journal of research in science teaching volume 50 issue 7 2013 [doi 10.1002_tea.21090] price, c....

29
JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN SCIENCE TEACHING VOL. 50, NO. 7, PP. 773–801 (2013) Research Article Changes in Participants’ Scientific Attitudes and Epistemological Beliefs During an Astronomical Citizen Science Project C. Aaron Price 1 and Hee-Sun Lee 2 1 American Association of Variable Star Observers, Museum of Science and Industry, Chicago, Illinois 2 University of California, Santa Cruz, California Received 21 March 2013; Accepted 11 May 2013 Abstract: Citizen science projects provide non-scientists with opportunities to take part in scientific research. While their contribution to scientific data collection has been well documented, there is limited research on how participation in citizen science projects may affect their scientific literacy. In this study, we investigated (1) how volunteers’ attitudes towards science and epistemological beliefs about the nature of science changed after six months of participation in an astronomy-themed citizen science project and (2) how the level of project participation related to these changes. Two main instruments were used to measure participants’ scientific attitude and epistemological beliefs and were administered before they registered for the program and six months after their registration. For analysis, we used pre- and post-test data collected from 333 participants who responded to both tests. Among them, nine participants were randomly chosen for interviews. Participants’ responses were analyzed using the Rasch Rating Scale Model. Results show that overall scientific attitudes changed positively, p < 0.01. The change was strongest in attitudes towards science news and citizen science projects. The scientific attitudinal change was related to participant social activity in the project. There was a negative change in their evaluation of their knowledge. The interviews suggest that this is due to a greater appreciation for what they have yet to learn. Epistemological beliefs about the nature of science significantly improved from the pre- to the post-tests, p < 0.05. Overall, we found volunteers’ participation in social components of the program was significantly related to their improvement in scientific literacy while other project participation variables (such as amount of data contributed to the project) was not. # 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Res Sci Teach 50: 773–801, 2013 Keywords: citizen science; informal science; nature of science (NOS); science literacy Most people spend the majority of their lives outside of school, yet science learning in non- school settings is often overlooked (National Research Council [NRC], 2009). Informal learning opportunities can support lifelong learning (Dierking, Falk, Rennie, Anderson, & Ellenbogen, K., 2003; Falk, Storksdieck, & Dierking, 2007), engage underrepresented populations in science (Center for Informal Learning and Schools [CILS], 2005) and create personal relationships with science (NRC, 2009) in ways different from formal science education opportunities. Informal science education is a rapidly growing field of research. Recent calls for more research on informal science learning have been made by the National Science Board (NSB, 2008) and the National Research Council (NRC, 2009). Contract grant sponsor: National Science Foundation DRL award; Contract grant number: 0840188. Correspondence to: C. Aaron Price; E-mail: [email protected] DOI 10.1002/tea.21090 Published online 11 June 2013 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). # 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Upload: alhanun

Post on 14-Sep-2015

218 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Journal of Research in Science Teaching Volume 50 Issue 7 2013 [Doi 10.1002_tea.21090] Price, C. Aaron; Lee, Hee-Sun -- Changes in Participants' Scientific Attitudes and Epistemological Beliefs Duri

TRANSCRIPT

  • JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN SCIENCE TEACHING VOL. 50, NO. 7, PP. 773801 (2013)

    ResearchArticle

    Changes inParticipants ScientificAttitudes andEpistemologicalBeliefsDuring anAstronomicalCitizenScienceProject

    C. Aaron Price1 and Hee-Sun Lee2

    1American Association of Variable Star Observers, Museum of Science and Industry, Chicago,

    Illinois2University of California, Santa Cruz, California

    Received 21 March 2013; Accepted 11 May 2013

    Abstract: Citizen science projects provide non-scientists with opportunities to take part in scientific

    research. While their contribution to scientific data collection has been well documented, there is limited

    research on how participation in citizen science projects may affect their scientific literacy. In this study, we

    investigated (1) how volunteers attitudes towards science and epistemological beliefs about the nature of

    science changed after sixmonths of participation in an astronomy-themed citizen science project and (2) how

    the level of project participation related to these changes. Two main instruments were used to measure

    participants scientific attitude and epistemological beliefs and were administered before they registered for

    the program and six months after their registration. For analysis, we used pre- and post-test data collected

    from 333 participants who responded to both tests. Among them, nine participants were randomly chosen for

    interviews. Participants responses were analyzed using the Rasch Rating Scale Model. Results show that

    overall scientific attitudes changed positively, p < 0.01. The change was strongest in attitudes towardsscience news and citizen science projects. The scientific attitudinal change was related to participant social

    activity in the project. There was a negative change in their evaluation of their knowledge. The interviews

    suggest that this is due to a greater appreciation for what they have yet to learn. Epistemological beliefs about

    the nature of science significantly improved from the pre- to the post-tests, p < 0.05. Overall, we foundvolunteers participation in social components of the programwas significantly related to their improvement

    in scientific literacy while other project participation variables (such as amount of data contributed to the

    project)was not.# 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Res Sci Teach 50: 773801, 2013

    Keywords: citizen science; informal science; nature of science (NOS); science literacy

    Most people spend the majority of their lives outside of school, yet science learning in non-

    school settings is often overlooked (National Research Council [NRC], 2009). Informal learning

    opportunities can support lifelong learning (Dierking, Falk, Rennie, Anderson, &Ellenbogen, K.,

    2003; Falk, Storksdieck, & Dierking, 2007), engage underrepresented populations in science

    (Center for Informal Learning and Schools [CILS], 2005) and create personal relationships with

    science (NRC, 2009) in ways different from formal science education opportunities. Informal

    science education is a rapidly growingfield of research.Recent calls formore research on informal

    science learning have been made by the National Science Board (NSB, 2008) and the National

    ResearchCouncil (NRC, 2009).

    Contract grant sponsor:National ScienceFoundationDRLaward;Contract grant number: 0840188.

    Correspondence to: C.AaronPrice;E-mail: [email protected]

    DOI10.1002/tea.21090

    Publishedonline 11 June 2013 inWileyOnlineLibrary (wileyonlinelibrary.com).

    # 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

  • Citizen science, defined as research collaborations between scientists and volunteers (Cornell

    Ornithology Lab, 2009), is an increasingly popular venue for informal science education

    (Cohn, 2008). While the scientific contributions of citizen science participants have been

    documented, such as contributing to the large-scale database of migrating bird populations

    (Hand, 2010) and discovering new galaxies (Christian, Lintott, Smith, Fortson, &

    Bamford, 2012), there is limited research addressing how citizen science projects impact

    volunteers science literacy (Conrad & Hilchey, 2011; Mueller, Tippins, & Bryan, 2012;

    Silvertown, 2009).

    In this study, we investigate volunteers involved in an astronomical citizen science project

    calledCitizen Skywhere participants collaboratewith scientists tomonitor and analyze data about

    a bright variable star. Aligned with Miller (1983, 1998, 2004), we use a civic oriented scientific

    literacy framework and measured two scientific literacy elements called attitudes towards

    science-related activities and epistemological beliefs about the nature of science. The research

    questions of this study are:

    How did volunteers attitudes towards science-related activities and epistemologicalbeliefs about the nature of science change over six months of participation in an

    astronomy-themedcitizen science project?

    Howdopatterns of project participation account for these changes?

    We first introduce the role citizen science plays in informal science education and categorize

    citizen science projects in terms of their participants level of involvement. We then describe

    Citizen Sky as our research context and provide rationale for measuring scientific literacy as an

    important outcome for citizen science projects. Next, we characterize our research methods

    including subjects, test designs, data collection and analysis. In the results section, we compare

    pre-/post-test differences in volunteers attitudes towards science-related activities and epistemo-

    logical beliefs about the nature of science and use interview data to explain some of those

    differences. Finally,we discuss findings and implications for citizen science participants, program

    designers, and researchers, alongwith limitations of the study.

    Citizen Science and Science Education

    Citizen science projects, at their core, provide an organized venue for non-scientists to take

    part in scientific endeavors ranging from passive (such as running a computer screen saver to

    process scientific data) to active engagement (such as amateur astronomers searching for and

    discovering newplanets). The spectrumof citizen science projects has been categorized intomany

    different and often overlapping categories (Brandt, Shirk, Jordan, Ballard, & Tomasek, 2010;

    Ely, 2008; Wiggins & Crowston, 2011). Citizen science can also be seen as a type of

    crowdsourcingthe process of using an extremely large number of participants to accomplish a

    focused task (Howe, 2006). Some citizen science projects, such as the large-scale GLOBE project

    (GLOBE, 2011; Penuel &Means, 2004), take place in classrooms. However, this study is focused

    on out-of-classroomprojects as their implementations are considerably different due to the lack of

    environmental controls and limitations on instructional strategies.

    Most citizen science projects are driven by their scientific goals with little attention to their

    educational impact on participants (Mueller et al., 2012). As a result, there are a limited number of

    educational research studies available in the literature. The vast majority of published articles are

    descriptions of projects and/or their targeted scientific accomplishments. This need for empirical

    research drives our research reported in this paper. With limited research about participants

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    774 PRICE AND LEE

  • learning through citizen science projects, there is lack of evidence in guiding the design of future

    citizen science projects (Jordan,Ballard,&Phillips, 2012).

    Citizen science plays an important role in the expanding field of informal science

    education (NRC, 2009; Ucko, 2010) and is seen as a leading trend in new projects funded under

    the informal science education program at the National Science Foundation (NSF, 2010). It

    allows participants to get involved in many different scientific practices while remaining

    focused on a single goal or outcome (Roth & Barton, 2004). It can often promote sustained

    engagement over decades of a persons life (Price & Paxson, 2011). These characteristics

    resonate well with free-choice learning, a critical element in development of lifelong scientific

    literacy (Falk & Dierking, 2010; Falk et al., 2007). At the same time, this flexibility creates

    unique challenges in sustaining participant interest (Nov, Arazy, & Anderson, 2011a),

    balancing the reality of data collection in uncontrolled environments with the fidelity to

    established scientific standards (Fore, Paulsen, & OLaughlin, 2001; Ottinger, 2010) and, for

    education researchers, developing rigorous research methodologies that are not intrusive into

    the participant experience.

    One of the unique characteristics of citizen science is in its focus on individual agency.Unlike

    traditional laboratory-style science education programs, citizen science projects allow partic-

    ipants to build their own view of the project rather than seeing it exclusively through the

    eyes of science (Roth & Barton, 2004). It is also an example of personal-curiosity science,

    a term coined by Aikenhead (2005) to describe scientific learning that is of personal interest of

    the student. Citizen science, with no ties to any classroom and no direct profit for the participant,

    relies on this personal interest to drive participation and sustain activity. Aikenhead (2005)

    believes that this type of personal connection is central to the success of science education writ

    large.

    This individual empowerment increases the value of social opportunities in citizen

    science projects. Since there is no formal classroom or physically shared space, it is difficult to

    build a community of practice among fellow participants. Communication between project

    scientists and participants can be challenging (Greaves, 2012). The scientists often have to

    serve as the teacher or mentor, a role for which they may or may not be trained, based on very

    little interaction with participants. This sometimes leads to a topdown flow of instructions

    from scientists to participants without requiring much independent thought and contribution

    by the participants (Ely, 2008; Mueller et al., 2012). The Internet has helped to overcome this

    barrier by making communication easier, at least for projects that are based online. An

    increasing number of online citizen science projects have turned to asynchronous discussion

    forums to create such virtual communities of practice among scientists and participants

    (Khare, Zevit, & Shirk, 2012a; Raddick et al., 2010; Roy et al., 2012). Other projects have

    begun partnering with museums and science centers to allow participants to interact directly

    with project scientists (Khare, Zevit, & Shirk, 2012b; Time Out Chicago, 2012). As citizen

    science increases in popularity, both in terms of diversity of projects and number of

    participants, finding creative ways to build supportive communities becomes more and more

    important.

    The role of their participants differs substantially between citizen science projects, which

    may determine the type and quality of participants experience and the learning outcomes after the

    projects are completed. The Center for the Advancement of Informal Science Educations Public

    Participation in Scientific Research inquiry group proposed three models of citizen science

    projects based on the participation level of the volunteers (Brandt et al., 2010). The models are

    idealized in that not all citizen science projects perfectly fit into a specific category, but they are

    useful as benchmarks along the continuumof citizen science projects.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 775

  • The Contributory Model

    Contributory citizen science projects use participants mostly as data collectors in distributed

    networks. This is the most common type of citizen science project (Karrow & Fazio, 2010). We

    further divide this category into two types of contributory models because the participant

    experience can be significantly different based on a single factor: whether they were actively or

    passively contributing data to the project.

    Passive ContributoryModel:After the initial recruitment phase, participants are asked tomonitor equipment that automatically collects data and transmits them to a central

    repository. One of the most successful of projects in this type is the Berkeley Open

    Infrastructure for Networked Computing (BOINC; Anderson, 2003) where the typical

    participant runs a computer screen saver to process project data.

    Active Contributory Model: Projects in this category actively engage participants in theprocess of data collection and/or data processing. Participants are required to make

    decisions such as how often to collect data and when to deviate from suggested protocol.

    The most popular projects in this category involve monitoring wildlife, including birds

    (Brossard, Lewenstein, & Bonney, 2005; Evans et al., 2005; Wee & Subaraj, 2009),

    insects (Howard & Davis, 2004; Phillips, 2008) and turtles (Somers, Matthews, &

    Carlone, 2009). But it is also common in astronomy (Ferris, 2002; Percy, 1999) and

    meteorology (Cifelli, 2005).

    To locate empirical studies on how the contributory model impacts citizen science

    participants, we used Google Scholar to look for a variety of specific and vague terms. Examples

    are scientific literacy citizen science, scientific literacy informal science education, citizen

    science education research, citizen science studies, and finally, simply citizen science by

    itself. However, our search did not lead to published empirical educational studies of projects

    within thismodel.

    The Collaborative Model

    In collaborative citizen science projects, participants are involved in developing descriptions

    and explanations, or performing basic forms of initial analysis. The most popular example of this

    type of model is the Galaxy Zoo project. The participants categorize photographic data to classify

    galaxies into groups based on their appearance, and later their results are further analyzed by

    professional astronomers. In addition, volunteers are encouraged to comeupwith explanations for

    anomalous galaxy shapes. One volunteers comments on a picture led to the famous discovery of a

    uniquegalaxy structure known asHannysVorweep (Cardamore et al., 2009).

    Most of the citizen science empirical research literature involves projects that belong to the

    Collaborative Model. In a study of The Birdhouse Network, researchers measured change in

    scientific attitudes, understanding of the scientific process and the knowledge of birds by project

    participants and found no change in attitudes or understanding of the scientific process (Brossard

    et al., 2005). However, Brossard et al. (2005) did detect an increase in knowledge of ornithology.

    In another ornithology project called The SeedPreferenceTest, researchers analyzed communica-

    tion between participants and project organizers to look for evidence of scientific thinking

    (Trumbull, Bonney, Bascom, & Cabral, 2000). While the researchers at the project did find

    evidence of scientific thinking in the communication, they could not isolate the project impact

    from other influences due to their study design. In addition, the researchers did not find any

    relationship between scientific thinking and the amount of data contributed by the participant.

    Another study analyzed e-mail communications between participants and project staff (Evans

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    776 PRICE AND LEE

  • et al., 2005) and found that changes in participants knowledge of birds were not apparent in email

    communications butwas indicated through interviewswith a sample of the participants.

    The Co-Created Model

    The co-created model is sometimes referred to as participatory action research (Cornwall

    & Jewkes, 1995). In this model, volunteers do everything from defining research questions to

    publishing results. These more demanding citizen science projects are often used as examples of

    distributed thinking (Hand, 2010). This is a relatively new category pioneered by groups such as

    the Bossa project (BOINC, 2009). There are relatively few active projects in this field (Bonney

    et al., 2009; Curren, 2013). This category is likely to grow (Roy et al., 2012) and there have been

    calls for involving citizens in these types of more authentic research roles (Cooper, Dickinson,

    Phillips,&Bonney, 2007; Lakshminarayanan, 2007;Wright, 2010).

    We found some empirical studies about the scientific output of co-created model projects on

    participants but none about their educational impact. One group of volunteers was trained to both

    take samples and do their own analysis (alongside professionals who did their own independent

    analysis) of water quality in a river shed over 3 years, with published results of their analysis

    (Wilderman, 2004). And recall in the Galaxy Zoo project one participant discovered their own

    galaxy type to great fanfare (Wright, 2010). That particular participants initiative elevated their

    experience to one similar to the co-created model in that they discovered anomalous data,

    investigated it and published the results (with professional guidance). However, there has been no

    published studies about how these projects, or any other Co-Created project, impacted learning.

    The focus has been on the scientific results.

    Research Context: The Citizen Sky Project and e AurigaeCitizen Sky (www.citizensky.org)was a 3-year, astronomical citizen science project launched

    in June 2009. The hosting organization was the American Association of Variable Star Observers

    (AAVSO), a citizen science organization that has been collecting astronomical data from amateur

    astronomers since 1911. The project was organized according to a set of design principles that

    were codified based on literature and the collective experience of AAVSO staff in running citizen

    science projects.

    Design Principle 1: Use a Context Where Volunteers Contribution Is Necessary and

    Meaningful for Their Scientific Inquiry

    The scientific goal of the project was to engage the public in monitoring a rare eclipse of a

    very bright starepsilon Aurigae. The star was so bright that the majority of professional

    observatories could not observe it with their sensitive instruments, but it is ideal for public

    observationeven from bright cities. The educational goal of the project was to increase general

    scientific literacy by involving participants in every stage of the scientific process. That is,

    participants were asked to do more than simply collect data. They made their own hypotheses,

    perform data analysis, tested theories andmodels, and eventually even published papers in a peer-

    reviewed astronomical research journal (Percy, 2012).

    Design Principle 2: Provide Internet Resources to Help Volunteers Interact With Peers

    and Scientists

    TheCitizen Sky project wasmostly coordinated via a central web site. Anyone could read the

    sites content, but participants who wished to actively contribute to the project had to register for

    an account. There were a number of ways participants could interact with the project. They could

    supply data by making brightness estimates of epsilon Aurigae and submitting it to a central

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 777

  • database. They could explore the data using a variety of analysis and modeling tools available on

    the web site. For social interactivity, the site had nine asynchronous online forums, which were

    moderated and facilitated by project staff. Also, synchronous live chats were held roughly every

    month. These free-for-all style chat sessions were scheduled around special topics and/or guests

    (usually professional astronomers). Ultimately, the participants were given the tools and other

    support to form collaborative teams focused around a mini-research project. Sometimes project

    topics were suggested by staff and sometimes the teams came up with their own. Their mission

    was to identify a topic, do their own research and develop a paper for submission to a peer-

    reviewed astronomical publication. A professional liaison was provided to each team to answer

    questions and give advice. Example team topics include developing a software package to

    statistically analyze submitted data, testing the use of consumer DSLR cameras to acquire data

    and designing visualizations based on the acquired data for use in press releases and other outreach

    venues. In the citizen science classification regime described in this paper, we place Citizen Sky in

    the co-created category.

    Design Principle 3: Actively Involve Scientists in a Role of Teaching and Communication

    One of the most important motivating factors in citizen science is direct and frequent

    interaction with professional researchers (Doering, Miller, & Veletsianos, 2008). Citizen Sky

    applied this principle by creating an environment with many channels of communication with the

    professional astronomical world. A project scientist (Ph.D. astronomer)maintained an interactive

    blog to keep participants informed of the latest research of epsilon Aurigae from professional

    journals and conferences. Also, a graduate student spent roughly 20 hours aweek interactingwith

    participants via the project web siteproviding themwith feedback, advice, and general support.

    Additional external scientists were invited to contribute blog posts about their own research into

    the star. Finally, the online chats provided a chance to interact directly, in a synchronous manner,

    with scientists (and each other).

    Design Principle 4: Support Participants for Analyzing and Presenting Their Own Data

    This principle was based on the concept of experiential learning (Kolb, 1984), which

    postulates that effective learning is based on a transformative experience. The experience here is

    the collection and analyzation of data by participants working on a real and pressing research

    problem. Using simulated or modeled data in an educational context can introduce misleading

    ideas about the scientific experience (Hollow, 2010). With their own data, participants had the

    same connection to the data as a researcherin fact, they became researchers in every sense of the

    term. In Citizen Sky, after they submitted data, participants were shown real-time graphs of their

    data superimposed over the data of other participants, so they could see how their data compared.

    They were also provided with tutorials and GUI tools to analyze the data, either separately or

    combined with data from others. Ultimately, many published their results at amateur and

    professional astronomymeetings and in journals.

    Design Principle 5: Encourage Participants to Become an Active Member of a Research

    Community

    The most advanced and successful citizen science projects in recent AAVSO history were

    ones in which the participants worked as a team. Constructivist approaches to science education

    also have shown the benefits of group work in classroom laboratory projects (Cummings &

    Kiesler, 2005), but remote projects need extra attention to foster this type of collaboration (Corter

    et al., 2007). As a result, the Citizen Sky web site had a section dedicated to the formation of

    different teams working on their own research project. These were private areas where team

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    778 PRICE AND LEE

  • members could communicate, share documents and chat. The formation of teams was fostered by

    staff during the second year of the project. But team participation was not mandatory. About 23

    teams eventually formed.

    These design principles are not unique to the Citizen Sky project. The first two principles are

    now considered as common sense for the construction of new, online citizen science projects. The

    third design principle, working closely with scientists, is becoming more common with many

    online citizen science projects. The Galaxy Zoo project, which has spawned a collection of other

    projects beneath the umbrella ZooUniverse organization, has maintained a very active section of

    their web sitewhere scientists interact with participants. The fourth design principle is common in

    active citizen science projects where participants generate their own data, such as the Christmas

    Bird Count. The fifth principle, working as a community, is perhaps themost unique to the Citizen

    Sky project. Many projects involve teams to collect data, such as the Mount Rainier Citizen

    Science Team (Bacher, 2011), but the teams tend to be highly localized and focused (Roy

    et al., 2012).We found none that supported teams involved in the broader process of creating their

    own research questions, analyzing data and ultimately publishing their results. None of these ideas

    are individually unique to Citizen Sky, collectively they are unique only because all of the design

    principleswere applied to a single project.

    Theoretical Framework: Scientific Literacy

    One of the most heralded promises of citizen science projects is to support the application of

    scientific thinking to everyday life. This is mainly because participants are engaged in a project

    that has become a part of their life outside of school or profession (Bonney et al., 2009; NRC,

    2009). This may help overcome some of the significant transfer issues that exist in science

    education (Gilbert, Bulte, & Pilot, 2011) by establishing the home as part of the participants

    learning environment extended beyond formal science education settings. That home is also both

    its own community and a part of a larger community. Roth and Barton (2004) propose an idea that

    scientific literacy and citizen science is linked through these communities, which are better able to

    address complex scientific issues than relying on individuals who were taught merely to follow

    directions. The complexity alsomakes learningmore authentic: . . . expecting one set of relations(institutional school) to prepare students for a world of many relations does not make sense, (p.

    2237). The participants role in the project is influenced not just by personal interest but also by

    their community (e.g., amount of time they can spend on the project is related to family and work

    demands). In this way, citizen science is unique in that it is an authentic scientific learning

    experience that takes advantage of the support structure tailored for the participant in their home,

    and thus can overcome the limitations of classrooms or laboratories.

    Scientific literacy in a citizen science context is a collective concept (Roth & Lee, 2005) that

    requires the integration of non-scientific considerations (Sadler & Zeidler, 2009) within a

    personally meaningful context (Holbrook & Rannikmae, 2007). Citizen science projects are

    generally created by organizations outside of the classroom, adapted to fit within non-scientific

    limitations provided by everyday life and ultimately fated to the personal interest of the

    participants. Thus, we have chosen to focus on a definition of scientific literacy closely aligned

    with the concept of civic engagement (Shen, 1975) and with an interest in its collective

    development by participants in the project. This differs from more traditional definitions that

    describe scientific literacy as a combination of scientific knowledge and awareness of scientific

    practices (NRC, 1996) or broad definitions such as that in the Project 2061 Benchmarks for

    Scientific Literacy that include civic elements, but as one component of a much larger picture that

    includes habits ofmind and the ability to observe and reflect on science (AmericanAssociation for

    theAdvancement of Science, 1993).

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 779

  • Miller (1983, 1998, 2004), described civic-based scientific literacy through three elements:

    (1) Vocabulary of science (science content): The vocabulary of basic scientific constructs

    needed to read and understand competing views from a popular science news source

    (e.g.,TheNewYorkTimesTuesdayScienceSection).

    (2) Understanding of scientific inquiry (nature of science): The process or nature of

    scientific inquiry.

    (3) Attitudes towards organized science and knowledge (attitudes towards science): The

    social impact of science on the individual and society.

    A 2004 meta-analysis of the international scientific literacy studies shows that about 17% of

    the US population was scientifically literate as described by this definition (Miller, 2004). Civic-

    based scientific literacy has been shown to be especially influenced by education in informal

    settings (Falk&Dierking, 2010).

    We assume that most participants of this project already had a command of scientific

    vocabulary at the level of fluency used byMillers (1998, 2004) studies (e.g., correctly being able

    to categorize astrology as scientific or not or know whether lasers focus sound or light waves). In

    addition, the participants were able to critically read the science section of a newspaper, or else it

    would not have been possible to understand any of the recruitment materials or sign up for the

    project. Thus, our measures were focused on the 2nd and 3rd elements of Millers definition: the

    understanding of scientific inquiry (epistemological beliefs about science) and attitudes towards

    organized science and knowledge (science-related activities).

    According to Bonney et al. (2009), in order to measure scientific literacy, citizen science

    projects can utilize project participation data (e.g., data submission logs), pre- and post-surveys,

    analysis of e-mail and listserv messages, self-report surveys, focus groups and interviews. This

    study collected participation data via the web site and collected data from pre- and post-surveys

    and interviews. We treated interviews as a secondary data source to explain some of the findings

    from the pre-/post-test analysis from the participants point of view. In another study, we analyzed

    online discourse of participants to identify emergent patterns of inquiry in their participation

    (Price, Borland,&Lee, 2012).

    Methods

    Instrument Design

    Scientific Attitude Instrument. An attitude instrument was assembled to match the unique

    characteristics of an older, informal science audience. It needed to be constrained in length, focus

    on the use of science in everyday life, and include questions that would measure behavior unique

    to a citizen science audience such as the pursuit of science news and attending scientific talks

    two hobbies not often found in the general population. There are a total of nine items answered

    with a 5-point Likert scale consisting of Strongly Disagree, Disagree, Neutral, Agree, and

    Strongly Agree categories. See Table 1 for item details. The reliability of the instrument was high,

    a 0.95.NSKS Instrument. Items for measuring epistemological beliefs about nature of science

    (Table 2) were based on the Nature of Scientific Knowledge Scale (NSKS) established by Rubba

    and Andersen (1978) (hereafter referred to as the NSKS instrument). We use the term

    epistemological belief becausewe feel it is flexible enough to reflect that attitudes, feelings and

    understanding change and are somewhat subjective. Other words such as knowledge or

    awareness imply a hard reality the participant is being judged against and oversimplifies what

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    780 PRICE AND LEE

  • constitutes nature of science, a term that stirs strong emotions inmany academics.While it is not

    a perfect term, we feel it best represents what we are attempting to measure in this study. There is

    no agreed upon definition of the nature of science, but there is consensus that . . . it is related toepistemology and values and beliefs for scientific knowledge (p. 409) and how that knowledge is

    developed, refuted, and changed (Ozgelen, 2012). The original NSKS instrument was validated

    by scientists and teachers and is commonly used in science education research (Bloom, 2008). It

    was chosen over other NOS instruments because of its extensive pedigree and also because it is a

    survey instrument. We piloted an open ended instrument which was strongly resisted by the

    participants due to its length. In fact, they rebelled on our public discussion forums at the length of

    the instrument. This is a common problem with informal science settings when participants are

    expected to be somewhat entertained in addition to educated. The items in the original NSKS

    included 48 items grouped into six categories of the nature of science (amoral, creative,

    developmental, parsimonious, testable, and unified).Each category included four positively stated

    items and four negatively stated items. To constrain the length of the overall test (in response to the

    pilot study), we omitted all negative items leaving four items per category for a total of 24 items in

    this study. See Table 2. The NSKS used a 5-point Likert scale consisting of Strongly Disagree,

    Disagree, Neutral, Agree, and StronglyAgree categories. The overall reliability for theNSKS test

    for this study was high, Cronbachs a 0.94, and was in general agreement with previousvalidation work on the original NSKS instrument, despite its shortened length (Rubba &

    Andersen, 1978;Meichtry, 1994).

    Interview Protocol. The interview protocol included questions about past participation in

    other citizen science projects (Have you participated in any other astronomical project similar to

    Table 1

    Scientific attitude individual item difficultiesbased on Rasch analysis

    Item (Abbreviation)

    Item Difficulty (Logits) SErasch Outfit

    Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post

    I actively seek out stories aboutastronomy in the news. (SEEK)

    1.05 1.23 0.09 0.09 1.03 0.90

    I am likely to attend a scienceseminar, class or talk. (ATTEND)

    0.81 1.17 0.10 0.09 1.45 1.21

    I plan to participate in other citizenscience projects in the future.(OTHER)

    0.69 1.54 0.10 0.09 0.86 0.87

    I am knowledgeable about science.(KNOWLEDGE)

    0.55 0.29 0.12 0.09 1.26 0.83

    I use knowledge of science toevaluate claims made aboutscience. (EVALUATE)

    0.04 0.29 0.11 0.11 0.94 0.82

    I will pay attention if an astronomynews item crops up in a mediasource I am already following.(ALREADY)

    0.46 0.49 0.11 0.11 0.75 0.82

    I use knowledge of science ineveryday life. (EVERYDAY)

    0.67 0.28 0.11 0.09 1.25 1.23

    I am interested in news aboutastronomy. (NEWS)

    0.84 0.17 0.11 0.11 0.67 0.68

    I am interested in science.(INTEREST)

    1.07 0.85 0.12 0.11 0.5 0.48

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 781

  • Table 2

    NSKS individual and sub-category item difficultiesbased on Rasch analysis

    Item Categories and ItemsWith Abbreviations

    Item Difficulty (Logits) SErasch Outfit

    Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post

    (a) Amoral 0.0 0.32 0.15 0.14The applications of scientific knowledgecan be judged good or bad; but theknowledge itself cannot.

    0.30 0.14 0.07 0.07 0.90 0.85

    Even if the applications of a scientifictheory are judged to be good, weshould not judge the theory itself.

    0.17 0.92 0.07 0.06 1.24 1.14

    A piece of scientific knowledge shouldnot be judged good or bad.

    0.06 0.07 0.08 0.07 0.91 0.82

    It is incorrect to judge a piece ofscientific knowledge as beinggood or bad.

    0.41 0.27 0.07 0.07 1.04 0.89

    (b) Creative 0.50 0.95 0.14 0.13Scientific knowledge is a product ofhuman imagination.

    0.78 1.11 0.06 0.06 1.31 1.25

    A scientific theory is similar to a workof art in that they both expresscreativity.

    0.49 1.0 0.07 0.06 1.16 1.02

    Scientific laws, theories, and conceptsexpress creativity.

    0.39 0.85 0.07 0.07 1.08 1.02

    Scientific knowledge expresses thecreativity of scientists.

    0.35 0.85 0.07 0.07 1.05 1.10

    (c) Developmental 0.22 0.08 0.17 0.16

    We accept scientific knowledge eventhough it may contain error.

    0.57 0.87 0.07 0.06 1.42 1.22

    Those scientific beliefs which wereaccepted in the past and since have

    been discarded should be judged intheir historical context.

    0.06 0.28 0.08 0.07 1.29 1.11

    Scientific knowledge is subject toreview and change.

    0.58 0.74 0.09 0.09 0.75 0.71

    Todays scientific laws, theories, andconcepts may have to be changed inthe face of new evidence.

    0.93 0.71 0.09 0.09 0.85 0.89

    (d) Parsimonious 0.39 0.74 0.15 0.13There is an effort in science to keepthe number of laws, theories, andconcepts to a minimum.

    0.82 1.29 0.07 0.06 1.14 1.20

    Scientific knowledge is comprehensiveas opposed to specific.

    0.53 1.07 0.08 0.06 1.17 1.44

    Scientific knowledge is stated as simplyas possible.

    0.18 0.86 0.07 0.06 1.15 1.22

    If two scientific theories explain ascientists observations equally well,the simpler theory is chosen.

    0.04 0.71 0.07 0.07 1.17 0.96

    continued

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    782 PRICE AND LEE

  • Citizen Sky?), level of participation in this project (What do you feel your role is in the Citizen

    Sky project?), views on the various categories of the NSKS (e.g., Do you see the creative aspect

    of the nature of science on display in the Citizen Sky project?) and included a few questions

    tailored to any anomalous responses each participant had to specific items. Therewere a total of 11

    planned questions, plus the tailored follow up questions. Interview transcripts were analyzed by

    listing verbatim transcripts of all answers to each common question separately and looking for

    emergent trends and differences. For the individually tailored questions, the transcripts were

    analyzed separately by comparing their interview response to their survey responses to see if there

    is any explanation for the anomalous responses.

    Subjects and Data Collection

    As of February 1, 2012, the projects web site had 6,491 registered users (participants). Of

    them 3,180 had taken the pre-test as part of the project registration process. They self-identified as

    78% male, 19% female, and 3% did not choose a gender. The mean age was 41 years old

    (SD 16). That the gender ratio and age skews towards an older, more male audience is typicalfor the amateur astronomy community. Sky& Telescopemagazine, the magazine of record for the

    community, reports 95% of their subscriber readership is male with a mean age of 51 (New Track

    Media, 2010). About a quarter of participants in this study reported no prior experience in

    astronomy. About 61% of the participants had a bachelors or higher degree, which was below that

    of subscribers reported by Sky&Telescopemagazine (77%).

    Item Categories and ItemsWith Abbreviations

    Item Difficulty (Logits) SErasch Outfit

    Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post

    (e) Testable 0.66 0.33 0.18 0.16

    Consistency among test results is arequirement for the acceptance ofscientific knowledge.

    0.48 0.21 0.09 0.08 0.86 0.90

    A piece of scientific knowledge will beaccepted if the evidence can beobtained by other investigatorsworking under similar conditions.

    0.58 0.07 0.09 0.07 0.88 0.82

    Scientific laws, theories, and conceptsare tested against reliableobservations.

    0.65 0.45 0.09 0.08 0.77 0.79

    The evidence for scientificknowledge must be repeatable.

    0.93 0.58 0.09 0.09 0.70 0.77

    (f) Unified 0.02 0.21 0.17 0.15

    Biology, chemistry, and physics aresimilar kinds of knowledge.

    0.53 0.32 0.08 0.07 0.98 1.08

    The various sciences contribute to asingle organized body of knowledge.

    0.20 0.32 0.07 0.07 1.07 1.39

    The laws, theories, and concepts ofbiology, chemistry, and physicsare interwoven.

    0.15 0.22 0.09 0.08 0.79 0.82

    The laws, theories, and concepts ofbiology, chemistry, and physicsare related.

    0.65 0.60 0.09 0.08 0.84 0.84

    Standard error of all items in thegroup added in quadrature.

    Table 2

    Continued

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 783

  • When participants in the project first registered via the web site, they were given the

    opportunity to take the pre-test that included the Scientific Attitude instrument and the shortened

    NSKS instrument along with many other instruments. Theywere compensated with placement in

    an annual drawing for a gift card. Most participants knew very little about the project at this point

    and had not formally participated in it at any level. The pre-test was offered to 6,491 participants,

    of whom 3,180 opted to complete at least a portion of it (49%). Theywere invited to take the post-

    test during their first login to theweb site after 6months had passed since registration. Ultimately,

    365 participants were offered the post-test and 333 opted to complete at least a portion of it (91%).

    The difference between the number of participants offered the pre-test and post-test is likely due to

    two factors. First, there was a high dropout rate and many people did not return to the project

    6 months later. Second, participants were not required to log into the site unless they needed to

    submit data or post to a forum. And web site login was a requirement to take the post-test (so we

    could match pre- and post-tests). So most participants who did return later did not need to log in.

    The gender distribution between the post-test group and the pre-test group is similar, but the post-

    test groups mean age was about 6 years older. Six months of project activity was chosen as a

    measurement point because, based on project staff experience,most participantswill have already

    reached their peak involvement at that point. In order to reach participants who were no longer

    active in the project, we also sent private e-mail invitations to thosewho had registered for theweb

    site 6months prior but had not logged in during the previous 3months.

    Fourteen participants who took both the pre- and post-tests online were randomly invited to

    participant in an interview session, of which nine accepted. They ranged in age from 18 to 64.

    Eightweremale and one female. Their education experience ranged fromhigh school graduates to

    one with a Ph.D. Their astronomy experience ranged from novice to professional, however, most

    were in the intermediate category. The interviews were conducted via the telephone or Internet

    communication software such as Skype, Google Voice, and Yahoo Messenger. They were each

    compensated with a gift card. The interview durations ranged from 25 minutes to 1 hour and

    15 minuteswith an average duration of 40 minutes.

    Data Analysis

    Coding. For each item on the two instruments, we scored responses by assigning a 1 for

    StronglyDisagree, 2 for Disagree, 3 for Neutral, 4 for Agree, and 5 for Strongly

    Agree.Unanswered questionswere treated asmissing data.

    Rasch Analysis. Likert scores were set on an ordinal, non-interval scale. This non-interval

    nature presents many complications for parametric analysis (Carifio & Perla, 2008;

    Jamieson, 2004; Knapp, 1990) which assumes equal intervals between two adjacent scores. To

    address this complication, the responses to the Likert scale were converted into an single interval

    scale through Rasch analysis (Rasch, 1960) based on the Rating Scale Model (RSM;

    Andrich, 1978;Muraki, 1990;Wright&Masters, 1982;), which is often used by science education

    researchers (Boone & Scantlebury, 2005). The RSM can be described through the following

    equation (Andrich, 1978; Linacre, 2002):

    logPnik=Pnik1 Bn Di FkwherePnik is the probability that participant n, on encountering item iwould be observed (orwould

    respond) in category k (item response) while Pni(k1) is the probability that the response would bein category k 1. Bn shows the amount of the trait the participant n has on a single numericalscale. In the measurement community, the amount of a trait is often referred to as an ability

    estimate even though some constructs such as attitudes towards science are not directly associated

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    784 PRICE AND LEE

  • with the concept of ability. We use ability or ability estimates hereafter in this paper to mean the

    amount of the trait an individual has for scientific attitude and nature of science constructs. Di is

    the difficulty of item i, on the same single numerical scale. And Fk is the difficulty of producing a

    category k response relative to a category k 1 on the same scale. Participant ability estimates(ability) and item difficulty estimates are placed on the same scale with normalized values

    typically ranging from4.0 to 4.0 logits (log-odds unit).The application of the RSMRasch analysis generates an ability estimate for each person and

    an item difficulty estimate for each item. The higher the person ability estimate the more the

    person has the ability to endorse an item. That is, the person with an ability estimate of 2.0 on the

    scientific attitude scale has higher scientific attitude than a person with 1.0. Similarly, higheritem difficulties are associated with increasing difficulty for an item to be endorsed. That is, an

    item with an item difficulty estimate of 2.0 is more difficult and thus requires a greater amount of

    ability to endorse than an item with 1.0. Therefore, whether an item, i, will be endorsed by aperson, n, (endorsement likelihood) with a response category of k depends on the difference

    between the persons ability and the difficulties associated with the item and the response

    category.

    Rasch analysiswas conducted using theWinsteps software program (Linacre, 2010).We only

    included responses from those who took both the pre- and post-tests. We first applied the RSM to

    pre-test responses. Then, we applied RSM to the post-test responses using the item and the person

    scales as anchors to equate both tests on the same scale,which is the traditionalmethod to compare

    pre- and post-tests usingRasch analysis (Bond&Fox, 2007).

    To check for psychometric validity and reliability on the two instruments,we usedfit statistics

    to evaluate how well the data fit the Rasch RSM model. For this analysis, the outfit statistic was

    used. A proposed range for acceptable fit statistics for polytomous data is 0.61.4 (Wright &

    Lincare, 1994) for rating scale items. No items were omitted from either test according to this

    criterion. For person ability estimates, 17 participants on the attitude test and seven participants on

    the NSKS test were omitted because their person outfit statistics were1.4. We kept participantswith outfit statistics

  • Chat Joinwas assigned a 0 if they never visited a live, synchronous online chat sessionor 1 if they havevisited at least one live chat.

    PostCountDiwas assigned a 0 if the participant had never posted amessage to aCitizenSky online, asynchronous discussion forum and a 1 if they had posted at least once to a

    forum.

    Since participating in chat sessions or posting messages on forums are considered social

    activities, we combined these two variables into variable called Participant Communication to

    reflect how active participants were in communicating with other participants. This variable had

    three categories: low,medium, and high. The lowcategory consisted of participantswhohad never

    joined a chat or posted a message in a forum. The medium category consisted of participants who

    have either joined a chat or posted at least one message to a forum. The high category consisted of

    participantswho have both joined a chat and also posted to a forum. For the analysis, we used three

    variables: Team, Active Observer, and Participant Communication to represent various levels of

    project participation.All project participationvariableswere categorical.

    Repeated Measures ANCOVA Analysis. Repeated measures ANCOVAs were used to

    investigate the main and interaction effects of project participation variables on differences in

    participants scientific attitudes and NSKS ability estimates between pre and post-tests. The

    independent variables in the analysis were related to the Participant Communication, Team, and

    Active Observer variables. Covariates were related to individual characteristics such as the

    AstronomyExperience, Gender, andAgevariables.

    Interviews.Post-test interviewswere transcribed into a spreadsheet so patterns could be easily

    identified among responses to the same question(s). The patterns were analyzed to characterize

    participants perspectives on results from the pre- and post-tests. For example, we looked for

    comments that may explain why some items showed significant change between pre- and post-

    tests while other items did not. We also asked questions about any anomalous answers in the

    participants survey results as compared to the rest of their responses. This was to examine if the

    differences were due to personally held epistemological beliefs or misunderstanding of the items

    on the tests. Every interviewed participant was asked about each category of the nature of science,

    but we only present analysis of the responses in which we detected a clear pattern or link to the

    survey data.

    Results

    Pre-Test Descriptive Statistics

    Our descriptive analysis includes all 3,180 responses to the pre-test. Overall responses to the

    survey questions were skewed. About 78% of all raw scores across all items lie between neutral

    and strongly agree on both instruments. This is not surprising considering thesewere volunteers in

    a citizen science project who are naturally motivated to participate in scientific activities and have

    strong epistemological beliefs about science. According to the Rasch analysis results on the

    attitude pre-test (Table 1), the easiest item with an item difficulty value of 1.05 was I activelyseek out stories about astronomy in the news. (hereafter referred to as the SEEK itemsee

    Table 1 for code words used for subsequent items). The INTEREST item was the most difficult

    item with the item difficulty of 1.07.While still quite positive, the responses to the NSKS pre-test

    (Table 2) itemswere less negatively skewed than the attitude pre-test items. On theNSKSpre-test,

    the easiest item to endorse was the There is an effort in science to keep the number of laws,

    theories, and concepts to a minimum, which had an item difficulty value of 0.82. The mostdifficult NSKS item was Todays scientific laws, theories, and concepts may have to be changed

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    786 PRICE AND LEE

  • in the face of new evidence and The evidence for scientific knowledge must be repeatable, both

    ofwhich had itemdifficulty values of 0.93.Table 2 also lists an average itemdifficulty value for each

    of the six sub-categories of NSKS epistemological beliefs. The easiest NSKS sub-category was

    creative with an average item difficulty value of0.50 and the most difficult was testable withthat of 0.66. See detailed itemdifficulty values andotherRasch analysis results inTables 1 and2.

    Changes in Participants Scientific Attitudes and Epistemological Beliefs About Nature of

    Science

    Analysis of change between the pre- and post-test is based only on responses from the 333

    participantswho tookboth tests.

    Attitude Towards Science. To identify significant changes between pre- and post-tests in

    scientific attitudes, we compared the difference in logit units between person ability and item

    difficulty in each of the nine scientific attitudes items (Figure 1). When the difference variable

    becomes zero the item will be endorsed by 50% of the participants. Positive values in the

    difference variable mean more than 50% chance of participant endorsement and negative values

    mean less than 50% chance of participant endorsement. Figure 1 indicates that, at the pre-test, the

    SEEK item was the most likely item to be endorsed by participants and the INTEREST item was

    the least likely. At the post-test, the differences significantly decreased, meaning the SEEK,

    ATTEND, OTHER, EVERYDAY, NEWS, and INTEREST items became easier to endorse,

    p < 0.05. There were no significant changes for ALREADY. There was a significant increase forKNOWLEDGE and EVALUATE, meaning they became more difficult to endorse. The item with

    Figure 1. Participants endorsement likelihood is determined by the distance between person ability and item difficultyon the scientific attitudes scale. The bars indicate the average distance in each of the nine scientific attitude items. Thesmaller the bar, themore likely the participants endorsed the item.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 787

  • the biggest overall change is the OTHER item, which is about the participants likelihood to join

    other citizen science projects.

    As seen in Table 3, ANCOVA results indicate that there was a significant change from pre to

    post in overall person ability estimates for the scientific attitude variable. The Active Observer

    variable was not significant, and neither was the Team variable. There were no significant

    interaction effects for either with the Time variable. However, Participant Communication had a

    significantmain effect on the scientific attitudes variable. ATukeys post hoc test indicated that the

    high participant communication groupwas significantly different, both at thep < 0.05 level, fromlow andmedium communication groups. This means that, coming into the project, thosewho had

    higher Participant Communication tendencies (i.e., became most active in the chat room and

    online discussion forums) had more positive attitudes at both testing times. There is a significant

    interaction effect between the Participant Communication variable and the Time variable (the

    repeatedly measured scientific attitude variable), meaning that those who were actively involved

    with Participant Communication changed to a greater extent from pre-test to post-test than those

    who did not. There was no significant difference between the low and medium Participant

    Communication groups on either test.

    Of the covariates, only Astronomy Experience was significantly related to the overall

    scientific attitudes variable. The Age and Gender variables were not significantly related to the

    overall scientific attitudes variable. A Tukeys post-hoc analysis of the Astronomy Experience

    variable on the scientific attitude variable showed significant differences between the low and

    medium experience groups, p < 0.05, and between the low and high experience groups,p < 0.01. Therewas no difference between themediumand high experiencegroups, p 0.76.

    Epistemological Beliefs About the Nature of Science. Figure 2 shows what NSKS item

    categories changed significantly between pre- and post-tests. There were significant positive

    changes in creative, parsimonious, amoral, and testable categories. There was no significant

    change in the developmental category. The overall NSKS ability estimates of thosewho took both

    pre- and post-test showed significant improvement between the two time points, according to

    Table 4, p < 0.05.

    Table 3

    Analysis of covariance on scientific attitudes

    Source df F Partial eta Squared r

    Fixed effectsTime (M) 1 18.6 0.166 0.000

    Participant communication (P) 2 3.76 0.075 0.027

    Active observer (AO) 1 0.353 0.004 0.554Team (T) 1 1.49 0.016 0.225M P 2 3.38 0.076 0.025M AO 1 2.55 0.058 0.061M T 1 0.072 0.001 0.789

    CovariatesAge 1 0.311 0.003 0.578Astronomy experience 1 15.0 0.138 0.000

    Gender 1 0.575 0.006 0.45

    p < 0.05.p < 0.01.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    788 PRICE AND LEE

  • As with the attitude test, repeated measures ANCOVA was used to look for differences in

    various groups of participants (Table 4). No significant main or interaction effects of project

    activity or individual characteristic variableswere detected.

    Post Interviews

    Social Communication.The interviewdata provide insight into someof the results detected in

    the tests. The attitude itemwith the greatest change is the OTHER item, which could be due to the

    Figure 2. Participants endorsement likelihood is determined by the distance between person ability and item difficultyon the scientific attitudes scale. The bars indicate the average distance in each of the six NSKS subcategories. The smallerthe bar, themore likely the participants endorsed the item.

    Table 4

    Analysis of covariance on beliefs in nature of science

    Source df F Partial eta Squared p

    Fixed effectsTime (M) 1 4.02 0.042 0.048

    Participant communication (P) 2 0.216 0.005 0.806Active observer (AO) 1 0.149 0.001 0.106Team (T) 1 0.000 0.000 0.997M P 2 0.458 0.010 0.634M AO 1 0.760 0.008 0.428M T 1 0.363 0.004 0.548

    CovariatesAge dichotomous 1 0.694 0.008 0.407Astronomy experience 1 0.074 0.001 0.786Gender 1 1.95 0.021 0.166

    p < 0.05.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 789

  • selection effect related to participants who are already interested in citizen science. However, this

    increase could also be related that their positive experience in Citizen Sky. Further validation of

    this interpretation can be done by comparing with other citizen science projects, for which we do

    not have data. The item with the second most change is the NEWS item. The other news related

    item, SEEK, also showed an increase. Our interviewdata shed some light on a possible connection

    between the two due to participants sharing news stories with each other via our online forums.

    When asked about changes in their news reading activities, three participants referred to posts to

    our discussion forums as new sources of news:

    Participant 4: I tend to read specific [news sources] that allow me to gain as much info as

    quickly as possible . . . I will tend to readCitizen Sky [forum] posts at nightwhen things are abit quieter.

    Participant 5 (referring to news updates from project staff): Ive always eagerly read any of

    those [forum] posts fromcitizen sky.

    Participant 9: To some degree CS has led me more into the blogosphere and the web with

    regard to news. At the same time Ive had to enhancemyway of critically reading such news

    and be able to try to deal with the sources it is coming from . . . the forums have haddiscussionswith regard to thevalidity of sources andmethods.

    In addition to the items related to news reading, the interviews suggest that working with

    others makes the project more interesting and also allows them to look at things from new

    perspectives. Regarding this increased interest, two participants commented on the importance of

    community and the collaborative nature of the Citizen Sky project, which was one of the design

    principles:

    Participant 2: Just in participating in it Ive learned things about interacting with other

    people and assumptions about sort of the knowledge and the interpretation skills of other

    people.

    Participant 5: [The project has] become a pretty important part of my life (laugh). These

    peopleits partly the people too its not just the science, its the combination of the two.

    One thing Ive really learned is that when you are doing science it is really helpful to have

    a team. It is really helpful to be able to throw ideas out there and bounce them around

    each other and have people with different expertises that can clarify things that you

    might now have understood completely or to see something in a different way than

    someone else did.

    Recall that the social aspect of the project was related to change in attitudes on the surveys.

    These two interview responses suggests that it was also an important role in helping participants

    see themselves as part of the scientific team.

    Self-Perception Regarding Their Knowledge. The KNOWLEDGE and EVALUATE

    items show the only decreases in endorsement. However, six of the nine interviewees stated

    that their knowledge increased or was otherwise unaffected through participation in the

    project:

    Participant 1: Im learningmore about variable stars as awhole.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    790 PRICE AND LEE

  • Participant 4: I have a better appreciation of the different kinds of variable stars and some of

    the characteristics of their light curves. Im learningmore about data analysis in this context

    as a result of asking questions and experimentingwith data.

    Participant 7: I always felt before that variable star observingwas too advanced forme. That

    alongwith the thought that [it] sounds boring. Its not so boring now.

    Participant 9: I dont think my knowledge of astronomy has been affected, no. But there has

    been a big effect onmyknowledge ofhow to do astronomy. (emphasis theirs)

    Therewas no statement in any interviewwhere a participant suggested theywere not learning

    or having trouble learning. An explanation for the discrepancy between what they said and what

    the test results show could be in how the KNOWLEDGE item was worded (I am knowledgeable

    about science.). That could be interpreted as a question about efficacy as opposed to knowledge.

    That is, participants are gaining more knowledge but this is also opening their eyes to how much

    more they have to learn (the more you know, the more you dont know). This was suggested by

    one of the participants:

    Participant 2: There have been many instances where I think peoples awareness of

    limitations of past knowledge has been increased. One of the overwhelming feelings I get is

    how much we dont know. So, many times its more like in what direction to move your

    boundaries of ignorance, as opposed to your boundaries of knowledge.

    Creativity in Science. The NSKS Creative category had the greatest increase in endorsement

    likelihood. The interview responses show consistent evidence that the project does involve broad

    creativity. All nine participants interviewed gave different examples of creativity in the project. Of

    them, one of the participants described their own problem solving skills as creative. But most of

    the examples of creativity involvedwatching other peoples application of creativity:

    Participant 3: In the part I participated in, I was just gathering data so other people could do

    the creative part of explaining the data. So I didnt domuch of the creative stuff but there are

    definitely other peoplewho did.

    Participant 4: In general, discussions in the CS forums in which people are coming up with

    alternative explanations for aspects of eps aur.

    The interviewer noted in their field notes that when asked about the creative items,

    participants often had to pause and think more so than in the other categories. Also, early in the

    project a particularly heated discussion occurred in the project forums over the topic of creativity

    in science. Investigation of the individual items found that in general the creative category of items

    were easiest to endorse in both pre-test and post-test (Table 2). Also, participant endorsement

    likelihood indicators changed the most with the creative category (see Figure 2). One participant

    in the online debate described the difficulty reconciling knowledge and creativity:

    Online Forum Participant 1: Scientists are creative when they develop theories, but is

    knowledge creative? No, knowledge is knowledge. Its like saying water freezes at 32 F,

    Imbeing creative by telling you this.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 791

  • This participants focus on the specific terminology is likely an example of issues many had

    with those questions. However, they also establish a difference between the presence of creativity

    at the beginning of the scientific process as opposed to the end. This is supported by many of the

    other participants in the discussion:

    Online ForumParticipant 2:While the scientic (sic.)method calls for strict reasoning and logic

    when checking hypotheses and making inferences, at the very begging (sic.) of a new theory,

    there is alwaysan informedguess.Apieceof intuition, imagination, somecreative event.

    Online Forum Participant 3: Of course, logic and mathematics are the means by which the

    world can be abstracted, represented and understood as models. But before models can be

    created and tested against evidence, surely imagination and creativity play a role. Einstein

    imagined (emphasis theirs) what it would be like to ride on a light beam, wondering how

    theworldwould look at close to the speed of light.

    Online Forum Participant 4: No matter how mundane, devising experiments (and creating

    (sic.) models) must surely often require creativity also. So, I think science is a mix of both

    creativity and logic. Its a human endeavour (sic.) after all.

    Parsimony in Science

    The other NSKS item category that was most easily endorsed by participants was the

    parsimonious category of items. The interviews suggest participants did not completely

    understand thewords definition:

    Participant 1: . . . this ismore difficult question than I thought . . .(laugh)

    Participant 5:What does thatmean? (laugh)

    Participant 7: Thats a bit harder towrapmyhead around.

    To better describe the concept, the interviewer referred to Occams Razor (summarized as

    when confronted with two explanations of equal accuracy, choose the simpler.), which is a

    common quotation in popular astronomy literature. After the definition was cleared up, everyone

    interviewed exhibited support for the parsimonious nature of science. Most often they quote the

    evolving theory of epsilonAurigae and also the ongoing development of a Theory of Everything

    in the physics community.

    Reinforcement. The change in the epistemological beliefs detected in the surveys is

    translational,meaning the center of the distributionmoved from lower to higher points in the scale

    without changing the shape of the distribution or changing the locations of items on the scale. The

    relative ranking of the six sub-categories of the nature of science did not change. This suggests a

    reinforcement of beliefs, which is also evidenced in language used in the interviews. The language

    illustrateswhere participants built on prior knowledge in phrases such as Im learningmore about

    variable stars as a whole, I have a better appreciation [of experimentation], [my interest] in

    science is greater, and I was reading about astronomy and physics and other science stuff even

    before Citizen Sky. In none of the interviews was there any discussion about fundamental

    changes in howparticipants view the nature of science. Instead, the commentswere about filling in

    gaps of knowledge and supporting prior epistemological beliefs.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    792 PRICE AND LEE

  • Discussion

    There are two main overall findings addressing each of the two elements of scientific literacy

    we studied. First, attitudes towards science increased and this increasewas significantly related to

    participant communication. Second, epistemological beliefs about the nature of science also

    increasedbut we did not detect a relationship with project participation levels. These two points

    together indicate that the participants attitudes towards science-related activities can be

    influenced by their positive social experience with others in citizen science projects. However,

    epistemological beliefs about science are less subject to the project participation experience

    possibly because epistemological beliefs are personal beliefs and thus harder to change after

    participating in only one citizen science project. Finally, whether the participant submitted data or

    was a member of a formal team did not have a direct relationship with changes in scientific

    attitudes or beliefs about the nature of science.

    Attitude Towards Science

    Interest in astronomy and science was very high on all of our attitude measures, as

    expected for a volunteer science project. Yet we still detected a significant change in the scientific

    attitude ability estimate as a whole from pre to post. Considering that other citizen science

    projects have not reported any change in scientific attitude (Brossard et al., 2005), our finding is

    noteworthy. The difficulty with finding changes in attitudinal constructs has been claimed to

    be related to the sensitivity of the instruments used in other citizen science projects (Brossard

    et al., 2005) and we agree. The change we detected is mostly through reinforcement of

    existing epistemological beliefs and our detected change is likely due to the use of a more

    sensitive analysis procedure. Thus we conclude that participation in a citizen science

    project alone is not likely to change overall attitudes towards science at a very high level,

    probably due to the fact that those entering such projects already have very positive

    attitudes. However, it can build on existing positive attitudes towards science and improve them

    further.

    The analysis procedure we used in this study, involving Rasch analyses of test responses

    combined with ANCOVAs using project participation variables, allowed us to uncover more

    significant item-related effects and could serve as an important analytical framework for future

    projects. Six of our attitude items became significantly easier to endorse. The OTHER items

    increase suggests they are more likely to participate in other citizen science projects in the

    future. The NEWS items increase is related to how participants share news stories and news

    sources with each other via the project web site and online forums. This is supported by

    the relationship between the survey scores and the Participant Communication variable. Also,

    the interviews provide insight into how that relationship manifested itself in the project (e.g.,

    through sharing of online news sources). Modern conventional wisdom is that collaboration and

    social factors are key to learning about science (Vygotsky, 1964), including online science

    learning (Linn, Davis, & Bell, 2004). The KNOWLEDGE survey item became more difficult to

    endorse. Interview data suggests that this is not due to participants losing (forgetting) knowledge

    but rather that they are becomingmore aware of howmuch they do not know. This important result

    speaks towards the fundamental relationship one has with science as the participants scientific

    horizons expand. The EVALUATE survey item also became more difficult to endorse, which we

    believe is related to the lowered self-perception found in the KNOWLEDGE item. That is,

    participants trust in their ability to apply scientific thinking to daily life decreased because

    they feel they understand a smaller fraction of the scientific field than they did when they took the

    pre-test.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 793

  • Epistemological Belief About the Nature of Science

    Epistemological beliefs about the nature of science increased significantly in this project.

    However, the relative rankings of the various NSKS categories did not change much between the

    pre- and post-test. This suggests that previous epistemological beliefs are being reinforced, rather

    than restructured. If they were being restructured, we would expect to see different levels of

    change between the various categories to such a degree that their relative importance would

    changefor example, creativity may suddenly become themost important element in participant

    epistemological beliefs. Instead, categories with the strongest beliefs remained the strongest and

    viceversa. Instead of structural difference, therewas an across-the-board difference.

    The creative category showed the greatest amount of change. A discussion about creativity

    was one of the most active and controversial threads in our online forums. That debate was more

    about where creativity exists in the scientific process (restricted to the beginning or infused

    throughout) rather thanwhether it is important at all. The amoral category also showed significant

    change and also was an active topic in the online forums, mainly through a heated discussion

    thread about global warming. Because our interview responses show confusion with vocabulary,

    we believe the parsimonious change is largely due to syntax and itemwording rather than anything

    related to the project. The change in the testable category may be associated with the ongoing

    presence of a staff blog on theweb site that continually discussed currentmodels of the star system,

    while comparing themwith new data and slowly constraining the cloud of uncertainty around the

    source of the eclipse. Through this process, participants saw their data in action in a very visible

    way and how itwas being used to solve the core scientific question of the project.

    There was a drop in epistemological belief that the goal of science is to create universal laws

    across various domains. This was a topic that was not addressed much at all in the projectwhich

    was focused almost exclusivelywithin the domain of astronomy.

    Our results are the first in the literature to show a change in epistemological beliefs in the

    nature of science through a citizen science project. One previous study reported high

    epistemological beliefs about the scientific process by their participants, but they could not

    attribute the beliefs to participation in the project (Trumbull et al., 2000) so it could be a reflection

    of the same selection effect we found in our pre-test results. Another study found no difference in

    understanding of the scientific process between those who participated in a citizen science

    project and a control group (Brossard et al., 2005). They suggested that since the primary

    motivation to join the project was an interest in the projects content (birding), participants did

    not view the project in a scientific light but rather as a knowledge-building exercise (i.e., an

    educational hobby). This is backed by Raddick et al. (2010) which reported only 12% of the

    Galaxy Zoo participants joined due to an interest in science or discovery. Themain reason to

    join was interest in the subject matter, astronomy, at 46%. On the other hand, the Citizen Sky

    project specifically refers to the scientific process throughout its training procedures. In fact,

    Citizen Sky recruitment materials focus on the opportunity to participate in all steps of the

    scientific method as a selling point to differentiate itself from other citizen science projects. So

    this level of meta-cognition may explain why this study finds more of an increase in

    epistemological beliefs, since participants went into the project more sensitive to how the entire

    process unfolds.

    The Role of Social Activity Involvement

    One of the important results of this study is the value of the social component of the project.

    After testingmany different variables, we found that the Participant Communication variablewas

    the only one related to change in scientific attitudes. Interview transcripts suggest this is due to

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    794 PRICE AND LEE

  • participants communicating with each other and using the online forums as a source of

    information and news. This could be a very positive development for the emerging field of

    citizen science. In the past, one of the challenges of such projects was the isolation in which

    participants worked. Nov, Arazy, and Anderson (2011b) found that establishing a community

    of volunteers is second only to intrinsic motivation in terms of sustaining commitment to a

    project.

    One explanation for this link between change in attitude and Participant Communication

    could be in the highly personalized nature of citizen science projects. The sense of agency that

    drives interest in citizen science also serves as an illustration of Aikenheads personal-curiosity

    science, which is part of a enculturation of science education into communities that are directed by

    the learners themselves. Citizen science projects have a greater opportunity to build a social

    community (as evidenced in the forums) and to empower its participants more than individual or

    even classroom-based science projects. This agency stems both from a closer sense of ownership

    of the process and its products and, for collaborative and co-created projects, also in the influence

    the participant has over the project structure. This sense of increasing control is important to

    scientific literacy and leads to citizen science becoming a strand of the learning process, one based

    on personal relationships (Roth & Lee, 2005). The sense of community and personal

    empowerment is fostered by an active community where the participant has a role beyond that of

    an anonymous data collector or processor. Also, online and interactive forums can support a more

    integrated community by narrowing the barrier between professional staff and participants, which

    has been recognized as an important goal by the citizen science community (Raddick et al., 2009).

    The Zooniverse project, arguably the most rapidly growing citizen science project ever, has

    reported 11,000members registered for their forums (Raddick et al., 2010). It is a rich community

    that supports a deeper investigation of the various Zooniverse projects than the original project

    goals intended. And participants have shown the initiative to investigate ideas on their

    own (Cardamore et al., 2009; Raddick et al., 2009). This has created its own challenges,

    such as determining how to effectively support such a large and active group when faced with

    limited resources. Our study suggests simply providing a forum to let participants communicate

    with each other is an easy and effective first step. Yet it is one that most citizen science

    projects are not yet taking. This stepwould go a longway to help put online citizen science projects

    in line with the greater science education communitys emphasis on collaborative and social

    learning.

    Other Project Participation Measures

    The Citizen Sky project was designed to give participants a chance to participate in every

    stage of the scientific process with the belief that this greater engagement will increase their

    scientific literacy. Participants were not required to engage at these other stages, but many did.

    Over a dozen papers have been published in a peer-reviewed scientific journal dedicated to

    participant projects. However, evidence of how participation levels and type affected change in

    literacy has been elusive. For example, one may expect to find a relationship between the number

    of variable star observations submitted to the database (the Active Observer variable) and

    epistemological belief in the testable aspect of theNSKS test, since the very nature of variable star

    observing is to combine lots of data from independent sources. However, we find no such

    statistical relationship in our data. Other research on citizen science projects have also found that

    contributing data did not increase participants epistemological beliefs in the nature of science

    (Trumbull et al., 2000). It may take more than just participation in data collection for participants

    to gain new insight into science.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    CITIZEN SCIENCE LITERACY 795

  • Implications

    Citizen science is a field experiencing explosive growth. It has great potential to not just help

    scientists but it also can help educators. This study suggests a number of ways a citizen science

    project can be designed to enhance scientific literacy of its participants. But education must be

    established early in a projects design as a primary goal of the project in order to implement many

    of these suggestions. First, we recommend a dedicated social aspect to the project. This data show

    that participant attitudes were most affected by direct communication with other participants.

    Online forums are simple and easy to install on almost any web site platform these days. We

    recommend using them along with live chats with project scientists to create a community of

    shared knowledge. This social component has the potential to help empower participants, which

    others have shown to have a significant impact on overall scientific literacy. This empowerment is

    somewhat unique to the personal nature of citizen science projects and additional research in this

    area would be compelling. Also, we suggest a direct illustration of how the participant is involved

    in the overall scientific process the researchers are conducting (perhaps using metacognitive

    strategies embedded in training materials). Other citizen science projects have found that

    participants sometimes have trouble understanding their role in the entire project (Evans et al.,

    2005) whereas this project emphasized and defined their role very early oneven in the training

    materials. Informal science has the luxury of not being restricted by classroomwalls, but it cannot

    completely ignore the importance of framing the issue. Finally, practitioners should not expect to

    see an increase in scientific attitudes or epistemological beliefs simply by having volunteers

    participate in data collection. They need to be more involved in other aspects of the project. Our

    results, along with that from other projects, shows that data collection alone has no established

    relationship with changes in scientific attitudes or epistemological beliefs about the nature of

    science. As for researchers, we showed how Rasch analysis can uncover results in coarse Likert

    datathe type often found in informal science education surveys. Past citizen science projects

    may need to apply amore sophisticated analytical model such as the Raschmodel to their data and

    re-analyze them for hidden results. These relatively easy steps should go a long way in turning a

    passive citizen science project into an active one that has the opportunity to change how its

    participants view science.

    Limitations

    This study is limited in a number of ways. First, both scientific attitudes and epistemological

    beliefs about nature of science instruments show certain degrees of a ceiling effect because

    participants already had an interest in science before joining the project. It is likely that this ceiling

    effect has depressed the measured range of responses and may be responsible for the relatively

    minor amounts of change detected. Most scientific attitude instruments in the literature are

    developed for children and adolescents in formal educational settings and are validated

    accordingly. In order to advance research in informal settings, new and targeted instruments need

    to be developed that take into consideration the unique aspects of informal science education

    audiences. For example, theymust not takevery long to complete since such audiences are usually

    volunteering their personal time (as opposed to formal environments which can require

    completion of an instrument). Also, they need to be sensitive to a broad range of prior enthusiasm

    for science. A second limitation is related to the representativeness of the sample used in this

    study. While we did make a significant effort to contact those who were no longer active in the

    project, themajority of the post-testswere filled out by thosewhowould be consideredmore active

    than the average participant. Third, the project did not have a control group to estimate the effect of

    the studied project as compared to other citizen science projects. Fourth, the attitude instrument

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    796 PRICE AND LEE

  • was created for this project and has not been externally validated in othersmeasurement research,

    however, the strong Rasch alpha and fit statistics reflect that the underlying latent variable is

    associated with the items chosen for the scientific attitudes instrument. Fifth, our conclusion that

    the change in epistemological beliefs is due to reinforcement rather than restructuring of beliefs is

    based on inferences in the survey data and phrases used in the interviews. A more in depth

    interview process would be helpful to provide direct evidence of this. Sixth, the post-test group

    was slightly older than the pre-test group, meaning that it may reflect a slightly more mature

    population compared to the general population of the project. Finally, the authors of this study

    were affiliatedwith theCitizen Skyproject.

    Conclusion

    This study found that overall attitudes towards science increased through participation in this

    citizen science project. The change was greatest in a few specific attitudes related to scientific

    news gathering and self-awareness of participants knowledge.We also found a positive change in

    overall epistemological beliefs in the nature of science, but it was slight and mainly through the

    reinforcement of previously held beliefs. The change in attitudes towards scientific news gathering

    was related to social participati