loots narrative self article

Upload: isiplaya

Post on 30-May-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    1/20

    http://qix.sagepub.com

    Qualitative Inquiry

    DOI: 10.1177/10778004083143562008; 14; 632Qualitative Inquiry

    Jasmina Sermijn, Patrick Devlieger and Gerrit LootsStory

    The Narrative Construction of the Self: Selfhood as a Rhizomatic

    http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/14/4/632

    The online version of this article can be found at:

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:Qualitative InquiryAdditional services and information for

    http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://qix.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/14/4/632SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms):

    (this article cites 7 articles hosted on theCitations

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://qix.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://qix.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/14/4/632http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/14/4/632http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/14/4/632http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://qix.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://qix.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    2/20

    632

    Authors Note: We thank Charlotte for sharing her story with us. We also thank Norman K.

    Denzin and the reviewers ofQualitative Inquiry for their useful comments.

    The Narrative Constructionof the Self

    Selfhood as a Rhizomatic Story

    Jasmina SermijnVrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium

    Patrick DevliegerKatholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium

    University of Illinois, Chicago

    Gerrit LootsVrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium

    University of Ghent, Belgium

    In this article, the authors use the metaphor of the rhizome of the French philoso-

    phers Deleuze and Guattari as an experimental methodological concept to

    study the narrative construction of the self. By considering the self as a

    rhizomatic story, the authors create a story structure that not only offers a

    useful view on the way in which people narratively construct their selfhood but

    also stimulates an experiment with alternative, nontraditional presentation

    forms. The researcher is no longer listening from a distance to the stories of

    the participant and subsequently represents these stories. She or he becomes

    a part of the rhizome. The authors illustrate this rhizomatic approach and its

    research possibilities by presenting story fragments from their research.

    Keywords: narrative construction; self; rhizome; nontraditional presentation

    forms

    Researcher: As I already mentioned by e-mail and phone, Im interested in

    the way people who have received a medicalpsychiatric diagnosis tell

    about themselves. So the idea is that you tell me more about yourself and

    your life today. I dont have any prepared questions, so its really the idea

    that you tell about whats important to you. Every once in a while Ill ask

    a question if I havent understood something.

    Qualitative Inquiry

    Volume 14 Number 4

    June 2008 632-650

    2008 Sage Publications

    10.1177/1077800408314356

    http://qix.sagepub.comhosted at

    http://online.sagepub.com

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    3/20

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 633

    Charlotte: OK. [laughs] OK, so where can I start. . . .

    Researcher:You really can start where you want to, tell about whats important

    to you.Charlotte: Yeah, . . . lets see, yeah, thats hard. [laughs] Yeah, I just say,

    thats tricky, its so hard to start from nothing, . . . Yeah, maybe Ill just

    tell what Im thinking right now. . . . So last year, or was it two years

    ago in the summer, I wasnt happy with myself. I didnt feel fat, but

    I didnt feel good about myself and I started to eat less and less, I didnt

    think I was pretty, but I still ate. But I ate less and less and then I noticed

    that that made me feel good and that I was so strict and imposed these rules

    on myself. So I did that for a year and it got worse and worse, eating less,

    eating nothing. It got worse and worse and then I really started to thinkthat I was fat. I think about something else: actually I was already inter-

    ested in anorexia earlier. I remember that I used to read books, novels

    about girls who thought they were fat, and once I gave a presentation

    about it. Maybe thats the reason, what made me actually get it. I dont

    know. . . . The psychologist says its other stuff, that it has to do with

    my relationship with my parents . . . but I dont believe that. I think

    that maybe it comes from those books. . . . What would my parents

    have to do with it? . . . Yeah, I knew it wasnt good, but I didnt want

    to admit that something was wrong because I felt perfect, still not good,but I felt better and better if I ate less and so and when I went to the hos-

    pital I had to go to the psychologist every week for an hour and I really

    liked that. In the beginning it was so hard there . . . then my eating

    schedule was adjusted. I had to eat more, that was really hard, all these

    things I never wanted to eat. Now I dont have problems anymore with

    it. Now Im completely better. . . . I dont know if Im really completely

    better but I feel completely better, but I dont know if Im completely

    better. I feel completely better. This periods kind of in the past. Yeah, its

    still . . . if I see other girls eating an apple for lunch, then I think, Uhoh, they have anorexia. I dont want to talk with anyone about it, espe-

    cially not with girls, its kind of being scared that others can do what I

    couldnt. Now I can just . . . if I went out to eat, Oh no, then I have to

    eat something again, oh no, not a school trip because then I have to eat

    again. Oh no, the whole time with food and now school trips arent a

    problem and I can go on vacation again with others. There are still some

    of those things in my head, there are still some of those things, I want to

    eat healthy: no fries, I never want to eat that, no cake, that kind of thing

    and still with other girls I notice what they eat. Watch what I eat. I usedto think about it all the time when I came home: Oh, she ate an apple,

    maybe shell get skinnier than me. Now its not like that anymore, now

    I think Stupid girl. I still notice that stuff, for instance yesterday there

    was a girl at school who only ate an apple for lunch. Yeah, just an apple!

    So now its all much easier, lifes a lot easier. Yeah, and otherwise . . .

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    4/20

    634 Qualitative Inquiry

    I have a lot of hobbies, especially sports, tennis and hockey, I really enjoy

    that and I want to do it well, just like at school. Im in my last year and

    get good grades, I expect a lot from myself. . . . Wait a minute, look,this is a photo of my dog who ran away a while ago. When I was six he

    showed up and my dad wanted a dog and me and the others, my mom

    and my sisters liked the idea too.

    The above story fragment is an excerpt of the first conversation I had

    with Charlotte1the very first participant whom I met in the context of a

    research project about the way narrative selfhood is constructed.2 When

    I asked her to tell about herself and her life experiences, I noticed that the

    manner in which she was telling was different from what I implicitly had

    expected. I expected a coherent story with a beginning, a middle, and an end.

    This implicit expectation originates from the (Western) dominant traditional

    discourse of how a self-story should look. The traditional notion considers

    a story as a linear and complete whole which is characterized by a plot, a

    unity which isjust like an embroidered quiltspatially and temporally

    structured (e.g., Bruner, 1986, 2002; Connelly & Clandinin, 1986, 1990;

    Polkinghorne, 1995; Ricoeur, 1983, 1985, 1990).3 From this viewpoint, the

    narrative self of a person can be seen as a traditional story which, althoughit is temporally variable, is characterized by the presence of a plot that turns

    the story (and the self) into a linear, structured whole.4 However, the story

    fragment that Charlotte told me (just like the rest of her tellings) about

    herself is neither completely coherent nor completely linearly structured

    around one plot. On the contrary, Charlotte rather told an amalgam of

    separatesometimes contradictoryfragments of memories, feelings, events,

    and ideas. Although some parts of her story do share some traditional story

    properties, there are just as many contradictory and discontinuous storyelements present as well.

    This experience formed the starting point for us to search for an alterna-

    tive story notion, an alternative view on narrative selfhood that can offer

    researchers a supporting framework when listening to, interpreting, and

    presenting the stories that research participants tell about themselves.

    Untamed Stories: Selfhood as a Postmodern Story

    The gap that we experienced between the traditional story notion and

    the manner in which Charlotte told us about herself automatically raised the

    following question: Are the narrative characteristicsas described in the

    traditional story notionactually typical for human nature? The postmodern

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    5/20

    perspective that everything is a story (Currie, 1998) stems from the idea that

    traditional story characteristics are not inherentin stories, nor in people, but

    rather must be viewed as sociocultural constructs (see Butler, 1990; Linde,1993; Maan, 1999). Numerous inter- and intracultural research projects

    (e.g., Carrithers, Collins, & Lukes, 1985; Foucault, 1975, 1976, 1988; Geertz,

    1973; Schneebaum, 1969; Shorter, 1977) have shown that the way people

    view themselves and tell about themselves is not universal and that the tradi-

    tional story characteristics and also the traditional story itself are no more

    than effects of discourse, creations that are used within certain subcultures.

    Postmodern narratologists hence assume that as narrative characteristics are

    not inherent in human nature, a universal definition of the essence of a storyis impossible. Although the vagueness and lack of boundaries that are typical

    for the postmodern story notion make it impossible to clearly define what a

    story is or is not, we do find several characteristics in postmodern narrative

    theory (see Currie, 1998; Gibson, 2004; Herman & Vervaeck, 2005) that are

    considered as typical for postmodern stories:

    No synthesis of heterogeneity (the story elements are not synthesized

    around a plot)

    No hierarchy but rather narrative laterality (a story is a compilation of

    horizontal story elements)

    Acceptance of the monster (of the entirety of elements that do not fit in

    a traditional story structure):5

    Monstrous time (nonlinearly organized time; e.g., story elements that

    are difficult to date or that conflict with the separation among past

    presentfuture)

    Monstrous causality (a lack of clear, linear cause and effect relationships)

    Monstrous space (space that is constantly in motion and that lacks a fixed

    central point)

    These characteristics clarify that the postmodern notion looks at stories

    through a completely different lens than does the traditional notion.

    Although the traditional notion emphasizes the necessity of streamlining all

    story elements into one complete, organized whole (like the motif of the

    embroidered quilt), the postmodern notion emphasizes everything that is

    excluded from the traditional story notion. The postmodern notion values

    the acceptance of everything that does not fit in a streamlined story, of thestory elements that do not find a place in a traditional story structure. Just

    like the motif of a patchwork quilt,6 a postmodern story is characterized not

    by an embroidered, continuous pattern but by the juxtaposition of more or

    less disjunctive elements. Consequently, postmodern stories are also referred

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 635

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    6/20

    to as untamed stories or les savages narratives (Herman & Vervaeck,

    2005, p. 114).

    Adopting the postmodern story notion, we could view the self as anuntamed story, a story that consists of a heterogeneous collection of hori-

    zontal and sometimes monstrous story elements that persons tell about

    themselves and that are not synthesized into one coherent story from which

    they derive their selfhood. This visionthe narrative self as a postmodern

    storyis related to the postmodern idea that the self has no stable core but

    is multiple, multivoiced, discontinuous, and fragmented (e.g., Davies &

    Harr, 1990; Derrida, 1976; Gergen, 1989, 1991; Lyotard, 1979). From this

    viewpoint, the self is not something that is inherently given, is fixed, or hasone core. On the contrary, the self can be compared with a buzzing beehive

    so agile and inconsistent, we can barely keep track of it (Rosseel, 2001,

    p. 127). In this buzzing beehive there arent fixed coherent and united

    stories but rather variable, temporary, interacting components.

    When we look back at Charlottes story fragment, we see that the post-

    modern story notion clearly fits better with daily narrative practice than the

    traditional story notion. The story that Charlotte tells about herself is not a

    traditional story but rather a heterogeneous collection of sometimes stream-lined, sometimes untamed story elements. Considering both story notions,

    for the time being we can conclude that the postmodern notion creates

    space for alternative story structures that connect better with the daily narra-

    tive practices we come in contact with as researchers.

    Despite the above-mentioned characteristics, the postmodern notion of a

    self-story remains vague and offers little dread to narrative researchers in

    their labyrinth of research practices. In the rest of this article, we therefore

    elaborate a metaphor of the self-as-a-story, that of the narrative self as a

    rhizome (Deleuze & Guattari, 1976). A metaphor is a literary device that

    figuratively specifies that X (a target) is Y (a source), thus providing a map

    of one concept to another (Schuh & Cunningham, 2004, p. 325). But

    metaphors are more than simple literary devices, they are a foundation for

    human thought processes (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980, quoted in Schuh &

    Cunningham, 2004, p. 326) and guide our views of the world and our

    inquiry into its characteristics (Schuh & Cunningham, 2004, p. 325). By

    comparing the narrative self with a rhizomatic story, we create a vision that

    can help researchers to reflecton the abstract concept of narrative selfhood,on the way selfhood is narratively constructed, and on their own positions

    in this construction work. When we use the word reflecthere, we refer to the

    fact that the researcher understand that she/he is also caught up in processes

    of subjectification and sees simultaneously the object/subject of her/his

    636 Qualitative Inquiry

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    7/20

    gaze andthe means by which the object/subject is being constituted (the

    co-constructed character of the self-story and the position of the researcher

    herein; Davies et al., 2004, p. 361). Furthermore, we argue that the metaphorof the self as a rhizomatic story contributes to dealing with the tensions in

    the ambivalent practices of reflexivity that risk to slip inadvertently into

    constituting the very real self that transcends the constitutive power of

    discourse (Davies et al., 2004).

    Before describing the metaphor of the rhizome, its characteristics, and

    its usefulness to reflect on narrative selfhood, we would like to emphasize

    that applying this metaphor does not necessarily mean that we will remain

    entirely consistent with the philosophy of Deleuze and Guattari. Adoptingthe words of Deleuze and Guattari (1976), Thinking is experimenting!

    and our application of rhizomatic thinking to narrative selfhood must be

    viewed as a thought experiment and not as a closed methodological or theo-

    retical vision. This is in line with Deleuze and Guattaris resistance to every

    form of totalitarian thinking and to primarily try to stimulate new forms of

    thinking. We are therefore looking not for the answer, the methodology, or

    the theory to explore narrative selfhood but rather for a new and possible

    perspective that can be a supporting framework in the labyrinth of narrativeresearch practices.

    The Rhizome as a Metaphor for the NarrativeConstruction of Selfhood

    A rhizome is an underground root system, a dynamic, open, decentralized

    network that branches out to all sides unpredictably and horizontally. A view

    of the whole is therefore impossible. A rhizome can take the most diverse

    forms: from splitting and spreading in all directions on the surface to the

    form of bulbs and tubers. The most important characteristic of a rhizome is

    that it has multiple entryways. From whichever side one enters, as soon as

    one is in, one is connected. There is no main entryway or starting point

    that leads to the truth. The truth or the reality does not exist within

    rhizomatic thinking. There are always many possible truths and realities that

    can all be viewed as social constructs. The existence of multiple entryways

    automatically implies multiplicity. With the principle of multiplicity, Deleuzeand Guattari refer to the existence of a multiplicity that does not get reduced

    to a whole on subject or object level but rather only consists of definitions

    or dimensions. The notion of unity only appears when a particular dimension

    (e.g., a particular discourse) takes over. But such a takeover can only be

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 637

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    8/20

    viewed as an artificial unity in the multiplicity. Within the multiplicity, there

    is no clear hierarchy, structure, or order. This implies that each point of a

    rhizome can be connected with any other point in the rhizome (the principleofconnection), and at whatever point a rhizome is ruptured or destroyed, it

    will always grow further according to different lines or connections (the

    principle ofasignifying rupture). Deleuze and Guattari compare the rhizome

    with a map (the principle ofcartography) and not with a blueprint or a tracing.

    Just like a map, a rhizome is open, receptive to include changes constantly.

    Here, we encounter the characteristic of multiple entryways: A map always

    has multiple entryways, all of which are equally good or equally important.

    With a map, one can start where one wants; no single entryway is privileged.The only thing that changes as one chooses a different entryway is the map

    of the rhizome itself (Deleuze & Guattari, 1976).

    How can this metaphor of the rhizome help researchers to reflect on the

    narrative construction of selfhood and their own positions in this process of

    construction? To explore this question, we take each of the characteristics or

    principles of the rhizome and apply them to narrative selfhood. We illustrate

    these principles using story fragments of Charlotte.

    Multiple Entryways

    When we view selfhood as a rhizomatic story, we assume that there is

    no single correct point of entry that can lead the researcher to the truth

    about the selfhood of the participant. We completely let go of the illusion

    of the so-called objective all-seeing eye/I (Davies et al., 2004, p. 363) that

    can capture the reality, the real narrative self of someone. In contrast to the

    traditional story, which has only one entry and exit point (the beginning and

    the end), selfhood as a rhizomatic story has many possible entryways, and

    each entryway will lead to a temporary rendering of selfhood. This implies

    that there is no such thing as a fixed authentic, prediscursive self that exists

    independent of the speaking. To use Barthess words, We give birth to our-

    selves in our writing and speaking (in Davies et al., 2004, p. 365). This

    means that the birth of selves is coincidental with the speaking and that we

    speak ourselves as multiple in the multiple stories we create of ourselves.

    The self as a noun (stable and relatively fixed) is moved to the self as a verb,

    always in process, taking its shape in and through the speaking (Davieset al., 2004). Each time we speak, at the same time a new self is born, embod-

    ied in the story constructionsable to be spoken and read in multiple ways

    (Davies et al., 2004). So each time the researcher asks a participant to tell

    about herself or himself, only one or a few possible and temporal entryways

    638 Qualitative Inquiry

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    9/20

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 639

    into the rhizomatic network are taken. Which entries are taken can depend

    on many factors, but will, among other things, be codetermined by the audience

    to whom the participant is speaking (in the first place, to the researcher), thecontext within which the speaking takes place (the social and cultural dis-

    course context, the research context), the research question (the way the

    researcher presents the research and asks questions), the positions of partici-

    pant and researcher (e.g., age, gender, objectives, ideas and ideologies, etc.),

    and the gazeboth the reflecting or critical gaze of the other (in the first

    place, the researcher) and the controlling self-disciplining gaze (Davies

    et al., 2004) of the speaking participant herself or himself. Along with this,

    the researcher becomes part of the rhizome: As soon as youre in, youreconnected. As researchers, we cannot possibly remain outside the rhizomatic

    story as objective observers: We are within the rhizomatic story as a part

    of the dynamic construction process.

    Suppose we view the narrative selfhood of Charlotte as a rhizomatic story

    with multiple entries. What exactly does this mean?

    To begin with, the principle of multiple entries implies that we assume

    that something like the right entry, the right question to discover Charlottes

    selfhood doesnt exist. What would the right entryway be anyway? Whensomething like a true core self doesnt exist and when the selves of Charlotte

    are born and reborn each time she speaks, there cannot be only one correct

    entryway to selfhood.

    Just as the other research participants, Charlotte indicates that she doesnt

    know where to start her story,

    Where can I start? . . . Its so hard to start from nothing.

    After a while she simply says that shell tell what shes thinking at that

    moment, after which she enters her story with the period when she first

    started to eat less:

    So last year, or was it two years ago in the summer, I wasnt happy with

    myself. I didnt feel fat, but I didnt feel good about myself and I started to

    eat less and less, I didnt think I was pretty, but I still ate.

    Why does sheor is it we?take(s) this entry? Would she take the sameentry if, in another context, she would tell someone else about herself?

    A bit later in the conversation, I asked her this question:

    Researcher: When I told you in the beginning of our conversation that you

    could tell about yourself, that you could tell about whats important to

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    10/20

    640 Qualitative Inquiry

    you, you said that you found it difficult to know where you should start.

    Then you started to tell about the period when you started to eat less.

    Would you start your story there if you would tell it to someone else, forinstance someone new who you meet and who asks you to tell about

    yourself?

    Charlotte: No, I would never tell that part, never that part of myself which

    I now told to you. No . . . I even think that I wouldnt ever tell it to

    someone new who I met . . . for me its part of the past. I told you

    because I know youre interested in it, for your research and stuff.

    Charlottes reply clearly shows that the entry someone uses must be placed

    in the context within which the telling takes place. Charlotte starts her storywith telling about that part of herself that she thinks is importantfor me,for

    my research. The discourse context implicitly determines the possibilities of

    the speaking subject. Because Charlotte knows that Im interested in the way

    people who have received a diagnosis tell about themselves, she tries to

    constitute herself as a good participant, that is, a person who tells about her

    or his experiences of getting a psychiatric diagnosis. In other contexts, to

    other persons, she would probably take very different entries, construct very

    different stories about herself. However, this does not mean that the storyCharlotte tells about herself cannot be considered as real or true. To use

    the words of Saukko (2000), the individual stories of persons are

    real and rich accounts of how they have used and been used by diverse dis-

    courses in a particular local situation. Even if the individual stories are true

    per se, they are only a part of a larger discursive panorama. (p. 303)

    Or to use the words of Davies et al. (2004),

    The self both is and is not a fiction, is unified and transcendent and frag-

    mented and always in the process of being constituted, can be spoken of in

    realist ways and it cannot, and its voice can be claimed as authentic and there

    is no guarantee of authenticity. (p. 384)

    Every speaking, every voice, and thus every manifestation of the self is

    embedded in a specific discourse context, a context that on one hand makes

    the speaking possible but on the other hand shapes and limits what can besaid in a particular situation. Consequently, it is important that one as a

    researcher is aware and reflexive of the fact that the discourse context and

    the gaze (also the one of the researcher herself or himself) have an influence

    on the narrative construction process of selfhood.

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    11/20

  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    12/20

    642 Qualitative Inquiry

    bound within one coherent meta-story and to see itself in all its shifting, con-

    tradictory multiplicity and fragility (Davies et al., 2004). Each entry of the

    rhizomatic story leads to other, sometimes contradictory, story fragments.The self-as-story need no longer be viewed as an embroidered quilt (a com-

    plete, organized whole) but rather as a patchwork of infinite, never-ending

    narrative constructions about oneself. Through time, the stitching of the

    patchwork quilt takes on a course that connects certain elements, providing

    a time-limited embroidered piece that, however, could never account for the

    entire self (see also Saukko, 2000). In this stitching, there is always a continual

    ambiguity between the multiple or always shifting I and the traditional I,

    that seeks for unity and continuity in its telling. The result of that is a neverending quilt of which certain parts have an embroidered motif, while other

    parts are patched without following an organized pattern.

    When we link the principle of multiplicity back to the example of

    Charlotte, this means first of all that we assume that Charlottes narrative

    selfhood is composed of a multitude of stories which cannot be reduced to

    one whole and of which weas researchers only get a glimpse of. Even when

    we only look at the story fragments we used as an example, we notice that

    these do not form one unity but consist of a multitude of story elements, amultitude of different voices, some of which are coherent and linearly struc-

    tured, while others are contradictory.

    Now Im completely better

    I dont know if Im really completely better

    I feel completely better

    Its the past for me, that period

    Theres still things some of those things in my head . . .

    How could we reduce such shifting and contradictory elements to a whole?

    And imagine that one as a researcher would one way or another forcefully

    create a coherent whole (by imposing an hypothetical linear causality, for

    instance), wouldnt this be a disservice to what Charlotte tells about herself?

    The fact that rhizome thinking views unity as an illusion, doesnt mean

    that Charlotte experiences the unity and coherencewhich she tries to create

    between certain story elements as artificial. When she tells, for instance, that

    she thinks that the reason or cause for her anorexia stems from the fact thatshe read many books about anorexia when she was young,

    I remember that I used to read books, novels about girls who thought they

    were fat, and once I gave a presentation about it. Maybe thats the reason,

    what made me actually get it. . . . I think that it comes from those books.

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    13/20

    She views this story construction at the moment she tells it as the truth. At

    that moment, there is a temporary take-over by this story construction with

    the result that other possible constructions are excluded. But the fact thatshe says a little bit later that her psychologist thinks that it could be other

    things, other possible connections already shows that other entryways and

    stories can exist which are not taken at that moment:

    The psychologist says its other stuff, that it has to do with my relationship

    with my parents . . . but I dont believe that

    Connection and Asignifying Rupture

    The principle of connection refers to the fact that the stories which people

    co-construct about themselves are not always structured according to logical,

    linear connections. The traditional embroidered quilt (linear, cause

    effect) thinking lapses and is replaced by a patchwork thinking: a thinking

    in terms of infinite possible connections (every line has the potential to be

    connected with every other line), some of which (depending on the entryways

    which one takes) are linked during the speaking, while many others remain

    unlinked. Here too we can see the ambiguity between the multiple/always

    shifting I and the I that seeks for coherence. However, the speaking I is always

    in motion; it tries to make those necessarily situated connections (temporary

    stitches in the quilt) that can help her or him to survive (Braidotti, 1994).

    When Charlotte makes the causal connection between her anorexia and the

    reading of books about girls who thought they were fat or the way her parents

    treat her, she makes a situated connection that can help herhowever

    temporaryto grasp the origin of her anorexia.

    However, this creation of coherent connections is not always possible.When, for instance, we look back at the contradictory voices that emerge in

    Charlottes story (now Im completely better, I dont know if Im really

    completely better, I feel completely better, etc.), we notice that at the

    moment when she speaks these voices, she is not able to connect them with

    logicallinear principles. The voices she struggles with at that moment

    cannot be integrated into a coherent statement.

    The principle of the asignifying rupture implies that the connections

    between the story elements can be shattered at any moment and replacedby new connections. In a rhizome, a rupture is never fatal, as new connections

    that create new paths always arise.

    We can see an example of such a rupture when we look at the following

    fragment, which Charlotte told 2 weeks later:

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 643

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    14/20

    . . . In the meantime Ive also spoken with my psychologist about the

    possible reasons, the causes for my anorexia. Do you still remember . . .

    last time I said that I thought I got it because I started reading books about itwhen I was young. But in the meantime I have another idea about it. . . .

    I was thinking and I now I know that it didnt come from that. Those books,

    that was already the beginning, already a first symptom. Now I know that it

    has to do with my parents, with the way they treat me. You know, I always

    have to do all sorts of things for them, they know everything about me, they

    want to control everything about me. And the anorexia, they couldnt control

    that. . . . It was my way to resist them . . . something that was only mine.

    This fragment from Charlottes story nicely shows how a certain connectioncan be broken and a new connection can appear to take its place. Charlotte

    and her psychologist co-created a new possible entryway in the rhizomatic

    story network to look at her anorexia. This example shows also the impor-

    tance of discourse and the presence of the gaze of the Other in the con-

    struction process of selfhood. Charlotte constructs and reconstructs her

    selfhood in the language that is available to her and in her interactions with

    others: psychologists, researchers, friends, parents. The voice that she speaks

    is therefore never a pure voice; it is always a voice that is shaped by theavailable discourses (Saukko, 2000).

    Cartography

    The principle of cartography implies that we can compare narrative self-

    hood with a dynamic map of narrations (and not with a tracing of reality),

    a map that is always open and always changing. The narrations someone

    tells about herself or himself are never complete; they form an ongoing

    process of co-construction and co-reconstruction. As a researcher, one can

    thus never have a view on the complete map of ones participant, seeing that

    this map is co-constructed, multiple, and constantly changing. We can only

    explore several temporal regions and paths knowing that we are taking part

    in the exploration.

    Looking back on my conversations with Charlotte and everything she

    told me about herself, I could conclude by saying that Charlotte and I

    made a trip together like two adventurous nomads: We passed through

    and settled temporarily in certain parts of the map, other parts we only

    caught a glimpse of, and still others remain unknown to us. After our joint

    trip, our nomadic trails dont die; they grow further according to other lines

    and connections.

    644 Qualitative Inquiry

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    15/20

    Rhizomatic Stories: Consequences for Presentation

    The way we look at self-stories automatically has consequences for theway we present these stories. The search for a possible presentation form is

    something that every narrative researcher is confronted with. One way or

    another, ultimately every narrative researcher must present the stories that

    she or he has co-created with the participant to the reading public. The form

    this takes varies depending on the researchers story vision. A traditional

    researcher will be inclined to put all the participants narratives in a tradi-

    tional story that is coherent around one plot with a beginning, middle, and

    end. She or he will present the story from a distance (the all seeing eye/I thatsees and speaks about the truth of the other) as if she or he herself or him-

    self is outside the situation being described, hiddenan unobtrusive

    camerareporting, even on self activities (Denzin, 1997, p. 224). This

    presentation form creates for the reader the illusion that the presented story

    forms a mirror of the true self/personality or life of the other, a mirror in

    which the researcher remains absent. But what about the rhizome thinker?

    How can she or he present the self stories and the way in which these stories

    are co-constructed without lapsing into realist/traditional story writing?A presentation of a rhizome on paper is impossible as such. How could

    one grasp a rhizome (and consequently selfhood as a rhizomatic story) on

    paper when one takes into account the principles of infinite entrances, multi-

    plicity, infinite connections, resistance against ruptures, and cartography? A

    rhizome is never tangible as it is infinite and always changing. The moment

    one tries to put rhizomatic selfhood into text or book form, one automatically

    goes against the most important principle, that is, the principle of multiple

    entryways. In contrast with everything someone co-constructs about herself

    or himself during her or his life, a paper text or book necessarily only has

    one or a limited number of entryways and exits. Besides, every paper text

    fixesless or morethat which is written in it; its mobility and openness

    are always limited.

    What does this mean for narrative researchers? First of all, this means

    that narrative researchers have to let go the idea that they can present the

    complete rhizomatic selfhood of their participants (this is impossible

    because a rhizome is always dynamic and never finished). Just as they can

    only follow one or a few possible paths in the rhizomatic story, they canonly present these paths, knowing that this is merely a needle in a haystack.

    When one views selfhood as a rhizomatic story, as a researcher one knows

    that one is not presenting the participants true self but merely one of the

    many possible context-bound, co-constructed presentations of the self. The

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 645

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    16/20

    temporary constellation between researcher and participant creates a time-

    limited possibility for the self to be understood and communicated, but what

    results is never a pure or a true self; it is a self that uses and is used by thebroader discourse context wherein it is embedded (Saukko, 2000). When

    we speak about presentation or presenting, one has to consider that

    presenting itself is a performance (Denzin, 1997), a new construction, a way

    of framing reality (Denzin, 1997, pp. 224-225) and not a pure represen-

    tation of an outside reality. In any act of writing, the discursive constitutive

    work is at play, the gaze is always present (Davies et al., 2004). This

    implies also that weas writerly researchersare always present in our

    texts. Just like in the speaking, in the writing we give birth to our selves andthe selves of others (also those of our participants!).

    The idea that one as a researcher cannot present the complete rhizomatic

    selfhood of ones participant stimulates the researcher to experiment with

    new forms of writing. She or he has to search for writing forms that do not

    create the illusion of direct representability (and the absence of the

    researcher therein) nor of the existence of a traditional self but rather evoke

    the rhizomatic thinking to the reader. To evoke the rhizomatic thinking to the

    reader entails that one as a writerly researcher tries to bring the rhizomaticthinking with all its principles as much as possible in the written text and

    that she or he herself or himself becomes a part of the writing project

    (Denzin, 1997, p. 224).

    For example, by explicitly pointing out to the reader that the text one

    presents (possibly together with the participant) is but one of the many pos-

    sible presentations (or entrances), one can avoid the illusory idea of the

    existence of a true core self that can be objectively captured into written

    words. In addition, one can also (although always to a limited extent) address

    the other rhizomatic principles by allowing the multitude, the nonlinear

    connections, the contradictions, the ruptures and new linkages (in sum, the

    monster!) that occur in the stories to exist as much as possible and also to

    explicitly present these on paper. One can do this by using poststructuralist

    writing techniques such as writing from different I voices, writing in

    columns, writing multiple storylines, introducing multiple entrances and

    exits, and so on. Also, the idea that the researcher forms a part of the con-

    struction and presentation work can be manifested in the text. The researcher

    is not an objective narrator who stands outside or above the written text,she or he is present in the writing. By visibly reflecting on her or his own

    positions in the writing, as a researcher she or he dismantles the illusion of

    direct representation and of the detached researcher with her or his all

    seeing eye/I.

    646 Qualitative Inquiry

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    17/20

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 647

    An example of such reflexive, rhizomatic writing we can recognize is

    what Denzin (1997, following Marcus, 1994) calls messy texts. Messy texts

    are reflexive texts that try to break with the representational technologiesthat are typical for the traditional, realist writing forms. They are reflexive

    because they are aware of their own narrative apparatuses, they are sensitive

    to how reality is socially constructed, and they understand that writing is a

    way of framingreality (Denzin, 1997, p. 224). A messy text announces its

    politics and ceaselessly interrogates the realities it invokes while folding the

    tellers story into the multivoiced history that is written (p. 225). Just like a

    rhizome, messy texts are many sited, intertextual, always open ended, and

    resistant to theoretical holism (p. 224). They refuse to impose meaning onthe reader (p. 224), they make readers work while resisting the temptation

    to think in terms of simplistic dichotomies; difference, not conflict is fore

    grounded (p. 225). In contrast to traditional texts in which the writer remains

    hidden as an unobtrusive camera, a messy text makes the writer a part of

    the writing project (p. 224). But as Denzin points out, messy texts are more

    than subjective accounts of experiences because they attempt to reflexively

    map the multiple discourses that occur in a given social space and hence they

    are always multivoiced (p. 225).

    A Temporary Conclusion . . .

    The fact that a rhizome as such cannot be presented does not mean,

    however, that we cannot extend rhizomatic thinking to thinking about presen-

    tation. As we have already emphasized in this article, as researchers we are

    automatically confronted with limitations of which we must be aware and

    which we must necessarily accept. We can neverknow the complete narra-

    tive selfhood of people, the complete map of the rhizome. And the same

    is true concerning presentation: We can nevermap the complete rhizomatic

    story from which a person derives her or his selfhood. We can onlypresent

    one of the many possible context-bound, co-constructed presentations of

    the self. All of this does not mean however that narrative research is less

    interesting. To use the words of Deleuze and Guattari once again: You can

    enter a rhizome wherever you like, no single entryway has the privilege.

    The only thing that changes depending on your choice of entryway is themap of the rhizome. Although researcher and participant will only travel a

    few parts, a few landscapes of the map, these landscapes can contain much

    valuable information.

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    18/20

    At last, rhizomatic thinking and writing seem to contribute to the problem

    of subjectivity in reflexive research and writing. As Davies et al. (2004)

    stated, by turning our gaze on the researching gaze from which we investi-gate a phenomenon, we risk incorporating ourselves into our research as the

    very real selves that transcendent the constitutive power of discourse. On the

    other hand, it is not acceptable to write as if the author were not present at

    each stage of the discursive constitutive work of research, when we reject the

    objective I/eye of positivist research. By considering selfhood as a rhizomatic

    story, researchers and participants are conceived as discursive processes,

    taking continuously their shapes in and through speaking and writing narra-

    tives about the narratives they have just told or written, always from thecontinuously changing perspective of narrating after the just told. In rhi-

    zomatic thinking and writing, a fixed or meta-linguistic subject is absent. The

    subjectwhether participant or researcheris continuously (re)born in the

    perspective of the narrating after the just narrated, always turning language

    back on itself in a horizontally moving way, that is characterized by multiple

    entryways, multiple connections and asignifying ruptures.

    Notes

    1. TheIspeaking here refers to the voice of Jasmina Sermijn, the first author. Charlotte is

    an 18-year-old girl who was diagnosed 2 years ago as anorexic. Since the age of 16, when she

    was admitted to a hospital, she has been regularly supervised by doctors and a psychologist.

    At the time of the interview, she was in her last year of secondary school and intended to go

    to the university the following year.

    2. This research addresses the way people who have received a medical or psychiatric diag-

    nosis in the course of their life construct their selfhood narratively. During an initial phase in

    this research, five people were questioned. With questioning, we refer to multiple, regular

    conversations in which the participants told about themselves, their lives in general, and theirexperiences with psychiatry. Most of these conversations were tape-recorded and then tran-

    scribed. Charlotte was one of the participants with whom we had multiple conversations. At

    her request, her name has been changed in this article.

    3. We refer here to the quilt metaphor used by Deleuze and Guattari (1987, pp. 474-500;

    also see Saukko, 2000). An embroidery quilt is a quilt that has a central motif (even if

    extremely complex) and that exists out of a continuous pattern that forms a whole.

    4. We also find this idea of the narrative self as a traditional story in traditional biographic

    research (for an overview, see Angrosino, 1989; Bertaux, 1981; Langness & Frank, 1981;

    Plummer, 1983), in which the life or the self of the research participant is presented as a com-

    plete, coherent story with a beginning, middle, and end.

    5. Term derived from Gibson, following Foucault and Derrida (see Herman & Vervaeck,

    2005, p. 114).

    6. With the metaphor of the patchwork quilt, Deleuze and Guattari (1987, pp. 474-500;

    also see Saukko, 2000) refer to a quilt as a never-ending work of juxtaposition of disjunctive

    elements. A patchwork quilt has no center, and the basic motif (the patch) is multifaced.

    648 Qualitative Inquiry

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    19/20

    Sermijn et al. / Narrative Construction of the Self 649

    References

    Angrosino, M. V. (1989). Documents of interaction: Biography, autobiography, and lifehistory in social science perspective. Gainesville: University of Florida Press.

    Bertaux, D. (1981).Biography and society. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Braidotti, R. (1994).Nomadic subjects. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Bruner, J. (1986).Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Bruner, J. (2002).Making stories. Law, literature, life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University

    Press.

    Butler, J. (1990). Gender trouble: Feminism and the subversion of identity. New York:

    Routledge.

    Carrithers, M., Collins, S., & Lukes, S. (Eds.). (1985). The category of the person. Cambridge,

    UK: Cambridge University Press.Connelly, F. M., & Clandinin, D. J. (1986). On narrative method, personal philosophy and

    narrative unities in the story of teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 23(5),

    283-310.

    Connelly, F. M., & Clandinin, D. J. (1990). Stories of experience and narrative inquiry.

    Educational Researcher, 19(5), 2-14.

    Currie, M. (1998). Postmodern narrative theory. New York: Macmillan.

    Davies, B., Browne, J., Gannon, S., Honan, E., Laws, C., Mueller-Rockstroh, B., et al. (2004).

    The ambivalent practices of reflexivity. Qualitative Inquiry, 10(3), 360-389.

    Davies, B., & Harr, R. (1990). Positioning: The discursive production of selves.Journal for

    the Theory of Social Behaviour, 20, 43-63.

    Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1976).Rhizme, introduction. Paris: Editions de Minuit.

    Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1987). A thousand plateaus: Capitalism and schizofrenia.

    Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Denzin, N. K. (1997).Interpretative ethnography: Ethnographic practices for the 21st century.

    Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Derrida, J. (1976). Of grammatology (G. C. Spivak, Trans.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins

    University Press.

    Foucault, M. (1975). Surveillir et punir. Naissance de la prison [Discipline and punish: The

    birth of the prison]. Paris: Gallimard.

    Foucault, M. (1976).Histoire de la sexualit. I. La volont de savoir[The history of sexuality.

    Vol. I: The will to knowledge]. Paris: Gallimard.

    Foucault, M. (1988). The political technology of individuals. In L. H. Martin, H. Gutman, &

    P. H. Hutton (Eds.), Technologies of the self. A seminar with Michel Foucault(pp. 145-163).

    London: Tavistock.

    Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York. Basic Books.

    Gergen, K. J. (1989). Warranting voice and the elaboration of the self. In J. Shotter & K. J. Gergen

    (Eds.), Texts of identity (pp. 70-81). London: Sage.

    Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self. Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life. New York:

    Basic Books.

    Gibson, A. (2004). Towards a postmodern theory of narrative. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh

    University Press.

    Herman, L., & Vervaeck, B. (2005). Vertelduivels. Handboek verhaalanalyse [Narrative devils.

    Handbook of narrative analysis]. Brussels, Belgium: VUBPress/Vantilt.

    Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980).Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    distribution. 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized

    at Universiteitsbibliotheek VUB on May 9, 2008http://qix.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/http://qix.sagepub.com/
  • 8/9/2019 Loots Narrative Self Article

    20/20

    Langness, L. L., & Frank, G. (1981). Lives. An anthropological approach to biography.

    Novato, CA: Chandler and Sharp.

    Linde, C. (1993).Life stories. The creation of coherence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Lyotard, J. F. (1979). La condition postmoderneRapport sur le savoir[The postmodern

    condition: A report on knowledge]. Paris: Editions de Minuit.

    Maan, A. K. (1999).Internarrative identity. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

    Marcus, G. E. (1994). What comes (just) after post? The case of ethnography. In N. K. Denzin &

    Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research (pp. 563-574). Thousand Oaks,

    CA: Sage.

    Plummer, K. (1983).Documents of life. An introduction to the problems and literature of a

    humanistic method. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Polkinghorne, D. E. (1995). Narrative configuration in qualitative analysis. In J. A. Hatch &

    R. Wisniewski (Eds.),Life history and narrative (pp. 5-24). London: Falmer.Ricoeur, P. (1983). Temps et rcit 1. Lintrigue et le rcit historique [Time and narrative Vol. I].

    Paris: Seuil.

    Ricoeur, P. (1985). Temps et rcit 3. Le temps racont[Time and narrative Vol. III]. Paris: Seuil.

    Ricoeur, P. (1990). Oospronkelijke uitgave: Soi-mme comme un autre [Original edition:

    Oneself as another]. Paris: Seuil.

    Rosseel, E. (2001).Het onschatbare subject. Aspecten van het postmoderne zelf[The invaluable

    subject. Aspects of the postmodern self]. Brussels, Belgium: VUBpress.

    Saukko, P. (2000). Between voice and discourse: Quilting interviews on anorexia. Qualitative

    Inquiry, 6, 299-317.

    Schneebaum, T. (1969). Keep the river on your right. New York: Grove.Schuh, K. L., & Cunningham, D. J. (2004). Rhizome and the mind: Describing the metaphor.

    Semiotica, 149(1-4), 325-342.

    Sermijn, J., Loots, G., & Devlieger, P. (2007). Who are we? From a modern Cartesian self to

    alternative views on selfhood. Qualitative Research Journal, 7(2), 36-51.

    Shorter, E. (1977). The making of the modern family. New York: Basic Books.

    Jasmina Sermijn is a systemic therapist and a doctoral student in the Faculty of Psychology

    and Educational Sciences at the Vrije Universiteit Brussel. Her research concerns explorative

    study of the interaction between psychiatric diagnoses and the construction of selfhood.

    Patrick Devlieger, PhD, is senior lecturer in the Department of Social and Cultural

    Anthropology of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven and visiting lecturer in the Department of

    Disability and Human Development of the University of Illinois at Chicago. His fields of

    interests are anthropology, disability studies, and ethnographic research.

    Gerrit Loots, PhD, is lecturer in the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the

    Vrije Universiteit Brussel and visiting lecturer at the Department of Special Needs Education

    of the Universiteit Gent. His fields of interests are psychotherapy and special needs education.

    650 Qualitative Inquiry