how neuroscience contributes to better public policies for early childhood

56
HOW NEUROSCIENCE CONTRIBUTES TO BETTER PUBLIC POLICIES FOR EARLY CHILDHOOD Charles A. Nelson III, Ph.D. Professor of Pediatrics and Neuroscience Harvard Medical School Richard David Scott Chair, Boston Children’s Hospital Harvard Center on the Developing Child SÃO PAULO , Brazil 12 September 2012

Upload: fundacao-maria-cecilia-souto-vidigal

Post on 22-Mar-2016

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Pol

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

HOW NEUROSCIENCE CONTRIBUTES TO BETTER PUBLIC POLICIES FOR EARLY CHILDHOOD

Charles A. Nelson III, Ph.D.

Professor of Pediatrics and Neuroscience

Harvard Medical School

Richard David Scott Chair,

Boston Children’s Hospital

Harvard Center on the Developing Child

SÃO PAULO , Brazil 12 September 2012

Page 2: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

I. How experience affects brain development

II. The effects of early psychosocial deprivation on brain-behavioral development

OUTLINE

Page 3: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Genetics: supplies basic blueprint for brain development.

• Experience: adjusts the blue print and shapes the architecture of its neural circuits, according to the needs and distinctive environment of the individual

PRINCIPLES OF BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

Page 4: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

15 1/2 WKS 22 WEEKS 23 WEEKS ~25 WEEKS

http://medstat.med.utah.edu

27 weeks Full term brain Adult

Page 5: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

PART II: HOW Does the structure of experience

work its way into the structure of the brain?

Page 6: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Genetics specifies the properties of neurons and neural connections to different degrees in different pathways and at different levels of processing.

But, because many aspects of an individual’s world are not predictable, the circuitry of the brain relies on experience to customize connections to serve the needs of the individual.

Experience shapes these neural connections and interactions but always within the constraints imposed by genetics

INDIVIDUALITY IS THE PRODUCT OF BOTH PERSONAL EXPERIENCES & BIOLOGICAL INHERITANCE

Page 7: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Specific experiences vary enormously under identical environmental conditions, depending on the history, maturation, and state of the individual’s brain

Brain maturity has impact on experience:

Different areas of the nervous system mature at different rates

Lower level processing areas mature earlier than those at a higher level.

Thus, a less mature brain is affected largely by more fundamental features of the environment, such as patterned light or the speech train.

EXPERIENCE IS THE PRODUCT OF AN ONGOING, RECIPROCAL INTERACTION BETWEEN THE ENVIRONMENT

AND THE BRAIN

Page 8: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• As the brain matures and changes with experience, more detailed aspects of the environment influence it.

• Thus, as an individual’s brain changes, particularly during the early developmental periods, the same physical environment can result in very different experiences.

CON’T

Page 9: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

I. The impact of experience on the brain is not constant throughout life.

II. Early experience often exerts a particularly strong influence in shaping the functional properties of the immature brain.

III. Many neural connections pass through a period during development when the capacity for experience-driven modification is greater than it is in adulthood.

IV. Such phases are referred to as sensitive or critical periods.

CONCLUSIONS

Page 10: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Standard way to examine role of experience on development is to observe what happens when experience is lacking.

• In study will talk about, have focused on early psychosocial deprivation, as this provides a good model system for the millions of children around the world who experience neglect

WHAT HAPPENS WHEN CRITICAL EXPERIENCES ARE LACKING AT CRITICAL PERIODS OF DEVELOPMENT?

Page 11: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

THE EFFECTS OF EARLY PSYCHOSOCIAL DEPRIVATION THE BUCHAREST EARLY INTERVENTION PROJECT ON BRAIN AND BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT

Page 12: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Brain requires wide-ranging input (e.g., linguistic, cognitive, social, etc.) during critical periods of development.

• Experiences exist on a continuum: some children reared in environments that provide all their needs and others in environments that provide next to none of their needs

• Study of institutionalized children represents a model system for understanding role of experience in development, as such children are generally profoundly deprived of most experiences.

BACKGROUND

Page 13: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

CHILDREN REARED IN INSTITUTIONS

• Children raised in institutions are at dramatically increased risk for a variety of social and behavioral problems:

• Disturbances of social relatedness and attachment

• Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

• Deficits in IQ and executive functions • Syndrome that mimics autism • Growth stunting (next slide)

CHILDREN REARED IN INSTITUTIONS

Page 14: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

14 year old girl 17 year old girl

Page 15: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Insensitive care • regimented daily schedule • non-individualized care

• Isolation • no response to distress • unchecked aggression

• Lack of psychological investment by caregivers • rotating shifts • high child/caregiver ratio

WHY INSTITUTIONAL REARING MIGHT BE BAD FOR THE BRAIN

Page 16: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood
Page 17: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• IQ

• Brain

BASELINE FINDINGS (BEFORE RANDOMIZATION – MEAN AGE=22 MONTHS)

Page 18: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Smyke et al (2007), J. of Child Psych. & Psychiatry

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT (AT BASELINE)

Page 19: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

NIG

institutionalized children

never institutionalized children

Marshall, Fox, et al (2004) J. of Cog Neuro

EEG ACTIVITY AT BASELINE

Page 20: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• IQ

• Brain

INTERVENTION EFFECTS

Page 21: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

Me

an

De

velo

pm

en

tal Q

uo

tie

nt

(DQ

)

Care as usual group

Foster care group

Never institutionalized group 86

103

42 months

77

WHAT HAPPENS TO DQ AT 42 MONTHS OF AGE FOR ALL THREE GROUPS?

Page 22: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Age at placement

70

75

80

85

90

95

0-18 18-24 24-30 30+

De

velo

pm

en

tal Q

uo

tie

nt

(DQ

)

N 0-18 14 18-24 16 24-30 22 30+ 9

Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke A, & Guthrie D (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science, 318, 1937-1940.

HOW DOES DEVELOPMENT QUOTIENT DIFFER FROM CHILDREN IN FOSTER CARE AS A FUNCTION OF AGE

ENTRY?

Page 23: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

2.44μV2

3.80μV2

CAUG FCG > 24

NIG

FCG < 24

Vanderwert et al (2010) PLoS One

DOES BRAIN ACTIVITY (EEG) CHANGE AS A FUNCTION OF INTERVENTION AND TIMING?

Page 24: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Institutionalization has a very detrimental effect on cognitive and brain development.

• Foster care appears to be effective in improving cognitive function and EEG for those children placed before age 2.

• Duration of time in foster care does not influence timing effects.

• Thus, timing is everything.

SUMMARY

Page 25: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Perhaps children have fewer brain cells (neurons) or connections between brain cells (synapses)

• Performed Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) when children were 8-10 years old

3 Tesla

Magnet

8 Year Old Child

MRI Testing

Head Coil

HOW TO ACCOUNT FOR REDUCTION IN BRAIN ACTIVITY?

Page 26: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

*

TOTAL CORTICAL GREY MATTER

Page 27: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

*

Sheridan, M.A., Fox, N.A., Zeanah, C.H., McLaughlin, K.A., & Nelson, C.A. (2012). Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

TOTAL CORTICAL GREY MATTER

Page 28: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Exposure to institutionalization early in life….

• Leads to reductions in brain size (which may account for the reduction in brain activity we have observed in the EEG and perhaps reduction in head circumference).

• Has a differential effect on gray vs. white matter, with

• Cortical grey matter being unaffected by foster care intervention whereas

• White matter volume is increased by foster care intervention

• Thus, profound early neglect leads to dramatic changes in brain structure. White, but not gray matter shows improvement after placement in family care.

CONCLUSIONS

Page 29: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

ATTACHMENT IN BEIP

Page 30: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

SECURE VS. INSECURE AT BASELINE

Page 31: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

ns

Χ 2 =

10.58

p = .001

ns

SECURE VS. INSECURE BY AGE OF PLACEMENT 42 MONTHS

Page 32: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Caregiver/mother and child answer door (pre-arranged).

• RA: “Come with me, I have something to show you.”

• Walk out the door and around the corner to find RA from previous home visit.

A FUNCTIONAL LOOK AT ATTACHMENT: STRANGER AT THE DOOR AT 54 MONTHS

Page 33: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

54 months 8 years

STRANGER AT THE DOOR BY GROUP

Page 34: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Are two types of RAD:

• Emotionally withdrawn/Inhibited

• Indiscriminately social/Disinhibited

PATHOLOGICAL FORM OF ATTACHMENT: REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER

Page 35: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER

Page 36: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

Baseline 30 mos 42 mos 54 mos 8 years

CAU

FCG

NIG

RAD WITHDRAWN/DEPRESSED CAREGIVER REPORT

Page 37: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

Baseline 30 mos 42 mos 54 mos 8 years

CAU

FCG

NIG

RAD DISINHIBITED CAREGIVER REPORT

Page 38: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Assessment Age

EFFECTS ON TIMING OF PLACEMENT ON INDISCRIMINATE BEHAVIOR

Page 39: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Attachment is profoundly compromised in institutionalized children

• Foster care ameliorates some attachment disturbances

• Earlier foster care is disproportionately effective

CONCLUSIONS

Page 40: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

PSYCHIATRIC MORBIDITY

Page 41: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• The caregiver was administered the Romanian version of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (Romanian PAPA) when the children in the BEIP were 54 months old.

• The PAPA is a comprehensive structured psychiatric interview assessing mental health symptoms and disorders in children ages 2 through 5 years old.

INCIDENCE OF MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS

Page 42: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

Did the foster care intervention decrease the rates of psychiatric disorders for ever institutionalized children?

INCIDENCE OF MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS

Page 43: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0%

5%

10%

15%

20%

25%

ADHD

IG FCG NIG

No difference between

IG and FCG

ADHD

Page 44: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0%

2%

4%

6%

8%

10%

12%

14%

16%

ODD/CD

IG FCG NIG

No difference between

IG and FCG

DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR DISORDERS

Page 45: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0%

1%

2%

3%

4%

5%

Depression

IG FCG

DEPRESSION

Page 46: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

0%

5%

10%

15%

20%

25%

30%

35%

40%

45%

Any anxiety disorder

IG FCG NIG

ANY ANXIETY DISORDER

Page 47: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

GENDER DIFFERENCES AND TOTAL PSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMS

Page 48: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Higher rates of psychiatric disorders and impairment in children who have been institutionalized compared with children in the community.

• No significant difference in rates of ADHD or disruptive behavior disorders in children in the institution versus those in foster care.

• Children in the foster care group had significantly lower rates of emotional disorders, anxiety disorders, RAD, and impairment (trend) than children who remained in the institution.

• Being a girl may be a protective factor.

• No evidence of effects of timing on rates of psychopathology or impairment. Zeanah CH, Egger HL, Smyke AT, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, & Guthrie D (2009). Altering early experiences reduces psychiatric disorders among institutionalized Romanian preschool children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 777-785.

CONCLUSIONS

Page 49: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Brain development begins a few weeks after conception

• Most of brain architecture laid down late prenatal and first few postnatal years but adult brain not evident (at least based on MRI data) till early adulthood

• Early experience exerts powerful effect on brain and behavioral development, during so-called sensitive periods

• What happens early can have a lasting impact many years later, including adult mental and physical health

CONCLUSIONS

Page 50: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

POLICY CHANGES AS A RESULT OF BEIP

Page 51: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Passed legislation forbidding institutionalization of any child <2 years (unless severely handicapped)

• Began to close down institutions (from 100,000 children in 2000 to <30,000 in 2011)

• Began network of foster care

CHANGES IN ROMANIA

Page 52: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Based on the scientific findings of BEIP

• Our foster care program is being exported to several countries

• Is a movement towards closing down institutions

• The EU recently (June, 2012) was considering legislation that would lead to abandoning the practice of institutionalizing parentless children throughout Europe

CHANGES ELSEWHERE IN THE WORLD

Page 53: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• In Romania, rush to deinstitutionalize children meant Government hadn’t considered changes in child welfare and child protection. Thus, many children were placed with their biological families who didn’t want them, yet there was little monitoring of these placements

• Government established foster care but failed to put the resources necessary to create good foster care, and as a result, many gov’t foster care homes were not ideal

• Government failed to put effort in preventing child abandonment

• Under political pressure, Government (in 2002) banned all international adoption. Since there was little in the way of domestic adoption, this led to many children being placed in foster care, which is really a temporary placement.

UNINTENDED CONSEQUENCES: UNANTICIPATED POLICY CHANGES

Page 54: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

• Let’s learn from the science

• Let’s inform policy makers, politicians, and clinicians about the disadvantages of raising children in institutions and the benefits of raising them in families

• And let’s inform these same groups that there are lessons learned from BEIP that extend to all children at risk for substandard parental care. – Duration of time spent in early adversity powerfully influences later

development

– Age of child when removed from adverse environment and placed in “good” home also critical in influencing outcome

– Early adversity can have life-long effects on both psychological and physical development; thus

• First years of life are important

BOTTOM LINE

Page 55: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

EFFECTS OF EARLY EXPERIENCE LAST A LIFE TIME

Page 56: How Neuroscience Contributes to Better Public Policies for Early Childhood

THE END