journal of literacy research 1990 golden 203 19

Upload: alexandru-rou

Post on 03-Jun-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    1/18

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/Journal of Literacy Research

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1080/10862969009547707

    1990 22: 203Journal of Literacy ResearchJoanne M. Golden and Annyce Gerber

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text: The Picture Story Book Event

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Literary Research Association

    can be found at:Journal of Literacy ResearchAdditional services and information for

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This?

    - Sep 1, 1990Version of Record>>

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203http://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.nrconline.org/http://jlr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jlr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jlr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://jlr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.refs.htmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.full.pdfhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.full.pdfhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.full.pdfhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://jlr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://jlr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.nrconline.org/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/content/22/3/203http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    2/18

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    1990, VolumeXXII No. 3

    A SEMIOTIC PERSPECTIVE OF TEXT:

    THE PICTURE STORY BOOK EVENT

    Joanne M. Golden

    University of Delaware

    Annyce Gerber

    Indian Oasis Baboquivari School District, Sells, Arizona

    ABSTRACT

    The focus of this paper is on the exploration of the nature of the picture story book

    event from a semiotic perspective. One purpose of the study was to examine how

    pictures and words relate in picture books. A second purpose was to explore how

    one picture story book was constructed during teacher-student-text interaction in a

    second-grade classroom. The classroom event involved an oral reading and discus-

    sion of the text, interpretations of the main character's traits, letters to the author,

    and illustrations of dream worlds. The picture book was viewed as a potential text

    in terms of verbal, visual, and performance cues and as a realized text in terms of

    students' interpretations. The inquiry illustrates the value of semiotic theory for

    providing insights into the nature of text and text events.

    Text is a term which evokes various mean ings reflecting particular disciplinary

    perspectives. Text has been represented in cognitive psychology as the author's

    propositions, in ethnopoetics as the union of story and performance, in social

    semiotics as a social exchange of m eanings, in poststructuralist theory as productiv-

    ity, and so forth. For others, including som e deconstructionists, the text is illusory,

    everywhe re yet nowh ere fully present. The picture story book for children offers an

    interesting case for illuminating dimensions of text. This is evident for at least two

    reasons. First, contributions of the visual and verbal in the picture book result in a

    "great efficiency in communicating meaning . . . high semantic or semiotic capac-

    ity" (Landes, 1987, p. 320). Second, books for young children both historically

    and currently are illustrated (Node lman, 1988). Thus , they merit close examination

    3

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    3/18

    20 4

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    because they provide children with an introduction to and foundation for written

    narratives.

    In the picture story book, the potential sign, the network of cue systems avail-

    able to the reader/listener, is characterized by dual sign systems of verbal language

    and the language of the image. Author and illustrator (who may or may not be

    synonymous) contribute verbal and visual cues which interconnect in various ways

    to convey story inform ation. Because the picture book is often read by an adult to a

    child who is in the process of acquiring skills for constructing texts, the adult's

    rendition of the written text provides another type of cue which guides the child

    listener. The reader/perfo rmer interprets the text through a variety of paralinguistic,

    kinesic, and proxemic cues reflecting his or her own interpretation of the text for a

    particular aud ience.

    Thus, performed text integrates the reader's oral text, the author's written text,

    and the illustrator's visual text. From this view, the narrow conception of text as

    author's text is thus extended into the sphere of the social context of the reading

    event where the text is a display of author's, illustrator's, and performer's cues.

    Another dimension of the event is the adult's mediation of the text expressed in the

    talk about text that may accompany the reading of the story, such as asking ques-

    tions and/or commen ting on certain aspects of the boo k. To a great degree, picture

    books are performed texts which function to acculturate children to literacy as well

    as to entertain them. Thus, the picture story book event with its multiple, intersect-

    ing symbo l systems can provide insights into the nature of texts and the processes

    related to literacy acquisition.

    The purpose of the present paper is to examine the nature of the picture story

    book symbol. More specifically, one goal is to consider the symbol of the picture

    book and the ways in which w ords and pictures relate. A second goal is to explore

    potential and realized signs by analyzing how a picture book symbol is transformed

    during teacher-student-text interaction in a second-grade classroom. Theoretical

    constructs from semiotic theory which contribute to the exploration of text events

    frame the present inquiry. These constructs are discussed in the following section.

    SEMIOTIC THEORY

    Central to the process of semiosis is the triadic relationship involving the sign,

    the interpretant, and the object. A sign is defined by Peirce (1955) as "something

    which stands to somebody for something in some respect or capacity" (p. 99). In

    the mind of the interpreter the sign generates an equivalent sign, the interpretant,

    which stands for an object. The object in Peirce's view is not a concrete thing, but

    rather a "so rt of i de a" (p. 99 ). When the triadic relation is applied to the narrative

    symb ol, the sign en comp asses the textual cues which guide the reader in construct-

    ing an interpretant, a particular story world. The object or idea of the sign is the

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    4/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text

    20 5

    sequence of events to which the story world refers (Scholes, 1982). In a fictional

    narrative, such as Potter's (1902)The Ta le of Peter Rabbit, the story events refer to

    themselves rather than to a series of events w hich actually occurred. In non-fictional

    narratives, such as historical accounts and biographies, the sequence of events

    constructed by the reader refers to actual events.

    An important dimension of the triadic relation is the notion of the sign as

    generative in that it spawns a series of other signs which, in turn, function as signs.

    Peirce (1955) characterized this process in the following way: "in use and in

    experience, its [a sign's] meaning grows" [and] "lives in the minds of those who

    use it" (p. 115). This conception of the symbol is echoed in Ingarden's (1973)

    depiction of the symbol of the literary work as a "living being" (p. 344) with

    different phases in its life. The picture book narrative, composed by an author and

    illustrator, functions as a symbol which grows in the minds of the respondents in

    different contexts. The equivalent sign created in the mind of the audience is an

    interpretant which also functions as a symbol (Peirce, 1955). The symbol can grow

    in the mind of the same reader over various readings as well as in the minds of

    different readers. If the symbol occurs in a social process, other readers can in-

    fluence the development of the interpretant. A picture book narrative, then, has the

    potential of generating multiple interpretations depending upon what the symbol

    offers,

    what the reader brings, and how the text is mediated by participants in a

    social context.

    A second construct from semiotic theory that contributes to the understanding

    of the picture book sign is the relation of the sign to its object. A ccording to P eirce

    (1955),

    there are three kinds of signs w hich relate to the object in different way s: the

    icon, the index, and the symbol. The iconic sign refers to objects it denotes primar-

    ily by similarity. The portrait of a man, for example, is a resemblance or likeness.

    The indexical sign shares an existential relation to an object, having a quality in

    common with the object. The sign is modified by the object rather than resembling

    it. The position of a weathercock, for example, indicates the direction of the wind.

    Sundials and demonstrative pronouns are also indexical signs. The symbol refers to

    an object by a law, general ideas representing a general object. Words, sentences,

    and books exemplify symbols which denote a kind of thing. The word

    bird

    for

    example, evokes the idea of a bird rather than the actual object. Wollen (1969) has

    argued that the triadic relations are necessary in the consideration of the aesthetic

    richness of cinema. According to Wolle n, although the iconic and indexical signs of

    cinema are the most powerful and the symbol signs are limited and secondary in

    film, all three are co-present and important signs of the cinema. The symbol con-

    veys conceptual meaning, the icon, pictorial beauty, and the index, documentary

    truth (Wollen, 1969).

    The same observations apply to the picture story book though it can be argued

    that the symbol has a more predominant role in at least some picture story books.

    Verbal language reflecting the sym bol is integrated w ith the iconic signs present in

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    5/18

    20 6 Journal of Reading Behavior

    the illustrations. Iconic signs are arranged linearly to reflect the sequence of the

    narrative. Together words and pictures convey the story. Although iconic aspects

    are predominant in illustrations and symbols in the verbal language, each may

    represent other dimensions of signs. In Ezra Jack Keats' (1962)The Snowy Day, for

    example, the text describes Peter's feet as "pointing out, like this" and "in, like

    that" (unpaged) as he makes tracks in the snow, relaying the indexical nature of

    demonstrative pronouns. The photographic images in George Mendoza's (1969)

    And I Must Hu rry for the Sea is Coming In

    relate the imaginative play of a boy in an

    urban ghetto, suggesting the existential bond or the indexical relation between the

    natural world and the image. Onomatopoeia, such as the "scr-r-ritch, scratch,

    scratch, scritch" of Mr. MacGregor's hoe in The Tale of Peter Rabbit (Potter,

    1902, p. 4 9) , represents iconic aspects of language. The fox, wh o is not referred to

    in the text, is a symbol of the villain in the illustrations of Pat Hutchins' (1968)

    Rosie's Walk. The picture story book thus represents a marriage of the predomi-

    nantly symbol signs of words with the predominantly iconic signs of illustrations,

    with both words and illustrations reflecting triadic relations of their own.

    A third aspect of semiotics concerns the process of semiosis which occurs in a

    social context. To capture the picture book symbol as it unfolds in a social context

    with child listeners and adult mediators, Morris' (1964) view of semiosis as a five-

    part relation provides a framework. Morris expanded Peirce's triadic relation by

    formulating how the sign-interpretant-object relations appear in context. He posited

    a five-part relation: " in which v sets up in w the disposition to react in a certain kind

    of way , x, to a certain kind of object, y (not then acting as a stimulus), under certain

    conditions, z " (p. 2). In a picture book reading event, the v is the picture book

    (sign), the w's are the listeners and mediators (interpreters), the x's are the mental

    representations of the sign (interpretants), the y's are the general objects

    (significations), and the z's are the home, school, or library settings (contexts) in

    which the event oc curs. Th is view of the process of semiosis captures picture boo ks

    as text events in context (Golden, 1989).

    Morris (1964) differed from Peirce in characterizing the interpretant in behav-

    ioral terms as "t he disposition to react in a certain kind of way because of the s ig n"

    (p . 178). Thu s, behav ior in this sense is observable and open to objective study. T he

    behavioral perspective, ho weve r, limits the interpretant by no t capturing other w ays

    of depicting interpretants. The power of the five-term relation, if Peirce's view of

    the interpretant is maintained, is the inclusion of context. A sign is not only viewed

    in terms of its grammatical and logical structures but also'in terms of its pragmatic

    structure. Pragmatics, according to Morris (1938), is the relation of signs to their

    interpreters and concerns the psychological, biological, and sociological conditions

    influencing the usage of signs. The interpreter brings a set of expectations of sign

    usage to particular situations. A similar conception has been explored by cognitive

    psychologists in terms of the schemata readers bring to the interpretation of events

    (e.g., Ruddell & Haggard, 1985). The notion of the effects and usages of signs

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    6/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text

    207

    within the context in which they occur is central to the examination of semiotic

    events in classrooms.

    THE PICTURE BOOK SYMBOL: THE POTENTIAL SIGN

    In considering the p icture book sym bol, we can first foreground the sign in the

    triadic relation. T he sign is, in part, the string of textual cues w hich guide the reader

    in constructing the interpretant. In this view, the sign is connected to the ground or

    grammar wh ich enables it to mean (Peirce, 1955). The gramm ar of the picture book

    is reflected in the arrangement of words (symbols) and images (icons). When the

    picture book is read to children, oral performance and instructional cues are woven

    together, thereby creating a multi-layered system of intersecting cues. Cues which

    contribute to the ground of the sign are discussed below.

    Verbal-Visual Cues

    The picture story book as a potential sign conveys a narrative through verbal

    language (symbol) and illustration (icon). The respondent is engaged in a dual

    process. The respondent transforms the black ink marks (or sounds) in the surface

    plane of the text into various textual perspectives of the narrative (e.g., character,

    narrator, plo t, reader in the text) by identifying, c omb ining, and predicting informa-

    tion as well as by filling in the gaps of the unw ritten text (Iser, 1978). In addition,

    the respondent transforms the m arks on the illustrated page into narrative images by

    engaging in sim ilar processes identified above. In this sen se, w e read the painting as

    well as the poem (Goodman, 1968). Two dots and a line become the eyes and

    mouths of the 12 little girls in Ludwig Bemelmans' (1953) Madeleine's Rescue. A

    shape of green meets a shape of blue to suggest a mountain and a lake in Uri

    Shulevitz's (1974)Dawn. Smudges of black against white evoke an image of a herd

    of buffaloes in GammelPs illustrations of Baker's (1981)

    Where the Buffaloes

    Begin. Thus, in constructing the image as beholders of art, according to Gombrich

    (1969),

    we are "watching our imagination come into play, transforming the medley

    of color into a finished page" (p. 199).

    In a picture book narrative, it is the relationship between the picture and the

    words that constitutes the narrative. T he picture book is a unique kind of symbol in

    that meaning is generated simultaneously from verbal and visual cues in a particular

    kind of relationship. Verbal and visual cues provide what Barthes (1977) termed as

    a relay function with each system complementing the other. Words and image thus

    contribute to a "unity of message" (p. 41) at the story level. In Potter's (1902)T he

    Tale of Peter Rabbit,

    for examp le, the text describes Peter looking for parsley to

    settle his queasy stomach: "And then, feeling rather sick, he went to look for some

    parsley" (p. 22). The illustration, on the opposite page shows Peter holding his

    stomach, drooping facial features, shaky posture, and ears pointed straight upward.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    7/18

    20 8

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    In this sense, the picture functions as an interpretant of the words and the words

    serve as an interpretant of the picture. The information in the text does not, how-

    ever, always represent such a close correspondence. In Sendak's (1963)W here the

    Wild Things Are, the moth er referred to in the text does not appear in the illustra-

    tions and the antics of the "wild rumpus" (unpaged) are depicted solely in the

    illustrations. This suggests that the artist does more than reflect the text, he or she is

    an interpreter of the text (Marantz, 1977).

    Text conveys literal and symbolic meanings as does image so the reader/listener

    is presented with m ore than on e signified in text and imag e. The text, to some extent

    serves to anchor the image, as Barthes (1977) contended, in order to reduce the

    range of significations. The multiple visual stimuli of colors and shapes in

    Wildsmith's (1971) The Owl and the Woodpecker are anchored by the text which

    relates "a great storm shook the forest" and the owl's tree began to "sway and

    fall" (unpaged). Similarly, it could be argued that the illustrations can anchor the

    text by reducing the signifieds. In Preston's (1974)

    Squawk to the Moon, Little

    Goose,

    the text describes the fox seeing " a cheese as big as the mo on " (unpaged) in

    the pond. The illustration by Cooney indicates the reflection of the moon in the

    water which the fox believes is cheese.

    Although it is conceivable that in some picture books, a story could be told

    either through th e text or the pictures, it would not be the same story told when bo th

    systems work together to create the sign. Individually, words and pictures are

    fragments of a larger syntagm (B arthes, 1977). The relationship between words and

    image varies depending upon the author and illustrator. In some picture books,

    images convey a major portion of the narrative and in wordless books, the entire

    narrative is conveyed through the ordered sequence of pictures as in Hutchins'

    (1971)

    Changes, Changes.

    In other books, the text assumes a more prominent role

    and illustrations function to interpret selected aspects of the text as in some illus-

    trated folktales such as Grimm and Grimm's (1972) Snow White and the Seven

    Dwarfs illustrated by Bu rkert. The words convey a n umber of details regarding the

    story events while the pictures reflect selected scenes such as Snow White running

    through the forest, the chamber of the stepmother, and so forth.

    Oral Performance Cues

    W hen th e picture story b ook sign is embedded in a social event, other cue,s for

    interpreting the story a re available to the listener. In m any situations the young ch ild

    hears a story read aloud by an adult. In this process, various interpretations of story

    are provided through auditory and visual channels of the performance. Auditory

    cues include intonation, stress, pausing, voice quality, and rhythm, among other

    cues, which influence how the story is conveyed. Golden (in press) compared two

    recordings of a tale by Hans Christian Andersen and found that differences between

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    8/18

    ASemiotic erspectiveofText

    209

    the performers in the use of auditory cues affected how the story, particularly the

    characters, was interpreted. In one rendition, the shepherdess appeared as a petu-

    lant, spoiled child, whereas in the other as a fragile and naive figure. The actual

    words were similar in both versions, yet the intonation patterns, pausing, and stress

    influenced how the characters were presented and how the listener empathized with

    the characters.

    The performer m ay also employ facial ex pressions which add to interpretations

    such as a frown accompanying reading about a particular character's action or a

    fearful expression to enhance susp ense. Fin e's (1984) analysis of an oral telling of a

    written version of the taleStagolee revealed how the written text was transformed

    during performance. This transformation was reflected in the style of language,

    characterization, and tone, indicating the interaction between word, voice, and

    body. In addition, there is the consideration of proxemics which affects how the

    story unfolds. A parent and child in a lap reading event sh are a different degree of

    intimacy than the teacher w ho reads to a g roup of children seated at their desks in a

    classroom. Thus, through the use of paralinguistic, kinesic, andproxe mic cues, the

    adult reader constructs an interpretant of the sign which is displayed to the child

    along with author's and illustrator's cues.

    Bauman's (1986) notion of the story as verbal art performance is central here.

    A literary critic's focus on the author's text and an anthropologist's emphasis on the

    event do not fully account for the story. When a story is performed (read or told),

    the story is a combination of the author's words, the illustrator's images, and the

    speaker/reader's nonverbal cues. The child listener is thus presented with multiple

    cue systems for interpreting the story which include the words of the text, the

    illustrations, and the nonverbal cues of the performer.

    Instructional Cues

    A picture book reading event as a performance event shares similar features

    with a traditional storytelling performance as noted above. There are, however,

    differences between the events which relate to the mediating role of the adult in the

    story reading event. In some reading events, the adult will provide what Bruner

    (1983) labeled as "sc affo ldin g" or talk about the text (p. 133). The adult may draw

    the child's attention to certain aspects of the text, such as labelling the picture,

    connecting pictures to real life o bjects, and so forth. In other instances, adults m ay

    focus the child on aspects of print and/or s tory. In these wa ys, the adult is establish-

    ing the "contracts of literacy" (Snow & Ninio, 1986, p. 119). Eventually, this

    external scaffolding by the adult is assumed by the child wh o engages in construct-

    ing an internal scaffolding of the text. After subsequent exp eriences with a text, the

    child gradually approximates both the adult's interaction with the text and the

    author's text. Some children internalize the written text to such an extent that they

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    9/18

    21 0

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    memorize it through listening, prior to acquiring the ability to match up the story

    told with the story written.

    In addition to talk about the text, the adult may also modify the author's actual

    text during the interaction by word changes, omission of text segments, and elab-

    oration of text, thereby utilizing the author's text as a springboard for labelling

    pictures rather than processing stories especially with younger children. In this way,

    the mediator has rewritten the author's text and altered its story function to accom-

    mod ate his or her purpo se. It should be noted, how ever, that not all parents scaffold

    the text and for those that do, there is variance in how they do it. Some observers

    suggest that some ways of scaffolding are more consistent with schooling experi-

    ences than others.

    Moreover, some research has suggested that adults' structured interactions with

    books reflect both individual styles (e.g ., M artinez & Teale, 1989) and commun ity-

    cultural patterns (e .g ., Hea th, 1983 ; W ells, 1986 ). The way in which stories are

    scaffolded can influence how children engage in story discourse. Preliminary re-

    search findings, for example, indicate that scaffolding can affect children's compre-

    hension of stories (e.g., Green, Harker, & Golden, 1987). Teachers influence the

    formulation of the interpretant by signaling different aspects of a story which they

    deem as important in con structing texts such as plot events, characters' reactions,

    etc.

    As teachers and parents establish the "rules for reading" (Snow & Ninio,

    1986,

    p. 119), they signal what they think is important about the narrative itself

    (e.g ., even ts, characters) and the function of narrative (e.g ., as a vehicle for teach-

    ing reading or concepts, as an aesthetic experience). The child constructs meaning

    guided by the exp ectations g overning the event; that is, what is projected as success-

    ful behavior, what conventions are necessary for engaging in story discourse, and so

    forth.

    The extent of the adult's influence on the child's approach to subsequent

    stories, however, is open to question. In some instances, schooling processes result

    in the legitimization of some interpretations over others as Eagleton (1983) sug-

    gested, as well as the fostering of particular ways of interacting with texts. When

    reading stories occurs in the classroom setting, there is also the influence of other

    peers if they are allowed to share responses (sometimes the group shapes the

    individual's interpretations; at other times the individuals are not influenced by

    these interpretations). Readers' interactions with texts are therefore affected by the

    teacher's recognition of the multiple interpretive possibilities of the symbol and the

    role of interpretations in constructing meaning.

    Thus far, the picture book has been examined in terms of grammatical cues

    which can influence the child's construction of the picture book narrative. These

    cues constitute the potential sign. In the next section, the focus is on the exploration

    of how multiple cue systems are at work during a picture book event in one

    classroom, thereby addressing the realized sign or the interpretant.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    10/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text 211

    A PICTURE STORY BOOK READING EVENT: THE REALIZED SIGN

    The picture story book discussed here was constructed in a second-grade class-

    room comprised of 24 Native American children in a public school on a reservation

    in the western United States. The event was one in a series of literary experiences

    the children participated in during the course of the school year and was chosen to

    illustrate how semiotic theory can be applied to such events . In this particular even t,

    the teacher wanted the children to focus on the literary element of character and on

    visual imagery (Golden, G erber, & Lew is, 1989). As the picture book evolved from

    the potential to a realized sign, it progressed through a series of four phases: (a)

    listening to the story read aloud, (b) exploring the traits of the character, (c) writing

    a letter to the author, and (d) visualizing a personal dream world. Since a primary

    goal of this inquiry was to examine the nature of the picture book symbol, the

    emphasis of this discussion is on the multiple cue systems operating in the first

    phase of the event, though the other phases will be addressed.

    Phase 1

    Phases of the picture book event can be captured in terms of Morris's (1964)

    description of the process of sem iosis as a five-part relation. In Phase 1, the picture

    book sign, Sendak's (1963)Where the Wild Things Are,provided verbal and visual

    cues for constructing the narrative of a character's journey to the land of the wild

    things. Accompanying these cues were the teacher's oral performance and instruc-

    tional cues. The students as interpreters constructed an interpretant of the sign

    which was partially visible to the observer in terms of their articulated responses.

    The signification reflected the events in the character's (Max's) life since events in

    fiction refer to themselves rather than to actual occurrences. The context was a

    second-grade classroom on a reservation. Phase 1 of the event is represented in

    Morris's terms in Table 1.

    In order to exam ine this phase m ore fully, cue systems of the picture book sign

    are considered. As noted previously, the students experienced the story by hearing

    the teacher's performance of the author's words while viewing his illustrations. At

    various points, the teacher or a child offered comments or questions about the text

    as the story unfolded. T wo segm ents, one at the beginning and one in the middle of

    the story, are employed to illustrate the display of author-illustrator, performance,

    and instructional cues. The author-illustrator's cues are examined first.

    In the first segment of the narrative, the words appear on the left page: "The

    night Max wore his wolf suit and mademischief... (unpaged). On the opposite

    page in a centered frame bordered by white spaces, Max is pictured in a wolf suit

    with a scowl on his face conveyed by a down-turned line for his mouth and u pturned

    eyebrows. He is standing on books, hammering the end of a rope of knotted sheets

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    11/18

    21 2

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    Table 1

    Phase 1 of the Picture Book Event

    Sign

    Picture book

    (author-illus-

    trator's ver-

    bal-visual cues;

    teacher's per-

    formance-in-

    structional

    cues)

    Interpreters

    Students

    Teacher

    Interpretant

    Students' tacit

    and articulated

    construction of

    text during

    group reading

    Signification

    Events in the

    character's

    life

    Context

    2nd grade class-

    room in a

    public, reser-

    vation school

    with a large nail. A big ho le in the wall suggests the damage to the plaster. A stuffed

    animal hangs by its arm from a coat hanger on the rope. A blanket is thrown over

    the rope with a stool underneath. Words and picture function as a relay system in

    which story information is enhanced by utilizing both image and language. The

    word mischief is delineated by the illustration which examplifies it. The costume

    Max is wearing in the picture is clarified in the language as awolf suit. In this way,

    words function as an interpretant for images and image s serve as an interpretant for

    words.

    On the following page, the words "and another" (unpaged) are presented on

    the left page. On the opposite page, the framed picture is enlarged. Max, with an

    ornery look on his face, is leaping off the stairs, holding a fork extended over his

    head. A dog is running from him. There is a picture of a monster with sharp teeth

    and a horn drawn by Max hanging on the wall by the stairs. Again, the picture

    exemplifies the word mischief by portraying Max's actions.

    Following this illustration, the words, again on the left page read: "his mother

    called him 'WILD THING ' and Max said 'I 'LL EAT YOU UP' so he was sent to

    bed without eating anything" (unpaged). In this case, the print is capitalized to

    stress the conflict between the mother and son and to convey the anger that Max felt.

    The m other's goal of punishing him for his outburst is implied by M ax's banishment

    to his room. In the illustration on the opposite page, which is larger than the

    previous illustrations, Max is in his bedroom looking back toward the closed door

    with an angry expression, depicted by a down-turned line for his mouth and fur-

    rowed eyebrows. His hand is positioned on his hip to convey anger. In his room, a

    moon is pictured throu gh the shutters of an open window . It is possible to infer from

    the pictures alone that Max is making m ischief and consequently is sent to his room .

    But this information, anchored by the words, specifies the conflict between mother

    and child and Max's state of anger. The illustrations function in a similar way by

    providing information and by adding connotations of the character.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    12/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text 21 3

    When the teacher read the author's words discussed above, she interpreted

    them through stress, pausing, volume, and intonation patterns (only the auditory

    channel cues are noted). These oral performance patterns are evident in the follow-

    ing representation in which stressed words are capitalized, pauses occur at the end

    of a line, raised voice through rising letters, and elongated words through dashes

    (Golden, 1989).

    The night MAX wore his WOLF suit and made MISCHIEF of ONE KIND

    (child: "of a kind")

    And another (soft tone)

    (child: responds inaudibly)

    His mother called him WILD thing

    and MAX said

    I LL eat you up

    and so he went to bed withOUT

    eating ANYthing

    The teacher emphasized through stress patterns, certain words such as Max,wolf

    mischief

    on e

    kind wild

    I'll, out

    and

    any,

    thus highlighting key aspects of the

    narrative. I'll was elongated, conveying a sense of Max's angry mood. Pausing

    patterns were at times an artifact of the break-up of the text (i.e., one sentence

    continues over several pages) and at other times for effect such as in the pause

    between Max said and / ' / / eat you up. The teacher did not interject talk about the

    text though two children did. In on e case, a child repeated

    of a kind

    after the teacher

    read on e kind. This repetition of the author's language was apparent in other

    instances. In another case, the child's response was inaudible.

    In the first page of the book, the author's words "mischief of one kind"

    (unpaged) are delineated in the illustration, depicting Max in a particular mood

    engaging in one kind ofmischief.S imilarly, the teacher stressed the words

    mischief

    and one kind and the child repeated of akind. The illustration accompanying the

    second page of text depicts another kind of mischief with the teacher stressing the

    second syllable in another. On the next pages, the print is capitalized for "WILD

    THING" and "I'LL EAT YOU UP" (unpaged) which emphasizes the emotional

    flare-up that occurred between Max and his mother. The illustration shows Max

    glaring angrily in the direction of his mother, providing another representation of

    the character's state. The teacher stressed words pertaining to the character such as

    wild Max, an dI'll, accentuating his rebellious mood and desire to be in control.

    Although patterns may be identified in story reading events, one phase in the

    event is not always equivalent to another. A subsequent segment in which the wild

    things respond to Max's arrival contrast with the beginning of the text, particularly

    in terms of the teacher's mediation. In this text segment, the author's words are:

    "and in and out of weeks and almost over a year to where the wild things are"

    (unpaged). These words appear on the left-hand page, but the corresponding illus-

    tration fills the entire opposite page and part of the page where the words appear.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    13/18

    21 4

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    Thus,

    as Max's imagination grows, the illustrations assume a more prominent role

    in the story. Max is pictured in his wolf suit in a sail boat facing a wild thing. His

    former scowl is replaced by a look of uncertainty conveyed in the less rigid line of

    his mouth and ey ebrow s. His arm is raised and extended to reflect mom entary pause

    at seeing the monster.

    On the next page, the author's words and the illustrations are presented in a

    double-page spread with the words appearing at the bottom of the page, like a

    caption: "A nd when he cam e to the place where the wild things are they roared their

    terrible roars and gnashed their terrible tee th " (un paged). The wo rds continue at the

    bottom of the opposite pa ge: "a nd rolled their terrible eyes and show ed their terrible

    claws" (unpaged). The illustration depicts Max having recovered his command of

    the situation shown by the scowl on his face, hand on one hip, and other arm over

    the edge of the boat, facing the wild things. The wordswild thinguttered by Max's

    mother have now taken shape in the visual representation of creatures, not unlike

    Max's drawing of a monster, who are large-sized, half animal-half monster, charac-

    terized by sharp teeth, furry-scaly hides, sharp claws, and horns.

    The teacher performed and mediated this phase of the story in the following

    way.

    And in and out of WEEKS

    and almost OVER a year

    to where the WIL D things are (hushed tone)

    T:

    Can you make some noises you think he might hear?

    S:

    Yeah (whisper)

    And when he CAME to the place where the

    wild things are, they ROARED their tor-TERRIBLE roars

    T:

    Let me hear your terrible roars.

    S: roaring sounds

    T:

    Oh, that d oesn't sound like very many w ild things are even there. Where are all

    the wild things? Let me hear you.

    S:

    asleep

    S:

    asleep

    T:

    Let me hear you.

    S:

    louder roaring sounds

    T: We have some wild things in here.

    In this process, the teacher invited children to take on the behaviors of some of the

    characters by enacting what the words conveyed. Students' responses in this way

    functioned, like the illustrator's with the word

    mischief

    to interpret the words

    terrible roar

    through sounds.

    In the first part of the segment, the words relate to the setting both in terms of

    time and place"almost over a year to where . . . ." (unpaged). In the illustra-

    t ions,

    Max has already arrived at the place, indicated by the wild thing in the water.

    The words give no clue to Max's reactions which are conveyed instead through the

    illustrations which capture his initial uncertainty. The teacher emphasized time

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    14/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text

    21 5

    aspects of setting in stressing

    weeks

    an d

    over

    and place aspects in stressing

    wild.

    Her hushed tone contributed to the suspense of the story. In terms of instructional

    cue s, the teacher asked a question related to her goal of having children imagine and

    one child responded by indicating she could imagine.

    In the second part of the segment, the words convey Max's arrival at the place

    where the wild things are telling how they responded to Max. In the illustration,

    Max's response, again not mentioned in the words, is shown through his scowling

    expression and somewhat arrogant body stance. The illustration thus provides con-

    notations abou t the character that suggest he is undaunted and in control of his own

    imagination . In reading the word s, the teacher stressed the wordscame,

    roared

    and

    terrible, thereby focusing on place and the wild things. The mediation also reflects

    her emphasis on focusing the children on the characters of the wild things. She

    subsequently asked them to

    gnash their terrible teeth

    and

    show their terrible claws.

    When these phrases are introduced later in the story, the children responded with

    similar enactments without being prompted by the teacher.

    Phases 2-4

    Following the reading event, the students constructed Max's character, wrote a

    letter to the author, and created pictures of their personal dream world. These

    phases are represented in Morris's five-term relation in Table 2.

    The three ph ases illustrate Peirc e's notion of the sign giving birth to other signs.

    In a chain of signification, the author-illustrator's picture book sign generated

    various interpretants in students in all four phases of the event. In Phase 1, the

    interpretant was only partially visible (e.g., students' articulated responses). In

    Phases 2-4, the interpretants were more evident in the students' representation of

    aspects of the narrative. Viewing interpretants as more than mental representations,

    according to Eco (1979), is consistent with Peirce's allowance for various types of

    Table 2 '

    Phases 2-4 of the Picture Book Even t

    Sign

    Picture book

    (author-illus-

    trator's

    verbal-visual

    cues)

    Interpreters Interpretants

    Students Studen ts' words

    describing

    Max; letters to

    author; and il-

    lustrations of

    dream worlds

    following

    group reading

    Signification

    Max's charac-

    ter; stu-

    dents'

    dream

    worlds

    Context

    2nd grade class-

    room in a

    public, reser-

    vation school

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    15/18

    216 Journal of Reading Behavior

    interpretants (e.g., behavioral, visual, and objectal), although Peirce did not

    categorize these types.

    In Phase 2 of the event, students worked in pairs or small groups with multiple

    copies of the picture book to construct their view of the character, Max. Descrip-

    tions formulated during this pro cess were later shared in a whole class discussion in

    which students indicated why they thought Max had a particular trait. Various

    students described Max as

    bossy

    when he ordered the wild things about, as

    vicious

    when he chased his dog with a fork, asimaginativewhen he w ent to the land of the

    wild things, and as serious when he missed home. These and other descriptions

    generated by the studen ts from their interpretation of both the words and pictures of

    the text were put on a large size drawing of Max and placed in the book corner.

    Thus, the students' construction of the character, Max, served as a sign itself with

    the potential of generating interpretants.

    As a result of one stud ent's suggestion to write the author (Phase 3), the teacher

    developed a center the next day in which students wrote letters to the author about a

    variety of topics of their choice. In this way, the interpreters of the sign com-

    municated with the producer of the sign. In Phase 4, students and their teacher aide

    discussed Max's dream world and how the illustrator created the world. Examples

    of illustrators' techniques in other books the students had previously read were also

    discussed. The emphasis, then, was on how visual cues are utilized to create

    images. Following a discussion of their own dream worlds, the students chose

    different media to represent their own dream worlds. These representations were

    posted in the hallway, functioning as signs for others to see. In the phases discussed

    above, then, students employed verbal (symbol) and visual (iconic) cue systems for

    interpreting the picture boo k sign in a variety of ways. Their interpretations, in turn,

    became signs for others to interpret.

    DISCUSSION

    A major focus of the present paper was the exploration of a type of text with

    high semantic or semiotic capacity (Landes, 1987). The distinction was made

    between the potential sign , as a network of cue system s, and the realized sign as the

    interpretant or the reader's representation of the text. In the potential sign, the

    author-illustrator's display of verbal and visual cues provides information

    specifically revealed in either language or image, information repeated by both

    sources, information delineated by one source, and information from one source

    providing connotations for the other. These relationships between words and image

    are generally apparent in picture story books, although the specific relationships

    vary across picture bo oks . Ho w the listener/reader uses these cues to guide interpre-

    tation will depend on factors related to both reader and text.

    W hen the picture book is read to children, the oral performance of the author's

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    16/18

    A

    Semiotic erspective

    of

    Text 217

    written text provides other information cues for interpreting the text. The perform-

    er's expression may accentuate aspects of plot such as suspense or nuances of

    character states such as anger. Other performers might emphasize other aspects of

    plot or interpret characters in a different way. Although paralinguistic cues add to

    the display of information presented to the child reader, they are also visible inci-

    dences of how the performer has realized the text. Some realization on the part of

    the performer, for exam ple, is necessary for making decisions about how to read the

    story.

    Evidence of the realized text is more difficult to ascertain than are the cue

    systems of the potential sign since response remains primarily in what Polanyi

    (1967) termed as "tacit knowing" (p. 9). The talk about text that may accompany

    reading does, however, provide some insight into the reader/listener's process of

    constructing text. It is possible to ascertain through talk, for exam ple, aspects of the

    text the adult views as important to signal for the child. Further, the comments

    initiated by the child or those in response to an adult's question or request offer

    some evidence about the child's realization of text. In a picture story book reading

    event, w e are able to look in on an act of production, on a "m ethodo logical field"

    in Barthes' (1979) terms, where participants work together to construct a literary

    text (p. 74). We can overhear a conversation about text.

    In a classroom event, the methodological field may extend beyond the actual

    reading of the story thereby p roviding additional insights into how the reader inter-

    prets the text as in the classroom event involving Where the Wild Things Are.

    Following the teacher's reading of the story, the children wrote descriptive words

    about M ax, illustrated their own dream world, and wrote letters to the author. These

    representations functioned as signs for others to interpret since the children's work

    was either displayed for others to read or sent to the author. In this sense, symbols

    grew out of symbols and lived in the minds of the audience.

    Scholes (1982) has described similar types of activities in terms of the au thor's

    production of a primary text and the teacher's, critic's, and student's production of

    secondary texts in relationship to the primary text. While the words, primary and

    secondary are problematic (i.e., why isn't the reader's constructed text the primary

    text?),

    the notion of an evolving text with different realizations in different contexts

    is important. It might be mo re useful to consider the author-illustrator's sign as the

    first sign in a chain of signs rather than as a primary sign.

    The exploration illustrates the value of a semiotic perspective in illuminating

    dimensions of the text, such as the author-illustrator's verbal-visual cues, and

    dimensions of the text in context, including the teacher's performance and instruc-

    tional cues. Each of these dimensions requires further study with a range of texts

    and classroom events. In addition, the exploration suggests the need to examine

    more closely the relationship between the first sign (i.e., the author's text) and the

    equivalent sign produced by the interpreter. What is determined to count as an

    equivalent sign depends, in part, on our assumptions about the nature of the sign,

    the interpretant, and the object.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    17/18

    21 8

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    REFERENCES

    Baker, O. (1981).

    Where the buffaloes begin.

    (S. Gammell, Illus.). New York: Warne.

    Barthes, R. (1977).

    Image music text

    (S. Heath, Trans.). New York: Hill and Wang.

    Barthes, R. (1979). From work to text. In J. V. Harari (Ed.),

    Textual strategies: Perspectives in post-

    structuralist criticism

    (pp. 73-81). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Bauman, R. (1986).

    Story, performance and event: Contextual studies in oral narrative.

    Cambridge,

    MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Bemelmans, L. (1953).

    Madeleine's rescue.

    New York: Viking Press.

    Bruner, J. (1983)

    Child's talk: Learning to use language.

    New York: Norton.

    Eco, U. (1979).

    A theory of semiotics.

    Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Eagleton, T. (1983).

    Literary theory: An introduction.

    Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Fine, E. C. (1984).

    The folklore text: From performance to print.

    Bloomington, IN: Indiana University

    Press.

    Golden, J. M. (1989). Reading in the classroom context: A semiotic event.

    Semiotica. 73

    (1/2),

    6 7 - 8 4 .

    Golden, J. M. (in press).

    The narrative symbol in childhood literature.

    West Berlin: Mouton de

    Gruyter.

    Golden, J. M., Gerber, A., & Lewis, S. (1989).

    The growth of a story.

    Unpublished manuscript,

    University of Delaware, Newark, DE.

    Gombrich, E. H. (1969).

    Art and illusion: A study in the psychology of pictorial representation.

    Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Goodman, N. (1968).

    Languages of art: An approach to a theory of symbols.

    Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-

    Merrill.

    Green, J. L., Harker, J. O., & Golden, J. M. (1987). Lesson construction: Differing views. In G. W.

    Noblit & W. T. Pink (Eds.),

    Schooling in social context: Qualitative studies

    (pp. 46-77). Nor-

    wood, NJ: Ablex.

    Grimm, J . , & Grimm, W . (1972).

    Snow White and the seven dwarfs.

    (R . Jarrell, Trans.). (N . E. Burkert,

    Illus.). New York: Farrar, Straus.

    Heath, S. B. (1983).

    Ways with words.

    New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Hutchins, P. (1968).

    Rosie's walk.

    New York: Macmillan.

    Hutchins. P. (1971).

    Changes changes.

    New York: Macmillan.

    Ingarden, R. (1973).

    The literary work of art: An investigation on the borderline of ontology, logic, and

    theory of literature

    (G. Grabowicz, Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Iser, W. (1978).

    The act of reading: A theory of aesthetic response.

    Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins

    University Press.

    Keats, E. J. (1962).

    The snowy day.

    New York: Viking.

    Landes, S. (1987). Picture books as literature. In B. Harrison & G. Maguire (Eds.),

    Innocence and

    experience: Essays conversations on children's literature

    (pp. 315 -322). New York: Lothrop,

    Lee,

    & Shepard.

    Marantz, K. (1977). The picture book a s art object: A call for balance.

    Wilson Library Bulletin, 52

    (2),

    1 4 8 - 5 1 .

    Martinez, M., & Teale, W. (1989). Classroom storybook reading: The creation of texts and learning

    opportunities,

    Theory Into Practice, 28(2),

    126-135 .

    Mendoza, G. (1969).

    And I m ust hurry for the sea is coming in.

    Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Morris , C. (1938).

    Foundations of the theory of signs.

    Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Morris, C. (1964).

    Signification and significance: A study of the relations of signs and values.

    Cam-

    bridge, MA: M.I .T. Press .

    Nodelman, P. (1988).

    Words about pictures: The narrative art of children's picture books.

    Athens, GA:

    University of Georgia Press.

    by guest on July 6, 2013jlr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/http://jlr.sagepub.com/
  • 8/12/2019 Journal of Literacy Research 1990 Golden 203 19

    18/18

    A Semiotic Perspective of Text

    21 9

    Peirce, C. S. (1955). Logic as semiotic: The theory of signs. In J. Buchler (Ed.),Philosophical writings

    of Peirce

    (pp. 98-119). New York: Dover.

    Polanyi, M. (1967). The tacit dimension. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

    Potter, B. (1902). The tale of Peter Rabbit. London: Warne.

    Preston, E. M. (1974). Squawk to the moon, little goose. (B. Cooney, Illus.). New York: Viking.

    Ruddell, R. B., & Haggard, M. R. (1985). Oral and written language and the reading process. In H.

    Singer & R. B. Ruddeli (Eds.),

    Theoretical models and processes ofreading

    (3rd ed., pp. 63- 80) .

    Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Scholes, R. (1982). Semiotics and interpretation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Sendak, M. (1963). Where the wild things are. New York: Harper & Row.

    Shulevitz, U. (1974).

    Dawn.

    New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.

    Snow, C. E., & Ninio, A. (1986). The contracts of literacy: What children learn from learning to read

    books. In W. H . Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.),Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 116-138).

    Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Wells, G. (1986).

    The meaning makers: Children learning language and using language to learn.

    Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Wildsmith, B. (1971). The owl and the woodpecker. New York: Franklin Watts.

    Wollen, P. (1969).Signs and meaning in the cinema.London: Seeker & Warburg in association with the

    British Film Institute.