ilness perception

Upload: bitter-moon

Post on 03-Apr-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    1/11

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/Journal of Health Psychology

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/1359105311416874

    2012 17: 399 originally published online 22 August 2011J Health PsycholHannah DeJong, Jessica Hillcoat, Sarah Perkins, Miriam Grover and Ulrike Schmidt

    Illness perception in bulimia nervosa

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:Journal of Health PsychologyAdditional services and information for

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This? - Aug 22, 2011OnlineFirst Version of Record

    - Mar 15, 2012Version of Record>>

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399.refs.htmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/08/19/1359105311416874.full.pdfhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/08/19/1359105311416874.full.pdfhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399.full.pdfhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/08/19/1359105311416874.full.pdfhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399.full.pdfhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.sagepublications.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/399http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    2/11

    Journal of Health Psychology

    17(3) 399408

    The Author(s) 2011

    Reprints and permission:

    sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/1359105311416874

    hpq.sagepub.com

    Illness perception refers to the cognitive

    representations a person has about their ill-

    ness. One of the most influential models of

    illness perception is the Self Regulatory Model

    (SRM) (Leventhal et al., 1997; 2003; 1984),

    which proposes five major components of ill-

    ness perception: identity, timeline, conse-

    quences, causes and cure. Identity refers torepresentations about the nature of the illness,

    the symptoms associated with it and the labels

    attached to the illness. Timeline includes

    cognitions about the duration or chronicity of

    the illness, and also about whether it has a

    cyclical course. The consequences compo-

    nent concerns perceptions about the severity

    of the illness and the impact it has on general

    functioning. Causes refers to the persons

    ideas about what factors are responsible forcausing the illness. Cure is sometimes also

    labelled as controllability, and involves cog-

    nitions about the extent to which the illness is

    amenable to control or cure, either through

    ones own personal efforts or through treat-

    ment. The consistency and validity of these

    proposed components has been confirmed by

    several reviews (Skelton and Croyle, 1991).

    Illness perception is considered an important

    element of understanding peoples experience

    of their illness, and is closely related to severalimportant outcomes, such as coping, function-

    ing and emotional distress. Patients who per-

    ceive their illness as being chronic, having

    serious consequences and not being responsive

    to control or cure are likely to display poor

    physical and social functioning, passive coping

    styles and a high degree of distress (Heijmans,

    Illness perception inbulimia nervosa

    Hannah DeJong, Jessica Hillcoat, Sarah Perkins,

    Miriam Grover and Ulrike Schmidt

    AbstractThe study was designed to extend our understanding of illness perceptions in patients with bulimia nervosa

    (BN). Seventy-eight participants with BN or BN-type Eating Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (EDNOS-BN)

    completed the Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R) (Moss-Morris et al., 2002). Clinical

    variables were also assessed. Participants experienced their ED as chronic, with serious consequences and

    high associated levels of anxiety and depression. The disorder was attributed primarily to psychological

    causes. The results indicate the perceived severity of BN, and high level of associated distress. These findings

    highlight the potential for targeting illness perceptions in treatment.

    Keywords

    bulimia nervosa, eating disorders, illness perceptions

    Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London, UK

    Corresponding author:

    Hannah DeJong, Institute of Psychiatry, Box P059, De

    Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK.

    Email: [email protected]

    16874HPQXXX10.1177/1359105311416874DeJong et al.Journal of Health Psychology

    Article

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    3/11

    400 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

    1998; 1999; Scharloo et al., 1998; Vaughan

    et al., 2003). Illness perceptions are also

    related to expectations about treatment,

    engagement and adherence with treatment and

    treatment outcomes. For example, following a

    myocardial infarction, patients are more likely

    to make healthy lifestyle changes and attend

    rehabilitation sessions if they believe that

    their condition is amenable to control or cure,

    and if they relate its causality to their lifestyle

    (Cooper et al., 1999; Petrie et al., 1996,

    Weinman et al., 2000). Understanding and tar-

    geting patients perceptions of their illnesses

    may therefore have clinical benefits.

    The Illness Perception Questionnaire and its

    revised version (IPQ and IPQ-R; Moss-Morris

    et al., 2002, Weinman et al., 1996) are based on

    the SRM, and have been developed as methods

    for assessing illness perception in large groups.

    The IPQ-R contains nine subscales, based

    around the components of Leventhals SRM:

    identity, timeline-duration, timeline-cyclical,

    consequences, personal control, treatment con-

    trol, emotional representations, illness coher-

    ence and causes. The emotional representations

    subscale was added to the revised version, in

    recognition that emotional aspects of illness

    perception are neglected in the SRM, which

    focuses exclusively on cognitive representa-

    tions (Moss-Morris et al., 2002). Illness coher-

    ence was also added, as a measure of the extent

    to which people feel their illness perceptions

    provide a coherent and useful understanding of

    their illness.

    Illness perceptions may help to account for

    the high degree of ambivalence about change

    and reluctance to engage with treatment that are

    common barriers to treatment of BN (Killick

    and Allen, 1997, Vitousek et al., 1998). For

    example, a perceived lack of amenability to

    control or cure may explain reluctance to

    engage in treatment. BN is also associated with

    high levels of distress, maladaptive coping

    strategies (Troop et al., 1994) and poor reported

    quality of life (Mond et al., 2005). These factors

    have been shown to be associated with illnessperceptions, such as perceived chronicity and

    serious consequences, in other patient groups.

    We might therefore predict similar patterns of

    illness perception in people with BN. Previous

    work by Mond et al. (2008), using a clinical

    vignette to elicit illness perceptions, indicates

    that women with bulimic disorders tend to

    regard these disorders as difficult to treat and

    prone to relapse. They also perceived these dis-

    orders as distressing and most commonly

    named low self-esteem as a likely cause.

    A handful of studies have previously used

    the IPQ-R to assess illness perception in people

    with eating disorders. Holliday et al. (2005)

    examined illness perception in people with ano-

    rexia nervosa (AN) and also evaluated lay peo-

    ples perceptions of AN. They showed that

    people with AN perceived their illness as

    chronic, distressing, having serious negative

    consequences and being resistant to control or

    cure. In contrast, lay people were more optimis-

    tic, rating the illness as less chronic and more

    amenable to control or treatment. Both groups

    endorsed primarily emotional and psychologi-

    cal causes. Stockford et al. (2007) used a modi-

    fied version of the IPQ-R in a mixed eating

    disorder (ED) group, and related this measure

    to a readiness to change scale. They found that

    these two scales were closely related, with high

    levels of emotional distress and perceived nega-

    tive consequences predicting increased readi-

    ness to change.

    Three studies by a Spanish group (Quiles

    and Terol, 2010, Quiles Marcos et al., 2007;

    2009) have used a translated and modified ver-

    sion of the IPQ-R in mixed ED groups and also

    the relatives of these patients. IPQ-R scores

    were related to emotional adjustment and psy-

    chosocial adaptation, levels of distress, depres-

    sion and anxiety. In particular, patients who

    viewed their illness as highly distressing and

    chronic, with a large number of associated

    symptoms and low treatment control, experi-

    enced more psychological distress. Conversely,

    patients who perceived their illness to be sub-

    ject to a high degree of personal control and

    curability had lower levels of depression andanxiety. There was also a positive relationship

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    4/11

    DeJong et al. 401

    between the degree of dissimilarity in patients

    and relatives views of the illness, and patient

    levels of distress, depression and anxiety.

    In previous studies of illness perception, BN

    has tended to be under-represented, with only

    small numbers of participants with BN included

    in mixed ED samples. The aim of the present

    study was to assess illness perception in a larger

    group of individuals with BN and relate this to

    clinical measures, including symptomatology,

    duration of illness, anxiety and depression. We

    hypothesized that individuals with BN would

    describe their disorder as distressing and

    chronic, with serious negative consequences,

    and that they would view it as being resistant to

    control or cure. We hypothesized that strong

    perceptions of this nature would be associated

    with higher levels of anxiety and depression,

    more severe ED symptoms and a longer dura-

    tion of illness.

    Method

    Participants

    Participants were recruited to take part in atreatment study designed to assess the effec-

    tiveness of a CD-ROM based cognitive behav-

    ioural intervention for BN and EDNOS-BN

    (Schmidt et al., 2008). Of the 97 participants

    in the CD-ROM study, 78 completed meas-

    ures for the present study. Participants were

    recruited from consecutive referrals to the

    adult Eating Disorder Outpatients Service in

    the South London and Maudsley National

    Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust dur-ing 20032006.

    Inclusion criteria for the study were a diagno-

    sis of BN or EDNOS-BN, confirmed by a senior

    clinician, as well as key bulimic behaviours (ie

    bingeing and compensatory behaviours) at a

    minimum average frequency of once per week

    over the preceding three months. Exclusion cri-

    teria were insufficient knowledge of English or

    poor literacy skills, severe learning disability,

    anorexia nervosa, severe depression, acute sui-cidality and alcohol or substance dependence.

    Exclusion criteria were assessed via clinical

    interview. Patients on antidepressant medication

    were included, provided they had been on a

    stable dose for a minimum of four weeks prior

    to assessment. All participants provided writ-

    ten informed consent and ethical approval for

    the study was obtained from the joint research

    ethics committee of the Institute of Psychiatry

    and the South London and Maudsley NHS

    Foundation Trust.

    Measures

    The questionnaires were given as part of a

    larger battery of questionnaires associated with

    the CD-ROM study. All measures reported herewere taken at baseline. Clinical interviews were

    used to determine diagnosis and duration of ill-

    ness. Measures of current height and body

    weight were used to calculate Body Mass Index

    (BMI; kg/m2).

    Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R).

    Patients perceptions of BN were assessed using

    the IPQ-R (Moss-Morris et al., 2002), which

    measures patients cognitive and emotionalrepresentations of their illness. It consists of

    nine subscales: identity, timeline-duration,

    timeline-cyclical, consequences, personal con-

    trol, treatment control, emotional representa-

    tions, illness coherence and causes. The

    questionnaire has good internal reliability, dis-

    criminant validity and predictive validity

    (Moss-Morris et al., 2002).

    Illness identity is measured by the number of

    symptoms that participants report having expe-rienced and consider to be related to their eating

    disorder. A total score was calculated, with each

    item coded as yes = 1 or no = 0, giving a maxi-

    mum possible score of 14. Cause is assessed

    by asking participants to rate the extent to which

    they agree or disagree that listed factors caused

    their illness. This gives mean score for each

    causal item (where 1 = strongly disagree and 5

    = strongly agree). We also calculated the per-

    centage of participants who either agreed orstrongly agreed that each item played a causal

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    5/11

    402 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

    role in their illness. The remaining seven sub-

    scales were scored by calculating a mean score

    from all the relevant items. Each item is rated

    from 1 to 5 (where 1 = strongly disagree and

    5 = strongly agree) giving a maximum possible

    score of five for each subscale (see Appendix 1

    for sample items).

    Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS).

    The HADS is a 14-item scale that measures

    anxiety and depression (Zigmond and Snaith,

    1983). This scale has been used with a wide

    variety of patient groups, and gives clinically

    meaningful results when used as a psychologi-

    cal screening tool or in correlational studies

    (Herrmann, 1997).

    Eating Disorder Examination (EDE). The EDE

    (Cooper and Fairburn, 1987) is a reliable

    interview-based assessment of bulimic symp-

    tomatology with good discriminant validity

    and satisfactory internal reliability (Cooper

    et al., 1989). It measures frequency of physi-

    cal symptoms (bingeing, self-induced vomit-

    ing, laxative use, diuretic use and excessive

    exercise), as well as thoughts and attitudesrelated to eating disorders. Four subscale

    scores are generated: dietary restraint, eating

    concern, shape concern and weight concern.

    The mean of these scores can be given as a

    global indicator of eating disorder pathology.

    Analysis

    Data were analysed with SPSS Version 15.0 for

    Windows. Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests showed

    that the IPQ-R subscales were non-normally

    distributed and so non-parametric tests wereused throughout the analysis. Spearmans rho

    correlation coefficients were used to investigate

    relationships between the IPQ-R and clinical

    variables and among the IPQ-R subscales.

    Results

    Participant characteristics

    Out of the 78 participants, 49 were diagnosedwith bulimia nervosa and 29 were diagnosed

    with EDNOS-BN. Four males and 74 females

    participated in the study. Clinical and demo-

    graphic characteristics of the sample are shown

    in Table 1. Median values for both HADS anxiety

    and depression were above the clinical cut-off

    for probable anxiety and depression (Zigmond

    and Snaith, 1983).

    Illness perception questionnaire

    All subscales of the IPQ-R showed good inter-

    nal reliability. Cronbachs alpha values ranged

    from 0.73 (consequence subscale) to 0.90 (ill-

    ness coherence subscale).

    Table 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics.

    Sample characteristic Median Inter-quartile range Range

    Age (years) 26.0 22 31 17 51

    Duration of illness (years) 7.0 3 12.50 0.5 32.0

    BMI 21.70 20.50 24.60 18.83 42.35

    EDE dietary restraint 3.40 2.75 4.40 0.2 5.8

    EDE eating concern 2.80 1.60 3.85 0.0 5.75

    EDE shape concern 4.38 3.12 5.19 0.0 6.0

    EDE weight concern 3.60 2.40 4.60 0.4 6.0

    EDE global score 3.41 2.60 4.36 0.35 5.47

    HADS anxiety 12.0 11.0 13.0 4 16HADS depression 11.0 9.0 12.0 4 16

    Notes: BMI = Body Mass Index; EDE = Eating Disorder Examination; HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    6/11

    DeJong et al. 403

    Identity. All symptoms were endorsed by at

    least one participant as being related to their ill-

    ness but overall participants appeared to have a

    relatively low illness identity, with a mean score

    of 7.8 [SD = 3.40].

    The symptoms most commonly associated

    with BN were fatigue (80.9%), loss of strength

    (76.1%), weight loss (74.2%), dizziness

    (72.9%) and upset stomach (70.0%). Two of the

    least frequently related symptoms were sore

    eyes (18.8%) and wheeziness (10.8%).

    Timeline, control, consequences, illness coherence

    and emotional representations. For all of the

    previously mentioned subscales, the maximum

    possible score is five. The median scores and

    inter-quartile ranges were as follows: timeline-

    chronic median = 3.33, IQR = 0.83; timeline-

    cyclical median = 3.75, IQR = 1.25; personal

    control median = 3.67, IQR = 0.83; treatment

    control median = 3.8, IQR = 0.6; consequences

    median = 3.83, IQR = 0.83; and illness coher-

    ence median = 3.25, IQR = 1.4; emotional

    representations median = 4.08, IQR = 0.71.

    Causes. Seventeen of the 18 possible causes

    were endorsed as contributory factors in the

    development of the illness (see Table 2).

    The most strongly endorsed were emotional

    state (endorsed by 87.3% of participants), own

    behaviour (79.5%) and stress/worry (78.2%).

    The lowest ranked causes were pollution in the

    environment (0%), altered immunity and germ/

    virus (both 2.6%).

    Correlations between IPQ-R subscales. There were

    several significant correlations between subscales

    of the IPQ-R. Emotional consequences were pos-

    itively correlated with identity [r = .370, p =

    .010], consequences [r= .355,p = .002] and ill-

    ness coherence [r= .308,p = .008]. Consequences

    were also correlated with identity [r= .332,p =

    .021] and treatment control [r= .278,p = .016].

    Treatment control and personal control were cor-

    related with one another [r= .326,p = .004].

    Correlations between IPQ-R subscales and clinical

    measures. Relationships between the IPQ-R

    subscales and clinical measures were also

    Table 2. Causal attributions about bulimia nervosa.

    Proposed cause Percentage of participants whoagree or strongly agree (%)

    Mean score (SD)

    Emotional state 87.3 4.4 (0.67)

    Own behaviour 79.5 4.1 (0.98)

    Stress/worry 78.2 4.0 (0.95)

    Mental attitude 75.7 4.1 (0.98)

    Diet/eating habits 75.7 4.2 (0.98)

    Personality 65.4 3.7 (1.20)Family problems and worries 55.1 3.6 (1.21)

    Overwork 34.6 2.8 (1.34)

    Alcohol 16.6 2.0 (1.24)

    Ageing 15.4 2.1 (1.28)

    Smoking 11.6 1.8 (1.12)

    Chance/bad luck 9.0 1.9 (1.03)

    Poor medical care in my past 6.4 1.8 (0.98)

    Accident/injury 5.1 1.6 (0.84)

    Altered immunity 2.6 1.8 (0.98)

    Germ/virus 2.6 1.39 (0.74)

    Pollution in environment 0 1.37 (0.51)

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    7/11

    404 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

    investigated. BMI was negatively correlated

    with perceived illness identity, personal control

    and negative consequences. Illness duration

    was negatively correlated with perceived treat-

    ment control. Anxiety was positively correlated

    with perceived chronicity, negative conse-

    quences, emotional representations and illness

    coherence. All subscales of the EDE and the

    global EDE score were positively correlated

    with emotional representations. The EDE die-

    tary restraint subscale was positively correlated

    with illness identity, while the EDE shape con-

    cern subscale was negatively correlated with

    perceived personal control (see Table 3).

    Discussion

    This study extends previous findings of illness

    perceptions in the eating disorders to a large,

    clinical sample of outpatients with BN and

    EDNOS-BN. Participants with these disorders

    perceived their illness as moderately chronic,

    with a degree of variation over time. They

    reported strong negative consequences of the

    disorder and a very high degree of related

    emotional distress. This was consistent with

    the high levels of anxiety and depression

    reported. However, perceptions of control

    were also quite high, suggesting that partici-

    pants felt able to manage the eating disorder to

    some degree and were optimistic about the

    utility of treatment. This pattern of findings is

    similar to that found in patients with AN

    (Holliday et al., 2005), with the exception that

    BN is perceived as being more cyclical. This

    fits intuitively with the binge-purge cycle

    associated with this disorder.

    Participants in the present study associated

    only a moderate number of symptoms with their

    eating disorder. The number of symptoms

    endorsed was considerably lower than for the

    AN sample in Holliday et al.s (2005) study.

    This could suggest a fairly weak illness identity,

    but may actually be a reflection of the use of the

    unadapted IPQ-R. Several previous studies

    have used altered versions of this measure, withthe addition of more symptoms that are related

    to eating disorders (e.g. dry and rough skin,

    irregularities in menstruation). The results

    reported here, using the unaltered version, may

    therefore underestimate the number of symp-

    toms associated with BN. However, the scores

    reported here are similar to those found by

    Quiles Marcos et al. (2007) across all ED diag-

    noses, using an adapted version of the IPQ-R.

    In the present study, participants primarily

    attributed their eating disorders to psycholog-

    ical factors, such as emotional state and stress/

    worry, but also seemed to view the disorder as

    partially self-inflicted (eg endorsing own

    behaviour as a causal factor). The perceived

    role of psychological factors coheres well

    both with the results of previous studies

    (Quiles Marcos et al., 2007; 2009) and with

    current models of EDs (Fairburn et al., 2003;

    Schmidt and Treasure, 2006; Sassaroli and

    Ruggiero, 2005). The causal attributions

    endorsed for BN are also similar to those

    reported for AN (Holliday et al., 2005; Quiles

    Marcos et al., 2007).

    Although the role of biological factors in the

    development and maintenance of bulimia ner-

    vosa has been emphasized in much previous

    research (Kaye, 2008; Mathes et al., 2009;

    Monteleone and Maj, 2008; Steiger and Bruce,

    2007), participants in the current study did not

    strongly endorse these causes. This may par-

    tially be attributable to the wording of the IPQ-

    R. However, given that participants strongly

    endorsed psychological and self-inflicted dis-

    order explanations, we might hypothesize that

    they tend to underestimate the role of biologi-

    cal factors factors that may be less stigmatis-

    ing than psychological and self-inflicted causal

    factors. This seems to be the case for AN,

    with one study showing that student nurses

    expressed more positive attitudes towards peo-

    ple with AN when given information about the

    biological and genetic causes of AN, than when

    given information about sociocultural influ-

    ences (Crisafulli et al., 2008). Therefore, edu-

    cating patients about the role of genetic and

    biological variables in the onset of eating dis-orders may be beneficial.

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    8/11

    DeJong et al. 405

    There were several relationships found

    between different aspects of illness perception.

    A strong illness identity, perceived negative

    consequences and a coherent illness under-

    standing were all associated with greater emo-

    tional responses to the disorder. It seems likely

    that experiencing more symptoms and negative

    consequences related to ones disorder contrib-

    utes to a more negative affective response.

    Personal and treatment control were alsorelated, implying that personal management of

    symptoms and any treatment provided may

    be seen as complementing one another.

    Surprisingly, participants who viewed their dis-

    order as having serious consequences were

    more likely to perceive treatment as being ben-

    eficial. It may be the case that this surprising

    relationship reflects perceived need for special-

    ist treatment, or ease of accessing treatment

    those who view their eating disorder as havingsevere consequences may be more likely to

    both recognize a need for professional help and

    be offered this.

    Illness perceptions were also related to sev-

    eral clinical variables in this study. Participants

    who experienced more symptoms, as measured

    by the EDE, had stronger emotional responses

    to the disorder. These emotional responses were

    also associated with high levels of anxiety.

    Participants with a coherent illness perception,

    who viewed their disorder as chronic and hav-ing serious consequences also had elevated

    anxiety levels. The generally elevated anxiety

    and depression levels in this sample indicate

    that BN is a disorder with serious emotional

    consequences and a high degree of associated

    distress. These results also cohere well with

    previous findings that illness perceptions are

    related to anxiety levels and psychological dis-

    tress (Quiles Marcos et al., 2007).

    Low BMI was associated with high illnessidentity and increased negative consequences,

    Table 3. Correlations between IPQ-R subscales and clinical measures.

    Clinicalmeasure

    IPQ-R subscale

    Illnessidentity

    Timelinechronic

    Timelinecyclical

    Treatmentcontrol

    Personalcontrol

    Consequences Emotionalrepresentations

    Illnesscoherence

    Illnessduration

    .078 .194 .011 .259* .139 .186 .196 .014

    BMI .301* .123 .028 .202 .276* .354** .176 .057

    HADS anxietyscore

    .218 .262* .027 .186 .034 .313** .450** .280**

    HADSdepressionscore

    .076 .002 .186 .039 .056 .029 .034 .094

    EDE dietaryrestraint

    .298* .186 .202 .092 .049 .045 .337** .033

    EDE eatingconcern

    .238 .076 .157 .064 .104 .038 .270* .043

    EDE shapeconcern

    .267 .050 .068 .100 .237* .007 .333** .123

    EDE weightconcern

    .163 .122 .119 .034 .139 .015 .248* .097

    EDE globalscore

    .279 .036 .188 .067 .175 .013 .351** .083

    p* < .05, p** < .01Notes: HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; EDE = Eating Disorder Examination; BMI = Body Mass Index.

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    9/11

    406 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

    but also with greater perceived personal

    control. It may be that patients who are better

    able to limit the frequency of their binge epi-

    sodes, or to maintain a high level of dietary

    restraint between binge episodes, are likely to

    both maintain a lower BMI and perceive them-

    selves as having a high degree of control over

    their eating behaviours. A high degree of die-

    tary restraint is likely to have various negative

    physical effects. This may help to explain why

    patients at lower BMIs tend to perceive their

    disorder as having severe negative conse-

    quences and why they endorse experiencing

    more physical symptoms related to their disor-

    der (ie have a strong illness identity).

    Interestingly, longer illness duration was asso-

    ciated with low perceived treatment control. This

    relationship was also present in the data collected

    by Stockford et al. (2007), suggesting that this is

    a strong and reliable association. It is possible

    that this reflects the likelihood that chronically ill

    patients have experienced unsuccessful treatment

    previously. This relationship has clinical implica-

    tions, as patient expectations have been demon-

    strated to be closely related to treatment outcomes

    (Greenberg et al., 2006). This suggests a need for

    motivational work with chronically ill patients in

    order to encourage these individuals to engage

    with treatment. The tendency for high levels of

    shape concern to be related to low perceived per-

    sonal control also has clinical implications, indi-

    cating a possible need for a stronger focus on

    these body shape concerns in treatment, aimed at

    increasing patients perceived ability to manage

    these concerns.

    Strengths and limitations

    This study extends previous findings in AN and

    mixed ED samples to a group of patients meet-

    ing criteria for BN or EDNOS-BN. The relative

    lack of previous data on illness perceptions in

    this patient group makes the study an important

    addition to this area of research. The large sam-

    ple size and availability of clinical information

    (illness duration, symptom levels etc.) are alsostrengths. An important limitation is the use of a

    clinical sample of treatment-seeking individu-

    als. This group may not be representative of

    many individuals with BN, as a large propor-

    tion of this population do not seek treatment for

    their eating disorder (Kendler et al., 1991).

    Previous research suggests that poor recogni-

    tion of eating disordered behaviours is associ-

    ated with low treatment seeking in women

    with bulimic-type EDs (Mond et al., 2006).

    Therefore, we predict that individuals who seek

    treatment for BN are likely to have a greater

    understanding and awareness of their disorder,

    and may also view their disorder as more ame-

    nable to control or cure than those who do not

    seek treatment.

    Conclusion

    It seems that illness perception in BN is broadly

    similar to that found in groups of patients with

    AN. Patients view their disorder as fairly

    chronic, with serious negative consequences

    and a high degree of related emotional distress.

    Illness perceptions are related to clinical meas-

    ures, such as symptom severity and anxiety levels.

    These findings have implications for clinical

    practice, in terms of identifying and targeting

    factors that may hinder treatment progress.

    Future extensions of this work could seek to

    establish causality in the relationships between

    illness perception and clinical variables, by

    examining change in these measures over time.

    The role of illness perceptions in recovery, and

    ability of various interventions to alter these

    perceptions, should also be considered.

    References

    Cooper Z and Fairburn C (1987) The eating disorder

    examination: A semi-structured interview for the

    assessment of the specific psychopathology of

    eating disorders.International Journal of Eating

    Disorders 6(1): 18.

    Cooper A, Lloyd GS, Weinman J and Jackson G

    (1999) Why patients do not attend cardiac reha-

    bilitation: role of intentions and illness beliefs.

    Heart82(2): 234236.

    Cooper Z, Cooper P and Fairburn C (1989) The

    validity of the eating disorder examination and

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    10/11

    DeJong et al. 407

    its subscales. The British Journal of Psychiatry

    154(6): 807812.

    Crisafulli MA, Von Holle A and Bulik CM (2008)

    Attitudes towards anorexia nervosa: The impact

    of framing on blame and stigma. International

    Journal of Eating Disorders 41(4): 333339.Fairburn CG, Cooper Z and Shafran R (2003) Cog-

    nitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A

    transdiagnostic theory and treatment. Behav-

    iour Research and Therapy 41(5): 509528.

    Greenberg RP, Constantino MJ and Bruce N (2006)

    Are patient expectations still relevant for psycho-

    therapy process and outcome? Clinical Psychol-

    ogy Review 26(6): 657678.

    Heijmans M (1998) Coping and adaptive outcome in

    chronic fatigue syndrome: importance of illness

    cognitions. Journal of Psychosomatic Research45(1): 3951.

    Heijmans MJWM (1999) The role of patients illness

    representations in coping and functioning with

    Addisons disease. British Journal of Health

    Psychology 4(2): 137149.

    Herrmann C (1997) International experiences with

    the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-A

    review of validation data and clinical results.

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research 42(1): 1741.

    Holliday J, Wall E, Treasure J and Weinman J

    (2005) Perceptions of illness in individuals withanorexia nervosa: A comparison with lay men

    and women.International Journal of Eating Dis-

    orders 37(1): 5056.

    Kaye W (2008) Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia

    nervosa.Physiology & Behavior94(1): 121135.

    Kendler K, Maclean C, Neale M, Kessler R, Heath

    A and Eaves L (1991) The genetic epidemiology

    of bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 148(12):

    16271637.

    Killick S and Allen C (1997) Shifting the Balance

    motivational interviewing to help behaviourchange in people with bulimia nervosa. Euro-

    pean Eating Disorders Review 5(1): 3341.

    Leventhal H, Benyamini Y, Brownlee S, Diefenbach

    M, Leventhal E, Patrick-Miller L and Robitaille

    C (1997) Illness representations: Theoreti-

    cal Foundations. In: Petrie KJ and Weinman J

    (eds)Perceptions of Health and Illness: Current

    Research and Applications. Amsterdam: Har-

    wood Academic Publishers, 1945.

    Leventhal H, Brissette I and Leventhal EA (2003)

    The common-sense model of self-regulation ofhealth and illness. In: Cameron LD (ed.) The

    Self-regulation of Health and Illness Behaviour.

    New York City, NY: Routledge, 4265.

    Leventhal H, Nerenz DR and Steele DS (1984) Ill-

    ness representations and coping with health

    threats. In: Baum A, Taylor SE and Singer JE

    (eds) Handbook of psychology and health.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 219252.

    Mathes WF, Brownley KA, Mo X and Bulik CM

    (2009) The biology of binge eating. Appetite

    52(3): 545553.

    Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B and Owen C (2006)

    Self-recognition of disordered eating among

    women with bulimic-type eating disorders: A

    community-based study. International Journal

    of Eating Disorders 39(8): 747753.

    Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B and Owen C (2008)

    Eating disorders mental health literacy: Whatdo women with bulimic eating disorders think

    and know about bulimia nervosa and its treat-

    ment?Journal of Mental Health 17(6): 565575.

    Mond JM, Owen C, Hay PJ, Rodgers B and Beumont

    PVJ (2005) Assessing quality of life in eating

    disorder patients. Quality of Life Research 14(1):

    171178.

    Monteleone P and Maj M (2008) Genetic suscep-

    tibility to eating disorders: Associated poly-

    morphisms and pharmacogenetic suggestions.

    Pharmacogenomics 9(10): 14871520.Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie K, Horne R,

    Cameron L and Buick D (2002) The Revised Ill-

    ness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R).Psychol-

    ogy & Health 17(1): 1 16.

    Petrie K, Weinman J, Sharpe N and Buckley J (1996)

    Role of patients view of their illness in predict-

    ing return to work and functioning after myo-

    cardial infarction: A longitudinal study. British

    Medical Journal312(7040): 11911194.

    Quiles Marcos Y, Terol Cantero MC, Romero Esco-

    bar C and Pagn Acosta G (2007) Illness per-ception in eating disorders and psychosocial

    adaptation. European Eating Disorders Review

    15(5): 373384.

    Quiles Marcos Y, Weinman J, Terol Cantero MC

    and Belendez Vazquez M (2009) The dissimi-

    larity between patients and relatives percep-

    tion of eating disorders and its relation to patient

    adjustment.Journal of Health Psychology 14(2):

    306312.

    Quiles Y and Terol M (2010) Using common sense

    model in eating disorders.Journal of Health Psy-chology 16(2): 208216.

    at Alexandru Ioan Cuza on March 14, 2013hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/http://hpq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Ilness perception

    11/11

    408 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

    Sassaroli S and Ruggiero GM (2005) The role of

    stress in the association between low self-esteem,

    perfectionism, and worry, and eating disorders.

    International Journal of Eating Disorders 37(2):

    135141.

    Scharloo M, Kaptein AA, Weinman J, Hazes JM,Willems LNA, Bergman W and Rooijmans GM

    (1998) Illness perceptions, coping and func-

    tioning in patients with rheumatoid arthritis,

    chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and

    psoriasis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    44(5): 573585.

    Schmidt U and Treasure J (2006) Anorexia nervosa:

    Valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal

    maintenance model and its implications for

    research and practice.British Journal of Clinical

    Psychology 45(3): 343366.Schmidt U, Andiappan M, Grover M, Robinson S,

    Perkins S, Dugmore O, et al. (2008) Randomised

    controlled trial of CD-ROM-based cognitive-

    behavioural self-care for bulimia nervosa.British

    Journal of Psychiatry 193(6): 493500.

    Skelton JA and Croyle RT (eds) (1991)Mental Rep-

    resentation in Health and Illness. New York:

    Springer-Verlag.

    Steiger H and Bruce K (2007) Phenotypes, endo-

    phenotypes, and genotypes in bulimia spectrum

    eating disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry52(4): 220227.

    Stockford K, Turner H and Cooper M (2007) Ill-

    ness perception and its relationship to readiness

    to change in the eating disorders: A preliminary

    investigation. British Journal of Clinical Psy-

    chology 46(2): 139154.

    Troop NA, Holbrey A, Trowler R and Treasure JL(1994) Ways of coping in women with eating

    disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental

    Disease 182(10): 535540.

    Vaughan R, Morrison L and Miller E (2003) The

    illness representations of multiple sclerosis and

    their relations to outcome. British Journal of

    Health Psychology, 8(3): 287301.

    Vitousek K, Watson S and Wilson GT (1998)

    Enhancing motivation for change in treatment-

    resistant eating disorders. Clinical Psychology

    Review 18(4): 391420.Weinman J, Petrie K, Sharpe N and Walker S (2000)

    Causal attributions in patients and spouses fol-

    lowing first-time myocardial infarction and

    subsequent lifestyle changes. British Journal of

    Health Psychology 5(3): 263273.

    Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Moss-Morris R and Horne R

    (1996) The illness perception questionnaire: A new

    method for assessing the cognitive representation

    of illness.Psychology & Health 11(3): 431445.

    Zigmond AS and Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anx-

    iety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand67(6): 361370.

    Appendix I. Sample items from the IPQ-R.

    Subscale Item

    Timeline duration My illness will last for a long time.

    Timeline cyclical I go through cycles in which my illness gets better and worse.

    Consequences My illness has major consequences on my life.

    Personal control I have the power to influence my illness.

    Treatment control My treatment can control my illness.Emotional representations When I think about my illness I get upset.Illness coherence My illness doesnt make any sense to me.