mercadotecnia the effects of political advertising on young voters

Upload: fadli-noor

Post on 03-Apr-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    1/16

    http://abs.sagepub.com

    American Behavioral Scientist

    DOI: 10.1177/00027642073000392007; 50; 1137American Behavioral Scientist

    Gail LeGrangeLynda Lee Kaid, Monica Postelnicu, Kristen Landreville, Hyun Jung Yun and Abby

    The Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/50/9/1137The online version of this article can be found at:

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:American Behavioral ScientistAdditional services and information for

    http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/50/9/1137Citations

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/50/9/1137http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/50/9/1137http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    2/16

    1137

    Authors Note: Special thank you to UVote team members who assisted with data collection for the

    advertising experiment projects.

    American Behavioral Scientist

    Volume 50 Number 9

    May 2007 1137-1151

    2007 Sage Publications

    10.1177/0002764207300039

    http://abs.sagepub.com

    hosted athttp://online.sagepub.com

    The Effects of Political

    Advertising on Young VotersLynda Lee KaidMonica PostelnicuKristen LandrevilleHyun Jung YunAbby Gail LeGrangeUniversity of Florida

    Political advertising effects on candidate evaluations, issue recall, political cynicism,

    and gender differences are explored in this pretestposttest examination of 764 young

    adult participants. Results show no major gender differences in evaluation of candi-

    dates. Participants reported learning more about Bushs image and more about Kerrys

    issues through the ads. Exposure to ads did not produce increased cynicism among the

    participants but significantly increased political information efficacy.

    Keywords: George W. Bush; John Kerry; political advertising; young voters; candi-

    date image; campaign issues; agenda setting

    Young voters were among the most targeted segments of the electorate during the2004 U.S. presidential campaign. Political parties, nonprofit organizations,issue groups, mass media, student organizations, and even popular entertainers

    joined efforts to convince young people to show up at the polls. Engaging this tradi-

    tionally apathetic public has become more essential in the aftermath of the 2000

    presidential election, decided by only 500 votes and just barely half (51%) of all eli-

    gible voters (Federal Election Commission, 2004).

    Political advertising has the potential to serve as a valid source of informationabout the candidates during a political campaign. Numerous studies have shown that

    voters exposed to political ads on television retain knowledge and information about

    the candidates, such as their name, stance on issues, or image attributes (Atkin &

    Heald, 1976; Kaid, 2002; Martinelli & Chaffee, 1995; Valentino, Hutchings, & Williams,

    2004). Exposure to political ads is also effective in influencing viewers evaluations

    of the candidates (Kahn & Geer, 1994; Kaid, Chanslor, & Hovind, 1992; Kaid &

    Sanders, 1978; Tinkham & Weaver-Lariscy, 1993) as well as voters perceptions of

    the political process in general and their political behavior (Ansolabehere & Iyengar,

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    3/16

    1138 American Behavioral Scientist

    1995; Kaid et al., 1992; Lemert, Wanta, & Lee, 1999). This article explores the

    effects of television political advertising on young voters.

    Research and Theory on Advertising Effects

    Television political advertising was adopted in 1952 by Dwight D. Eisenhowers

    presidential campaign as a way of promoting his presidency to voters. During the

    past five decades, presidential candidates have devoted ever-higher amounts of their

    campaign budgets to produce and broadcast political spots. The 2004 presidential

    campaign set a new record for advertising spending (more than $600 million), a

    235% increase compared to 2000 (Devlin, 2005).

    Effects of Advertising Exposure on Candidate Image Evaluations

    The importance given by candidates and campaigns to political advertising has

    prompted substantial research about its effects. Even the earliest research on politi-

    cal advertising validated the candidates decisions to rely on this communication

    tool. Researchers have shown that television advertising is successful in conveying

    candidate messages to voters, overcoming selective exposure (Atkin, Bowen,

    Nayman, & Sheinkopf, 1973) and gaining attention from 70% of voters.One of the most important outcomes of this attention to political television ads

    may be the impact on voter evaluations of the candidates featured in the ads.

    Candidates make use of advertising to generate positive feelings among the elec-

    torate about their own qualities or to denigrate their opponents. Research has shown

    that advertising exposure can influence a voters evaluations of the candidates, either

    in a positive or negative direction. Kaid (1994, 1998, 2001, 2002, 2003) conducted

    several experiments exposing college students as well as adult voters to spots from

    various presidential campaigns and concluded that exposure to ads can significantly

    change ratings of candidates. The change in candidate evaluations is determined bymany factors, such as the channel on which the ad is shown (Kaid, 2002; Kaid &

    Postelnicu, 2005) or the content of the ad. For instance, issue-focused ads are more

    likely to trigger a positive attitude toward the candidate than image-focused ads, and

    nonattack ads are more likely to create support for the candidate than attack ads

    (Christ & Thorson, 1994; Kahn & Geer, 1994; Pinkleton, Um, & Weintraub Austin,

    2002). Negative ads are successful in denigrating the candidate who is the target of

    the attacks (Johnson-Cartee & Copeland, 1989; Perloff & Kinsey, 1992) but may

    also result in a backlash effect against the candidate who makes the attack

    (Garramone, 1984).Regardless of the direction of the attitude provoked by the ad, mass communica-

    tion scholars largely agree that television advertising makes an impact on votersper-

    ceptions of the candidate. There is evidence that people with lower interest in politics

    and with less information about a campaign are more likely to change their attitude

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    4/16

    toward the candidates after viewing political ads on television (Cundy, 1986;

    Rothschild & Ray, 1974). Therefore, we expected political advertising to have a strong

    impact on young voters evaluations of the candidate, because this group of voterstraditionally has lower levels of interest and involvement than older voters, leading

    to our first hypothesis:

    Hypothesis 1: Exposure to political television ads will significantly increase positive evalua-

    tions of candidates Bush and Kerry.

    However, not all voters react the same to the presentation of a candidate in tele-

    vision advertising. Research has shown that the gender of the voter can make a dif-

    ference in the reaction to political spots. For instance, findings have suggested thatreactions to political ads mirror the gender gap identified in voting behavior. That is,

    women appear to evaluate Democratic presidential candidates more positively than

    men do, and men are more positive about Republican candidates (Kaid, 1994, 1998;

    Kaid & Tedesco, 1999). Although they did not find large differences between male

    and female voter reactions to candidate ads, Bystrom, Banwart, Kaid, and Robertson

    (2004) found some evidence that women respond more positively to positive cam-

    paign messages. In a broader, multicountry study, Kaid and Holtz-Bacha (2000)

    found that women tend to be generally more susceptible to televised political spots

    and rate presidential candidates higher after viewing. This research suggested oursecond hypothesis:

    Hypothesis 2: Women will evaluate the candidates significantly higher after advertising expo-

    sure than will men.

    Effects of Political Advertising Exposure on Information Recall

    An extensive body of research supports the finding that exposure and attention to

    political advertising leads to increased voter knowledge about candidates and issues(Faber & Storey, 1984; Groenendyk & Valentino, 2002; Kaid & Sanders, 1978).

    Recall of information is particularly high after exposure to negative ads (Basil,

    Schooler, & Reeves, 1991; Johnson-Cartee & Copeland, 1989; Kahn & Kenney,

    2000; Lang, 1991; Newhagen & Reeves, 1991). The amount of time spent in front

    of the TV set is another mediating factor of how much information from the ads

    people remember. Heavy TV viewers are more likely to recall seeing ads than people

    who watch television less than 3 hours daily. According to a recent poll, an average

    American adult spends a little more than 3 hours daily watching television (Yang,

    2004). Recall is also directly linked to a series of other factors, such as ones inter-est in the race (Atkin et al., 1973; Rothschild & Ray, 1974) or ones attitudes toward

    politics (Christ & Thorson, 1994).

    A great deal of research has concentrated on identifying issue learning from polit-

    ical ad exposure, and findings suggest that issue learning from exposure to political

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1139

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    5/16

    ads is surprisingly high, even greater than issue learning from television news expo-

    sure (Brians & Wattenberg, 1996; Patterson & McClure, 1976; Zhao & Bleske, 1995)

    or even televised debates (Holbert, Benoit, Hansen, & Wen, 2002; Just, Crigler, &Wallach, 1990).

    Regardless of the multitude of factors that influence recall and information reten-

    tion, it is generally recognized that political ads have the potential to provide view-

    ers with knowledge about the candidates and the campaign. One of the consequences

    of acquiring information from such sources is that viewers may change their issue

    agenda to match the issues discussed in the ads (Herrnson & Patterson, 2000;

    Roberts, 1992; West, 1993). Issue ads are effective in making policy issues more

    salient to audiences, but image ads are equally successful in increasing the salience

    of candidates attributes, a process called second-level agenda setting (McCombs,Lopez-Escobar, & Llamas, 2000).

    These prior findings on the successful communication of issue information and

    the agenda-setting effects of political ad exposure led to the next two hypotheses

    tested in this study:

    Hypothesis 3: Exposure to political television ads will result in higher levels of candidate

    image learning than issue learning.

    Hypothesis 4: Exposure to political television ads will have a significant agenda-setting effect,

    resulting in changes in the issues that respondents judge as most important.

    Ad Exposure and Political System Effects

    Apart from affecting viewers evaluations of candidates and issues, political

    advertising can trigger more complex emotions and attitudes. Ansolabehere and

    Iyengar (1995) believe that attack ads in particular contribute to increasing political

    cynicism among voters, together with television (Putnam, 1995) and strategic cam-

    paign coverage by the media (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997). Rahn and Hirshorn

    (1999) found that exposure to negative advertising altered young peoples political

    attitudes, although it did not significantly affect their desire to vote. On the contrary,

    young people with high levels of political efficacy felt stimulated in their political

    beliefs after viewing negative ads. However, Kaid, McKinney, and Tedesco (2000)

    found young voters were more cynical after exposure to political spots in the 1996

    campaign. The connection between negative advertising, political apathy, and cyni-

    cism has not been clearly confirmed by other studies (Garramone, Atkin, Pinkleton,

    & Cole, 1990). Pinkleton et al. (2002) found no evidence for such claims, whereas

    Kaid and Postelnicu (2005) actually found that undergraduate college voters

    expressed lower levels of political cynicism after exposure to a mixture of positive

    and negative ads. These findings led to the fifth hypothesis:

    Hypothesis 5: Exposure to political television ads will result in no significant change in polit-

    ical cynicism levels for young citizens.

    1140 American Behavioral Scientist

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    6/16

    Getting young citizens to become involved in the political process is not an easy

    task. Delli Carpini (2000) painted a pessimistic portrait of Americans younger than

    30: Overall, they are more cynical than the older population, less interested in publicaffairs, less likely to register or to vote, and significantly less knowledgeable about

    politics. Several surveys found that this lack of information about candidates, par-

    ties, the government, and the act of voting is the number one cause of political apa-

    thy (Declare Yourself, 2003; National Association of Secretaries of State [NASS],

    1999). Respondents reported that they need to know the candidates stances on

    issues, their personal qualities, and political competence level before making an

    informed decision to vote. Lack of this type of information translates into feelings

    of low political efficacy that leads to apathy (Declare Yourself, 2003; NASS, 1999).

    Young voters appear to be aware of their low knowledge levels, and the ThirdMillennium study of young voters motivations for voting and nonvoting found that

    the young generation often cited as a reason for not voting in 2000 the fact that they

    did not feel they have enough time or information (Murphy, 2000). In their work

    on citizen engagement in 1996 and 2000, Kaid et al. (2000; Kaid, McKinney, &

    Tedesco, 2004) have found that young voters low levels of political information effi-

    cacy is a significant cause of nonvoting. However, Kaid, Landreville, Postelnicu, and

    Martin (2005) found that exposure to both television ads and debates can increase

    young voters feelings of political information efficacy. Research has not yet

    explored whether there are gender differences in political information efficacy.These concerns led to our final hypothesis and related research question:

    Hypothesis 6: Exposure to the political television ads will significantly increase feelings of

    political information efficacy.

    Research Question 1: Will men experience significantly higher levels of information efficacy

    after political advertising exposure than women?

    Method

    Participants

    An experimental design was used to test the above hypotheses. Participants were

    764 undergraduate students from 13 different universities1 in the United States.

    Experiments took place at the same time in all 13 locations, 1 week before the

    November 2004 Election Day. The total sample was composed of 44% males and

    56% females, with an average age of 21. Their party affiliation was 35% Republican,

    41% Democrat, and 24% Independent or affiliated with other parties. Both the gen-

    der and the party identification distributions are typical for American collegestudents. The large number of participants from diverse geographic locations cover-

    ing both battleground and nonbattleground states further guaranteed that the sample

    is representative of college voters nationwide.

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1141

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    7/16

    Procedure

    On arrival at the various experiment locations, participants were asked to fill out

    a pretest questionnaire. After finishing the questionnaire, students were shown a col-lection of 10 political television ads (5 ads sponsored by George Bush and 5 by John

    Kerry, alternated by candidate). These ads were typical of those running in the cam-

    paign during the past few weeks.2 Exposure to this ad stimulus was followed by ask-

    ing participants to fill out a posttest questionnaire.

    Measuring Instruments

    Both pretest and posttest questionnaires contained measures of participants eval-

    uations of the candidates and their levels of political cynicism and information effi-cacy. Evaluations of Bush and Kerry were measured using a feeling thermometer

    scale ranging from 0 (cool) to 100 (warm) like the one traditionally used by the

    National Election Studies to measure attitudes toward the candidates (Rosenstone,

    Kinder, Miller, & the National Election Studies, 1997). Candidate evaluations were

    also measured using a 12-item semantic differential scale3 developed for measuring

    candidate image and used for nearly four decades as a measure of candidate image

    (Kaid, 2004). The 12-item image scale achieved high reliability when used as an

    index with Cronbachs alpha reliability levels of+.90 in the pretest and +.89 in the

    posttest for Bush, and +.87 in the pretest and +.92 in the posttest for Kerry.Several other measures were used to evaluate participants levels of political cyn-

    icism and information efficacy. Political cynicism was measured with an eight-item

    index4 with Cronbachs alpha reliability of +.70 in the pretest and +.83 in the

    posttest. Information efficacy was measured with a four-item index,5 and Cronbachs

    alpha levels were + .86 in the pretest and +.88 in the posttest.

    Results

    Effects of Ad Exposure on Candidate Image

    The first hypothesis predicted that exposure to the television ads would result in a

    higher positive evaluation of both candidates. Table 1 provides evidence that this hypoth-

    esis was not supported. Using the semantic differential scale of 12 adjectives to evaluate

    the candidates, young respondents gave Bush a composite mean rating of 53.9 in the

    pretest and 53.6 in the posttest. Responses to Kerry were similar in the overall sample with

    a 53.7 pretest and a 53.5 posttest rating. Neither difference was statistically significant.

    The second hypothesis stated that women would rate the candidates more positivelyafter ad exposure than would men. This hypothesis was also rejected. As Table 1

    shows, there was no significant difference between mens and womens evaluations

    of Bush or Kerry before the ads were shown, and exposure did not result in any

    major changes for either candidate.

    1142 American Behavioral Scientist

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    8/16

    Table 1 also shows that partisan affiliation of the respondents had little relation-

    ship to postexposure changes in evaluations for George W. Bush. However, viewing

    the spots did have a negative effect on Kerrys image ratings among Republican

    respondents who rated Kerry at 44.64 in the pretest but gave him a significantly

    lower rating of 43.05 in the posttest, t = 2.54, df = 265,p = .01. Those who identi-

    fied themselves as Independent or sympathetic to another political party did not

    change their evaluations of either candidate after viewing.

    Effects of Exposure on Issue and Image Learning

    The third hypothesis that predicted exposure to the television ads would result in

    more learning about the candidates issues than about their image qualities received

    mixed support. Respondents were asked to indicate if they had learned a great deal (7)

    or very little (1) about the issues from the ads they saw for each candidate. They were

    also asked to similarly respond to questions about how much they learned about the

    personal qualities of Bush and Kerry. Table 2 indicates that participants did believethey had learned significantly more about the personal qualities of Bush (M = 3.95,

    SD = 1.87) than they did about the issues (M = 3.27, SD = 1.85) from his ads, t =

    9.62, df = 762,p = .001. However, the opposite was true for Kerry. Participants felt

    they had learned more about the issues (M = 3.49, SD = 1.85) than about Kerrys per-

    sonal qualities (M = 3.26, SD = 1.69) from his ads, t = 3.45, df = 762,p = .001.

    This learning pattern is also clear when comparing the learning about each can-

    didate. Thus, Kerrys issue learning score (3.49) was significantly higher than Bushs

    issue score (3.27), t = 4.24, df = 762,p = .001. Likewise, Bushs personal quali-

    ties learning score (3.95) is higher than the personal image score for Kerry (3.27),t = 9.67, df = 762,p = .001.

    This superiority for personal qualities learning from the Bush ads is also appar-

    ent when comparing the actual number of personal qualities that respondents listed

    in an open-ended question that asked them to list specific personal qualities they

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1143

    Table 1

    Effect of Advertising Exposure on Image Evaluations of Candidates (N= 764)

    Bush Kerry

    Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest

    Overall sample 53.9 53.6 53.7 53.3

    Males (n = 336) 54.4 54.3 52.6 52.5

    Females (n = 428) 53.6 53.2 54.4 53.9

    Democrats (n = 311) 44.55 44.50 60.68 61.43

    Republicans (n = 267) 66.80 66.21 44.64 43.05a

    Independent/Other (n = 186) 51.29 51.07 54.76 53.99

    a. ttest indicates difference between pretest and posttest ratings is significant atp < .05.

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    9/16

    recalled about each candidate. Table 2 shows that respondents recalled a mean

    number of 1.47 items for Bush but only 1.25 for Kerry, t = 3.51, df = 762,p = .001.

    Of these personal qualities, Bush also received significantly more positive mentions

    (M = 0.76, SD = 2.38) than did Kerry (M = 0.52, SD = 2.10), t = 2.93, df = 762,

    p = .003. There was no significant difference in the number of negative items

    recalled about each candidate.

    Agenda-Setting Effects of Ad Exposure

    The fourth hypothesis predicted that exposure to the candidates television ads

    would have an agenda-setting effect, resulting in a change in the issues that young

    citizens judged important. This hypothesis was not confirmed for the overall sample

    in this study. As Table 3 shows, young citizens found the economy, the war in Iraq,

    health care, education, terrorism, taxes, foreign policy, and the environment to be the

    most important issues (in that order) in the pretest. The posttest agenda of issues was

    highly correlated (Spearmans rank order correlation = +.91).

    However, it is interesting to note that exposure to the ads did have an effect from a

    gender comparison standpoint. As Table 3 shows, men and women did not have sig-

    nificantly correlated issue agendas before viewing the ads (rs = .64), viewing the ads

    brought women and men into alignment on the issues they judged most important, and

    their posttest agendas were, like the overall sample, significantly correlated (rs = .82).

    A major aspect of this agenda realignment was the fact that after exposure to the

    ads, women had an elevated concern about the importance of the economy, which

    remained at the top spot in both the pretest and posttest issue agendas for men.

    Advertising Exposure and Political Cynicism

    The fifth hypothesis posited that exposure to political advertising would not affect

    the levels of cynicism in young citizens. This hypothesis was confirmed. On the

    1144 American Behavioral Scientist

    Table 2

    Learning and Recall of Issues and Image Characteristics From Ads (N = 764)

    Bush Kerry

    Learn about issues 3.27 3.49a

    Learn personal qualities 3.95b 3.26a,b

    Number of image items recalled 1.47 1.25a

    Number of positive items 0.76 0.52a

    Number of negative items 0.50 0.52

    a. ttest shows difference between Bush and Kerry is significant atp < .01.

    b. ttest shows difference between issues and personal qualities is significant at p < .001.

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    10/16

    composite cynicism scale, respondents scored at almost identical levels on the

    pretest and the posttest. Young women appear to be more cynical than young men,

    but exposure to the ads did not affect their cynicism levels or the differential level

    between the genders.

    Ad Exposure and Information Efficacy

    The last hypothesis suggested that exposure to the candidates ads would result inincreased feelings of information efficacy for the young citizens. This hypothesis

    was measured by comparing the pretest and posttest responses to respondents per-

    ceptions of their confidence in their understanding of politics and their information

    about the campaign. Table 4 shows that exposure to the television ads does, indeed,

    increase young respondents feelings of information efficacy, confirming this hypoth-

    esis. On the pretest information efficacy scale, the mean score was 13.83 (SD = 4.15),

    which increased significantly to a mean of 14.46 (SD = 3.73) in the posttest, t =

    8.60, df = 762,p = .001.

    The related research question queried whether men had a higher level of infor-mation efficacy after viewing the ads than before. As Table 4 indicates, men do expe-

    rience higher levels of political information efficacy after ad viewing (M = 15.03,

    SD = 3.74) than do women (M = 14.06, SD = 3.68), t = 3.54, df = 762,p = .001.

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1145

    Table 3

    Agenda-Setting Effects of Exposure to Political Ads

    Total Females Males

    Before After Before After Before After

    Rank order of issues

    Economy 1 2 4 2 1 1

    Iraq War 2 3 2 3 2 2

    Health 3 1 1 1 5 3

    Education 4 5 3 4 3 5

    Terrorism 5 4 2 6 3 4

    Taxes 6 6 6 4 7 6

    Foreign policy 7 7 7 7 6 7Environment 8 8 8 8 8 8

    Spearman correlations:

    .91* .87* .87*

    Before After Before After Before After

    .64

    Before Before

    .82*

    After After

    *indicates that the Spearmans rho is significant atp < .05.

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    11/16

    However, whereas men and women do have different levels of information efficacy,

    it is also important to note that exposure to the television ads significantly increases

    the information efficacy levels for both men and women.

    Discussion

    With the dominance of television as a political news source, image can mean every-

    thing. Political advertising is just one way candidates can shape their images. Past

    research on political ads has shown that candidate image ratings can improve after

    viewing ads (Kaid, 2002). However, positive evaluations of Bush and Kerry did not

    increase after watching the ads in this study, and gender did not influence candidate

    evaluations. One possible explanation is that a few days before Election Day, when the

    experiment was performed, most voters had made their voting choices, they knew what

    qualities they preferred in a candidate, and they were unlikely to be influenced by ads.

    With regard to gender, women did not evaluate the candidates any more favorably

    than men overall. However, there were some differences between the effects of the ads

    on men and women in regard to some of the specific scales used to measure candi-

    date image evaluations. For instance, men found Bush significantly more honest

    (M = 4.32, SD = 2.09 on the posttest compared toM = 4.08, SD = 2.37 on the pretest),

    t = 3.21, df = 336, p = .001. Exposure to the ads also resulted in mens finding

    Kerry significantly less sincere in the posttest (M = 4.32, SD = 1.85) than in the

    pretest (M = 4.46, SD = 1.66), t = 1.73, df = 333, p = .05. On the other hand,

    women found both Bush and Kerry less qualified after viewing than before.

    However, women found Bush significantly more aggressive after viewing (M =

    5.30, SD = 1.46) than before (M = 5.06, SD = 1.59), t = 3.72, df = 426,p = .001.

    1146 American Behavioral Scientist

    Table 4

    Effects of Advertising Exposure on Information

    Efficacy and Cynicism (N=

    764)Pretest Posttest

    Information efficacy

    Overall 13.83 14.46b

    Males 14.45a 15.03b

    Females 13.33a 14.04b

    Cynicism level

    Overall 25.6 25.6

    Males 25.1 24.8

    Females 25.9a 26.2a

    a. ttest shows difference between males and females is significant at p < .001.

    b. ttest shows difference between pretest and posttest is significant at p < .03.

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    12/16

    In addition to finding Kerry less qualified, women also found him less sophisticated

    (t = 4.12, df = 426,p = .001) and less friendly (t = 2.60, df = 426,p = .01) after

    seeing the spots.One demographic variable that yielded differences in candidate image evalua-

    tions was political party affiliation. Among Democrats, Kerrys image rating increased

    after viewing the ads; conversely, among Republicans, Kerrys image rating

    decreased; and there were no changes for voters with other or no affiliation. Bushs

    ads elicited no differences across parties. It is possible that participants acknowl-

    edged that the incumbents image was solidified, yet the challengers image was

    more flexible in either a positive or negative direction. Exposure to Kerrys ads

    inspired Democrats to evaluate him more favorably and encouraged Republicans to

    dislike him more.Considering the mixed results on image and issue learning, the candidate ads

    seemed to have a greater influence in this area. Participants learned more about per-

    sonal qualities than issues from the Bush ads and more about issues than personal

    qualities from the Kerry ads. Also, Kerrys issue learning score was significantly

    higher than Bushs issue learning score and vice versa for image learning scores. Yet

    more evidence for this finding is the number of positive comments about the candi-

    dates personal qualities: Bush had significantly more positive mentions than did

    Kerry. This finding on image and issue learning could relate to the fact that Kerry

    was often described as a policy wonk who knew the issues but was somewhat bor-ing, whereas Bush was described as a laid-back, regular guy from Texas who knew

    how to get things done but was rather unsophisticated. Bush had more to gain if he

    ran on his image and personal qualities than if he emphasized the Iraq war or the

    economy. Kerry had more to gain if he could use his thorough knowledge of policy

    than if he emphasized his personality. Thus, the candidate advertising was appropri-

    ate to each candidates strong points, and it seems participants reactions reflected

    these candidate strategies.

    There were few differences in the learning or recall scores of the candidates based

    on gender. However, female respondents said they learned significantly more aboutthe personal qualities of George Bush (M = 4.07, SD = 1.89) from the ads than did

    males (M = 3.78, SD = 1.82), t = 2.07, df = 762,p = .04.

    Another aspect of issue influence from political ads is their potential agenda-

    setting effect. For these young voters, exposure to ads did not have a significant agenda-

    setting effect. The issues ranked before and after ad exposure were similar. Again, it

    is important to keep in mind the experiment was conducted only days before the

    election. The campaigns were coming to a close, and most voters could probably

    name the most discussed issues of the campaign. When asked to list the five most

    important issues facing the nation, it is possible many participants relied on their rec-ollection of the most talked-about issues. This could be why exposure to candidate

    advertising did not significantly change their opinionsthe candidatesmessage and

    agenda and/or the overall media message and agenda had already been absorbed. It

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1147

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    13/16

    is interesting that womens agendas were more influenced by ad exposure. Before ad

    exposure, women and men differed significantly on their agendas. For example, the

    economy was ranked the number one issue by men and the fourth issue by women.However, after ad exposure, whereas men kept the economy as their number one

    issue, the issue jumped to number two on the womens ranking. This could reflect

    the lower levels of political information efficacy for women, because it appears

    women are more easily influenced by the ads issue agendas.

    Critics of political advertising claim that ads hurt democracy because voters do

    not gain any valuable information from ads and the ads only make voters more cyn-

    ical of the election process. This study shows otherwise: Young voters feelings of

    political information efficacy significantly increased, and general political cynicism

    levels did not significantly change. It could be that young voters have become soaccustomed to political advertising that they accept it as a legitimate source of infor-

    mation. Young voters seem satisfied with ads and feel comfortable using the infor-

    mation in ads for decision making. Additionally, both men and women exhibit higher

    levels in political information efficacy after viewing the ads. However, women

    showed somewhat lower levels of political information efficacy than men. On a sim-

    ilar note, women seem more cynical than men. A possible reason for this is the tra-

    ditional masculine qualities of politics and the recent emergence of women in

    politics. There are fewer female role models in the political world, and women polit-

    ical reporters are not as abundant as men. Without a reflection of themselves in thepolitical sphere, women may feel more alienated from politics and become more

    cynical and less confident in their political information efficacy.

    Notes

    1. The 13 universities and the number of participants in each location were as follows: University of

    Florida (n = 113), University of Missouri (n = 88), Virginia Tech (n = 47), Iowa State (n = 123), University

    of Kansas (n = 80), University of Colorado, Denver (n = 35), University of Akron (n = 18), St. Cloud State

    University (n = 27), Texas A&M Commerce (n = 30), University of Oklahoma (n = 23), University of

    New Haven (n = 31), Consumes River College (n = 16), and University of Texas at San Antonio (n = 133).

    2. The ads were obtained from theNational Journals campaign ad archive and were retrieved October 22,

    2004, from http://www.nationaljournal.com.

    3. The 12 bipolar adjective pairs used were qualifiedunqualified, sophisticatedunsophisticated, honest

    dishonest, believableunbelievable, successfulunsuccessful, attractiveunattractive, friendlyunfriendly,

    sincereinsincere, calmexcitable, aggressiveunaggressive, strongweak, activeinactive.

    4. The eight-item cynicism index was composed of the following measures: (a) Whether I vote or not

    has no influence on what politicians do, (b) One never knows what politicians really think, (c) People like

    me dont have any say about what the government does, (d) Sometimes politics and government seem so

    complicated that a person like me cant really understand whats going on, (e) One can be confident that

    politicians will always do the right thing, (f) Politicians often quickly forget their election promises after

    a political campaign is over, (g) Politicians are more interested in power than in what the people think,

    and (h) One cannot always trust what politicians say.

    5. The four-item information efficacy index was composed of the following measures: (a) I consider

    myself well qualified to participate in politics, (b) I think that I am better informed about politics and

    1148 American Behavioral Scientist

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    14/16

    government than most people, (c) I feel that I have a pretty good understanding of the important political

    issues facing our country, and (d) If a friend asked me about the presidential election, I feel I would have

    enough information to help my friend figure out who to vote for.

    References

    Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How political advertisements shrink and polar-

    ize the electorate. New York: Free Press.

    Atkin, C. K., Bowen, L., Nayman, O. B., & Sheinkopf, K. G. (1973). Quality versus quantity in televised

    political ads. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 209-224.

    Atkin, C. K., & Heald, G. (1976). Effects of political advertising. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40, 216-228.

    Basil, M., Schooler, C., & Reeves, B. (1991). Positive and negative political advertising: Effectiveness

    of ads and perceptions of candidates. In F. Biocca (Ed.), Television and political advertising (Vol. 1,pp. 245-262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Brians, C. L., & Wattenberg, M. P. (1996). Campaign issue knowledge and salience: Comparing reception

    from TV commercials, TV news, and newspapers.American Journal of Political Science, 40, 172-193.

    Bystrom, D. G., Banwart, M. C., Kaid, L. L., & Robertson, T. (2004). Gender and candidate communi-

    cation: Videostyle, webstyle, newstyle. New York: Routledge.

    Cappella, J. N., & Jamieson, K. H. (1997). Spiral of cynicism: The press and the public good. New York:

    Oxford University Press.

    Christ, W. G., & Thorson, E. (1994). Do attitudes toward political advertising affect information process-

    ing of televised political commercials?Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 38(3), 251-271.

    Cundy, D. T. (1986). Political commercials and candidate image: The effects can be substantial. In L. L.

    Kaid, D. Nimmo, & K. R. Sanders (Eds.),New perspectives on political advertising (pp. 210-234).

    Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Declare Yourself. (2003).Major national survey shows increased promise, new strategies for increasing

    youth vote. Youth Vote 2004 survey shows narrow window of opportunity to engage young voters;

    points to political incompetence as major barrier to voting. Retrieved on June 20, 2005, from http://

    www.declareyourself.org/press/pressroom.htm

    Delli Carpini, M. X. (2000). Gen.com: Youth, civic engagement, and the new information environment.

    Political Communication, 17, 341-349.

    Devlin, L. P. (2005). Contrasts in presidential campaign commercials of 2004. American Behavioral

    Scientist, 49(2), 279-313.

    Faber, R. J., & Storey, M. C. (1984). Recall of information from political advertising. Journal ofAdvertising, 13, 39-44.

    Federal Election Commission. (2004). Data drawn from Congressional Research Service reports,

    Election Data Services Inc., and state election offices. Retrieved January 6, 2006, from http://www

    .infoplease.com/ipa/A0781453.html

    Garramone, G. M. (1984). Voter responses to negative political ads.Journalism Quarterly, 61, 250-259.

    Garramone, G. M., Atkin, C. K., Pinkleton, B. E., & Cole, R. T. (1990). Effects of negative advertising

    on the political process.Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 34, 299-311.

    Groenendyk, E. W., & Valentino, N. A. (2002). Of dark clouds and silver linings: Effects of exposure to issue

    versus candidate advertising on persuasion, information retention, and issue salience. Communication

    Research, 29(3), 295-319.

    Herrnson, P. S., & Patterson, K. D. (2000). Agenda-setting and campaign advertising in congressionalelections. In J. A. Thurber, C. J. Nelson, & D. A. Dulio (Eds.), Crowded airways: Campaign adver-

    tising in elections (pp. 96-112). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

    Holbert, R. L., Benoit, W. L., Hansen, G. J., & Wen, W.-C. (2002). The role of communication in the for-

    mation of an issue-based citizenry. Communication Monographs, 69, 296-310.

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1149

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    15/16

    Johnson-Cartee, K. S., & Copeland, G. A. (1989). Southern voters reaction to negative political ads in

    1986.Journalism Quarterly, 66(4), 888-893.

    Just, M., Crigler, A., & Wallach, L. (1990). Thirty seconds or thirty minutes: What viewers learn from

    spot advertisements and candidate debates.Journal of Communication, 40, 120-133.Kahn, K. F., & Geer, J. G. (1994). Creating impressions: An experimental investigation of political adver-

    tising on television. Political Behavior, 16(1), 93-116

    Kahn, K. F., & Kenney, P. J. (2000). How negative campaigning enhances knowledge of Senate elections.

    In J. A. Thurber, C. J. Nelson, & D. A. Dulio (Eds.), Crowded airwaves: Campaign advertising in

    elections (pp. 65-95). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

    Kaid, L. L. (1994). Political advertising in the 1992 campaign. In R. E. Denton Jr. (Ed.), The 1992 pres-

    idential campaign (pp. 111-127). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Kaid, L. L. (1998). Videostyle and the effects of the 1996 presidential campaign advertising. In

    R. E. Denton Jr. (Ed.), The 1996 presidential campaign: A communication perspective (pp. 143-159).

    Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Kaid, L. L. (2001). Technodistortions and effects of the 2000 political advertising.American Behavioral

    Scientist, 44(12), 2370-2378.

    Kaid, L. L. (2002). Political advertising and information seeking: Comparing exposure via traditional and

    Internet channels.Journal of Advertising, 31(1), 27-36.

    Kaid, L. L. (2003). Effects of political information in the 2000 presidential campaign: Comparing tradi-

    tional television and Internet exposure.American Behavioral Scientist, 46(5), 677-689.

    Kaid, L. L. (2004). Measuring candidate images with semantic differentials. In K. L. Hacker (Ed.),

    Presidential candidate images (pp. 231-236). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Kaid, L. L., Chanslor, M., & Hovind, M. (1992). The influence of program and commercial type on polit-

    ical advertising effectiveness.Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 36, 303-320.

    Kaid, L. L., & Holtz-Bacha, C. (2000). Gender differences in response to televised political broadcasts:A multicountry comparison.Harvard Journal of International Press/Politics, 5(2), 17-29.

    Kaid, L. L., Landreville, K. D., Postelnicu, M., & Martin, J. D. (2005, May).Enhancing information effi-

    cacy for young voters: The effects of political advertising and debates. Paper presented at the

    International Communication Association Convention, New York.

    Kaid, L. L., McKinney, M. S., & Tedesco, J. C. (2000). Civic dialogue in the 1996 presidential campaign:

    Candidate, media, and public voices. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton.

    Kaid, L. L., McKinney, M. S., & Tedesco, J. C. (2004, November).Information efficacy and young vot-

    ers. Paper presented at the National Communication Association Conference, Chicago.

    Kaid, L. L., & Postelnicu, M. (2005). Political advertising and responses of young voters: Comparison of

    traditional television and Internet messages.American Behavioral Scientist, 49(2), 265-278.

    Kaid, L. L., & Sanders, K. R. (1978). Political television commercials: An experimental study of the typeand length. Communication Research, 5, 57-70.

    Kaid, L. L., & Tedesco, J. C. (1999). Presidential candidate presentation: Videostyle in the 1996 presi-

    dential spots. In L. L. Kaid & D. G. Bystrom (Eds.), The electronic election: Perspectives on the 1996

    campaign communication (pp. 209-221). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Lang, A. (1991). Emotion, formal features, and memory for televised political advertisements. In F. Biocca

    (Ed.), Television and political advertising (Vol. 1, pp. 221-243). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Lemert, J. B., Wanta, W., & Lee, T. (1999). Party identification and negative advertising in a U.S. Senate

    election.Journal of Communication, 49, 123-134.

    Martinelli, K. A., & Chaffee, S. H. (1995). Measuring new-voter learning via three channels of political

    information.Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 72, 18-32.

    McCombs, M., Lopez-Escobar, E., & Llamas, J. P. (2000). Setting the agenda of attributes in the 1996Spanish general election.Journal of Communication, 50(2), 77-92.

    Murphy, K. (2000, April 25). Report: Young voters, candidates share blame for apathy. Kansas City Star.

    Retrieved January 10, 2004, from http://www.kcstar.com

    1150 American Behavioral Scientist

    by Roberto Hernandez Sampieri on October 16, 2008http://abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 Mercadotecnia the Effects of Political Advertising on Young Voters

    16/16

    National Association of Secretaries of State. (1999). New millennium survey: American youth attitudes

    on politics, citizenship, government, and voting. Retrieved on October 11, 2005, from http://www

    .stateofthevote.org/survey/

    Newhagen, J. E., & Reeves, B. (1991). Emotion and memory responses for negative political advertising:A study of television commercials used in the 1988 presidential election. In F. Biocca (Ed.), Television

    and political advertising (Vol. 1, pp. 197-220). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Patterson, T. E., & McClure, R. D. (1976). The unseeing eye: Myth of television power in politics.

    New York: Putnam.

    Perloff, R. M., & Kinsey, D. (1992). Political advertising as seen by consultants and journalists.Journal

    of Advertising Research, 36, 53-60.

    Pinkleton, B. E., Um, N.-H., & Weintraub Austin, E. (2002). An exploration of the effects of negative

    advertising on political decision making.Journal of Advertising, 31(1), 13-25.

    Putnam, R. D. (1995). Turning in, turning out: The strange disappearance of social capital in America.

    PS: Political Science and Politics, 28, 664-683.

    Rahn, W. M., & Hirshorn, R. M. (1999). Political advertising and public mood: A study of childrens

    political orientations. Political Communication, 16, 387-407.

    Roberts, M. (1992). Predicting voter behavior via the agenda-setting tradition. Journalism Quarterly,

    69(4), 878-892.

    Rosenstone, S. J., Kinder, D. R., Miller, W. E., & the National Election Studies. (1997).American national

    election study 1996: Pre- and post-election survey [Computer file]. Ann Arbor: University of

    Michigan, Center for Political Studies (Producer) and Inter-university Consortium for Political and

    Social Research (Distributor).

    Rothschild, M. L., & Ray, M. L. (1974, July). Involvement and political advertising effect: An exploratory

    experiment. Communication Research, 1, 264-285.

    Tinkham, S. F., & Weaver-Lariscy, R. A. (1993). A diagnostic approach to assessing the impact of nega-tive political television commercials.Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 37(4), 377-400.

    Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & Williams, D. (2004). The impact of political advertising on knowl-

    edge, Internet information seeking, and preference.Journal of Communication, 54(2), 337-354.

    West, D. M. (1993).Air wars: Television advertising in election campaigns, 1952-1992. Washington, DC:

    Congressional Quarterly.

    Yang, S. (2004). Americans spend more energy and time watching TV than on exercise, finds new study.

    UC Berkeley News. Retrieved December 2, 2005, from http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/

    2004/03/10_amtv.shtml

    Zhao, X., & Bleske, G. L. (1995). Measurement effects in comparing voter learning from television news

    and campaign advertisements.Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 72(1), 72-83.

    Lynda Lee Kaid (PhD, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale) is a professor of telecommunication

    at the University of Florida.

    Monica Postelnicu (PhD, University of Florida) is an assistant professor in the Manship School of Mass

    Communication at Louisiana State University.

    Kristen Landreville was a graduate student in the College of Journalism and Communications at the time

    this study was conducted.

    Hyun Jung Yun is a graduate student in the College of Journalism and Communications and theDepartment of Political Science at the University of Florida.

    Abby Gail LeGrange is a graduate student in the College of Journalism and Communications at the

    University of Florida.

    Kaid et al. / Advertising Effects on Young Voters 1151

    http://abs.sagepub.com/