marisa l. kfrerer, nicholas g. martin and julie aitken schermer* a … · 2019. 7. 24. · the...

15
Marisa L. Kfrerer, Nicholas G. Martin and Julie Aitken Schermer* A behavior genetic analysis of the relationship between humor styles and depression https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2017-0098 Abstract: The present study examined the relationship between humor styles and depression using two methods of examination: (1) the mean humor style differ- ences between individuals who reported that they had been diagnosed with depression versus those who did not report being depressed; and (2) the pheno- typic, genetic, and environmental correlations between humor styles and a short scale assessing depressed affect created from preexisting measures in archival data. Participants were 1154 adult Australians, consisting of 339 monozygotic twin pairs and 238 dizygotic twin pairs. With respect to mean differences, depressed individuals were found to use self-defeating humor more and self-enhancing humor less than non-depressed adults. When the depressed affect scale score was analyzed, negative correlations were found with both affiliative and self- enhancing humor. A positive correlation was found between depressed affect and both aggressive and self-defeating humor. These phenotypic correlations were also found to have some significant genetic and environmental correlations. Keywords: humor styles, depression, adult, behavior genetics 1 Introduction The most recent diagnostic definition of depressive disorders from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5th Edition (DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association 2013), defines Depressive Disordersas the feelings of sadness, emptiness or irritableness in addition to other cognitive or physiological symptoms that interfere with ones ability to function. Depression impedes both personal and social relationships, and is associated with an increased sensitivity to criticism (Barrett and Barber 2007; Dinger et al. 2015; Natoli et al. 2016). As humor styles play an important role in how an individual *Corresponding author: Julie Aitken Schermer, Management and Organizational Studies, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada, E-mail: [email protected] Marisa L. Kfrerer, Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada E-mail: [email protected] Nicholas G. Martin, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Herston, Queensland, Australia E-mail: [email protected] Humor 2019; 32(3): 417431 Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ Library Authenticated Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

Upload: others

Post on 08-Feb-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Marisa L. Kfrerer, Nicholas G. Martin and Julie Aitken Schermer*

    A behavior genetic analysis of the relationshipbetween humor styles and depression

    https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2017-0098

    Abstract: The present study examined the relationship between humor styles anddepression using two methods of examination: (1) the mean humor style differ-ences between individuals who reported that they had been diagnosed withdepression versus those who did not report being depressed; and (2) the pheno-typic, genetic, and environmental correlations between humor styles and a shortscale assessing depressed affect created from preexisting measures in archivaldata. Participants were 1154 adult Australians, consisting of 339 monozygotic twinpairs and 238 dizygotic twin pairs. With respect to mean differences, depressedindividuals were found to use self-defeating humor more and self-enhancinghumor less than non-depressed adults. When the depressed affect scale scorewas analyzed, negative correlations were found with both affiliative and self-enhancing humor. A positive correlation was found between depressed affectand both aggressive and self-defeating humor. These phenotypic correlationswere also found to have some significant genetic and environmental correlations.

    Keywords: humor styles, depression, adult, behavior genetics

    1 Introduction

    The most recent diagnostic definition of depressive disorders from theDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5th Edition (DSM-V;American Psychiatric Association 2013), defines “Depressive Disorders” as thefeelings of sadness, emptiness or irritableness in addition to other cognitive orphysiological symptoms that interfere with one’s ability to function. Depressionimpedes both personal and social relationships, and is associated with anincreased sensitivity to criticism (Barrett and Barber 2007; Dinger et al. 2015;Natoli et al. 2016). As humor styles play an important role in how an individual

    *Corresponding author: Julie Aitken Schermer, Management and Organizational Studies, TheUniversity of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada, E-mail: [email protected] L. Kfrerer, Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Western Ontario,London, Ontario, Canada E-mail: [email protected] G. Martin, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Herston, Queensland, AustraliaE-mail: [email protected]

    Humor 2019; 32(3): 417–431

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • feels about their self and their relationships with others, the present studyfurthers the understanding of depression and depressed affect by examiningthe relations with humor styles.

    1.1 Humor and mental health

    Positive psychology research has demonstrated that humor has powerful effectson both physical and mental health (Cann et al. 2010; Chen and Martin 2007).For example, humor has been shown to benefit those suffering from cancer,mental health disorders, and even the dying (Richman 2006; Rose et al. 2013).Martin et al. (2003) proposed humor to have both positive and negative func-tions. Specifically, the four styles of humor in Martin et al.’s (2003) theoryinclude two positive styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) and two negativehumor styles (aggressive and self-defeating).

    Positive styles of humor have been shown to benefit mental health, specifi-cally in feelings of happiness, good overall health, and social self-esteem (Kuiperand McHale 2009; Yue et al. 2014). Affiliative humor includes teasing, practicaljoking, and playful humor and is used to amuse others, relieve tension, and toform positive interpersonal relationships. Self-enhancing humor includes copingwith mishaps and stressors in life with a positive and humorous perspective,similar to Freud’s view of a healthy defense mechanism (Martin et al. 2003).

    Negative styles of humor have been shown to be benign or even detrimentalto one’s wellbeing and mental health and have been found to be correlated withneuroticism, social anxiety, depressive symptoms, and suicide ideation(Schermer et al. 2013; Tucker et al. 2013a, Tucker et al. 2013b). Because negativehumor styles can make it difficult to connect with others, and often demon-strates inadequate self-esteem, negative humor styles have also been linked to aspecific vulnerability to depression (Frewen et al. 2008). Aggressive humorincludes hostile and rude jokes and is used to threaten or manipulate othersby putting them down. Self-defeating, or self-deprecating, humor includes put-ting one’s self down for the sake of the joke. Therefore self-defeating humor isused to gain approval from others or to avoid one’s own negative thoughts andfeelings (Martin et al. 2003). This suggests that exhibiting humor styles that areperceived as disparaging or rude to someone else (aggressive), as well asavoidant or self-deprecating (self-defeating), have negative consequences onthe self. Not only is it seen as hurtful towards others, but negative humor stylesmay also hinder the social connection someone is trying to achieve with humor.Therefore, it is valuable to understand the personal and social consequencesassociated with certain styles of humor.

    418 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • 2 Heritability of depression and humor

    Although no specific gene has been identified to cause depression, twin andadoption studies have demonstrated that the risk of depression among first-degree relatives is high (Jang et al. 2004; Lohoff 2010) and that the heritabilityestimates of depression are equal for men and women (Kendler and Prescott1999). Sullivan et al. (2000) performed a meta-analysis that looked at adultmajor depression disorder in families and concluded that approximately 40%of the likelihood of depression was due to genetic factors (see also review byLohoff 2010). Jang et al. (2004) examined the heritability estimates for specificsymptoms of depression and depressed affect and found variability in thegenetic estimates. For example, some physiological symptoms of depression(such as loss of appetite and libido) as well as depressed affect includingfeelings of guilt and hopelessness were heritable with estimates ranging from18% for positive affect and suicidal thoughts to 35% for insomnia across 14depression factors. In contrast, depression dimensions such as phobias, tearful-ness, and nausea and headaches were not found to have a significant herit-ability estimate. Wichers et al. (2007) also found that twins with a geneticvulnerability to depression were biased toward negative affect when exposedto major life stressors. These findings suggest that some of the components ofdepression have a genetic basis.

    Humor is an essential component of interaction with a strong evolutionaryhistory (Polimeni and Reiss 2006) and humor styles have been found to beheritable with heritability estimates ranging from 5% for self-defeating humorto 47% for both the aggressive and self-defeating humor styles (Baughman et al.2012; Schermer et al. 2017; Vernon et al. 2008). Because both humor styles anddepression have been found to have a genetic component, the present studyexamines the genetic and environmental correlations between depression andhumor styles to assess if the observed phenotypic correlations have any possiblecommon genetic and/or environmental factors.

    3 Depression and humor predictions

    Recent literature suggests that self-directed styles of humor (including self-enhancing and self-defeating) are the most relevant in terms of coping withand buffering stressors in life (Cann et al. 2010; Oktug 2017). As self-directedhumor styles are defined by intrapersonal thinking and evaluation, whichinvolves the reflection of one’s self and the attempt to connect with others, it

    A behavior genetic analysis 419

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • may not be surprising that self-directed humor styles have been found to becorrelated with borderline personality disorder, loneliness, perceptions of socialsupport, and spitefulness (Schermer et al. 2015, Schermer et al. 2017; Vrabel et al.2017; Zhao et al. 2014). These findings suggest that both self-enhancing and self-defeating humor styles are crucial when looking at factors that relate to psycho-logical well-being and mood disorders such as depression. The self-defeatinghumor style, which is associated with negative self-evaluative standards, lowersocial self-esteem, greater self-report loneliness, and overall lower psychologicalwellbeing, may be one of the strongest correlates with depression (Kuiper andMcHale 2009; Schermer et al. 2017).

    The present study explores the relationships between humor styles anddepression in an adult population from an archival data set. Mean differencesin humor styles were tested to better understand the possible differences in theuse of humor in depressed versus non-depressed individuals. The study alsoexamines the correlations between a depressed affect measure (created from thescales completed by the participants) and humor styles at the phenotypic leveland the bivariate genetic level. It was hypothesized that participants whoidentified as being diagnosed with depression would exhibit higher levels ofself-defeating humor than non-depressed participants. It was also hypothesizedthat individuals who have reported to have been diagnosed with depressionwould exhibit lower levels of both positive humor styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) than the non-depressed group. Finally, it was hypothesized thatpositive correlations may be found between negative humor styles (self-defeat-ing and aggressive) with depressed affect; that negative correlations will befound between positive humor styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) anddepressed affect; and that these phenotypic correlations may themselves havesignificant genetic and/or environmental correlations.

    4 Method

    4.1 Participants

    Participants were 1154 adult Australians, consisting of 339 monozygotic (MZ)twin pairs (236 MZ female and 103 MZ male) and 236 same-sex dizygotic (DZ)twin pairs (177 DZ female and 61 DZ male), who partook in The Twin and FamilyStudy at The Queensland Institute of Medical Research. The average age of thesample was 34.76 years (SD = 2.53, range = 30 to 45).

    420 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • 4.1.1 Measures and procedure

    Participants completed a set of questionnaires including demographic informa-tion (age, sex, marital status, general health, and family history) as well as thescale and items described below (see Wright and Martin 2004). Randomlyselecting one twin from each pair, 145 individuals were found to have statedthat they were diagnosed with depression after age of 14. Responses to this itemwere used to distinguish the depressed versus non-depressed groups.

    4.2 Humor styles questionnaire (HSQ; Martin et al. 2003)

    The 32-item Humor Styles Questionnaire (HSQ; Martin et al. 2003) measuresfour constructs of humor, including: two positive dimensions, affiliative humor(engaging in humor to relieve tension, to form relationships, to be amusing),and self-enhancing humor (the use of humor to cope or regulate one’s negativeemotions); and two negative dimensions, aggressive humor (sarcastic or dis-paraging humor used without regard for other’s feelings) and self-defeatinghumor (putting one’s own self down to be humorous, gain approval, or defendone’s self). The humor style model is based on the assumption that everyperson uses humor in ways that are indicative of their personality traits.Affiliative humor, which may be used to enhance relationships with others(example item, “I enjoy making people laugh”), and self-enhancing humor,which may be used to alleviate one’s own stress (example item, “If I am feelingdepressed, I can usually cheer myself up with humor”) have both been linkedto positive personality traits such as openness, agreeableness, as well aspsychological wellbeing and social relatedness (Martin et al. 2003). In contrast,the aggressive humor style (example item, “If I don’t like someone, I often usehumor or teasing to put them down”) and the self-defeating humor style(example item, “I will often get carried away in putting myself down if itmakes my family or friends laugh”) have both been linked to constructsincluding hostility, and negative personality traits such as neuroticism(Martin et al. 2003). The HSQ demonstrates construct validity, correlatingwith other personality measures, as well as other humor scales, (Martin et al.2003). Items in the HSQ are responded to using a 7-point Likert scale, whichmeasures the extent to which participants disagree (1) or agree (7) with eachitem. Each of the four subscales consists of eight items. Martin et al. (2003) hasshown that the four subscales of the HSQ are reliable, and in the presentsample, the internal consistency values were 0.86 for affiliative, 0.82 for self-enhancing, 0.71 for aggressive, and 0.83 for self-defeating.

    A behavior genetic analysis 421

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • 4.3 Selected depression items

    Although the sample did not complete a depression scale, to better under-stand the relationship between depressed affect and humor styles, three itemswere selected from the larger battery of questionnaires given to participantsas they pertain to depression and the DSM-5 definition of DepressiveDisorders, specifically, “the feelings of sadness, emptiness or irritablenessin addition to other cognitive or physiological symptoms that interfere withone’s ability to function” (American Psychiatric Association 2013) to create acontinuous measure. The diagnostic features as outlined by the DSM-5updated criterion guided the selection of these three items. The first itemwas, “Sometimes I feel terribly empty inside” taken from the PAI-BOR:Personality Assessment Inventory- Borderline Features Questionnaire (Morey1991). This item addresses what is labelled as criterion A1 of the DSM-5 MajorDepressive Disorder, in which the individual feels “sad, empty, hopeless”(American Psychiatric Association 2013). By subjective report, one may feelemptiness and tearfulness in the presence of a depressive disorder, in aperiod longer than two weeks, including loss of interest and depressedmood most of every day. The second item was, “Sometimes I feel completelyworthless” from the NEO-FFI: Five Factor Inventory Questionnaire (McCraeand Costa 2004). Unrealistic negative cognitions in depressive disorders mayresult in disproportionate feelings of worthlessness and guilt (Criterion A7 forMajor Depressive Disorder; American Psychiatric Association 2013). This isdue to the individual’s hindered ability to correctly interpret day-to-dayevents in a neutral manner, leading in some cases to a strong sense ofblame, personal defect, and worthlessness. The final item was, “I am seldomsad or depressed” from the NEO-FFI: Five Factor Inventory Questionnaire,(McCrae and Costa 2004). This item, when negatively keyed, pertains tocriterion A1 as well, in which the individual experiences feelings of sadness,a “down in the dumps” outlook on life (American Psychiatric Association2013). Due to the secondary data nature of the present study, these threeitems were used to generate a depressed affect scale to better justify depres-sive disorders beyond the single item “Diagnosed with Depression” in thedemographic information, in which participants answered yes or no. Thecontent of these items may reflect depression as a trait because, as isrecognized by the American Psychiatric Association (2013), depression canreflect periods of negative affect and not constant negative states, suggestingthat terms such as “sometimes” may be tapping into the negative emotions.

    422 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • 5 Results

    5.1 Sex differences and correlations with age for the humorstyles

    Table 1 lists the sex differences for the humor style scale scores. Specifically,males tend to use aggressive, affiliative, and self-defeating humor more thanfemales. Age did not have robust correlations with humor styles with thecorrelations ranging from 0.03 with self-enhancing to −0.08 with affiliative.

    5.2 Sex and age analyses for depression groups

    Chi-square analyses showed a significant association between sex anddepression status (X2 = 67.68, p < 0.001). Consistent with previous literature(Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus 1994), females were nearly two and a half timesmore likely to be depressed than males, odds ratio = 2.391. Age wasnot significantly different for the depressed versus non depressed groups(t = 0.04, p < 0.60).

    5.3 Humor style differences by depression

    Table 2 provides the means for the humor styles for depressed versus non-depressed individuals. The pattern confirms the hypothesis that individualsdiagnosed with depression exhibit higher levels of the self-defeating (defensive)humor style. In addition, adults diagnosed with depression tend to use positivestyles of humor less than non-depressed adults, including both affiliative and

    Table 1: Humor style differences for men and women.

    Humor Style Men (N= ) Women (N = ) F tMean SD Mean SD

    Affiliative . . . . .* .*Self-enhancing . . . . .* .Aggressive . . . . .* .*Self-defeating . . . . . .*

    *p < 0.001, two-tailed

    A behavior genetic analysis 423

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • self-enhancing humor. This difference was most apparent for the self-enhancinghumor style. The depressed group was also found to have greater variability(based on F-tests) in the affiliative, self-enhancing, and self-defeating scalesthan the non-depressed group. The mean difference in aggressive humor bydepression status was not significant.

    5.4 Depression affect scale

    Cronbach’s alpha for the three items chosen to measure depressive affect was0.53 suggesting that the scale was moderately reliable with respect to internalconsistency for such a short scale and because of the fact that the items werefrom different scales and were responded to with different response options (theNEO-FFI utilizes a 5-point scale and the PAI-BOR has a 3-point scale). The meaninter-item correlation was 0.30 and Cronbach’s alpha did not increase after theremoval of any item. A scale score was created by aggregating across the threeitems. Men (M = 6.19, SD = 2.01) were not found to differ significantly fromwomen (M = 6.24, SD = 2.01; F = 0.41, t=−0.75) on the depressive affect scalescore. A small negative correlation was found between age and the depressiveaffect scale (r = −0.10, p < 0.001).

    The depressive affect scale scores were then compared for the depressed(M = 7.58, SD = 2.13) versus not depressed (M = 5.83, SD = 1.79) groups.A significant difference was found for both the test of variances (F = 30.48,p < 0.001) in that there was greater scale variance in the depressed group, andfor the test of means (t = 18.58, p < 0.001) with higher depressive affect scalescores in the depressed group. These findings suggest that the depressive affectscale score provides an additional measure of depression for analyses with thehumor styles.

    Table 2: Humor style differences for depressed versus non-depressed participants.

    Humor Style Depressed (N = ) Not depressed (N = ) F tMean SD Mean SD

    Affiliative . . . . .* −.Self-enhancing . . . . .* −.*Aggressive . . . . . −.Self-defeating . . . . .* .*

    *p < 0.001, two-tailed

    424 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • 6 Behavior genetic analyses

    For the depressed affect scores, the MZ intra-class correlation of 0.25 wasfound to be higher than the DZ correlation of 0.16. Following standard uni-variate genetic analyses using the software Mx (Neale et al. 2006), the additivegenetic (a2), common environment (c2), and unique environment (e2) estimateswere estimated. In particular, the full ACE model was tested against thereduced AE, CE, and E-only models and fit was assessed. The AE model wasfound to be the best fitting model based on the lowest chi-square per degree offreedom and most negative AIC value. The additive genetic estimate was foundto be 0.26 (95% confidence interval = 0.17 to 0.35) and the unique environ-mental estimate was found to be 0.74 (95% confidence interval = 0.65 to 0.83).The results of the univariate genetic analyses for the HSQ for this sample wererecently published (see Schermer et al. 2017), with additive genetic effectsaveraging 0.32 and unique environmental effects averaging 0.58 across thefour humor styles.

    Table 3 reports the phenotypic correlations (rp) between the depressed affectscale and the humor style scales. Significant negative correlations were foundbetween the depressive affect scale score and the two socially positive humorstyles of affiliation and self-enhancing. In contrast, the depressive affect scalehad positive correlations with the socially negative humor styles of aggressiveand self-defeating. Bivariate genetic analyses were then calculated to furtherexamine the covariance between the humor scales and the depressed affect scalescores. Cholesky or triangular decomposition (see Neale and Cardon 1992) wasapplied to the MZ and DZ mean square between- and within-pair covariancematrices to calculate genetic and environmental correlations using the programMx (Neale et al. 2006). Similar to the univariate results, the AE model was found

    Table 3: Phenotypic, genetic, and environmental correlations between depressed affect scoresand humor styles.

    Humor Style Phenotypic Genetic Unique Environmental

    Affiliative −.* −. (−. to .) −. (−. to −.)Self-enhancing −.* −. (−. to −.) −. (−. to −.)Aggressive .* . (−. to .) . (. to .)Self-defeating .* . (. to .) . (. to .)

    *p < 0.001; two-tailed; 95% confidence intervals are in the brackets; those which do notcontain zero are deemed to be significant and are in bold text. All cross-correlations were bestfit by an AE model.

    A behavior genetic analysis 425

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • to have the best fit for each of the bivariate genetic models, indicating that thephenotypic correlations were best explained by common genetic and uniqueenvironmental factors.

    The results of the bivariate genetic analyses are reported in Table 3.Correlations with a 95% confidence interval (values within the brackets) thatdo not include zero are considered to be statistically significant. Because theresults were found to be very similar (or the same) when the uncorrected and thecorrected (age and sex regressed) data were analyzed, the uncorrected resultsare presented in the present study. As reported in Table 3, the self-enhancingand self-defeating genetic correlations were significant, suggesting that theremay be a common genetic factor influencing the phenotypic correlations. All ofthe unique environmental correlations were significant, suggesting that thephenotypic correlations are also due to overlapping unique environmentalinfluences which includes measurement error as well as individualized experi-ences within the twin pair. For example, unique environmental influences reflectdifferent classroom environments and different peer experiences for each twin(Vernon et al. 1997).

    7 Discussion

    The present study furthers our understanding of how humor relates to depres-sion. Humor styles that are detrimental to psychological well-being, in particularself-defeating humor, were associated with depression and higher scores on thedepressed affect scale. This finding is similar to previous research which hasfound that self-defeating humor is associated with loneliness (Schermer et al.2017), difficulty in both identifying and describing feelings and general alex-ithymia (Atkinson et al. 2015), and borderline personality characteristics includ-ing: affect instability, identity disturbance, negative relationships, and self-harm(Schermer et al. 2015).

    Individuals diagnosed with depression also tend to not use positive styles ofhumor as often as the non-depressed individuals. Therefore, in accordance withprevious research, the pattern of high self-defeating humor and low social andself-enhancing humor may result in a vulnerability to depression (Frewen et al.2008). These findings reflect the results by Stockton et al. (2016) who reportedthat self-enhancing and affiliative humor are positively correlated with reasonsfor living and gratitude.

    The use of aggressive humor was not significantly different betweendepressed and non-depressed individuals, likely because this type of humor

    426 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • has more to do with hurting another individual rather than hurting one’s self(Martin et al. 2003). If someone uses aggressive humor, it could also imply thatthey are not employing the self-awareness needed to control the desire toridicule or tease. Aggressive humor has been positively linked to neuroticismand negatively correlated with conscientiousness (Schermer et al. 2013).Therefore, aggressive humor may have less to do with depression and one’sintrapersonal interactions, and more to do with general aggression and a lack ofawareness towards others.

    Age was found to not correlate significantly with humor styles, consistentwith previous findings (Martin et al. 2003). This result suggests that there wereno generational differences for humor styles in the present sample which maynot be surprising as the age range was quite narrow. Age also did not have asignificant relationship with depression. Females were nearly two and a halftimes more likely to be depressed, consistent with the literature showing thatwomen are more vulnerable to depressive symptoms, as well as depression ingeneral (Frewen et al. 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus 1994).

    The short depressive affect scale, which was generated from three itemstaken from other measures, was found to have a genetic component with aheritability estimate of 25% which is similar to the values reported by Jang et al.(2004). Because humor styles have also been found to be heritable, the observedphenotypic correlations were examined for possible genetic and/or environmen-tal correlations. All of the correlations between the humor styles and depressedaffect had significant unique environmental correlations. Of great interest werethe moderately strong genetic correlations between depressed affect and self-defeating humor (in the positive direction) and self-enhancing humor (in thenegative direction). These results may not be too surprising as self-enhancinghumor has been shown to correlate with positive affect and self-defeating humorhas been shown to strengthen the relationship between life stress and depres-sion, specifically strengthening the link between social anxiety and depressionoutcomes (Tucker et al. 2013a, Tucker et al. 2013b). Self-defeating humorinvolves self-disparaging acts which are used to gain approval from others andto avoid the underlying negative thoughts and feelings an individual may haveabout their self (Rnic et al. 2016) which may result in more feelings of emptinessand depression. Self-defeating humor may, therefore, have its roots in depres-sion itself, in that depressed individuals may use humor in an attempt toalleviate feelings of emptiness and sadness. Coyne’s (1976) interpersonal theoryof depression supports this hypothesis, suggesting that depressed individualsseek reassurance from others, but then doubt this reassurance afterward.Therefore, the use of self-defeating humor may actually hinder the depressedindividual’s ability to overcome their self-doubt and sadness. Self-defeating

    A behavior genetic analysis 427

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • humor is therefore an important construct for future research investigating mooddisorders as well as aspects of mood disorders such as distorted cognitions,psychological well-being, and treatment.

    8 Limitations and future research

    In this study, participants self-reported to be diagnosed with depression,therefore, there is a possibility that some participants were diagnosed withdepression, but did not report that fact. Ideally independent verification ofdepression diagnoses should be obtained in future studies. Also, the partici-pants in this study were diagnosed with depression after the age of 14. Due tothe cross-sectional nature of this data, it is unknown whether these partici-pants had stable humor styles before the diagnosis of depression. The presentstudy is also limited in that a recognized and more reliable depression scalewas not utilized but, because of the archival nature of the data, a short scalewas created from three items from different measures and used in the analyses.Future research may also want to investigate different types of depressivedisorders in order to clarify the relationship between humor styles anddepression.

    9 Conclusions

    As predicted, the self-directed humor styles, which include the socially positiveself-enhancing and the socially negative self-defeating, had the strongest rela-tionships with depression both with respect to mean differences and correla-tional strength. The correlations between the self-directed humor styles and thedepressed affect were also found to have significant genetic correlations sug-gesting that the phenotypic correlation is influenced by common genetic factors.

    Acknowledgements: Thank you to Rod A. Martin and Philip A. Vernon(Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Science, The University ofWestern Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada), Lucia Colodro Conde (QIMRBerghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia), DixieStatham (University of the Sunshine Coast, Queensland, Australia), and MichaelT. Lynskey (Addictions Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology andNeuroscience, King’s College) for their contributions to the collection of the data.

    428 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • References

    American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders,fifth edition: Depressive disorders. Retrieved from: http://dsm.psychiatryonline.org/doi/full/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm04.

    Atkinson, B. E., D. Lipton, H. M. Baughman, J. A. Schermer, J. Harris & P. A. Vernon. 2015. Howdo emotional restrictions affect the use of humor? A behavior genetic analysis of alex-ithymia and humor styles. Twin Research and Human Genetics 18(2). 138–141.

    Barrett, M. S. & J. P. Barber. 2007. Interpersonal profiles in major depressive disorder. Journalof Clinical Psychology 63. 247–266. doi:10.1002/jclp.20346.

    Baughman, H. M., E. A. Giammarco, L. Veselka, J. A. Schermer, N. G. Martin, M. Lynskey &P. A. Vernon. 2012. A behavioral genetic study of humor styles in an Australian sample.Twin Research and Human Genetics 15(5). 663–667.

    Cann, A., K. Stilwell & K. Taku. 2010. Humor styles, positive personality and health. Europe’sJournal of Psychology 6(3). 213–235. doi:10.5964/ejop.v6i3.214.

    Chen, G. H. & R. A. Martin. 2007. A comparison of humor styles, coping humor, and mentalhealth between Chinese and Canadian university students. Humor–International Journal ofHumor Research 20(3). 215–234.

    Coyne, J. C. 1976. Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry 39. 28–40.doi:10.1080/00332747.1976.11023874.

    Dinger, U., M. S. Barrett, J. Zimmermann, H. Schauenburg, A. G. C. Wright, F. Renner &J. P. Barber. 2015. Interpersonal problems, dependency, and self-criticism in majordepressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology 71. 93–104.

    Frewen, P. A., J. Brinker, R. A. Martin & D. J. A. Dozois. 2008. Humor styles and personality-vulnerability to depression. International Journal of Humor Research 21(2). 179–195.doi:10.1515/HUMOR.2008.009.

    Jang, K. L., W. J. Livesley, S. Taylor, M. B. Stein & E. C. Moon. 2004. Heritability of individualdepressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders 80. 125–133. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00108-3.

    Kendler, K. S. & C. A. Prescott. 1999. A population-based twin study of lifetime major depres-sion in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry 56(1). 39–44.

    Kuiper, N. A. & N. McHale. 2009. Humor styles as mediators between self-evaluative standardsand psychological well-being. The Journal of Psychology 143(4). 359–376.

    Lohoff, F. W. 2010. Overview of the genetics of major depressive disorder. Current PsychiatryReports 12(6). 539–546. doi:10.1007/s11920-010-0150-6.

    Martin, R. A., P. Puhlik-Doris, G. Larsen, J. Gray & K. Weir. 2003. Individual differences in usesof humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the humor stylesquestionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality 37(1). 48–75.

    McCrae, R. R. & P. T. Costa. 2004. A contemplated revision of the NEO five-factor inventory.Personality and Individual Differences 36(3). 587–596.

    Morey, L. C. 1991. Personality assessment inventory. Odessa, FL: Routledge.Natoli, A. P., S. M. Nelson, K. J. Lengu & S. K. Huprich. 2016. Sensitivity to criticism differen-

    tially mediates the relationship between interpersonal problems and state and traitdepression. Personality and Mental Health 10(4). 293–304.

    Neale, M. C., S. M. Boker, G. Xie & H. H. Maes. 2006. Mx: Statistical modeling manual, 7th edn.Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia.

    A behavior genetic analysis 429

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • Neale, M. C. & L. R. Cardon. 1992. Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families.Norwell, MA: Kluwer.

    Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & J. S. Girgus. 1994. The emergence of gender differences in depressionduring adolescence. Psychological Bulletin 115(3). 424. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424.

    Oktug, Z. 2017. The moderating role of employee’s humor styles on the relationship betweenjob stress and emotional exhaustion. Journal of International Business Research 10(4). 131.doi:10.5539/ibr.v10n4p131.

    Polimeni, J. & J. P. Reiss. 2006. The first joke: Exploring the evolutionary origins of humor.Evolutionary Psychology 4(1). 347–366. doi:10.1177/147470490600400129.

    Richman, J. 2006. The role of psychotherapy and humor for death anxiety, death wishes, andaging. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying 54(1). 41–51. doi:10.2190/D0NX-7V03-W1H0-4614.

    Rnic, K., D. J. Dozois & R. A. Martin. 2016. Cognitive distortions, humor styles, and depression.Europe’s Journal of Psychology 12(3). 348. doi:10.5964/ejop.v12i3.1118.

    Rose, S. L., R. J. Spencer & M. M. Rausch. 2013. The use of humor in patients with recurrentovarian cancer: A phenomenological study. International Journal of Gynecological Cancer:Official Journal of the International Gynecological Cancer Society 23(4). 775–779.doi:10.1097/IGC.0b013e31828addd5.

    Schermer, J. A., R. A. Martin, N. G. Martin, M. Lynskey & P. A. Vernon. 2013. The general factorof personality and humor styles. Personality and Individual Differences 54(8). 890–893.

    Schermer, J. A., R. A. Martin, N. G. Martin, M. T. Lynskey, T. J. Trull & P. A. Vernon. 2015. Humorstyles and borderline personality. Personality and Individual Differences 87. 158–161.

    Schermer, J. A., R. A. Martin, P. A. Vernon, N. G. Martin, L. C. Conde, D. Statham &M. T. Lynskey. 2017. Lonely people tend to make fun of themselves: A behavior geneticanalysis of humor styles and loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences 117. 71–73.

    Stockton, J. G., R. P. Tucker, E. M. Kleiman & L. R. Wingate. 2016. How does gratitude affect therelationship between positive humor styles and suicide-related outcomes? Personality andIndividual Differences 102. 240–244. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.016.

    Sullivan, P. F., M. C. Neale & K. S. Kendler. 2000. Genetic epidemiology of major depression:Review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 157. 1552–1562. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552.

    Tucker, R. P., M. R. Judah, V. M. O’Keefe, A. C. Mills, W. V. Lechner, C. L. Davidson &L. R. Wingate. 2013a. Humor styles impact the relationship between symptoms of socialanxiety and depression. Personality and Individual Differences 55(7). 823–827.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.008.

    Tucker, R. P., L. R. Wingate, V. M. O’Keefe, M. L. Slish, M. R. Judah & S. Rhoades-Kerswill.2013b. The moderating effect of humor style on the relationship between interpersonalpredictors of suicide and suicidal ideation. Personality and Individual Differences 54.610–615.

    Vernon, P. A., K. J. Jang, J. A. Harris & J. M. McCarthy. 1997. Environmental predictors ofpersonality differences: A twin and sibling study. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology 72(1). 177–183.

    Vernon, P. A., R. A. Martin, J. A. Schermer & A. Mackie. 2008. A behavioral genetic investigationof humor styles and their correlations with the Big-5 personality dimensions. Personalityand Individual Differences 44. 1116–1125.

    Vrabel, J. K., V. Zeigler-Hill & R. G. Shango. 2017. Spitefulness and humor styles. Personalityand Individual Differences 105. 238–243. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.001.

    430 Marisa L. Kfrerer et al.

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM

  • Wichers, M., I. Myin-Germeys , N. Jacobs, F. Peeters, G. Kenis, C. Derom, R. Vlietinck , P. Delespaul& J. Van Os. 2007. Genetic risk of depression and stress-induced negative affect in daily life.The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191(3). 218-223. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.032201

    Wright, M. J. & N. G. Martin. 2004. Brisbane adolescent twin study: Outline of study methodsand research projects. Australian Journal of Psychology 56(2). 65–78.

    Yue, X. D., K. W. Liu, F. Jiang & N. A. Hiranandani. 2014. Humor styles, self-esteem, andsubjective happiness. Psychological Reports 115(2). 517–525. doi:10.2466/07.02.PR0.115c18z6.

    Zhao, J., Y. Wang & F. Kong. 2014. Exploring the mediation effect of social support and self-esteem on the relationship between humor style and life satisfaction in Chinese collegestudents. Personality and Individual Differences 64. 126–130.

    Bionotes

    Marisa L. Kfrerer

    Marisa Kfrerer, MSc. is currently a research assistant at The University of Western Ontario. Herresearch interests include humor, depression, and other individual differences.

    Nicholas G. Martin

    Nicholas G. Martin, Ph.D. is a Senior Scientist in Genetic Epidemiology at QIMR BerghoferMedical Research Institute, Queensland, Australia.

    Julie Aitken Schermer

    Julie Aitken Schermer, Ph.D. (personality psychology) is a professor in the Management andOrganizational Studies Department at The University of Western Ontario, London, Canada.

    A behavior genetic analysis 431

    Brought to you by | University of Queensland - UQ LibraryAuthenticated

    Download Date | 7/24/19 3:14 AM