emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation...

14
1 Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation should be aware of Candidate: Audrius Rutkauskas Module Title: Patterns of Action Dissertation Module Tutor: Prof. David Clarke Degree: BSc (Hons) Psychology Year: 2013

Upload: others

Post on 15-Aug-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

1

Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation should

be aware of

Candidate: Audrius Rutkauskas

Module Title: Patterns of Action Dissertation

Module Tutor: Prof. David Clarke

Degree: BSc (Hons) Psychology

Year: 2013

Page 2: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

2

Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation should be aware

of

1. Introduction

Human beings spend a substantial amount of time in their workplace, thus there is no

surprise they exhibit different emotions in organisational settings: we feel happy if our

project is successful; we are angry or frustrated if our co-workers have let us down or we

feel anxious due to the upcoming deadline. These emotions can have either a positive or a

negative effect on the later interactions with our colleagues or clients (Ashkanasy, Hartel

and Daus, 2002). Such anecdotal examples are an important step to acknowledge that

emotions play a substantial role while humans are at work. However, one of the most

interesting questions here is: in what ways do emotions affect organisations? According to

Forgas and George (2001) such an inquiry was largely ignored due to the fact that for

decades psychology was dominated by behaviourist paradigm and cognitive approach which

did not pay sufficient amount of attention to affect - it was believed that emotions and

cognition were not interdependent. Moreover, at that time a widely accepted Expectancy

Theory (Vroom, 1964) presented a view of employees as being calculative, logical

information processors. Such statements completely opposed to the idea that affect can

have an effect in organisations.

It is surprising and yet worrying for how long both science and organisations neglected the

importance of emotions to human behaviour. Fortunately, some psychologists opposed to

such neglect. Bower’s (1981) model of human thinking takes into account emotions;

researcher showed that affect plays a major role in cognition. Also, Bechara, Damasio and

Damasio (2000) provided empirical evidence arguing that emotions are essential for normal

and effective decision-making processes. Similar studies have concluded that affect is

extremely important to be taken into account while managing complex organisational

situations. Experimental findings influenced the change in perception on the significance

that emotions play in everyday life; it prompted organisational psychologists to take a closer

look at affect in work environment. For example, Weiss (2002) concluded that affective

Page 3: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

3

experiences at work are the best predictors of job satisfaction and this in turn leads to

better productivity. There are numerous studies now showing various effects of emotions in

organisational environment. Without any doubt, this calls organisations to take serious

actions in order to increase the bottom line of organisational efficiency.

The aim of the present essay is to present some empirical evidence which suggest that

affect plays a crucial role in shaping employees’ behaviours. Two main arguments will be

presented to support the notion. Firstly, the essay will analyze mood effects in

organisations. It will present a theoretical model which analyzes the extent to which our

judgements can be influenced by different moods. Moreover, it will present some empirical

evidence supporting the proposed theoretical framework. Lastly, the present essay will

move from the effects of mood to a specific emotion of joy and its relation to motivation. It

will invite organisations to pay more attention to emotion-based intrinsic motivation in

order to improve the bottom line performance.

2. Mood-congruent effects to cognition

2.1. Effect of mood on decision-making and memory

The notion that moods have a profound effect on human beings is not a new one.

Psychological literature covered a wide range of phenomenon showing the influence of

affect to cognition. For example, Raghunathan’s and Pham’s (1999) study showed that

negative mood (sadness) increases risky preferences. Participants had to choose between

two gambling options (or between two jobs). The task was designed in a way that it had a

trade-off between risk and reward. Risk preferences were highest for participants induced

to feel sad, followed by controls and anxious individuals were risk averse. Also, Insen,

Daubman and Nowicki (1987) showed that positive mood leads to being more risk averse in

increased risk situations. Furthermore, Bower (1984) used a hypnotic suggestion technique

in participants who were required to learn lists of unrelated words and their recall was

tested whilst in the same or opposite mood; it was found that participants who learned a list

of words whilst in a happy mood, showed higher levels of retention when recalling whilst in

a happy mood compared to a sad mood - the opposite was demonstrated for those who

Page 4: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

4

learnt a list of words whilst in a sad mood. Following such observed effects, Bower (1991)

suggested that the possible mechanism behind the phenomenon was a network model. He

argued that mood states might be linked to different mental representations within a single

associative network. If this assumption was correct, Bower and Forgas (2001) postulated

that affect could influence the usage of mood-congruent memories or associations that

people rely on when making judgements or decisions. This has important practical

implications for organisational behaviour. For example, whenever we make decisions we

usually consider past experiences (memories) as references in order to have more

information when evaluating possible outcomes. In this case, difficult decisions might be

highly influenced by the type of memories we recall which, as shown above, depend on

mood congruency. If we are happy at the time we make a decision, we are more likely to

recall happy memories associated with similar situations and use them as reference points.

Moreover, it is argued that mood can influence behaviour in a different fashion. Depending

on an employee’s mood, different situations might be interpreted in a mood-congruent

way. Forgas (1995) proposed a theoretical approach which accounts for the affective

influences over mood-related information processing strategies that employees might use –

the Affect Infusion Model.

2.2. Affect Infusion model (AIM)

According to Forgas et al. (2001), the model refers to “the process whereby affectively

loaded information exerts and influence on, and becomes incorporated into, a person’s

cognitive and behavioural processes, entering into their constructive deliberations and

eventually coloring the outcome in a mood-congruent direction” (p. 9). This model analyzes

the degree to which mood can influence our judgement and subsequent behaviour. For

example, the reaction of getting a negative feedback from your boss (and behaviour

afterwards) is substantially influenced by one’s mood prior to the event. Forgas (1995)

argued that mood infusion varies depending on the complexity of the situation. If an

encountered situation is complex or ambiguous it is more likely that a subject will be

influenced by their mood compared to “simple” encounters. Researchers argue that affect

infusion takes place due to the fact that planning and performing complicated

Page 5: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

5

organisational behaviours makes subjects to search for pre-existing knowledge base and

associations to make sense of a situation and produce a response (Forgas et al., 2001). It

was proposed that there are four processing strategies - each of them representing a

different mood infusion potential: 1) the direct access of a pre-existing evaluation; 2)

motivated processing of a pre-existing goal 3) heuristic constructive processing and 4) a

substantive generative processing strategy. One of the biggest advantages of Affect

Infusion model is its capability to explain instances when mood does affect judgement and

behaviour and when it does not. The first two strategies explain when infusion does not

occur; the third – when infusion sometimes occurs and the fourth strategy, one of most

important one in organisational settings, explains when mood infusion does occur (Forgas,

1996).

Direct access strategy involves using already pre-computed responses therefore extensive

processing is not required. Also, it does not rely on open memory search so mood does not

affect such processing. According to Quellette and Wood (1988) employees rely on direct

access strategy in organisational routines and well-rehearsed processes. Another example

when mood infusion does not usually occur is motivated processing. The usage of this

strategy is defined to take presence when subject takes on a highly targeted and selective

thinking style which does not rely on primed affective information. For example, if employee

wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational goal will not

allow mood to influence his behaviour. Moreover, Forgas et al. (2001) argued that

employees sometimes use heuristic processing, which is adopted when the task is simple

and of a low relevance to the subject. In this case of thinking individuals are aiming to

produce a response without putting much effort – they use simplifications available at hand,

such as their own mood. They might produce a response according to how they feel at that

particular moment. This type of thinking is one of the ways when affect is infused.

The most interesting and, I would argue, relevant to organisations is a processing strategy

referred to as substantive processing. In this case, researchers define substantive as “those

cognitive tasks which involves active elaboration and transformation of the available

stimulus information, require the activation and use of previous knowledge structures, and

result in the creation of a new knowledge from the combination of stored information and

new stimulus details “ (Forgas et al., 2001, p.9). This type of processing is highly affected by

Page 6: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

6

mood infusions as people rely on previously stored associative ideas and memories. The

next paragraph is devoted to show that AIM is not based only on assumptions, but in fact is

highly supported by empirical evidence.

2.3. Experimental evidence supporting AIM

As mentioned above, AIM predicts that different kinds of processing strategies people use

affect levels of mood infusion. Substantive processing is thought to produce the biggest

effects on processing. To test this, Forgas, Bower and Krantz (1984) asked happy and sad

participants to judge social interactions depicted in a video. As social interactions usually

can be ambiguous, this would require participants to employ a substantive processing.

Researchers found out that participants interpreted interactions differently depending on

their mood: participants viewed encounters as being skilled and poised whilst being in a

happy mood while the same participants judged the exact interactions to be awkward and

unskilled while being in a negative mood. Such mood-based biases have profound

implications in organisational settings, especially when interviewing a potential employee

for a position. A happy interviewer would evaluate the interaction in a more positive way

compared to a sad interviewer. Usually organisations do not have a capacity to interview all

the candidates at the same day and as moods are very sensitive to change at a short period

of time this can lead to a profound decision-making bias when selecting employees. Strictly

speaking, even though a person has the correct credentials to be successful at the job,

he/she might not get employed due to his interviewer’s mood. Mood-congruent bias is also

present when recruitment managers read CVs. Forgas (1994) showed that happy individuals

make more positive inferences when reading personal information compared to sad people.

As CVs contain different career achievements and previous job roles, this can substantially

influence managers’ choices. Companies should be aware of such phenomenon and try to

reduce the likelihood of mood-congruent bias. Some studies (e.g. Berkowitz, 2000) have

shown that informing individuals of the effects mood can have on people’s decisions help to

prevent affect infusions.

Page 7: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

7

Furthermore, communication between individuals (both verbal and written) in organisations

is majorly affected by moods also. As nowadays organisations tend to have quite a complex

structure, effective communication between employees and departments is crucial. Forgas

(1999) set out to test whether individuals communicate in different manner while being in

different mood states. Researcher tested participants under different mood conditions and

exposed subjects to either a simple and unambiguous or difficult and ambiguous

interpersonal situation. Results confirmed AIM predictions. The observed effects of mood

were substantially greater in an ambiguous situation compared to simple one. This

happened as participants had to involve themselves in a more substantive thinking which

led to affect infused responses. Also, happy subjects preferred more direct, less polite and

less elaborate responses while sad individuals produced more indirect, polite and elaborate

responses. Again, organisations should make sure that their employees are aware of such

mood effects. While communicating with clients, happy people might be too direct and

impolite thus damaging the future relationship.

Moreover, Forgas (1998) demonstrated that affect plays a significant role in negotiations.

Researchers tested participants in happy, neutral and sad moods who took part in a

negotiation task. Experimental findings suggested that positive mood leads to choosing

more ambitious goals, setting higher expectations and choosing more cooperative strategies

in the task compared to both neutral and sad subjects. George et al. (1996) backed up such

experimental findings and even showed that happy participants produced better

negotiation outcomes. These results suggest that when bargaining and forming new

relationships between companies, sending happy individuals to negotiate can result in

negotiating better terms.

To conclude, AIM provides a lot of valuable insight for organisations. Firstly the model

outlines situations when mood does not affect employees; it also presents cases when

mood has a significant effect on judgement and behaviour. It proposes that the more

complex and ambiguous the encounter is, the more likely people will process information in

a substantive way. In these situations people have a tendency to be influenced by their

moods. It can be stated that managers and individuals within higher ranks in organisation

Page 8: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

8

are affected by affect infusion the most. They often make difficult decisions and encounter

situations with high complexity and uncertainty every day. Organisations which aim to

eliminate or decrease the level of mood-congruent biases should inform their employees

about possible effects of positive and negative mood. Furthermore, besides the importance

of positive/negative moods to AIM, researchers have shown that positive mood improves

creativity skills (Insen & Daubman, 1984), increases organisational prosocial behaviour

(Williams & Shiaw, 1999) and also improves performance (Wright & Staw, 1999). The

relationship between being happy and being a productive worker is discussed below.

3. Happy-productive hypothesis

Happy-productive hypothesis simply refers to the positive relationship between positive

affect and productivity. The idea that satisfied and happy workers are statistically more

productive than others is quite widely accepted not only to occupational psychologists but

also to management theorists (e.g. McGregor, 1960). Many empirical findings are consistent

with such a notion and Weiss and Cropanzano (1996) even named such a relationship as a

“Holy Grail” of organisational behaviour research. It is believed that the well-being of

individual leads to higher performance at an individual-level which in turns leads to an

increase of an overall organisational performance (Taris & Schruers, 2009). Moreover, often

happiness in work environment is measured by job satisfaction - Judge and colleagues

(2001) found out that job satisfaction is moderately positively related to individual-level

performance. Moreover, Zelenski, Murphy and Jenkins (2008) backed up such results and

showed that positive affect is an especially significant contributor to productivity. On the

other hand, some experimental findings show scepticism about the relationship between

positive affect and productivity. Meta-analysis by Vroom (1964, as cited in Taris et al., 2009)

concluded that the correlation was only .14 while Iaffaldano and Muchinsky (1995, as cited

in Taris et al., 2009) obtained a correlation of .17. These studies infer that there might be

other variables involved between positive affect and performance.

Different researchers have proposed various possible explanations for the happy-productive

relationship. For example, Fredrickson (2001) argued that positive emotions broaden and

build skills and social bonds. Researcher believed that because people, who are in positive

Page 9: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

9

mood, are more cooperative and helpful therefore they are more likely to improve

productivity by working together. On the other hand, as positive emotions were linked to

enhanced problem-solving skills, Madjar (2002) suggested that such a link leads to a better

performance as employees are more capable of solving complex organisational problems.

Furthermore, according to Adams (1963, as cited in Taris et al., 2009), satisfied employees

feel obliged to put their efforts in order to achieve higher results as employers made them

feel happy at their workplace. He proposed equity theory which argues that when inequity

between two “actors” occurs, one of the actors tries to reach the equilibrium by investing

either less or more into the relationship. In this case, employee puts more effort to reach

the equity between him and his employer. Lastly, Taris et al. 2009 suggested that there

might be a link between affective phenomena and motivation. It was argued that high levels

of job satisfaction would lead to high levels of motivation which would influence individuals

to perform better.

The notion that positive emotions have an impact on human motivation has been adopted

by different motivational theories. In particular, Self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci,

2001) argues that often people engage in activities with a sole purpose of achieving

enjoyment. This notion is extremely important and it needs to be taken into account in

organisational settings.

4. Emotion-based motivation – Self-determination theory

Before the introduction of the Self-determination theory (SDT), human motivation was

mostly viewed from a cognitive perspective: it was believed that people behave in particular

ways in order to reach a desired goal e.g. people go to work (action) because they want to

get paid (goal). However, Deci and Ryan (1991) proposed a completely different model of

human motivation. Central to SDT is the clear distinction between two types of motivations:

autonomous (intrinsic) and controlled (extrinsic). Intrinsic motivation involves acting with a

sense of volition. It explains that human beings posses an innate drive towards personal

growth which is affected by emotion-based needs: competence, autonomy and relatedness.

Moreover, intrinsic motivation is believed to be facilitated by joy. It explains that human

beings engage in activities not because of instrumental values but because they enjoy doing

Page 10: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

10

things which give them satisfaction (Deci and Ryan, 2000). Such conclusions were made by

Harlow and Meyer (1950) who found out that monkeys performed a task because the task

itself provided them with an intrinsic value. When researchers gave puzzles to monkeys

these animals immediately started solving them without any rewards or punishments. It was

shown that monkeys learned to solve the puzzle purely because of the enjoyment of doing

the task itself. This has led to a suggestion that emotions are fundamentally related to

intrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner & Ryan, 1999). It was suggested that emotions of

enjoyment and excitement accompanied by the experience of competence, autonomy and

relatedness represent the rewards for intrinsically motivated behaviour. Understanding the

importance of intrinsic motivation is crucial if organisations want to improve their

performance and efficiency. I take autonomy as an example and analyze it in more detail

providing some suggestions to how organisations could help their employees to boost their

productivity.

Autonomy can be understood as the need to perform tasks of one’s choice without an

external interference. I believe that the whole construct of autonomy depends on both

affect and cognition, in particular cognitive appraisals. Whenever people encounter a

specific stimulus, which interferes with their freedom of acting with the sense of volition, it

affects emotional properties resulting in feeling less autonomous. In this case, Rodin (1986)

has shown that the feeling of having a lack of control over events at work is a source of

work-related stress. Furthermore, Wharton (1993) concluded that people who reported

high autonomy had lower emotional exhaustion and also autonomy was positively related

to job satisfaction. Now these findings support SDT’s view on the importance of such

feeling. Deci et al. (2000) has also reported that extrinsic rewards (such as financial

incentives) might reduce employees’ feeling of autonomy which usually results in the

diminishment of intrinsic rewards that leads to worse performance at work. All these

findings suggest one important notion: organisations have to ensure that their employees

feel that they have autonomy at work. By providing people with a feeling of control over

their actions, companies can expect not only better performance at work, but also will make

their employees more satisfied and less stressful.

Page 11: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

11

5. Conclusion

For too long the importance of emotions was neglected in organisational environment. The

present study aimed to draw attention to the influences affect can have on our thinking,

reasoning, judgements and behaviour while being at work. A theoretical framework was

presented which gave a clear insight into situations which can lead to mood-congruence

biases. Moreover, the present essay outlined that in order to overcome such biases -

organisations should inform employees about them and also suggested that these biases

can be used as an advantage (f.e. in negotiations). The dissertation then moved to discuss

the happy-productive hypothesis which outlines that happy employees positively affect the

overall performance of the organisation. Lastly, an emotion-based motivation theory was

briefly discussed in order to support the idea that affect plays a crucial role in organisations.

By giving more attention to the influences emotions have on the employees, organisations

could significantly improve their performance and efficiency.

Word count: 3445

Page 12: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

12

REFERENCES:

Ashkanasy, N.M., Hartel, C.E.J., & Daus, C.S. (2002). Diversity and emotion: the new frontiers

in organizational behaviour research. Journal of Management, 28, 307-340.

Berkowitz, L., Jaffee, S., Jo, F., & Troccoli, B. T. (2000). On the Correction of Feeling-Induced

Judgmental Biases. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social

cognition (pp. 131–152). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the

orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295-307.

Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

Deci, E.L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R.M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments

examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin,

125, 627-668.

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: integration in personality. In

R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Perspectives on motivation, 38,

237-288. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press

Forgas, J. P. (1994). Sad and guilty? Affective influences on the explanation of conflict

episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 56–68.

Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological

Bulletin, 117 (1), 39–66.

Forgas, J. P. (1998). Asking nicely? Mood effects on responding to more or less polite

requests. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 173–185.

Forgas, J. P. (1999). On feeling good and being rude: Affective influences on language use

and request formulations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 928–939.

Page 13: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

13

Forgas, J.P., George, J.M. (2001). Affective influences on judgements and behaviour in

organizations: an information processing perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 86 (1), 3-34.

Forgas, J. P., Bower, G. H., & Krantz, S. (1984). The influence of mood on perceptions of

social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 497–413.

Forgas, J.P. & Fiedler, K. (1996). Us and them: Mood effects on intergroup discrimination.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 28-40

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-

and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.

George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1996). Motivational agendas in the workplace: The effects of

feelings on focus of attention and work motivation. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),

Research in organizational behavior , 18, 75–109. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Harlow, H, F. Harlow, M, V. and Meyer, D, R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulation

drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40 pp. 228–234.

Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative

problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.

Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217.

Madjar, N., Oldham, G. R., & Pratt, M. G. (2002). There’s no place like home? The

contributions of work and non-work creativity support to employee’s creative performance.

Academy of Management Journal, 45, 757–767.

Page 14: Emotions at work: experimental findings every organisation ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/ddc/c8cxpa/... · wants to impress his boss he might be more helpful and such motivational

14

Quellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple

processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–

74.

Raghunathan, R., Pham, M.T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: motivational

influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 79 (1), 56-77.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic

motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78

Rodin, J. (1986). Aging and health: effects of the sense of control. Science, 233 (4770), 1271-

1276.

Taris, T.W., & Schreurs, P.J.G. (2009). Well-being and organizational performance: an

organizational-level test of the happy-productive worker hypothesis. Work & Stress: An

International Journal of Work, Health & Organisations, 23(2), 120-136.

Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY: Wiley

Williams, S., & Shiaw, W. T. (1999). Mood and organizational citizenship behavior: The

effects of positive affect on employee organizational citizenship behavior intentions. Journal

of Psychology, 133, 656–668.

Wright, T. A., & Staw, B. M. (1999). Affect and favorable work outcomes: Two longitudinal

tests of the happy-productive worker thesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 1–13.

Weiss, H.M. (2002) Deconstructing job satisfaction: searating evaluations, beliefs and

affective experiences. Human Resources Management Review. In press

Wharton, A.S. (1993). The affective consequences of service work: managing emotions on

the job. Work and Occupations, 20 (2), 205-232

Zelenski, J.M., Murphy, S.A., & Jenkins, D.A. (2008). The happy-productive worker thesis

revisited. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 521-537.