mibos in the field

Upload: qwertyzaz1989

Post on 07-Jul-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    1/10

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    1991. Vol, 76 , No, 5 ,619 -627

    Copy righi 1991 by Lhe Ame rican Psychological Asso cialion, Inc.

    0021-9010/91/13 .00

    Construction and Validation of an Instrument for Measuring

    Ingratiatory Behaviors in Organizational Settings

    Kamalesh Kumar

    Department of Market ing and Management

    Arkansas State University

    Michael Beyerlein

    University of North Texas

    A measure ofthe frequency of employees' use of ingratiatory behavior at work was tested with a

    sample of employees { 716) working in a wide variety of organizations and jobs. Pilot testing

    reduced a 65-item poo to a 24-item instru men t with four factors: Othe r Enha ncem ent. Opinion

    Conformity, Self-Presentation, and Favor-Rendering. Internal consistency reliabihty

     was

     .92; test-

    retest reliability over one month

     was

     .73, Evidence for content, convergent, and discriminant valid-

    ity was substantial. The Measure of Ingratiatory Behaviors in Organizational Settings (MIBOS)

    should enable researchers to focus on the empirical study of ingratiatory behaviors in organ-

    izations.

    Power, influence, and political behavior are ubiquitous in

    formal organizations. The concept of power and influence in

    organizational settings can be broadly referred to as the general-

    ized ability to change the actions of others in some intended

    fashion (Mowday, 1978). Research o n in traorganiza tional influ-

    ence has focused on both downward influence (the ways in

    which supervisors influence subordinates) and upward influ-

    ence (the ways in which subordinates influence their supervi-

    sors; Tedeschi, Schlenker, & Linkskold, 19 72).

    Subordinates use a number of upward influence strategies to

    obtain personal benefits or satisfy organizational goals (Allen,

    Porter, Renwick, & Mayes, 1979; Kipnis, Schmidt, & Wilkin-

    son, 1980; Mowday, 1978). Such strategies include upward ap-

    peal, assertiveness, blocking, coalition, exchange, rationality,

    support building, and ingratiation (Kipnis et al. , 1980; Schrie-

    sheim & Hin kin , 199 0). In this study, we concen trated solely on

    ingratiation as an upwa rd influence techn ique directed at im-

    med iate superiors. Although ingratiation is just one of the up-

    ward influence strategies used in organizational settings, it is a

    distinct con struct with its own set of causes and consequ ences

    and therefore deserves to be studied separately from other up-

    ward influence strategies (Liden & Mitchell, 1988).

    I n g ra t i a t i o n : A S t r a t e g y o f U p w a rd In f l u e n c e

    The definition of ingratiation that guided this study is based

    on Tedeschi and Melburg's (1984) definition ofthe term. In an

    organizational context, ingratiation refers to a set of assertive

    This study

     is

     based on Kamalesh Kumar's doctoral dissertation. He

    wishes to express

     his

     appreciation for the assistance from his disserta-

    tion committee, especially Warren Watson, Chair, and Mary Thi-

    bodeaux.

    We

     also thank three reviewers for their feedback on earlier versions

    of this article.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ka-

    malesh Kumar. D epartment of Marketing and Management, College

    of Business Administration, Arkansas State University, State Univer-

    sity, A rkansas 72467.

    tactics that are used by organizational members to gain the

    approbation of superiors who control significant rewards for

    them. These rewards are foreseeable and rather imminent. As

    one of a large class of political influence processes that are

    ongoing in orga nizations, ingratiation involves strategic behav-

    iors designed to enhance one's interpersonal attractiveness. In-

    gratiating actions are usually directed tow ard objectives that are

    not m ade explicit by the par ties involved. Thus, although ingra-

    tiators may behave as though the issue at hand were their only

    concern, they may be doing so to enhance their images in the

    target person's eyes or to achieve other personal goals of which

    the target person is unaware (Wortman & Linsenmeier, 1977).

    In studies of upward influence in organizational settings,

    ingratiatory tactics have been among the strategies most com-

    monly used (Allen et al. , 1979; Kipnis et al. , 1980; Madison,

    Allen, Porter, Renwick, & Mayes, 1980; Porter, Allen, & Angle,

    1981). Ingratiation ap pea rs to be used in organizational settings

    for the same reason it is used in general social settings—to

    increase one's attractiveness in the eyes of a more powerful per-

    son (Jones & Wortman, 1973). Enhanced attractiveness may

    improve a subordinate's chances of positive rewards (such as a

    raise, a promotion,

     etc.

    or reduce his or her chances of receiving

    a negative outcome (such as an adverse assessment, a cut in

    pay, etc.).

    Ta c t i c a l V a r i a t i o n s o f I n g ra t i a t i o n

    In the organizational c ontext, ingratiation can tak e all or any

    oft he forms by which interpersonal a ttraction may be solicited.

    In their laboratory experiments, Jones and Wortman   (1973;

    Jones, 1964) demonstrated the use of four major ingratiation

    tactics: other enhancement, opinion conformity, rendering fa-

    vors, and self-presentation. B ecause ingratiatory behaviors pri-

    marily involve power-enhancing or dependence-reducingstrate-

    gies, the use of these tactics ought to be endemic in organiza-

    tional settings, which abound in relationships involving

    differential power. Although overt manifestations of such be-

    haviors may at times be restricted, partially inhibited by legiti-

    macy considerations, and occasionally eschewed because ofthe

    619

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    2/10

    620

    KAMALESH KUMAR AND MICHAEL BEYERLEIN

    risks involved, the tendency toward such behaviors

     is

     neverthe-

    less present in organizations.

    The use of other enhancement as an ingratiation tactic iti-

    volves commun ication of directly enhan cing, evaluative state-

    ments. T he ingratiator find s ways to express a positive evalua-

    tion of

     the

     target person and emphasizes various strengths and

    virtues. While distorting and exaggerating the target person's

    admirable qualities to convey the impression that he or she is

    highly thought of, the ingratiator calls little attention to or to-

    tally ignores negative attributes.

    Another set of techniques used by the ingratiator involves

    expressing opinion s or behaving in a ma nner tha t is consistent

    with the opinions, judgm ents, or behaviors ofthe target person.

    The tactics involved in opinion conformity can range from sim-

    ple agreement with expressed opinions, through more elabo-

    rate attempts at trying to articulate the position presumably

    held by the target person, to extremely complex forms of imita-

    tion and identification {Jones. 1964).

    Favor doing is a logical ingratiation tactic because people

    usually react in a positive manner when someone does some-

    thing nice for them {Jones  Wortman. 197.3). This behavior is

    based on the reciprocity norm— doing a favor for another per-

    son can induce an obligation to reciprocate (Gouldner. I960).

    Favor doing can also help foster an identity as a helpful, friendly,

    and conside rate person (Tedeschi Melburg. 1984).

    Self-presentation as an ingratiation tactic consists of making

    explicit verbal statements of one's own attributes to increase the

    likelihood of being judged attractive by the target person.

     Self

    presentation has two related aspects: (a) providing explicit de-

    scriptions about one's own characteristics and behaviors and {b)

    behaving in ways that imply that o ne possesses certain charac-

    teristics (Jones  Wortman, 1973).

    Ingratiation in Organizational Settings

    In recent years there has been considerable interest in ingra-

    tiatory tactics and consequences, but the topic has received

    little empirical attention from organizationai researchers.

    Much ofthe attention the topic has received is from social psy-

    chologists {e.g.. Baumeister. 1982; Jones, 1964; Jones Wort-

    man. 1973: Riggio Friedman. 1986. Tedeschi.

     1981;

     Tedeschi

      Melburg. 1984). who made no attempt to generalize the fin d-

    ings to organizational settings.

    Study of ingratiation strategies within organizations has been

    extremely sporadic. Porter et al. (1981) recently observed that

    the subject of ingratiation has long been regarded as taboo be-

    cause of its mildly disturbing, negative connotations, and that

    researchers should try to develop a better understand ing ofth e

    subject. However, in spite of occasional ex horta tions like Porter

    et al.'s {Liden  Mitchell.

     1988;

     Ralston, 1985; Tedeschi  Mel-

    burg. 1984). little has been done to improve the situation.

    The study of ingratiation in organizational settings requires

    identification of specific tactics and some m ethod of mea suring

    the frequency with which such tactics are used. In this article,

    we report the development and validation of an instrum ent that

    can be used to gather organizational members' perceptions of

    the use of such tactics. Such an in strume nt should help to bring

    the topic of ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings

    within the realm of empirical research and to create interest in

    a topic that remains under researched.

    Previous Measurement Techniques

    A review of the literature on ingratiatory behavior in organi

    zational settings revealed that no instrument had been devel-

    oped to specifically measure ingratiatory behaviors

     in

     organiz

    tional settings. Previous researchers, most notably those in so

    cial psychology, relied primarily on experimental designs to

    measure this behavior or used items put together in an ad hoc

    mann er {e.g., Pandey Bohra, 1984; Pandey   Rastogi, 1979)

    Examination of the scales used by these researchers yielded

    very few items applicable to the study of ingratiatory behaviors

    in organizational settings.

    The only effort to construc t a scientifically validated scale to

    measure ingratiation in organizational settings was made by

    Kipnis et al. {1980). In the course of their research on intraor-

    ganizational influence tactics, Kipnis et al. identified a large

    number of influence tactics, which they later factor analyzed to

    create a number of subscales. One of the subscales {with six

    items) was labeled

      ingratiation.

      Although the influence-tact

    typology developed by Kipnis et al. has been generally sup-

    ported by other researchers

     {e.g.,

     Ansari Kapoor. 1987; Ere

    Rim , Keider, 1986; Yukl Falbe, 1990), evidence regarding

    the psychometric properties ofthe scale has been lacking. Re-

    cently, Schriesheim and Hinkin {1990) extensively critiqued th

    research of K ipnis et

     al.

     {1980). Schriesheim and Hinkin foun

    that many of Kipnis et al.'s items do not have strong content

    validity and that the factor structure found by Kipnis et al. does

    not hold up particularly well.

    After a number of studies {during which a num ber of items

    were added and deleted), Schriesheim and Hink in (1990) con-

    structed a refined 18-item instrument {six dimensions with 3

    items each). However, by their own ad mission, their study dealt

    with only a very limited subset of psychometric properties that

    must be considered essential in a measuring instrument. Also,

    the subscaie that measured ingratiation contained oniy 3 item

    Clearly, the subscaie did not include the four types of ingratia-

    tion tactics noted by previous researchers. Although Schrie-

    sheim and Hinkin's subscaie may be useful for the study of

    infiuence tactics in general, it is not comprehensive enough for

    the specific study of ingratiatory behaviors in organizational

    settings.

    Instrument Development

    The instrument developed and validated in this study is

    called Measure

     of

     Ingratiatory ehaviors in Organizational

     

    tings

     {MIBOS). The scale was designed to measure the fre

    quency with which ingratiatory tactics are used by subordinate

    in superior-subordinate relationships.

    Previous researchers have noted that the use of influence

    tactics will vary depen ding on the relationship between individ-

    uals (e.g., Falbo Peplau , 1980) and that combining different

    perspectives (superior, peer, subordinate) can substantially alter

    or distort the results relating to the use of influence tactics

    (Schriesheim  Hink in, 1990). Therefore, we deemed it impor

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    3/10

    INGRATIATORY BEHAVIORS

    6

    tant to ground the measure in the context of subordinates' di-

    recting the tactics toward superiors.

    Item Construction

    On the basis of previous theory {Jones, 1964; Jones & Wort-

    man. 1973; Liden & Mitchell, 1988; Ralston. 1985; Tedeschi &

    Melburg, 1984; Wortman & Linsenmeier. 1977) and research

    related to upward influence behaviors in organizations (Kipnis

    et al., 1980; Madison et al., 1980; Mowday. 1978; Porter et al..

    1981; Schilit & Locke, 1982). we generated a pool of ingratia-

    tory behaviors typically shown in organizational settings. Dis-

    cussions with a number of employees, first-level supervisors,

    and middle-level managers working in diverse environments

    contributed further to the pool of items.

    Sixty-five items were generated for the initial pool. Of these

    65 items. 17 described opinion conformity, 18 were related to

    other enhancement, and 15 items each were descriptions of

    self-presentation and favor-render ing behaviors. These items

    were analyzed

     by

     judges with different expertise (industrial psy-

    chology, organizational behavior, strategic management, and

    organizational communication). Each judge was an academic

    with a doctoral degree and substantial industry and manage-

    ment consulting experience. The judges reviewed the items for

    clarity, appropriateness, and content validity There was a gen-

    eral consensus among the judges about the items included in

    the initial poo of items.

    All ofthe items in the

     poo

    were also examined by a group of

    employees and managers working in different environments.

    On the basis ofthe reviews ofthe experts and the employees,

    items that appeared to be ambiguous or subject to response bias

    were either rewritten or omitted. This screening process re-

    sulted in the elimination oflO items, leaving a pool of 55 items

    in the initial test instrument.

    Preiesting

    The instrument with 55 items was first administered to a

    sample of management students  N = 78) who were full-time or

    part-time employees attending evening classes. The subjects

    were employed in a wide variety of organizations (retail, manu-

    facturing, wholesale, service, government, etc.) and jobs (man-

    ual,

     clerical, first-line supervisor, middle-level manager, etc.).

    The instrument required the subjects to indicate the extent to

    which they actually used the behaviors described by the items

    to influence their supervisors. Subjects were specifically cau-

    tioned not to make any judgment about the desirability or un-

    desirability of the behaviors described and to merely report the

    frequency with which they showed each of these behaviors

    when dealing with their supervisors. Responses were recorded

    on a 5-point Likert-type scale with the following anchors: never

    do

     it  1),

     seldom do

     il (2),

     occasionally do

     it (3), often do it  4), and

    almost always do it  (5). Because subjects were to report the

    frequency of actual behaviors, high scores should indicate more

    use of ingratiatory tactics.

    Item Selection

    One ofthe objectives at this stage of scale construction was to

    select those items that provided the most accurate and appro-

    priate description of the behavior under investigation. Items

    with higher item-total correlations were retained. To minimize

    skewness and maximize variance, items with both larger means

    (around 3.0) and larger variances were retained. These proce-

    dures resulted in the elimination of 28 itetns, leaving the instru-

    ment with 27 items. The item-total correlations of these 27

    items ranged from .38 to

     .69.

     The scale was further reviewed for

    clarity, and where necessary, minor changes in wording were

    made.

    Phase I: Item Selectioti

    The scale with 27 items was administered to a new sample of

    business students (A'= 148) who were employed either full time

    or part time. Once again, the sample represented a wide variety

    of jobs and organizations. Subjects were simultaneously admin-

    istered the short version of Crown and Marlowe's Social Desir-

    ability Scale (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972) to check if they were

    responding to the various items on the scale in a socially desir-

    able manner.

    Item Review

    Two criteria were used in item selection at this stage: item-to-

    tal correlations and lack of significant correlation with the So-

    cial Desirability Scale. On the basis of these criteria, 3 more

    items were dropped from the scale, leaving a total of 24 items.

    The item-total correlations for the remaining 24 items ranged

    from .45 to .66. The total score on the ingratiation scale did not

    correlate significantly with the total score on the Social Desir-

    ability Scale  r = .02). Correlations between each ingratiation

    item and the total score on the Social Desirability Scale ranged

    from .00 to .09 and were not statistically significant.

    Of the final 24 items, 7 each represented the categories of

    opinion conformity and other enhancement, 6 items repre-

    sented the favor-render ing category, and 4 items represented the

    self-presentation category The median score on the scale was

    67.

     the mean was 66.24, and the standard deviation was 14.65.

    The means, standard deviations, and item-total correlations

    for MIBOS are presented in Table 1.

    Factor nalysis

    Scores obtained on MIBOS were factor analyzed with the

    principal components method and oblique factor rotation. Be-

    cause the purpose of factor analysis at this stage was to obtain

    theoretically meaningful dimensions, oblique factor rotation

    was considered more desirable than orthogonal rotation (Hair,

    Anderson, & Tatham, 1987). Nineteen ofthe 24 items loaded

    on two factors (eigenvalues 7,6 and 1.4), which accounted for

    over 37% of the variance (A^ = 148). Five items loaded rather

    weakly on more than one factor. Thus, the results ofthe factor

    analysis at this stage were somewhat inconclusive.

    Subsequently, a second factor analysis was conducted with

    data from another and much larger group ofsubjects (Â   346).

    Results of this second factor analysis yielded a four-factor solu-

    tion, generally interpretable in terms of the four ingratiation

    ditnensions that were identified from theory and past research.

    These four factors accounted for

     56.1

    ofthe variance. How-

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    4/10

    622

    KAM ALES H KUM AR

      A N D

      MICHAEL BEYERLEIN

    Table

      I

    Means. Standard Deviations, and Item-Total Correlatiom for 24-Item Measure

    of  Ingratiating Behavior  in  Organtzational Settings MIBOS)

    Hem

    M S

    Item-

    total

    r

     t m

    M

    SD

    I tem

    total

    r

    1

    Impress upon your supervisor that

    only he/she can help you in a given

    situation mainly t o make him/her

    feel good about himself/herself.

    2

    Show him/her that  you share his/

    her enthusiasm about his/her

     new

    idea even when you may not

    actually like

     i t.

    3 Try to let  him/her know that yo u

    have a  reputation  for being hked.

    4 Try to  make sure that he/she is

    aware o f your successes.

    5

    Highlight

      th e

      achievements made

    under his/her leadership  in a

    meeting  not being attended  by

    him/her.

    6. Give frequent smiles  to express

    enthusiasm/interest about

    something he/she  is  interested  in

    even

      if

     yo u

     do not

      like

     it .

    7

    Express work attitudes that ar e

    similar  to  your supervisor's as a

    way o f  letting him/her know that

    th e

     two of

     yo u

     a re

     alike.

    8. Tell him/her that  you can  learn a

    lot from his/her experience.

    9. Exaggerate his/her admirable

    qualities  to convey  th e  impression

    that  you think highly o f  him/her.

    1 0

    Disagree on trivial o r unimportant

    issues bu t agree o n  those issues

    in which he/she expects support

    from

      you.

    1 1 Try to  imitate such work behaviors

    of your supervisor

      a s

      working late

    or occasionally working on

    weekends.

    1 2

    Look

      for

      opportunities

      to let the

    supervisor know your virtues/

    strengths.

    2.89

    3.34

    3.51

    2.77

    0.89

    0.88

    1.23

    1.13

    .55

    .4 5

    .4 5

    .51

    3.25

      1.13

    3.10  1.03

    3,01  .1O

    3.11  1.20

    3.57  1.02

    3.44  0.93

    3.34  1.09

    2.58  1.04

    .58

    .57

    .64

     63

    .65

    .52

    .52

    .56

    1.06

    1,04

    1.08

    .5 6

    .6 0

    .6 5

    1 3

    Ask your supervisor  for advice  in

    areas in which he/she thinks he/she

    is smart  to let him /her fee that

    you admire his/her talent.  3.11 1.06 .66

    1 4

    Try to do   things  for your

    supervisor that show your selfless

    generosity.   3.16

    1 5

    Look

      out for

      opportunities

      to

    admire your supervisor.  3.36

    1 6

    Let  your supervisor know  th e

    attitudes y ou share with him/her.  2.80

    1 7

    Compliment your supervisor on

    his/her achievement, however

    trivial  it may actually  be to you

    personally.

      3.22 1.07 .52

    1 8

    Laugh heartily

     at

     your supervisor's

    jokes even when they are not really

    funny  3.68

    1 9

    Go out of your way to run an

    errand  for your supervisor.  3.07

    2 0

    Offer  to help your supervisor  by

    using your personal contacts.  3.23

    2 1

    Try to  persuasively present your

    own qualities when attempting

      to

    convince your supervisor about

    your abilities.  2.95 1.09 .59

    2 2

    Volunteer to be of help  to your

    supervisor

      in

      matters like locating

    a good apartment, finding a good

    insurance agent, etc.  3.47 1.22 .53

    2 3 Spend time listening  to  your

    supervisor's personal problems

    even

      if

      you have

      no

      interest

      in

    them.  3.11 1.23 .45

    2 4

    Volunteer

      to

      help your supervisor

    in his/her work even  if it means

    extra work for you.  2.85 1.07 .63

    Tot al 66 24 14 65

    1.02

    I . I 4

    1.18

    .51

    .54

    .60

    N o t e .

      N-

      1 4 8 S c o r e s w e r e n e a r l y n o r m a l l y d i s t r i b u t e d R e s p o n s e s r a n g e d

      ko m

      n e v e r  do

     it

     {\

    to

     a l m o s t a l w a y s

      do it

      { 5 .

    ever, four ofth e items continued to load (.30 or above on more

    than one factor. Also, three of the items did not load oti the

    specific ingratiation dimension that they were intended to as-

    sess.

    Table 2 lists the items intended to assess each of the four

    ingratiation dimension s (e.g., other enh ancement, opinio n con -

    formity, self-presentation, and favor rendering). Also presented

    are the loadings o fthe items, eigenvalues ofthe four factors, and

    the percentage of variance explained by each factor.

    Analysis of interfactor relationships revealed that these fac-

    tors were m oderately to highly correlated (between .22 and .65)

    with each other. This find ing is somewhat in line with th e re-

    sults of p revious researchers who have investigated the use of

    ingratiation in laboratory settings. For example, both Jones

    (1964) and Jones and Wortman (1973) found strong relation-

    ships between different ingratiation tactics. In experimental re-

    search in which subjects were asked to respond to items de-

    scribing various types of ingratiation tactics. Pandey (1981)

    noted that subjects did not discriminate much between differ-

    ent tactics—they either behaved or did not behave in an ingra-

    tiating manner.

    Reliability

    Two

     appro aches w ere adopted for determ ining the reliability

    oft he instrum ent: internal consistency (consistency of individ-

    ual items with each other) and test-retest reliability (the stabil-

    ity of scores over time). The internal consistency of M IBOS was

    .92 (Cronbach's alpha; N   =   148). This high alph a level sugg ests

    that subjects responded to the individual items in a consistent

    manner throughout the test. The split-half reliability test, a

    method th at estimates the consistency of responses through the

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    5/10

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    6/10

    624

    KAMALESH KUMAR AND MICHAEL BEYERLEIN

    escence

     is

     better Interpreted

     in

     terms

     of

     a subject's reaction

     to

    the content ofthe items and that content-independent response

    styles are not a major contaminant of questionnaire responses

    (e.g., Rorer. 1965). However, it was  still deemed important to

    determine whether the scores on M IBOS were aiFected to any

    significant extent

     by

     acquiescence response

     set.

    To determine

      the

      influence

      of

     acquiescence,

      we

     correlated

    scores  on  MIBOS with scores  on the  Acquiescence Scale

    (Couch & Keniston, I960). The  Acquiescence Scale measures

    the general tendency  to agree or disagree with questionnaire

    items, regardless of  their content. Scores from the two scales

    (A'

     =

     51) correlated  .11.  thereby confirming that the manner in

    which

     the

     MIBOS items were worded

     had

     no significant etfect

    on the responses given.

    Phase 2: Cotitent and  Convergent Validities

    MIBOS  was administered  to  various types  of  employees

    working in different work environments. The first sample con-

    sisted of 353 graduate business administration and senior un-

    dergraduate students. All students were employed full-time or

    part-time and had at  least one year of  full-time work experi-

    ence. Seven students

     were

     dropped from

     the analysis

     because

     of

    missing data. Fifty-nine percent ofthe respondents were

     men,

    and 41 were women. Over 80% of the respondents had three

    years or more of full-time work experience. The majority ofth e

    respondents (82%) were  in  either nonmanagerial  or  first-line

    supervisory positions; 17% were in  middle-level management

    positions; only 1% were in  top-management positions. Such a

    sample meant that m ore respondents had supervisors than had

    subordinates. This

     was

     particularly suitable

     for our

     study

     be-

    cause MIBOS specifically measures ingratiatory behaviors ex-

    hibited

     by

     subordinates

     in

      superior-subordinate relationships.

    The second sample consisted  of 52 employees working in

    home electronics  and  household appliances manufacturing

    companies in the mid-South. AH the employees in this sample

    were

     in

     either nonmanageria

    or

     first-

     o

     middle-level positions .

    They w ere working on a variety of technical and administrative

    jobs.

    Content Validity

    A  scale  has  content validity  if the  substance of the  items

    included

     in the

     instrument

     tap the

     construct

     of

     interest

     to be

    measured and if the items are representative ofthe content area.

    We selected items for M IBOS after an extensive search of both

    the theoretical

     and

      empirical research literature.

     All

      possible

    tactical variations of  ingratiatory behaviors were noted, and

    each was given a fair representation in the scale. The contents of

    the scale were also examined by employees and managers from

    many different types

     of

     organizations

     for the

      appropriateness

    ofthe behavior descriptions. The  individual items on the scale

    were further reviewed

     by

      experts from

      the

     field

     of

      industrial

    psychology, organizational behavior, strategic management,

    and organization the ory As such, the scale can be expected to

    have both face validity and logical content validity

    Convergent Validity

    The classical view of convergent validity suggests that a new

    measure

     of

     a construct like ingratiation ought

     to

     eovary with

    other measures that purport  to  measure  the same construct

    Proceeding  on  this line,  we  compared MIBOS scores with

    scores on the  ingratiation subscaie of Kipnis et al. (1980) and

    the refined three-item version

      of

      this subscaie recently

     con

    structed by Schriesheim and Hin kin (1990).

    Convergent validity also refers to the association of

     the

     mea

    sure being validated with measures

     of

     other theoretically rele

    vant constructs. To test the convergent validity of MIBOS, w

    administered

      it

      simultaneously with

      the

      following measures

    Work Locus of Control Scale {Spector, 1988), Mach IV Scale

    (Christie & Geis, 1970), Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder, 1974)

    and the Need for Power scale from  the Manifest Needs Ques

    tionnaire (Steers

     &

     Braunstein, 1976). Each of

     these

     constructs

    has been identified as a  critical factor in the study of  ingratia

    tory behaviors in o rganizations (Liden

     &

     Mitchell,

     1988;

     Porte

    et al., 1981; R alston, 1985), and as such, scores on MIBOS oug

    to correlate significantly with scores on these scales. The ratio

    nale

     for

     using these scales for convergent validation is discussed

    below. The correlations are presented  in Table 3.

    Self-monitoring skill.

      A

      series

      of

      studies conducted

      b

    Snyder and his colleagues (Snyder, 1974) revealed that individ-

    uals differ

     in the

     extent

     to

     which they are attentive

     and

     respon-

    sive to situational cues as guides to appropriate b ehaviors. Peo

    ple who score high on  self-monitoring seek more information,

    exhibit more accuracy in  diagnosing social situations, and are

    more able to pragmatically tailor their behavior to it he situa

    tion. High self-monitors also seem

     to be

     more adept

     at

     impres-

    sion management (Schlenker & Leary, 1982). Self-monitoring

    skill, therefore,

      is an

      important individual characteristic that

    may help determine a  person's propensity  for  political influ-

    ence strategies like ingratiation. Scores obtained  on  MIBOS

    eorreiated  .46 for the working student sample and .37 for the

    manufacturing employee sample with scores

     on

     the Self-Moni-

    toring Scale.

    Table 3

    Correlations Between Measure of Ingratiating Behavior

    in

     Organizational Settings MIBOS)

     a nd

    Measures of Similar Constructs

    Scale

    Self-Monitoring Scale

    (Snyder.  1974)

    Need for Power

    (Steers & Braunstein. 1976)

    Work Locus of Control

    (Spector,

      1988)

    Mach ]V Scale

    (Christie & Geis, 1970)

    Schriesheim & Hinkin's (1990)

    ingratiation scale

    Kipnis, Schmidt,  Wilkinson's (1980)

    ingratiation subscaie

    Working

    student

    sample'

    .46**

    .40**

    .35**

    .34**

    Manufacturing

    employee

    sample

    .37*

    .46**

    .29*

    .63**

    .57**

    Note.  The manufacturing employees did not complete the Mach IV

    scale, and the working students did not complete either ofthe ingratia-

    tion scales.

      ^ = 3 4 5 . ^ - 5 2 .

    *p< .01 .

      p

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    7/10

    I NGRATI ATORY BEHAVI ORS

    625

    Need for power People with a high need for power attem pt

    to achieve control over their work environment and try to influ-

    ence other people. Such people can be expected to increase

    influence attempts as a way of affecting important outcomes

    (Liden Mitchell, 1988). Therefore, a strong positive correla-

    tion can be expected between MIBOS scores and the need for

    power as measured by the Manifest Needs Questionnaire

    (Steers Braunstein, 1976). M IBO Sand need for power scores

    correlated .40 for the working student sample and .46 for the

    manufacturing employee sample.

    Locus of control.

      Rotter's (1966) theory of the locus of con-

    trol holds that individuals differ in a systematic man ner in tbeir

    beliefs about their personal successes and failures. Individuals

    with an internal locus of control tend to believe that their out-

    comes are the result of the ability and effort that they apply,

    whereas individuals with an external locus of control believe

    that their personal outcomes are the result of factors outside

    their own control or luck. Therefore, individuals with internal

    locus of control ought to be more inclined to try to affect the

    outcomes they receive. In situations in which ingratiation is

    likely (for example, situations involving supervisor-subordinate

    dyads), internals ought to make greater efforts to secure desired

    outcomes and o btain tbeir personal objectives {Ralston, 1985).

    Hence, persons with an internal locus of control can be ex-

    pected to use ingratiatory tactics more often than persons witb

    an external locus of control. Scores on MIBOS correlated .35

    (for the working student samples) and .29 (for the manufactur-

    ing employee sample) with scores on the Work Locus of Control

    scale (Spector, 1988).

    Machiavellianism. People who score high on the Mach IV

    Scale bave been characterized as manipulators of other people.

    High Machiavellians tend to initiate and control the structure

    of interpersonal relations (Christie Geis, 1970). Ralston

    (1985)

     proposed that individuals

     who seek

     to control and m anip-

    ulate others tend to use manipulative tactics, such as ingratia-

    tion, m ore often. T bere is also some experimental sup port for

    this contention (Pandey Rastogi. 1979). Thus, scores on the

    Mach IV Scale can be expected to correlate reasonably highly

    with scores on MIBOS. The correlation was .34.

    Phase

     3:

     Discriminant Validity

    To establish discrimina nt validity, we administered MIBOS

    to multiple and diverse samples. The first sample consisted of

    52 employees (also used for convergent validation) working in

    manufacturing industries in the mid-South.

    The second sample (A'

     =

     216) consisted of a number of sub-

    samples. Of tbe 216 employees in this sample, 57 employees

    were working in government organizations, 88 em ployees were

    in retailing, and 71 employees were in service organizations

    (banking , insurance, real estate,

     etc.).

     These employees w orked

    in a variety of jobs (technical, clerical, administrative, etc.) and

    were simultaneously enrolled in on e of two major universities in

    the South and Southwest.

    Beginning with the landma rk work of K ipnis et al. (1980), a

    number of researchers have examined tbe interpersonal influ-

    ence processes in organ izations (e.g., Ansa ri Kapoor, 1987;

    Erez Rim, 1982; Erez et al., 1986; Schriesbeim Hink in.

    1990). Tbese researchers identified a number of interpersonal

    influence tactics used in organizational settings. Important

    among these are assertiveness. rationality, exchange of benefits,

    upward appeal, blocking, and coalition.

    As interpersonal influence tactics shown by subordinates in

    organizations, ingratiatory behaviors ought to be related to

    other types of interpersonal influence tactics shown by employ-

    ees in organizations. However, if ingratiatory behavior is to be

    explicated successfully and identified as a unique interpersonal

    behavior, it must dem onstrate acceptable

     levels

     of discriminant

    validity wben compared with other interpersonal influence tac-

    tics.

    In tbe past, researchers have used similar methods to estab-

    lish the discriminant validity of new organizational behavior

    constru cts (Mowday

     

    Steers, 1979), Accordingly, to investigate

    the discriminant validity of

     MIBOS,

     we compared it witb five

    other interpersonal influence tactics; assertiveness. rationality,

    exchange of benefits, upward appeal, and coalition. These influ-

    ence tac tics were measured witb the refined and revised version

    of Kipn is et al.'s (1980) scale (Schriesheim Hinkin. 1990).

    Results are p resented in Table 4.

    Several Ii

     nes

     of evidence

     emerge

     from

     these

     results,

     each

     dem-

    onstrating the discriminant validity of MIBOS. All five influ-

    ence tactics used for comparison involve upward influence—

    attempts to influence someone higher in formal authority in

    the organization (Porter et

     al.,

     1981). First, the relationship be-

    tween ingratiation and tbe exchange measure was the highest,

    ranging from .23 to .35 across the four samples. In an upward

    influence situation, ingratiation and exchange are both used to

    gain the approbation ofa superior who controls significant re-

    wards. Because the behaviors are somewhat similar, a positive

    correlation between the two would be expected. However, be-

    cause ingratiation as a construct is distinct from otber upward

    influence tactics, such correlations should be only moderate.

    The extent of correlation between tbe m easures of ingratiation

    and exchange is quite similar to that noted by Yukl and Faibe

    (1990), who measured the use of these two behaviors w ith sin-

    gle-item scales.

    Second, correlations between MIBOS scores and scores on

    tbe assertiveness and upward-influence measures were among

    the lowest, ranging from .08 to .28. When compared with the

    manipulative intent that is often associated with the use of in-

    Table 4

    Discriminant Validity o f Measure of Ingratiating ehavior

    in Organizational Settings (MIBOS)

    Upward-influence

    t a c t i c '

    Assertiveness

    Rationality

    Exchange

    Upward influence

    Coalit ion

    Manufactur ing

    employees

    (tt = 52)

    .13

    ,26*

    .23*

    .13

    .11

    Employed students in

    Gove r nme n t

    {n -  57 )

    .08

    .1 6

    .33**

    .23*

    .28*

    Retailing

    (n  = 88)

    .20

    .11

    .25*

    .27*

    .20

    Service

    (« = 71)

    .28*

    .09

    .35**

    .18*

    .12

      Measured with Schriesheim and Hinkin's (1990) revision of Kipnis,

    Schm idt, and Wilkinson's (1980) scale.

    * p < . 0 5 .

      * * / 3 < . O l .

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    8/10

     

    KAMALESH KUMAR AND MICHAEL BEYERLEIN

    gratiation, both assertiveness and upward influenee are more

    direct, demanding, and somewhat less political influence tac-

    tics.

     Thus, even though all these taaics are designed to secure

    favorable outcomes from one's supervisor, they are different

    construc ts. The low correlations (mean correlations of. 17) b e-

    tween ingratiation and these two other measures of upward

    influence provide some indication of an acceptable level of

    discriminant validity for MIBOS.

    Ingratiation

     is

     a political strategy that is largely manipulative

    in nature.

     As

     such, it should not be related to the direct persua-

    sion tactic of rationality. Correlations ranged from .09 to .26.

    The low relationship between rationality and MIBOS scores

    provides further suppo rt for the distinctiveness of ingratiation

    as an influence strategy

    Finally, across all four samples, correlations between MIBOS

    and coalition scores ranged from .11 to .28. Coalition tactics

    include persuasion and creating pressure by obtaining the sup-

    port of others (Kipnis et al.. 1980). Although such behaviors

    may involve covert influence (Yukl & Falbe, 1990), the pro-

    cesses and strategies involved in using them are quite different.

    This fact is confirmed by the low correlation between the two

    measures.

    The percentage of common variance shared by MIBOS and

    other measures of upward influence did not exceed  13 and

    averaged less than 5 . The magnitudes of these relationships

    are clearly low enough to demonstrate, rather conclusively, the

    discriminant validity of MIBOS. This is particularly true be-

    cause the magn itudes of

     the

     correlations of M IBOS with mea-

    sures of other influence strategies are quite similar to those

    reported recently by Yukl and Faibe (1990).

    Discussion

    Tbis article presents the development and validation of

    MIBOS, an instrument designed to assess ingratiatory behav-

    iors of employees in organizations. Overall, the results of the

    validity tes ts provide good evidence of convergent and dis crimi-

    nant validity The tests conducted during the first phase ofthe

    study confirmed the high internal consistency and stability of

    the ins trume nt. T he test for internal consistency, conducted on

    the responses of 346 employees, yielded a Cronbach's alpha

    coefficient of .92. reconfirming the fact that MIBOS is ex-

    tremely reliable. Phases 2 and 3 produced good evidence for the

    construct validity of MIBOS in the form of content validity,

    covergent validity, and discriminant validity However, it is

    never possible to address

     every

     relevant issue in any

     single

     study.

    For further validation ofthe scale,

     it seems

     ap propriate

     to

     assess

    ingratiatory behaviors as seen from other perspectives {e.g.. su-

    pervisors, co-workers) and to examine correlations between

    these different

     sources.

     Assessment of MIBOS from a criterion-

    related perspective^What outcomes can be expected to corre-

    late with tbe scale?—would also be useful.

    Although ingratiatory behaviors

     have

     been empirically inves-

    tigated in laboratory studies for over

     25 years

     {Jones, 1964), an d

    tbeir use as an upward influence strategy has been studied in

    organizational settings for over

     10

     years {e.g., Allen etal., 1979;

    Wortman & Linsenmeier. 1977). few empirical studies have

    explored the use of ingratiation in organizations. Most recent

    studies have been conceptual and theoretical (e.g., Liden

     

    Mit-

    chell, 1988; Ralston, 1985; Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984). These

    studies have provided a n umbe r of testable propositions, but s

    far the propositions have remained untested. Indeed, ingratia

    tory behavior in organizational settings continues to remain

    both an intriguing and highly underresearched topic in the

    field of organizational behavior.

    The absence ofa measuremen t instrum ent designed to focu

    specifically on ingratiation seems to be one ofthe major rea-

    sons for the absence of empirical investigation o fthese behav

    iors. The development of a scientifically validated instrumen

    for the measurement of ingratiation therefore constitutes the

    first major step toward em pirical investigation. Only when ps

    chologists are able to assess and m easure this phenom enon can

    they begin to relate it to major social issues within organiza-

    tions. The construction and validation of

     MIBOS

     should hel

    to spur research interest in a topic that has long been ignored

    and warrants much greater interest from organizational scien-

    tists.

    References

    Allen,R. W Porter, D. L. Renwick

    P.

     A.,&M ayes,B.T(1979).Oi^an

    zational politics: Tactics and characteristics of its actors.

     Califor

    Management Review 22

    77-83.

    Ansari, M. A., & Kapoor, A. (1987). Organizational context and up-

    ward influence tactics.

     Organizational Behavior and Human D e

    sion Processes 40

    39-40.

    Baumelster, R. F (1982). A self-presentational view of social phen om

    ena.

     Psychological Bulteiin

    91 . 3-26.

    Christie. R., & Geis, E L. (1970).

      Studies in M achiavellianism

    Sa

    Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Couch, A., & Keniston. K. (1960). Yeasayer and naysayer.

     Journalof

    Abnormal and Social Psychology 60,151 -174.

    Cronbach, L. (1986).

     Essentials o f psychological testing

     (3rd ed.). N

    York: Macmillan.

    Erez, M.., & Rim, T. (1982). The re latio nship between goals, influence

    tactics and personal and organizational variables.

     Human Relations

    J5 ,

      871-878.

    Erez, M., Rim, Y.  Keider, I. (1986). The two sides ofthe tactics of

    influence: Agent vs. target.

     Journal of Occupational

     Psychotogy.

     

    25-39.

    Ealbo, T,

     

    Peplau, L. A. (1980). Power strategies in intim ate relation-

    ships.

     Journal of Personality and Sociat Psychology  38

    618-628

    Gouldner. A. (1960). The norms of reciprocity.

     American Sociologi

    Review. 25

    161-178.

    Hair, X, Anderson, R. O.,  Tatham, R. (1987).

     Multidimensional da

    analysis

     (pp. 244-2 75). New York: M acmillan.

    Jones,

     E. E. (1964).

     Ingratiation.

     New York: Appleton.

    Jones, E. E., & Wortman. C. (1973).

     Ingratiation: An attributional a

    proach.

     Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

    Kipnis, D. Schmidt, S. M.,  Wilkinson,

     J.

     (1980). Intraorganization

    influence tactics: Explorations in getting one's way.

     Journat of Ap-

    plied

     Psychology

    65

    440-452.

    Liden, R. C,  Mitchell, T R. (1988). Ingratiatory beh aviors in organi-

    zational settings.

     Academy o f Management Review

    13 572-587.

    Madison, D. L., Allen. R. W, Porter. L. W, Renwick. R. M..  Mayes

    B.  T (1980). Organizational politics: An exploration of manager'

    perception.

      Human Relations 33

    79-100.

    Mowday, R. T. (1978). The exercise of upward influence in organiza-

    tions.

     Administralive Science

     Quarterly.

      23 . 137-156.

    Mowday, R. T ,  Steers, R. M. (1979). The measurement of organiza-

    tional commitment.

     Journat o f Vocational

     Behavior

    14

    224-247

    Pandey, J. (1981). Effectsof Machiavellianism and degree of org aniza-

    tional formalization on ingratiation. Psychologia

    24

    41-46.

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    9/10

    INGRATiATORY BEHAVIORS

      7

    Pandey,

     1.  Bohra, K.

     A.

     (1984). Ingratiation a sa function of organiza-

    tional characteristics and supervisory styles.

     International Reviewof

    Applied Psychotogy. 33,

     381-394.

    Pandey. J.,  Rastogi, R. (1979). M achiavellianism and ingratiation.

    Journal of Social  Psychology. 108,

      221-225.

    Porter. L. W, Allen. R. W. Angle, H. L. (1981). The politics of up-

    ward influence in organizations.

     Research in Organizational Behav-

    ior.

      3,

     109-149.

    Ralston,

     D.

     A. (1985). Employee ingratiation: The roleof managem ent.

    Academy of Management  Review, 10.

      477-487.

    Riggio,

     R. E..

      Friedman,

     H.

     S. (1986). Impression formation:

     The

    TohofexprtssivebthavioT. Journ al ofPersonality and Social Psychol-

    ogy. 50,421-^21.

    Rorer, L. G. {1965). The great response style myth.

     Psychological Bulle-

    tin, 63, \29-\S6.

    Rotter,

     J. B.

     (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus exter-

    nal locus of control.

     Psychological Monographs. 80.

    1-28.

    Schilit, W

     K.,

      Locke, E. A. (1982). A study

     of

     upward influence

     in

    organizations.

     Administrative Science Quarterly, 27,

      304-316.

    Schlenker.

     B.

     R.,  Leary, M. R . (1982). Social anxiety and self-presen-

    tation: A conceptualization and model.

     Psychological Bulletin.

      92.

    641-669.

    Schriesheim, C. A.. Hin kin. T. R. (1990). Influence tactics used by

    subordinates: A theoretical and empirical analysis and refinement of

    the Kipnis, Schmidt, and  Wilkinson subscales.

     Journal of Applied

    Psychology,

      75 ,  246-257.

    Snyder. M . (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior.

     Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 4,

     526-537.

    Spector, P. E. (1988). Development of the work locus of control scale.

    Journal of Occupational Psychology, 61.

      335-340.

    Steers, R. M., Braunstein, D. N. (1976). A behaviorally-based mea-

    sure of manifest needs in a work setting.

     Journal of Vocational Behav

    ior 9.

      251-266

    Strahan , R., Gerb asi. K. C. (1972). Shor t, homogeneous version of

    Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale.

     Journal of Clinical Psy-

    chology , 28. \9\-\9i.

    Tedeschi, J. T. (1981).

     Impression

     management:

     Theory and social psy

    chological research.

     San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Tedeschi,

     J. T,

    Melburg,

     V

      (1984). Impression management

     and

    influence

     in the

     organization.

      Research  in Sociology of Organiza-

    tions, 3. 31-5 .

    Tedesc hi, J. X. Schlenker,

     B.

     T ,

     

    Linkskold. S. (1972). The exercise of

    power and influence.  In J. T  Tedeschi (Ed.),

     The social influence

    process (pp.

     287 -345). Chicago: Aldine.

    Wortman, C.

     B.,

      Linsenmeier. J. A. (1977). Interpersonal attraction

    and techniques of ingratiation in  organizational settings. In B.  M.

    Staw G. R.Salanik(Eds.).

     N ew directions in organizational behavio

    (pp. 133-178). New York: St. Claire Press.

    Yukl,

     G.,

    Falbe,

     C.

     (1990). Influence tactics

     and

     objectives

     in up-

    ward, downward, and lateral influence attempts.

     Journatof Applied

    Psychology.

      75, B2~14Q.

    Received February

     8,1990

    Revision received March 8,1991

    Accepted March 8,1991  •

    Call for Nom inat ions for JEP: Human Perception

     and

      Performance

    The Publications and Communications (P C) Board has opened nominations for the editor-

    ship ofthe Journal of Experimental Psyehology: Human Pereeption and Performance for a 6-year

    term starting January 1994. James E. Cutting is the incumbent editor.

    Candidates must be members of  APA  and should be available to start receiving manuscripts

    early in 1993 to prepare for issues published in 1994. Please note that the P C Board encour-

    ages more participation by members of underrepresented groups in the publication process

    and would particularly welcome such nominees. To nominate candidates, prepare a statement

    of one pj^e or less in support of each candidate. Submit nominations to

    Howard E. Egeth, Chair, Search Committee, JEP: HPP

    Department of Psychology

    Johns Hopkins University

    Charles 34th Streets

    Baltimore, Maryland 21218

    Other members ofthe search committee are Lynn A. Cooper, Robert G. Crowder, and David E.

    Meyer. First review of nominations will begin January 15,1992.

  • 8/18/2019 MIBOS in the field

    10/10