mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

13
Anim .Behav ., 1992,43,907-919 Matingstrategiesintwospeciesofdart-poisonfrogs :acomparativestudy KYLESUMMERS* MuseumofZoology,UniversityofMichigan,AnnArbor,MI48109, U.S.A . (Received7February1991 ;initialacceptance13March1991 ; finalacceptance9October1991 ;MS .number:45969) Abstract. Comparativefieldstudiesofspeciesofdart-poisonfrogsinthegenus Dendrobates werecarried outtotestpredictionsfromtwohypothesesthatattempttoexplainfemale-femalecompetitionformates inspeciesof Dendrobates withmaleparentalcare .Thesexrolereversalhypothesisproposesthatmales investsomuchtimeandenergyinparentalcarethatreceptivemalesarerarerelativetoreceptivefemales, andfemalescompetetofindandmatewithreceptivemales .Theparentalqualityhypothesisproposesthat femalescompetetomonopolizetheparentaleffortofparticularmales,becausetheypotentiallysuffera costwhentheirmatescarefortheoffspringofotherfemales .Comparisonsbetweenspecieswithmale parentalcare (Dendrobatesleucomelas) andfemaleparentalcare (Dendrobateshistrionicus) contradicted predictionofthesexrolereversalhypothesis,butwereconsistentwithpredictionsoftheparentalquality hypothesis .Male D .histrionicus didnotcompeteformatesmoreaggressivelythanmale D .leucomelas, and male D .leucomelas werenotmoreselectiveaboutmatingthanmale D .histrionicus . Female D .leucomelas and D .histrionicus werebothselectiveaboutmating ;female D .leucomelas associatedwithandcompeted forparticularmales,whereasfemale D .histrionicus didnot . Dart-poisonfrogsofthegenus Dendrobates (Den- drobatidae)arefoundintropicalforestsofSouth andCentralAmerica(Silverstone1975) .Thesefrogs areterrestrialanddiurnal ;allknownspeciesare aposematicallycolouredandproducehighlytoxic alkaloidsinskinglands(Myers&Daly1983) .All speciesstudiedsofardisplayparentalcareinwhich oneparentcarriesthetadpolesfromanoviposition siteintheleaflittertopoolsofwater(Wells1981 ; Zimmermann&Zimmermann1981 ;Weygoldt 1987).Insomespeciesthisbehaviourisperformed bythemale,inothersbythefemale(Silverstone 1975 ;Zimmermann&Zimmermann1988) . Unlikemostspeciesoffrogs,whichmateinor nearaquatichabitats(Salthe&Mecham1974), dendrobatidscourtandmateawayfromwater (Wells1977) .Complexandelaboratecourtship behaviourhasbeenrecordedformanyspeciesof Dendrobates, includingtactileinteractions,long sequencesofleading(usuallybythemale),andfol- lowingassociatedwithexplorationoftheleaflitter forovipositionsites,andspecificposturaldisplays andcalltypes(Silverstone1973 ;Wells1977) . Physicalaggressionandagonisticcallingand displaybehaviourhavebeenobservedinmany *Presentaddress :SmithsonianTropicalResearchInsti- tute,P.O .Box2072,Balboa,RepublicofPanama . 0003-3472/92/060907+13$03 .00/0 speciesof Dendrobates (Senfft1936 ;Crump1972 ; Zimmermann&Zimmermann1980) .Inmostof thesespecies,maleaggressionappearstobe associatedwithterritorydefence(Bunnell1973 ; McVeyetal .1981 ;Zimmermann&Zimmermann 1981) . InthispaperIcomparetheresultsoffield investigationsoftwospeciesofdart-poisonfrogs, onewithmale,theotherwithfemaleparentalcare . Incaptivity, Dendrobatesleucomelas malesattend theireggsperiodicallywhiletheeggsareintheleaf litterdevelopingintotadpoles .Thiscareinvolves sittingontheeggs,movingthemaroundwiththeir hindlegsandsheddingwateronthem .Whenthe eggshavedevelopedintotadpoles,themaleallows themtowriggleontohisback(usuallyoneata time) ;hethencarriesthemtosmallpoolsofwater (Zimmermann&Zimmermann1980) . Dendrobates histrionicus femalesperformsimilarcareincap- tivity,exceptthatthetadpolesareplacedinsmaller pools (e .g . leafaxilsofbromeliads)andfemales returnperiodically(aboutonceperweekfor2 months)toeachaxilinwhichtheyhaveplaceda tadpoleanddepositinfertileeggsforthetadpoles tofeedon(Weygoldt1980 ;Zimmermann& Zimmermann1981,1982) .Hence,thereisacon- siderabledifferencebetweenthetwospeciesinthe roleofthesexesinparentalcare . ©1992TheAssociationfortheStudyofAnimalBehaviour 907

Upload: others

Post on 27-Jul-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

Anim. Behav., 1992,43,907-919

Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a comparative study

KYLE SUMMERS*Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI48109, U.S.A .

(Received 7 February 1991 ; initial acceptance 13 March 1991 ;final acceptance 9 October 1991; MS. number: 45969)

Abstract. Comparative field studies of species of dart-poison frogs in the genus Dendrobates were carriedout to test predictions from two hypotheses that attempt to explain female-female competition for matesin species of Dendrobates with male parental care . The sex role reversal hypothesis proposes that malesinvest so much time and energy in parental care that receptive males are rare relative to receptive females,and females compete to find and mate with receptive males . The parental quality hypothesis proposes thatfemales compete to monopolize the parental effort of particular males, because they potentially suffer acost when their mates care for the offspring of other females . Comparisons between species with maleparental care (Dendrobates leucomelas) and female parental care (Dendrobates histrionicus) contradictedprediction of the sex role reversal hypothesis, but were consistent with predictions of the parental qualityhypothesis . Male D. histrionicus did not compete for mates more aggressively than male D. leucomelas, andmale D. leucomelas were not more selective about mating than male D. histrionicus . Female D. leucomelasand D. histrionicus were both selective about mating; female D. leucomelas associated with and competedfor particular males, whereas female D. histrionicus did not .

Dart-poison frogs of the genus Dendrobates (Den-drobatidae) are found in tropical forests of Southand Central America (Silverstone 1975) . These frogsare terrestrial and diurnal ; all known species areaposematically coloured and produce highly toxicalkaloids in skin glands (Myers & Daly 1983) . Allspecies studied so far display parental care in whichone parent carries the tadpoles from an ovipositionsite in the leaf litter to pools of water (Wells 1981 ;Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1981 ; Weygoldt1987). In some species this behaviour is performedby the male, in others by the female (Silverstone1975; Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1988) .

Unlike most species of frogs, which mate in ornear aquatic habitats (Salthe & Mecham 1974),dendrobatids court and mate away from water(Wells 1977). Complex and elaborate courtshipbehaviour has been recorded for many species ofDendrobates, including tactile interactions, longsequences of leading (usually by the male), and fol-lowing associated with exploration of the leaf litterfor oviposition sites, and specific postural displaysand call types (Silverstone 1973 ; Wells 1977) .

Physical aggression and agonistic calling anddisplay behaviour have been observed in many

*Present address : Smithsonian Tropical Research Insti-tute, P.O. Box 2072, Balboa, Republic of Panama .

0003 -3472/92/060907+13$03 .00/0

species of Dendrobates (Senfft 1936; Crump 1972 ;Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1980) . In most ofthese species, male aggression appears to beassociated with territory defence (Bunnell 1973 ;McVey et al . 1981 ; Zimmermann & Zimmermann1981) .In this paper I compare the results of field

investigations of two species of dart-poison frogs,one with male, the other with female parental care .In captivity, Dendrobates leucomelas males attendtheir eggs periodically while the eggs are in the leaflitter developing into tadpoles . This care involvessitting on the eggs, moving them around with theirhind legs and shedding water on them . When theeggs have developed into tadpoles, the male allowsthem to wriggle onto his back (usually one at atime) ; he then carries them to small pools of water(Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1980) . Dendrobateshistrionicus females perform similar care in cap-tivity, except that the tadpoles are placed in smallerpools (e .g . leaf axils of bromeliads) and femalesreturn periodically (about once per week for 2months) to each axil in which they have placed atadpole and deposit infertile eggs for the tadpolesto feed on (Weygoldt 1980 ; Zimmermann &Zimmermann 1981, 1982) . Hence, there is a con-siderable difference between the two species in therole of the sexes in parental care .

© 1992 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour907

Page 2: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

908

Dendrobates leucomelas has not been previouslystudied in the field . Zimmermann & Zimmermann(1980) observed captive animals in terraria . Theyreported that males did not appear to be aggressiveor territorial, but that females were aggressive anddeveloped a hierarchical rank order if kept togetherin a terrarium . They observed courtship behaviour,in which the female followed and stroked the maleas he searched for an oviposition site .

Silverstone (1973) studied D. histrionicus at afieldsite in the Choco region of Colombia . Markand recapture studies and observations of male-male aggression suggested that males were terri-torial . Observations on male-female interactionsindicated that males and females engaged in bothtactile interactions and coordinated movementpatterns during courtship . Male territorialityand complex courtship behaviour has also beenobserved in captive D. histrionicus (Zimmermann &Zimmermann 1981, 1982) .

Dendrobates leucomelas males produce a trilllasting from a few seconds to several minutes(Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1988) . The trillapparently functions both as an advertisement call(i .e . a general purpose call to attract females andwarn other males, Wells 1977) and as a courtshipcall (Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1980) . Callsheard during D. leucomelas courtships in the fieldappeared to be softer and shorter than calls madein other contexts (personal observations), but thishas not been verified with recordings . Dendrobateshistrionicus males produce three types of calls ; anadvertisement call, a courtship call, and a releasecall (Zimmermann & Zimmermann 1981, 1982) .The advertisement call is a series of chirps, repeatedso rapidly that they blur together to the human ear(Silverstone 1973 ; Myers & Daly 1976) . It lastsfrom several seconds to almost 5 min. The court-ship call is a softer, shorter series of chirps, and therelease call is a short chirp given when a male isclasped or otherwise disturbed (Zimmermann &Zimmermann 1981, 1982) .

The purpose of comparing these species is to testpredictions from two hypotheses that could explainthe evolution of female-female competition overmates in species of Dendrobates with male parentalcare . The sex role reversal hypothesis (Trivers 1972)proposes that females compete for males becausemales invest more time and energy in each off-spring, resulting in a shortage of receptive males(Williams 1966; Trivers 1972; Wells 1981) . Thishypothesis appears to explain the occurrence of

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

female competition for mates and the high degreeof male selectivity for mates observed in speciesfrom a variety of taxa . These include pipefishes(Nerophis ophidion ; Berglund et al . 1986a, b ;Rosenqvist 1990), jacanas (Jacana spinosa ; Jenni& Collier 1972), sandpipers (Actittis macularia;Oring & Maxson 1978), katydids (Anabrus simplex ;Gwynne 1981), and water bugs (Abedus herberti;Smith 1979) .

The parental quality hypothesis (Summers 1989)has been described in varying amounts of detail byseveral authors (e .g . Wittenberger 1979 ; BlafferHrdy & Williams 1983) . It proposes that femaleswill compete for mates if they potentially suffer acost when their mate also mates with other females .One such cost might be a reduction in the quality orquantity of parental care .

These two hypotheses make different predictionsconcerning differences between males and femalesin species with male parental care, and about dif-ferences between species with male and femaleparental care . Previous research tested the predic-tions of the two hypotheses concerning differencesbetween males and females in D. auratus, a specieswith male parental care (Summers 1989, 1990a, b) .In this paper I present tests of predictions concern-ing differences between males and females in D .leucomelas (with male parental care), and betweenD. leucomelas and D. histrionicus (with femaleparental care) .

The sex role reversal hypothesis predicts thefollowing. (1) In D. leucomelas, female compe-tition for mates will be more frequent and intensethan male competition for mates, because recep-tive males will be rare relative to receptivefemales. (2) Male D. leucomelas will be moreselective than females about mating becausemales invest more per mating, and hence stand tosuffer a higher cost from choosing a low qualitymate, or from mating at a time or place that willreduce offspring survival . (3) Dendrobates histrio-nicus males will compete for mates more intenselythan D. leucomelas males, because the repro-ductive success of male D. leucomelas is notlimited by access to females, as it is in D. histrioni-cus. (4) Dendrobates leucomelas males will bemore selective than D. histrionicus males, becausethe number of females they will mate with isconstrained by the demands of parental care . (5)Dendrobates leucomelas females will attempt tocompete for many different males, because theirreproductive success is limited by their ability to

Page 3: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

find receptive males ; D. histrionicus females will notcompete for mates, because their reproductive suc-cess is limited by the demands of egg productionand maternal care . (6) Dendrobates histrionicusfemales will be more selective about mating thanD. leucomelas females, because they typically investmore per mating .

The parental quality hypothesis predicts thefollowing. (1) Female D. leucomelas will competefor mates, but competition for mates amongfemales will not be more intense than competitionamong males, because males will compete for accessto receptive females . (2) Dendrobates leucomelasfemales will be more selective about mating thanmales, because the amount of parental investmentthat males provide to their offspring is more vari-able . (3) Dendrobates leucomelas males will com-pete for females as intensely as D. histrionicusmales, because their reproductive success increaseswith the number of females that they mate with . (4)Dendrobates leucomelas and D. histrionicus maleswill both be relatively non-selective about mating(i .e . willing to mate with any female at any time),because males in both species can increase theirreproductive success by mating with many females .(5) Dendrobates leucomelas females will guardparticular males to prevent them from matingwith other females and caring for their offspring.Dendrobates histrionicus females will not guardtheir mates, because male polygyny does notimpose a cost on the fitness of their offspring . (6)Both D. leucomelas and D. histrionicus females willbe selective about mating . Dendrobates leucomelasfemales will be selective because the amount ofparental care provided by males varies dependingon the number of clutches their mate is caring for .Dendrobates histrionicus females will be selectivebecause they invest more per mating than males,and hence will suffer a high cost from mating with alow quality male, or from mating at a time or placethat reduces offspring growth or survivorship .

METHODS

The research on D. leucomelas was carried out intropical dry forest at Maria Luisa, a stream nearthe Guri hydroelectric dam in central Venezuela . Imade observations on 4 days in May, 28 days inJune, I 1 days in July and 8 days in August, 1987, fora total of 242 study hours. The study area was ashallow slope at the base of a mountain, bordering

Summers: Mating strategies in dart-poison frogs

909

the stream . A rectangular area (44 x 28 m) nextto the stream was marked with red flags at 4-mintervals, to facilitate mapping of individual pos-itions . When observing a frog, I would visualize animaginary 16-block grid within the 16-m 2 blockformed by the flags, and record the I-m 2 block(within the 16-m 2 block) in which the frog was seen .This allowed me to plot the location of each obser-vation of each frog with reasonable precision . Allindividuals seen in this plot were caught, measured(snout-vent length), and marked by toe clipping .The colour pattern on the dorsum of each individ-ual was drawn on an identification card to allowrecognition of the individual from a distance .These colour patterns were highly variable betweenindividuals, yet highly distinctive, allowing eachindividual to be recognized easily. Because of lowpopulation density, I also marked individuals on a400-m circular transect in the same area to increasethe number of individuals available for behaviouralsampling. All frogs caught along this transect weremarked and measured in the same way as thosecaught on the plot .

Dendrobates histrionicus was studied near a smallstream, approximately 5 km east of the coastaltown of San Lorenzo, on the northwest coastof Ecuador. I made observations on 12 days inFebruary, 18 days in March, and 8 days in April,for a total of 236 study hours . A rectangular grid(60 x 54 m) was constructed, with flags placed at2-m intervals . Data on the location of frogs wererecorded using methods identified above exceptthat the size of the blocks formed by the flags were4 m2 , rather than 16 m 2 , so that the locations ofindividuals were recorded as 0 .5-m 2 blocks. Thestudies of both species took place during the rainyseason .

Identification of individual D. histrionicus usingnatural markings was not possible, so each frog wasgiven two types of identification mark . I tied aunique combination of coloured beads around thewaist of each frog when it was first captured, usingcotton thread soaked in paraffin or waxed dentalfloss . This allowed identification of each individualfrom a distance, without the need to recapture theindividual . Each frog was also toe-clipped in aunique pattern to allow identification in case theidentifying beads were lost . Each frog was weighedand measured as described above .

The sex of individual D. leucomelas was assessedby examining size, shape and toepad width .Females are larger, have larger, more rounded

Page 4: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

910

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

abdomens, and narrower toepads than males(Silverstone 1975, personal observations) . Thesex of individual D. histrionicus was assessed byeither playing calls at them, or by putting them inplastic bags and shaking the bags (this treatmenttypically causes males to respond with a release call,Silverstone 1973) . Individuals that did not respondto one of these treatments by calling were assumedto be females . Silverstone (1973) used this tech-nique successfully with this species in Colombia(confirmed by dissection) .

I made observations by patrolling the study areasand recording when and where individual frogswere sighted, along with a description of theirbehaviour . During patrols of the study areas, Iattempted to identify (or capture and mark ifunmarked) each individual sighted . I recordedcalls from individuals in the field using an Audio-Technica microphone (model AT9400) and aGeneral Electric tape recorder (model 3-5016D) . Iplayed calls at the areas occupied by marked males(from the edge of the area), and recorded thepresence or absence of a response (calling back and/or moving toward calls) . I played calls at the frogswith a General Electric model 3-5016D, or model3-5300B tape recorder . I placed the volume settingat the highest level with the smaller model (model3-5300B), and at three quarters of full volume forthe larger model, which yielded sound levels thatsounded equivalent to me . I was unable to controlthe precise distance from the frog at which callswere played, because I did not always know wherethe frog was at the start of the playback . However,for all cases in which frogs did not respond whencalls were played at them, the frogs were observedto be within the maximum distance at which theyhad responded on other occasions . Hence, it isunlikely that lack of response when the frog waspresent was a result of inability to perceive thestimulus .If two or more individuals were observed

interacting, then the patrol was interrupted and Irecorded the sequence of behaviour performed byeach individual until the end of the interaction .

I considered as residents all males that werepresent for at least 2 weeks, and seen in the samearea on at least 5 separate days . Males that did notmeet these criteria were assumed to be transients .When comparing the relative aggression of males ofthe two species, it is preferable that the populationdensities of resident males be similar . Populationdensity was estimated by the cumulative number of

residents in an area divided by the total area of thestudy plot . Both study sites had male populationdensities of slightly less than one resident maleper 100-m 2 (D. leucomelas : 0 . 7 males/100-m 2 ; D .histrionicus : 0 . 9 males/100-m2 ) . Only residents wereused in the analysis of calling behaviour .

Calls were played at the areas where residentmales had been captured previously to determinehow responsive they were to intrusions by othermales. I sampled different males on different days,and varied the number of times that each male wassampled from day to day. I did this because maleswere first captured on different dates, and someindividuals were seen in their areas more frequently,or on different days or times than others . Also,patrols of the study site were occasionally inter-rupted to follow courtships, so males were notsampled an equal number of times on those days .Because of equipment failure and observer error,calls were not played at each male's area every timethat area was visually scanned . Hence, the word`trial' will be used to denote only instances when thearea was visually scanned and calls were played,whereas the word `sample' will be used to refer to allcases in which a male's area was visually scanned,whether calls were played or not . Any male that didnot have calls played at his area at least 10 times wasnot included in the analysis .

The home ranges, inter-individual distances andthe degree of overlap between the home rangesof individuals were calculated with the Newcartmapping program, written by Daniel Fox for theMichigan Interactive Data Analysis System (nodocumentation available) . Home ranges were cal-culated as minimum convex polygons that included90% of the locations where each individual wasseen during the study . The grid on the study plot atMaria Luisa in Venezuela (for D. leucomelas) wasnot completed until near the end of the study, so theaverage number of days during which the locationof a male was recorded for D. leucomelas males wassmall (X=8 days) .

The criteria used to classify the terminations offemale-male encounters were as follows . A femalewas considered to have rejected a male if shecourted with him and then moved away or did notrespond when he called, followed or stroked her . Afemale was considered to have ignored a male ifshe moved away or did not respond when heapproached, called to her, followed her or strokedher. The reciprocal occurrences were taken asevidence for a male rejecting or ignoring a female,

Page 5: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

Table I. The average number of days between the first and last capture or sighting ofmarked individuals recaptured or resighted at least once (Interval), and the averagenumber of days those individuals were seen, for both species

respectively . Cases in which a female approachingor courting a male was driven away by anotherfemale were classified as `female chases' . Instancesin which the male and female separated withoutclear rejection by either party were classified in the`separate' category

The categories used to described female-maleassociations were as follows. The female numbercategory refers to the identification numbers of theindividuals. The observations category shows theratio of the number of times a females was seen withthe male to the total number of times she was seen .Time span refers to the amount of time betweenthe first and last sighting of the pair together . Theinteractions category lists the results of focalobservations on the pairs each time they were seen .The aggression category notes whether the femalewas seen competing for the male in any of theinteractions .

Statistical analyses were performed usingSYSTAT (Wilkinson 1987) .

RESULTS

Territoriality

Forty-five male and 23 female D . leucomelas werecaptured and marked in Venezuela . Sixty-nine maleand 60 female D. histrionicus were captured andmarked in Ecuador . The number of days betweenthe first and last capture of males was longer thanthat of females in D. histrionicus (Table I,Fig. 1 ; Mann-Whitney U-test, N=127, U=1010,P<0-001), but not in D . leucomelas (Table I,Fig. 1 ; Mann-Whitney U-test, N=68, U=473,

Summers : Mating strategies in dart-poison frogs 911

P=0 . 553). Males were recaptured more fre-quently than females in D . histrionicus (Table I,Fig. 2; Mann-Whitney U-test, N=129, U=859,P<0-001), but not in D . leucomelas (Table I, Fig .2; Mann-Whitney U-test, N=68, U=400, P=0 . 118). Resident males from both species were seenrepeatedly near the area where they were first cap-tured; there was no significant difference betweenthe species in the percentage of samples that residentmales were observed in their areas (D . leucomelas :N=17, X±sE=9.7±0 .94 days out of 43 ; D .histrionicus: N=29, X±sE=11 .3± 12 days out of38; Mann-Whitney U-test, U=315, P=0 .073) .The period between a resident male's first captureand last recapture averaged 46 .7±9 .7 days forD. leucomelas males and 45 .9±22 days for D.histrionicus males; this difference was not sig-nificant (Mann-Whitney U-test, N=45, U=250,P=0.788) .

Home range sizes calculated for D. histrionicusaveraged 3 . 9 +0-5 in' for all resident males (N= 29,range =0-9 . 2 ; Fig . 3), and 5 . 1 ±0 . 6 in' for maleswhose locations were recorded on at least 10 dif-ferent days during the study (N=11, range= 2-8 . 1) . Home range sizes averaged 19 .9±8 .1 m2 forresident D . leucomelas males (N=9, range =2-81 ;Fig. 3), and 38 . 7 m2 for males whose locations wererecorded on at least ten different days (N= 3 .range= 6-81) . The home ranges of D . leucomelasmales were significantly larger than those of D .histrionicus males (Mann-Whitney U-test, N=38,U=37, P=0 .001) .

Resident males of both species usually respondedaggressively when calls were played at them, byeither calling at the recorder, moving towards it, or

D. histrionicus D. leucomelas

Male Female Male Female

IntervalN 53 21 29 23X 37 . 02 28 . 38 35 . 41 34 .62SE 2 . 14 3 . 14 424 6 .92

Days sightedN 53 23 29 13X 7-66(20%) 2-65(7%) 7 . 21 (15%) 4-39(9%)SE 0 . 86 027 0 . 81 0 . 57

Page 6: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

20

15

10

Il

both. Males of both species were seen fighting withother males (D. leucomelas: N= 11, D . histrionicus :N= 2), and also engaged in calling bouts, in whichtwo males faced each other several metres apart andcalled back and forth for periods ranging fromseveral minutes to over an hour. These obser-vations suggest that resident males of both speciesare territorial .

Number of days

Figure 1 . The distribution of the number of days between the first and last sighting of all marked individuals . Proportionper standard unit is the proportion of cases in a bar divided by the standard deviation of the sample as a proportion ofthe mean. This facilitates comparisons between histograms that utilize different scales . (a) D. histrionicus males ; (b) D .histrionicus females ; (c) D. leucomelas males; (d) D. leucomelas females.

1 .0(b) -

-0-8

-0-6

- 0 .4 SN

30

0

20

-0-2 ~°T ~

10

C01 1r"-7 1 0.0

1 .0 a0

.0

6 20mE 15

0.80

Z

0.6 010

10

a8

0.464

0.22

0 .14

0 0

14

280

Number of days seen

Figure 2 . The distribution of the number of days during which marked individuals were seen, for males and females ofboth species . (a) D. histrionicus males; (b) D . histrionicus females ; (c) D. leucomelas males ; (d) D . leucomelas females .

(d) -

Male-Male Competition

To assess whether D. histrionicus males competefor females more intensely than D. leucomelas males,I investigated two aspects of calling behaviour andthe frequency of physical aggression .

(1) One way of competing for females may be tospend more time calling to attract them . I recorded

912

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

1 .0(a)- 50 (b)-

0. 840

0.63025

20 20 0 .415

0 100 10 0 .2 0

50 I

I 00.0

00 1 4 1 .0 a25 (c)- 2 (d)-0 0.8 0

E 20 10 00 .6 az 15 8 0

a`6 0 . 410 4

0 .25 2

0 1

1 0.00

30

60

90

0

30

60

90

Page 7: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

40

0c 30

Cc

E IC7z

20

0-5•

6-10 11-15 16-20 21-25 26-30 31-35

Home range size (m 2 )Figure 3. The distribution of home range sizes of D .histrionicus (C7) and D. leucomelas (∎) males. One D.leucomelas male was not included because ofproblems ofscale ; his home range was 81 m 2 .

whether a male was calling from his territory whenI approached it, before calls were played, as ameasure of the time males spent advertising their

territories. I calculated the number of samples inwhich a male was found calling in his territorydivided by the total number of samples in which themale was seen in his territory . This was used as anestimate of the probability that a male would becalling to attract females when he was in his terri-tory, and compared between species . There was nosignificant difference between the two species in thepercentage of samples in which the male was callingwhen he was seen in his area (Fig . 4 ; D. leucomelas :N=17, X±SE=27±5%, D. histrionicus: N=29,X±sE=22±4%; Mann-Whitney U-test, U--197,P=0.329) .

(2) The percentage of trials in which a maleresponded to calls when he was seen in his area wastaken as a measure of a male's willingness to defendhis territory against intruders, which in turn shouldcorrelate with the intensity of competition overmates . Dendrobates leucomelas males were signifi-cantly more likely to respond to the recorder when

they were known to be present than D. histrionicusmales (Fig . 5 ; D. leucomelas : N=17, X±sE=94±2% ; D. histrionicus: N=29, X±SE=82±3% ;Mann-Whitney U-test, U=350, P=0-007) . Thisresult is unlikely to be due to differences in theplayback conditions, because playback soundintensities and distances from the frog were similar,and frogs that did not respond were within themaximum response distance for both species. Ifmale D. leucomelas were less likely to be seen whennot responding to calls, then this could have biasedthe results in favour of higher response rates by D .leucomelas males. This should have caused the totalpercentage of response to calls (over all trials) to be

Summers: Mating strategies in dart-poison frogs

913

Samples male called

Figure 4 . The distribution of the percentage of samples inwhich males were calling when seen in their areas . (a) D .histrionicus males ; (b) D. leucomelas males .

Trials male responded

Figure 5 . The distribution of the percentage of trials inwhich males responded, when seen in their areas . (a) D,histrionicus males; (b) D . leucomelas males .

lower for D. leucomelas males . I calculated thetotal number of times each male responded tocalls, for all trials between the first and last timethe male was sighted in his area . There wasno significant difference between D. leucomelasand D. histrionicus in the percentage of alltrials during which males responded to calls(Fig . 6 ; D. leucomelas : N= 17, X±sE=41 ±5% ;

1 .0(a) -

0-812 0-6108 0-4 C

N 60 a

4 0-2a 2 VI 0-0 0

C NN1-0

(b)- 0E 0.8z 12 0

10 -0-6 08 -0-4

a

64 -0-2

0 . 5 0-6 0-7 0.8 0-9 1 .00.0

1 .0(a) -

-0-8108 I -0-6

6 -0 .4 'ca 4 v0 0 .2 6

~' 0 IvC

0.0 -°c0

0 a

E(b)' c

0.80

E oz 0 .6 00-

4 a-0-432 0-2

0 .00-0 0 .2 0.4 .0.6 0.8

10

Page 8: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

9 1 4

0.0 0 .2 0.4 0 .6 0.8Trials male responded

1 .0

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

I•0

Figure 6. The distribution of the percentage of trials inwhich males responded to calls played in their areas,for all trials . (a) D. histrionicus males ; (b) D. leucomelasmales .

D . histrionicus : N=29, X±sE=33±4% ; Mann-Whitney U-test, U=197, P=0 . 104) .

(3) Males of both species fought with other malesduring this study . The males approached eachother, stopping frequently to call back and forth .When they were near each other, one male mightjump on the other and attempt to get on its backand clasp it just under the forelegs . If this occurred,the clasping male would then attempt to push hisopponent away from his area, or to press him to theground. The other male resisted by locking hisforelegs straight out and blocking his opponent'sattempts to push him, or by attempting to throw themale off his back . Males also grappled face to face,and whirled in a circle, each one apparently tryingto get on the other's back. The number of bouts ofphysical aggression between males was taken as anindicator of the intensity of male-male competi-tion for mates. The frequency of aggression wasmeasured as the number of bouts per day . Therewere more samples per day for D. histrionicus (agreater number of resident males and observers),which could bias the results towards a higherfrequency of fights in D . histrionicus . There weresignificantly more fights per day between D .leucomelas males than between D . histrionicusmales (D . Leucomelas : N=46, X±sE=0 .26±0.07fights per day; D. Histrionicus: N=38, X±sE=0.05±0.04 fights per day ; Mann-Whitney U-test,U=710, P=0.019) .

C,

10

0

N 4E LE . F n

< 1

1-5 5-10 10-20 20-40 >40

Duration of fight (min)

Figure 7 . The distribution of aggressive interactiondurations for male ( •) and female (O) D. leucomelas(does not include calling bouts between males) .

Female Aggression

Female D . leucomelas were observed engagingin intrasexual aggression four times during thestudy. Female-female aggression always occurredin the presence of a calling male, and both femalesrepeatedly attempted to approach the male duringthe fight. There were more male-male fightsper day than female-female fights (Fig . 7 ;males: X±sE=0 .26±0 . 07 fights per day ; females :X±sE=0 .087±0 .04 fights per day; Mann-Whitney U-test, N=46, U=895, P=0 .047). Therewas no significant difference between male andfemale fight durations in D . leucomelas (Fig. 7 ;males: X±sE=4 .73±1 .98 min; females: X± SE=15 . 83 ± 11 . 29 min; Mann-Whitney U-test, N= 15,U=28, P=0 .468) .

Courtship

Courtship leading to oviposition was observednine times in both D. leucomelas and D. histrionicus .Courtship behaviour in D . leucomelas typicallyinvolved prolonged exploration of the leaf litter,with the male leading . The female frequentlystroked, nudged and climbed on the male as she wasfollowing him . The male called at the female inter-mittently during the courtship . Males appeared tocall more frequently when they became separatedfrom the female, or when they were out of thefemale's sight (e .g . under the leaf litter) . Oviposi-tion was not observed in D . leucomelas because italways occurred under the leaf litter .

During D . histrionicus courtships, the maletypically approached and called at the female whenshe approached the male's perch . If the female didnot move away, then the male moved away, and

(a)0 .8

10 0 .686 0 .4

j

N 4 a00 0 .2 00000 .0 ND

1 .0 0.0(b) 0

0.8E0 az 00 .6 a

4 0.432 0.2

0.0

Page 9: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

then stopped and called until the female followed .The male would then move away again . Thissequence was repeated until the male went underthe leaf litter and called, and the female followed .Oviposition would then occur under the litter .Few tactile interactions were observed duringthese sequences, although males were occasionallyobserved to stroke, climb, or jump on the femalewhen the female ignored or rejected the male . Incontrast to D. leucomelas, D. histrionicus pairs didnot engage in extensive leaf litter exploration .Oviposition was observed on one occasion in D.histrionicus, when a pair mated under an overhang-ing leaf that allowed enough light to observe theinteraction. After moving onto the leaf on whichoviposition occurred, both the female and the maleperformed several types of movements on the leaf .These movements consisted mostly of crouchingdown on the leaf and rotating in place (bothclockwise and anticlockwise), and rapid kickingmovements of the hind legs . Head bobbing andforeleg patting also occurred . In addition, the malerepeatedly stroked the female with his forelegduring these movements. When the female beganlaying eggs, the male climbed on top of her backbriefly, and then climbed off and left . The femalecontinued to sit on the eggs and rotate, forapproximately 30 min .

Courtship averaged at least 246±20 min in D.leucomelas (N= 8) and 71 ± 7 min in D. histrionicus(N=9) . These are minimum estimates becausesome courtships had already begun when thepair was observed . During courtship, female D.leucomelas were more active in tactile stimulation(i .e . the number of times the female stroked,nudged, climbed on or jumped on the male, andvice versa) than males (Wilcoxon signed-rankstest, N=22, P=0 .001). In contrast, D. histrionicusfemales were not observed to stroke, nudge, climbon. or jump on males .

Selectivity

The most accurate measures of mate selectivityavailable for dendrobatids are the results ofmale-female encounters (Summers 1989) . Femalesrejected or ignored males 30 times in D. leucomelas,and 28 times in D. histrionicus, but males did notreject or ignore females in either species (Fig .8 ; chi-squared test ; D. leucomelas : 30 versus 0,x 2 =20, 4f= 1, P<0 . 001 ; D . histrionicus : 28 versus0, y 2 =18,df=1,P<0 .001) .

Summers: Mating strategies in dart-poison frogs

C0U04)

30

20

8

6E

66

Figure 8 . The results ofencounter terminations observedin D. leucomelas (/) and D. histrionicus (EI) . The numberover each bar indicates the percentage of the total numberof observations (for each species) that the bar represents .Behavioural categories are described in the Methodssection .

Female-Male Associations

Some female D. leucomelas were frequentlyassociated with particular males (Table II) . A femalewould repeatedly court the same male, even thoughshe did not mate with him in that interaction . Twofemales (females 5 and 6), on separate occasions,actively courted a male that they had fought over,and then rejected him 10-20 min after the otherfemale had left. One of these females was observedmating with the male she had fought for previously(over 3 weeks before) . In contrast, no D. histrionicusfemales were seen to associate with the same maletwice, and females were never observed to activelycourt males . Observations on both species weremade during the rainy season, during similar timesof day, and mating was observed with equal fre-quency in both species . Hence, the differences infemale-male association patterns between the twospecies are unlikely to be due to differences inenvironmental conditions .

Parental Care

Several male D. leucomelas were observedcarrying tadpoles to treeholes on the study site .Female D. histrionicus were observed carrying tad-poles on several occasions, but tadpole depositionwas witnessed only once . The female deposited asingle tadpole (out of three on her back) in a stemaxil of a Heliconia plant. Another female wasobserved extruding an egg into the stem axil of a

2

∎4)

y N _N NO

O U O ~o ~~2ioCL

Cn

Behavioural category

0'

915

Page 10: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

9 1 6

Table II. Female--male associations in D. teucornelas

See Methods section for category descriptions .

Calathea plant. Further investigation revealed D . range centre . The home range centre of eight D .histrionicus tadpoles in the stem axils of both of histrionicus males were within 5 m of their nearestthese types of plant on the study site .

neighbour ; whereas all D. leucomelas male homerange centres were further than 5 m apart . Therewas slight overlap between the home ranges of twopairs of D. histrionicus males, but there was nooverlap between the home ranges of D. leucomelasmales .Zimmermann & Zimmermann (1980) reported

that captive D. leucomelas males were not aggressiveor territorial in terraria, while females wereaggressive . The results reported here show that thebehaviour of D. leucomelas males in the field wasquite different from that in captivity . Observationsof male site specificity, response to calls andaggression against intruders, suggest that males areterritorial in the wild . As in D. auratus, obser-vations on aggressive intra-sexual interactions donot support the sex role reversal hypothesis, whichpredicts that female-female aggression will be morecommon and intense than male-male aggression .Female D. histrionicus were not observed tocompete for mates, as predicted by both hypotheses .

DISCUSSION

Aggression

Dendrobates histrionicus males did not call morefrequently or respond to calls more aggressivelythan D. leucomelas males. This suggests that maleswere not defending their territories more aggres-sively, or trying to attract females more vigorouslyin D. histrionicus . Dendrobates leucomelas engagedin physical aggression more frequently than D .histrionicus . These results do not support the sexrole reversal hypothesis, but are consistent with theparental quality hypothesis .

Dendrobates leucomelas males generally hadlarger home ranges than D. histrionicus males .Wells (1978) suggested that male D . auratusmight keep clutches from different females widelyseparated to prevent their mates from finding anddestroying the clutches of other mates. The differ-ences between species in home range size are consis-tent with this possibility, although the home rangesof D. leucomelas males were quite variable in size .The average and median distance between thecentres of home ranges was higher for D. histrionicusmales, but the larger sample size and the patchydistribution of D. histrionicus males on the studysite meant that more D. histrionicus males werefound within 5 m of their neighbour's home

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

Courtship

Courtship in D. leucomelas in the field wassimilar to that of captive animals, as described byZimmermann &Zimmermann (1980) . Courtship inD. histrionicus was similar to that described bySilverstone (1973) in the field, and to descriptionsof courtship in captive animals (Zimmermann &Zimmermann 1981) . Silverstone described nine

Female no . Observations Time span Interactions Aggression

6 2/3 2 weeks 2 rejections Yes9 4/5 2 months 3 rejections

2 7/8 6 weeks

I mating

5 rejections

5 3/3 4 weeks2 matings2 rejections Yes

4 6/7 5 weeks

I mating

5 rejections

7 4/4 10 days

1 separate

4 rejections

Page 11: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

different components of courtship ; calling, pur-suing, touching, sitting, hugging, shaking (hind andforelimb), bowing, crouching and circling. With theexception of hugging, all these behaviour patternswere observed in this study . However, touchingwas done only by males, and males appeared tolead females to the oviposition site, rather thanthe reverse. Zimmermann & Zimmermann (1981)reported that males stroked females, but notthe reverse. In contrast, D . leucomelas femalesappeared to take a more active role in courtship .

SelectivityPatterns of courtship behaviour and encounter

terminations in D . leucomelas were similar to thoseseen in D. auratus (Wells 1978 ; Summers 1989) .Females actively courted males, but also rejected orignored males frequently, whereas males did notreject or ignore females . Hence, the active tactilecourtship of males by females did not indicate thatD. auratus (Summers 1989) or D . leucomelas maleswere more selective than females about mating .In contrast, females of both species were moreselective than males about mating, as predicted bythe parental quality hypothesis. Dendrobatesleucomelas males were no more selective than theircounterparts in D . histrionicus, as indicated by thelack of rejection of females by males in both species .

Mate GuardingIn D. auratus some females guard their mates

by remaining in or near their mates' territories,courting them frequently, and attacking any otherfemales they encounter courting them (Summers1989). My observation of a female D . leucomelasdriving away another female, then courting themale for a while, and then rejecting him, suggestthat females may court males actively in order toprevent them from courting and mating with otherfemales . The duration of some female-male associ-ations suggests that some females will guard aparticular male over a long period of time . Theseresults are consistent with the prediction ofmate guarding derived from the parental qualityhypothesis . In contrast, female D. histrionicus didnot actively court males, and were not observedrepeatedly with the same male . In his study on D .histrionicus in Colombia, Silverstone (1973) notedthat 3 out of 51 females captured near a male (onthe same or a neighbouring plant) were recaptured

Summers: Mating strategies in dart-poison_ frogs

9 17

near that male again after periods of 2--11 days .He did not report any interactions between theseindividuals. A female was never seen near the samemale more than twice . Silverstone concluded thatthe associations he saw did not support his hypoth-esis that female and male D . histrionicus formedpair bonds to enhance the effectiveness of parentalcare . Silverstone also noted that females sometimescourted males by stroking them, but did not recordthe frequency of this tactile stimulation by females .In the two courtships which he described in detail,only the male courted tactually .

Parental CareAlthough observations on parental care in D .

leucomelas were sparse, the patterns observed weresimilar to those seen in D. auratus (Summers 1989,1990b) ; males apparently deposit tadpoles in smallpools of water in treeholes . Silverstone (1973)recorded D, histrionicus tadpoles from bromeliadleaf axils, but the use of Heliconia and Calatheastems for deposition of tadpoles had not beenrecorded previously in this species .

Sexual Selection in DendrobatesThe comparisons between D . leucomelas and

D. histrionicus support the parental quality hypoth-esis, and do not support the sex role reversalhypothesis as an explanation of female-femalecompetition for mates in species of Dendrobate.swith male parental care . Female-female aggressionin D . leucomelas appears to be part of a mate guard-ing strategy that some females employ to preventtheir mates from caring for the offspring of otherfemales . The similarities between D . leucomelas andD. auratus males and females in mating, aggressiveand associative behaviour supports the hypothesisthat the potentially variable quality of maleparental care in Dendrobates in general has ledto the evolution of mate guarding strategies byfemales in this genus . Because D . leucomelas and D .auratus probably share a common ancestor morerecently than either do with D . histrionicus or anyother species with female parental care (C . Myers,personal communication), the association of maleparental care with mate guarding by females islikely to be homologous rather than convergent .Hence, the comparison between D . leucomelas andD. auratus provides support for the hypothesis ofa causal relationship between male parental care

Page 12: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

9 1 8

and female aggression, but does not provide thestrong evidence that would be provided by theindependent evolution of the association in separ-ate lineages (Brooks & McLennan 1991) . Female-female competition for mates in association with aconflict of interest between males and females hasnot been documented in any other genus of frog, buthas been indicated in other taxa, particularly red-winged blackbirds (Yasukawa & Searcy 1982) andpied flycatchers (Breiehagen & Slagsvold 1988) .

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I am deeply indebted to Arnold Kluge, RichardAlexander, A . Stanley Rand, Nancy Knowlton,Richard Wrangham and Brian Hazlett for discus-sion, advice, criticism and support . I thank MichaelBarry for excellent assistance in the field ; DrFausto Sarmiento R ., Dr Sergio Figuerroa S ., theEcuadorean National Museum of Natural Sciencesand the Ecuadorean Ministry of Agriculture andLivestock for assistance with logistics and permitsin Ecuador; Stefan Gorzula for friendship, logisti-cal support, and for letting me stay in his housefor three months ; Luis Balbas and Asterio Farrerafor support and guidance in the field ; EDELCAfor permission to work at Guri, Venezuela ; CharlesMyers for advice and for data . This project wassupported by funding from the SmithsonianInstitution, the Smithsonian Tropical ResearchInstitute, the Sigma Xi Scientific Society, theExplorers Club, the Horace H . Rackham School ofGraduate Studies, the Museum of Zoology and theDepartment of Biological Sciences, University ofMichigan.

REFERENCES

Berglund, A ., Rosenqvist, G . & Svensson, I . 1986a . Matechoice, fecundity and sexual dimorphism in two pipe-fish species (Syngnathidae) . Behav . Ecol. Sociobiol ., 19,301-307 .

Berglund, A ., Rosenqvist, G . & Svensson, I. 1986b .Reversed sex roles and parental energy investment inzygotes of two pipefish (Syngnathidae) species. Mar .Ecol. Progr. Ser., 29,209-215 .

Blaffer Hrdy, S . & Williams, G. C. 1983 . Behavioralbiology and the double standard . In : Social Behavior ofFemale Vertebrates (Ed. by S . K . Wasser), pp. 3-17 .New York : Academic Press .

Breiehagen, T. & Slagsvold, T . 1988 . Male polyterritori-ality and female-female aggression in pied flycatchersFicedula hypoleuca . Anim . Behav ., 36,604-605 .

Animal Behaviour, 43, 6

Brooks, D . R. & McLennan, D . A . 1991 . Phylogeny,Ecology and Behavior. Chicago : University of ChicagoPress .

Bunnell, P . 1973 . Vocalizations in the territorial behaviorof the frog, Dendrobates pumilio. Copeia, 1973,-284 .

Crump, M . L . 1972 . Territoriality and mating behaviorin Dendrobates granuliferus (Anura : dendrobatidae) .Herpetologica, 28, 195-198 .

Gwynne, D . J . 1981 . Sexual difference theory : mormoncrickets show role reversal in mate choice . Science, 213,779-780 .

Jenni, D . A. & Collier, G . 1972 . Polyandry in theAmerican jacana (Jacana spinosa) . Auk, 89, 743-765 .

McVey, M. E., Zahary, R. G ., Perry, D . & Dougal, J . M .1981 . Territoriality and homing behavior in the poisondart frog (Dendrobates pumilio) . Copeia, 1981, 1-8 .

Myers, C . W . & Daly, J . W . 1976 . Preliminary evaluationof skin toxins and vocalizations in taxonomic and evolu-tionary studies of poison-dart frogs (Dendrobatidae) .Am. Mus . Novit ., NY ., 157,173-262 .

Myers, C . W . & Daly, J . W . 1983 . Dart-poison frogs . Sci .Am., 248, 120-133 .

Oring, L. W. & Maxson, S . J . 1978 . Instances of simul-taneous polyandry by a spotted sandpiper, Actitismacularia. Ibis, 120, 349-353 .

Rosenqvist, G . 1990 . Male mate choice and female--female competition for mates in the pipefish Nerophisophidion. Anim . Behav ., 39, 1110--1115 .

Salthe, S . N. & Mecham, J . S . 1974 . Reproductive andcourtship patterns . In : Physiology of the Amphibia .Vol. III (Ed. by B . Lofts), pp . 309-521 . New York :Academic Press .

Senftt, W. 1936 . Das Brutgeschaft des Baumsteiger-frosches (Dendrobates auratus Girard) . Gefangenschaft .Zool. Garten ., 8, 122-136 .

Silverstone, P . A . 1973 . Observations on the behavior andecology of a Colombian poison-arrow frog, the Kokoe-pa (Dendrobates histrionicus Berthold). Herpetologica,29,295-301 .

Silverstone, P . A . 1975 . A revision of the poison-arrowfrogs of the genus Dendrobates Wagler . L . A. Nat . Hist .Mus . Sci. Bull., 21, 1-55 .

Smith, R . L . 1979 . Paternity assurance and altered rolesin the mating behaviour of a giant water bug, Abedusherberti (Heteroptera : Belostomatidae) . Anim. Behav.,27,716-725 .

Summers, K. 1989 . Sexual selection and intra-femalecompetition in the green dart-poison frog, Dendrobatesauratus. Anim . Behav ., 37, 797-805 .

Summers, K. 1990a . Parental investment and sexualselection in dart-poison frogs (Dendrobatidae) . Ph .D .thesis, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor .

Summers, K. 1990b . Parental care and the cost ofpolygyny in the green dart-poison frog, Dendrobatesauratus. Behav . Ecol. Sociobiol ., 27, 307-313 .

Trivers, R . L . 1972 . Parental investment and sexualselection . In : Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man1871-1971 (Ed . by B . Campbell), pp . 136-179 . Chicago :Aldine-Atherton .

Wells, K . D. 1977 . The courtship of frogs . In : TheReproductive Biology of Amphibians (Ed. by D. H .Taylor & S . I . Guttman), pp . 233-262 . New York :Plenum Press .

Page 13: Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a

Wells, K. D . 1978 . Courtship and parental behaviorin a Panamanian poison-arrow frog, (Dendrobatesauratus). Herpetologica, 34,148-155 .

Wells, K. D . 1981. Parental behavior of male and femalefrogs . In: Natural Selection and Social Behavior (Ed . byR. D. Alexander & D. W . Tinkle), pp . 184-197 . NewYork : Chiron Press .

Weygoldt, P . 1980 . Complex brood care and reproductivebehaviour in captive poison-arrow frogs, Dendrobatespumilio 0 . Schmidt . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol ., 7,329-332 .

Weygoldt, P . 1987 . Evolution of parental care in dart-poison frogs (Amphibia : Dendrobatidae) . Z. Zool.Syst . Evolut. forsch ., 25, 51-67 .

Williams, G . C . 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection .Princeton : University of Princeton Press .

Wilkinson, L . 1987 . SYSTAT: the System for Statistics .Evanston, Illinois : Systat .

Wittenberger, J . F . 1979. The evolution of mating sys-tems in birds and mammals . In : Handbook of Behav-ioral Neurobiology. Vol. 3: Social Behavior and

Summers: Mating strategies in dart-poison frogs

919

Communication (Ed. by P . Marler & J . G . Vandenburg ),pp . 271-349. New York : Plenum Press .

Yasukawa, K . & Searcy, W. A. 1982 . Aggression infemale red-winged blackbirds : a strategy to ensure maleparental investment . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol ., 11, 13--17 .

Zimmermann, H . & Zimmermann, E . 1980. DurchNachzucht erhalten : der Baumsteiger Dendrobatesleucomelas . Aquarien. Mag., 14,211-217 .

Zimmermann, H. & Zimmermann, E . 1981. Sozial-verhalten, Fortpflanzngsverhalten and Zucht derFarberfrosche Dendrobates histrionicus and D. lehmannisowie einiger anderer Dendrobatiden . Z. Kolner Zoo .,24,83-99 .

Zimmermann, E. & Zimmermann, H . 1982 . SozialeAktionen, Brutpflege and Zucht des PfeilgiftfroschesDendrobates histrionicus . Salamandra, 18,150--167 .

Zimmermann, H. & Zimmermann, E . 1988. Ethotaxono-mie and Zoographische Artenggruppenbildung beiPfeilgiftfroschen (Anura : Dendrobatidae) . Salamandra,24,125-160 .