k.j.mclaughlin

10
CHRONIC STRESS ENHANCES SPATIAL MEMORY IN OVARIECTOMIZED FEMALE RATS DESPITE CA3 DENDRITIC RETRACTION: POSSIBLE INVOLVEMENT OF CA1 NEURONS K. J. MCLAUGHLIN,* S. E. BARAN, R. L. WRIGHT AND C. D. CONRAD* Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, 950 South McAl- lister, Box 1104, Tempe, AZ 85287, USA Abstract—Emerging data report sex differences in how the brain responds to chronic stress. Here, we investigated the effects of chronic restraint stress (6 h/day/21 days) on hip- pocampal morphology and function in ovariectomized female rats. Chronic restraint stress caused CA3 apical dendritic retraction in short- and long-shafted neurons, while it re- duced basal dendritic arbors in long-shafted neurons only. Chronic restraint did not affect CA1 dendritic arborization, although it increased the proportion of CA1 spine heads compared with controls. Both stressed and control animals performed well on the Y-maze, a spatial memory task. How- ever, chronic stress enhanced Y-maze performance com- pared with controls, which may reflect facilitated spatial memory or reduced habituation. Y-maze performance corre- lated with CA1 spine head proportion. This relationship sug- gests that spatial ability in females may be more tightly cou- pled with CA1 morphology, which may override the influence of CA3 dendritic retraction. Thus, this research provides ad- ditional evidence that CA3 morphology does not always par- allel spatial memory. © 2005 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Key words: hippocampus, chronic stress, females, spatial recognition memory, dendritic spines. Sexual dimorphisms in the stress response have been identified for some time, and a growing literature shows that stress impacts hippocampal cognitive ability differently in the two sexes. In humans, stressed males, but not females, exhibit elevated glucocorticoid levels (Kirsch- baum et al., 1992), which negatively correlate with hip- pocampal-dependent declarative memory (Wolf et al., 2001). Similarly in animal models, acute restraint stress impairs hippocampal-dependent spatial memory in male rats, but facilitates spatial memory in female rats (Conrad et al., 2004a). These reports indicate that understanding the stress response and cognitive ability in males may not extrapolate to females. However, few studies use females to investigate the influence of stress on brain morphology and cognitive ability, which is alarming given statistics on mental health in humans. Women with a history of long- term depression have a 10 –15% reduction in hippocampal volume compared with controls (Sheline, 1996; Sheline et al., 1999) and are almost twice as likely as men to be diagnosed with depression (Heller, 1993; Kessler et al., 1993; Weissman et al., 1993). In animal models, depres- sive-like symptoms are often preceded by chronic stress or stressful life events, and females cope differently with stressful situations than males (Westenbroek et al., 2003). Although recent research has begun to focus on such gender differences, care and treatment of women has been derived predominantly from research on males. Therefore, more research on females is needed to inves- tigate stress effects on the brain and body, and to better address women’s health. In the brain, stress produces sexually dimorphic re- sponses in both hippocampal morphology and function. In male rats, restraint for 6 h/day/21 days retracts hippocam- pal dendritic arbors in the CA3 region (Watanabe et al., 1992c; Magarinos and McEwen, 1995a; Magarinos et al., 1998; Conrad et al., 1999), which corresponds to impaired hippocampal-dependent spatial memory (Luine et al., 1994a,b; Conrad et al., 1996). Pharmacological blockade of CA3 dendritic retraction also correlates with the preven- tion of spatial memory deficits in separate studies (Wa- tanabe et al., 1992a,b; Luine et al., 1994a,b; Conrad et al., 1996). In contrast, chronically-stressed, gonadally-intact females have less robust dendritic retraction in basal CA3 dendrites compared with the apical CA3 dendritic retrac- tion seen in males (Galea et al., 1997). In addition, spatial memory appears unaffected or improved by chronic stress in females (Bowman et al., 2001, 2003; Conrad et al., 2003; Kitraki et al., 2004). Together, these data suggest that females can have intact or even facilitated spatial memory despite the possible presence of hippocampal dendritic retraction. Ovarian hormones may modulate the effects of stress on hippocampal morphology and function in females. Es- trogen levels have been found to induce changes in both CA1 spine density and spine shape in rodents (Gould et al., 1990; Woolley et al., 1997; Li et al., 2004; Gonzalez- Burgos et al., 2005). In addition, ovarian hormones and stress appear to have interactive effects on behavior. After 21 days of stress, female rats in proestrus show impaired spatial memory compared with rats in other stages of the estrous cycle; however, after 28 days of stress females in proestrus show improved spatial memory compared with their control counterparts (Bowman et al., 2001). Through- *Corresponding authors. Tel: 1-480-965-2573; fax: 1-480-965- 8544 (K. McLaughlin), Tel: 1-480-965-7761; fax: 1-480-965-8544 (C. D. Conrad). E-mail addresses: [email protected] (K. McLaughlin), [email protected] (C. D. Conrad). Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; CORT, corticosterone; LS, long-shafted neurons; LTP, long-term potentiation; OVX, ovariec- tomy/ovariectomized; SS, short-shafted neurons. Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx 0306-4522/05$30.000.00 © 2005 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.083 1 ARTICLE IN PRESS

Upload: hijoderemilputas

Post on 28-Sep-2015

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

NEURONAL

TRANSCRIPT

  • C TIAO DESR MEN

    K.C.

    Deliste

    AbbraeffporatretduChaltcopeevepamelatgepleofditalleAll

    Kerec

    Seidethainfembapo20imrats, but facilitates spatial memory in female rats (Conradet al., 2004a). These reports indicate that understandingtheexto

    anmetervoal.dia19sivstrstrAltgebeThtigad

    spmapa1919hip19oftiotan19femdetiomein20thamede

    Ovarian hormones may modulate the effects of stresson hippocampal morphology and function in females. Es-

    *Co854(C.E-mconAbbLS,tom

    Ne

    030doi

    ARTICLE IN PRESSstress response and cognitive ability in males may nottrapolate to females. However, few studies use femalesinvestigate the influence of stress on brain morphology

    trogen levels have been found to induce changes in bothCA1 spine density and spine shape in rodents (Gould etal., 1990; Woolley et al., 1997; Li et al., 2004; Gonzalez-Burgos et al., 2005). In addition, ovarian hormones andHRONIC STRESS ENHANCES SPAVARIECTOMIZED FEMALE RATSETRACTION: POSSIBLE INVOLVE

    J. MCLAUGHLIN,* S. E. BARAN, R. L. WRIGHT ANDD. CONRAD*

    partment of Psychology, Arizona State University, 950 South McAl-r, Box 1104, Tempe, AZ 85287, USA

    stractEmerging data report sex differences in how thein responds to chronic stress. Here, we investigated theects of chronic restraint stress (6 h/day/21 days) on hip-campal morphology and function in ovariectomized females. Chronic restraint stress caused CA3 apical dendriticraction in short- and long-shafted neurons, while it re-ced basal dendritic arbors in long-shafted neurons only.ronic restraint did not affect CA1 dendritic arborization,hough it increased the proportion of CA1 spine headsmpared with controls. Both stressed and control animalsrformed well on the Y-maze, a spatial memory task. How-r, chronic stress enhanced Y-maze performance com-red with controls, which may reflect facilitated spatialmory or reduced habituation. Y-maze performance corre-ed with CA1 spine head proportion. This relationship sug-sts that spatial ability in females may be more tightly cou-d with CA1 morphology, which may override the influenceCA3 dendritic retraction. Thus, this research provides ad-ional evidence that CA3 morphology does not always par-l spatial memory. 2005 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd.rights reserved.

    y words: hippocampus, chronic stress, females, spatialognition memory, dendritic spines.

    xual dimorphisms in the stress response have beenntified for some time, and a growing literature showst stress impacts hippocampal cognitive ability differentlythe two sexes. In humans, stressed males, but notales, exhibit elevated glucocorticoid levels (Kirsch-

    um et al., 1992), which negatively correlate with hip-campal-dependent declarative memory (Wolf et al.,01). Similarly in animal models, acute restraint stresspairs hippocampal-dependent spatial memory in male

    uroscience xx (2005) xxxstr21spesprothe

    rresponding authors. Tel: 1-480-965-2573; fax: 1-480-965-4 (K. McLaughlin), Tel: 1-480-965-7761; fax: 1-480-965-8544D. Conrad).ail addresses: [email protected] (K. McLaughlin),[email protected] (C. D. Conrad).reviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; CORT, corticosterone;long-shafted neurons; LTP, long-term potentiation; OVX, ovariec-y/ovariectomized; SS, short-shafted neurons.

    6-4522/05$30.000.00 2005 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.083

    1L MEMORY INPITE CA3 DENDRITICT OF CA1 NEURONS

    d cognitive ability, which is alarming given statistics onntal health in humans. Women with a history of long-m depression have a 1015% reduction in hippocampallume compared with controls (Sheline, 1996; Sheline et, 1999) and are almost twice as likely as men to begnosed with depression (Heller, 1993; Kessler et al.,93; Weissman et al., 1993). In animal models, depres-e-like symptoms are often preceded by chronic stress oressful life events, and females cope differently withessful situations than males (Westenbroek et al., 2003).hough recent research has begun to focus on suchnder differences, care and treatment of women hasen derived predominantly from research on males.erefore, more research on females is needed to inves-ate stress effects on the brain and body, and to betterdress womens health.In the brain, stress produces sexually dimorphic re-

    onses in both hippocampal morphology and function. Inle rats, restraint for 6 h/day/21 days retracts hippocam-l dendritic arbors in the CA3 region (Watanabe et al.,92c; Magarinos and McEwen, 1995a; Magarinos et al.,98; Conrad et al., 1999), which corresponds to impairedpocampal-dependent spatial memory (Luine et al.,94a,b; Conrad et al., 1996). Pharmacological blockadeCA3 dendritic retraction also correlates with the preven-n of spatial memory deficits in separate studies (Wa-abe et al., 1992a,b; Luine et al., 1994a,b; Conrad et al.,96). In contrast, chronically-stressed, gonadally-intactales have less robust dendritic retraction in basal CA3

    ndrites compared with the apical CA3 dendritic retrac-n seen in males (Galea et al., 1997). In addition, spatialmory appears unaffected or improved by chronic stressfemales (Bowman et al., 2001, 2003; Conrad et al.,03; Kitraki et al., 2004). Together, these data suggestt females can have intact or even facilitated spatialmory despite the possible presence of hippocampalndritic retraction.ess appear to have interactive effects on behavior. Afterdays of stress, female rats in proestrus show impairedatial memory compared with rats in other stages of thetrous cycle; however, after 28 days of stress females inestrus show improved spatial memory compared withir control counterparts (Bowman et al., 2001). Through-.

  • ou(CpeetalsThtheamphexacexespade

    Su

    ThCorespercohstraChranstre17-convehwebera teacc

    OV

    Onwexylabdiodmoovi(Etwit

    17

    Has.cUStheschpro4 w2pre198Injeadmsta

    Re

    Beres21resferwhunda s

    tesreslarghelHaY-mducnottwo

    Y-m

    Thmewdomcouofusiwe

    streRaandtheinexpwatificexppaw

    Go

    RadecRaUSonethe(Mslidpapcestheing

    His

    Foing2) tthehippusDe

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx2

    ARTICLE IN PRESSt the estrous cycle, the amount of corticosteroneORT) released in response to stress differs, with greateraks in proestrous rats (Viau and Meaney, 1991; Conradal., 2004a). Fluctuations in estradiol and prolactin cano stimulate CORT secretion (Lo and Wang, 2003).erefore, these changes may modulate stress effects onbrain and subsequent behaviors. The interactions

    ong stress, ovarian hormones, and hippocampal mor-ology and function are complex; thus, we designed anperiment using ovariectomized (OVX) female rats andute estrogen replacement. This study is the first study toamine the influence of chronic restraint stress and acutetrogen treatment in OVX female rats on both hippocam-l morphology (CA3 and CA1 regions) and hippocampal-pendent spatial recognition memory.

    EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

    bjects

    e Arizona State University Institutional Animal Care and Usemmittee approved all subjects and procedures used for thisearch. All efforts were made to minimize the number of animalsgroup and any potential suffering of these subjects. Twoorts initially totaling 80 female SpragueDawley rats (CDin), weighing approximately 230 g, were purchased fromarles River Laboratories (Hartford, CT, USA). All rats weredomly assigned to either a control (n40) or chronically-ssed group (n40). In addition, rats were administered either estradiol or sesame oil, giving rise to four experimentalditions: control/vehicle (CV), control/estradiol (CE), stressed/icle, (SV) stressed/estradiol (SE). Control and stressed ratsre housed in separate, but identical chambers. Housing cham-s were on a 12-h light/dark schedule (lights on at 7:00 AM) withmperature of 2122 C. Rats were housed two per cage withess to food and water ad libitum.

    X

    e week after arrival, rats received bilateral ovariectomies. Ratsre anesthetized using a ketamine cocktail (10 ml ketamine, 5 mlazine, 2 ml acepromazine, 3 ml 0.9% NaCl, 1 ml/kg, i.m.). Theomen was rubbed with Betadine surgical scrub (povidone-ine 7.5%) and a longitudinal, 1 cm incision was made to re-ve the ovaries, which were tied off and then cut from theduct. The muscle was sutured with coated Vicryl suture threadhicon, Inc., Somerville, NJ, USA), and the skin was securedh wound clips, which were removed one week post-surgery.

    - Estradiol administration

    lf of the control and stressed rats received injections (0.1 mL,.) of either 5 g 17- estradiol (Steraloids, Inc., Newport, RI,A) or sesame oil. Injections were given to each rat at 48 h andn again at 24 h prior to behavioral training on the Y-maze, aedule designed to parallel estrogen levels similar to those inestrus (Chesler and Juraska, 2000; Korol and Kolo, 2002). Theeek delay between OVX and estrogen treatment is within the8 week window that acute estrogen replacement has beenviously shown to effectively influence behavior (Becker et al.,7; Frye, 1995; Korol and Kolo, 2002; Markham et al., 2002).ctions coincided with the last two days of restraint, and were

    inistered to the animals between 8 and 9 AM before restraint

    rted.putUSstraint and handling

    ginning 10 days after surgery, half of the rats underwent chronictraint stress for 6 h/day (from approximately 9 AM to 3 PM) fordays. Rats were restrained in their home cages in wire meshtrainers (18 cm circumference24 cm long) and were trans-red to larger restrainers (23 cm circumference28 cm long)en they outgrew the smaller restraint. Control rats remainedisturbed during the time of restraint stress and were housed ineparate but identical chamber.To reduce potential exploration anxiety during the day of

    ting, rats were handled for 2 weeks, beginning on day 7 oftraint. All rats were held for one minute, allowed to explore in ae box for approximately twothree minutes, and then wered again for one minute before being returned to their cage.ndling occurred in two different rooms, both separate from theaze room which was used for behavior assessment. To re-e any stress associated with the experimenter, handling wasperformed by researchers who assisted with restraint or withinhours after the restraint session.

    aze testing

    e Y-maze was used for behavioral testing of spatial recognitionmory. The apparatus consisted of three identical arms (50 l1632 h cm) made of black Plexiglas. The three arms were ran-ly designated as the start, novel, and other arm and werenterbalanced between rats. Animal bedding covered the floorthe maze and was mixed between trials to prevent rats fromng odor cues. Visual cues, in a variety of colors and shapes,re located outside of the maze.The first behavioral trial took place one day after restraintss termination and two hours after the change in light cycle.ts were placed in the start arm and allowed to explore the startother arms for 15 min, while a black Plexiglas divider blockeddesignated novel arm. Four hours later, rats underwent testingwhich they were allowed to explore all arms for 5 min. Theerimenter was not visible to the rats during testing; behaviors videotaped and then quantified at a later date. During quan-ation, the researcher was blind to the arm assignments and theerimental conditions. Entries were defined as entrance of fronts into an arm.

    lgi procedure

    ts were injected with a lethal dose of Nembutal (1 ml, i.p.), andapitated, then unperfused brains were rapidly removed. FDpid Golgistain kits (FD NeuroTechnologies, Baltimore, MD,A) were used for Golgi staining, and staining was successful forcohort of rats. Brains were prepared as directed by the kit and

    n quickly frozen in 2-methylbutane and cut in 100 m sectionsicrotome HM 500 OM Cryostat, 30/32 C). Once placed on ae, brains were firmly pressed by hand using Bibulous blottinger (Fisher Scientific International Inc., USA). Brains were pro-sed, left in the dark to dry for 12 weeks, stained according toGolgistain Kit, and then coverslipped with Permount Mount-Media (Fisher Scientific International Inc.).

    tology

    r proper Golgi analysis, cells were chosen based on the follow-criteria: 1) the cell body and dendrites were fully impregnated,he cell was relatively isolated from surrounding neurons, and 3)cell was located in either the CA3c or CA1 region of the

    pocampus. A camera lucida drawing tube attached to an Olym-BX51 microscope was used to trace all neurons (320).

    ndritic length was quantified using a Scion Image Microcom-

    er Imaging Device Program (Scion Corporation, Frederick, MD,A) attached to an Olympus BX 50 microscope. Branch points of

  • theysesam

    ratnatasloc(FisurFoaveBaLS

    weeacneucelfouobtlen

    Goacrandin tratspietapidrittraSpnumencspiafteheaaraspi

    Sta

    Thme(co

    (codenmobassubanawaratspipro

    tioncomnotthere-wehor

    depingageencencperintomo199

    ior,comor

    andablusewe

    (StPeweothSP

    Eseffweverelrat(con

    Effmo

    rat2apolenfewcocemoovwitwetoapP

    duarb(tretwe

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx 3

    ARTICLE IN PRESSdendritic tree were also counted. The neurons used for anal-s were randomly selected with the only restriction being thatple sizes were equal between the groups.

    CA3. Six rats from each experimental group (total of 24s) were chosen in which sufficient numbers of Golgi-impreg-ed cells could be identified. CA3 neurons were further labeledshort-shaft (SS) or long-shaft (LS) depending on their relativeation in the stratum pyramidale and proximal apical shaft lengthtch et al., 1989). Dendritic length and branch points were mea-ed separately for the apical and basal sections of the neuron.r each rat, apical analyses included two SS and two LS neuronsraged to obtain one value for SS and LS cells, respectively.sal analyses were performed similarly, with three SS and threecells separately averaged for each rat.

    CA1. A total of five rats from each group (total of 20 rats)re chosen in which sufficient Golgi staining was present. Forh rat, six neurons were analyzed. CA1 cells also express tworonal types; however, cell complexity does not differ like CA3ls (Bannister and Larkman, 1995). Therefore, both cell typesnd in the CA1 region were analyzed together and averaged toain one measure of branch points and one measure of branchgth per rat.CA1 spine densities from pyramidal cells that had consistent

    lgi impregnation were also analyzed because spines fluctuateoss the estrous cycle (Woolley et al., 1990; Shors et al., 2001)estrogen is one of the most robust modulators of spine density

    he CA1 region (Shors et al., 2001). Three cells each from fives in each group (total of 20 rats) were chosen randomly forne analyses. Following previously established protocols (Gouldal., 1990; Woolley et al., 1997), spines from the most lateralcal tertiary dendrite and the most lateral basal secondary den-e were quantified. A camera lucida drawing tube was used toce spines that could clearly be seen in one focal field (1250).ines were then counted, and the density was calculated as theber of spines/10 m of dendrite. There were visible differ-es between CA1 spine shapes. In general, young hippocampalnes are headless, and exhibit heads as they mature, usuallyr 24 weeks in vitro (Papa et al., 1995). Therefore, spines withds were differentiated from those without heads, and we sep-tely compared the proportion of each with total dendriticnes.

    tistical analyses

    ree rats were removed due to surgical complications or equip-nt errors, and the final analyses for the Y-maze included 77 ratsntrol n39, stress n38).

    Golgi. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with treatmentntrol, stress) and hormone (vehicle, estradiol) as the indepen-t variables was used to analyze the effects on CA3 and CA1rphology. Separate comparisons were made for apical andal branch points and length. For CA3 analyses, a within-ject design for cell type (SS, LS) was employed. For CA1lyses, an ANOVA with spine density as the dependent variables computed. Since CA1 dendritic spines were represented asios of dendritic spines with heads to total dendritic spines, thene density data were transformed according to the equation forportions, 2arcsin (square root (Y)) (Cohen et al., 2003).

    Y-maze. To assess spatial memory within a given condi-, a Wilcoxon non-parametric matched pairs test was used topare entries into the novel and other arms. The start arm wasincluded in this analyses because rats were initially placedre by the experimenter, potentially creating a bias againstentering that arm. Differences in spatial memory across groups

    re determined by ANOVA using treatment (control, stress) andmone (vehicle, estradiol) as the independent variables. The

    F(1Pendent variable was a difference score, generated by subtract-the percentage of entries into the other arm from the percent-of entries into the novel arm. Positive scores reflect a prefer-e for the novel arm, while negative scores indicate a prefer-e for the other arm. A zero score indicates chanceformance with no arm preference. Total entries (sum of entriesall three arms) were also used to determine whether motor ortivational factors differed among the groups (Conrad et al.,6).To assess the relationship between morphology and behav-Spearman correlations (one-tail, P0.05) were performed topare CA3 dendritic properties or CA1 spines with total entries

    difference scores.

    Body weights. An ANOVA with treatment (control, stress)hormone (vehicle, estradiol) was the between-subjects vari-

    e and day (1, 7, 14, and 21) as the within-subject variable wasd to determine the effects of stress or estrogen on bodyight.

    Software. The statistical software program STATISTICAatsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA) was used for all ANOVAs andarson correlations. Significant effects from ANOVA (P0.05)re further analyzed using Newman-Keuls post hoc tests, unlesserwise noted. Spearman correlations were conducted usingSS (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA, version 12.0).

    RESULTS

    trogen treatment failed to produce statistically significantects on morphology (P0.10), behavior (P0.10), oright (P0.1). Therefore, the data were collapsed acrosshicle- and estrogen-treated groups, and only analysesated to stress effects are reported. The final number ofs per group was the following for morphology: CA3ntrol n12, stress n12), CA1 (control n10, stress10), CA1 spines (control n10, stress n10).

    ects of chronic stress on hippocampalrphology

    CA3 apical dendritic arborization. Chronically-stresseds displayed apical CA3 dendritic retraction (Fig. 1). A2 mixed-factor ANOVA (treatmentcell type) revealedsignificant main effect of treatment for apical branchints, F(1,22)11.40, P0.01, and total dendritic branchgth, F(1,22)5.22, P0.05. Restrained rats expresseder branch points and less total branch length than

    ntrols (Fig. 2A, B). As expected, a significant effect ofll type was also demonstrated, with SS cells havingre branch points, F(1,22)9.35, P0.01, and greatererall branch length, F(1,22)4.47, P0.05, comparedh LS cells, regardless of treatment. All other effectsre not significant (P0.10). Pearson correlations faileddemonstrate any significant relationships between CA3ical branch points and length and difference scores,0.10

    CA3 basal dendritic arborization. Chronic stress re-ced branch points and length of LS, but not SS, basalors (Fig. 2C, D). A 22 mixed-factor ANOVAatmentcell type) indicated a significant interaction be-en treatment and cell type for branch points,

    ,22)9.59, P0.01, and branch length, F(1,22)5.96,0.05. Further analyses using separate one-way ANO-

  • VAthaba

    str(0.

    FigrepstuLS.D)the

    Fig reased bSS comparres mparedme(n

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx4

    ARTICLE IN PRESSs for the control and stress conditions demonstratedt indeed, chronic restraint specifically decreased LSsal dendritic properties without affecting SS basal den-

    . 1. Golgi-Stained CA3 Neurons. Camera lucida drawings (320)resent experimental conditions of the two CA3 neuronal typesdied. (A) Control SS, (B) Stress SS, (C) Control LS, and (D) StressNote the reduction in apical dendritic complexity in the stressed (B,vs. control neurons (A, C). Basal dendritic retraction was evident inLS stressed group only (D) compared with LS control neurons (C).

    . 2. CA3 apical and basal dendritic morphology. Chronic restraint deccells had more apical branch points and overall apical branch lengthtraint reduced both LS basal branch points (C) and length (D) co

    ansS.E.M. * P0.05 stress vs control. P0.05 SS vs. LS. For basal data12 rats/group).tes. All other effects were not significant (P0.10). Ad-ionally, Pearson correlations failed to demonstrate anynificant relationships between CA3 basal dendritic prop-ies and differences scores, P0.10.

    CA1 apical and basal dendritic arborization. CA1 api-l and basal dendritic arborization were unaffected by theatment condition. There were no significant main effectsinteractions for CA1 branch points or branch lengths0.10; Table 1).

    CA1 dendritic spines. A one-way ANOVA for treat-nt showed no significant effects for apical (groups means:trol9.77, stress10.80.7) or basal CA1 spine den-es (control9.80.7, stress9.80.8, Ps0.10). How-er, chronic stress significantly influenced basal spineape (Fig. 3). Chronically restrained rats exhibited a sig-cantly greater proportion of basal spines with headsmpared with controls, F(1,18)8.56, P0.01. Chronicess did not alter spine shape on apical dendrites800.02, 0.840.04, for proportion of spines with

    oth the apical branch points (A) and branch length (B). In addition,ed with LS cells for both stressed and control conditions. Chronicwith SS branch points and length. Data points represent group

    le 1. CA1 apical and basal dendritic morphology (meansS.E.M.)

    Apical Basal

    Branchpoints

    Branch length(m)

    Branchpoints

    Branch length(m)

    ntrol 24.81.4 1624.7120.7 16.90.9 1445.786.2ess 21.91.7 1475.0108.7 16.20.8 1460.338.73driditsigert

    catreor(P

    meconsitievshnifico

    Tab

    CoStr, statistical significance is indicated by means with different letters

  • heeff

    Eff

    Adifmiangretheana tconwiticathedit(3.the

    thacoonF(1

    hibco

    driicatriebehedidrs(pomasp(Fi

    Bo

    AsthamicaPica4921an

    FigCaspiexaheawithme

    Figgro* Pposvs.n

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx 5

    ARTICLE IN PRESSads in control and stress groups, respectively). No otherects were significant, P0.10.

    ects of chronic stress on the Y-maze

    one-way ANOVA for treatment showed no statisticalference for total entries between groups for the full fivenutes, P0.05. However, a recent study demonstratedinteraction between sex and minute for entries, with theatest locomotor activity for female rats occurring withinfirst minute (Conrad et al., 2003). Due to this precedent

    d a possible indication of female habituation to the Y-maze,wo-way mixed-factor ANOVA (treatmentminutes) wasducted to analyze behavior in the first minute comparedh the subsequent four minutes. All groups made signif-ntly more entries during the first minute compared withsubsequent minutes, F(1,75)104.80, P0.001. In ad-

    ion, restrained rats made significantly fewer entries50.2) than their control counterparts (4.20.2) duringfirst minute, F(1,75)5.14, P0.05.All groups entered the novel arm significantly moren the other arm during minute 1, according to a Wil-xon matched pairs test (P.01; Fig. 4A). Moreover, a

    . 3. Basal CA1 dendritic spine proportions (heads to total). (A)mera lucida drawings (1250) represent CA1 basal dendriticnes from control and stress groups. The closed arrow points to anmple of a headless spine whereas the open arrow points to a spined. (B) Stressed rats had significantly more basal dendritic spinesheads compared with controls. Data points represent group

    ansS.E.M. * P0.05 (n10 rats/group).e-way ANOVA for treatment showed a significant effect,,75)4.32, P0.05, with chronically restrained rats ex-

    trospiting higher positive difference scores compared withntrols (Fig. 4B).Spearman correlations performed between CA3 den-

    tic retraction and behavior showed no statistically signif-nt results for morphological correlations with total en-s or difference scores, P0.10. However, a correlationtween CA1 basal spine head proportion (spines withads/total spines) and difference score on the Y-maze

    demonstrate a significant, positive relationship,8)0.54, P0.05. As difference scores became moresitive, the proportion of spine heads increased. Thejority of rats with the greatest difference scores andine heads represented rats from the restrained groupg. 5). There were no other significant effects, P0.10.

    dy weights

    predicted, restrained rats gained weight more slowlyn controls over 21 days of chronic restraint. A 24xed-factor ANOVA (treatmentday) revealed statisti-lly significant main effects of treatment, F(1,77)72.28,0.001 and day, F(3,231)212.6, P0.001 and a signif-nt interaction between treatment and day, F(3,231).44, P0.001. Stressed females gained less weight overdays of restraint (268.92.4 g, 291.73.5 g, for days 1d 21, respectively) compared with non-restrained con-

    . 4. Influence of chronic stress on Y-maze performance. (A) Bothups entered the novel arm more than the other arm,0.05 Nvel vs. Other. (B) Stressed rats had significantly greateritive difference scores compared with control rats, * P0.05, stresscontrol. Data points represent group meansS.E.M. (control

    39, stress n38).ls (272.82.8 g, 329.53.1 g, for days 1 and 21, re-ectively).

  • Thonfemfolfemovharetpospnuco(BodeinttheoryCA

    Effmo

    strregcuregbraretusweetappoal.Thchleastr

    prefeabebe(LaharesstuamGosebemepoan(Fitioch

    itregabal.bepriarethofunadheforpuWhbo1920ch

    Eff

    Cheremehanohapstatmearm20taspethr

    torfac

    Figof Cshithedrit

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx6

    ARTICLE IN PRESSDISCUSSION

    is experiment investigated the effects of chronic stresshippocampal morphology and spatial memory in OVXale rats. The results indicate that CA1 spine shape

    lowing chronic stress may parallel spatial memory inale rats despite robust CA3 dendritic retraction. More-

    er, spatial memory can be intact or perhaps even en-nced in situations in which hippocampal CA3 dendriticraction is present. Consequently, this study did not sup-rt the global interpretation that CA3 morphology predictsatial memory, as demonstrated in male rats. Althoughmerous studies have suggested that ovarian hormonesntribute to functional spatial memory in stressed femaleswman et al., 2002; Bisagno et al., 2003), we havemonstrated that ovarian hormones are not necessary foract spatial memory. Overall, these findings suggest thatCA1 region may play an important role in spatial mem-ability and that it may override the contribution of the3 region following chronic stress in females.

    ects of chronic stress on hippocampalrphology

    CA3. A previous study using intact females demon-ated CA3 dendritic retraction in the basal, but not apicalion (Galea et al., 1997). In contrast, OVX females in therrent study showed apical dendritic retraction in the CA3ion, which was consistent across both measures ofnch points and total branch length. This apical dendriticraction was similar to that seen in previous researching males (Watanabe et al., 1992c; Magarinos and McE-n, 1995a; Galea et al., 1997; Conrad et al., 1999; Sousaal., 2000; Vyas et al., 2002). Perhaps OVX enabled CA3ical dendritic retraction to occur by the removal of anytential neuroprotective effects of estrogen (Simpkins et, 1997; Lee and McEwen, 2001; Bisagno et al., 2003).e current study also supported previous data thatronic stress reduced CA3 basal arbors in females (Ga-

    . 5. Correlation between Y-maze difference scores and proportionA1 basal spine heads. There was a significant, positive relation-

    p for greater difference scores on the Y-maze (i.e. preference fornovel arm) to be associated with higher proportions of CA1 den-ic spines with heads, rs(8).54, P0.05 (n10 rats/group).et al., 1997). In addition, we present novel findings thatess-induced CA3 basal dendritic retraction is carried

    minudominantly by LS and not SS neurons. This uniqueture of stress targeting one cell type in females has alsoen observed in males, but with SS apical dendritesing more susceptible to stress than LS apical arborsmbert et al., 1998). These current findings indicate thatving a bias for one cell type could potentially skewults, which may account for the discrepancy betweendies. An alternate explanation for the discrepanciesong studies (Galea et al., 1997) could be differences inlgi methodology, but this interpretation is mitigated forveral reasons. First, potential artifacts should be similartween experimental treatments. Second, our consistentasures of dendritic retraction derived from branchints and total branch length support our interpretationd confirm that SS cells are more complex than LS cellstch et al., 1989). Finally, observing CA3 dendritic retrac-n using different Golgi procedures lends support thatronic stress alters hippocampal morphology.

    CA1. Chronic stress did not affect spine density, butincreased the proportion of spine heads in the CA1ion. Spines change shape as they mature, most notice-ly by developing a head at the end of the spine (Kasai et, 2003). Large, more mature spines express high num-rs of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepro-onic acid receptors, greater glutamate sensitivity, andassociated with long-term potentiation (LTP), which isught to reflect greater synaptic activity and memoryction (Matsuzaki et al., 2001; Kasai et al., 2003). Indition, drugs that inhibit actin filaments within spineads interfere with LTP, which is thought to be importantmemory coding and consolidation within the hippocam-s (Mesches et al., 1999; Ackermann and Matus, 2003).ile ovarian hormones and androgens induce changes inth spine density and shape in rodents (Gould et al.,90; Woolley et al., 1997; Leranth et al., 2004; Li et al.,04; Gonzalez-Burgos et al., 2005), these data show thatronic stress modulates CA1 spine shape as well.

    ects of chronic stress on the Y-maze

    ronically-stressed rats showed greater novel arm pref-nce compared with controls on the spatial recognitionmory component of the Y-maze. Stress may have en-nced performance by interfering with habituation to thevel arm. This interpretation is supported by impairedbituation to novel environments in male rats followingychosocial stress (Park et al., 2001). Another interpre-ion is that chronic stress facilitated females spatialmory, similar to the facilitative effects found in the radialmaze and object placement tasks (Bowman et al.,

    01, 2002, 2003; Beck and Luine, 2002). This diversity ofks that use novelty or appetitive training suggests thatrformance in females is facilitated by chronic stressough neural mechanisms underlying spatial memory.Non-mnemonic mechanisms, such as changes in mo-ability or motivation, may have also contributed to theilitated performance of stressed females. During the five

    nutes of testing, control and stressed rats made a similarmber of entries in the Y-maze, suggesting that both

  • groWientrieabarm

    erebythasucutoWhior

    mafemthathaMcgethadisbowhCAingMaThce

    anmeenbeshocrespreno(Bomamopa(BuhigmaGasastr20noal.althipofstr

    fluto

    Thmo

    ThgromeCAetCAmaoliz(W17simpa20CAin19fortorelberesbesigtrodlinThhaou

    shmeobstrOVbefrapeSaPegetinoryGronproexrec(Ganmoallha

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx 7

    ARTICLE IN PRESSups had similar motor behavior and motivational ability.thin minute 1, however, stressed females made fewertries than controls. These data indicate that fewer en-s are not necessarily synonymous with reduced motorility or motivation because rats showing the best novelpreference made the fewest entries.Another interpretation is that stress influenced the pref-nce for novelty and altered performance in the Y-mazeaffecting entries into the novel arm. However, male ratst are impaired in the traditional version of the Y-mazeccessfully seek out the novel arm when salient, intrinsices are provided or when the intertrial-interval is reduced1 min (Wright and Conrad, 2005; Kleen et al., 2004).ether chronic stress enhances novelty-seeking behav-in females has yet to be determined.Differences in the perception of restraint betweenles and females may explain the lack of impairment inale spatial memory ability. Some reports have shownt males have less weight gain following chronic stressn females (Watanabe et al., 1992c; Magarinos andEwen, 1995b; Conrad et al., 2003, 2004b), which sug-sts that males may perceive restraint as more stressfuln females. However, the females in the current studyplayed robust CA3 hippocampal dendritic retraction inth the apical and basal regions of the hippocampus,ich is more extensive than the amount of stress-induced3 dendritic retraction previously reported for males us-the same restraint protocol (Watanabe et al., 1992c;garinos and McEwen, 1995a; Conrad et al., 1999).erefore, the females in our study appear to have per-ived restraint as stressful as males.Neurochemical and hormonal changes following OVX

    d/or chronic stress may have contributed to spatialmory performance. Young and aged rats who experi-ce long-term OVX (1.56 months) display significantlytter performance in the water radial-arm maze than theiram counterparts, most likely from the hormonal shiftcurring after OVX (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003). Chronictraint stress affects the dopaminergic system in thefrontal cortex, and these stress-dependent changes aret demonstrated in gonadally-intact stressed femaleswman et al., 2002). Three weeks following OVX, fe-les have a faster recovery of adrenocorticotropin hor-ne (ACTH) and CORT response to shock stress com-red with females given chronic estrogen replacementrgess and Handa, 1992). In general, females haveher basal levels of CORT and release more CORT thanles in response to stress (Atkinson and Waddell, 1997;lea et al., 1997; Rivier, 1999; Kitraki et al., 2004). At theme time, female rats habituate more quickly to restraintess compared with males (Bowman et al., 2001; Luine,02), but these sex differences in CORT production aret always observed (Figueiredo et al., 2002; Conrad et, 2004a). In addition, chronic stress sex-dependentlyers the two CORT receptors within the CA1 region of thepocampus (Kitraki et al., 2004). Any one or combinationthese changes may influence the success of chronically

    essed females in spatial memory tasks. Moreover, these

    tactioctuations may affect other areas in the brain, in additionhippocampal CA3 and CA1 cells.

    e lack of an estrogen influence on hippocampalrphology and behavior

    e similarity of CA1 spine densities across experimentalups may seem surprising, as it has been well docu-nted that acute estrogen treatment increases apical1 spine density (Woolley and McEwen, 1993; Woolleyal., 1997). Within 48 h of administering 17-estradiol,1 spine density increases and these maximal levels areintained for an additional 48 h after estrogen is metab-ed, then density slowly declines over the next few daysoolley and McEwen, 1993; Woolley, 1998). While the-estradiol dose used in this study (0.1 mL of 5 g) isilar to that which has been used in other behavioralradigms (Chesler and Juraska, 2000; Korol and Kolo,02), this dose is too low to fully reproduce increases in1 apical spine density which have been demonstratedmorphological studies (0.1 mL of 10 g; Gould et al.,90; Woolley and McEwen, 1993; Woolley, 1998). There-e, estrogen treatments used in future research will needinclude higher doses in order to determine a more clearationship between changes in the brain and subsequenthavior. In addition to dose, differences between pastearch and the present study may involve a difference inhavioral training and handling procedures. For example,nificant differences in CA1 spine densities between es-gen and vehicle treated rats disappear following han-g for 5 min/day/14 days (Garza-Meilandt et al., 2002).us handling, combined with estrogen replacement, mayve retarded OVX-induced changes in spine density inr rats.Estrogens inability to alter Y-maze performance

    ould also be further explored. Acute estrogen replace-nt can be given 3 weeks to 4 months after OVX toserve changes in memory (Korol and Kolo, 2002; Sand-om and Williams, 2004). Administering 17-estradiol toX females either 72 and 48 h, or 48 and 24 h beforehavioral assessment, which is consistent with our timeme, can either improve or impair spatial memory, de-nding on the task, compared with controls (Frye, 1995;ndstrom and Williams, 2001; Korol and Kolo, 2002).rhaps our handling procedure may have negated estro-ns effects on the Y-maze because of the enriched set-g. Environmental enrichment can increase spatial mem-ability on several tasks (Fernandez-Teruel et al., 1997;

    esack and Frick, 2004). However, a recent finding dem-strated that while mice treated with estrogen had im-ved spatial memory, estrogen combined with previousposure to an enriched environment did not alter objectognition and actually impaired work memory acquisitionresack and Frick, 2004). Although this study used miced an enriched environment protocol lasting severalnths, the results provide an interesting possibility. Sinceof the rats in this study were exposed to the samendling and environmental cues and all animals had in-

    t spatial memory, we cannot separate possible interac-ns between estrogen and the environment.

  • Sig

    Ouingpreretchpaco19mesppretremeal.alwfinOuspin Oshtreanthedeog

    AcThedgJamSe

    Ack

    Atk

    Ban

    Bec

    Bec

    Bim

    Bis

    Bow

    Bow

    Bow

    Bur

    Ch

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Co

    Fer

    Fig

    Fitc

    Fry

    Ga

    Ga

    Go

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx8

    ARTICLE IN PRESSnificance

    r research contributes to growing evidence demonstrat-that CA3 hippocampal morphology does not alwaysdict spatial memory function. Although CA3 dendriticraction is associated with memory deficits in males,ronically-stressed male rats exhibit increased hippocam-l-dependent contextual fear conditioning compared withntrols, despite CA3 dendritic retraction (Conrad et al.,99, 2001). In addition, inhibiting CORT synthesis withtyrapone on the day of training prevents stress-inducedatial memory deficits when dendritic retraction should besent (Wright et al., 2004). Finally, three weeks of CORTatment causes CA3 dendritic retraction, but spatialmory remains intact on the Y-Maze (Coburn-Litvak et, 2003). Therefore, CA3 dendritic retraction does notays parallel spatial memory deficits in males, and ourdings with females contribute to this growing literature.r study indicates that stress-induced changes in CA1ine shape are associated with enhanced spatial memoryVX female rats. These data are consistent with studies

    owing that changes in CA1 spines, following estrogenatment, parallel spatial memory retention (Sandstromd Williams, 2004). Together these findings suggest thatpresence of mature spine heads on CA1 hippocampal

    ndrites may play a more critical role than CA3 morphol-y in predicting spatial memory in females.

    knowledgmentsThis work was funded by MH64727 (Conrad).e contributions of following individuals are gratefully acknowl-ed: Rudy Bellani, Meghan Ellis, Juan Gomez, Gillian Hamilton,es Harman, Jonathan Kleen, Jamie Jackson, Scott Seganti,

    rgey Tsekhanov, and Lindsay Wieczorek.

    REFERENCES

    ermann M, Matus A (2003) Activity-induced targeting of profilin andstabilization of dendritic spine morphology. Nat Neurosci6:11941200.inson HC, Waddell BJ (1997) Circadian variation in basal plasmacorticosterone and adrenocorticotropin in the rat: Sexual dimor-phism and changes across the estrous cycle. Endocrinology138:38423848.nister NJ, Larkman AU (1995) Dendritic morphology of CA1 pyra-midal neurones from the rat hippocampus: I. Branching patterns.J Comp Neurol 360:150160.k KD, Luine VN (2002) Sex differences in behavioral and neuro-chemical profiles after chronic stress: role of housing conditions.Physiol Behav 75:661673.ker JB, Snyder PJ, Miller MM, Westgate SA, Jenuwine MJ (1987)The influence of estrous cycle and intrastriatal estradiol on senso-rimotor performance in the female rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav27:5359.onte-Nelson HA, Singleton RS, Hunter CL, Price KL, Moore AB,Granholm AC (2003) Ovarian hormones and cognition in the agedfemale rat: I. Long-term, but not short-term, ovariectomy enhancesspatial performance. Behav Neurosci 117:13951406.agno V, Bowman R, Luine V (2003) Functional aspects of estrogenneuroprotection. Endocrine 21:3341.man RE, Beck KD, Luine VN (2003) Chronic stress effects onmemory: sex differences in performance and monoaminergic ac-tivity. Horm Behav 43:4859.

    man RE, Ferguson D, Luine VN (2002) Effects of chronic restraintstress and estradiol on open field activity, spatial memory, andmonoaminergic neurotransmitters in ovariectomized rats.Neuroscience 113:401410.man RE, Zrull MC, Luine VN (2001) Chronic restraint stressenhances radial arm maze performance in female rats. Brain Res904:279289.gess LH, Handa RJ (1992) Chronic estrogen-induced alterations inadrenocorticotropin and corticosterone secretion, and glucocorti-coid receptor-mediated functions in female rats. Endocrinology131:12611269.esler EJ, Juraska JM (2000) Acute administration of estrogen andprogesterone impairs the acquisition of the spatial Morris watermaze in ovariectomized rats. Horm Behav 38:234242.burn-Litvak PS, Pothakos K, Tata DA, McCloskey DP, Anderson BJ(2003) Chronic administration of corticosterone impairs spatial ref-erence memory before spatial working memory in rats. NeurobiolLearn Mem 80:1123.hen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS (2003) Applied multiple re-gression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.nrad CD, Galea LA, Kuroda Y, McEwen BS (1996) Chronic stressimpairs rat spatial memory on the Y maze, and this effect isblocked by tianeptine pretreatment. Behav Neurosci 110:13211334.nrad CD, Grote KA, Hobbs RJ, Ferayorni A (2003) Sex differencesin spatial and non-spatial Y-maze performance after chronic stress.Neurobiol Learn Mem 79:3240.nrad CD, Jackson JL, Wieczorek L, Baran SE, Harman JS, WrightRL, Korol DL (2004a) Acute stress impairs spatial memory in malebut not female rats: influence of estrous cycle. Pharmacol BiochemBehav 78:569579.nrad CD, Jackson JL, Wise LS (2004b) Chronic stress enhancesibotenic acid-induced damage selectively within the hippocampalCA3 region of male, but not female rats. Neuroscience125:759767.nrad CD, LeDoux JE, Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1999) Repeatedrestraint stress facilitates fear conditioning independently of caus-ing hippocampal CA3 dendritic atrophy. Behav Neurosci113:902913.nrad CD, Mauldin-Jourdain ML, Hobbs RJ (2001) Metyrapone re-veals that previous chronic stress differentially impairs hippocam-pal-dependent memory. Stress 4:305318.nandez-Teruel A, Escorihuela RM, Castellano B, Gonzalez B, To-bena A (1997) Neonatal handling and environmental enrichmenteffects on emotionality, novelty/reward seeking, and age-relatedcognitive and hippocampal impairments: focus on the Roman ratlines. Behav Genet 27:513526.ueiredo HF, Dolgas CM, Herman JP (2002) Stress activation ofcortex and hippocampus is modulated by sex and stage of estrus.Endocrinology 143:25342540.h JM, Juraska JM, Washington LW (1989) The dendritic morphol-ogy of pyramidal neurons in the rat hippocampal CA3 area. I. Celltypes. Brain Res 479:105114.e CA (1995) Estrus-associated decrements in a water maze taskare limited to acquisition. Physiol Behav 57:514.lea LA, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Deak T, Spencer RL, Dhabhar FS(1997) Sex differences in dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neu-rons in response to chronic restraint stress. Neuroscience81:689697.rza-Meilandt A, Cantu RE, Stone KK, Zamora DA, Jaffe DB, Clai-borne BJ (2002) Effects of estrogen treatment and handling onspatial learning and spine densities on hippocampal CA1 neurons.In: 2002 Abstract Viewer: Society for Neuroscience. No. 574.14,Washington, DC.nzalez-Burgos I, Alejandre-Gomez M, Cervantes M (2005) Spine-

    type densities of hippocampal CA1 neurons vary in proestrus andestrus rats. Neurosci Lett 379:5254.

  • Go

    Gre

    He

    Kas

    Ke

    Kirs

    Kitr

    Kle

    Kor

    Lam

    Lee

    Ler

    Li C

    Lo

    Lui

    Lui

    Lui

    Ma

    Ma

    Ma

    Ma

    Ma

    Me

    Pap

    Par

    Riv

    San

    San

    She

    She

    Sho

    Sim

    Sou

    Via

    Vya

    Wa

    Wa

    Wa

    We

    We

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx 9

    ARTICLE IN PRESSuld E, Woolley CS, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS (1990) Gonadalsteroids regulate dendritic spine density in hippocampal pyramidalcells in adulthood. J Neurosci 10:12861291.sack JE, Frick KM (2004) Environmental enrichment reduces themnemonic and neural benefits of estrogen. Neuroscience128:459471.ller W (1993) Gender differences in depression: perspectives fromneuropsychology. J Affect Disord 29:129143.ai H, Matsuzaki M, Noguchi J, Yasumatsu N, Nakahara H (2003)Structure-stability-function relationships of dendritic spines. TrendsNeurosci 26:360368.ssler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB(1993) Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey.I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. J Affect Disord29:8596.chbaum C, Wust S, Hellhammer D (1992) Consistent sex differ-ences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. PsychosomMed 54:648657.aki E, Kremmyda O, Youlatos D, Alexis MN, Kittas C (2004) Gen-der-dependent alterations in corticosteroid receptor status andspatial performance following 21 days of restraint stress. Neuro-science 125:4755.en JK, Sitomer MT, Killeen PR, Conrad CD (2004) Changes inoperant behavior during and after chronic stress: A look at moti-vation, motor ability, and memory. In: 2004 Abstract Viewer: Soci-ety for Neuroscience. No. 426.6, San Diego.ol DL, Kolo LL (2002) Estrogen-induced changes in place andresponse learning in young adult female rats. Behav Neurosci116:411420.bert KG, Buckelew SK, Staffiso-Sandoz G, Gaffga S, CarpenterW, Fisher J, Kinsley CH (1998) Activity-stress induces atrophy ofapical dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons in male rats.Physiol Behav 65:4349.SJ, McEwen BS (2001) Neurotrophic and neuroprotective actionsof estrogens and their therapeutic implications. Annu Rev Phar-macol Toxicol 41:569591.anth C, Hajszan T, MacLusky NJ (2004) Androgens increase spinesynapse density in the CA1 hippocampal subfield of ovariecto-mized female rats. J Neurosci 24:495499., Brake WG, Romeo RD, Dunlop JC, Gordon M, Buzescu R,Magarinos AM, Allen PB, Greengard P, Luine V, McEwen BS(2004) Estrogen alters hippocampal dendritic spine shape andenhances synaptic protein immunoreactivity and spatial mem-ory in female mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:21852190.MJ, Wang PS (2003) Relative and combined effects of estradioland prolactin on corticosterone secretion in ovariectomized rats.Chin J Physiol 46:103109.ne V (2002) Sex differences in chronic stress effects on memory inrats. Stress 5:205216.ne V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS (1994a) Repeated stresscauses reversible impairments of spatial memory performance.Brain Res 639:167170.ne V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS (1994b) Stress-depen-dent impairments of spatial memory. Role of 5-HT. Ann N Y AcadSci 746:403404.garinos AM, McEwen BS (1995a) Stress-induced atrophy of apicaldendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors.Neuroscience 69:8388.garinos AM, McEwen BS (1995b) Stress-induced atrophy of apicaldendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: involvement of glucocor-ticoid secretion and excitatory amino acid receptors. Neuroscience69:8998.garinos AM, Orchinik M, McEwen BS (1998) Morphologicalchanges in the hippocampal CA3 region induced by non-inva-

    sive glucocorticoid administration: a paradox. Brain Res 809:314318.

    Workham JA, Pych JC, Juraska JM (2002) Ovarian hormone replace-ment to aged ovariectomized female rats benefits acquisition of theMorris water maze. Horm Behav 42:284293.tsuzaki M, Ellis-Davies GC, Nemoto T, Miyashita Y, Iino M, Kasai H(2001) Dendritic spine geometry is critical for AMPA receptor ex-pression in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Nat Neurosci4:10861092.sches MH, Fleshner M, Heman KL, Rose GM, Diamond DM (1999)Exposing rats to a predator blocks primed burst potentiation in thehippocampus in vitro. J Neurosci 19:RC18.a M, Bundman MC, Greenberger V, Segal M (1995) Morphologicalanalysis of dendritic spine development in primary cultures ofhippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 15:111.k CR, Campbell AM, Diamond DM (2001) Chronic psychosocialstress impairs learning and memory and increases sensitivity toyohimbine in adult rats. Biol Psychiatry 50:9941004.ier C (1999) Gender, sex steroids, corticotropin-releasing factor,nitric oxide, and the HPA response to stress. Pharmacol BiochemBehav 64:739751.dstrom NJ, Williams CL (2001) Memory retention is modulated byacute estradiol and progesteone replacement. Behav Neurosci115:384393.dstrom NJ, Williams CL (2004) Spatial memory retention is en-hanced by acute and continuous estradiol replacement. HormBehav 45:128135.line YI (1996) Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: a resultof depression-induced neurotoxicity? Mol Psychiatry 1:298299.line YI, Sanghavi M, Mintun MA, Gado MH (1999) Depressionduration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medicallyhealthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci19:50345043.rs TJ, Chua C, Falduto J (2001) Sex differences and oppositeeffects of stress on dendritic spine density in the male versusfemale hippocampus. J Neurosci 21:62926297.pkins JW, Green PS, Gridley KE, Singh M, de Fiebre NC, Raja-kumar G (1997) Role of estrogen replacement therapy in memoryenhancement and the prevention of neuronal loss associated withAlzheimers disease. Am J Med 103:19S25S.sa N, Lukoyanov NV, Madeira MD, Almeida OF, Paula-BarbosaMM (2000) Reorganization of the morphology of hippocampal neu-rites and synapses after stress-induced damage correlates withbehavioral improvement. Neuroscience 97:253266.u V, Meaney MJ (1991) Variations in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to stress during the estrous cycle in the rat.Endocrinology 129:25032511.s A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Chattarji S (2002)Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodelingin hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci 22:68106818.tanabe Y, Gould E, Cameron HA, Daniels DC, McEwen BS (1992a)Phenytoin prevents stress- and corticosterone-induced atrophy ofCA3 pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus 2:431435.tanabe Y, Gould E, Daniels DC, Cameron H, McEwen BS (1992b)Tianeptine attenuates stress-induced morphological changes inthe hippocampus. Eur J Pharmacol 222:157162.tanabe Y, Gould E, McEwen BS (1992c) Stress induces atrophy ofapical dendrites of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. BrainRes 588:341345.issman MM, Bland R, Joyce PR, Newman S, Wells JE, WittchenHU (1993) Sex differences in rates of depression: cross-nationalperspectives. J Affect Disord 29:7784.stenbroek C, Ter Horst GJ, Roos MH, Kuipers SD, Trentani A, denBoer JA (2003) Gender-specific effects of social housing in ratsafter chronic mild stress exposure. Prog NeuropsychopharmacolBiol Psychiatry 27:2130.

    lf OT, Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, McEwen BS, Kirschbaum C(2001) The relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and

  • Wo

    Wo

    Wo

    K. J. McLaughlin et al. / Neuroscience xx (2005) xxx10

    ARTICLE IN PRESSmemory differs between men and women. Psychoneuro-endocrinology 26:711720.olley CS (1998) Estrogen-mediated structural and functionalsynaptic plasticity in the female rat hippocampus. Horm Behav34:140148.olley CS, Gould E, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS (1990) Naturallyoccurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocam-pal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 10:40354039.olley CS, McEwen BS (1993) Roles of estradiol and proges-terone in regulation of hippocampal dendritic spine density dur-ing the estrous cycle in the rat. J Comp Neurol 336:293306.

    Woolley CS, Weiland NG, McEwen BS, Schwartzkroin PA (1997)Estradiol increases the sensitivity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidalcells to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic input: correlation withdendritic spine density. J Neurosci 17:18481859.

    Wright RL, Conrad CD (2005) Chronic stress leaves novelty-seekingbehavior intact while impairing spatial recognition memory in theY-maze. Stress 8:151154.

    Wright RL, Lightner EN, Bellani R, Harman JS, Conrad CD (2004)Chronic stress-induced spatial memory deficits are prevented bymetyrapone treatment on the day of behavioral assessment. In:2004 Abstract Viewer: Society for Neuroscience. No. 316.3, SanDiego.

    (Accepted 16 June 2005)

    CHRONIC STRESS ENHANCES SPATIAL MEMORY IN OVARIECTOMIZED FEMALE RATS DESPITE CA3 DENDRITIC RETRACTION: POSSIBLE INVOLVEMENT OF CA1 NEURONSEXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURESSubjectsOVX17- Estradiol administrationRestraint and handlingY-maze testingGolgi procedureHistologyCA3.CA1.

    Statistical analysesGolgiY-mazeBody weightsSoftware

    RESULTSEffects of chronic stress on hippocampal morphologyCA3 apical dendritic arborizationCA3 basal dendritic arborizationCA1 apical and basal dendritic arborizationCA1 dendritic spines

    Effects of chronic stress on the Y-mazeBody weights

    DISCUSSIONEffects of chronic stress on hippocampal morphologyCA3.CA1.

    Effects of chronic stress on the Y-mazeThe lack of an estrogen influence on hippocampal morphology and behaviorSignificance

    AcknowledgmentsREFERENCES