palermo final

Upload: impunitywatch

Post on 30-May-2018

224 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    1/38

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    2/38

    argue that the use of torture is ineffective due to its likelihood to create false confessions and

    interrogative suggestibility.

    I. LEGISLATIONIN RESPONSETO TERRORISM

    Terrorism poses a threat to nations around the world.6 In response to the fear of terrorist

    attacks, countries have taken steps to protect their citizens, such as passing anti-terrorism

    legislation.7 Moreover, in the United States, new terrorism laws were created following the

    World Trade Center bombing in 1993, the Oklahoma City bombing in 1995, and the September

    11, 2001 attacks.8 Similarly, Great Britain enacted new criminal laws against terrorism

    following the IRA bombings of 1974.

    9

    Unfortunately, however, in the midst of thwarting

    potential terrorist attacks, some governments have overstepped the bounds of human decency.10

    Specifically, the threat of terrorism has been used by countries as justification for implementing

    the use of torture during interrogation.11

    II. TORTURE TODAY

    6 Kent Roach & Gary Trotter, Miscarriages of Justice in the War against Terror, 109 PENN ST. L. REV. 967, 973

    (2005).

    7 PDD 39: U.S. Policy on Counter-Terrorism, http://www.emergency-management.net/laws_pdd39.htm (last visited

    Jan. 14, 2008) [hereinafter PDD 39]; Authorization for the Use of Military Force (AUMF), Pub. L No. 107-40, 115

    Stat. 224 (2001); Privacy International, UK Prevention of Terrorism Act 2005, Mar. 11, 2005, ch. 2, available at

    http://www.privacyinternational.org/article.shtml?cmd%5B347%5D=x-347-159871 (last visited Jan. 14, 2008);

    Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 974.

    8 PDD 39,supra note 7;see generally AUMF, 115 Stat. 224;see Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 974.

    9

    CAIN: HSMO, Prevention of Terrorism (Temporary Provisions) Act 1974, http://cain.ulst.ac.uk/hmso/pta1974.htm(last visited Mar. 4, 2007); Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 974.

    10 Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 974.

    11 Australian Human Rights & Equal Opportunity Commission, Seventh International Conference for National

    Human Rights Institutions, John von Doussa, President, Conflict and Countering Terrorism: Civil and PoliticalRights and the Rule of Law (Sept. 14-17, 2004),

    http://www.humanrights.gov.au/about/media/speeches_president/2004/koreaterrorismworkshop.html (last visited

    Jan. 14, 2008); Torture Worldwide,supra note 2;see Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1.

    2

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    3/38

    Today, torture is still used by many countries throughout the world.12 Human Rights

    Watch, an independent organization devoted to protecting the rights of humans throughout the

    world, has published information detailing the use of torture by several governments.13

    Specifically, according to one Human Rights Watch report, countries such as China, Egypt, Iran,

    Iraq, Israel, Nepal, and others commonly engage in torture to procure information from detainees

    and prisoners.14 For example, this report suggests that in Xinjiang, China [d]etainees have

    reported beatings with shackles, electric shocks, and being kicked to the point of

    unconsciousness.15

    There is also evidence that the United States government has used torture in response to

    terrorism and the September 11, 2001 attacks.16 For instance, there is strong evidence suggesting

    that the United States government tortured Iraqi prisoners and Guantanamo Bay detainees.17 In

    particular, following interviews with former detainees and officials from the United States, the

    United Nations recently reported that the United States violated the detainees rights, and

    perhaps practiced torture, during their detentions at Guantanamo.18 Additionally, an ex-

    Guantanamo Bay detainee, Murat Kurnaz, reported to a Turkish website (CNN Turk.com) that

    12Torture Worldwide,supra note 2; Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 582.

    13 Human Rights Watch, Defending Human Rights Worldwide, http://hrw.org/campaigns/torture.htm (last visited

    Jan. 14, 2008).

    14Torture Worldwide,supra note 2.

    15Id.

    16 The "Taguba Report" On Treatment of Abu Ghraib Prisoners In Iraq, Article 15-6 Investigation

    of the 800th Military Police Brigade, available athttp://news.findlaw.com/hdocs/docs/iraq/tagubarpt.html (lastvisited Jan. 14, 2008) [hereinafter Taguba Report]; Report of the Human Rights Committee,supra note 2, 84, at

    13.

    17 Taguba Report,supra note 16; Report of the Human Rights Committee,supra note 2, 84, at 13; Murphy,supra

    note 2.

    18 U.N. Econ. & Soc. Council [ECOSOC], Commission on Human Rights,Economic, Social and Cultural Rights

    Civil and Political Rights, E/CN.4/2006/120, 91 (Feb. 27, 2006), http://documents-dds-

    ny.un.org/doc/UNDOC/GEN/G06/112/76/pdf/G0611276.pdf?OpenElement (last visited Jan. 14, 2008).

    3

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    4/38

    he was tortured by American soldiers during his detention at the military base.19 The report

    indicates that Americans refused to give Kurnaz food and subjected him to electric shocks. 20

    Reports have also uncovered a secret CIA rendition program.21 According to these

    reports, suspected terrorists are captured by the United States government, sent to other

    countries, such as Egypt and Syria, and subjected to torture and other forms of violence during

    interrogations.22

    For example, a recent United Nations ruling,Agiza v. Sweden, stated that Sweden

    violated the Convention against Torture treaty by assisting the United States in transferring

    individuals from Sweden to Egypt.

    23

    In late 2001, CIA operatives transferred Mohammed al-

    Zari and Ahmed Agiza, individuals suspected of terrorist activities, from Sweden to Egypt with

    the assistance of Swedish officials.24 The United Nations Human Rights Committee held in this

    landmark decision that ... the State partys expulsion of the complainant was in breach of article

    3 of the Convention. The procurement of diplomatic assurances, which provided no mechanism

    for their enforcement, did not suffice to protect against this manifest risk.25

    19 Murphy,supra note 2.

    20Id.

    21 Human Rights Watch, Sweden Violated Torture Ban in CIA Rendition: Diplomatic Assurances against Torture

    offer No Protection from Abuse CIA Rendition (Nov. 10, 2006), available at

    http://hrw.org/english/docs/2006/11/09/sweden14548.htm (last visited Jan. 14, 2008) [hereinafterSweden Violated

    Torture Ban].

    22 U.N. Gen. Assembly, Torture and other Cruel, Inhuman or Degrading Treatment or Punishment,Report of theSpecial Rapporteur on Torture and other Cruel, Inhuman or Degrading Treatment or Punishment, 42-45, U.N.

    Doc. A/60/316 (Aug. 30, 2005) [hereinafterReport on Torture]; Bernard Hibbits, Guantanamo Detainees Claim USPrisoner Transfers Exposed Them to Torture, JURIST, Apr. 26, 2006, available at

    http://jurist.law.pitt.edu/paperchase/2006/04/guantanamo-detainees-claim-us-prisoner.php (last visited Jan. 14,2008).

    23Report on Torture,supra note 22; Sweden Violated Torture Ban,supra note 21.

    24Id.

    25Report on Torture,supra note 22, 44. The Convention against Torture provides that: No State Party shall

    expel, return (refouler) or extradite a person to another State where there are substantial grounds for believing that

    he would be in danger of being subjected to torture. Convention against Torture,supra note 1, art. 3, 1.

    4

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    5/38

    Furthermore, other European nations such as Poland have been accused of helping the

    CIA in transferring suspected terrorists aboard.26 As a result of these allegations, the European

    Parliament and the United States Senate are investigating the CIAs secret rendition program.27

    Preliminary investigations suggest that captives have been tortured abroad under the current

    rendition program.28 All of this information, which includes United Nations findings, provides

    evidence suggesting that the United States government has and continues to use torture against

    alleged terrorists.29

    III. INTERNATIONAL PROHIBITION AGAINST TORTURE

    Although there is strong evidence suggesting that the United States and other countries

    engage in the use of torture, it remains illegal under international law.30 The use of torture is

    prohibited by international documents, which includes several treaties, the Third and Fourth

    26 Draft Report on the Alleged use of European Countries by the CIA for the Transportation and Illegal Detention of

    Prisoners, 136, EUR. PARL. DOC. PR\641327EN (2006) [hereinafter European Parliament Draft Report]; Adam

    Easton, MEPs Probe Poland Rendition Claim, BBC NEWS, Nov. 8 2006, available at

    http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/europe/6127132.stm (last visited Jan. 14, 2008).

    27 European Parliament Draft Report,supra note 26; Easton,supra note 26; James Sturcke, Senate to Investigate

    Rendition Abuses, GUARDIAN UNLIMITED, Nov. 14, 2006, available at

    http://www.guardian.co.uk/usa/story/0,,1947647,00.html (last visited Jan. 14, 2008).

    28See Sturcke,supra note 27.

    29See International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights, Human Rights Committee, Consideration of Reports

    Submitted by States Parties Under Article 40 of the Covenant, 13, CCPR/C/USA/CO/3/Rev.1 (July 10-28 2006)

    [hereinafterConsideration of Reports]. The Committee recommends that the United States ensure that their

    interrogation techniques fall within Article 7 of the International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights. Id. Where

    article 7 states: No one shall be subjected to torture or to cruel, inhuman or degrading treatment or punishment. In

    particular, no one shall be subjected without his free consent to medical or scientific experimentation. InternationalCovenant on Civil and Political Rights, G.A. Res. 2200A (XXI), U.N. GAOR, 21st Sess., Supp. No. 16, at 52, U.N.

    Doc. N6316, Dec. 18, 1966, 999 U.N.T.S. 171, art. 7, available athttp://www.unhchr.ch/html/menu3/b/a_ccpr.htm(last visited Jan. 14, 2008) [hereinafter ICCPR].

    30Consideration of Reports,supra note 29; see also Third Geneva Convention, Relative to the Treatment of

    Prisoners of War, 6 U.S.T. 3316, T.I.A.S. 3364 [hereinafter Geneva Convention III]; Fourth Geneva Convention,

    Relative to the Protection of Civilian Persons in Time of War, 6 U.S.T. 3516, T.I.A.S. 3365 (last visited March 5,

    2007) [hereinafter Geneva Convention IV].

    5

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    6/38

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    7/38

    Therefore, even the threat of terrorism within a country is not a legitimate reason to engage in

    torture against suspected terrorists.

    Article 3 of the Convention against Torture stipulates that nation-states may not expel,

    return or extradite a person to another State where there are substantial grounds for believing that

    he would be in danger of being subjected to torture.41 In other words, a country cannot create a

    rendition program that allows terrorists or other individuals to be transferred to nations where

    there is a strong likelihood that torture will take place. Moreover, the Convention against

    Torture treaty denounces the use of torture throughout the world; thus, a violation of the terms of

    the treaty is a violation of international law.

    42

    Furthermore, the prohibition of torture is considered customary international law.43 The

    Supreme Court of the United States held in The Paquette Habana case, when there is no treaty

    and no controlling executive or legislative act or judicial decision, resort must be had to the

    customs and usages of civilized nations.44 Customary law is law that develops over time

    through common usage within the law of nations.45 Thus, it is binding throughout the

    international community even without physical documentation.46

    Torture had been prohibited by customary international law prior to the establishment of

    the Geneva Conventions and the Convention against Torture.47 These treaties have merely

    41Id. art. 3, 1.

    42See Convention against Torture,supra note 1; SeeNagan & Atkins,supra note 37.

    43

    Filartiga v. Pena-Irala, 577 F. Supp. 860, 863 (E.D.N.Y. 1984); Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 586.

    44 The Paquete Habana, 175 U.S. 677, 700 (1900).

    45Id. at 709.

    46See id. at 700.

    47See Filartiga, 577 F. Supp. at 863.

    7

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    8/38

    codified the customary international law prohibiting torture.48 Therefore, even without the

    Geneva Conventions and the Convention against Torture, torture would still be prohibited

    through customary international law.49

    Moreover, customary international law binds nations to one another throughout the

    international community, regardless of whether the state adopts the law domestically.50 Thus,

    even if there are no legal ramifications at the domestic level, states in violation of international

    law, may still face political consequences at the international level.51

    Additionally, the prohibition of torture is considered jus cogens.52 Jus cogens is a

    peremptory norm of general international law.

    53

    In Committee of U.S. Citizens Living in

    Nicaragua v. Reagan, the court stated that jus cogens (or compelling law) enjoy the highest

    status in international law and prevail over both customary international law and treaties.54

    Therefore, torture is prohibited throughout the international community by treaties, customary

    international law, and jus cogens.

    IV. INTERNATIONALAND DOMESTIC EFFECTSONTHE

    PROHIBITIONOF TORTUREINTHE UNITED STATES

    48 Geneva Convention III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30; Convention against Torture,supra

    note 1.

    49 See Filartiga, 577 F. Supp. at 863.

    50

    Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 586.

    51Id. at 596; Marcy Strauss, Torture, 48 N.Y.L. SCH. L. REV. 201, 256 (2003/2004).

    52 ANTHONY AUST, MODERN TREATY LAWAND PRACTICE 257-258 (2000).

    53Id.

    54 Committee of U.S. Citizens Living in Nicaragua v. Reagan, 859 F.2d 929, 935 (D.C. Cir. 1988).

    8

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    9/38

    Torture is prohibited by law in the United States at both the international and domestic

    levels.55 The United States ratified the Third and Fourth Geneva Conventions in 1955,56 which

    obligated the United States to prohibit torture against prisoners of war and protected persons.57

    Furthermore, the United States ratified the Convention against Torture in 1994.58 During

    this ratification, however, the United States stipulated several reservations and declarations.59 In

    particular, the United States considers Articles 1-16 to be non self-executing.60 Non self-

    executing stipulations within a treaty, and non self-executing treaties in general, require the

    implementation of legislation within a State, who is party to the treaty, before it is domestically

    enforceable.

    61

    Therefore, a violation of the treaty has different legal consequences

    internationally than domestically.

    At the international level, the United States is legally bound to prohibit torture.62 The

    United States bound itself to the international community by ratifying the Geneva Conventions

    and the Convention against Torture.63 Further, the United States is forbidden to practice torture

    55 Geneva Convention III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30; Convention against Torture, supra

    note 1; Torture Act, 18 U.S.C.A. 2340 (2004); Military Commissions Act of 2006 (MCA), Pub. L. No. 109-366,

    120 Stat. 2600, 2627 (2006); Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 586.

    56 Geneva Convention III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30; Taylor,supra note 32, at 10.

    57 Geneva Conventions III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30.

    58See Convention against Torture,supra note 1;see also Nagan & Atkins,supra 37, at 89 n. 12.

    59 Convention against Torture,supra note 1; Committee against Torture, Office of the High Commission on Human

    Rights, Declarations and Reservations, Apr. 23, 2004, United States of America,

    http://www.unhchr.ch/html/menu2/6/cat/treaties/convention-reserv.htm (last visited Mar. 7, 2007) [hereinafter

    Convention Reservations]; Taylor,supra note 32, at 11.

    60

    Taylor,supra note 32, at 11;see Convention Reservations,supra note 59, at I, 1.

    61 Whitney v. Robertson, 124 U.S. 190, 194 (1888); Taylor,supra note 32, at 11.

    62See Geneva Convention III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30; Convention against Torture,supra note 1.

    63 Geneva Convention III,supra note 30; Geneva Convention IV,supra note 30; Convention against Torture,supra

    note 1;seeNagan & Atkins,supra note 37.

    9

    http://www.unhchr.ch/html/menu2/6/cat/treaties/convention-reserv.htmhttp://www.unhchr.ch/html/menu2/6/cat/treaties/convention-reserv.htm
  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    10/38

    internationally because it is banned by customary law and jus cogens.64 Therefore, because jus

    cogens supersedes all international documents and treaties, the international scope of the

    Convention against Torture is not limited by the United States reservations.65

    However, at the domestic level, the provisions within the Convention against Torture

    treaty are more narrowly construed as a result of subsequent legislation enacted in the United

    States.66 For example, Article 1 of the Convention defines torture more broadly than the United

    States does in 18 U.S.C. 2340.67 The Convention against Torture defines torture as:

    any act by which severe pain or suffering, whether physical or mental, is

    intentionally inflicted on a person for such purposes as obtaining from him or a

    third person information or a confession, punishing him for an act he or a thirdperson has committed or is suspected of having committed, or intimidating or

    coercing him or a third person, or for any reason based on discrimination of anykind, when such pain or suffering is inflicted by or at the instigation of or with the

    consent or acquiescence of a public official or other person acting in an official

    capacity. It does not include pain or suffering arising only from, inherent or

    incidental to lawful sanctions.68

    Whereas the United States defines torture more narrowly:

    (1) "torture" means an act committed by a person acting under the color of law

    specifically intended to inflict severe physical or mental pain or suffering (other

    than pain or suffering incidental to lawful sanctions) upon another person withinhis custody or physical control; (2) "severe mental pain or suffering" means the

    prolonged mental harm caused by or resulting from

    (A) the intentional infliction or threatened infliction of severe physical pain orsuffering; (B) the administration or application, or threatened administration or

    application, of mind-altering substances or other procedures calculated to disrupt

    profoundly the senses or the personality; (C) the threat of imminent death; or

    (D) the threat that another person will imminently be subjected to death, severephysical pain or suffering, or the administration or application of mind-altering

    64Filartiga, 577 F. Supp. at 863; AUST,supra note 52; Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 586.

    65Committee of U.S. Citizens Living in Nicaragua, 859 F.2d at 935; Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 586.

    66See 18 U.S.C.A. 2340.

    67 Convention against Torture,supra note 1, art. 1, 1; 18 U.S.C.A. 2340.

    68 Convention against Torture,supra note 1, art. 1, 1.

    10

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    11/38

    substances or other procedures calculated to disrupt profoundly the senses or

    personality . . . .69

    Moreover, a Memorandum for the General Counsel of the Secretary of Defense, dated

    January 15, 2003, provides [i]n sum, the obligations under the Torture Convention apply to the

    interrogation of unlawful combatant detainees, but the Torture Convention prohibits torture only

    as defined in the U.S. Understanding.70

    Additionally, in the United States, domestic laws against terrorism have implications on

    torture.71 Following the September 11, 2001 attacks on the United States, Congress passed a

    joint resolution, known as the Authorization for the Use of Military Force (AUMF), that

    allowed the President to use all necessary and appropriate force against those nations,

    organizations, or persons he determines planned, authorized, committed, or aided the terrorist

    attacks . . . .72 Pursuant to the AUMF, the U.S. Armed Forces invaded Afghanistan in hopes of

    capturing individuals that were associated with the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001.73

    During the hostilities with Afghanistan, the United States captured Salim Ahmed

    Hamdan and transferred him to the military prison located in Guantanamo Bay.74 The United

    States charged Hamdan with offenses related to terrorism.75 He petitioned the Supreme Court of

    69 18 U.S.C.A. 2340 (2004).

    70OFFICEOFTHE SECRETARYOF DEFENSE, DETAINEE INTERROGATIONSINTHE GLOBAL WARON TERRORISM: ASSESSMENTOF

    LEGAL, HISTORICAL, POLICY, AND OPERATIONAL CONSIDERATIONS 6 (2003), available at

    http://www.defenselink.mil/news/Jun2004/d20040622doc8.pdf.

    71See Jeffrey F. Addicott,Into The Star Chamber: Does the United States Engage in the Use of Torture or Similar

    Illegal Practices in the War on Terror?, 92 KY. L. J. 849, 853 (2004).

    72 AUMF, 115 Stat. at 224.

    73 Hamdan v. Rumsfeld, 126 S.Ct. 2749, 2760 (2006).

    74Id.

    75Id. at 2761.

    11

    http://www.defenselink.mil/news/Jun2004/d20040622doc8.pdfhttp://www.defenselink.mil/news/Jun2004/d20040622doc8.pdf
  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    12/38

    the United States for habeas relief, alleging that the military commission lacks authority to try

    him.76

    The Supreme Court held that the military commissions in place at Guantanamo Bay

    violated domestic and international law.77 Although the Court did not ban the use of military

    commissions entirely, the Court did hold that procedures currently in place violated the Uniform

    Code of Military Justice (UCMJ).78 For example, the military commissions procedures

    provided that an accused and his civilian counsel may be excluded from, and precluded from

    ever learning what evidence was presented during, any part of the proceeding . . . .79 However,

    UCMJ Article 36(b) requires that rules for courts-martial trials and military commissions must

    be uniform insofar as practicable.80 For example, under the rules for courts-martial and the

    UCMJ, the accused must be present.81 The Court held that the military commissions did not

    comply with Article 36 because there was no evidence that it would be impracticable for courts-

    martial rules to be applied in this situation.82

    More importantly, the Court held that the military commissions were inconsistent with

    the requirements of the Geneva Conventions.83 For example, Common Article 3 of the Geneva

    Conventions requires that detained individuals be tried in a regularly constituted court.84

    76Id. at 2759.

    77Id.

    78Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2759.

    79Id. at 2755.80

    Id. at 2756.81

    Id.82

    Id. at 2792.83

    Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2756.84

    Geneva Convention III,supra note 30, at art. 3, 1(d);Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2757.

    12

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    13/38

    Although the Geneva Conventions do not define this phrase, other sources suggest that it means

    an ordinary military court established and organized accordance with the laws and procedures

    already in force in a country.85 The military commissions created to try Hamdan were

    inconsistent with ordinary United States military courts, such as the courts-martial, and thus are

    not compatible with the Geneva Conventions.86

    This holding was extremely important because, barring few exceptions, the Court rarely

    interferes with the Presidents actions in foreign affairs.87 Furthermore, as the abuse of prisoners

    at Abu Ghraib depicts, the United States is likely ignoring international law to serve its own

    agenda.

    88

    Therefore, the Court inHamdan reaffirmed the necessity for the United States

    government to abide by international law.89

    In response toHamdan, Congress passed the Military Commissions Act of 2006

    (Military Commissions Act) in October 2006, which attempted to remedy the many problems

    inherent to the operation of the military commission tribunals.90 Additionally, the Pentagon

    released in early January 2007, a 238-page Manual for Military Commissions.91 This guide

    85

    Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2796-97 (citing Int'l Comm. of the Red Cross, 1 CUSTOMARY INTL HUMANITARIAN LAW

    355 (2005)).86

    Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2757.87

    Id. at 2823; U.S. v. Curtiss-Wright Export Corp., 299 U.S. 304, 315 (1936).88

    International Committee of the Red Cross (ICRC),Report On the Treatment by the Coalition Forces of Prisoners ofWar and other Protected Persons by the Geneva Conventions in Iraq during Arrest, Internment, and Interrogation,

    at 3.2, Feb. 2004, available athttp://www.informationclearinghouse.info/pdf/icrc_iraq.pdf; Taguba Report,supra

    note 16, at Regarding Part One of the Investigation, I Make the Following Specific Findings of Fact, 5

    (describing that between October and December 2003, at the Abu Ghraib Confinement Facility (BCCF), numerousincidents of sadistic, blatant, and wanton criminal abuses were inflicted on several detainees. This systemic and

    illegal abuse of detainees was intentionally perpetrated by several members of the military police guard force . . . .);Kim Lane Scheppele,Hypothetical Torture in the War on Terrorism, 1 J. NATL SECURITY L. & POLY 285, 291

    (2005).89

    See Hamdan, 126 S.Ct. at 2757.

    90 MCA,supra note 55.91

    13

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    14/38

    helps military personnel interpret and implement the newly instated military commissions.92

    Military judge advocates and attorneys from the Department of Defense and the Department of

    Justice modeled the Manual for Military Commissions after the Manual for Courts-Martial.93

    Thus, several components of the courts-martial trials have been adopted in the current military

    commission procedures.94 For example, the Military Commissions Act requires that the military

    commission tribunals have similar evidentiary rules as the courts-marital trials.95

    Despite these positive additions to the Military Commissions Act, some changes have

    sparked much debate.96 For example, terrorist suspects can be convicted on hearsay evidence

    alone.

    97

    Moreover, although the Military Commissions Act excludes statements obtained by

    torture, it does allow statements that were procured through coercion if the totality of the

    circumstances renders the statements reliable and probative.98

    Furthermore, the Military Commissions Act suspends the writ of habeas corpus for

    enemy combatant detainees.99 Interestingly, the writ has not been suspended since the American

    OFFICEOFTHE SECRETARYOF DEFENSE, MANUALFORMILITARY COMMISSIONS (2007), available at

    http://www.defenselink.mil/pubs/pdfs/The%20Manual%20for%20Military%20Commissions.pdf [hereinafter

    Manual for MC];Pentagon Reveals Rules for Terror Suspect Trials, GUARDIAN UNLIMITED, Jan. 19, 2007, available athttp://www.guardian.co.uk/guantanamo/story/0,,1994380,00.html [hereinafterPentagon Reveals].92

    Manual for MC,supra note 91, at Executive Summary.93

    Id.94

    Id.95

    Id. at I-2;see 10 U.S.C. 949a(b)(2)(A); 10 U.S.C. 949a(b)(2)(B); 10 U.S.C. 949a(b)(2)(C); 10 U.S.C.

    949a(b)(2)(D); 10 U.S.C. 949a(b)(2)(E).96

    See Pentagon Reveals,supra note 91.97

    MCA,supra note 55, at Stat 2608-2609. Where hearsay evidence not otherwise admissible under the rules of

    evidence applicable in trial by general courts-martial may be admitted in a trial by military commission if the

    proponent of the evidence makes known to the adverse party, sufficiently in advance to provide the adverse party

    with a fair opportunity to meet the evidence, the intention of the proponent to offer the evidence, and the particulars

    of the evidence (including information on the general circumstances under which the evidence was obtained). Id.

    98Id. at Stat 2607.

    99

    Id. at Stat 2636.

    14

    http://www.guardian.co.uk/guantanamo/story/0,,1994380,00.htmlhttp://www.guardian.co.uk/guantanamo/story/0,,1994380,00.html
  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    15/38

    Civil War,100 and under the United States Constitution the writ should only be suspended when

    in cases of rebellion or invasion the public safety may require it.101 Additionally, it does not

    allow detainees to use any provision of the Geneva Conventions to file a lawsuit against the

    United States, its soldiers, or interrogators.102 In addition, torture is redefined in the Military

    Commissions Act as:

    an act specifically intended to inflict severe physical or mental pain or suffering

    (other than pain or suffering incidental to lawful sanctions) upon another person

    within his custody or physical control for the purpose of obtaining information ora confession, punishment, intimidation, coercion, or any reason based on

    discrimination of any kind, shall be punished, if death results to one or more of

    the victims, by death or such other punishment as a military commission under

    this chapter may direct, and, if death does not result to any of the victims, by suchpunishment, other than death, as a military commission under this chapter may

    direct.103

    Although this language seems to be fairly consistent with the Convention against Torture,104 it is

    yet to be determined how the United States will interpret this provision in practice.

    V. ISTHE UNITED STATES COMMITTING TORTURE?

    One must look to policy within a jurisdiction to see how an administration is

    implementing a statute. For example, the Bush administration declared that the Geneva

    Conventions would not apply to our conflict with al-Qaeda in Afghanistan or elsewhere in the

    world because, among other reasons, a1-Qaeda is not a High Contracting Party to Geneva.105

    100

    See Ex parte Milligan, 71 U.S. 2 (1866).101

    U.S. CONST. art. 1, 9, cl. 2.102

    MCA,supra note 55, at Stat 2602.103

    Id. at Stat 2627.104

    Convention against Torture,supra note 1, art. 1.

    105 Memorandum from President Bush, To his National Security Advisors Concerning the Application of Geneva

    Convention in the Afghanistan Conflict, 2(a), (Feb. 7, 2002), available at

    http://www.gwu.edu/~nsarchiv/NSAEBB/NSAEBB127/02.02.07.pdf (last visited March 4, 2007).

    15

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    16/38

    Although the Military Commissions Act has language similar to the Convention against Torture,

    it does not mean the United States interpretation of torture is consistent with the international

    interpretation. After all, no State is going to expressly declare in a statute that it condones

    torture.106 Rather, one must look for evidence of the use of torture in that jurisdiction.

    As mentioned above, recent reports, which include United Nations findings, suggest that

    the United States engages in torture (i.e. the CIA rendition program).107 This evidence implies

    that the United States blatantly disregarded international law and domestic law by using torture

    during interrogations of suspected terrorists prior to the enactment of the Military Commissions

    Act. If the United States practiced torture before passing the Military Commissions Act, one can

    infer that the United States still uses torture after the enactment of the Military Commissions Act.

    Further, the government may be able to contend that statements obtained during the CIA

    rendition program and the Guantanamo Bay detentions were procured through methods of

    coercion, rather than torture.108 Using coercion to obtain information is permissible in some

    instances under the Military Commissions Act.109 Specifically, it provides that statements

    obtained by torture are not admissible, but statements in which the degree of coercion is

    disputed may be admitted if reliable, probative, and the admission would best serve the interests

    of justice.110 Furthermore, throughout the Military Commissions Act, coercion is not explicitly

    defined.111 The military judge is allotted also broad discretion in determining the difference

    106

    Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1.107

    Report on Torture,supra note 22;Hibbits,supra note 22.108

    MCA,supra note 55, at Stat 2607.109

    Id.

    110 Manual for MC,supra note 91, at I-1;see also 10 U.S.C. 948r(b); MCA,supra note 55, at Stat 2607.111

    See MCA,supra note 55.

    16

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    17/38

    between torture and coercion based on the totality of the circumstances.112 This provision

    allows the government to use semantics to bypass the ban on torture. Rather than calling its

    techniques torture, the government will refer to them as coercion, which is permissible under

    the Military Commissions Act.

    VI. ARGUMENTS FORAND AGAINST TORTURE

    Despite the universal condemnation against the practice of torture, evidence establishes

    that nations use torture as an interrogation device.113 The logical question becomes, why? There

    are several arguments for and against the use of torture.114 In TortureThe Case for Dirty Harry

    and against Alan Dershowitz, Uwe Steinhoff asks whether torture can be morally justified:

    What is so bad about torturing people, anyway? People also kill people. Soldierskill people, policemen kill people, doctors kill people, executioners kill people

    and ordinary people kill people. Some of these killings are justified. So why

    shouldnt it be justified in some cases to torture people? After all, being killed

    seems to be worse than being tortured.115

    As Steinhoff suggests, it seems illogical to condone war, which involves killing enemy forces,

    while condemning torture, which does not result in death, during the War on Terror. Other

    proponents for the use of torture as an interrogation tool argue that it is only acceptable to thwart

    grave harm to others.116 This argument suggests that a utilitarian calculation must be made: if

    more lives can be saved by torture than are harmed by it, the act is [morally] justified. 117

    112

    Manual for MC,supra note 91, at III-8;see 10 U.S.C. 948r.; MCA,supra note 55, at 2607.113

    Report on Torture,supra note 22; European Parliament Draft Report,supra note 26; Consideration of Reports,supra note 29.114

    See Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 596-615;see also Philip N.S. Rumney,Is Coercive Interrogation of

    Terrorist Suspects Effective? A Response to Bagaric and Clarke, 40 U.S.F. L. REV. 479, 483-485 (2006).115

    Uwe Steinhoff, TortureThe Case for Dirty Harry and against Alan Dershowitz, 23 J. OF APPLIED PHIL. 337, 337

    (2006).

    116 Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 584-85; Rumney,supra note 114, at 479.117

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 255.

    17

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    18/38

    Therefore, following utilitarian reasoning, saving innocent lives easily outweighs the life of a

    suspected terrorist.118

    Additionally, inNot Enough Official Torture in the World? The Circumstances in which

    Torture is Morally Justifiable, authors Mirko Bagaric and Julie Clarke establish five variables to

    consider before administering torture.119 These variables also establish the degree of torture that

    should be used.120 The five variables are (1) the number of lives at risk; (2) the immediacy of

    the harm; (3) the availability of other means to acquire the information; (4) the level of

    wrongdoing of the agent; and (5) the likelihood that the agent actually does possess relevant

    information.

    121

    It is easy see how nations use the utilitarian balancing test to morally justify

    using torture to prevent future terrorist attacks.

    In a recent work by Alan Dershowitz, he expands the utilitarian argument.122 Dershowitz

    portrays a scenario where the FBI can prevent the terrorist attacks of 9/11 (2001) from occurring

    by using a form of non-lethal torture on an individual who has information regarding the

    attacks.123 In this scenario, Dershowitz describes the following technique to be used: a sterilized

    needle inserted under the fingernails to produce unbearable pain without any threat to health or

    life.124 He provides that it is a simple cost-benefit analysis; it is better to torture one

    individual than to allow thousands of innocent people to die.125

    118

    Id.119

    Bagaric & Clarke,supra note 1, at 585.120

    Id.121

    Id.122

    ALAN M. DERSHOWITZ, WHY TERRORISM WORKS, CHAPTER FOUR: SHOULDTHE TICKING BOMB TERRORIST BE TORTURED? 131-

    63 (2002).

    123Id. at 143-44.124

    Id. at 144.125

    18

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    19/38

    Dershowitz contends that individuals are visibly repulsed by the example of non-lethal

    torture that he provides in his scenario, but are more accepting to discussions of the death

    penalty.126 Dershowitz explains that the condemned prisoner no longer faces the breaking of the

    neck, burning of the brain, or the bursting of internal organs.127 Moreover, the prisoner goes to

    sleep at the hand of medicine.128 All this tends to cover up the reality that death is forever

    while nonlethal pain is temporary. In our modern age death is underrated, while pain is

    overrated.129 Therefore, Dershowitz suggests that peoples views on torture are contradictory to

    their views on corporal punishment. Moreover, if Americans can justify punishing individuals

    through the death penalty, it is counter-intuitive that Americans disapprove of torture, which

    inflicts pain and not death.

    Uwe Steinhoff expands this argument in his article by offering that people cannot relate

    with those who are dead.130 Steinhoff provides that we do not know what it truly means to be

    dead, and there is certainly no possibility tofeeldead (emphasis in original).131 Individuals

    however, are able to feel pain.132 Therefore, if we see how pain is inflicted on another person,

    we can feel quite literally com-passion. There is no such compassion possible with the dead (we

    can only feel sorry for them . . . ).133

    Id.126

    Id. at 148.127

    DERSHOWITZ, supra note122, at 149.128

    Id. at 148-49.129

    Id. at 149.130

    Steinhoff,supra note 115, at 340.131

    Id.132

    Id.133

    Id.

    19

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    20/38

    Perhaps this ability to relate to pain is the reason why people find torture morally

    reprehensible. Pain is experienced physically, while death is experienced in the abstract. People

    can appreciate the unbearable pain caused by torture, but people can only know death. However,

    if people could choose pain over death, then they would most likely choose pain. Therefore,

    Dershowitz and Steinhoff agree that torture should be allowed because it is not as bad as

    sentencing criminals to death. Finally, proponents also justify torture through the self-defense

    doctrine.134 This doctrine allows an individual to use deadly force to save ones life or to save

    others.135 Proponents of this argument provide that it should be permissible to use torture to

    protect others from the dangers of terrorism.

    136

    Conversely, there are several arguments against the practice of torture, even as an

    interrogation tool to procure information from suspected terrorists.137 First of all, torture is

    morally repulsive.138 It is detestable for states to deliberately inflict pain and suffering on any

    individual, even a suspected terrorist.139 This is evidenced by the global ban on torture.140

    Secondly, by practicing torture, a state abandons the rule of law for the rule of force.141

    Instead of using the legal system to deal with terrorism, some states, including the United States

    as indicated above, are using torture to procure information regarding terrorist attacks.142 In

    134

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 258.135

    Id.136

    Id.137

    Rumney,supra note 114, at 512-13. Strauss,supra note 3, at 260-65.

    138 Strauss,supra note 3, at 253.139

    Id.140

    Convention against Torture,supra note 1.141

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 254.142

    Report on Torture,supra note 22, 42-46; European Parliament Draft Report,supra note 26, 40 ;

    20

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    21/38

    other words, nations that use torture to procure information are acting similarly to terrorists

    because in both instances, pain and suffering are used to achieve the end result.143

    Thirdly, nations, such as the United States, that engage in torture lose their ability to

    protect their own citizens abroad from being tortured.144 The United States is one of the five

    permanent members of the Security Council for the United Nations.145 The Security Council has

    the responsibility to maintain international peace and security.146 It is thus logical to infer that

    when the United States was accused of using torture in places such as Iraq and Guantanamo Bay,

    it lost its credibility to promote peace and security internationally.147 Other countries could

    reasonably deduce that if a permanent member of the Security Council engages in torture, they

    too are free to do so.148 Thus, by employing torture, the United States places its citizens abroad

    at risk of facing similar treatment from other countries that use torture as an interrogation tool.149

    Finally, opponents of torture argue that self-defense cannot be used as a justification.150

    Under the self-defense doctrine, the use of deadly force is only justified because of an imminent

    threat of harm. However, torturing a suspected terrorist for information he/she may or may not

    Consideration of Reports,supra note 29.143

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 256.144

    Id. at 257.145

    U.N. Security Council Webpage, Members, available at http://www.un.org/sc/members.asp (last visited Jan. 22,

    2008) [hereinafter Security Council Webpage, Members].146

    U.N. Security Council Webpage, Functions and Powers, available at

    http://www.un.org/Docs/sc/unsc_functions.html (last visited March 4, 2007) [hereinafter Security Council Webpage,

    Functions].147

    See Strauss,supra note 3, at 257.

    148See Memorandum from William H. Taft, IV, Legal Adviser, U.S. Dept. of State, to Counsel to the President, onComments on Your Paper on the Geneva Convention, 1-2 (Feb. 2, 2002), available at

    http://www.fas.org/sgp/othergov/taft.pdf (last visited March 4, 2007) [hereinafter Comments on Your Paper];

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 257.149

    See Comments on Your Paper,supra note 148; Strauss,supra note 3, at 257.150

    Strauss,supra note 3, at 260.

    21

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    22/38

    have does not constitute an imminent threat.151 Therefore, if a terrorist does not have

    information, they do not pose an imminent threat. Thus, proponents of the use of torture cannot

    rely on the doctrine of self-defense.

    After assessing the arguments both for and against the use of torture as an interrogation

    tool, the opponents arguments are more compelling. Besides being morally reprehensible,

    peace and security of the international community depends on every nations ability to prevent

    torture.152 Furthermore, as mentioned below, the use of torture is ineffective in procuring

    information.

    VII. FALSE CONFESSIONS

    As mentioned above, it is quite possible that torture has been used to procure information

    regarding terrorist attacks.153 In the context of using torture to gain information regarding prior

    terrorist acts, there are often concerns of false confessions.154 Psychologists have studied the

    phenomenon of false confessions in the framework of coercive police interrogation.155 This

    paper suggests that the phenomenon of false confessions is just as likely to occur when military

    personnel use torture to procure information about past terrorist attacks. The psychological and

    physical pressures induced by torture are similar, yet more amplified in the torture context, and

    thus, have a greater likelihood of causing false confessions.

    151

    Id.152

    Security Council, Members,supra note 145.153

    Consideration of Reports,supra note 29.

    154See Roach and Trotter,supra note 6, at 981;see also Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4; Blagrove,supra note 4;

    Kassin et al.,supra note 4; Russano et al.,supra note 5; Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5.155

    Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 125; Blagrove,supra note 4, at 48; Kassin et al.,supra note 4, at 187; Leo &

    Ofshe,supra note 5, at 430-432.

    22

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    23/38

    In the United States, it is difficult to determine how many wrongful convictions have

    occurred as a result of false confessions.156 The three main reasons for this difficulty are (1) no

    organization collects statistics on the annual number of interrogations and confessions or

    evaluates the reliability of confession statements; (2) most interrogations leading to disputed

    confessions are not recorded; and (3) the ground truth (what really happened) may remain in

    dispute even after the defendant has pled guilty or been convicted.157 However, the Innocence

    Project estimates that defendants have made false confessions in over 25% of wrongful

    convictions that have been overturned by DNA evidence.158

    There are two types of coerced false confessions, coerced-compliant and coerced

    internalized false confessions.159 If an individual confesses to prevent a negative interrogation,

    that is considered a coerced-compliant false confession.160 Psychologically speaking, coerced-

    compliant false confessions occur when an individual believe[s] that the short-term benefits of

    confessingsuch as being left alone, fed, or releasedoutweigh the long-term costs associated

    with prosecution, the loss of reputation, and incarceration.161 It is logical to suggest that if a

    suspected terrorist is being tortured, he or she will give false information to avoid further pain

    and suffering.162 This is similar to the coerced-compliant false confession given in the

    156

    Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5, at 431.157

    Id.158

    Innocence Project: Fix the System, False Confessions & Mandatory Recording of Interrogations, available at

    http://www.innocenceproject.org/fix/False-Confessions.php (last visited March 4, 2007).159

    Saul M. Kassin, Confessions: Psychological and Forensic Aspects, in INTERNATIONAL ENCYCLOPEDIAOFTHE SOCIALBEHAVIORAL SCIENCES 3.3 (Smelser & Baltes eds. 2001) [hereinafter Kassin, Confessions].

    160 Kassin, Confessions,supra note 159.161

    Id.162

    Id.

    23

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    24/38

    interrogation context.163 Furthermore, this makes sense on a psychological level, because in the

    case of an individual being tortured, the short-term benefit of stopping severe physical and/or

    mental pain outweighs the long-term cost of incarceration.

    Coerced-internalized false confessions occur when an innocent personanxious, sleep-

    deprived, confused, and subjected to a highly suggestive interrogation that often includes the

    presentation of false evidenceactually comes to believe that he or she committed the crime.164

    Although this type of confession is less likely to occur, it is still possible that suspected terrorists

    are tortured while being presented with false evidence in order to induce a false belief that they

    are guilty of committing some terrorist attack.

    In the past, false confessions have resulted from the use of torture during terrorist

    investigations.165 The most famous example of this phenomenon occurred in the British case

    known as the Guildford Four.166 In October and November of 1974, the Irish Republican Army

    (IRA) bombed several pubs in England and killed seven people.167 Four individualsPaul Hill,

    Carole Richardson, Gerald Conlon, and Patrick Armstrongwere arrested for the bombings and

    convicted of murder.168 During the trial, no evidence was admitted to show that these individuals

    were associated with the IRA.169 Rather, the prosecution relied mostly on the confessions

    obtained from each of the defendants.170

    163

    Id.164

    Id.165

    See Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 967-977.

    166Id. at 968-969.167

    CHARLES PATRICK EWING & JOSEPH T. MCCANN, MINDSON TRIAL: GREAT CASESIN LAWAND PSYCHOLOGY 45 (2006)

    [hereinafter MINDSON TRIAL]; Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 977.168

    Roach & Trotter,supra note 6, at 977-988.169

    MINDSON TRIAL,supra note 167, at 49.170

    24

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    25/38

    After nearly thirteen years of public scrutiny, the Guildford Four case was re-opened.171

    After reviewing the case, it was determined that it was highly possible that the four defendants

    wrongly confessed to the IRA bombings, which occurred in 1974.172 For example, Hill claimed

    that he wrongly confessed after a gun was held to his head during the twenty-four hour

    interrogation and threats were made that his pregnant girlfriend would be harmed. 173 In the end,

    all members of the Guildford Four were exonerated.174

    Furthermore, there has been much psychological research investigating the phenomenon

    of false confessions.175 In an empirical study, The Consequences of False Confessions:

    Deprivations of Liberty and Miscarriages of Justice in the Age of Psychological Interrogation ,

    Richard A. Leo and Richard J. Ofshe reviewed sixty cases that involved disputed confessions.176

    All of the selected cases were chosen because the individuals were arrested based primarily on

    their confessions and because each of their confessions were later determined to be a proven

    false confession or a highly likely false confession.177

    Id.171

    Id. at 53.172

    Id.173

    MINDSON TRIAL,supra note 167, at 47-48.174

    Id. at 53-54.175

    See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4; Blagrove,supra note 4; Kassin et al.,supra note 4; Russano et al.,supra note

    5; Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5.

    176 Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5, at 435.177

    Id. at 436.

    25

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    26/38

    Thirty-four of these cases were considered proven false confessions, because the

    accused individuals were exonerated by independent pieces of evidence.178 For example, a

    defendants confession was classified as proven false if the murder victim turned up alive.179

    Findings from these cases depicted four sub-types of proven false confessions.180 The

    first type of proven false confession occurs when the suspect confesses to a crime that did not

    take place.181 Secondly, proven false confessions occur when other evidence is produced that

    clearly demonstrates that the defendant is not the perpetrator of the crime.182 Thirdly, the actual

    perpetrator was determined and guilt was ascertained.183 Finally, the fourth sub-type of proven

    false confessions occurs when scientific evidence is found that exonerates the defendant.

    184

    This information is useful because it provides evidence of false confessions arising after

    police interrogations were performed in the United States.185 Additionally, this review provides

    explanations for why false confessions occur.186 For example, interrogators sometimes become

    so committed to closing a case that they improperly use psychological interrogation techniques

    to coerce or persuade a suspect into giving a statement that allows the interrogator to make an

    arrest.187

    178

    Id.179

    Id.180

    Id. at 449.181

    Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5, at 449.182

    Id.183

    Id. at 429.184

    Id.

    185Id.186

    Leo & Ofshe,supra note 5, at 429.187

    Id. at 440.

    26

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    27/38

    Additionally, an empirical review of research offers reasons for false confessions that

    parallel those proffered in real life occurrences of false confessions.188 In The Psychology of

    Confessions: A Review of the Literature and Issues, Saul M. Kassin and Gisli H. Gudjonsson,

    look at a compilation of research to address false confessions and other related issues.189 This

    literature review provides that in controlled laboratory experiments, fatigued and sleep-deprived

    individuals that were isolated for prolonged periods of time were more susceptible to

    manipulation.190 Additionally, long periods of detention cause individuals to become more

    fatigued, to face more despair, and to be more uncertain about their future, which likely increases

    their vulnerability to giving false confessions.

    191

    This evidence suggests that individuals exposed to methods of torture, such as sleep-

    deprivation and isolation, for prolonged periods of time are more susceptible to manipulation. 192

    Furthermore, this evidence also implies that suspected terrorists tortured for long periods of time,

    such as those detained at Guantanamo Bay, are more vulnerable to the pressures that induce false

    information.193

    A laboratory experiment conducted by Saul M. Kassin and Katherine L. Kiechel in 1996

    suggested that false incriminating evidence may cause individuals to accept guilt for an act they

    were not responsible for.194 In this study, 79 undergraduates (40 male, 39 female), participating

    188

    Saul M. Kassin & Gisli H. Gudjonsson, The Psychology of Confessions, A Review of the Literature and Issues, 5

    PSYCHOL. SCI. INTHE PUB. INTEREST 33 (2004).189

    Id.190

    Blagrove,supra note 4, at 54-55; Kassin & Gudjonsson,supra note 188, at 53.191

    See Kassin & Gudjonsson,supra note 188, at 53 (citingR.A. Leo,Inside the Interrogation Room, 86 J. OF CRIM. L.& CRIMINOLOGY 266-303 (1996b)).

    192 Blagrove,supra note 4, at 54-58; see Kassin & Gudjonsson,supra note 188, at 53.193

    See Blagrove,supra note 4, at 54-56; Kassin & Gudjonsson,supra note 188, at 53; Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4;

    Blagrove,supra note 4; Kassin et al.,supra note 4.194

    27

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    28/38

    for extra credit, were given typing tests on a computer.195 The participants typed at two different

    speeds and were not allowed to touch the ALT key because it would cause the computer to

    crash, and lose the experiment.196 After 60 seconds, the computer would cease to function, and

    the experimenter accused the subject of pressing the ALT key.197 All participants were

    innocent and initially denied pressing the key.198

    Kassin and Kiechel varied two independent variables, the subjects level of vulnerability

    (or their subjective certainty concerning their own innocence) and the use of false

    incriminating evidence.199 The vulnerability of the subject was measured by varying the pace

    of the task, either slow and relaxed or frenzied.

    200

    The use of false incriminating evidence

    was manipulated by implementing either a false-witness situation, where a confederate testifies

    to seeing the subject pressing the ALT key, or a no-witness situation, where the same

    confederate states that she did not see what happened.201

    The study assessed the dependent measures, compliance and internalization. 202 For the

    compliance component, the subject was asked by the experimenter to sign a handwritten

    confession created by the experimenter.203 If the subject refused to confess, he/she was asked a

    Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 125.195

    Id. at 126.196

    Id.197

    Id.198

    Id.199

    Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 126.

    200Id.201

    Id.202

    Id.203

    Id.

    28

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    29/38

    second time.204 To measure internalization, the experimenter and subject left the room, meeting a

    confederate in the reception area.205 The experimenter told the confederate (who was blind to the

    subjects behavior during the experiment) that the subject would have to reschedule for another

    session.206 The experimenter then left the room while the confederate asked the subject what had

    occurred during the experiment.207 The responses were recorded and coded by experimenters

    blind to the subjects condition as responses suggesting or not suggesting internalized guilt.208

    The results of the study indicated that 69% of the 75 participants signed confessions and

    28% exhibited internalized guilt.209 The data also showed that the individuals in the slow-

    pace/no-witness control group were the least likely to confess or internalize guilt, whereas, the

    fast-pace/witness group were the most likely to demonstrate compliance or internalization.210

    As Kassin and Kiechel concede, there are several limitations to these findings.211 For

    example, how do these results generalize to the interrogation process of criminal suspects?212

    The consequence of falsely confessing to pressing the ALT key is miniscule to the

    consequence of falsely confessing to a criminal act, such as homicide or stealing.213

    204

    Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 126.205

    Id..206

    Id.207

    Id.208

    Id.

    209 Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 127.210

    Id..211

    Id.212

    Id.213

    Id.

    29

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    30/38

    Furthermore, the ramifications in both instances are at opposite ends of the spectrum.214 In the

    experiment the confession causes guilt, while in the criminal setting, the confession leads to

    incarceration or death. Finally, Kassin and Kiechel admit that the compliance rates in the present

    experiment are high and may have resulted because of the limited consequences involved in the

    experiment.215

    Despite these limitations, this study still presents evidence which supports the conclusion

    that false confessions occur more often following the presentation of false incriminating

    evidence during interrogations.216 The presentation of false incriminating evidence is a common

    interrogation technique employed by police officers.

    217

    In the context of interrogating terrorist

    suspects, it is logical to infer that the suspects are susceptible to similar interrogation techniques.

    Therefore, if terrorist suspects are presented with false incriminating evidence during their

    interrogations, it is plausible that they may falsely confess in the face of such evidence.

    Concededly, as Kassin and Keichel suggest, the consequence of falsely confessing to a

    terrorist attack is grave in comparison to wrongly confessing to ruining an experiment.

    However, as the experiment implies, individuals exposed to greater pressures (i.e. fast

    pace/witness control group) are more likely to confess or internalize guilt.218 In the context of

    interrogating terrorists, the pressure used by many nations is torture.219 Thus, it can be argued

    that an individual will choose the consequential effect of falsely confessing to a terrorist act, i.e.

    214

    See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4, at 127.215

    Id..216

    Id.

    217Id.218

    Id..219

    See U.N. Doc. A/61/40,supra note 2.

    30

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    31/38

    imprisonment, over being continually tortured. Specifically, when faced with wrongly

    confessing to a crime or enduring physical and/or emotional suffering, it is reasonable to suggest

    that the suspect will choose to confess.

    In another experiment, Kassin and his colleagues assessed how the interrogators

    presumption of guilt affected his/her behavior during the interrogation.220 In this study,

    psychology students, receiving extra credit, acted as either interrogators or suspects.221 The

    experimenter described to the interrogator a mock theft that had occurred on campus.222 The

    interrogators were divided into two conditions: (1) guilty expectation condition, where the

    experimenter told the interrogator that four out of five suspects were guilty or (2) innocent

    expectation condition, where the experimenter told the interrogator that one out of five suspects

    was guilty.223

    The experimenter told the interrogators to devise plans for their interviews.224 The

    interrogators were asked to choose six questions from a list of 13 questions.225 They were also

    asked to choose six interrogation techniques from 13 possible choices.226 Some examples of the

    techniques included: making repeated accusations, exposing inconsistencies in the suspects

    story, threatening to involve others [and] appealing to the suspects self-interest...227 The

    220

    Kassin, et al.,supra note 4, at 187.221

    Id. at 190.222

    Id. at 191.

    223Id.224

    Id.225

    Kassin, et al.,supra note 4, at 191.226

    Id. The tactics were derived from Inbau et al., Criminal Interrogation and Confessions 2001.227

    Id.

    31

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    32/38

    suspects were assigned to either the guilty condition or the innocent condition.228 In the guilty

    condition, the suspects actually committed the mock theft, whereas in the innocent condition, the

    suspects were exposed to the area where the crime had occurred, but were not given information

    regarding the crime.229 All participants were instructed not to confess and were offered gift

    certificates if the interrogators viewed them as innocent.230

    The results suggest that the interrogators expectations influence their behavior

    throughout the interrogation process.231 Interrogators in the guilty expectation control group

    selected more guilt-presumptive questions, used more techniques at the outset of interrogation

    and, in the end, judged the suspects guilty.

    232

    The results also demonstrated that the actual guilt

    or innocence of the suspect did not affect the interrogators behaviors.233 Furthermore, the

    evidence showed that interrogators exerted the most pressure on individuals who were actually

    innocent.234

    Of course, there are limitations to this laboratory study.235 Skeptics may question the

    reliability of this study to actual police-suspect interactions.236 For example, the participants in

    this experiment were college students aware of the simulated crime scene.237 Further, the stakes

    228

    Id.229

    Id.230

    Kassin et al.,supra note 4, at 192.231

    Id. at 195.232

    Id.233

    Id. at 195-196.234

    Id. at 197.235

    Kassin et al.,supra note 4, at 200.236

    Id.237

    Id.

    32

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    33/38

    are greater in real criminal investigations than in simulated experiments.238 Students do not have

    the same incentives as interrogators do to perform the interrogations, and thus, the experiment is

    limited to the good faith of the participants.239

    In spite of these limitations, the results of the experiment are still applicable to the real-

    life interrogation of suspected terrorists. In the context of counter-terrorism, it is likely that those

    interrogating suspected terrorists presume that the suspects are guilty. The results proffered by

    the Kassin et al. study suggest that these interrogators are more likely to use guilt-presumptive

    questions, employ more interrogation techniques, and find more suspects guilty.240 The Kassin et

    al. study also demonstrates that interrogators presume guilt regardless of the suspected terrorists

    actual guilt or innocence.241 Furthermore, the experiment suggests that interrogators pressure

    innocent suspects the most. Therefore, if interrogators are using torture as a form of pressure,

    then innocent suspects are the most likely people to be tortured. This presents major human

    rights concerns if countries like the United States are indeed torturing suspected terrorists.

    VIII. A FORMOF TORTURE SLEEP DEPRIVATION

    In addition to false confession research, psychologists have also studied the effects of

    sleep deprivation during interrogations.242 The effects of sleep deprivation during interrogations

    are relevant for the purposes of this paper since the United Nations has recently published

    findings indicating that the United States has authorized sleep adjustments as an enhanced

    interrogation technique.243 Furthermore, international law prohibits the use of interrogation

    238

    Id.239

    Id.

    240See Kassin et al.,supra note 4, at 200.241

    See id.242

    Blagrove,supra note 4, at 48.243

    33

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    34/38

    techniques like sleep deprivation over a protracted time whether employed individually or in

    combination with other forms of enhanced interrogation techniques, such as prolonged stress

    positions and isolation, sensory deprivation, hooding, exposure to cold or heat, 20-hour

    interrogations, removal of clothing and deprivation of all comfort and religious items, forced

    grooming, and exploitation of detainees individual phobias.244 Simply put, these techniques

    cause severe physical and mental suffering and are considered forms of torture.245

    One experiment by Mark Balgrove, addressing the effect of sleep deprivation,

    recommended that police interrogations should not occur in situations where the individuals have

    been deprived of sleep.

    246

    The experiment contained three separate studies.

    247

    Two of the studies

    involved one night without sleep, while the third study involved two nights without sleep.248 In

    all of the studies, individuals were divided into two groups based on their self-reported habitual

    sleep durations.249 Members of one group stayed in the laboratory all night and were not

    allowed to sleep, while the other group members were allowed to sleep at their respective

    homes.250

    To test for suggestibility, each group listened to an audiotape recording illustrating a

    short story about a robbery.251 Participants were required to immediately give free recall about

    U.N. Doc A/61/40,supra note 2, at 84, 13.244

    Id.245

    See id;see also supra note 29.

    246 Blagrove,supra note 4, at 48.247

    Id. at 50.248

    Id.249

    Id.250

    Id.251

    Blagrove,supra note 4, at 50.

    34

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    35/38

    the story to ensure that all groups originally learned the same information.252 After fifty minutes,

    the suspects provided the experimenter with another free recall of the events occurring on the

    audiotape.253 The individuals were then given 20 leading questions that could not be answered

    from information provided by the story.254 Then the suspects were given negative feedback

    regarding their responses, were urged to give accurate answers, and were asked the questions

    over again.255

    Although this experiment is fairly complex, the overall results indicate that sleep

    deprivation enables interrogative suggestibility.256 Specifically, the results demonstrated that

    individuals who were deprived of 43 hours of sleep were the most susceptible to leading

    questions.257 Importantly, individuals who were deprived of only 21 hours of sleep also showed

    a trend towards suggestibility.258 Furthermore, the experiment provided that sleep deprivation

    reduces individuals abilities to discriminate and detect discrepancies between original and

    misleading information.259

    As insinuated above, there are several limitations consistently found throughout

    laboratory experiments. For example, experimental research usually involves participants who

    have received some form of compensation, i.e. extra credit or money.260 Many skeptics also find

    252

    Id.253

    Id.254

    Id. at 49-50.

    255

    Id. at 50.256

    Blagrove,supra note 4, at 48, 53.257

    Id. at 48, 53.258

    Id.259

    Id.260

    35

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    36/38

    it difficult to relate psychological findings to real life scenarios.261 In this particular study,

    Blagrove concedes that sleep deprivation could increase the probability of a truthful confession

    because sleep deprivation reduces cognitive and motivational consequences.262

    The results of this experiment have major implications on the interrogation techniques

    used on suspected terrorists.263 Although Blagrove concedes that sleep-deprivation may produce

    true confessions, the results of the experiment more favorably suggest that the use of sleep-

    deprivation as a form of torture negatively impacts the interrogation process.264 Specifically, the

    longer a suspected terrorist goes without sleep, the more likely that individual will be susceptible

    to interrogative suggestibility.

    265

    Thus, during an interrogation concerning suspected terrorists,

    individuals who have gone without sleep are less able to discern between accurate and

    misleading information.266 Furthermore, the United Nations findings indicate that the United

    States has implemented sleep-deprivation as an interrogation technique.267 Therefore, it is

    reasonable to infer that through the use of sleep-deprivation as a form of torture that the United

    States has manipulated suspected terrorists into believing information that is inaccurate.

    See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4; Blagrove,supra note 4; Kassin et al.,supra note 4; Russano et al.,supra note5.261

    See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4; Blagrove,supra note 4; Kassin et al.,supra note 4.262

    Blagrove,supra note 4, at 56.

    263 See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4; Blagrove,supra note 4; Kassin et al.,supra note 4.264

    See Blagrove,supra note 4.265

    See id.266

    See id.267

    See U.N. Doc. A/61/40,supra note 2, at 84, 13.

    36

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    37/38

    As previously mentioned, many individuals suggest that empirical research cannot be

    generalized to real-life.268 However, even the United States government has conceded that

    coercive methods have their limitations.269 In the 1950s, the CIA began conducting field trials

    and research to investigate human behavior and psychology.270 This work was conducted

    to find new ways of gathering information through the interrogation of detainees.271

    As a result of this research, two manuals were produced by the CIA.272 The first manual,

    was created in 1963 and is entitledKUBARKCounterintelligence Interrogation. The second

    manual, known as theHuman Resource Exploitation Training Manual, was created in 1983.273

    The second manual refers to the futility of coercion: [e]experience indicates that the use of force

    is not necessary to gain cooperation of sources. Use of force is a poor technique, yields

    unreliable results, may damage subsequent collection efforts, and can induce the source to say

    what he thinks the interrogator wants to hear.274 Furthermore, the KUBARK manual makes

    specific references to the phenomenon of false confessions.275 Intense pain is quite likely to

    produce false confessions, concocted as a means of escaping from distress. The research of the

    government and the subsequent manuals help further the reliability on empirical research of false

    confessions and interrogative suggestibility in the context of torture.

    268

    See Kassin & Kiechel,supra note 4;see Blagrove,supra note 4;see Kassin et al.,supra note 4;see Russano etal.,supra note 5.269

    Rumney,supra note 114, at 493.270

    Id. at 492.

    271Id.272

    Id.273

    Id.274

    Rumney,supra note 114, at 492.275

    Id. at 494.

    37

  • 8/9/2019 Palermo Final

    38/38

    CONCLUSION

    Torture is morally reprehensible and also futile. It is prohibited throughout the

    international community and many nation-states have enacted legislation domestically

    prohibiting it. However, there is abundant evidence indicating that torture is still occurring

    throughout the world. The question remains why.

    The previously mentioned research strongly suggests that using torture in ineffective.

    For one reason, using torture as an interrogation tool produces inaccurate information such as

    false confessions. Additionally, specific techniques of torture, such as sleep-deprivation, cause

    interrogative suggestibility. In plain language, an individual being tortured will say anything to

    stop the pain.

    When dealing with something as grave as the War on Terror, it is easy to dehumanize

    suspected terrorists. It is almost as easy to treat them cruelly, and perhaps submit them to torture

    in the hopes of saving innocent lives. It is important to keep in mind that these aresuspected

    terrorists, and whether they have committed acts of terrorism is undetermined. The United States

    presumes innocence until proven guilty and this principle should apply universally.

    Therefore, states should not use torture under any justification. Not only does the

    international community proscribe torture, empirical research shows that it does not work.

    Specifically, the United States would be better off by complying with international and domestic

    law because it would procure more accurate information from suspects if it used more reliable

    techniques. By not following international law, the United States creates a risk that its citizens

    might receive the same treatment from other countries using torture. Finally, states must enact

    legislation and ensure that they enforce existing legislation prohibiting torture, even if it means

    punishing their own citizens.