gender stereotypes of scientist characters in television programs

39
Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs Popular Among Middle School-Aged Children Jocelyn Steinke 1 , Marilee Long 2 , Marne J. Johnson 1 , Sayani Ghosh 1 1 Western Michigan University, 2 Colorado State University Paper presented to the Science Communication Interest Group (SCIGroup) for the Annual Meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication (AEJMC) Chicago, IL August 2008 1

Upload: tranhanh

Post on 13-Feb-2017

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs Popular Among Middle School-Aged Children

Jocelyn Steinke1, Marilee Long2, Marne J. Johnson1, Sayani Ghosh1

1Western Michigan University, 2Colorado State University

Paper presented to the Science Communication Interest Group (SCIGroup) for theAnnual Meeting of the Association for Education in

Journalism and Mass Communication (AEJMC)

Chicago, ILAugust 2008

1

Page 2: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs Popular Among Middle School-Aged Children

Abstract

This study examined gender stereotyping in portrayals of scientist characters in television programs popular among or likely to be viewed by middle school-aged children. The following 14 television programs were analyzed: Bill Nye the Science Guy, CSI, CSI- Miami, CSI-New York, Danny Phantom, Dexter's Laboratory, DragonflyTV, Friends, Kim Possible, MythBusters, Strange Days at Blake Holsey High, The Adventures of Jimmy Neutron: Boy Genius, The Simpsons, and The X Files. Across all programs, male scientist characters were found to be both more prevalent than female scientist characters and also found to be present in more scenes; within program genres, male scientist characters also outnumbered female scientist characters for cartoons and drama programs, but not for educational programs. Male scientist characters were more likely to be shown with the masculine attributes of independence and dominance, but not athleticism. Female scientist characters were not more likely than male scientist characters to be shown with the feminine attributes of dependence, caring, and romantic. No differences were found in the portrayals of male and female characters’ scientific professional roles, marital status, and parental status. The implications of the findings on middle school-aged children’s perceptions of scientists and in cultivating girls’ future interest in science careers are discussed.

2

Page 3: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs Popular Among Middle School-Aged Children1

Introduction

Greater participation of women and other underrepresented groups in science,

engineering, and technology (SET) is needed to ensure a diverse and abundant workforce in

these areas in the future. Developing the SET workforce will be increasingly more important in

the next decade in the United States as advances in research and technology create additional

demands for professionals in these areas and as more scientists and engineers retire from the SET

workforce (National Science Board 2006). Despite increases in the number of women receiving

degrees in SET, the percentage of women in the SET workforce remains below their proportion

in the U.S. population; women are still particularly underrepresented in several SET disciplines

including physics, engineering, and computer science (Congressional Commission on the

Advancement of Women and Minorities in Science Engineering and Technology Development,

2000; National Science Board, 2003, 2006). In addition, developing girls’ interest in SET is

essential for promoting scientific literacy and knowledge among all in order to increase public

understanding of important social issues related to SET such as global warming, AIDS,

nanotechnology, cancer, biotechnology, and renewal energy.

Increasing the representation of women in SET requires substantial efforts to cultivate

and support girls’ interest in science and engineering during their childhood and adolescent years

in order to develop a desire to pursue SET careers in the future. Research indicates that by the

1 The authors gratefully acknowledge support provided by the National Science Foundation (HRD-0624583). Any opinions and recommendations expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the National Science Foundation. The authors also acknowledge Nikki Crocker, Emily Crespin, Megan Stewart, and Dana Strongin for their assistance with this research.

3

Page 4: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

age of 12 many girls have already lost interest in SET (American Association of University

Women, 1998, 2000), and during the middle and high school years both girls’ and boys’ attitudes

toward science decline (George, 2000). Promoting girls’ interest in science is important not only

for strengthening the SET workforce, but also for promoting equity in society to ensure that all

children have opportunities to fully develop their abilities in SET.

Media images of science and scientists in U.S. popular culture influence public attitudes

toward science and scientists (National Science Board 2006). For many adults and children,

television is the primary source of information about science and a purveyor of both fictional and

real-life images of scientists. Research shows that adults are most likely to obtain information

about science and scientists from television (National Science Board 2006). Research also

indicates that television is a major source of information about scientists for middle school-aged

children (Steinke et al., 2007), and adolescent girls report spending most of their leisure time

watching television, an average of 1.67 hours a day (Cherney & London, 2006). In addition,

middle-school students have reported that television programs are among the factors that have

positively influenced their attitudes toward and interest in science (Gibson & Chase, 2002). The

influence of popular television images of scientists on children’s perceptions of scientists may

also then have a considerable impact on their future interest in SET careers.

While some studies have documented positive effects of media images of scientists on

children’s attitudes toward science, other studies have noted negative effects of these images.

One study found that although images from a popular science fiction film were more memorable

to middle school students than hands-on science experiences, the information provided by these

popular images was a source of students’ misconceptions of scientific concepts (Barnett et al.,

4

Page 5: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

2006). Another study found that images of scientists on popular children’s television programs

promoted gender stereotypes of scientists and reinforced the view of science as a masculine

domain (Steinke & Long, 1996). Another study found that middle school-aged boys who

reported the media as being important in their lives had more negative views towards women in

science (Steinke et al., 2007). As these studies suggest, research on the portrayals of scientist

characters on television merits closer examination because it can help researchers better

understand how these images may shape children’s and adolescents’ attitudes toward science and

interest in science careers.

Presenting positive televised images of women scientists may be a particularly effective

strategy for providing role models to promote girls’ interest in science, particularly when direct

interaction with human role models is not possible. The popularity of television provides an

opportunity to present children with access to media models of women scientists through

“vicarious contact” (A. Bandura, 2002; Fujioka, 1999) when interaction with actual adult

scientists is not possible because of limitations related to access and resources. Recent research

has examined the potential influence of media models on adolescents’ perceptions of gender

roles and of scientists. One study found that middle-school students who participated in a media

literacy program that included watching video clips of stereotyped and counter-stereotyped

portrayals of women SET professionals as well as getting instruction on the stereotyped

portrayals of women and women SET professionals in the media were less likely to agree that

women are accurately portrayed in the media than students who did not watch the video clips or

receive the instruction, although specific changes in attitudes toward SET were not found

(Steinke et al., 2007). Research on the influence of virtual models on young women’s science-

5

Page 6: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

related beliefs (Baylor, 2004; Baylor & Plant, 2005) found that college-aged women were more

likely to identify with female pedagogical agents that were rated as attractive, young, and cool

and were more likely to perceive agents rated as older and “uncool” to be most like engineers

(Baylor & Plant, 2005). A study of images of scientists in multimedia software found that

college-aged students’ exposure to counter-stereotypical images of scientists was effective in

promoting less negative stereotypical perceptions about who becomes a scientist (Edwards,

1999).

Historically, images of male scientists and engineers have dominated the U.S. media.

Male scientists have appeared more frequently in primetime dramas, where researchers have

found three male scientists for every female scientist (Signorielli, 1993). In films, researchers

have found fewer than one-fifth of scientists were female (Weingart, Muhl, & Pansegrau, 2003).

In science educational programming for children, researchers have found that male characters

outnumbered female characters both in number and in screen time (e.g., Leaper, Breed,

Hoffman, & Perlman, 2002; Long, Boiarsky, & Thayer, 2001; Steinke & Long, 1996). When

women engineers and scientists do appear in popular U.S. media programs, these images often

perpetuate gender stereotypes that show science as an inappropriate career for most “ordinary”

women (LaFollette, 1988, 1990). Media portrayals typically reinforce gender stereotypes

through images that downplay women scientists’ expertise (Corbett, 2001; LaFollette, 1981,

1988, 1990; Nelkin, 1987; Steinke & Long, 1996), focus on the conflicts involved in balancing

women’s professional and personal lives (LaFollette, 1988, 1990; Nelkin, 1987), show women as

younger and more attractive than men (Weingart et al., 2003), highlight women’s lack of

masculine traits and skills needed to conduct scientific research (LaFollette, 1988), emphasize

6

Page 7: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

women as potential distractions to their male colleagues (LaFollette, 1990), and feature women

and men interacting in ways that reveal both overt and subtle forms of stereotyping and

discrimination against women (Steinke, 2005).

The purpose of this study is to add to our understanding of the portrayals of scientists in

the mass media by examining portrayals in television programs popular among or likely to have

been seen by middle school-aged students. The focus of this research on middle school-aged

children, ages 12 to 14 years old, is important because research indicates that this is the age when

many girls report losing interest in SET (American Association of University Women, 1998,

2000) and children’s attitudes toward science decline during the middle and high school years

(George, 2000). This study examined demographics and lifestyle characteristics of the scientist

characters as well as the prevalence of several traditional gender-stereotyped attributes or traits.

One of the goals of this study was to investigate whether the program genre of the television

programs in which the scientist characters appeared affected the portrayals of these characters

related to the depictions of gender stereotypes and gender roles. Specifically, this study

investigated portrayals of male and female scientist characters in four television program genres:

dramas, cartoons, situation comedies, and science educational programs.

Theoretical Framework and Literature Review

Gender Schema Theory, Media Models, and Developing Identities

Throughout childhood and into adolescence, children develop specific views about

gender roles as defined by the culture and society in which they live (Bem, 1981). Children

learn about gender roles not only from media sources, like televised role models, but also from

an array of other socializing agents like parents, teachers, and peers (Bandura, 2002; Bussey &

7

Page 8: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Bandura, 1999). According to Gender Schema Theory, children have a tendency and readiness

to process information they learn based on gender or sex-linked associations that are part of their

gender schemas (Bem, 1981). Bem describes a schema as “a cognitive structure, a network of

associations that organizes and guides an individual’s perceptions” (Bem, 1981) p. 355.

Children use gender schemas to develop their self-concepts (Bem, 1981) that then lead them to

define gender roles and conceive specific perceptions of themselves, including views of their

future personal and professional roles.

The media are important sources of information about gender roles and are important

influences during the development of gender schemas. During identity formation, as adolescent

girls develop “possible selves” (Ruvolo & Markus, 1992) that represent who they “could

become, would like to become, and are afraid of becoming” (p. 96), media models can shape

their conceptions of self. Adolescence marks a critical time for girls when the “possible selves”

they envision for themselves have great potential to influence their future behavior (Markus &

Nurius, 1986). The socializing influence of media images of women may be especially strong

for some adolescent girls, who during the transition from childhood to adulthood, lose self-

confidence and become preoccupied with being popular, fitting in, looking thin and attractive,

and finding romance (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Orenstein, 1994). Many adolescent girls find

that acting feminine is a way to gain approval, acceptance, and popularity, and to avoid conflict

(Brown & Gilligan, 1992). Strong female media models can provide positive self-schemas that

serve as “enabling self-images” (Arnold 1993) for adolescent girls. Positive portrayals of

televised images of women in non-traditional roles can be effective role models for changing

girls’ perceptions of gender roles. However, media models that overemphasize women’s

8

Page 9: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

physical appearance and focus on stereotypes of women provide negative self-schemas that

convey “constraining self-images” that can limit girls’ future potential (Arnold, 1993).

A number of studies have documented the resistance of gender schemas to change

(Durkin, 1985; Durkin & Nugent, 1998; Martin & Charles F. Halverson, 1983; Nihlen & Bailey,

1988; Renn & Calvert, 1993; Signorella & Liben, 1984). However, research also indicates that

interventions can alter existing gender schemas and current views of gender roles. For example,

one study found that pre-adolescent and adolescent girls who identified originally with

traditional feminine gender roles also later identified with televised images of women in counter-

stereotypical gender roles (Eisenstock, 1984). Another study found that elementary school-aged

girls reported interpersonal attraction to female characters who did not exhibit traditional gender

stereotyped or feminine stereotyped behavior (Aubrey & Harrison, 2004). Images of scientists

that present counterstereotypes or dispel traditional views of science as masculine may be critical

for implementing changes in girls’ perceptions of scientists as well as their perceptions of self as

a scientist in the future.

The media are important sources of information about gender roles for boys as well as for

girls. Media content that primarily shows men in dominant roles may negatively influence boys’

perceptions of the role of women in society. A study of middle school-aged children found that

boys who indicated the media were very important had more negative attitudes toward women in

science than those who indicated the media were less important (Steinke et al., 2007). These

findings are important because 1) boys’ expectations of how girls should act can influence girls’

behavior during their childhood and adolescent years, and 2) boys’ perceptions of women’s roles

developed during childhood influences their adult views of women’s professional roles in the

9

Page 10: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

workforce and their perceptions of women’s personal roles within family structures. Images of

scientists that show women as SET professionals as well as both men and women scientists

working together are needed in programs viewed by children and adolescents in order to enable

them to see the culture of science as inclusive of all people.

Social Learning Theory, Observational Learning, and Identification

Social learning theory (later called social cognitive theory; Albert Bandura, 1986)

provides an explanation for how children learn specific attitudes and behaviors from the images

and characters they encounter in the media. According to this theory, children learn cultural

patterns of behavior through repeated observations of both actual models in their social

environments, like parents and teachers, as well as symbolic models in their social environments

like those depicted in the media (Albert Bandura, 1969). This theory describes how children

learn to imitate the behavior of others in their environments through a process known as

“identificatory learning” (Albert Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963a), 533).

Early studies on identification with television characters and observational learning

documented children’s ability to learn from and model the behavior of characters they saw in

films (Albert Bandura et al., 1963a; Albert Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963b; Maccoby & Wilson,

1957). In these studies, researchers noted that seventh-graders were most likely to identify with

characters most like them and that children were more likely to learn from characters whom they

perceived as similar to themselves and with whom they identified, particularly characters of the

same sex (Maccoby & Wilson, 1957). For example, boys were most likely to identify with male

characters, especially those who represented their “aspired social class,” and more likely to

remember aggressive content (Maccoby & Wilson, 1957). Girls were most likely to identify

10

Page 11: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

with female characters, especially those who represented their “aspired social class,” (p.86), and

were more likely to remember girl-boy interactive content (Maccoby & Wilson, 1957).

More recent studies of children’s observational learning from television have investigated

children’s and young adults’ wishful identification and identification with televised characters.

Wishful identification is “the desire to be like or behave in ways similar to the

character”(Feilitzen & Linne, 1975) p. 390. These studies have found that children show wishful

identification with same–sex characters and characters who children perceive to share their own

attitudes and attributes (Hoffner, 1996; Hoffner & Buchanan, 2005). Identification is “a

mechanism through which audience members experience reception and interpretation of the text

from the inside, as if the events were happening to them” (Cohen, 2001) p. 245. Other studies

have noticed the tendency for identification with same-sex characters (Calvert, Kotler, Zehnder,

& Shockey, 2003; Calvert, Murray, & Conger, 2004; Reeves & Miller, 1978), particularly for

preadolescent girls (Calvert et al., 2003).

Like early studies on observational learning, more recent research on wishful

identification and identification also has examined the specific attributes or features of televised

characters that leads children to identify with them. Research found elementary and middle

school-aged boys were more likely to identify with characters who were humorous, and girls

were more likely to identify with characters who they perceived as less like real-life people

(Reeves & Miller, 1978). A study of children ages 7 to 12 found both boys and girls were likely

to express wishful identification for male characters who were intelligent, and only girls

expressed wishful identification for male characters who were humorous and for female

characters who were attractive (Hoffner, 1996). In another study, researchers found older

11

Page 12: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

children identified with characters that exhibited prosocial conduct and noted that these

characters may possibly serve as positive role models (Calvert et al., 2004). A study of young

adults found young men were more likely to identify with male characters they perceived as

successful, intelligent, attractive, and admired, while young women were more likely to identify

with female characters they perceived as successful, intelligent, attractive, and admired (Hoffner

& Buchanan, 2005).

Wishful identification and identification may play a critical role in determining the

efficacy of using televised characters as role models to promote girls’ interest in SET and SET

careers. Understanding the specific attributes of televised portrayals of scientists, both male and

female, is important for determining how children respond to these media portrayals as role

models. In addition, understanding the specific gender stereotyped attributes of televised

portrayals of scientists is important for understanding how children view gender and science.

Research indicates that adolescents’ attitudes toward science is linked to their “acceptance of the

stereotyped view of male dominance in science” (Handley & Morse, 1984), and their interest in

SET is linked not only to gender and their self-concept, but also to their perceptions of scientists

(Lee, 1998a). One researcher posits that “students’ internalized meanings about self and others

in science help to explain gendered patterns in scientific and technological interests”(Lee,

1998b), p. 214, and these views then lead many girls to see themselves more as members of the

female sex rather than as science students, whom they perceive to be male (Lee, 1998a). Images

of female scientists that counter or dispel views of science as exclusively masculine are critical

for implementing changes in girls’ and boys’ perceptions of scientists and perceptions of self.

Hypotheses and Research Questions

12

Page 13: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

This study examined the portrayals of scientist characters in currently or recently

broadcast television programs that are popular among or likely to be watched by middle school-

aged children. Specifically, this research examined the frequency of male and female scientist

characters as well as the demographics, lifestyle, and gender stereotyped attributes in portrayals

of these characters. Based on a review of the literature, the following hypotheses and research

questions were posed:

H1: Portrayals of male scientist characters will be more frequent than portrayals of female scientist characters.

RQ1: Within program genre, does the proportion of male scientist characters differ from the proportion of female scientist characters?

H2: Male scientist characters will be more likely than female scientist characters to be portrayed in high-status scientific positions.

RQ2: Do program genre and biological sex of the character interact to predict the professional status of scientist characters?

H3: Female scientist characters will be more likely than male scientist characters to be portrayed as being married.

RQ3: Do program genre and biological sex of the character interact to predict the marital status of scientist characters?

H4: Female scientist characters will be more likely than male scientist characters to be portrayed as being a parent.

RQ4: Do program genre and the biological sex of the character interact to predict the parental status of scientist characters?

H5: The portrayals of the scientist characters will be gender stereotyped.

H5a: Female scientist characters will be more likely than male scientist characters to be portrayed with female gender stereotyped attributes.

H5b: Male scientist characters will be more likely than female scientist characters to be portrayed with male gender stereotyped attributes.

RQ5: Will the gender stereotyped attributes of male and female scientist characters vary by program genre?

13

Page 14: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Method

Program Sample

The sample consisted of television programs that featured scientist characters and were

determined to be popular among or likely to be seen by middle school-aged children. In an effort

to compile as comprehensive a list as possible, the researchers examined previous research that

identified television programs with scientist characters reported to be popular among middle

school-aged children (Steinke et al., 2007), data from Nielsen ratings listing the top television

programs watched by children ages 12 to17 (Nielsen Media Research, 2004), and a list of

television science programs sponsored by the National Science Foundation (National Science

Board, 2006). Based on this information, the following 14 television programs were identified

as those most likely to be popular among or seen by middle school-aged children: Bill Nye the

Science Guy, CSI, CSI- Miami, CSI-New York, Danny Phantom, Dexter's Laboratory,

DragonflyTV, Friends, Kim Possible, MythBusters, Strange Days at Blake Holsey High, The

Adventures of Jimmy Neutron: Boy Genius, The Simpsons, and The X Files.

To obtain the episodes for analysis, eight episodes of each program broadcast during

April and May 2006 were randomly selected. Twelve of the 14 programs were on air during

this time, and all but one program, CSI-New York2, aired multiple times a week during the two-

month sampling period. DragonflyTV was not aired in the local market during the sampling

period; however, copies were obtained from the program’s producer for all episodes for the year

and a half that the program had been in production, and eight episodes were randomly chosen.

Bill Nye the Science Guy was no longer being broadcast at the time of the study. However, all

2 At the time of this study, CSI-New York was only running one episode per week in the local TV market.

14

Page 15: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

episodes of this program are available for purchase, so eight episodes were randomly selected

from the program’s entire run. In all, 112 episodes were analyzed.

Scientist Character Coding

To be coded as a scientist character, a character had to be human, appear on screen and

speak, and the character had to meet one of the following criteria: (1) self-identify as a scientist3

or be identified by another character as a scientist, (2) wear a laboratory coat, or (3) perform at

least two of the following scientific activities—conduct an experiment, collect scientific samples

for analysis, or discuss scientific phenomena.

Intercoder reliability for identifying scientist characters was assessed using two trained

coders who independently coded one randomly selected episode of each program. Intercoder

reliability using Cohen’s kappa was .81. In all, 196 scientist characters were identified in the

sample.

Demographics and Lifestyle Characteristics Coding

Each scientist character was coded for the following demographics and lifestyle

characteristics: biological sex, race/ethnicity, age, professional status in the scientific workforce,

marital status, and parental status. These characteristics were selected to assess the portrayals of

scientist characters in television programs that are currently popular among or likely to be seen

by middle school-aged children and to compare these portrayals with earlier research on

portrayals of scientist characters on educational science programs (Long et al., 2001; Steinke,

1997, 1999, 2005; Steinke & Long, 1996).

3 For this study, we defined a scientist broadly to include scientists, engineers, research assistants, and teachers of science and engineering.

15

Page 16: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Biological Sex—Characters were coded as either a male or a female.

Race/Ethnicity—Characters were coded as belonging to one of the following groups: (1) White/Caucasian, (2) African-American/Black, (3) Hispanic/Latino, (4) Asian or Pacific Islander, (5) Native American, or (6) cannot determine.

Age—Characters were identified as either an (1) adult, (2) teenager or young adult, or (3) child. Adult characters were defined as in their mid-to-late 20s or older; teenage or young adult characters were defined as in their teens or early 20s; children were defined as younger than 13 years of age.

Professional Status in Scientific Workforce—Characters were identified as (1) a scientist or researcher; (2) student, research assistant, or technician; or (3) administrator. These portrayals were adapted from previous research on images of scientists (Steinke & Long 1996; Steinke 2005).

Marital Status—Characters were identified by their current marital status. Characters were identified as either married or not married if another character mentioned that character’s marital status.

Parental Status—Characters were identified as being a parent or not being a parent. Characters were identified as either a parent or not a parent if another character mentioned that character’parental status.

Two trained coders worked independently to code one episode of each program in order

to assess the intercoder reliability for the demographic variables. Reliability, as assessed using

Cohen’s kappa, was as follows: biological sex = .91, race/ethnicity = .77, age = .77, professional

status in scientific workforce = 1.0, marital status = .65, and parental status = .63. According to

(Landis & Koch, 1977), kappas in the .61 to .80 range indicate good agreement, and kappas

above .80 indicate very good agreement.

Gender Stereotype Attributes of Scientist Characters

The attributes or traits of each scientist character were coded on a per-scene basis to

determine whether the character exhibited six gender stereotypical attributes or behaviors. The

six traditional gender stereotyped attributes selected for this analysis were derived from previous

16

Page 17: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

research on gender stereotyping on television (Calvert et al., 2003), which had adapted these

attributes from the Bem Sex-Role Inventory scale (Bem, 1974).

Calvert et al. 2003 listed the following attributes as feminine: deferent-yielding-

dependent; affectionate-warm-gentle-compassionate; romantic; body conscious; harm avoidant;

shy. For this study, the attribute labels were modified slightly and the following attributes were

coded as feminine: dependence, caring, and romantic. The feminine attributes body conscious,

harm avoidant, and shy were not included for the study because few instances of these attributes

were found in preliminary viewing of the television programs or because of low intercoder

reliability.

Calvert et al. 2003listed the following attributes as masculine: dominant-aggressive;

autonomous-independent; analytic; competitive-ambitious; athletic; and heroic. For this study,

the attribute labels were modified slightly and the following attributes were coded as masculine:

independence, athletic, and dominance. The masculine attributes analytic, competitive-

ambitious, and heroic were not included in this study because few instances of these attributes

were found in preliminary viewing of the television programs or because of low intercoder

reliability.

Definitions were developed for the three feminine gender stereotyped attributes selected

for this study. Characters exhibited dependence when they were shown relying on others for

assistance (e.g., asked other characters for help completing a task, were shown physically unable

to accomplish a task and other characters came to their aid). Characters demonstrated caring

when they exhibited behaviors or made statements designed to comfort or help others in need

17

Page 18: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

(e.g., fed a character who was hungry, expressed sympathy or empathy for another character’s

plight, offered to help another character accomplish a task). Characters were coded as romantic

when they expressed feelings of intimate attraction to other characters (e.g., stated that they

loved another character) or demonstrated feelings of intimate attraction to another character

through their actions (e.g., kissed or held hands with another character).

Definitions were developed for the three masculine gender stereotyped attributes selected

for this study. Characters exhibited independence when they made statements or performed

actions indicating that they were not relying on others for assistance or advice, or when they

made statements or performed actions that showed they refused to do what another character had

told them to do. Characters exhibited dominance when they exerted authority or influence over

other characters (e.g., told or showed other characters what to do; told other characters that they

were wrong). Characters exhibited athleticism when they participated in a physical activity that

demonstrated strength and/or stamina, talked about participating in such an activity, or were

shown wearing athletic garb (e.g., football uniform, workout clothing).

For the above gender stereotyped attributes, the extent to which a scientist character

exhibited the attribute was determined by coding the number of scenes in which the character

demonstrated the attribute. For this study, a scene was defined as the environment in which

action was set. In other words, a scene was the location of the action in both space (e.g., a

kitchen, a car, an office) and time (e.g., morning, evening). A scene, therefore, ended when there

was a change in the location of the action or a change in time. To assess intercoder reliability for

scene identification, trained coders worked independently to identify scenes in one randomly

selected episode of each program. Because scene changes might manifest themselves differently

18

Page 19: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

across the variety of programs in our sample, intercoder reliability for scenes was assessed in

three groups: for live-action, narrative drama programs (CSI, CSI-New York, CSI-Miami, X

Files, Strange Days at Blake Holsey High, and Friends); for animated cartoon programs

(Dexter’s Laboratory, The Simpsons, Kim Possible, The Adventures of Jimmy Neutron: Boy

Genius, and Danny Phantom); and for science educational and demonstration programs

(MythBusters, DragonflyTV, and Bill Nye the Science Guy). Intercoder reliability statistics, as

assessed using Cohen’s kappa and Scott’s pi, indicated good reliability for scene identification

(live action, narrative programs = .82 to .87; animated programs = .72; and science

demonstration programs = .90).4 Scott’s pi is based on the same principles as kappa,

subsequently, the ranges provided by Landis and Koch (1977) also apply to this statistic.

To assess intercoder reliability for the scientists exhibiting gender stereotyped attributes

within scenes, trained coders coded all scientists who appeared within a randomly selected 15-

minute portion of a randomly selected episode of each program. Intercoder reliability statistics,

as assessed using Cohen’s kappa and Scott’s pi, were as follows: dependence = .66, caring = .

72, romantic = .80, independence = .54, dominance = .59, and athleticism = .62. In addition to

the kappa ranges discussed earlier in this paper, kappa and pi statistics in the .41 to .60 range

indicate adequate agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977).

Results

Descriptive Statistics for Scientist Characters

4 Reliability ranges are provided for some variables because multiple coding pairs participated in the reliability work. In some cases, the calculated statistics for all pairs were identical; in other cases, they were not.

19

Page 20: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Across the 112 episodes of the 14 programs in this study, 196 scientist characters were

identified. Of these, 113 (58%) were male and 83 (42%) were female. The race/ethnicity of the

scientist characters was as follows: White/Caucasian (72%), Black/African American (13%),

Hispanic Latino (7%), Asian/Pacific Islander (6%), Native American (1%), Cannot determine

(1%). In the programs analyzed, 56% of the scientist characters were adults, 25% were young

adults/college-aged, and 19% were children.

Hypotheses and Research Question Testing

Hypothesis 1 predicted that there would be more portrayals of male scientists than of

female scientists. This hypothesis was supported. There were significantly more male scientist

characters (n = 113) than female scientist characters (n = 83), χ2 (1) = 4.59, p < .05). Because

Hypothesis 1 essentially predicts that male scientists will have a larger presence on programs

than will female scientists, this hypothesis was tested by determining whether male scientists

were in more scenes than were female scientists. Results of this second test also support

Hypothesis 1. Male scientists were in significantly more scenes (M = 23.7 scenes) than female

scientists (M = 15.1 scenes), t (184.94) = 8.68, p < .05.5

To test research questions posed about program genre, the 14 programs were grouped

into four program genres, as shown in Table 1. However, situation comedies were removed from

the research question analyses because this program genre contained only five scientists. The

number of scientists in the other program genres was as follows: cartoon = 16, drama = 62, and

educational = 113.

5 The degrees of freedom for this hypothesis test were adjusted because the Levene’s test for equality of variances indicated that the variances in the two samples were not equal.

20

Page 21: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Insert Table 1 here

Research Question 1 asked whether, within program genre, the proportion of male

scientist characters differed from the proportion of female scientist characters. The proportion of

male and female scientist characters differed for cartoon and drama programs, but not for

educational programs (cartoons, χ2 (1) = 4.00, p < .05; dramas, χ2 (1) = 4.13, p < .05; educational,

χ2 (1) = 0.08, p = .78).

Hypothesis 2 predicted that male scientist characters would be more likely than female

scientist characters to be portrayed in high-status scientific roles. This hypothesis was not

supported. Male scientists were no more likely than female scientists to be portrayed in high-

status scientific positions, χ2 (1) = 1.04, p =0.31. For males, 80.5% were in high-status scientific

positions, and 74.4% of females were in high-status scientific positions.

Research Question 2 asked whether the program genre and the biological sex of the

character interact to predict the professional status of the scientist characters. A binary logistic

regression was used to test this research question. Results indicate that program genre and

gender did not interact to predict the professional status of the scientist characters, χ2 (1) = 0.61, p

= 0.44.

Hypothesis 3 predicted that female scientists would be more likely than male scientists to

be portrayed as married. This hypothesis was not supported. In fact, for the 42 adult and young

adult scientist characters for which marital status could be determined, more males (n = 12) were

portrayed as married than females (n = 8), though this difference was not significant (χ2 (1) =

0.31, p = 0.58.

21

Page 22: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Research Question 3 asked whether the program genre and the biological sex of the

character interact to predict the marital status of the scientist characters. A binary logistic

regression was used to test this research question. Program genre and gender did not interact to

predict the marital status of the scientist characters, χ2 (1) = 0.44, p = 0.51.

Hypothesis 4 predicted that female scientist characters would be more likely to be parents

than would male scientist characters. Counter to our prediction, more adult and young adult

male scientist characters (n = 8) were portrayed as parents than were adult and young adult

female scientist characters (n = 5), though, again, this difference was not significant (χ2 (1) =

0.16, p = 0.69).

Research Question 4 asked whether the program genre and the biological sex of the

character interact to predict the parental status of the scientist characters. A binary logistic

regression was used to test this research question. Program genre and gender did not interact to

predict the parental status of the scientist characters, χ2 (1) = 1.53, p = 0.22.

Hypotheses 5a and 5b investigated the extent to which female and male scientist

characters exhibited selected gender stereotypical behaviors. To test these hypotheses, the fact

that males were in more scenes than were females was controlled by using the mean percent of

scenes in which an attribute was present as the dependent variable. Each of the attributes was

tested individually because scales for the three female gender stereotypical behaviors (i.e.,

dependence, caring, and romantic) and the three male gender stereotypical behaviors (i.e.,

dominance, independence, and athleticism) were not reliable.

22

Page 23: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

The nonparametric Mann-Whitney U statistical test was used for Hypotheses 5a and 5b

because the data for the gender-stereotyped variables were not normally distributed. Hypothesis

5a, which predicted that female scientists would be more likely than male scientists to exhibit

female gender stereotyped behaviors, was not supported (See Table 2). Female scientist

characters did not exhibit caring, dependence, or romantic attributes in significantly higher mean

percentages of scenes than did male scientists. Hypothesis 5b, which predicted that male

scientists would be more likely than female scientists to exhibit male stereotyped behaviors, was

largely supported (See Table 2). Male scientist characters exhibited independence and

dominance in significantly higher mean percentages of scenes than did female scientist

characters; however, this did not hold for athleticism.

Insert Table 2 here

Research Question 5 investigated whether gender stereotypical behaviors varied by

television program genre. Because, as has been mentioned previously, the gender-stereotyped

behavior data were not normally distributed, a Scheire-Ray-Hare two-way ANOVA of ranks was

performed to test this research question. This test is more powerful than standard t-tests for non-

normally distributed data (Scheirer, Ray, & Hare, 1976). As Table 3 shows, gender stereotypical

behavior did not differ by program genre.

Insert Table 3 here

Discussion

This study examined gender-stereotyping in portrayals of scientist characters in television

programs popular among or likely to be viewed by middle school students. Male scientist

23

Page 24: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

characters outnumbered female scientist characters and were also found to be present in more

scenes than were female scientist characters. Earlier studies of children’s educational television

programs had found more male than female scientist characters (Long et al., 2001; Steinke &

Long, 1996) and had found male scientists shown more frequently on screen than female

scientists (Long et al., 2001). However, within program genre comparisons revealed a different

pattern -- cartoons and dramas had significantly more male scientist characters than female

scientist characters, while educational programs did not favor one gender over the other. These

findings that take the program genre into account appear to indicate that progress has been made

in ensuring equitable representation of male and female scientists, specifically for educational

programs; however, there was notable variance by individual program. For example, in Bill Nye

the Science Guy there were 18 females and 10 males; in Mythbusters there were 3 females and

16 males; and in DragonflyTV there were 55 females and 58 males. It is interesting to note that

the educational science programs where female scientist characters were equally represented or

represented in higher numbers than male scientist characters were programs that receive funding

from the National Science Foundation.

In addition to analyzing the distribution of the biological sex of the scientist characters,

this study also analyzed the characterization of television scientist characters. The analysis of

the scientist characters’ professional status, which serves as an indication of the importance of

characters’ role within the scientific community, found that across all programs female and male

scientist characters were just as likely to be portrayed in high-status occupational positions in the

scientific workforce. This finding differed from those of earlier studies of children’s educational

24

Page 25: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

television programs that showed female scientists were often more likely to be portrayed in

secondary roles such as research assistants and students (Steinke & Long, 1996).

The equity in scientific community status for female and male characters found in the

current study is encouraging and suggests greater breadth in the roles for female scientist

characters in current television programming. Although not formally coded in this study, the

following observations of interactions of male and female scientist characters in some of the

scenes analyzed in this study were indicative of this change in roles for female scientist

characters. In cartoon programs, for example, male scientist characters were shown being

challenged by female scientist characters like Dee Dee, Dexter’s sister in Dexter’s Laboratory,

or were shown being assigned to work with equally competent female classmates like Cindy in

The Adventures of Jimmy Neutron: Boy Genius. In drama programs, research pairs or teams of

male and female scientists were often shown being directed by female lead forensics

investigators in programs like CSI, CSI-Miami, CSI-New York. Similarly, educational programs

also showed research pairs or teams of females and males in Mythbusters or teams of female and

male child-age collaborators working on science projects or experiments in programs like

DragonflyTV or Bill Nye the Science Guy.

In examining variables related to the personal lifestyles of the scientist characters, male

and female scientist characters were portrayed similarly, indicating that traditional gender

stereotyping was not found. In fact, contrary to our predictions, more adult and young adult

male scientist characters were portrayed as married and as parents than adult and young adult

female scientist characters. Research on television dramas from the 1990s found that married

women on these programs were portrayed with fewer options, “for they can rarely successfully

25

Page 26: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

combine marriage and employment (Signorielli & Kahlenberg, 2001). While the findings of this

study may initially suggest greater sensitivity in televised portrayals of male scientists related to

work and family life issues, these results should be interpreted with caution because of the

relatively small number of any references at all to the scientist characters’ marital and parental

status in these television programs. The overall lack of attention and focus on work and family

life issues for scientist characters, male or female, reinforces traditional stereotypes of scientists

as individuals who devote their entire lives to their professional work at the expense of their

personal lives. It is possible that characterizations of scientists as not having personal

relationships and family may unintentionally keep adolescent girls, who are already thinking

about how to balance future professional and family roles, from seeing science as a viable career

for women.

The analysis of the gender stereotyped attributes found in portrayals of the scientist

characters featured in these 14 television programs showed gender stereotyping of male scientist

characters, but not of female scientist characters. Male scientist characters were more likely than

female characters to be shown with the masculine attributes or traits of independence and

dominance, but not athleticism. However, female scientist characters were not more likely than

male scientist characters to be portrayed with the traditional feminine attributes of dependence,

caring, and romantic. No differences were detected in female and male scientist characters for

the gender stereotyped attributes by program genre. Previous research on children’s programs

that included several different program genres in the analysis and examined a couple attributes

similar to those coded in this study found that female characters were more likely than male

characters across all genres to be shown as acting romantic and acting supportive(Leaper et al.,

26

Page 27: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

2002). Although not all traditional gender stereotyped-attributes could be coded in this analysis,

the findings for the gender stereotyped-attributes that were coded suggest that portrayals of

scientist characters with even just a couple traditionally masculine stereotyped traits along with

the absence of traditionally feminine stereotyped traits may serve to reinforce existing gender

schemas (Bem, 1981). Given the resilient nature of gender schemas (Durkin, 1985; Durkin &

Nugent, 1998; Martin & Charles F. Halverson, 1983; Nihlen & Bailey, 1988; Renn & Calvert,

1993; Signorella & Liben, 1984), direct and strong interventions that include

counterstereotypical portrayals of scientists may be needed in order to change perceptions of

gender roles and perceptions of gender and science.

While it was beyond the scope of this paper to assess the specific influence of viewing

images of scientists on middle school-aged children’s perceptions of scientists and their interest

in future SET careers, repeated viewing of images of scientists that predominantly show male

scientists and that include traditionally male attributes, even though small in number, reinforce

gender stereotypes of science as a masculine domain. Researchers have stated that “given that

film and television images are visually appealing, easily understood, and appear to be supported

by scientific authorities (e.g., NASA) through the use of science consultants, they have a high

potential to achieve high status in the minds of students” (Barnett et al., 2006, p. 189). In

addition, the gender and race of characters has been found to be particularly important in

assessing the effects of characters on adolescents (J. D. Brown & Pardun, 2004). Researchers

have noted that “race and gender are basic motivators for choice of television content, and that

adolescents may, indeed, be seeking models with whom they can identify as they develop a sense

of themselves in the larger culture” (J. D. Brown & Pardun, 2004, p. 275). This study examined

27

Page 28: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

only six different gender stereotyped attributes (three traditionally masculine and three

traditionally feminine) and found some evidence for a greater prevalence of the traditionally

masculine attributes of independence and dominance over the other attributes analyzed.

Additional research is needed to assess whether televised images of scientist characters with

masculine attributes rather than scientist characters with feminine attributes influences girls’

perceptions of the attributes and traits embodied by scientists and whether girls think women

scientists need to adopt these masculine attributes in order to succeed in science, engineering and

technological professions. Pre-adolescent and adolescent girls, who often are experiencing a

heightened sense of awareness of social roles and femininity during this time and who tend to

identify with feminine ways, may see this world of science as closed to them unless they are

willing to conform to masculine behaviors and traits.

Increasing the number of female scientist role models on the television programs most

often watched by pre-adolescent and adolescent girls may be one important factor for increasing

the number of girls interested in science and engineering careers. Television programming needs

to present positive portrayals of female scientists to promote observational learning and

identification in children as a way to promote interest in SET. As one researcher writes:

“Closing the gaps between girls and boys in SME interests probably entails closing the gaps

between gendered self-concepts and perceptions of SME disciplines” (Lee, 1998a), p. 214.

Limitations of the Study

One of the limitations of the study was the fact that only six gender-stereotyped attributes

were coded. The selection of these gender-stereotyped attributes was carefully determined based

28

Page 29: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

on which ones were most likely to be shown in the programs selected for analysis, but ideally, all

gender-stereotyped attributes would have been coded. Future research should include the use of

other measures of gender stereotyping in televised content. This study attempted to develop a

gender-stereotyped scale or index for ascertaining the relative degree of gender stereotyping in

the portrayals of scientist characters. However, each attribute had to be tested individually

because scales for the three female gender stereotypical behaviors (i.e., dependence, caring, and

romanticism) and the three male gender stereotypical behaviors (i.e., dominance, independence,

and athleticism) were not reliable. Further research is needed to explore other ways of

measuring gender-stereotyped behaviors and traits in television characters.

It is important to note that the gender stereotyped attributes found were present in very

low amounts in the programs studied. This could be a function of how these variables were

operationalized (i.e., we may have missed some major ways that these variables would manifest

themselves in the programs we studied) or it could be a function of them just not being there. In

the latter case, it is important to note that even when a significant difference between male and

female scientists was found for the gender-stereotyped attributes, the attributes are present in

such small amounts that the likelihood of them affecting a viewer may be low.

One point to note about the second test of Hypothesis 1 (i.e., where the mean number

of scenes for male and female scientists was compared) is that this test may have a bias. That is,

neither the scenes in the programs nor the programs themselves were of uniform length. Thus, it

is possible that a scientist who was in a longer program that had many scenes would be present in

a higher number of scenes than a scientist who was in a shorter program that had fewer scenes.

However, there is no reason to expect that the number of scenes in a program or the length of

29

Page 30: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

those scenes would vary as a function of whether male or female scientists are present.

Consequently, the comparison of the mean number of scenes that male and female scientists

appear in is a reasonable test of Hypothesis 1.

Directions for Future Research

One of the unique contributions of this research was the focus on television programs

currently found to be popular among or likely to be viewed by middle school-aged students.

Continuous replication and extension of earlier research on televised images of scientists is

needed given changes in media content and the potential influence of these images on children’s

perceptions of science and scientists.

Another unique contribution of this research was the focus on different television

program genres. Future research on images of scientists on television should continue to take

program genre into account given differences in viewer preferences by program genre, portrayals

of characters, and program content. Future research with this focus also is important because

studies indicate gender differences in children’s preference for television programs with boys

reporting liking cartoons, situation comedies, and entertainment, and more action-oriented shows

and girls reporting liking cartoons, entertainment shows, and teenage drama and people-oriented

shows (Cherney & London, 2006).

Future research needs to carefully examine the impact of viewing television scientist

characters with gender-stereotyped attributes not only on middle school-aged children’s

perceptions of gender roles, but also on their attitudes toward science, interest in future SET

careers, and perceptions of women in SET. A number of theoretical perspectives in the field of

30

Page 31: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

communication, such as social learning theory (Albert Bandura, 1986), cultivation theory

(Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, & Signorielli, 1994), wishful identification (Hoffner, 1996; Hoffner &

Buchanan, 2005), identification (Calvert et al., 2003), and parasocial interaction (Hoffner, 1996),

all suggest the potential influence of televised images on children’s learning and perceptions.

Carefully designed studies are needed to tease out the specific influence of an array of gender-

stereotyped attributes of scientist characters in order to determine the specific impact of these

images on middle school-aged children’s perceptions of scientists and their views of themselves

as potential scientists in the future.

31

Page 32: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Table 1. Television Programs Selected for Analysis by Program Genre

Cartoons Dramas Educational Situational Comedy

Danny Phantom CSI Bill Nye the Science Guy

Friends

Dexter’s Laboratory CSI-Miami DragonflyTV The Simpsons

Kim Possible CSI- New York Mythbusters

The Adventures of Jimmy Neutron: Boy Genius

Strange Days at Blake Holsey High

The X Files

32

Page 33: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Table 2. Gender Stereotyped Attributes for Male and Female Scientists

Attributes Male Scientists (n = 113) Female Scientists (n =83) Mann-Whitney U

Mean % of Scenes

Stand. Dev. Mean % of Scenes Stand. Dev.

Caring .02 .05 .04 .16 4466.50

Dependence .01 .06 .01 .05 4337

Romantic .02 .07 .04 .14 4465.50

Athleticism .08 .23 .09 .26 4573

Dominance .20 .23 .15 .25 3867.50*

Independence .03 .08 .03 .13 3921*

*p < .05.

33

Page 34: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Table 3. Gender Stereotypical Behavior by Program Genre

Gender Stereotype Attribute

Mean % of Scenes for Cartoons

Mean % of Scenes for Dramas

Mean % of Scenes for Educational Programs

Scheire-Ray-Hare Test of Ranks

Female (n = 4)

Male(n = 12)

Female (n = 23)

Male(n = 39)

Female (n = 55)

Male(n = 58)

Dependent .05 .04 .02 .01 .00 .00 .03

Caring .04 .03 .09 .04 .02 .00 .21

Romantic .03 .06 .12 .01 .00 .00 .47

Athletic .00 .05 .00 .00 .14 .14 .18

Dominant .30 .30 .35 .28 .06 .13 .59

Independent .14 .12 .08 .05 .00 .00 .08

34

Page 35: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

References

American Association of University Women. (1998). Gender gaps: Where schools still fail our children. Washington, D.C.: American Association of University Women.

American Association of University Women. (2000). Tech-savvy: Education girls in the new computer age. Washington, D.C.: AAUW Education Foundation.

Arnold, K. D. (1993). Undergraduate aspirations and career outcomes of academically talented women: A discriminant analysis. Roeper Review, 15(3), 169-175.

Aubrey, J. S., & Harrison, K. (2004). The gender-role content of children's favorite television programs and its links to their gender-related perceptions. Media Psychology, 6(2), 111-146.

Bandura, A. (1969). Social-learning theory of identificatory processes. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 213-262). Chicago,IL: Rand McNally College Publishing Company.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (2002). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 121-153). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963a). A comparative test of the status envy, social power, and secondary reinforcement theories of indentificatory learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(6), 527-534.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963b). Imitation of film-mediated agressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66(1), 2-11.

Barnett, M., Wagner, H., Gatling, A., Anderson, J., Houle, M., & Kafka, A. (2006). The impact of science fiction film on student understanding of science. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 15(2), 179-191.

Baylor, A. L. (2004). What we know (and don't) regarding the effectiveness of pedagogical agents. Paper presented at the Training for Tomorrow.

Baylor, A. L., & Plant, A. E. (2005). Pedagogical agents as social models for engineering: The influence of agent appearance on female choice. Paper presented at the Artificial Intelligence in Education Amsterdam.

35

Page 36: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162.

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354-364.

Brown, J. D., & Pardun, C. J. (2004). Little in common: Racial and gender differences in adolescents' television diets. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48(2), 266-278.

Brown, L. M., & Gilligan, C. (1992). Meeting at the crossroads: Women's psychology and girls' development. New York: Ballantine Books.

Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106(4), 676-713.

Calvert, S. L., Kotler, J. A., Zehnder, S. M., & Shockey, E. M. (2003). Gender stereotyping in children's reports about educational and informational televised programs. Media Psychology, 5, 139-162.

Calvert, S. L., Murray, K. J., & Conger, E. E. (2004). Heroic DVD portrayals: What US and Taiwanese adolescents admire and understand. Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 699-716.

Cherney, I. D., & London, K. (2006). Gender-linked differences in the toys, television shows, computer games, and outdoor activities of 5- to 13-year-old children. Sex Roles, 54, 717-726.

Cohen, J. (2001). Defining identification: A theoretical look at the identification of audiences with media characters. Mass Communication & Society, 4(3), 245-264.

Congressional Commission on the Advancement of Women and Minorities in Science Engineering and Technology Development. (2000). Land of plenty: Diversity as America's competitive edge in science, engineering and technology. Washington, D.C.

Corbett, J. B. (2001). Women, scientists, agitators: Magazine portrayal of Rachel Carson and Theo Colborn. Journal of Communication, 51(4), 720-747.

Durkin, K. (1985). Television and sex-role acquisition 1: Content. British Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 101-113.

Durkin, K., & Nugent, B. (1998). Kindergarten children's gender-role expectations for television actors. Sex Roles, 38(5/6), 387-402.

36

Page 37: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Edwards, L. D. (1999). Using multimedia to counter stereotypes in science classrooms: New perceptions of who becomes a scientist. Paper presented at the National Convention of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology, Houston, TX.

Eisenstock, B. (1984). Sex-role differences in children's identification with counterstereotypical televised portrayals. Sex Roles, 10(5/6), 417-430.

Feilitzen, C. v., & Linne, O. (1975). The effects of television on children and adolescents: Identifying with television characters. Journal of Communication, 25(4), 51-55.

Fujioka, Y. (1999). Television portrayals and African-American stereotypes: Examination of television effects when direct contact is lacking. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 76(1), 52-75.

George, R. (2000). Measuring change in students' attitudes toward science over time: An application of latent variable growth modeling. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 9(3), 213-225.

Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1994). Growing up with television: The cultivation perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 17-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gibson, H. L., & Chase, C. (2002). Longitudinal impact of an inquiry-based science program on middle school students' attitudes toward science.

Handley, H. M., & Morse, L. W. (1984). Two-year study related adolescents' self-concept and gender role perceptions to achievement and attitudes toward science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 21(6), 599-607.

Hoffner, C. (1996). Children's wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 40, 389-402.

Hoffner, C., & Buchanan, M. (2005). Young adults' wishful identification with television characters: The role of perceived similarity and character attributes. Media Psychology, 7(4), 325-351.

LaFollette, M. C. (1981). Wizards, villains, and other scientists: The science content of television for children: Report prepared for Action for Children's Television, Newton, MA.

LaFollette, M. C. (1988). Eyes on the stars: Images of women scientists in popular magazines. Science, Technology and Human Values, 13(3/4), 262-275.

LaFollette, M. C. (1990). Making science our own: Public images of science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

37

Page 38: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Landis, R. J., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159-174.

Leaper, C., Breed, L., Hoffman, L., & Perlman, C. A. (2002). Variations in the gender-stereotyped content of children's television cartoons across genres. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(8), 1653-1662.

Lee, J. D. (1998a). Which kids can "become" scientists? Effects of gender, self-concepts, and perceptions of scientists. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61(3), 199-219.

Lee, J. D. (1998b). Which kids can "become" scientists? Effects of gender, self-concepts, and perceptions of scientists. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61(3), 199-219.

Long, M., Boiarsky, G., & Thayer, G. (2001). Gender and racial counter-stereotypes in science education television: A content analysis. Public Understanding of Science, 10, 255-269.

Maccoby, E. E., & Wilson, W. C. (1957). Identification and Observational Learning from Films. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 76-87.

Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41(9), 954-969.

Martin, C. L., & Charles F. Halverson, J. (1983). The effects of sex-typing schemas on young children's memory. Child Development, 54, 563-574.

National Science Board. (2003). The science and engineering workforce: Realizing America's potential: National Science Foundation.

National Science Board. (2006). Science and Engineering Indicators 2006. Arlington, VA.

Nelkin, D. (1987). Selling science: How the press covers science and technology. New York: W.H. Freeman.

Nielsen Media Research. (2004). Broadcast rank 9/22/2003-5/26/2004, Primetime Daypart, ages 12-17.

Nihlen, A. S., & Bailey, B. A. (1988). Children's display of gender schemas through interaction with nontraditional workers. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 19(2), 155-162.

Orenstein, P. (1994). School girls: Young women, self-esteem, and the confidence gap. New York, NY: Doubleday.

Reeves, B., & Miller, M. M. (1978). A multidimensional measure of children's identification with television characters. Journal of Broadcasting 22(1), 71-86.

38

Page 39: Gender Stereotypes of Scientist Characters in Television Programs

Renn, J. A., & Calvert, S. L. (1993). The relation between gender schemas and adults' recall of stereotyped and counterstereotyped televised information. Sex Roles, 28(7/8), 449-459.

Ruvolo, A. P., & Markus, H. R. (1992). Possible selves and performance: The power of self-relevant imagery. Social Cognition, 10(1), 95-124.

Scheirer, C. J., Ray, W. S., & Hare, N. (1976). The analysis of ranked data derived from completely randomized factorial design. Biometrics, 32(2), 429-434.

Signorella, M. L., & Liben, L. S. (1984). Recall and reconstruction of gender-related pictures: Effects of attitude, task difficulty, and age. Child Development, 55, 393-405.

Signorielli, N. (1993). Television, the portrayal of women, and children's attitudes. In G. L. Berry & J. K. Asamen (Eds.), Children & television: Images in a changing sociocultural world (pp. 229-242). Newbury Park, NJ: Sage Publications, Inc.

Signorielli, N., & Kahlenberg, S. (2001). Television's world of work in the nineties. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 45(1), 4-22.

Steinke, J. (1997). A portrait of a woman as a scientist: Breaking down barriers created by gender-role stereotypes. Public Understanding of Science, 6, 409-428.

Steinke, J. (1999). Women scientist role models on screen: A case study of Contact. Science Communication, 21(2), 111-136.

Steinke, J. (2005). Cultural representations of gender and science: Portrayals of female scientists and engineers in popular films. Science Communication, 27, 27-63.

Steinke, J., Lapinski, M., Zietsman-Thomas, A., Nwulu, P., Crocker, N., Williams, Y., et al. (2007). Middle School-Aged Children's Attitudes toward Women in Science, Engineering and Technology and the Effects of Media Literacy Training. Journal of Women and Minorities in Science and Engineering, 12(4), 295-323.

Steinke, J., & Long, M. (1996). A lab of her own? Portrayals of female characters on children's educational science programs. Science Communication, 18(2), 91-115.

Weingart, P., Muhl, C., & Pansegrau, P. (2003). Of power maniacs and unethical geniuses: Science and scientists in fiction film. Public Understanding of Science, 12, 279-287.

39