experiments in linguistic meaning, sept. 17, 2020

55
Discourse behavior of possessives reflects the importance of interpersonal relationships Jesse Storbeck & Elsi Kaiser University of Southern California [email protected] Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Upload: others

Post on 20-Nov-2021

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Discourse behavior of possessives reflects the importance of interpersonal relationships

Jesse Storbeck & Elsi KaiserUniversity of Southern California

[email protected]

Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Page 3: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Possessives in discourse● As people comprehend language, they keep track of relevant entities in a

mental model of the discourse

● Most of the work on how entities are represented on the discourse level has focused on simple nominal phrases (i.e. one NP → one discourse referent)

● What about complex NPs like possessive constructions?○ Sam’s car refers to two entities → two discourse referents

● How are possessives represented on the discourse level?○ Does the extremely broad range of semantic relations possessives can

express affect the way the two referents are represented in discourse?

3

Page 4: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Roadmap1. Background on discourse, possessives, and interpersonal relationships

● Animate possession → interpersonal relationship (e.g. her brother)

2. Three competing hypotheses about discourse representations of possessions

3. A sentence-continuation experiment to assess our hypotheses

● We tested how animacy and being possessed affect a referent’s likelihood of re-mention in subsequent discourse

4

Page 5: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Discourse-level representation and prominence● Entities in discourse vary in prominence (i.e. salience)

● Many factors can influence referent prominence, including:

○ Grammatical role■ Subjects usually more prominent than objects (e.g. Chafe, 1976;

Crawley et al., 1990)

○ Animacy■ Animates usually more prominent than inanimates ■ Cross-linguistic effects on word order, choice of nominal form, etc.

(e.g. Bock et al., 1992; Dahl & Fraurud, 1996)

5

Page 6: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Possessives in discourse● the car → one entity explicitly mentioned simple nominal

● Sam’s car → two entities explicitly mentioned possessivepossessor possession

1. How are the two entities represented in discourse?○ Simply two independent discourse referents?○ Linked referents?

2. If linked, does the link differ across various types of possession relations?○ e.g. Sam’s car [ownership], Sam’s arm [part-whole], Sam’s dad [kinship]○ Same morphosyntactic realization in English, different semantics

6

Page 7: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

● Little work on the discourse-level behavior of possessives● Conflicting claims by accounts within Centering Theory (Grosz et al., 1995):

○ Possessors more prominent than possessions (Chae, 2003)■ Sam’s car → Sam > car

○ Possessors more prominent than inanimate possessions but not animate possessions (Di Eugenio, 1998)

■ Sam’s car → Sam > car■ Sam’s doctor → doctor > Sam

7

Prior work: conflicting claims about prominence of possessors vs. possessions

Page 8: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Prior work: the need for further research● Centering Theory addresses the relative prominence of possessors and

possessions (i.e. it ranks all referenced entities according to prominence)

○ Less clear how being possessed affects a referent’s discourse representation (i.e. what’s the effect of being possessed per se?)

● Possessive constructions are commonplace

○ Yet current discourse theories struggle with how to represent them○ Thus, the lack of research on their behavior in discourse represents a

significant theoretical gap

8

Page 9: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Interpersonal relationships● With animate possessions, possessives denote interpersonal relationships

(e.g. her father, his cousin, their boss)

● Interpersonal relationships are critical for human health and well-being (e.g. Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009; Eisenberger & Cole, 2012)

○ Longevity (e.g. Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010)○ Disease resistance (e.g. Miller et al., 2009)○ Loneliness and cognitive decline (e.g. Tilvis et al., 2004)

● Interpersonal relationships are cognitively privileged○ What about in discourse?

9

Page 10: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Three competing hypotheses about the discourse prominence of possessed referents:

1. The animacy hypothesis

2. The possessive hypothesis

3. The interaction hypothesis

10

Page 11: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

1. The animacy hypothesis● In general, animate referents are more prominent in discourse and memory

(e.g. Bock et al., 1992; Dahl, 2008; Gelin, 2019)

● Therefore, animate possessions are more prominent than inanimate ones

○ Sam’s car → Sam > car○ Sam’s doctor → doctor > Sam

● The animacy hypothesis predicts that this effect should be comparable in magnitude to animacy effects in simple nominals ([a] doctor > [a] car)

11

Page 12: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

2. The possessive hypothesis● Possessive NPs are referentially more complex than simple NPs

○ They refer to two entities, instead of just one

● Representational complexity promotes retrieval from memory (e.g. Hofmeister, 2011; Karimi & Ferreira, 2016; Troyer et al., 2016)

● Therefore, possessions are more prominent than simpler nominals

○ [Sam’s] car > [a] car

■ Being possessed → increased complexity → increased prominence

12

Page 13: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

3. The interaction hypothesis● If animacy hypothesis and possessive hypothesis hold →

animate possessions should be most prominent○ Maybe another contribution to animate possessions’ prominence?

■ Interpersonal relationships

● Interpersonal relationships support human health → cognitive privilege○ Adaptive memory theories: better memory for animates arose from

evolutionary importance of identifying threats, mates, and social groups (i.e. interpersonal relationships) (e.g. Nairne et al., 2013)

● Due to the physiological, cognitive, and adaptive importance of interpersonal relationships, animate possessions are exceptionally prominent in discourse

13

Page 14: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Research questions● How are possessives represented on the discourse level?

● Do different semantic possession relations affect representation in discourse?○ Sam’s car [ownership] vs. Sam’s doctor [interpersonal relationship]

● Are different types of possessed entities more or less likely to be mentioned subsequently in the discourse? With what linguistic form?

● Can we find support for our animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, or interaction hypothesis?

14

Page 15: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Method: sentence continuation paradigm● Sentence continuation task measures the discourse prominence of competing

referents (e.g. Arnold, 2001; Stevenson et al., 1994)

● Participants (n=40 MTurkers) get a prompt sentence, produce an additional sentence which naturally continues the discourse

15

Page 16: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Analysis of sentence continuations● Dependent variable(s): which entities from the prompt do people mention in

their continuations? In what grammatical position? With what form?○ Higher likelihood of mention → more prominent○ Focus today on likelihood of mention (ask me about pronominalization data in Q&A 😃)

● Subject position is most prominent (e.g. Chafe, 1976; Crawley et al., 1990)○ Thus the most prominent referent from the prompt is likely to be realized

as the subject of the continuation (e.g. Stevenson et al., 1994)

● We understand mentions across the entire continuation as a correlated, perhaps more holistic measure

16

Page 17: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Experiment design: prompt structure

Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.● 2×2 design manipulating properties of direct object:

○ Animacy (human role nouns vs. alienable inanimates)○ Possessed vs. indefinite

● Nonce verbs to minimize potential effects of verb semantics○ Implicit causality (e.g. Hartshorne & Snedeker, 2013)○ Nonce verbs can be held constant within items○ Post-hoc analysis: no consistent biases for nonce verbs

● Role nouns’ stereotypical gender contrasted with name (Misersky et al., 2013)○ This is only in order to mitigate potential pronoun ambiguity

17

Page 18: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Example targets (2 of 24)

18

Page 19: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Example responses

● Jennifer pranned her surgeon.○ She asked him how long her surgery would last.○ The surgeon did a good job.○ It wasn't very sanitary for an OR.

19

Page 20: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Coding responses● Jennifer pranned her surgeon.

○ She asked him how long her surgery would last.

1. What is mentioned in the subject position of the continuation?She → preceding subject

2. What is mentioned anywhere in the continuation?She, her → preceding subject, him → preceding object

● Collapsing across all forms of mention (ask me about pronominalization data in Q&A 😃)

● 97% agreement in double-coded 10% subset of the responses

20

Page 21: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Statistical analysis● Generalized linear mixed-effects (glmer) models in lme4 (Bates et al., 2015)

● Models fit to binomial outcome of mentioning a given entity from the prompt in a given position of the continuation → combinations yield four models○ Preceding subject as continuation subject? Yes → 1, No → 0○ Preceding object as continuation subject?○ Preceding subject mentioned anywhere?○ Preceding object as mentioned anywhere?

● Maximal convergent models○ All have random intercepts for items and participants○ Random slopes added when they did not result in non-convergence

21

Page 22: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions● The animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, and interaction hypothesis

make different predictions of the results

● Predictions broadly relevant for subject position and across the continuation

○ Subject position is a “winner take all” view of prominence

○ Mentions across the continuation will be correlated but more inclusive

● These predictions relate mainly to mentions of the preceding object, since that is where the experimental manipulation takes place

22

Page 23: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions: the animacy hypothesis● The animacy hypothesis

○ Animate possessions are more prominent than inanimate ones

■ Mentions of animate objects > inanimate objects■ Animacy effect in possessives parallel to its effect in indefinites■ We expect a main effect of animacy in the models for mentions of

the preceding object

23

Page 24: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions: the possessive hypothesis● The possessive hypothesis

○ Possessions are more prominent than simple NPs (here, indefinites)

■ Mentions of possessions > indefinites■ Possessive effect in animates parallel to its effect in inanimates■ We expect a main effect of being possessed in the models for

mentions of the preceding object

● Note that differences in givenness and specificity between possessions and indefinites (e.g. Barker, 2000; von Heusinger, 2011) could affect this prediction, but more on this in the results/discussion

24

Page 25: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions: the interaction hypothesis● The interaction hypothesis

○ Animate possessions are exceptionally prominent in discourse

■ Superadditive effect of animacy and possession together

■ We expect an interaction of animacy and being possessed in the models for mentions of the preceding object

25

Page 26: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: two sets of figures● Jennifer pranned her surgeon.

○ She asked him how long her surgery would last.

1. What is mentioned in the subject position of the continuation?○ Subject position is mutually exclusive, so proportions within a

condition will sum to 1* (*except for a very small number of they responses)

2. What is mentioned anywhere in the continuation?○ Both entities can be mentioned across the continuation, so

proportions within a condition do not sum to 1

26

Page 27: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: subject position of continuations (Fig. 1)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

27

Page 28: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: subject position of continuations (Fig. 1)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

28

Page 29: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: subject position of continuations (Fig. 1)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

29

● Animacy effect: preceding subjects mentioned more often as continuation subject when preceding object is inanimate (p<0.01)

Page 30: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: subject position of continuations (Fig. 1)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

30

● Animacy effect: animate preceding objects more likely than inanimates to be mentioned in subject position of continuations (p<0.001)

Page 31: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: subject position of continuations (Fig. 1)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

31

● Interaction: animate possessed preceding objects are extra likely as continuation subjects (i.e. animacy:possession interaction, p=.05; simple effect, p=.03)

Page 32: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: all positions of continuations (Fig. 2)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

32

Page 33: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: all positions of continuations (Fig. 2)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

33

Page 34: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: all positions of continuations (Fig. 2)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

34

● No significant effects: preceding subjects are equally likely across conditions to be mentioned anywhere in the continuation

Page 35: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: all positions of continuations (Fig. 2)

“Melissa lupted a/her chauffeur/backpack.”

35

● Interaction: animate possessed preceding objects are extra likely to be mentioned anywhere in the continuation (i.e. animacy:possession interaction, p<.01)

Page 36: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Results: summary● Animacy effects in subject position for

preceding subjects and objects, but not across the entire continuation

● Interactions: animate possessions particularly likely to be mentioned○ Effects in both subject position and

across the entire continuation○ These effects not accounted for by

either animacy or possession alone○ Cognitive importance of

interpersonal relations influencing language?

What is mentioned in subject position of continuation?

What is mentioned anywhere in continuation?

Page 37: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions revisited1. Animacy Hypothesis

○ Mentions of animate objects > inanimate objects○ Animacy effect in possessives parallel to its effect in indefinites

2. Possessive Hypothesis○ Mentions of possessed objects > indefinite objects○ Being possessed affects animates and inanimates similarly

3. Interaction Hypothesis○ Possessed animate objects mentioned most often○ Superadditive effect of animacy + possession

37

Page 38: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Predictions revisited

1. ✗ Animacy Hypothesis○ Mentions of animate objects > inanimate objects○ Animacy effect in possessives parallel to its effect in indefinites

2. ✗ Possessive Hypothesis○ Mentions of possessed objects > indefinite objects○ Being possessed affects animates and inanimates similarly

3. ✓ Interaction Hypothesis○ Possessed animate objects mentioned most often○ Superadditive effect of animacy + possession

38

Page 39: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Research questions reviewed● Are different types of possessed entities more or less likely to be mentioned

subsequently in the discourse?

✓ YES

● Do different possession relations affect representation in discourse?

✓ YES results compatible with such a theory

● Can we find support for our animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, or interaction hypothesis?

39

Page 40: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Research questions reviewed● Are different types of possessed entities more or less likely to be mentioned

subsequently in the discourse?

✓ YES

● Do different possession relations affect representation in discourse?

✓ YES results compatible with such a theory

● Can we find support for our animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, or interaction hypothesis?

40

Page 41: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Research questions reviewed● Are different types of possessed entities more or less likely to be mentioned

subsequently in the discourse?

✓ YES

● Do different possession relations affect representation in discourse?

✓ YES, results compatible with such a theory

● Can we find support for our animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, or interaction hypothesis?

41

Page 42: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Research questions reviewed● Are different types of possessed entities more or less likely to be mentioned

subsequently in the discourse?

✓ YES

● Do different possession relations affect representation in discourse?

✓ YES, results compatible with such a theory

● Can we find support for our animacy hypothesis, possessive hypothesis, or

✓ interaction hypothesis?

42

Page 43: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Discussion● Possessed animates are especially prominent in discourse, as measured by

their likelihood of re-mention○ In general, animates are more prominent in discourse and memory, but

possessed animates seem to get a special boost in prominence■ Possessed animates explicitly denote interpersonal relationships■ May relate to non-linguistic theories on importance of interpersonal

relationships (social, physiological, and adaptive benefits)

● Compatible with Di Eugenio’s (1998) Centering Theory account○ Furthermore, we show that this boost is due to more than a simple

animacy effect or the additive effects of animacy and possession

43

Page 44: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Discussion● We observe expected animacy effects (e.g. competition for prominence

between animate referents) but what about an overall possessive effect?○ Possessions and indefinites may differ in givenness and specificity, which

affect prominence (e.g. Barker, 2000; Kaiser, 2011; von Heusinger, 2011)■ Potential to mask a main effect of possession■ Maybe a different result with definites?

● Crucially, givenness/specificity differences do not explain interactions○ Givenness and specificity should not vary with animacy○ Choosing a different class of nominals to compare with possessions

should modulate possessive effect, but interactions should remain

44

Page 45: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Thank you for your attention and feedback!

[email protected]

45

Page 46: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

46

Appendix: pronominalization results● Mentions of the preceding subject were much more likely to use a pronoun

Page 47: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

47

Appendix: pronominalization results● This holds across conditions, even in subject position with preceding animate

objects, where the preceding object was more likely as continuation subject○ Likelihood of mention ≠ likelihood of pronominalization (Kehler & Rohde, 2013)

Page 48: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

48

Appendix: pronominalization results

Page 49: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Appendix: item details

49

Name Nonce Verb

Animate Object

Inanimate Object Name Nonce

VerbAnimate Object

Inanimate Object

Carol debbed butler violin Megan gerped chiropractor clarinet

Emily pletched stockbroker saxophone Melissa lupted chauffeur backpack

Helen swudged tattooist unicycle Rebecca gweeshed bodyguard umbrella

Jennifer pranned surgeon blanket Sarah chooped plumber scarf

Jessica rulked electrician chandelier Stephanie shupped mechanic flashlight

Kimberly chabbed chef fork Veronica brilted priest towel

Page 50: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Appendix: item details

50

Name Nonce Verb

Animate Object

Inanimate Object Name Nonce

Verb Animate Object Inanimate Object

Anthony jolfed florist toaster Jason joiped congresswoman wheelbarrow

Brad churbed receptionist screwdriver Joseph tammed babysitter microwave

Brian sibbed waitress basket Kevin blorned stewardess dictionary

Daniel zatted nurse jacket Mark meared housekeeper stereo

Fred risped nanny broom Robert dasped secretary television

James fammed hairdresser thermometer William dreezed psychic helmet

Page 51: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

Appendix: glmer formulaesubject_model <- glmer(glmer_subj ~ animacy * det + (1 + animacy | subjectID) + (1 | itemID)

object_model <- glmer(glmer_obj ~ animacy * det + (1 + animacy + det | subjectID) + (1 | itemID)

subject_model_anywhere <- glmer(itemSubjMention ~ animacy * det + (1 + animacy | subjectID) + (1 | itemID)

object_model_anywhere <- glmer(itemObjMention ~ animacy * det + (1 + animacy | subjectID) + (1 | itemID)

51

Page 52: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

ReferencesArnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31(2), 137-162.

doi:10.1207/S15326950DP3102_02

Arnold, J., & Griffin, Z. M. (2007). The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 521-536. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.007

Barker, C. (2000). Definite possessives and discourse novelty. Theoretical Linguistics, 26(3), 211. doi:10.1515/thli.2000.26.3.211

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations. Psychological Review, 99(1), 150-171. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.150

Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(10), 447-454.

Chae, S. (2003). Possessives in naturally occurring discourse: A centering approach. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 9(1). Retrieved from https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol9/iss1/6

Chafe, W. L. (1976). Giveness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.

52

Page 53: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

ReferencesDahl, Ö. (2008). Animacy and egophoricity: Grammar, ontology and phylogeny. Lingua, 118(2), 141-150. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2007.02.008

Dahl, Ö., & Fraurud, K. (1996). Animacy in grammar and discourse. In T. Fretheim, & J. K. Gundel (Eds.), Reference and Referent Accessibility. Pragmatics & Beyond New Series, (pp. 47-64). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Di Eugenio, B. (1998). Centering in Italian. In M. Walker, A. Joshi & E. Prince (Eds.), Centering theory in discourse (pp. 115-137)

Eisenberger, N. I., & Cole, S. W. (2012). Social neuroscience and health: neurophysiological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nature neuroscience, 15(5), 669-674.

Fukumura, K., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2010). Choosing anaphoric expressions: Do people take into account likelihood of reference? Journal of Memory and Language, 62(1), 52-66. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.001

Gelin, M., Bonin, P., Méot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2018). Do animacy effects persist in memory for context? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 965-974. doi:10.1080/17470218.2017.1307866

Grosz, B. J., Joshi, A. K., & Weinstein, S. (1995). Centering: A framework for modeling the local coherence of discourse. Computational Linguistics, 21(2), 203.

Hartshorne, J. K., & Snedeker, J. (2013). Verb argument structure predicts implicit causality: The advantages of finer-grained semantics. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(10), 1474-1508.

53

Page 54: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

ReferencesHofmeister, P. (2011). Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(3),

376-405. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.492642

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS medicine, 7(7), e1000316.

Kaiser, E. (2011). Focusing on pronouns: Consequences of subjecthood, pronominalisation, and contrastive focus. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 1625-1666. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.523082

Karimi, H., & Ferreira, F. (2016). Informativity renders a referent more accessible: Evidence from eyetracking. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(2), 507-525.

Kehler, A., & Rohde, H. (2013). A probabilistic reconciliation of coherence-driven and centering-driven theories of pronoun interpretation. Theoretical Linguistics, 39(1), 1-37. doi:10.1515/tl-2013-0001

Miller, G., Chen, E., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Health psychology: Developing biologically plausible models linking the social world and physical health. Annual review of psychology, 60, 501-524.

Misersky, J., Gygax, P. M., Canal, P., Gabriel, U., Garnham, A., Braun, F., ... Sczesny, S. (2014). Norms on the gender perception of role nouns in Czech, English, French, German, Italian, Norwegian, and Slovak. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 841-871. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0409-z

Nairne, J. S., VanArsdall, J. E., Pandeirada, J. N., Cogdill, M., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of animacy. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2099-2105.

54

Page 55: Experiments in Linguistic Meaning, Sept. 17, 2020

ReferencesStevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes,

9(4), 519-548. doi:10.1080/01690969408402130

Tilvis, R. S., Kähönen-Väre, M. H., Jolkkonen, J., Valvanne, J., Pitkala, K. H., & Strandberg, T. E. (2004). Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(3), M268-M274.

Troyer, M., Hofmeister, P., & Kutas, M. (2016). Elaboration over a discourse facilitates retrieval in sentence processing. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 374.

von Heusinger, K. 2011. Specificity. In K. von Heusinger, C. Maienborn, & P. Portner (eds.), Semantics. An international handbook of natural language meaning, vol. 2, 1025–1058. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.

55