e.g. combining the depression group

with the anxiety group. Thus, their findings may be limited in telling a clear story about

anxious children’s ability to recognize emotions. As illustrated above, there seems to be

little agreement on emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders.

What Are the Factors Potentially Contributing to Inconsistent Results of Past Studies?

Measurement limitations in assessing emotion recognition in young children

with anxiety disorders.

Previous studies relied on static pictures of adult and child facial emotions for

assessing emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders. However, there may be

some measurement issues in using these pictorial cues for measuring young children’s ability

to recognize emotions. First, the use of static photographs of facial expressions in traditional

lab settings may not adequately capture the emotional tenor in real social interactions

because emotional messages in real-life settings are often conveyed via a combination of

different types of emotional cues (e.g. facial cues, bodily cues, contextual/situational cues).

Contextual cues in particular are known to provide additional crucial information when

appraising others’ emotional states (Carroll & Russell, 1996). In other words, if past studies

had provided more emotional information, while making the lab settings more generalizable,

findings might have been more consistent.

Page 35: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

26

Second, most of these instruments require verbal labelling of emotions based on

choices for each item, and responses are scored as dichotomous outcomes (i.e. right or

wrong). This dichotomous scoring method may cause statistical distortions, especially with

small sample sizes (Comrey, 1988), and can skew the interpretation of the results. Because

previous studies typically involved small sample sizes, this type of forced-choice format and

dichotomous scoring may have contributed to inconsistent past results.

Third, some of these instruments, such the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal

Accuracy (DANVA), do not assess recognition accuracy for some basic and complex

emotions that young children may gradually learn to understand during the course of

development. Examples include some neutral emotions (relaxed, tired, exhausted) as well as

disgust, guilt, pride, and jealousy. Of particular interest, assessing the recognition of ‘disgust’

seems to have some clinical implications in CBT for certain types of anxiety disorders, such

as OCD (Rector, Daros, Bradbury, & Richter, 2012). Failing to provide a wider array of

complex and clinically relevant emotions may have hindered attainment of developmentally

meaningful and reliable outcome data, potentially leading to the mixed findings in this area

of research.

Lastly, emotional cues used in previous studies are motionless. During social

interactions, however, we perceive emotional expressions as dynamic and spontaneous

processes rather than perceiving them as static or motionless in time. Research suggests that

providing even a little dynamic information when presenting emotional cues allows more

accurate emotion recognition compared to using exclusively static information (Elfenbein &

Ambady, 2002; Wehrle, Kaiser, Schmidt, & Scherer, 2000). Although a very recent study

argues that young children may not have any significant advantage in emotion recognition

Page 36: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

27

using dynamic expressions than using static expressions (Nelson & Russell, 2012), the

children in this study were limited to 4-7 years of age. Participants in past studies in this area

of research ranged from 7 to 17.

New research, therefore, needs to assess emotion recognition ability in children with

anxiety disorders using a developmentally sensitive tool that is well-validated in children of

varying ages, while addressing the above shortcomings of traditional experimental designs.

Other limitations of previous studies.

There are other limitations within the experimental design of previous studies. First,

sample sizes were generally small in previous studies. Only two previous studies are

exceptions to this observation; however, even these studies had some flaws in sampling. For

example, one study (Manassis & Young, 2000) had an anxiety group size similar to the

present study, but this study included preschool children with limited emotional vocabulary,

which may have affected results. The other one, by Guyer et al. (2007), had a much larger

total number of child and adolescent participants than the present study, involving various

clinical groups. However, there were only 14 children diagnosed with anxiety disorders, and

these children were mixed with children with depression to form a single experimental

group. Thus, results could not be clearly attributed to the presence of anxiety disorders.

Second, the effect of age on emotion recognition accuracy has not been examined in

children with anxiety disorders. However, age seems to have major impacts on emotion

recognition accuracy in children without anxiety disorder (Durand, Gallay, Seigneuric,

Robichon, & Baudouin, 2007; Feldman & Philippot, 1990). Careful control of this variable

in children with anxiety disorders might have yielded more meaningful and reliable outcome

data in past studies. For example, in some past studies, the age range was large despite a

Page 37: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

28

small number of participants, so results may have been largely due to age effects. Therefore,

it may be important to scrutinize the effect of this developmental proxy in order to explain

the inconsistent results of past studies.

Third, diverse types of anxiety disorders of childhood were often combined into one

experimental group without assessing the relative impact of distinct primary diagnoses on

emotion recognition accuracy. This conventional design is often used because anxiety

disorder types are highly comorbid with each other. However, since each anxiety disorder

has unique clinical features and symptoms, this variability in characteristics may have

differentially influenced emotion recognition accuracy in child participants of past studies.

Therefore, it is imperative to examine the effect of anxiety type on emotion recognition in

children.

Lastly, previous studies neglected to carefully assess gender effects on emotion

recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders. It is well-established, however, that

gender effects on emotion recognition accuracy exist in non-anxious individuals. Women

are generally more accurate than men in recognizing emotions (Hall, 1978; Kirouac & Dore,

1985; Rotter & Rotter, 1988). Therefore, gender difference may have interacted with clinical

status, affecting the results of previous studies.

Merits of the Present Study and General Aims

In contrast to previous studies bearing the limitations above, the present study

examines a substantial sample of child participants who are old enough to verbally express a

range of simple and complex emotions.

Age effects on emotion recognition accuracy are also scrutinized and controlled for

when comparing anxiety diagnoses for accurate statistical analyses. The present research is

Page 38: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

29

the first study to assess the effect of different types, comparing SAD, GAD, and SP types, on

children’s ability to recognize emotions. Furthermore, the present study aims to examine the

effect of gender on emotion recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety

disorders.

Lastly, the present study utilizes Mood Assessment via Animated Characters

(MAAC; Manassis et al., 2009) a computerized instrument that displays an animated

character’s dynamic facial, bodily, and situational cues in order to measure sixteen specific

feelings in young children with anxiety disorders (Appendix A). There are some key

advantages of using MAAC over static facial photographs for young anxious children’s

ability to recognize emotions. First, MAAC was specifically designed for and validated in

young children with anxiety disorders. Second, MAAC displays dynamic and subtle types of

emotional cues (facial, bodily, and situational cues) using an animated character. These

animations may provide more accurate and clear emotional messages to the child for

appraisal than do still facial photographs removed from context. Third, MAAC’s displays of

a child-friendly animated character may help reduce participants’ anxiety during assessment,

minimizing the effect of confounding factors such as state anxiety. Fourth, the standard

scoring of responses on MAAC introduces three-level ordinal scores (incorrect, close to

correct, correct), allowing researchers to capture the developmental patterns or ‘growth’ of

emotion recognition ability in children with anxiety disorders, while mitigating the statistical

bias associated with dichotomous scoring. All of these advantages justify the use of MAAC

in the present study. More details on this instrument are explained in Chapter 2.

This thesis has four main aims: (a) to compare children with anxiety disorders and

children without anxiety disorder in their accuracy of recognizing the emotional states

Page 39: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

30

expressed by an animated character on MAAC, (b) to examine age-related changes in

emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders in comparison with that in

children without any anxiety disorder, (c) to determine how the most documented types of

anxiety disorders for anxious youth, namely SAD, GAD, and SP, are related to children’s

ability to recognize emotions, and (d) to assess any gender effect on emotion recognition

accuracy in children with anxiety disorders.

In addition to these main research questions, this thesis also reports secondary

analyses in a separate chapter (Chapter 4) that explores the effects of other potential

confounding factors (e.g. state anxiety, task completion time, depressive/anxiety symptoms)

on emotion recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

Hypotheses

Objective #1: to determine emotion recognition accuracy in children with and

without anxiety disorder (examined in Chapter 2).

For this objective, I predict that children with anxiety disorders will show lower

emotion recognition accuracy than non-anxious children. A recent meta-analysis

(Demenescu et al., 2010) on emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders

has suggested that presence of anxiety disorders is not associated with children’s ability to

recognize various emotions. In this review, however, the sampling criterion was based on

the presence of any type of anxiety disorder, without specifying types of anxiety disorders.

Some included studies only contained children with SP, excluding children with SAD or

GAD as primary diagnosis. Such sampling design may not accurately represent the

proportion of different anxiety groups in a treatment setting or in the general population, and

fails to consider unique clinical features of each type of anxiety disorders. By studying a

Page 40: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

31

diverse anxiety group in a treatment setting with careful attention to anxiety types,

differences between anxious and non-anxious children may be elucidated.

Objective #2: to determine the effect of age on emotion recognition accuracy in

children with and without anxiety disorder (examined in Chapter 2).

This second objective examines the effect of age on emotion recognition accuracy in

children with and without anxiety disorders. It is predicted that there will be a positive

correlation between age and overall emotion recognition accuracy in both children with and

without anxiety disorders because age represents a conventional proxy for development.

This prediction is based on the previous literature on the effect of age on emotion recognition

in children without anxiety disorder. In previous studies, age had a positive correlation with

emotion recognition accuracy in children and adolescents (Durand et al., 2007; Feldman &

Philippot, 1990). Furthermore, it is predicted that the rate of age-related improvement in

emotion recognition accuracy will be significantly lower in children with anxiety disorders

than in children without anxiety disorder because clinically anxious children tend to have

more problems in social and emotional domains than non-anxious children. However, this

particular prediction has not been previously tested, so the results will be preliminary.

Objective #3: to determine the effects of anxiety subtypes on emotion recognition

accuracy in children with anxiety disorders (examined in Chapter 2).

The third objective examines the effect of primary type of anxiety disorders on

emotion recognition accuracy in children. It is hypothesized that children with SP will be

significantly more impaired in recognizing others’ emotional states than children with other

types of anxiety disorder or children without anxiety disorder because the primary symptoms

of SP pertain to social features. However, there has been no study that evaluates the relative

Page 41: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

32

effects of anxiety subtype on children’s emotion recognition accuracy, so the findings of this

objective will be preliminary.

Objective #4: to determine effect of gender on emotion recognition accuracy in

children with and without anxiety disorders (examined in Chapter 3).

For the fourth objective, examining the effect of gender on emotion recognition

accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorder, it is predicted that girls, regardless of

clinical status, will be significantly more accurate in recognizing emotions than boys. This

prediction is based on the finding of a systematic review on emotion recognition accuracy in

children and adolescents (McClure, 2000). This systematic review indicates that girls

without any clinical diagnosis are more accurate than boys without a clinical diagnosis in

recognizing basic emotions when facial emotion cues are presented (McClure, 2000).

As these gender differences exist in predicting emotion recognition accuracy in

children without anxiety disorder (McClure, 2000), the gender factor may also influence

emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders. Therefore, it is predicted in

this study that girls with anxiety disorders will be more accurate than boys with anxiety

disorders in emotion recognition. Further, non-anxious girls will be more accurate in

emotion recognition than non-anxious boys. The present study is the first attempt to

comprehensively examine potential gender effects on emotion recognition accuracy in

children with anxiety disorders, so findings will be preliminary.

Page 42: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

33

Chapter Two

Effects of Age and Subtype on Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders

Contents of this chapter have been published in Canadian Journal of Psychiatry:

Lee T. C., Dupuis, A., Jones, E., Guberman, C., Herbert, M., & Manassis, K. (2013). Effects

of age and subtype on emotional recognition in children with anxiety disorders: Implications

for cognitive-behavioural therapy. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry [In Press].

Page 43: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

34

Abstract

This study examined whether an anxiety diagnosis, age, and subtype are associated

with emotion recognition accuracy in school-age children. Children with anxiety disorders

performed comparably with children without anxiety disorder in emotion identification. In

both groups, accuracy for disgust increased significantly each year of age. When age and

primary anxiety types were considered, however, children with separation anxiety disorder

(SAD) showed a deficit in overall emotion recognition, compared with children with other

subtypes or without anxiety disorder. Further regression analyses showed that children with

generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) presented significantly lower accuracy than control

children at a young age, but this deficit disappeared with increased age. Children with

anxiety disorders as a group may not appear to be impaired in emotion recognition. However,

when age and subtypes are considered, children with SAD and young children with GAD

appear to have difficulty, compared with children without anxiety disorder.

Page 44: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

35

Introduction

Childhood anxiety disorders, especially separation anxiety disorder (SAD),

generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), and social phobia (SP), are most effectively treated with

a combination of medication and cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) (Walkup et al., 2008).

CBT or medication (for example, sertraline) alone is more effective than placebo, however,

CBT tends to cause fewer side effects than medication (Walkup et al., 2008).

For some clinicians, therefore, CBT may be preferred as a first-line treatment for

anxiety disorders in children. In an evidence-based, manualized CBT for anxiety disorders

in children (such as, Coping Cat CBT; Kendall & Hedtke, 2006), clinicians include activities

that may help clinically anxious children facilitate emotion recognition (for example,

discussion of nonverbal cues for feelings or pictorial representations of feelings) (Kendall &

Hedtke, 2006). However, there is a lack of consistent empirical evidence for these children’s

deficits in recognizing others’ emotions (Easter et al., 2005; Manassis & Young, 2000;

McClure et al., 2003; Guyer et al., 2007; Melfsen & Florin, 2002; Simonian et al., 2001).

Rather, recent research has identified deficits in other emotional domains in children with

anxiety disorders, such as emotion regulation skills (Suveg & Zeman, 2004) and identifying

their own emotional states (Zeman, Shipman, & Suveg, 2002).

Therefore, a new emotion-focused CBT for children with anxiety disorders has been

developed. This program adds sessions to existing CBT protocols (Suveg et al., 2006)

focusing on improving emotion regulation and identifying one’s own emotional states. The

emphasis is less clearly placed on training children with anxiety disorders to identify others’

emotions. As the ability to recognize others’ emotions is crucial for social interactions

(Ciarrochi, Heaven, & Supavadeeprasit, 2008), and is postulated to help children with

Page 45: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

36

anxiety disorders adaptively regulate their emotional experiences (Suveg et al., 2009),

scrutinizing the developmental trajectory of this conscious ability informs both anxiety- and

emotion-focused CBT for children with anxiety disorders.

Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders

It remains unclear whether children with anxiety disorders are impaired in

identifying the emotional states of others (Easter et al., 2005; Manassis & Young, 2000;

McClure et al., 2003; Guyer et al., 2007; Melfsen & Florin, 2002; Simonian et al., 2001).

Findings for children with SP are particularly inconsistent (Melfsen & Florin, 2002;

Simonian et al., 2001), and emotion recognition accuracy in children with SAD or GAD

(commonly treated with CBT) has not been examined to date.

Conflicting results may relate to study limitations. First, previous studies generally

had small sample sizes. Only one study (Manassis & Young, 2000) had an anxiety group

size comparable to that of the present study, but this study contained few control subjects

without anxiety disorders and included preschool children whose emotional vocabulary is

generally limited (Aldridge & Wood, 1997). Another study (Guyer et al., 2007) included a

much larger total number of participants aged between 7 and 18 years, but there were only 14

children with anxiety disorders, and they were mixed with children with depression into a

single experimental group. Second, there was a lack of well-validated, developmentally

sensitive tools to measure the ability to identify both simple and complex emotions in young

children with anxiety disorders. Past research relied on facial pictures, which have not been

well-validated for use with children younger than 8 years. Also, greyscale facial pictures

may be unappealing to children. Further, these stimuli cannot provide some types of

emotional cues that children often use. For example, preschool children with anxiety

Page 46: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

37

disorders can use both facial and bodily cues for identifying emotions (Nelson & Russell,

2011), and with increased age, school-age children rely more on situational cues than facial

cues (Hoffner & Badzinski, 1989). Third, age effects on emotion recognition ability have

not been examined in children with anxiety disorders, although age significantly predicts the

ability to recognize emotions in children without anxiety disorders (Durand et al., 2007;

Feldman & Philippot, 1990). The effect of this developmental proxy may explain the

inconsistent results of past studies. Finally, past studies have lumped diverse anxiety

disorders into one proband group without assessing the relative impact of distinct anxiety

types on emotion recognition.

By contrast, our study examines a large sample of children old enough to express a

range of feelings using a developmentally sensitive tool that displays facial, bodily, and

situational cues. We pursued three main objectives: to compare the accuracy in identifying

others’ emotions in children with and without anxiety disorders, to examine age effects on

the emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders, and to determine how

the anxiety disorders commonly treated with CBT in youth with anxiety disorders (SAD,

GAD, and SP) are related to children’s emotion recognition accuracy.

Page 47: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

38

Methods

Participants

A total of 130 participants between 6 and 11 years of age were initially recruited,

including 65 referred patients with anxiety from an outpatient anxiety clinic in the Hospital

for Sick Children in Toronto and 65 volunteer control participants via community

advertisement in the Toronto area. In terms of demographics, children in both case and

control groups were predominantly from well-educated Caucasian families. The institutional

review board (IRB) of the hospital reviewed the research protocols and approved this study.

Children were excluded from the sampling if they were: (a) taking psychoactive medication

(e.g. selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor) or receiving any type of psychological treatment

(e.g. CBT); (b) suffering from any psychosis or intellectual disability; or (c) presenting with

the primary diagnosis of a mood disorder or developmental disorder. All the included

participants and their parents completed informed assent and consent, respectively.

All participants completed a semi-structured diagnostic interview (Anxiety Disorders

Interview Schedule or ‘ADIS’) (Silverman & Albano, 1996) as part of clinical assessment,

administered by trained psychiatrists with at least three years of experience using this

instrument. After this screening process, the sample was reduced to 122 participants because

six children from the control group had marked difficulties understanding instructions in

English, and two children from the anxiety group had only subclinical levels of anxiety.

The anxiety group of 63 children mainly had primary diagnoses of SAD (n = 13),

GAD (n = 35), and SP (n = 10), and a few children had specific phobia (n = 3), and post-

traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; n = 2). Among the 63 clinically anxious children in the

final sample, 38.1% had a secondary comorbid anxiety diagnosis, and 8.0% had a comorbid

Page 48: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

39

non-anxiety diagnosis (e.g. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder). The control group of 59

children in the final sample was screened for psychopathology and did not meet full criteria

for any non-anxiety DSM diagnosis (e.g. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, major

depressive disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, learning disability).

Instruments

Mood Assessment via Animated Characters (MAAC).

The Manassis Lab developed and validated a computer-based self-report instrument,

MAAC, which was specifically designed for young anxious children to elicit feeling states

associated with their psychopathology. MAAC displays a female teenage animated character

(“Teena”) expressing 16 different types of feeling states (relaxed, bored, exhausted,

surprised, sad, guilty, ashamed, angry, irritable, jealous, scared, nervous, disgusted, happy,

elated, and pleased) in animation for about three to four seconds (Manassis et al., 2009).

Using the child-friendly animated character, instead of plain text and scale, MAAC measures

a young child’s ability to recognize feelings of others (i.e. emotion of Teena) and self (by

comparing himself/herself to animated emotions expressed by Teena). The interface of

MAAC displays a tableau of sixteen still facial expressions of Teena’s simple and complex

emotions. If a child presses the emotion representation on the tablet screen with a PC stylus

pen, the selected emotion picture becomes a short animated cartoon. These animations show

Teena’s dynamic and engaging facial, bodily, and situational cues that capture the character’s

current emotional state. The child can play and/or replay the emotion-related clips in any

order of his or her preference.

State-trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAIC).

Page 49: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

40

The STAIC contains two sets of 20 items using a 3-point scale that measures the

intensity of both long-term trait anxiety (i.e. how children usually feel or their general

tendency to be anxious) and transitory state anxiety (i.e. how children feel right now or a

temporary state of anxiety specific to situations) (Spielberger, Edwards, Lushene, Montuori,

& Platzak, 1973). Therefore, the STAIC state anxiety scores represent changes in transient

anxiety that children experience during psychological testing or treatment, whereas the

STAIC trait anxiety scores are used to identify children with “high levels of neurotic anxiety”

(Spielberger et al., 1973). The STAIC can be administered to children between the ages of 6

and 14. Participants in this study completed the STAIC at the time of MAAC administration

to determine if the children were feeling anxious during the testing, which could potentially

be a confounding variable in the relation between a diagnosis of anxiety disorder and

emotion recognition.

Procedures

A graduate student with a master’s degree introduced MAAC to a child based on

standardized protocols (“On this computer is a cartoon character named Teena who has a

number of different feelings.”). The child was first allowed to freely explore pictorial

representations of the animated character, selecting any picture(s) to view the emotion-

specific animations and gaining familiarity with MAAC (“Right now, she is just sort of

hanging out. If you press a button at the bottom of the screen, she will act out one of her

feelings”). Following the introduction, the child was asked to pick the emotion(s) that he/she

was feeling at the moment and rate intensity (“Pick the button where Teena seems to feel the

way you’re feeling right now. How can you tell? If 5 checkmarks is a perfect match between

how you feel right now and how Teena feels and “X” means you don’t feel that way at all

Page 50: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

41

right now, show how much you feel like Teena right now on the scale”). Then, the graduate

student and child visited each animated emotion together in order from top to bottom, left to

right, asking the child to identify the emotional state of Teena (“Tell me how Teena is

feeling”). The same instruction was repeated until all sixteen emotions on MAAC were

viewed (“Let’s have a look at Teena’s other feelings.”), selecting all the buttons not

previously selected. All verbal responses of the child were tape-recorded for scoring of

his/her emotion identification accuracy.

For scoring, each response was converted to a numerical score based on how

accurately the child identified the emotion: 0 (incorrect), 1 (close but not exactly correct),

and 2 (correct). The child’s overall or general ability to identify the emotional states of

others was defined by a total accuracy score, obtained by adding the individual scores across

the sixteen emotions presented on MAAC. Therefore, the maximum total score is 32 points

(2 points multiplied by 16 items). To establish inter-rater reliability, two graduate students

blindly and independently scored the responses endorsed by participants, and the Kappa

statistic was computed.

Statistical Analyses

For the first main analysis, comparing the emotion recognition accuracy between

clinically anxious children and non-anxious children, both a Wilcoxon Rank Sum test and an

independent t-test were used to compare total accuracy scores between the two groups. The

parametric t-test was conducted because the residual distributions of the total accuracy scores

were normally distributed for both anxiety and control groups (the findings on the normality

assumption will be reported in the results section). However, the dependent variable (total

accuracy score) was defined as the sum of sixteen ordinal scores in this study, and such

Page 51: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

42

restricted ordinal score range could also justify the use of the non-parametric rank sum

comparison. Therefore, the present study reported both parametric and non-parametric

results. Further, ordinal regression analyses were conducted to compare these two groups’

ability to identify each of sixteen specific emotions.

For the second main analysis, examining age effects on emotion recognition, linear

regression analyses were conducted in the anxiety and control groups for modeling the

relationship between age (predictor) and total accuracy score (outcome). Then, ANCOVA

was conducted for comparing the slopes and intercepts of these two regression lines in order

to determine if the general ability to identify emotions in anxious children differentially

correlates with age in comparison with that of non-anxious children. Additionally, ordinal

logistic regression analyses were conducted to determine if clinically anxious children’s and

non-anxious children’s ability to identify specific types of emotions differentially changes

with age.

For the third main analysis, comparing the emotion recognition accuracy as a

function of the primary anxiety diagnosis, children with specific phobia or PTSD were

dropped because these samples were too small for this analysis. An ANCOVA was

conducted to contrast differences in mean total accuracy scores among the anxiety groups of

SAD, GAD, and SP, while age was controlled for. Then, Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise

comparisons were conducted with the two-tailed alpha level set at 0.05 for statistical

significance. Furthermore, linear regression analyses were used to model the association

between age and total accuracy score in each primary anxiety diagnosis group to examine the

developmental trajectory of the general ability to recognize others’ emotions in these groups.

Using ANCOVA, the slopes and intercepts of these regression lines were compared with

Page 52: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

43

those of the non-anxious control group by checking for the presence of any interaction

between the lines.

Page 53: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

44

Results

Mean ages of the anxiety group (M = 8.7, SD = 1.2) and non-anxious control group

(M = 8.3, SD = 1.4) were similar, t (120) = - 1.60, p = 0.11. A one-way ANOVA indicated

that SAD (M = 8.5, SD = 1.3), GAD (M = 8.8, SD = 1.1), SP (M = 8.4, SD = 1.3), and

control groups were also comparable in mean age, F (3, 113) = 1.18, p = 0.32. The normality

assumption was tested for age and total accuracy score variables and found that the data of

these variables were normally distributed within each comparison group. The state anxiety

scores did not differ among the SAD, GAD, SP, and control groups, F (3, 86) = 0.79, p =

0.50.

Objective #1: Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety

Disorder

Total accuracy score in anxious children was not significantly different from non-

anxious children when a t-test was conducted, t (120) = 0.73, p = 0.68. The restricted,

ordinal total accuracy score range could also justify the use of the nonparametric test.

However, the result was unchanged when a non-parametric test of a Wilcoxon Rank Sum test

was conducted, z = - 0.72, p = 0.47, as the mean of the ranks of total accuracy score in the

anxiety group was 59.27 and that of the control group was 63.88.

Ordinal logistic regression analyses failed to reveal any significant difference in

recognition accuracy for any specific types of emotions on MAAC between children with

anxiety disorders and children without anxiety disorder (Table 2). It is noteworthy that

children with anxiety disorders as a group performed exceptionally well on correctly

identifying certain basic emotions, such as happy, angry, and scared

Page 54: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

45

Objective #2: Effect of Age on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder

Age was positively correlated with total accuracy score received on MAAC (i.e. age

was correlated with the general ability to identify emotions) for both anxious children (r =

0.52, p = 0.001) and non-anxious children (r = 0.36, p = 0.005). Linear regression analyses

were conducted to model the linear relationship between age (predictor) for total accuracy

score (outcome) in both anxiety and control groups: (1) Predicted total accuracy score in the

anxiety group = 7.79 + 1.49 Age (years); (2) Predicted total accuracy score in the control

group = 14.35 + 0.81 Age (years). An ANCOVA indicated that these slopes were similar,

Delta B1 (ANX-CONT) = 0.68, 95% CI -0.13 to 1.52, p = 0.10, and their intercepts were also

comparable, Delta B0 (ANX-CONT), = -6.56, 95% CI -13.87 to 0.39, p = 0.06.

Ordinal logistic regression analyses revealed that non-anxious children‘s recognition

accuracy for ‘disgusted’, ‘jealous’, and ‘proud’ emotions increased significantly each year

(Table 3). Similarly, clinically anxious children’s recognition for ‘disgusted’ and ‘jealous’

emotions also increased significantly during this period; however, their identification

accuracy for ‘tired’ and ‘nervous’ emotions was also found to increase significantly.

Objective #3: Effects of Anxiety Subtypes on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in

Children with Anxiety Disorders

A Kruskal-Wallis test was initially conducted to examine the effect of subtypes on

anxious children’s emotion recognition accuracy, due to the small SP group size with a bi-

modal data distribution. This non-parametric test did not take into account age effect on

emotion recognition, and the results revealed that there was a significant group difference in

emotion recognition accuracy, X2 (3) = 9.49, p = 0.02. A post-hoc test using a series of

Page 55: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

46

Mann-Whitney tests with Bonferroni adjustment and the alpha level set at 0.008 (0.05 / 6)

showed significant differences in total accuracy score received on MAAC between the SAD

and control groups (U = 191.50, Z = - 2.83, p = 0.005, r = 0.33) and between the SAD and

GAD groups (U = 104.50, Z = - 2.87, p = 0.004, r = 0.41).

A parametric test of ANCOVA was also conducted to compare the mean total

accuracy scores across different anxiety disorder groups, while controlling for possible age

effects. In this type of analysis, the assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes must be

met as it relates to how the covariate and the dependent variable are associated with each

other for every comparison group (Field, Miles, & Field, 2012, pp. 466). If this assumption

is violated, the results and conclusions will be misleading.

The assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes was met for the ANCOVA, F (3,

109) = 2.28, p = 0.08. The result of ANCOVA indicated that group differences by primary

anxiety diagnosis (covarying for age) were significant, F (3, 112) = 4.47, p = 0.004, eta-

squared = 0.11 (Figure 1). Pairwise comparisons between SAD (M = 18.27, SD = 0.75) and

all other groups were significant with Bonferroni corrections (GAD [M = 21.05, SD = 0.46, p

= 0.01]; SP [M = 21.42, SD = 0.86, p = 0.04]; control [M = 21.28, SD = 0.35, p = 0.003]),

but those among GAD, SP, and control groups were not. However, interpreting this result

required caution because age was significantly correlated only with the total accuracy score

in the SAD (r = 0.60, p = 0.03) and GAD groups (r = 0.68, p < 0.001), but not in the SP

group (r = 0.20, p = 0.59).

Therefore, we repeated an ANCOVA without the SP group, while age was controlled

for. The ANCOVA for group differences by primary diagnosis was still significant, F (2,

103) = 7.06, p = 0.001, eta-squared = 0.12, but very close to violating the homogeneity of

Page 56: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

47

regression slopes assumption, F (2, 101) = 2.90, p = 0.05. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise

comparisons showed that the SAD group was still significantly lower on total accuracy score

than the GAD (p = 0.001) and control groups (p = 0.006). However, the GAD and control

groups were not significantly different from each other (p > 0.99).

Linear regression equations of age predicting total accuracy score in both SAD and

GAD groups were: (1) Predicted total accuracy score in the SAD group = 6.64 + 1.36 Age

(years); (2) Predicted total accuracy score in the GAD group = 3.90 + 1.99 Age (years).

Both of these regression lines had an intercept lower than that for the control group, but only

one for the GAD group was statistically significant (SAD [Delta B0 (SAD-CONT) = -7.71, 95%

CI - 19.19 to - 3.77, p = 0.18; GAD [Delta B0 (GAD-CONT) = -10.45, 95% CI - 19.04 to - 1.87, p

= 0.02). The slope of the SAD group line was not significantly different from that for the

control group, Delta B1 (SAD-CONT) = 0.55, 95% CI - 0.79 to 1.89, p = 0.42, whereas the slope

of the GAD group line was significantly steeper than that for the control group, Delta B1 (GAD-

CONT) = 1.18, 95% CI 0.20 to 2.16, p = 0.02 (Figure 2).

To compare emotion recognition accuracy of specific emotions between the SAD

group and the control group, an ordinal regression analysis was performed for each of sixteen

specific emotions. However, the analyses did not detect any deficit in recognition of a

specific type of emotion (Table 2).

Page 57: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

48

Discussion

The present study was the first to examine age effects on anxious children’s emotion

recognition and the first to examine how some of the most documented anxiety disorders for

anxious children, SAD and GAD, are related to the ability to recognize others’ emotions.

Children in our study ranged from 6 to 11 years, when children’s verbal skills are not

correlated with the ability to identify emotions (Steele, Steele, & Croft, 2008). With these

considerations, we revisited the question of whether anxiety diagnosis is associated with

children’s ability to recognize the emotional states of others, illuminating explanations for

contradictory findings in the past.

Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorder

Because the prevalence of specific anxiety disorders in children varies across

epidemiologic studies (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2006), our anxiety group may not show the

same diagnostic distributions as children with anxiety disorders in the general population.

Initially, we placed different anxiety types into one proband group to replicate conventional

research designs that neglected to examine various types of primary diagnosis.

When children with and without anxiety disorders are compared with non-anxious

children on emotion recognition abilities, our results show that children with anxiety

disorders can identify the emotional states of others as accurately as children without anxiety

disorder. This finding is consistent with a recent meta-analysis of emotion recognition in

children with anxiety disorders (Demenescu et al., 2010) and with the findings of McClure et

al., (2003) Manassis and Young (2000), and Guyer et al. (2007). Moreover, we failed to

detect any difficulty in children with anxiety disorders in recognizing any specific type of

emotion compared with children without any anxiety disorder. Rather, children with anxiety

Page 58: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

49

disorders seem proficient in recognizing basic emotions when various types of emotion-

related cues are available.

Effects of Age and Anxiety Subtypes on Emotion Recognition Accuracy

Our findings indicate that, as in children without anxiety disorder, the general ability

to recognize others’ emotions in children with anxiety disorders increases significantly with

age, and the rate of improvement is comparable with that of children without anxiety

disorder. In both children with and without anxiety disorders, recognition of ‘disgust’

improves significantly between 6 and 11 years of age. Furthermore, in both groups,

identification accuracy of some complex emotions increases significantly each year,

especially emotions conveyed via bodily or contextual cues (e.g. ‘jealous’, ‘pleased/proud’,

‘tired’ and ‘nervous’). On the other hand, the recognition of ‘nervous’ appears to improve

significantly each year during the elementary years in children with anxiety disorders,

whereas the recognition of ‘proud’ improves significantly each year in children without

anxiety disorders.

When age effects were controlled, children with SAD demonstrated a significantly

lower overall ability to recognize the emotional states of others, compared with children

without anxiety disorder and children with the primary diagnosis GAD or SP. Like children

with GAD or children without anxiety disorder, children with SAD demonstrated age-

dependent improvement in emotion recognition accuracy. On the other hand, children with

GAD also showed difficulty at a young age, but their ability to identify others’ emotions

improved with age at a faster rate to catch up with that of children without anxiety disorder

during school years.

Limitations and Clinical Implications

Page 59: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

50

Our study has limitations. First, the present findings are limited to children ages 6

through 11 years. Second, our findings do not address whether children with anxiety

disorders have problems recognizing their own emotional states, but the scope of our

research is limited to recognizing others’ emotional states through a female animated

character. Third, including more participants with SAD or SP would have much improved

statistical confidence/power, especially regarding age effects on emotion recognition

accuracy in these subgroups. Fourth, we did not measure general intelligence or verbal skills

of our participants as a covariate. Fifth, children with anxiety disorders in our study were not

more state anxious than their non-anxious counterparts at the time of testing. Therefore, our

findings do not capture these children’s ability to recognize emotions during anxiety-

provoking situations (for example, social activities for children with SP). Sixth, there may

have been ceiling effects in the measure as participants generally made few errors on

recognizing basic emotions. Lastly, MAAC does not measure processing time for emotion

recognition.

Our findings suggest that augmenting emotion recognition skills may help children

with SAD and early school-age children with GAD, as they appear to have difficulty

identifying various emotions, compared with their non-anxious counterparts. However, the

treatment may be more effective if clinicians discuss various types of cues for both basic and

complex feelings, not limited to facial cues for basic emotions only, with these children.

Finally, our findings support the flexible use of anxiety-focused CBT, allowing for increased

emphasis on understanding emotions in children with anxiety disorders with deficits in social

or emotional understanding. However, such flexibility may be less crucial in children with

GAD, whose impaired emotion recognition seems transitory at a young age.

Page 60: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

51

Future Directions

First, future directions include a comparison of emotion recognition deficits with

other clinical groups using a developmentally sensitive tool in school-age children with

SAD. Second, as the ability to identify others’ emotions in children with SP was not linearly

commensurate with age, this clinical subgroup needs to be re-examined with a larger sample

size of varying ages. Third, emotion recognition ability of children with anxiety disorders

needs to be measured during anxious states to determine if their recognition is distorted in

such situations, and in relation to gender. Lastly, longitudinal studies will be required to

confirm the developmental trajectory of emotion recognition ability in children with anxiety

disorders.

Page 61: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

52

Tables

Table 1. Characteristics of the Anxiety Group and the Control Group

Anxiety (n=63)

N %

Primary Anxiety Diagnosis

SAD 13 20.6

GAD 35 55.6

SP 10 15.9

Specific Phobia 3 4.8

PTSD 2 3.2

Secondary Anxiety Diagnosis

None 34 54.0

SAD 3 4.8

GAD 13 20.6

Social Phobia 5 7.9

Specific Phobia 3 4.8

Secondary Non-anxiety Diagnosis

ADHD 1 1.6

ODD 1 1.6

LD 3 4.8

Boys 28 44.4

Age <8 19 30.2

Age – mean (SD) 8.7 1.2

Control (N=59)

N %

Boys 29 49.2

Age <8 28 47.5

Age – mean (SD) 8.3 1.4

Page 62: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

53

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Analyses of Anxiety Diagnosis and Separation Anxiety Disorder Predicting Emotion Recognition

Accuracy in Comparison with the Control Group

Anxiety, Compared with Control Group SAD, Compared with Control Group

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Relaxed 1.0 (0.5, 2.0) >0.9 1.5 (0.5, 4.8) 0.5

Bored 1.2 (0.6, 2.5) 0.7 0.6 (0.2, 2.0) 0.4

Tired 0.7 (0.3, 1.4) 0.3 0.4 (0.1, 1.2) 0.1

Surprised 0.6 (0.2, 1.3) 0.2 0.3 (0.06, 1.9) 0.2

Sad 0.9 (0.4, 1.8) 0.7 0.3 (0.09, 1.0) 0.05

Guilty 0.9 (0.5, 1.9) 0.8 0.3 (0.06, 1.3) 0.1

Ashamed 0.7 (0.3, 1.5) 0.4 0.5 (0.1, 1.6) 0.2

Angry 1.5 (0.3, 6.9) 0.6 0.8 (0.09, 8.0) 0.9

Irritable 0.9 (0.5, 1.8) 0.8 0.4 (0.1, 1.2) 0.1

Jealous 1.2 (0.6, 2.3) 0.7 1.3 (0.3, 2.5) 0.7

Scared 1.0 (0.4, 2.5) 0.9 2.5 (0.3, 22.2) 0.4

Nervous 1.5 (0.8, 3.0) 0.2 1.1 (0.3, 2.9) 0.9

Disgusted 0.9 (0.4, 1.7) 0.7 0.4 (0.1, 1.3) 0.1

Page 63: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

54

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Analyses of Anxiety Diagnosis and Separation Anxiety Disorder Predicting Emotion Recognition

Accuracy in Comparison with the Control Group (Continued)

Anxiety, Compared with Control Group SAD, Compared with Control Group

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Happy 1.6 (0.5, 4.9) 0.4 2.0 (0.2, 16.2) 0.5

Elated 1.1 (0.5, 2.1) 0.9 0.6 (0.2, 2.3) 0.5

Proud 1.1 (0.5, 2.5) 0.7 0.4 (0.09, 1.9) 0.3

Page 64: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

55

Table 3. Ordinal Regression Analyses of Age Predicting Emotion Recognition Accuracy for Children with and without Anxiety

Disorder

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Disgusted 2.1 (1.3, 3.3) 0.002 Disgusted 2.6 (1.6, 4.3) <0.001

Jealous 1.8 (1.2, 2.7) 0.003 Tired 2.1 (1.3, 3.3) 0.003

Proud 2.0 (1.2, 3.3) 0.004 Nervous 1.7 (1.1, 2.7) 0.02

Jealous 1.7 (1.1, 2.7) 0.02

Bored 1.5 (1.0, 2.5) 0.05

Ashamed 1.4 (1.0, 2.2) 0.06 Surprised 1.7 (1.0, 3.0) 0.06

Relaxed 1.4 (1.0, 2.0) 0.09 Bored 1.5 (0.9, 2.4) 0.1

Happy 1.6 (0.9, 3.0) 0.1 Irritable 1.4 (0.9, 2.2) 0.1

Guilty 0.8 (0.5, 1.1) 0.1 Happy 1.6 (0.7, 3.3) 0.2

Nervous 1.3 (0.9, 1.8) 0.2 Sad 1.3 (0.8, 2.0) 0.3

Irritable 1.2 (0.8, 1.8) 0.3 Scared 0.7 (0.4, 1.3) 0.3

Angry 0.7 (0.3, 1.5) 0.3 Ashamed 1.3 (0.8, 2.0) 0.3

Surprised 1.2 (0.8, 1.8) 0.4 Guilty 0.8 (0.5, 1.2) 0.3

Page 65: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

56

Table 3. Ordinal Regression Analyses of Age Predicting Emotion Recognition Accuracy for Children with and without Anxiety

Disorder (continued)

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Sad 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) 0.6 Relaxed 0.8 (0.5, 1.2) 0.4

Elated 1.1 (0.7, 1.6) 0.7 Angry 1.5 (0.5, 4.0) 0.5

Tired 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) 0.7 Proud 1.2 (0.7, 2.0) 0.5

Scared 1.0 (0.6, 1.6) 0.9 Elated 1.0 (0.7, 1.6) 0.8

Page 66: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

57

57

Figures

Figure 1. Total Accuracy Score by Anxiety Types: SAD, GAD, SP, and Control Groups

- Significant difference among test groups, F(3,112)=4.47, p=0.004, eta-squared=0.11

Page 67: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

58

58

Figure 2. Regression Lines for Age and Total Accuracy Score on MAAC in the SAD and GAD

Groups, Comparison with the Control Group

- Compared to the control group, the y-intercept of the SAD group was not significantly

lower (p = 0.18), and the slope was not significantly different (p = 0.4).

- Compared to the control group, the y-intercept of the GAD group was significantly lower

(p=0.02), and its slope was significantly steeper (p=0.02)

Page 68: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

59

59

Chapter Three

Effect of Gender on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with Anxiety Disorders

This chapter was prepared as a brief report for journal submission

Page 69: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

60

60

Abstract

The present study examined the link between gender and emotion recognition accuracy in

school-age children with and without anxiety disorders. Gender failed to predict overall emotion

recognition accuracy. However, disgust recognition was significantly less accurate in clinically

anxious girls than in clinically anxious boys, and was also less accurate, albeit not significantly

so, than in age-matched non-anxious girls.

.

Page 70: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

61

61

Introduction

The ability to accurately recognize emotions through nonverbal cues mediates children’s

social and academic outcomes (Izard et al., 2001). This ability may be associated with anxious

children’s social difficulties and hence requires scrutiny. There is a lack of theoretical model

that explains for gender differences in emotion recognition patterns. Nevertheless, in non-

anxious children, girls are significantly more accurate than boys in recognition of emotions via

facial cues (McClure, 2000), and this female advantage is also consistent across different

cultures (Elfenbein et al., 2002).

In children with anxiety disorders, gender differences have not been extensively

examined in relation to emotion recognition accuracy. To elucidate the nature of their emotional

difficulties, however, it may be important to characterize gender differences in emotion

recognition accuracy in this clinical population. More specifically, gender-specific patterns of

emotion recognition accuracy in anxious children may reveal a novel biobehavioral marker for

anxiety disorders of childhood. Therefore, we examined gender effects on emotion recognition

accuracy in clinically anxious children, using developmentally sensitive, dynamic displays of

emotions. Then, we compared the result with that for non-anxious, age-matched counterparts.

Page 71: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

62

62

Methods

Data were obtained from a previous study by Lee et al. (2013), which included 122

school-aged children (57 boys and 65 girls) 6-11 years of age, consisting of 63 clinic-referred

children with an anxiety disorder at the Hospital for Sick Children in Toronto and 59 non-

anxious volunteers. Diagnostic interviews were conducted by experienced clinicians, using the

Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule. Any child with ongoing treatments or with a presentation

of psychosis or intellectual disability was not included in this study. The anxiety group mainly

consisted of children with separation anxiety disorder (n=13), generalized anxiety disorder

(n=35), and social phobia (n=10), with a few participants with specific phobia (n=3) and post-

traumatic stress disorder (n=2). The control group did not contain any anxiety or non-anxiety

DSM diagnoses.

Emotion recognition accuracy was measured with Mood Assessment via Animated

Characters (MAAC), a computerized self-report instrument, specifically designed for anxious

children (Manassis et al., 2013). MAAC presents a child-friendly character expressing sixteen

types of emotions (relaxed, bored, tired, surprised, sad, guilty, ashamed, angry, irritable, jealous,

scared, nervous, disgusted, happy, elated, and proud) via facial, bodily, and contextual cues in

dynamic motion.

All children viewed each of sixteen emotion-specific animations, and were asked to

identify the character’s emotional state. The child’s response for each emotion was scored for

accuracy (0=incorrect, 1=close to correct, 2=correct), and individual scores were summed for the

total accuracy score. Inter-rater agreement on scoring was previously computed (Lee, Dupuis,

Jones, Guberman, Herbert, & Manassis, 2013), and was excellent (kappa=0.92, p < 0.001).

Page 72: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

63

63

Statistical Analysis

The total accuracy score was compared between anxious boys and anxious girls using an

independent t-test. Then, ordinal regression analyses were used to compare emotion recognition

accuracy between the gender groups on each of sixteen emotions. Using the same method,

accuracy was also compared between genders in non-anxious children and between anxious girls

and non-anxious girls. Further, we measured state anxiety (T-score on the State-Trait Anxiety

Inventory for Children) and depressive symptoms (T-score on the Children’s Depression

Inventory) to control for potential confounding factors.

Page 73: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

64

64

Results

Boys with Anxiety Disorders vs. Girls with Anxiety Disorders

Boys with anxiety disorders (n = 28, M = 8.83, SD = 0.97) and girls with anxiety

disorders (n = 35, M = 8.51, SD = 1.25) were matched for age, t (61) = 1.09, p = 0.28, state

anxiety (p = 0.67), and depressive symptoms (p = 0.06). The normality assumption was satisfied

for total accuracy score within both groups.

When an independent t-test was conducted, there was no significant difference between

boys with anxiety disorders (M = 21.46, SD = 2.50) and girls with anxiety disorders (M = 19.94,

SD = 3.54) in the overall recognition accuracy (i.e. total score), t (61) = 1.89, p = 0.06. Ordinal

regression analyses revealed that clinically anxious boys and clinically anxious girls were not

significantly different in recognition accuracy on most of the specific emotions (Table 1).

However, girls with anxiety disorders performed significantly worse than boys with anxiety

disorders on recognition of disgust (p = 0.03), boredom (p = 0.02), and surprise (p = 0.02).

Boys without Anxiety Disorder vs. Girls without Anxiety Disorder

Boys without anxiety disorder (n = 29, M = 8.25, SD = 1.36) and girls without anxiety

disorder (n = 30, M = 8.34, SD = 1.38) were comparable for mean age, t (57) = - 0.25, p = 0.80,

state anxiety (p = 0.59), and depressive symptoms (p = 0.53). The normality assumption was

satisfied within both non-anxiety groups.

Gender did not have any significant effect on the outcome of the overall emotion

recognition accuracy, t (57) = - 1.07, p = 0.29. The results of ordinal regression analyses

indicated that there was no significant gender effect on recognition accuracy for most specific

emotions; however, girls without anxiety disorder recognized the ‘relaxed/calm’ emotion more

accurately than boys without anxiety disorder (p = 0.02).

Page 74: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

65

65

Girls with Anxiety Disorders vs. Girls without Anxiety Disorder

Girls with anxiety disorders and girls without anxiety disorders were matched for age, t

(63) = - 0.55, p = 0.59, and state anxiety (p = 0.14), but depressive symptoms were significantly

higher in clinically anxious girls (p=0.006). However, depressive symptoms did not significantly

predict total accuracy score in clinically anxious girls (r = 0.09, p = 0.64) or non-anxious girls (r

= 0.21, p = 0.34). The result indicated that clinically anxious girls (M = 19.94, SD = 3.54) and

non-anxious girls (M = 21.08, SD = 3.02) did not differ in recognition accuracy for overall, t (63)

= 1.87, p = 0.07), and specific emotions (Table 1). Accuracy in disgust recognition between

groups did not differ significantly , but came close to significance level with ordinal regression

analysis (p = 0.06) in which misidentifying disgust as anger was given the score of one (i.e. close

to correct). The partial score for the response of anger for disgust is justified by the fact that

anger and disgust share the same emotional valence (negative) and that in the disgust animation

the character looks somewhat angry when she throws down her lunch bag after sticking out her

tongue. However, one may also argue that previous studies in this field consistently used a

dichotomous scoring method (correct vs. incorrect), and that the response of anger should not be

given a partial credit. Therefore, a 2x2 contingency table was also generated in which

misidentifying disgust as anger was not given a score, and in this analysis girls with anxiety

disorders showed significantly lower accuracy in recognition of disgust than girls with anxiety

disorders (p = 0.04). However, no significant difference was detected on all other specific

emotions with the 2x2 contingency table.

Page 75: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

66

66

Discussion

Although limited by modest sample size, our findings suggest that gender plays a

minimal role in overall emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders. The

finding regarding children without anxiety disorder in this study is inconsistent with the result of

a meta-analysis by McClure (2002), and suggests that school-age girls are not more proficient

than boys in emotion recognition when a combination of various dynamic, non-verbal cues for

emotion are available. This inconsistency may suggest that non-anxious boys make effective use

of contextual cues to compensate their difficulty with facial emotion recognition compared with

girls. If so, the present results may be generalizable to real world social settings where various

non-verbal channels of expression and contextual cues are available to children.

Interestingly, girls with anxiety disorders in our study are particularly less accurate in

disgust recognition, often misidentifying disgust as anger. In children without anxiety disorder,

however, boys and girls can recognize disgust at a comparable level when dynamic facial, bodily,

and contextual cues are presented. Based on this evidence, inaccurate disgust recognition in girls

with anxiety disorder may be a characteristic of anxiety disorders of childhood. Impaired disgust

recognition has also been reported in individuals with certain types of OCD (Sprengelmeyer et

al., 1997; Rector et al., 2012) or with severe OCD (Parker, McNally, Nakayama, & Wilhelm,

2004). Thus, the present finding on disgust recognition may be implicated in identifying a shared

characteristic of anxiety disorders and OCD.

Due to sample size constraints, it was not possible to examine gender effects on emotion

recognition in children with each specific type of anxiety disorders. Because types of anxiety

disorders may have an effect on emotion recognition accuracy (Lee et al., 2013), future study of

this issue is indicated. Further, a deficit in disgust recognition in girls with anxiety disorders in

Page 76: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

67

67

comparison with girls without anxiety disorder was only apparent when one type of

scoring/analysis was used. Therefore, replication of our study is required. Including OCD

participants in future studies may clarify if deficit is a potential marker for childhood anxiety.

Lastly, use of vocal cues as well as visual emotion cues in future studies may further increase

generalizability of the findings of this study.

Page 77: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

68

68

Tables

Table 1 Ordinal Regression Analyses of Gender and Clinical Status Predicting Emotion Recognition Accuracy on Specific Emotions

Anxious Girls vs. Anxious Boys Anxious Girls vs. Non-anxious Girls

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Relaxed 1.1 (0.4, 2.7) 0.9 0.9 (0.4, 2.2) 0.8

Bored 0.2* (0.07, 0.7) 0.02 0.4 (0.1, 1.3) 0.1

Tired 0.8 (0.3, 2.2) 0.7 0.5 (0.2, 1.5) 0.2

Surprised 0.2* (0.07, 0.9) 0.02 0.4 (0.1, 1.3) 0.1

Sad 0.6 (0.2, 1.6) 0.3 0.5 (0.2, 1.5) 0.2

Guilty 1.4 (0.5, 3.7) 0.5 0.9 (0.3, 2.2) 0.8

Ashamed 0.8 (0.3, 2.5) 0.7 0.7 (0.2, 1.6) 0.4

Angry - - - 1.2 (0.2, 6.7) 0.8

Irritable 1.0 (0.4, 2.5) >0.9 0.7 (0.3, 1.8) 0.5

Jealous 1.1 (0.4, 2.7) 0.9 1.2 (0.5, 3.0) 0.7

Scared 0.7 (0.2, 2.5) 0.5 1.3 (0.4, 4.1) 0.7

Nervous 0.7 (0.2, 1.8) 0.4 1.4 (0.5, 3.7) 0.5

Page 78: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

69

69

Table 1 Ordinal Regression Analyses of Gender and Clinical Status Predicting Emotion Recognition Accuracy on Specific Emotions

(Continued)

Anxious Girls vs. Anxious Boys Anxious Girls vs. Non-anxious Girls

Emotion Odds Ratio (95% CI) P Odds Ratio (95% CI) P

Disgusted 0.3* (0.1, 0.9) 0.03 0.4 (0.1, 1.6) 0.06

Happy 0.6 (0.09, 3.3) 0.5 1.4 (0.3, 6.0) 0.6

Elated 1.8 (0.7, 5.0) 0.2 0.9 (0.4, 2.5) 0.9

Proud 1.7 (0.5, 5.0) 0.4 1.2 (0.4, 3.3) 0.7

- The asterisk (*) represents a significant result

- None of the anxious boys misidentified ‘angry’, resulting in an empty cell

Page 79: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

70

70

Chapter Four

Additional Analyses: Effects of State Anxiety, Depressive Symptoms, and Anxiety

Symptoms on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with Anxiety Disorders

Page 80: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

71

71

Abstract

This additional analysis examined the effects of state anxiety, severity of depressive

symptoms and anxiety symptoms on emotion recognition accuracy in children with and

without anxiety disorders. We found that state anxiety, task completion time, and

depressive/anxiety symptoms do not significantly predict overall emotion recognition

accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders. Thus, these factors may not play a

decisive role in emotion recognition in children with anxiety and without anxiety disorders.

Page 81: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

72

72

Introduction

The secondary analyses investigated possible effects of state anxiety, depressive

symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and time elapsed for MAAC completion on emotion

recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders. Based on previous

research, state anxiety does not seem to be associated with emotion recognition accuracy in

children without anxiety disorders. For example, Surcinelli et al. (2006) reports that state

anxiety generally failed to predict emotion recognition accuracy. On the other hand, the

effect of state anxiety on emotion recognition accuracy is still unknown for children with

anxiety disorders, and this study is the first attempt to examine the link between state anxiety

and emotion recognition accuracy in clinically anxious children. Moreover, the effects of

task completion time, depressive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms on emotion recognition

accuracy have not been reported for this clinical population. Therefore, this chapter

examines the effects of these variables, and any finding will be preliminary to the literature.

Aims and Hypotheses

Objective #1: to determine the effect of state anxiety on emotion recognition

accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

According to previous research, state anxiety seems to have a significant effect on

recognition accuracy in fear-related emotions in children without anxiety disorder (Surcinelli

et al., 2006). It is therefore predicted that state anxiety will significantly affect recognition

accuracy on fear-related emotions in children with anxiety disorders in this study.

Objective #2: to investigate the effect of depressive symptoms on emotion

recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

Page 82: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

73

73

Since the literature suggests that anxiety disorders often lead to depression in children

and adolescents (Cole, Peeke, Martin, Truglio, & Seroczynski, 1998), it is predicted that

depressive symptoms indicated by the CDI total T-score will be higher in children with

anxiety disorders compared with children without anxiety disorder. It is also predicted that

depressive symptoms will be negatively correlated with the overall total accuracy score

received on MAAC regardless of the clinical status since adults with depression have a

deficit in recognizing emotional expressions (Demenescu et al., 2010).

Objective #3: to characterize the effect of anxiety symptoms on emotion

recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

Since the anxiety group in the present study consists of children characterized by

clinically high levels of anxiety diagnosed by experienced clinicians, it is predicted that

anxiety symptoms will be significantly more elevated (indicated by T-score on MASC and

SCARED) in clinically anxious children than in non-anxious children.

We have further predicted that anxiety symptoms in children with and without

anxiety disorders will be negatively correlated with total accuracy score on MAAC (i.e. the

overall emotion recognition accuracy), but will be positively related to recognition accuracy

for fear related emotions (e.g. scared, nervous).

Objective #4: to determine the effect of time elapsed for completing MAAC on

emotion recognition accuracy in children with and without anxiety disorders.

For examining the effect of time elapsed for MAAC completion on emotion

recognition accuracy, there is no specific hypothesis as children with anxiety disorders might

make hasty and biased judgment on identifying the emotional states of others, or might work

Page 83: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

74

74

more slowly as a result of excessive worry about answering correctly or worrying about what

the examiner might think.

Page 84: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

75

75

Methods

Participants, procedures, and statistical analysis are consistent with those described in

the Methods section in Chapter 2. Additional instruments used are described in this section.

These additional surveys were completed either before or after the MAAC assessment to

measure their level of state anxiety, depressive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms since such

factors may cause confounding effects on research outcomes. Administration of measures

was counterbalanced to minimize order effects.

Additional Instruments Used

Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI).

The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) is a paper-and-pencil self-report survey

that measures the presence and severity of depression in children and adolescents (Kovacs &

Beck, 1977). It is used as a screening instrument and to monitor changes in depressive

symptoms over the course of treatment (Kovacs & Beck, 1977). The long form of the CDI

consists of 27 questions, whereas the short form consists of 10 questions, each with a 3-point

scale indicating severity of the symptoms (0 = symptoms absent, 1 = symptoms mild, 2 =

symptoms definite). The present study used the long form of the CDI. The subscales include

negative mood, interpersonal problems, ineffectiveness, anhedonia, and negative self-esteem,

and the sum of these subscale scores yield the CDI total score whose T-score of 65 is

interpreted as clinically significant. Reliability of the CDI in children and adolescents

computed by coefficient alpha, item-total score product-moment correlation, and test-retest

coefficients has been proven acceptable (Smucker, Craighead, Craighead, & Green, 1986).

Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC).

Page 85: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

76

76

The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) instrument is a self-report

questionnaire targeted for children and youth from the ages of 8 to 19 years. The measure

contains 39 items, and addresses four distinct dimensions of children’s anxiety, including

physical symptoms (tense/restless and somatic/autonomic), harm avoidance (perfectionism

and anxious coping), social anxiety (humiliation/rejection and public performance fears), and

separation anxiety/panic (March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, 1997). The

administration time of MASC is about 5-15 minutes, and the measure has an inconsistency

index that detects the presence of reckless responses. MASC is equipped with Profile Sheets

which allow the conversion of raw scores to standardized T-scores. Test-retest reliability

ranges from satisfactory to excellent, whereas the parent-child agreement on ratings of

anxiety is poor to moderate (March et al., 1997).

The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED).

The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) consists of

parent and self-report versions for screening anxiety disorders in children (Birmaher et al.,

1997). This instrument provides information regarding children’s symptoms of

somatic/panic disorder, general anxiety, separation anxiety, social phobia, and school phobia

(Birmaher et al., 1997), and contains 41 items pertinent to these symptoms, each with a

three-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat true, 2 = often true) (Birmaher et al., 1999).

Both the child and parent versions of SCARED show good internal consistency (alpha = .74

to .93), test-retest reliability (intraclass correlation coefficients = .70 to .90), and

discriminative validity (from other non-anxiety disorders as well as within anxiety disorders)

(Birmaher et al., 1997).

Page 86: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

77

77

Results

Objective #1: To determine the Effect of State Anxiety on Emotion Recognition

Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorders

The normality assumption was checked to determine if the dependent variable (i.e.

total accuracy score) were normally distributed for each level of the independent variable.

The histograms showed normally distributed data in the total sample, in the anxiety group,

and in the control group; however, Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) and Shapiro-Wilk (SW) tests

of normality assumed a normal distribution only for the control group.

When a one-way ANOVA was conducted, there was no significant group difference

among the SAD, GAD, SP, and control groups, F (3, 86) = 0.79, p = 0.50.

A correlation analysis revealed that state anxiety was not significantly correlated with

the average total accuracy score received in the total sample (p = 0.64). The correlation

analysis was also conducted in the anxiety group and the control group, but no significant

correlation was detected between state anxiety and total accuracy score in either anxious

children (p = 0.81) or non-anxious controls (p = 0.84). Therefore, the ANCOVA analysis for

the objective #3 of the present study excluded state anxiety as a covariate since the variable

failed to show any significant association with the total accuracy score.

Ordinal regression analyses were performed to predict a relationship between state

anxiety and recognition accuracy for specific emotions in both the anxiety and control groups.

The result revealed that in clinically anxious children, an increase in state anxiety

significantly decreased the accuracy for recognizing ‘surprised’, while significantly

increasing the recognition accuracy for ‘irritable’ emotion (Table 1). However, in non-

Page 87: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

78

78

anxious controls there was no significant association between state anxiety and recognition

accuracy for any specific emotions (Table 1).

Objective #2: The Effect of Depressive Symptoms on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in

Children with and without Anxiety Disorders.

The normality assumption for the CDI T-score was satisfied within the anxiety and

control groups, but the GAD group showed a positively skewed distribution shown by a

histogram and tests of normality: KS (p = 0.03) and SW (p = 0.03). However, the ratios of

skewness and kurtosis statistics by standard error for the GAD group were both below the

value of 2, thus correlation analyses were pursued.

The average T-score for the total score on CDI was compared between the anxiety

and control groups using an independent t-test. The Levine’s test showed that the variances

were unequal between the two groups (p = 0.02). The t-test result indicated that the t-scores

were significantly different between the anxiety group (M = 48.57, SD = 7.50) and the

control group (M = 44.63, SD = 5.82), t (95.87) = - 3.94, p = 0.005. A one-way ANOVA

detected a significant difference among the groups of SAD, GAD, SP, and control groups, F

(3, 90) = 3.42, p = 0.02). A post-hoc test revealed that the GAD group and the control group

were significantly different, such that CDI total t-score was higher in the GAD group (M =

49.66, SD = 8.16) than the control group (M = 44.63, SD = 5.82). However, no significant

difference was detected between the SAD (M = 47.60, SD = 7.53) and control groups or

between the SP (M = 45.22, SD = 5.93) and control groups.

Correlation analyses indicated that the T-score for the total CDI score was not

significantly correlated with total accuracy score either in the anxiety group (r = - 0.07, p =

Page 88: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

79

79

0.61) or in the control group (r = - 0.10, p = 0.50). Further, no significant correlation

between the variable and total accuracy score was reported within any of the subtype groups.

Ordinal logistic regression analyses in the anxiety group revealed that the T-score for

the total CDI score was not significantly associated with accurate recognition of any specific

type of emotions (p > 0.05) (Table 2). The variable was also not significantly associated

with any of the specific emotions in the control group (Table 2).

Objective #3: The Effect of Anxiety Symptoms on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in

Children with and without Anxiety Disorders.

The normality assumption for both the SCARED and MASC t-scores was satisfied

within the anxiety and control groups. The same assumption was met for both t-scores

within all of the different groups of anxiety disorders indicated by non-significant KS and

SW test results (p > 0.05). Correlation analyses showed that T-score for the SCARED and

MASC total score was not significantly related to total accuracy score in anxious children

([SCARED] r = - 0.06, p = 0.65; [MASC] r = 0.06, p = 0.73) and in non-anxious children

([SCARED] r = - 0.25, p = 0.06; [MASC] r = 0.10, p = 0.59).

The average T-scores for SCARED total score (M = 33.28, SD = 11.28), t (115) = -

5.16, p < 0.001, and MASC total score (M = 59.54, SD = 8.42), t (70) = - 4.23, p < 0.001,

were significantly higher in the anxiety group than the T-scores for SCARED (M = 22.46,

SD = 11.38) and MASC (M = 50.65, SD = 9.37) of the control group.

A one-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the presence of any group difference

in T-score for SCARED among the different types of anxiety disorders, and detected a

significant difference, F (3, 108) = 10.60, p < 0.001). A post-hoc test revealed that the SAD

(M = 38.83, SD = 10.58) and GAD (M = 32.53, SD = 10.81) groups, but not the SP group

Page 89: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

80

80

(M = 25.60, SD = 9.98), had significantly higher T-scores than the control group (M = 22.46,

SD = 11.38). On the other hand, the same type of parametric analysis was not recommended

for MASC T-scores because the SP group only had five scored MASC questionnaires. A

non-nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis test was conducted instead, and found a significant group

difference, chi square = 10.77, p = 0.01 with df = 3). Mann-Whitney tests using the Holm-

Bonferroni adjustment indicated that only the GAD group was significantly lower on the T-

score for the total MASC score (p = 0.005 with alpha level at 0.008) than the control group,

but there was no significant difference among the anxiety subtypes on this measure.

Ordinal logistic regression analyses in the anxiety group showed that the T-score for

the total SCARED score did not significantly predict the recognition accuracy for specific

types of emotions. The ‘disgusted’ emotion was close to the significance level (chi square =

3.69, df = 1), but it was not statistically significant (p = 0.06) (Table 3). In the control group,

the variable failed to significantly predict the accuracy outcome for any specific type of

emotions (Table 3).

In the anxiety group, the T-score for the total MASC score also did not significantly

predict the recognition accuracy for specific types of emotions. In the control group, the

MASC T-score was a significant predictor for recognizing the ‘sad’ emotion (chi square =

7.75, p = 0.005, df = 1) with Cox and Snell and Nagelkerke values of 0.22 and 0.27,

respectively. However, the MASC T-score failed to predict for other types of specific

emotions.

Objective #4: The Effect of Time Elapsed for Completing MAAC on Emotion

Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorders.

Page 90: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

81

81

The normality assumption for the variable of the MAAC completion time in minutes

was satisfied within the anxiety and control groups as well as within each anxiety disorder

group, indicated by the ratios of skewness and kurtosis statistics / standard error, histograms,

and KS and SW statistics (p > 0.05).

The average time elapsed for the completion of MAAC was not significantly different

between clinically anxious children (M = 14.05, SD = 2.36) and non-anxious controls (M =

13.62, SD = 2.32), t (120) = -1.03, p = 0.31. A one-way ANOVA was performed to detect

any significant difference among the different anxiety groups of SAD, GAD, SP, and control

groups, but no significant difference was detected, F (3, 113) = 1.43, p = 0.24.

Correlation analyses showed that the elapsed time for completing MAAC was not

significantly correlated with the average total accuracy score in the total sample (r = 0.06, p

= 0.50), the anxiety group (r = 0.04, p = 0.97), or the control group (r = 0.15, p = 0.28). The

effect of the time needed to complete MAAC on recognition accuracy for specific emotions

was not examined due to missing data.

Page 91: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

82

82

Discussion

Effects of State Anxiety and Time Elapsed for MAAC Completion on Emotion

Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorder

The findings of the present study imply that state anxiety is not an important factor

for emotion recognition accuracy in either children with anxiety disorders or children without

anxiety disorder. In particular, the present result regarding the effect of state anxiety on

emotion recognition accuracy supports the findings of previous studies. For instance, Cooper

et al. (2008) found that state anxiety does not significantly influence the recognition accuracy

for facial expressions of seven different types of emotions: anger, sadness, happiness, fear,

surprise, disgust, and neutral. Further, the same result is also consistent with the finding of

Surcinelli et al. (2006) that there is no association between state anxiety and recognition

accuracy for anger, sadness, happiness, surprise, disgust, and neutral emotions. However,

this previous study reported a significant association between state anxiety and recognition of

fearful facial emotion, whereas state anxiety failed to predict fear recognition accuracy in the

present study. Differences between this previous study and the present study may also relate

to the enrollment of non-clinical participants with high- and low- state anxiety in the

previous study versus our enrollment of clinical participants.

Clinically anxious children did not complete the MAAC tasks in haste or exhibited

excessive worry about selecting ‘correct’ answers when compared with non-anxious children.

Furthermore, task completion time (in minutes) does not seem to play a significant role in

predicting emotion recognition accuracy in children with or without anxiety disorders. This

finding may have some implications for MAAC administration in the future. For example, it

may not be crucial to record time taken in MAAC administrations for school-aged children,

Page 92: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

83

83

whether clinically anxious or not, since elapsed time for MAAC task completion does not

predict the child’s performance on emotion recognition tasks

Effects of Depressive Symptoms and Anxiety Symptoms on Emotion Recognition

Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorder

Findings of this study suggest that children with anxiety disorders are significantly

more depressed and anxious than children without anxiety disorder. Higher or more severe

anxiety symptoms in children with anxiety disorders than without are expected. Further, the

present findings are consistent with previous comorbidity research on anxiety disorders,

showing that depressive symptoms are very common in this population (Anderson, Williams,

McGee, & Silva, 1987; McGee et al., 1990; Seligman & Ollendick, 1998).

The findings of the present study suggest that the severity of anxiety symptoms or

depressive symptoms does not seem to directly influence children’s overall emotion

recognition accuracy. These findings, in conjunction with results of the main objectives in

this thesis, suggest that clinical status and type of anxiety disorder in children predict their

overall emotion recognition accuracy, but severity of anxiety symptoms does not.

Similarly, the severity of depressive symptoms may not play an important role in

predicting emotion recognition accuracy in either clinically anxious children or non-anxious

children. According to previous research, however, adult patients diagnosed with depression

are significantly more impaired in emotion recognition than their non-depressed counterparts

(Demenescu et al., 2010; Feinberg, Rifkin, Schaffer, & Walker, 1986; George, Huggins,

McDermut, Parekh, Rubinow, & Post, 1998; Leppanen, Milders, Bell, Terriere, & Hietanen,

2004). Therefore, it may be the case that clinical status of depression, reflecting clinicians’

judgment in the diagnosis of depression, may be more important in predicting emotion

Page 93: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

84

84

recognition accuracy than depressive symptoms in individuals measured by the CDI T-score.

Another possible explanation for the non-significant effect of depressive symptoms on

emotion recognition accuracy is that the effect may be gender-specific. For example, there is

a report discussing the significant association between depressive feelings and impaired

emotion recognition in non-anxious boys but not in non-anxious girls. Investigating this

gender-specific effect of depressive symptoms on emotion recognition accuracy goes beyond

the scope of the present study, but it may merit further research in the future.

Page 94: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

85

85

Tables

Table 1. Ordinal Regression Analyses of State Anxiety Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Relaxed 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.9 Relaxed 1.0 (0.8, 1.1) 0.6

Bored 0.9 (0.8, 1.1) 0.9 Bored 1.1 (1.0, 1.3) 0.1

Tired 1.2 (1.0, 1.3) 0.06 Tired 1.0 (0.8, 1.1) 0.5

Surprised 0.9 (0.8, 1.1) 0.2 Surprised 0.8* (0.7, 1.0) 0.04

Sad 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.6 Sad 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.9

Guilty 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.6 Guilty 0.9 (0.8, 1.0) 0.07

Ashamed 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.5 Ashamed 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) 0.9

Angry 1.0 (0.8, 1.3) 0.7 Angry 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) 0.9

Irritable 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.7 Irritable 1.2* (1.0, 1.3) 0.04

Jealous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9 Jealous 0.9 (0.8, 1.1) 0.3

Scared 0.9 (0.7, 1.1) 0.3 Scared 0.9 (0.7, 1.1) 0.3

Nervous 1.0 (0.8, 1.1) 0.5 Nervous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8

Page 95: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

86

86

Table 1. Ordinal Regression Analyses of State Anxiety Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder (Continued)

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Disgusted 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.8 Disgusted 1.2 (1.0, 1.4) 0.05

Happy 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) 0.7 Happy 1.1 (0.9, 1.4) 0.4

Elated 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8 Elated 1.1 (0.9, 1.2) 0.3

Proud 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9 Proud 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) 0.4

- The asterisk (*) denotes statistically significant result

Page 96: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

87

87

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Analyses of CDI T-Score Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Relaxed 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.5 Relaxed 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.4

Bored 0.9 (0.8, 1.1) 0.3 Bored 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.6

Tired 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.06 Tired 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.5

Surprised 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9 Surprised 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.6

Sad 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.5 Sad 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.5

Guilty 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.1 Guilty 0.9 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8

Ashamed 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.7 Ashamed 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.4

Angry 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) 0.6 Angry 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.7

Irritable 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.2 Irritable 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.4

Jealous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.6 Jealous 0.9 (0.9, 1.0) 0.08

Scared 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.8 Scared 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.7

Nervous 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.2 Nervous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.7

Page 97: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

88

88

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Analyses of CDI T-Score Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with and

without Anxiety Disorder (Continued)

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Disgusted 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9 Disgusted 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.05

Happy 1.1 (0.9, 1.2) 0.3 Happy 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9

Elated 0.9 (0.8, 1.0) 0.1 Elated 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.6

Proud 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8 Proud 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.3

Page 98: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

89

89

Table 3. Ordinal Regression Analyses of SCARED T-Score Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with

and without Anxiety Disorder

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Relaxed 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.6 Relaxed 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.8

Bored 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.4 Bored 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.8

Tired 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.5 Tired 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 0.4

Surprised 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.7 Surprised 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.2

Sad 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.9 Sad 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.5

Guilty 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.2 Guilty 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.2

Ashamed 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.6 Ashamed 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.5

Angry 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8 Angry 1.1 (1.0, 1.2) 0.3

Irritable 1.1 (1.0, 1.1) 0.05 Irritable 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.07

Jealous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.3 Jealous 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.5

Scared 1.1 (1.0, 1.1) 0.06 Scared 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.2

Nervous 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.8 Nervous 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.2

Page 99: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

90

90

Table 3. Ordinal Regression Analyses of SCARED T-Score Predicting Recognition Accuracy for Specific Emotions in Children with

and without Anxiety Disorder (Continued)

Control Anxiety

Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value Emotion Odds

Ratio

(95% CI) p-value

Disgusted 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.6 Disgusted 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.07

Happy 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8 Happy 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 0.9

Elated 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.9 Elated 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.9

Proud 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.2 Proud 1.0 (1.0, 1.1) 0.3

Page 100: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

91

91

Chapter Five

General Discussion

Page 101: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

92

92

General Discussion

To the author’s knowledge, the present study was the first to examine age effects on

emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders, and was the first to examine how

some of the most documented types of anxiety disorders for anxious children, SAD and

GAD, are related to the ability to recognize the emotional states of others. Furthermore, the

study explored gender effects on emotion recognition in children with anxiety disorders,

which had not been extensively studied in the past. Additionally, although not included as

one of the main research manuscripts, the present study conducted secondary analyses to

investigate potential impacts of state anxiety, task completion time, and depression/anxiety

symptoms on emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders, all of which

had not been reported in the past. The fact that children’s age was carefully controlled to

range from 6 to 11 years was a strength because at this age children’s verbal skills do not

seem to be correlated with the ability to identify emotions (Steele et al., 2008). Thus, the

study minimized the possible cofounding effect of individual verbal ability on emotion

recognition accuracy. Each of the study’s key findings will now be discussed in greater detail.

Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety Disorder

The prevalence of specific anxiety disorders in children varies across epidemiologic

studies (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2006), and due to the high rate of comorbidity among

specific disorders (Last, Strauss, & Francis, 1987) conventional research designs usually

study children with anxiety disorders as a single group. Therefore, in the first objective of

this study all the anxiety types of SAD, GAD, and SP (and a few children with PTSD or

specific phobia) were combined into a single experimental group to explore findings of these

past studies.

Page 102: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

93

93

The result for this first objective suggested that children with anxiety disorders do not

have difficulty identifying emotions accurately compared with children without anxiety

disorder. Using the same conventional designs of past studies, combining various types of

anxiety disorders into a single experimental group, the present study therefore produced

results similar to those of previous studies of emotion recognition in children with anxiety

disorders (Demenescu et al., 2010). Further, children with anxiety disorders did not seem to

have any difficulty recognizing any specific type of emotions in comparison with children

without anxiety disorder.

Effects of Age on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without Anxiety

Disorders

The result of the second objective indicated that emotion recognition accuracy

linearly increases with age in children with and without anxiety disorders. Unlike the initial

prediction of this study, however, the rate of improvement seemed to be comparable in the

two groups. It was also suggested that the recognition accuracy for the basic emotion of

disgust improves during the elementary school years in both children with and without

anxiety disorders. Improvement in recognition of disgust may be a common feature of

emotional development in children between 6 and 11 years of age. On the other hand, the

same children did not show age-related improvement in recognition accuracy for other basic

emotions (happy, sad, angry, and scared). This was the case because children with and

without anxiety disorder were highly accurate in recognizing these emotions at 6 years of age,

and statistical analyses could not detect any significant improvement beyond this age.

In both groups of children, recognition accuracy for some complex emotions

increased significantly each year, especially those that were conveyed via bodily or

Page 103: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

94

94

contextual cues (e.g. ‘jealous’, ‘proud/pleased’, ‘tired/exhausted’, ‘nervous’). This finding

seems to be consistent with the trend shown in children without anxiety disorder that with

increased age, representing wider social experiences and improved verbal ability to express

complex emotions, they can recognize others’ complex feelings more accurately (Gross &

Ballif, 1991).

Effects of Age and Anxiety Subtypes on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children

with Anxiety Disorders

When the age effects were included as a covariate, the present study indicated that

children with SAD have significantly lower overall emotion recognition accuracy, compared

with non-anxious children and children with the primary diagnosis GAD or SP.

Although children with SAD demonstrated age-dependent improvement in emotion

recognition accuracy in the present study, their recognition accuracy seems to be

significantly lower than that of children without anxiety disorder at an early school age, and

this deficit seems to continue throughout 6 to 11 years of age. By contrast, children with

GAD also showed a deficit in emotion recognition accuracy at a young school age, but

accuracy improved at a faster rate than that of SAD children to catch up with the accuracy of

children without anxiety disorder at later school years. These findings are only discernible

when age and different types of anxiety disorders are considered together. Conclusions for

the SP group are limited by small sample size, but children with SP failed to display

significant age effects on their overall emotion recognition accuracy.

The developmental delay in emotion recognition ability shown in children with SAD

may relate to the observation that children with SAD tend to lack early social experiences

due to their reluctance to leave their parents or attachment figures (Ollendick, King, & Yule,

Page 104: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

95

95

1994, pp. 146). The rationale for this hypothesis is based on evidence that recognition of

facial emotions can be influenced by children’s early social experiences (Pollak, Messner,

Kistler, & Cohn, 2008). Learning or expertise in emotion recognition seems to develop with

practice. On the other hand, children with GAD or SP may be more concerned than children

with SAD about others’ perceptions, as (in GAD) they may worry about being bullied or

scapegoated by peers and (in SP) they may worry about embarrassment and negative

evaluation by others (Albano et al., 2003, pp. 285-292). These concerns all involve the

children’s learning and inspection of others’ thoughts and feelings, potentially fostering

development of emotion recognition skills.

Effects of Gender on Emotion Recognition Accuracy in Children with and without

Anxiety Disorder

The findings on gender effects on emotion recognition accuracy in the present study

imply that gender plays a minimal role in emotion recognition accuracy children with and

without anxiety disorders. However, the present study found that gender is significantly

related to disgust recognition accuracy such that girls with anxiety disorders are more

impaired in recognizing disgust than boys with anxiety disorders or their non-anxious

counterparts.

It is noteworthy that gender was not associated with emotion recognition accuracy in

children without anxiety disorder in the present study. This is inconsistent with a previous

meta-analysis on gender effects in emotion recognition accuracy, which suggested that non-

anxious girls are more accurate than non-anxious boys on emotion recognition tasks

(McClure, 2000). One possible explanation relates to the fact that children in the present

study were shown various types of cues in dynamic motion. It is possible that boys in this

Page 105: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

96

96

study may have effectively used contextual cues or dynamic bodily motion of the character

to compensate for their delay, compared with girls, in facial emotion recognition. In real-life

social settings, however, children assess others’ emotional states based on a combination of

facial expressions, bodily gestures, and situational contexts. Therefore, findings of the

present study (which used all of these cues) may be more generalizable than previous studies

in the anxiety literature.

Limitations

The present study has a number of limitations. First, although participants were

mainly from well-educated Caucasian families, detailed data for children’s cultural or ethnic

background were not available for statistical analyses in this study. However, evidence

suggests that cultural background may modulate amygdala activation during emotion

processing, influencing emotion recognition accuracy. For instance, Asians tend to exhibit a

significantly more robust amygdala response upon perceiving angry faces in comparison

with Europeans, paralleled by decreased recognition accuracy (Durntl et al., 2012). Another

example of culture-specificity is provided by a meta-analysis on cross-cultural universality

and cultural specificity of emotion recognition. This study revealed that emotion recognition

accuracy tends to increase when emotions are expressed and identified by individuals of the

same national or ethnic group, reflecting in-group familiarity (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002).

In the present study, however, the main animated character of MAAC (Teena) is a white

teenage girl. Although this character captures the local majority group status in terms of

culture and ethnic background, her ethnicity may place children with minority status at a

disadvantage with respect to emotion recognition.

Page 106: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

97

97

Second, the analyses did not determine if the overall emotion recognition accuracy in

children with anxiety disorders continues to linearly increase in relation to age after the age

of eleven. Because the present research may have implications for child-focused CBT for

anxiety disorders which targets children of ages up to 13 (Kendall, Gosch, Furr, & Sood,

2008), the ideal experiment would therefore have included children older than 11 (i.e. 12 and

13 year-old children) with anxiety disorders. Replication involving the full age range up to

13 years is warranted in order to better understand the development of emotion recognition

accuracy in anxious children.

Third, the present findings are limited to recognition of the emotional states of a

female animated character, which may or may not be generalizable to other children or other

adults. Nevertheless, MAAC was used to ensure the validity of findings in young children

who may have difficulty labelling emotions based on facial expressions. This limitation may

be improved if both male and female characters, of varying ages, are presented in the future.

Fourth, the ability to recognize one’s own emotions or to recognize emotions using

verbal cues has not been addressed in this thesis. However, a study of the roles of verbal and

non-verbal cues in emotion recognition in persons with and without autism spectrum disorder

(ASD) shows that individuals with ASD can use both verbal and non-verbal cues for

emotions in the same way as individuals without ASD. High-functioning individuals with

ASD rely more on non-verbal cues than verbal cues to recognize a speaker’s emotional states

(Loveland et al., 1997). Based on this evidence from the literature of ASDs, the present

study may be limited in the sense that it fails to assess if clinically anxious children rely more

on non-verbal cues than verbal cues, or use both types of cues in the same manner as non-

anxious children. Further, although clinically anxious children show proficiency in

Page 107: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

98

98

recognizing basic emotions (e.g. happy, sad, angry) in the present study, it may be

worthwhile investigating to what extent verbal cues contribute to these children’s recognition

of complex emotions (e.g. jealous, irritable, proud). Such investigation would be relevant to

the generalizability of our findings, as it is a common sense that we perceive both verbal and

non-verbal cues in everyday situations.

Fifth, the present study contained moderately unequal group sizes, and increasing the

sample size of certain comparison group(s) would have improved statistical confidence. For

example, the numbers of participants in the SAD group and in the SP group were only 10 and

13, respectively, whereas the numbers in the GAD group and in the control group were 35

and 59, respectively. A mild to moderate degree of unequal group sizes can be expected and

can sometimes reflect randomness (Schulz & Grimes, 2002). However, because all members

in the present study are assumed to be equally influential, unequal sample group sizes that do

not reflect the population distribution may cause a bias in the estimation of effect sizes

(Kenny, Mannetti, Pierro, Livi, & Kashy, 2002). Therefore, an increase in sample size for

both the SAD and SP groups could have improved the power of our analyses, especially

regarding age effects on emotion recognition accuracy in these groups. This is especially

true for the SP group, the only diagnostic group that failed to exhibit a significant correlation

between age and emotion recognition accuracy. To resolve this limitation, the proband

group in future studies must include a larger number of children with SAD or SP.

Sixth, the present study failed to investigate the effect of socioeconomic status (SES)

of participating children’s families. In fact, SES appears to significantly affect emotion

recognition accuracy such that persons with a higher SES tend to recognize others’ emotions

better compared with those with a lower SES (Edwards, Manstead, & Macdonald, 1984; Hall

Page 108: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

99

99

& Halberstadt, 1994). Thus, SES may have played a role in predicting emotion recognition

accuracy in children with anxiety disorders in the present study, although the range of SES is

generally narrow in this clinic (middle and upper middle class). Therefore, it may be more

informative if future research includes children from various SES backgrounds and then

examine the effects of various SES levels on emotion recognition accuracy.

Seventh, the present study did measure general intelligence of the child participants.

Such information was not collected because the present study was a secondary analysis to the

validation of MAAC (Manassis et al., 2013). However, the link between general intelligence

and emotion recognition accuracy in anxious children is still unknown, and this may be an

important factor in this analysis – ergo, this factor must be addressed in the future.

Eighth, children with anxiety disorders in the present study did not show higher state

anxiety than their non-anxious counterparts at the time of the MAAC assessment. Therefore,

our findings do not capture these children’s ability to recognize emotions during anxiety-

provoking situations (e.g. social activities for children with SP). In the future, therefore, the

present study may be repeated in anxious children when their state-anxiety is high (e.g.

children with SP in anticipation of social activities or public performance).

Lastly, attentional and motivational factors may have caused a bias in the results of

the present study as some scientists have argued that individual variance in emotion

recognition accuracy is simply a reflection of individual differences in attention or

motivation to decode emotional information from emotion-related stimuli (Buck, 1988).

However, none of the existing research has assessed the effect of individual differences in

attention and motivation on emotion recognition accuracy. The present study also did not

assess the potential interaction between the effect of anxiety diagnosis and the effect of

Page 109: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

100

100

attention and motivation on emotion recognition accuracy. These issues may also be

addressed in later studies.

Implications

The present study may have some implications for CBT in the treatment of childhood

anxiety disorders (e.g. Coping Cat program) as a part of the protocol guides clinicians and

children with anxiety disorders to discuss cues for the emotional states of others. However,

greater emphasis is still placed on recognition of one’s own emotions in these CBT programs.

Findings of the present study suggest, albeit within the limitations inherent to a cross-

sectional research design, that expanded emotion recognition training may be helpful for

children with anxiety disorders, especially children with the primary diagnosis of SAD.

Increased emotion recognition training, however, may be more essential for younger anxious

children (early school-age children) than older ones as the deficit in emotion recognition

skills seems to be more prominent in the younger anxiety group in the present study. For

example, both early school-age children with SAD and early school-age children with GAD

appear to have difficulty identifying various emotions compared with their non-anxious

counterparts in the present study. However, the deficit in emotion recognition ability

remains constant throughout the elementary school years in children with the primary

diagnosis of SAD, whereas the deficit seems to diminish with time in children with the

primary diagnosis of GAD. Therefore, early intervention involving additional training in

emotion recognition skills may lead to better social experiences for young anxious children

than improving these skills during late childhood, especially in children with a primary

diagnosis of GAD.

Page 110: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

101

101

The present findings also demonstrate that children with anxiety disorders tend to be

accurate in judging basic emotions (e.g. happy, sad, angry) of others (as displayed by the

animated character) that can be conveyed via simple facial cues. Such findings are

consistent with a meta-analysis of previous research that utilized static facial cues

(Demenescu et al., 2010). In the present study, however, the deficit in emotion recognition

ability in children with SAD becomes evident only when these children are tested on a range

of both simple and complex emotions that are dynamically expressed via all facial, bodily,

and situational cues. This observation suggests that emotion recognition training in CBT for

anxious children may be more effective if clinicians facilitate discussions of various types of

cues expressing a wide range of basic and complex emotions, not limited to facial cues for

basic emotions only.

Another intriguing finding involves the decreased recognition accuracy for disgust in

clinically anxious girls in comparison with clinically anxious boys and non-anxious girls. If

replicated, it is possible that this finding may represent a marker for the presence of anxiety

disorder since school-age girls without any anxiety diagnosis are not impaired in the ability

recognize disgust in others in the present study. Although this idea may seem very

speculative, due to the lack of supporting theoretical framework in the literature, it is not

unique in the field as some reports claim that disgust recognition is impaired in individuals

with OCD (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1997), and this association seems to depend on symptom

severity and general functioning (Corcoran, Woody, & Tolin, 2008).

Overall, the present findings support the flexible use of anxiety-focused CBT,

allowing for increased emphasis on emotion recognition in clinically anxious children with

deficits in social or emotional understanding (Beidas et al., 2010). Such flexibility may be

Page 111: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

102

102

less important, however, in children with the primary diagnosis of GAD whose impaired

emotion recognition may be transitory at a young age.

Page 112: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

103

103

Conclusion

Although the author must interpret the present findings in the context of study

limitations, children with SAD showed a deficit in the recognition of others’ emotions

relative to children without anxiety disorder. Young school-aged children with GAD also

showed difficulty in this emotional domain. These underlying associations can be masked,

however, if age and specific diagnosis factors are neglected. Lack of attention to these

details in past studies may help explain their inconsistent results. Moreover, although gender

plays a minimal role in emotion recognition accuracy in children with anxiety disorders,

anxiety diagnosis may be related to impairment in disgust recognition in girls. This finding

merits further study with the inclusion of other clinical groups. Clinically, facilitating

emotion recognition skills may be a useful component of CBT for school-aged children with

SAD and for younger children with GAD.

Page 113: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

104

104

Chapter Six

Future Directions

Page 114: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

105

105

Future Directions

First school-aged children with SAD (the group that showed emotion recognition

deficits in this study) must be compared with other clinical groups using a developmentally

sensitive tool (e.g. MAAC) to accurately assess the severity of this impairment. Although

statistically significant, the difference in emotion recognition accuracy between the SAD

group and the control group seems rather mild in this study, as children with SAD do not

appear to have any difficulty recognizing any specific emotion in comparison to their non-

anxious counterparts. Previously, McClure et al. (2003) found that children and adolescents

with bipolar disorder are more severely impaired in facial emotion recognition than those

with anxiety disorders or those without any psychiatric diagnosis. Therefore, comparing

children with SAD with other clinical groups, such as children with bipolar disorder, may

yield meaningful outcome data.

Second, emotion recognition accuracy in children with SP will need to be reassessed

with a larger sample size of varying ages because the size of the SP group was small in the

present study. Emotion recognition accuracy of children in this clinical group does not seem

to linearly commensurate with age. With a significantly higher number of child participants

with SP, however, future studies could assess the potential relationship between age and

emotion recognition accuracy in this clinical subgroup. Such studies could also examine if

other factors, such as certain demographic attributes (e.g. SES, ethnic/cultural background),

are significantly associated with emotion recognition accuracy in this group.

Third, additional neuropsychological tests could be used in future studies to measure

clinically anxious children’s general intelligence or IQ, verbal ability to express various

emotions, and personality traits. Potential interactions between these neuropsychological

Page 115: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

106

106

factors and age in predicting emotion recognition accuracy could then be investigated.

Personality traits are considered in this future direction because certain personality traits

seem to predict emotion recognition accuracy in non-anxious persons. ‘Openness to

experience’, for instance, is positively correlated with emotion recognition accuracy, whereas

‘neuroticism’ is negatively associated (Matsumoto et al., 2000). The latter finding is

particularly relevant to anxiety disorders because ‘neuroticism’ is highly correlated with the

presence of anxiety disorders in adults (Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010).

Fourth, emotion recognition accuracy of clinically anxious children needs to be

measured during anxious states to determine if their recognition is distorted in anxiety-

generating situations. Reliable methods of inducing particular moods include the use of

imagination, expressive behaviour, scripted/unscripted social situations, and music (Coan &

Allan, 2007, pp. 9), but films or film fragments are the most conventionally and reliably used

for inducing fearful mood or state anxiety in experimental conditions (Tovilovic, Novovic,

Mihic, & Jovanovic, 2009). Therefore, in future research, clinically anxious children and

non-anxious children could be shown film fragments validated in school-age children to

increase their state anxiety, and both child groups’ emotion recognition ability could then be

measured using MAAC. In the specific case of children with SP, an alternative method of

inducing state anxiety might be to have them engage in public speaking (Martin, 1990).

Such a method would be especially effective for inducing fear in children with SP whose

primary anxiety pertains to public performance, presumably yielding results that are highly

generalizable to real-life settings. Of course, anxiety induction would have to be time limited

and relatively mild to ensure ethical treatment of child research subjects.

Page 116: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

107

107

Fifth, the analysis of age effects on emotion recognition in the present study

represents an exploratory analysis since a cross-sectional research design was used.

Longitudinal studies will be required to confirm the developmental trajectory of emotion

recognition accuracy in anxious children. This method is more suitable than the cross-

sectional method for isolating the relationship between two variables without the interference

of third variables based on individual differences, allowing researchers to comprehensively

examine changes over time. Even with this method, however, some degree of sampling bias

and inconsistency is unavoidable (Bayley, 1965; Rajulton, 2001).

Lastly, there have been recent proposed revisions in the diagnostic criteria of GAD

for DSM 5 (i.e. removing the symptoms of fatigue, difficulty concentrating, irritability, and

sleep disturbance). Therefore, children diagnosed with GAD based on these new diagnostic

criteria must be re-examined in future studies.

Page 117: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

108

108

References

Abelson, J. L., & Alessi, N. E. (1992). Discussion of “Child panic revisited”. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 114-116.

Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and

neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(1), 21-62.

Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Cortical systems for the

recognition of emotion in facial expressions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16(23),

7678-7687.

Albano, A. M., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (2003). Childhood anxiety disorders. In E. J.

Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 279-329). New York,

NY: The Guilford Press.

Albano, A. M., & Silverman, W. K. (1996). Guide to the use of the Anxiety Disorders

Interview Schedule for DSM-IV-Child and Parent Versions. London: Oxford University

Press.

Aldridge, M., & Wood, J. (1989). Taking about feelings: Young children’s ability to express

emotions. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21(12), 1221-1233.

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. (2010). Practice parameter for the

assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with Posttraumatic stress disorder.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(4), 414-427.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders, (4th ed., text revision) (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American

Psychiatric Association.

Page 118: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

109

109

American Psychiatric Association. (2012). (2013, Jan 16). DSM-5 development. Retrieved

from http://www.dsm5.org/pages/default.aspx

Anderson, J. C. (1994). Epidemiological issues. In T. H. Ollendick, N. J. King, & W.

Yule (Eds.), International handbook of phobic and anxiety disorders in children and

adolescents (pp. 293–315). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Angst, J. & Vollrath, M. (1991). The natural history of anxiety disorders. Acta Psychiatrica

Scandinavica, 84, 446 -452.

Aschenbrand, S.G., Kendall, P.C., Webb, A., Safford, S.M., & Flannery-Schroeder, E.

(2003). Is childhood separation anxiety disorder a predictor of adult panic disorder and

agoraphobia? A seven-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1478-1485.

Atkinson, L., Quarrington, B., Cyr, J. J., & Atkinson, F. V. (1989). Differential classification

in school refusal. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 155(2), 191-195.

Bandura, A. (1986). The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal of

Clinical and Social Psychology, 4, 359-373.

Bandura, A. (1988). Self-efficacy conception of anxiety. Anxiety Research, 1, 77-98.

Barlow, D. H. (1988). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and

panic. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Barrett, P. M. (2000). Treatment of childhood anxiety: Developmental aspects. Clinical

Psychology Review, 20, 479-494.

Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M. R., & Rapee, R. M. (1991). Coping Koala workbook. Unpublished

manuscript, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Nathah, Australia.

Page 119: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

110

110

Battaglia, M., Bertella, S., Politi, E., Bernardeschi, L., Perna, G., Gabriele, A., & Bellodi, L.

(1995). Age of onset of panic disorder: The influences of familial liability to the disease

and of childhood separation anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152,

1362-1364.

Bauer, D. H. (1976). An exploratory study of developmental changes in children’s fears.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 69-74.

Beidas, R. S., Benjamin, C., Puleo, C., Edmunds, J., & Kendall, P. (2010). Flexible

applications of the Coping Cat Program for anxious youth. Cognitive and Behavioral

Practice, 17, 142-153.

Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1998). Shy children, phobic adults. Nature and treatment of

social phobia, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., & Morris, T. L. (1999). Psychopathology of childhood social

phobia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 643-

650.

Beidas, R.S., Benjamin, C., Puleo, C., Edmunds, J., & Kendall, P. (2010). Flexible

applications of the Coping Cat Program for anxious youth. Cognitive and Behavioral

Practice, 17, 142-153.

Bell-Dolan, D., Last, C. G., & Strauss, C. C. (1990). Symptoms of anxiety disorders in

normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

29, 759-765.

Berg, I., Marks, I., McGuire, R., & Lipsedge, M. (1974). School phobia and agoraphobia.

Psychological Medicine, 4(4), 428-434.

Page 120: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

111

111

Bernstein, G. A., & Borchardt, C. M. (1991). Anxiety disorders of childhood and

adolescence: A critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 519-532.

Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., Monk, K., Kalas, C., Clark, D. B., Ehmann, M., . . . Brent, D.

A. (2003). Fluoxetine for the treatment of childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(4), 415-423.

Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Chiappetta, L., Bridge, J., Monga, S., & Baugher, M. (1999).

Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders

(SCARED): a replication study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10), 1230-1236.

Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L., Kaufman, J., & Neer, S. M.

(1997). The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders: Scale construction

and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 545-553.

Bowers, D., Bauer, R. M., Coslett, H. B., & Heilman, K. M. (1985). Processing of faces by

patients with unilateral hemisphere lesions. I. Dissociation between judgments of facial

affect and facial identity. Brain and Cognition, 4, 258-272.

Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children

and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 244-255.

Brewin, C. R. (1996). Theoretical foundations of cognitive-behavior therapy for anxiety and

depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 33-57.

Buck, R. (1988). Nonverbal communication: Spontaneous and symbolic aspects. American

Behavioral Scientist, 31, 341-354.

Page 121: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

112

112

Buitelaar, J. K., Van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., & Van der Gaag, R. J. (1999).

Verbal memory and performance IQ predict theory of mind and emotion recognition

ability in children with autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control children.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(6), 869-881.

Camras, L. A., Grow, J. G., & Ribordy, S. C. (1983). Recognition of emotional expression

by abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12(3), 325-328.

Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions?:

Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,

205-218.

Cartwright-Hatton, S., Roberts, C., Chitsabesan, P., Fothergill, C., & Harrington, R. (2004).

Systematic review of the efficacy of cognitive behaviour therapies for childhood and

adolescent anxiety disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 421-436.

Cartwright-Hatton, S., McNicol, K., & Doubleday, E. (2006). Anxiety in a neglected

population: Prevalence of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children. Clinical

Psychology Review, 26(7), 817-833.

Cartwright-Hatton, S., Roberts, C., Chitsabesan, P., Fothergill, C., & Harrington, R. (2004).

Systematic review of the efficacy of cognitive behaviour therapies for childhood and

adolescent anxiety disorders. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 421-436.

Ciarrochi, J., Heaven, P. C. L., & Supavadeeprasit, S. (2008). The link between emotion

identification skills and socio-emotional functioning in early adolescence: A 1-year

longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 565-582.

Corcoran, K. M., Woody, S. R., & Tolin, D. F. (2008). Recognition of facial expressions in

obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(1), 56-66.

Page 122: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

113

113

Cole, D. A., Peeke, L. G., Martin, J. M., Truglio, R., & Seroczynski, A. D. (1998). A

longitudinal look at the relation between depression and anxiety in children and

adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 451-460.

Collin, L., Bindra, J., Raju, M., Gillberg, C., & Minnis, H. (2013). Facial emotion

recognition in child psychiatry: A systematic review. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 34(5), 1505-1520.

Comer, J. S., Pincus, D. P., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Generalized anxiety disorder and the

proposed associated symptoms criterion change for DSM-5 in a treatment-seeking

sample of anxious youth. Depression and Anxiety, 29, 994-1003.

Comrey, A. L. (1988). Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and

clinical psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(5), 754-761.

Connolly, S. D., Bernstein, G.A., & Work Group on Quality Issues. (2007). Practice

parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with anxiety

disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(2),

267-283.

Corsini, R. J., & Wedding, D. (2000). Current psychotherapies (6th ed.). Itasca, IL: F.E.

Peacock.

Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (1995). Epidemiology. In J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders

in children and adolescents (pp. 109-124). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and

development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence, Archives of General

Psychiatry, 60(8), 837-844.

Page 123: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

114

114

Craske, M. G., & Waters, A. (2005). Panic disorder, phobias and generalized anxiety

disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 197-226.

Da Fonseca, D., Seguier, V., Santos, A., Poinso, F., & Deruelle, C. (2009). Emotion

understanding in children with ADHD. Child Psychiatry and Human Development,

40(1), 111-121.

DeKosky, S. T., Heilman, K. M., Bowers, D., & Valenstein, E. (1980). Recognition and

discrimination of emotional faces and pictures. Brain and Language, 9, 206-214.

Demenescu, L. R., Kortekaas, R., den Boer, J. A., & Aleman, A. (2010). Impaired attribution

of emotion to facial expressions in anxiety and major depression. PLoS ONE, 5(12),

e15058.

Donovan, C. L., & Spence, S. H. (2000). Prevention of childhood anxiety disorders. Clinical

Psychology Review, 20(4), 509-531.

Derntl, B., Habel, U., Robinson, S., Windischberger, C., Kryspin-Exner, I., Gur, R. C., &

Moser, E. (2012). Culture but not gender modulates amygdala activation during explicit

emotion recognition. BMC Neuroscience, 13(1), 54.

Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F., & Baudouin, J. Y. (2007). The

development of facial emotion recognition: The role of configural information. Journal

of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(1), 14-27.

Easter, J., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Dhanani, M., Hodgdon, H., Leibenluft, E., . . . Ernst,

M. (2005). Emotion recognition deficits in pediatric anxiety disorders: implications for

amygdala research. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(4), 563-

570.

Page 124: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

115

115

Edwards, R., Manstead, A. S., & Macdonald, C. (1984). The relationship between children’s

sociometric status and ability to recognize facial expressions. European Journal of

Social Psychology, 14, 235-238.

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of

emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203-235.

Elfenbein, H. A., Marsh, A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the

recognition of emotion from facial expressions. In L. Feldman Barrett & P. Salovey

(Eds.), The wisdom of feelings: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence

(pp.37-59). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and cross cultural studies of facial expression. In P. Ekman (Ed.),

Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. New York, NY:

Academic Press.

Ezpeleta, L., Keeler, G., Erkanli, A., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2001). Epidemiology of

psychiatric disability in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 42(7), 901-914.

Feinberg, T. E., Rifkin, A., Schaffer, C., & Walker, E. (1986). Facial discrimination and

emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 43(3), 276-279.

Feldman, R. S. & Philippot, P. (1990). Age and social competence in preschoolers decoding

of facial expression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 43-54.

Field, A., Miles, J., & Field, Z. (2012). Discovering statistics using R (pp. 462-497). London:

SAGE Publications Ltd.

Page 125: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

116

116

Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., Cohen, D. (1983). Discrimination and imitation of

facial expressions by neonates. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child

Development, 16, 119-125.

Fletcher, K. E. (2003). Childhood posttraumatic stress disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A.

Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 330-371). New York, NY: The

Guilford Press.

Ford, T., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2003). The British child and adolescent mental health

survey 1999: The prevalence of DSM-IV disorders. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(10), 1203-1211.

Francis, G., Last, C. G., & Strauss, C. C. (1992). Avoidant disorder and social phobia in

children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 31, 1086-1089.

Fridlund, A. J. (1997). The new ethology of human facial expressions. In J. A. Russell & J.

M. Fernhdez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 103-129). Paris:

Cambridge University Press.

George, J. M. (2000). Emotions and leadership: The role of emotional intelligence. Human

Relations, 53, 1027-1055.

George, M. S., Huggins, T., McDermut, W., Parekh, P. I., Rubinow, D., & Post, R. M.

(1995). Abnormal facial emotion recognition in depression: Serial testing in an ultra-

rapid-cycling patient. Behavior Modification, 22(2), 192-204.

Getz, G. E., Shear, P. K., & Strakowski, S. M. (2003). Facial affect recognition deficits in

bipolar disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(4), 623-632.

Page 126: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

117

117

Ginsburg, G. S., La Greca, A. M., & Silverman, W. K. (1998). Social anxiety in children

with anxiety disorders: Relation with social and emotional functioning. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(3), 175-185.

Gross, A. L., & Ballif, B. (1991). Children’s understanding of emotion from facial

expressions and situations: A review. Developmental Review, 11, 368-398.

Guyer, A. E., McClure, E. B., Adler, A. D., Brotman, M. A., Rich, B. A., Kimes, A. S., . . .

Leibenluft, E. (2007). Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9), 863-871.

Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85,

845-857.

Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1994). “Subordination" and sensitivity to nonverbal cues:

A study of married working women. Sex Roles, 31, 149-165.

Heckelman, L. R., & Schneier, F. R. (1995). Diagnostic issues. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R.

Liebowitz, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.). Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and

treatment (pp. 261-309). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Henley, N. M. (1973). Status and sex: Some touching observations. Bulletin of Psychonomic

Society, 2, 91-93.

Hersov, L. (1972). School refusal. British Medical Journal, 3(5818), 102-104.

Hoffman, S. G., & Smits, J. A. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety

disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical

Psychiatry, 69(4), 621-632.

Hoffner, C., & Badzinski, D. M. (1989). Children’s integration of facial and situational cues

to emotion. Child Development, 60, 411–422.

Page 127: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

118

118

Hollander, E., Braun, A., & Simeon, D. (2008). Should OCD leave the anxiety disorders in

DSM-V? The case for obsessive compulsive-related disorders. Depression and Anxiety,

25, 317-329.

Hollander, E., Kim, S., & Zohar, J. (2007). OCSDs in the forthcoming DSM-V. CNS

Spectrums, 12, 320-323.

Hubbard, J. A., & Coie, J. D. (1994). Emotional determinants of social competence in

children's peer relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 1-20.

Hudson, J. L., Deveney, C., & Taylor, L. (2005). Nature, assessment, and treatment of

generalized anxiety disorder in children. Pediatric Annals, 34(2), 97-106.

Ishikawa, S., Okajima, I., Matsuoka, H. & Sakano, Y. (2007). Cognitive behavioural therapy

for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Child and Adolescent

Mental Health, 12, 164-172.

Izard, C. E., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001).

Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in

children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18-24.

Jablonka, O., Sarubbi, A., Rapp, A. M., & Albano, A. M. (2012). Cognitive behavior therapy

for the anxiety triad. In M. A. Rynn, H. B. Vidair, & J. U. Blackford (Eds.). Anxiety

disorders, an issue of child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America (pp.

541-553). Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Health Sciences.

Johnston, P. J., Stojanov, W., Devir, H., & Schall, U. (2005). Functional MRI of facial

emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenia and their electrophysiological correlates.

European Journal of Neuroscience, 22(5), 1221-1232.

Jurbergs, N. (2005). Separation anxiety disorder. Pediatric Annals, 34(2), 108-15.

Page 128: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

119

119

Keane, J., Calder, A. J., Hodges, J. R., & Young, A. W. (2002). Face and emotion processing

in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia, 40(6), 655-665.

Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in children: Results of a randomized

clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 100-110.

Kendall, P. C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., Panichelli-Mindel, S. M., Southam-Gerow,

M., Henin, A., & Warman, M. (1997). Therapy for youths with anxiety disorders: A

second randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(3),

366-380.

Kendall, P. C., Furr, J. M., & Podell, J. L. (2009). Child-focused treatment of anxiety. In J. R.

Weisz, & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and

adolescents, (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Kendall, P., Gosch, E., Furr, J., & Sood, E. (2008). Flexibility within fidelity. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(9), 983-993.

Kendall, P. C., & Hedtke, K. (2006a). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxious children:

Therapist manual (3rd ed.). Ardmore, PA: Workbook Publishing. Retrieved March 15,

2012, from www.workbookpublishing.com

Kendall, P. C., & Hedtke, K. (2006b). The Coping Cat Workbook (2nd ed.). Ardmore, PA:

Workbook Publishing. Retrieved March 15, 2012, from www.workbookpublishing.com

Kendall, P. C., & Pimentel, S. S. (2003) On the physiological symptom constellation in

youth with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 211-

221.

Page 129: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

120

120

Kenny, D. A., Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., Livi, S., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). The statistical

analysis of data from small groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1),

126-137.

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005).

Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national

comorbidity survey replication. The Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 602.

Kessler, H., Schwarze, M., Filipic, S., Traue, H.C., von Wietersheim, J. (2006). Alexithymia

and facial emotion recognition in patients with eating disorders. International Journal of

Eating Disorders, 39, 245-251.

Kessler, H., Roth, J., von Wietersheim, J., Deighton, R. M., & Traue, H. C. (2007). Emotion

recognition patterns in patients with panic disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 24(3), 223-

226.

Kirouac, G., & Dore, F. Y. (1985). Accuracy of the judgment of facial expression of

emotions as

a function of sex and level of education. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 3-7.

Klein, D. F., Mannuzza, S., Chapman, T., & Fyer, A. J. (1992). Child panic revisited.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(1), 112-116.

Klein, R. G., & Pine, D. S. (2002). Anxiety disorders. In M. Rutter & E. Taylor (Eds.) Child

and adolescent psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 486-509). Oxford, UK: Blackwell

Kohler, C. G., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C. M., Siegel, S. J., Kanes, S. J., . . .

Gur, R. C. (2003). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: Intensity effects and

error pattern. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(10), 1768-1774.

Page 130: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

121

121

Koran, L. (2000). Quality of life in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of

North America, 23(3), 509-517.

Kornreich, C., Philippot, P., Foisy, M. L., Blairy, S., Raynaud, E., Dan, B., . . . Verbanck, P.

(2002). Impaired emotional facial expression is associated with interpersonal problems

in alcoholism. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37, 394-400.

Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1977). An empirical-clinical approach toward a definition of

childhood depression. In J. G. Schulterbrandt & A. Raskin (Eds.), Depression in

childhood: Diagnosis, treatment, and conceptual models (pp. 1-25). New York, NY:

Raven Press.

Kovacs, M., Gatsonis, C., Paulauskas, S. L., & Richards, C. (1989). Depressive disorders in

childhood. IV. A longitudinal study of comorbidity with and risk for anxiety disorders.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(9), 776-782.

Kucharska-Pietura, K., Nikolaou, V., Masiak, M., & Treasure, J. (2004). The recognition of

emotion in the faces and voice of anorexia nervosa. The International Journal of Eating

Disorders, 35(1), 42-47.

Kushner, M. G., Sher, K. J., & Beitman, B. D. (1990). The relation between alcohol

problems and the anxiety disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 685-695.

Kuusikko, S., Haapsamo, H., Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Hurtig, T., Mattila, M. L., Ebeling,

H., . . . Moilanen, I. (2009). Emotion recognition in children and adolescents with

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 39(6), 938-

945.

La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer

relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(2), 83-94.

Page 131: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

122

122

Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., Reidel, R., Weldon, V., Kazniak, A., & Schwartz, G. E. (1996).

Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia. Psychosomatic

Medicine, 58(3), 203-210.

Lang, P. (1968). Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. In J.

Shlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy. Washington DC: American Psychological

Association.

Lapouse, R., & Monk, M. A. (1964). Fears and worries in a representative sample of children.

American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 29, 803-818.

Last, C. G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A. E. (1992). DSM-III-R anxiety disorders in

children: Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1070-1076.

Last, C. G., Philips, J. E., & Statfield, A. (1987). Childhood anxiety disorders in mothers and

their children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 18, 103-112.

Last, C. G., & Strauss, C. C. (1990). School refusal in anxiety-disordered children and

adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(1),

31-35.

Last, C. G., Strauss, C. C., & Francis, G. (1987). Comorbidity among childhood anxiety

disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 726-730.

Lee, T. C., Dupuis, A., Jones, E., Guberman, C., Herbert, M., & Manassis, K. (2013). Effects

of age and subtype on anxious children’s emotional recognition: Implications for

cognitive-behavioural therapy. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, in press.

Lembke, A., & Ketter, T. A. (2002). Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. The

American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(2), 302-304.

Page 132: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

123

123

Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, M., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Depression

biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research, 128,123-133.

Lewinsohn, P. M., Holm-Denoma, J. M., Small, J. W., Seeley, J. R., & Joiner, T. E., Jr.

(2008). Separation anxiety disorder in childhood as a risk factor for future mental illness.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(5), 548-555.

Ley, R. G., & Bryden, M. P. (1979). Hemispheric differences in processing emotion and

faces. Brain Language 7, 127–138.

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., & Straus, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence, personality, and the

perceived quality of social relationships. Personality and Individual Differences,

35, 641-658.

Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, Y. R., Ortegon, J., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K.

A., & Gibbs, M. C. (1997). Emotion recognition in autism: Verbal and nonverbal

information. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 579-593.

Macklem, G. L. (2011). Evidence-based school mental health services: Affect education,

emotion regulation training, and cognitive behavioral therapy. New York: Springer.

Magnusson, D. (1985). Situational factors in research in stress and anxiety: Sex and age

differences. In P. B. Defares (Ed.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 9, pp. 69-78). Washington,

DC: Hemisphere.

Manassis, K., Mendlowitz, S., Dupuis, A., Kreindler, D., Lumsden, C., Monga, S., &

Guberman, C. (2013). Mood assessment via animated characters: An instrument to

access and evaluate emotions in young children. Open Journal of Psychiatry, 3, 149-157.

Manassis, K., Mendlowitz, S., Kreindler, D., Lumsden, C., Sharpe, J., Simon, M.D., . . .

Adler-Nevo, G. (2009). Mood assessment via animated characters: A novel instrument

Page 133: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

124

124

to evaluate feelings in young children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child

and Adolescent Psychology, 38(3), 103-109.

Manassis, K., & Young, A. (2000). Perception of emotions in anxious and learning disabled

children. Depression and Anxiety, 12(4), 209-216.

Mandal, M. K., Jain, A., Haque-Nizamie, S., Weiss, U., & Schneider, F. (1999). Generality

and specificity of emotion-recognition deficit in schizophrenic patients with positive and

negative symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 87(1), 39-46.

Mandal, M. K., & Palchoudhury, S. (1985). Perceptual skill in decoding facial affect.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 96-98.

March, J. S., & Friesen, K. M. (1998). OCD in children and adolescents: A cognitive-

behavioral treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press.

March, J. S., Parker, D. A., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., & Conners, C. K. (1997). The

Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and

validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 554-

565.

Marks, I. (1977). Clinical phenomenon in the search of laboratory models. In Jack D. Maser

and Martin E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Experimental models. San

Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company, 174-213.

Marnane, C., & Silove, D. (2013). DSM-5 allows separation anxiety disorder to grow up.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 47(1), 12-15.

Masi, G., Mucci, M., & Millepiedi, S. (2001). Separation anxiety disorder in children and

adolescents: epidemiology, diagnosis and management. CNS Drugs, 15(2), 93-104.

Page 134: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

125

125

Masi, G., Mucci, M., Favilla, L., Romano, R., and Poli, P. (1999). Symptomatology and

comorbidity of generalized anxiety disorder in children and adolescents. Comprehensive

Psychiatry, 40, 210-215.

McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression

processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological

Bulletin, 126(3), 424-453.

McClure, E. B., Pope, K., Hoberman, A. J., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2003). Facial

expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders. The American

Journal of Psychiatry, 160(6), 1172-1174.

McLeod, J. D. (1994). Anxiety disorders and marital quality. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 103(4), 767-776.

Melfsen S, Florin I. (2002). Do socially anxious children show deficits in classifying facial

expressions of emotions? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26(2), 109–126.

Mueser, K. T., Doonan, R., Penn, D. L., Blanchard, J. J., Bellack, A. S., Nishith, P., &

Deleon, J. (1996). Emotion recognition and social competence in chronic

schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 271-275.

Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Sermon, A., & Zwakhalen, S. (1998). Worry in

normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

37, 703-710.

Muris, P., Schmidt, H., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The structure of specific phobia

symptoms among children and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(9),

863-868.

Page 135: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

126

126

Nakamura, K., Kawashima, R., Sato, N., Nakamura, A., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., . . . Zilles, K.

(2000). Functional delineation of the human occipito-temporal areas related to face and

scene processing. Brain, 123, 1903-1912.

Namiki, C., Hirao, K., Yamada, M., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, T., & Murai, T.

(2007). Impaired facial emotion recognition and reduced amygdalar volume in

schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 156(1), 23-32.

Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2011). Preschoolers’ use of dynamic facial, bodily, and vocal

cues to emotion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(1), 52-61.

Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2012). Do children recognize dynamic emotional expressions

better than static ones?. Journal of Vision, 12(9), 965-965.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Abnormal psychology (4th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Nowicki, S., & Carton, J. (1994). The measurement of emotional intensity from facial

expressions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133(5), 749-750.

Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal

communication of affect. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9-36.

Ohman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology,

evolutionary perspectives, and information processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M.

Haviland (Eds), Handbook of emotions (pp. 511-536). New York: Guilford Press.

Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Yule, W. (1994). International handbook of phobic and

anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Ollendick, T. H., & Schroeder, C. S. (2003). Encyclopedia of clinical child and pediatric

psychology (pp. 34-35). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Page 136: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

127

127

Parker, H. A., McNally, R. J., Nakayama, K., & Wilhelm, S. (2004). No disgust recognition

in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental

Psychiatry, 35(2), 183-192.

Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces:

Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36(5),

678-688.

Pollak, S. D., Messner, M., Kistler, D. J., & Cohn, J. F. (2008). Development of perceptual

expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition, 110(2), 242-247.

Rachman, S. J., & Shafran, R. (1998). Cognitive and behavioral features of obsessive-

compulsive disorder. In R. P. Swinson, M. M. Antony, S. Rachman, & M. A. Richter

(Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 51-78).

New York: The Guilford Press.

Rasmussen, S. A., & Eisen, J. L. (1990). Epidemiology of obsessive compulsive disorder.

Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51(2), 10-14.

Rector, N. A., Daros, A. R., Bradbury, C. L., & Richter, M. A. (2012). Disgust recognition in

obsessive-compulsive disorder: Diagnostic comparisons and posttreatment effects.

Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57(3), 177-183.

Regier, D. A., Narrow, W. E., Clarke, D. E., Kraemer, H. C., Kuramoto, S. J., Kuhl, E. A., &

Kupfer, D. J. (2012). DSM-5 field trials in the United States and Canada, Part II: Test-

retest reliability of selected categorical diagnoses. The American Journal of Psychiatry,

170(1), 59-70.

Rotter, N. G., & Rotter, G. S. (1988). Sex differences in the encoding and decoding of negative

facial emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 139-148.

Page 137: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

128

128

Sachs, G., Steger-Wuchse, D., Kryspin-Exner, I., Gur, R. C., & Katschnig, H. (2004). Facial

recognition deficits and cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 68(1), 27-

35.

Scapillato, D., & Mendlowitz, S. (1993). Coping Bear Workbook. Unpublished manuscript,

Department of Psychiatry, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Schulz, K. F., & Grimes, D. A. (2002). Unequal group sizes in randomised trials: Guarding

against guessing. Lancet, 359(9310), 966-970.

Shannon, M. P., Lonigan, C. J., Finch, A. J., & Taylor, C. M. (1994). Children exposed to

disaster: I. epidemiology of post-traumatic symptoms and symptom profiles. Journal of

the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(1), 80-93.

Shear, K., Jin, R., Ruscio, A. M., Walters, E. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2006). Prevalence and

correlates of estimated DSM-IV child and adult separation anxiety disorder in the

national comorbidity survey replication. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6),

1074-1083.

Sheldon, B. (1995). Cognitive-behavioural therapy: Research, practice and philosophy.

London: Routledge.

Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). The Anxiety disorders interview schedule for

children for DSM-IV: Child and parent versions. New York, NY: Psychological

Corporation.

Silverman, W. K., La Greca, A. M., & Wasserstein, S. (1995). What do children worry about?

Worries and their relation to anxiety. Child Development, 66(3), 671-686.

Page 138: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

129

129

Silverman, W. K., Pina, A. A., & Viswesvaran, C. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial

treatments for phobic and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(1), 105-130.

Simonian, S. J., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Berkes, J. L., & Long, J. H. (2001).

Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents diagnosed with social phobia.

Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 32(2), 137-145.

Singh, S. D., Ellis, C. R., Winton, A. S., Singh, N. N., Leung, J. P., & Oswald. D. P. (1998).

Recognition of facial expressions of emotion by children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Modification, 22(2), 128-142.

Smucker, M. R., Craighead, W. E., Craighead, L. W., & Green, B. J. (1986). Normative and

reliability data for the Children's Depression Inventory. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology. 14(1), 25-40

Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). A preliminary study of the emotion

understanding of youths referred for treatment of anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical

Child Psychology, 29(3), 319-327.

Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and understanding

implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22,

189-222.

Spielberger, C. D., Edwards, C. F., Lushene, R. E., Montuori, K., & Platzak, D. (1973). The

state-trait anxiety inventory for children (preliminary manual). Palo Alto: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Page 139: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

130

130

Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Pundt, I., Sprengelmeyer, A., Calder, A. J., Berrios,

G., . . . Przuntek, H. (1997). Disgust implicated in obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Proceedings of Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264(1389), 1767-1773.

Steele, H., Steele, M., & Croft, C. (2008). Early attachment predicts emotion recognition at 6

and 11 years old. Attachment & Human Development, 10(4), 379–393.

Stein, D. J., Fineberg, N. A., Bienvenu, O. J., Denys, D., Lochner, C., Nestadt, G., . . .

Phillips, K. A. (2010). Should OCD by classified as an anxiety disorder in DSM-V?,

Depression and Anxiety, 27, 495-506.

Strauss, C. C., Lease, C. A., Kazdin, A. E., Dulcan, M. K., & Last, C. G. (1989).

Multimethod assessment of the social competence of children with anxiety

disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 184-189.

Surcinelli, P., Codispoti, M., Montebarocci, O., Rossi, N., & Baldaro, B. (2006). Facial

emotion recognition in trait anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 110–117.

Suguwara, M., Mukai, T., Kitamura, T., Toda, M. A., Shima, S., Tomoda, A., . . . Ando, A.

(1999). Psychiatric disorders among Japanese children. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(4), 444-452.

Suveg, C. & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders.

Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(4), 750-759.

Suveg, C., Kendall, P. C., Comer, J. S., & Robin, J. (2006). Emotion-focused cognitive-

behavioral therapy for anxious youth: A multiple-baseline evaluation. Journal of

Contemporary Psychotherapy, 36(2), 77-85.

Page 140: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

131

131

Suveg, C., Sood, E., Comer, J.S., Kendall, P.C. (2009). Changes in emotion regulation

following cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxious youth. Journal of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology, 38, 390-401.

Swedo, S. E., Rapoport, J. L., Leonard, H., Lenane, M., & Cheslow, D. (1989). Obsessive-

compulsive disorder in children and adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(4),

335-341.

Toro, J., Cervera, M., Osejo, E., & Salamero, M. (1992). Obsessive-compulsive disorder in

childhood and adolescence: A clinical study. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 33(6), 1025-1037.

Valleni-Basile, L. A., Garrison, C. Z., Jackson, K. L., & Waller, J. L. (1994). Frequency of

obsessive-compulsive disorder in a community sample of young adolescents. Journal of

the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 782-791.

Van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., & Farvolden, P. (2003). The impact of anxiety disorders

on educational achievement. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17(5), 561-571.

Van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., Patterson, B., & Boyle, M. H. (2008). Post-traumatic stress

disorder in Canada. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 14(3), 171-181.

Vasey, M. W. (1993). Development and cognition in childhood anxiety: The example of

worry. Advances in Clinical Child Psychology, 15, 1-39.

Vasey, M. W., Crnic, K. A., & Carter, W. G. (1994). Worry in childhood: A developmental

perspective. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 529-549.

Velting, O. N., Setzer, N. J., & Albano, A. M. (2004). Update on and advances in assessment

and Cognitive–Behavioral treatment of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 35(1), 42-54.

Page 141: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

132

132

Wagner, A. P. (2002). Worried no more: Help and hope for anxious children (pp. 16-23).

Rochester, NY: Lighthouse Press.

Wagner, H. L., MacDonald, C. J., & Manstead. A. S. (1986). Communication of individual

emotions by spontaneous facial expressions. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 50, 737-743.

Walkup, J. T., Albano, A. M., Piacentini, J., Birmaher, B., Compton, S. N., Sherrill, J. T., . . .

Kendall, P. C. (2008). Cognitive behavioral therapy, sertraline, or a combination in

childhood anxiety. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(26), 2753-2766.

Wallace, C. J. (1984). Community and interpersonal functioning in the course of

schizophrenie disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(2), 233-257.

Wehrle, T., Kaiser, S., Schmidt, S. & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Studying the dynamics of

emotional expression using synthesized facial muscle movements. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (1), 105-119.

Wilner, A., Reich, T., Robins, I., Fishman, R., & van Doren, T. (1976). Obsessive-

compulsive neurosis. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 17, 527-539.

Wood, J. J. (2006). Effect of anxiety reduction on children’s school performance and social

adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 42(2), 345-349.

Woodward, L. J. & Fergusson, D. M. (2001). Life course outcomes of young people with

anxiety disorders in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1086–1093.

Yehuda, R., Halligan, S. L., & Bierer, R. (2001). Childhood trauma and risk for PTSD:

Relationship to intergenerational effects of trauma, parental PTSD, and cortisol

excretion. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 733-753.

Page 142: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

133

133

Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness regulation: Predictions to

internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology, 31, 393-398.

Zitrin, C. M., & Ross, D. C. (1988). Early separation anxiety and adult agoraphobia. Journal

of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 621-625.

Page 143: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

134

134

Appendix A

Mood Assessment via Animated Characters (MAAC)

Mood Assessment via Animated Characters (MAAC) is a computerized self-report

instrument designed for clinicians and researchers to effectively communicate about emotion

in young children with anxiety disorders. MAAC provides various types of dynamic

emotion cues (facial cues, bodily cues, and situational cues) through a teenage female

animated character (“Teena”) in order to facilitate young children’s discussion of 16 types of

feeling states (calm/relaxed, bored, tired, surprised, sad, guilty, ashamed, angry, irritable,

jealous, scared, nervous, disgusted, happy, elated, and proud/pleased). This instrument was

created by a team of clinicians who are experienced in the assessment and treatment of

childhood anxiety disorders and researchers who have expertise in the clinical application of

computer devices and computer animation (Manassis et al., 2009).

MAAC assesses young children’s ability to express and identify a range of both

simple and complex (social) emotions of positive, negative, fearful and neutral valence. This

instrument has been validated in children, 4-10 years of age, with and without anxiety

disorders. The initial validation of MAAC included children younger than 8 because well-

validated assessment instruments had been largely missing in this age group.

In terms of context in which MAAC is designed to be used, this instrument may be

used in the clinic for screening for internalizing symptoms or anxious feelings in young

anxious children who may not have fully developed the cognitive ability to verbally describe

or label their feelings. However, the instrument can also be used in non-clinical settings (e.g.

home, school) to discuss everyday feelings of young children.

Administration, coding and scoring.

Page 144: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

135

135

The administration of MAAC takes about on average of 15 minutes. During the

procedure, children are asked to express their current feelings by comparing them to those

expressed by Teena, and asked to rate the intensity of these feelings on the Likert scale

between 0 (not at all) and 5 (being an exact match) by selecting/tapping on the screen.

Then, the child and the interviewer together visit each of 16 emotions in order on

MAAC screen, top to bottom and left to right, to view each of these emotion-specific

animations. Upon viewing each clip, the child is asked to identify Teena’s feeling.

Responses may be tape-recorded and scored based on how accurately the child identified

each feeling state of Teena: 0 (incorrect), 1 (close to correct), 2 (correct).

To minimize the effect of verbal ability on emotion identification accuracy, any

immature form of verbal responses, but with the correct emotional tenor, ought to be

carefully considered. For example, young children without the verbal ability to label

‘disgusted’ may identify the emotion in a simpler term, such as ‘yucky’, and such response

may be given a score of two. For this reason, it is highly recommended that multiple raters

blindly score responses on MAAC and report the inter-rater agreement on scoring.

Psychometric properties.

Face validity: Factors on MAAC (positive, negative, fearful, and neutral groups)

contain emotions of similar valence, suggesting face validity (Manassis et al., 2013).

Content validity: The content validity of each item has not been evaluated in relation

to the symptoms content of anxiety disorders.

Construct validity (convergent validity, discriminant validity): The convergent

validity has shown that children’s ratings of current feelings show a significant correlation

with scores on the STAIC, whereas ratings of feelings for the past two weeks significantly

Page 145: Emotion Recognition in Children with Anxiety Disorders ......vi Recognition in Young Children with Anxiety Disorders o Other Limitations of Previous Studies Merits of the Present Study

136

136

correlate with some of the measures of trait anxiety, such as KFQ, MASC, and SCARED.

These correlations were reported in the expected direction such that ratings on negative

emotions on MAAC predicted higher ratings on the anxiety measures of STAIC and

SCARED, whereas those on positive emotions predicted lower ratings on STAIC and MASC.

In terms of the discriminant validity for differentiating the anxiety and non-anxiety groups

based on MAAC ratings, clinically anxious children identified themselves to be significantly

less positive and less calm than non-anxious children (Manassis et al., 2013).

Internal consistency: Cronbach’s alpha values have been reported in four emotion

factors of positive, negative, fearful, and neutral emotions. These values were 0.83, 0.76,

0.71, and 0.55, respectively.

Inter-rater reliability: Inter-rater reliability is excellent (kappa = 0.92).

Test-retest reliability and criterion validity await further investigation.