perceiving musical stability the effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise

Upload: monta-tupcijenko

Post on 20-Feb-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    1/15

    Journal

    of Experimental Psychology:

    Human

    Perception and

    Performance

    1997, Vol.

    23, No. 3,808-822

    Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

    00%-15

    PerceivingMusical Stability:

    The

    Effect

    of

    TonalStructure,

    Rhythm,

    andMusical Expertise

    Emmanuel

    Bigand

    Universitfi

    deBourgogne

    The

    goal

    of thisstudywas to investigate

    several

    factorsthat

    determine

    musicalstabilityin

    unaccompanied tonalmelodies.Following M. R.Jones's (1987)theoryofdynamic

    attending,

    theauthorassumed that strongly accentedtonesact as stablemelodic

    reference

    points.Three

    mainresults

    wereobserved:

    (a)

    tonalstructure,

    rhythm,and

    melodic

    factors(i.e.,

    pitch skips

    or

    change in melodic contour) all contributed to

    defining

    the

    stability

    experiencedon the

    melodictones;(b) alinearcombination of 5 melodic and

    rhythmicfeatures

    provided a good

    fit to thestability

    ratings;

    and (c)

    some

    of

    thesefeatures

    contributed

    differently,depending

    on the

    extent

    of musicalexpertiseof the

    participants.

    The

    results

    are

    interpretedwithin

    C. L.

    Krumhansl's

    (1990) modelof

    tonal perception

    andJones'stheory of

    dynamic

    attending.

    Musical

    pieces

    are most commonly perceived to be

    sound

    structures matevolvedynamically through

    time:

    Atcertain

    points in a musicalpiece,we can evaluate whether the piece

    is

    finished, when

    it

    will reach

    its

    final point,

    what the

    forthcomingevents should be, and when they should occur

    intime. Thepresentstudy investigated severalfactorsthat

    cause musical

    piecesto

    sound like dynamic structures

    for

    listeners.

    According to music theorists, the impression that music

    progressesdynamically through time stems primarily

    from

    thesequence of stable and unstable events in time (Lerdahl

    &Jackendoff, 1983; Meyer, 1956, 1973; Schenker,1935).

    Ina given musical context, some events (tones or chords)

    are perceived to be more

    stable

    than others. Unstable events

    instill

    strong musical tensions that generate

    the

    expectancy

    thatmore stable events will follow. During the past decade,

    several empirical studies showed that musical stability and

    expectancy

    are

    influenced

    by

    several factors including

    me-

    lodic

    interval sizes

    (Bigand, Parncutt,

    & Lerdahl, 1996;

    Carlsen, 1981; Unyk& Carlsen, 1987), melodic contour

    (Schmuckler,1989,1990),rhythmic features

    (Boltz,

    1989a,

    1989b;Jones, Boltz, & Klein, 1993; Palmer &Krumhansl,

    1987a, 1987b), and tonal and harmonic structures (Bha-

    rucha&Stoeckig,1986, 1987; Bigand etal.,1996; Palmer

    & Krumhansl, 1987a, 1987b; Schmuckler,

    1989,

    1990;

    Schmuckler & Boltz, 1994). To date, none of these studies

    hasmanipulated these factors simultaneouslytodetermine

    their relative weighting. Therefore,

    the

    first goal

    of the

    present study was to determine how tonal structureand

    rhythmact in

    combination

    toaffect theperceived musical

    stability

    in

    relatively long

    and

    complex melodies.

    The

    sec-

    ondgoal was to test a quantitative model designed to predict

    IthankM.-C.Botte, C.Drake,C.Krumhansl,P. Lemaire, S.

    McAdams,

    and B.Repp for their

    insightful comments,

    which

    greatly improved thearticle.

    Correspondence concerningthis

    article

    should be addressed to

    EmmanuelBigand,

    LEAD-CNRS

    URA1838,6 Boulevard Gab-

    riel,

    Facult6

    des

    Sciences,F-21000Dijon,France.

    Electronic

    mail

    may be

    sent

    viaInternetto [email protected].

    the degree of stability experienced with each tone of a

    melody.

    Let

    us

    considereach

    of these melodic and

    rhythmic

    factors in

    greater detail

    and see how we can

    quantify their

    respective effects.

    Tonal Hierarchy

    Musical stability isrelated,in part, to thescaledegree of

    the

    tones

    in the

    melody's underlying diatonic scale. Accord-

    ing

    to

    Meyer

    (1956),

    Inthe

    majormode

    in Western

    music

    the tonic

    tone

    is the

    tone of ultimate rest toward whichall other

    tones

    tend to

    move.

    On the

    next

    higher

    level

    the third and

    fifth

    of the

    scale,

    though active

    melodic

    tones

    relative to the

    tonic,

    join

    the

    tonic

    as structural

    tones; and all the

    othertones,whether

    diatonic or

    chromatic,tendtoward one of these,(pp. 214-215)

    Sucha tonal hierarchy has received a great deal of empirical

    support:

    Once the key of a musical context is recognized,

    the 12 tones of the chromatic scales are perceived in a

    hierarchy of stability (Frances, 1958/1988; Krumhansl,

    1990).

    In

    major

    key

    contexts,

    the tonic

    (firstscale

    degree) is

    perceived

    to be

    more stable than

    the

    dominant(fifth scale

    degree), which

    in

    turn

    is

    perceived

    to be

    more stable than

    the mediant (third scale degree). Tones of the other scale

    degrees

    are

    perceived

    to be

    less stable

    but

    more stable than

    nondiatonic

    tones.

    Stable tones

    in a

    given musical context

    act as

    cognitive

    reference points to which the other tones are anchored

    (Bharucha,1984; Krumhansl, 1990; Laden, 1994). Chang-

    ing the key of thecontext leadstochangesinthesereference

    points

    so

    that events that were stable

    in a

    given context

    may

    become unstableinanotherkeycontext.As anexample,the

    two-tone sequence

    E-F

    instills

    a

    dynamic tension

    to

    relax-

    ationrelation in the key context of Fmajor:E is perceived

    to be an

    unstable tone

    and F, a

    verystable one. This

    stability

    profile maychange withthecontext.In the keycontextsof

    E

    major

    and E

    minor,

    E isperceivedto be

    morestablethan

    F. In

    these contexts,

    the two

    tonesdefine

    a new

    stability

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    2/15

    PERCEIVINGMUSICAL

    STABILITY

    809

    profile

    becausetheyinstill

    a relaxation to

    tension

    relation.

    Changes in the musical stability of the tones are of psycho-

    logical importance because they

    alter

    the dynamic shape of

    thesequence

    and, subsequently, modify

    its perceptual

    iden-

    tity.

    Indeed,

    Dowling(1986)showed that changing the key

    context of a six-tone melody makes that melodydifficultto

    recognize. Similar results were obtained by Bigand and

    Kneau(1996)with longer tone sequences.

    The

    connectionist model

    of tonal perception developed by

    Bharucha(1987)

    provides

    an

    elegant account

    for theeffects

    of

    key context on musical stability. In this model, the 12

    tones

    of thechromaticscalearelinkedtomajorandminor

    chords and to major and minor keys. Tonal hierarchies are

    represented by the strength of the

    connections

    that link tone

    units

    to

    chord

    and key

    units.

    Musicalstabilityis instantiated

    bythe amount of activation that spreads from key units to

    tone units. The more activated tone units correspond with

    the more

    stable

    tones in a given context and vice versa.

    Changing

    the key

    context would

    modify the

    amount

    of

    activation propagating to the tone units, so that a unit that is

    strongly activated in one context would be less activated in

    another.

    The first

    purpose

    of thepresent studywas totesthow key

    context effects

    may

    affect

    the

    perception

    of

    melodies

    shar-

    ing

    a long set of pitches in the same sequential order. Let us

    consider,for example, the

    melodies

    ofFigure1.Tlmelo-

    dies are in A minor. To change the musical stability, the key

    was

    changed in T2 by altering the pitches of the first three

    notes. According

    to

    Butler

    and

    Brown

    (1984),the

    tritone

    F#-Cinstillsastrong feeling of the key of G in T2. To

    maintain thiskeyinterpretation,the G#s (Numbers 1 and

    20) were lowered by a

    semitone.

    All the remaining notes are

    thesamein Tl and

    T2;

    theonly difference concerned their

    stability. According to tonal hierarchy,

    the

    A in Tl (Number

    2)

    is a stable tonic tone, but a more unstable tone (second

    scale degree) in

    T2.

    The B (Number 3) is an unstable second

    scale degreein Tl, but amore stable third degreein

    T2.

    By

    contrast,

    the C

    (Number

    4) is a

    relatively

    stable

    third

    degree

    in

    Tl,but a more unstablefourthdegree inT2.By changing

    a few

    notes,

    the

    stability

    of the

    melodic tones

    has

    been

    completely

    inverted

    fromTl to T2.

    Rhythmic

    Structures

    Thestability

    of

    tones

    is not

    determined entirely

    by the

    tonal hierarchy of the

    melody's

    underlying key. Varying

    some acoustic features

    (i.e.,

    timbre, intensity, duration) can

    alter stability and, thus, modify

    perceptionof thepiece.

    This

    suggests that other features should be included in a model of

    tonal hierarchy so as to account for the perception of mel-

    odies

    (Boltz,

    1989b;Laden, 1994).

    Generally, the stability of a note depends on the rhythmic

    context in which it appears. According to Lerdahl and

    Jackendoff

    (1983),

    rhythmic structures have a double

    func-

    tion.First,they divide the

    piece

    into groups and, then, they

    addrhythmic values to the tonal hierarchies to determinethe

    musical

    function

    of the events contained in the groups.

    234567 8

    9101112131415 16171819202122 23

    Stop

    notes

    1 23 4567 8

    9101112131415

    16171819202122 23

    T1R2

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 891011121314 15161718192021

    2223

    Stop

    notes

    1234567

    891011121314 15161718192021

    2223

    Figure 1.

    Four melodies used

    inExperiment1. The tonal structure (T) varies

    from

    Tl to Tl, and

    the rhythmic structure(R) variesfromRlto R2.

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    3/15

    810

    BIGAND

    From

    a

    cognitive point

    of

    view,

    the

    function

    of

    rhythm

    may

    be to

    create temporal accents that capture

    the

    listeners'

    attention and emphasize certain relationships among the

    tones of a melody

    (Boltz,

    1991a, 1993a, 1993b; Jones,

    1987). Jones, Boltz,

    and Kidd

    (1982) reported that

    it is

    easier

    to discern pitch changes in a melody when these

    changes occur on temporal accents. Changing the rhythm of

    a

    melody modifiesthemannerinwhich attentionis

    guided

    through time, such that tones that capture attention in one

    rhythmic

    context

    failto do so in

    another rhythmic context

    andvice versa (Boltz,1989a,1991a;Jones& Boltz, 1989;

    Jones

    &

    Ralston, 1991;

    Jones,

    Summerell,

    &

    Marshburn,

    1987).

    The second purpose of the present experiments was to

    investigate how a change in rhythm affects the stability

    perceived with each tone of long melody. I assumed that

    tones that

    receive

    rhythmic accents should

    be

    perceived

    to

    be stable melodic referencepointsbecause they can capture

    the

    listeners' attention. Changing

    the

    rhythm

    of a

    melody

    wouldshiftthe rhythmic accents and then alter the overall

    musical

    stability profile for this melody. Previous results

    reported

    by

    Palmer

    and

    Krumhansl

    (1987a,

    1987b;

    see

    also

    Bigand, 1993) and, more recently, by Laden(1994)sup-

    ported this assumption.

    Integration

    of thePitchand Rhythmic Structures

    Given that both pitch and rhythmic structures govern the

    perception

    of

    tonal melodies,

    the

    third goal

    of the

    present

    study

    was to

    investigate

    how

    these structures

    act in

    combi-

    nation.

    Peretzand

    Kolinsky (1993) distinguished

    two ac-

    counts of theprocessing

    of

    pitchandrhythmic structures.

    One isthat pitchandrhythmare twoseparate perceptual

    components,

    so

    that changing

    one of

    them

    in a

    melody

    would

    not

    necessarily interfere with

    the

    processing

    of the

    other. This

    two-component

    model

    was

    supported

    by

    several

    studies in neuropsychology (Peretz, 1990; Peretz & Kolin-

    sky,

    1993; Peretz &

    Morais,

    1989) and other empirical

    studies

    (Halpern, 1984; Monahan & Carterette, 1985;

    Monanan, Kendall, & Carterette, 1987; Palmer &

    Krum-

    hansl,1987a,1987b; Smith & Cuddy,1989).

    A

    second possibility

    is

    that pitch

    and

    rhythm

    are

    pro-

    cessed integrally,

    so

    that

    a

    change

    in one

    affects

    the

    per-

    ception

    of the

    melody

    as a

    whole. This single-component

    modelwassupportedbyseveral studies askingthepartici-

    pants

    to

    recallmelodies (Boltz, 1991a; Boltz

    &

    Jones, 1986;

    Deutsch,

    1980), torecognize

    melodies (Boltz,

    1993a;

    Jones,

    Summerell, & Marshbum, 1987; Jones & Ralston,1991),to

    evaluate

    how complete a melody is (Boltz, 1989a, 1989b),

    or to

    estimate

    the

    duration

    of

    melodies (Boltz, 1991b,

    1993b; Jones, Boltz, & Klein, 1993).According to Boltz

    (1989a, 1989b), the strongest evidence for a single-

    component model comesfromthe presence of interactions

    between pitch and rhythmic structure. For example, Boltz

    (1989b)

    foundthat melodies were judged

    to be

    more com-

    plete when ending

    on the

    dominant tone than

    when

    ending

    on

    the

    mediant tone,

    but

    only when

    the

    dominant tones were

    rhythmicallyaccented throughout

    the

    melody.

    The

    reverse

    phenomenon was observed when the mediant tones were

    rhythmically accented.

    In a

    similar vein, Boltz (1989a)

    argued that thedifferencein musical relaxation provided by

    certain pitch intervals may vary strongly depending on

    whether or not these intervals occur on time, relative to the

    preceding temporal context. Consistent data were reported

    bySchmuckler and Boltz(1994)with chord

    sequences

    and

    by

    Boltz (1993a) in a recognition task. The two sets of

    conflicting results are

    difficult

    to integrate within a single

    framework.

    A

    third possibility suggested by Peretz and Kolinsky

    (1993)is that pitch and rhythm are treated independently in

    an earlier stage of melodicprocessingand are then fused

    into

    a

    unified dimension

    in a

    second stage

    of

    processing.

    However, the question remains of specifying whether inte-

    gration that may occur at a second stage of processing relies

    on an additive

    or

    on an interactive combination ofboth

    structures. No definite answer to this question seems to be

    available currently. Thus,

    the

    third purpose

    of the

    present

    studywas to investigate this issuefurtherin an experimental

    design that

    differed

    in

    several regards

    from

    those that

    are

    commonly used.

    First,two rhythmic structures werecrossedwith two pitch

    structures

    in a 2 X 2

    factorial design. Second,

    the

    melodies

    differed

    in

    tonal structure

    but not in

    melodic features (i.e.,

    pitch skips and melodic contour), which were kept constant

    (see Figure 1). Third, the degree of musical stability expe-

    rienced

    by

    listeners was registered at several locations in the

    melodies. Such a design allowed us to determine whether

    the

    interactive effect occurs

    on

    only

    one or two

    specific

    tonesof themelodyor on themajorityof themelodic tones.

    Predicting

    Perceived Musical Stability

    As

    stated by Boltz(1989b),regardless of their additive or

    interactive nature, significanteffects ofboth rhythmicand

    tonal structures would demonstrate

    "the

    need to incorporate

    dynamic pattern influences into models of tonal perception

    (p.12).Thefourthgoalof thepresentstudywas toaddress

    this issue in defining a model that would predict the effect

    of several melodic

    and

    temporal factors

    on the

    perceived

    musical stability. The rationale for the present model was

    provided primarily

    by

    Jones (1987)

    and

    Jones

    and

    Boltz

    (1989).

    According to Jones (1987), pitch and rhythmic structures

    display several kindsofmelodic (in) and temporal (t) ac-

    cents. The manner in which these accents are temporally

    phased creates a

    unified

    accent structure (called Jointaccent

    structure) that dynamically orients

    the

    listeners'attention.

    According

    to

    Jones

    and

    Boltz (1989)

    the

    dynamic

    shapeof

    a musical sequence

    refers

    to the influence that thejoint

    accent

    structure

    has on the

    attentionaltrajectories developed

    during

    the listening of thepiece.The main property of a

    joint accent structure is that it contains melodic markers of

    different strengths: coupled

    or

    temporally coincident

    ac-

    cents (m plus f) yield stronger markers than

    do

    single

    accents

    (m

    or t), (Jones, 1987,p.625).Thestrengthof a

    marker

    was

    determined

    by the

    number

    of

    accents that

    co-

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    4/15

    PERCEIVING MUSICAL STABILITY

    811

    incide in time (Jones &

    Boltz,

    1989).Changingthe

    rhythm

    ofa

    melody changes

    the

    accent coupling, such

    that

    strong

    markers in one rhythmic structure may be weak markers in

    another and

    vice versa. Consequently, these changes should

    alter

    the

    dynamic shape

    of the

    sequence

    in

    that they

    modify

    the

    modes of attentional following in listeners. Changing

    thepitch structure provokes similar changes in the strength

    ofthe markers in thejointaccent structure and, thus, in the

    dynamic shape of the sequence.

    In the

    present study,

    the

    influence

    of

    several melodic

    rhythmic features

    on the dynamic shape of a melody was

    addressed by considering the

    effects

    of these

    features

    on the

    perceived musicalstabilityprofiles. Stability profilerefers

    to the alternation of stable and less stable tones in the

    melody. Strong markers were assumed to act as very stable

    melodic reference points because of the presumably strong

    attraction

    that they exert on the

    listeners' attention.

    The

    computed strengths of accentuation for each tone of a mel-

    ody should then provide a good fit to the perceived stability.

    Following Jones (1987),

    Jones

    and Boltz

    (1989),

    Se-

    rafine,

    Classman,

    and

    Overbeeke

    (1989), and Bigand

    (1993), five predictors were used: tonal

    weight,

    duration

    weight, metric weight, interval size weight, and melodic

    contour weight. The tonal weights, defined according to

    Krumhansl's(1990)model of tonal perception,

    reflect

    the

    importance of each tone in the tonal hierarchy of the mel-

    ody's

    underlying key. Following

    Krumhansl

    and

    Kessler's

    findings

    (1982,Experiment 2), when part of a musical piece

    suggests

    a new key

    (temporary modulation),

    the

    present

    model assigns to this part of thepiecethe key profile values

    of

    this

    new

    temporary

    key

    (see Experiment

    2). How the

    tonal

    weights (TW) were defined

    for the

    melodies

    of Ex-

    periment 1 (Figure 1) is displayed in Table 1, where they are

    shown to come from the major and minor key profiles

    reportedby

    Krumhansl

    and

    Kessler (1982)

    and Krumhansl

    (1990, p. 30). Because the main key of the

    Tl

    melodies is

    A

    minor, the values of Tl melodies are those of

    Krum-

    hansl's A minor

    profile.

    The values of T2 melodies are

    those of KmmhansFs G

    major

    profile. Illustratively, the

    tonal weight

    of the A

    (number

    2 in

    Figure

    1) is

    6.33

    in Tl

    because A is the tonic tone. The tonal weight for the same

    A

    in T2 is 3.48 because the A is the second degree in this

    key

    context and so on. Changes in values reflect the hypo-

    thetical changes in musical stability created by the two key

    contexts in

    which

    the tones appear. Not surprisingly, there

    was

    a high negative correlation between the Tl and the T2

    values, K21)

    = -.81, p

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    5/15

    812

    BIGAND

    melodic interval was computed

    (MIW

    valuesin

    Table

    1).

    To account for thepotentialinfluenceof change inmelodic

    contour, a value of 1 was assigned to the tones onwhicha

    change in

    contouroccurs.

    Avalueof 0 was

    assigned

    in the

    othercases

    (MCW

    values in Table 1).

    To summarize, we hypothesizethatseveral

    factors

    may

    affect theperceptionofstability.Two ofthese(i.e.,tonal

    structure and

    rhythm)

    were varied systematically in die

    following

    experiments

    and theothers(intervalsize,

    melodic

    contour)

    werekeptconstant.

    Three

    main

    assumptions

    were

    made:

    1.

    changing

    the

    tonal structure

    of a

    melody

    by

    modifyingonly

    a few

    pitches should stronglyaffect

    the

    stability

    for the

    majorityof the

    other

    tones;

    2. changing the rhythmic structure should also

    affect

    the

    stability of themelodic tones;and

    3. acombinationof fivekindsofmelodic andrhythmicac-

    cents(i.e.,tonal, duration, metric, interval size, and me-

    lodic contour weights) should predict the perceived

    stability.

    Inaddition,giventhat someofthesefactors rely onmore

    superficial

    features(duration,melodicintervalsizes,change

    in melodic contour) than others (tonal and metric struc-

    tures), some

    may be

    more important

    for

    musicians

    and

    some

    for nonmusicians. Thus, testing

    these

    factors

    provided

    an

    opportunity

    to investigate theeffectof

    musical expertise

    on

    theperceptionofmusicalstability.

    Experiment1

    Method

    Participants.

    Forty music teachers (hereinafter referred

    to as

    musicians)volunteered toparticipatein theexperiment.Allheld

    qualificationsfrom a French Music Conservatory and had received

    atleast

    10

    years

    of

    intensive training

    in

    music(i.e.,music theory,

    ear

    training,

    and

    instrumental performance). Fortycollege-age

    students(hereinafter referred to as nonmusicians) also took part

    hi

    theexperiment.Theyhadnever playedaninstrumentnorstudied

    music.Allwere students of psychology and participated in the

    experiment as

    volunteers.

    Material.

    Four melodies T1R1, T1R2, T2R1,

    and T2R2

    were

    defined(Figure 1). The

    Tl

    melodies

    differed

    from the T2 melodies

    by

    the key

    context induced

    by the

    firstmelodic tones. Following

    Palmer and

    Krumhansl (1987a, 1987b),

    the R2

    melodies were

    defined by

    shifting

    the

    rhythmic structure

    of

    Rl

    by one

    eighth

    note. Consequently, mostof thetones thatfallon astrong metric

    beat

    in Rl

    fall

    on a

    weak metric beat

    in R2 and

    vice versa. Several

    tones with

    a

    long duration

    in Rl

    have

    a

    short duration

    in R2 and

    viceversa. These four melodies were segmented into 23 frag-

    ments.Each segment started

    at the

    beginning

    of the

    melody

    and

    stopped on a

    different

    note called a slop note.Forexample, in

    T1R1, thefirstfragmentstoppedonG#,thesecondon A, and so

    on.

    The 23 fragments

    were played with sampled piano sounds

    produced

    by the

    EMT1O Yamaha Sound Expander

    at a

    tempo

    of

    80

    quarter notes

    per

    minute.

    The Yamaha

    sampler

    was

    controlled

    through a

    MIDI interface

    by a

    Macintosh computer running Per-

    former

    software. Velocity,

    aparameter related to the

    force

    with

    which a key is

    struck,

    was

    held constant

    for all

    pitches.

    The

    participants

    were allowedtoadjusttheoutputof theamplifierto a

    comfortable

    level. There

    was no

    silence

    between

    the

    offset

    of a

    tone and the onset of the succeeding tone.

    Task.

    The

    participants' task, similar

    to

    that used

    by

    Palmer

    and

    Krumhansl(1987a,

    1987b),

    consistedof

    evaluating

    the

    musical

    stability on each stop note using a judgment scale ranging from 1

    (very

    low

    stability)

    to 7

    (very great stability).

    In a preliminary

    session,the participantslistenedto musical examples having dif-

    ferent

    degrees

    of

    stability. They were told that strong stability

    at

    the end of a fragment

    evokes

    the

    feeling that

    the

    melody could

    naturallystop

    at

    this point.

    On the

    other hand,

    low

    stability evokes

    the

    feeling

    that there must be a continuation of the melody.

    Because stability

    may be

    varied

    in

    subtle ways

    in

    musk,

    the

    participants were encouraged to use all thedegreesof the judgment

    scale.In addition, at the end of the experiment, the musicians were

    required

    to

    identify

    the key of the

    melody that they

    had

    listened

    to

    and toidentifyitsmeter (i.e., theyhad todrawthe barlineson a

    score).

    Design and procedure. Four independent groupsof 10partic-

    ipants each were assigned

    to the

    fourconditions defined

    by

    cross-

    ingthe two independent variables: Key context (2) x Rhythm (2).

    Each participant gave

    a

    rating

    for

    each

    of the 23

    stop notes

    of

    only

    one

    melody.

    Theparticipants

    were seated

    in

    groups

    of

    two, each

    in

    front

    of a

    MIDI keyboard, connected

    to the

    computer, which

    recorded their answers. Seven keys corresponded to the 7 points of

    thejudgment

    scale.

    Before running the experiment, the partici-

    pants practiced evaluation of musical stability with 12 melodic

    fragments. No

    feedback

    was

    provided

    unless

    they used

    the

    scale

    in

    thereverse order.Theexperiment started withthe

    fragment

    ter-

    minating on

    Stop Note

    1. The

    participants

    had 8 s to

    give their

    stability judgment.

    After

    that,

    the fragment

    terminating

    on

    Stop

    Note

    2 was

    played, then

    the

    fragmentending

    on

    Stop Note

    3, and

    so on.

    This makes

    the

    judgments

    collectedon

    stop notes compa-

    rable because

    all the

    stop notes were judged when

    they

    were

    first

    heard. As observed by Bigand

    (1993,

    Experiment 1) random

    presentation

    of the fragments greatly confuses the participants

    because it

    oftenappears that

    a

    shortfragmentfollows

    a

    longer one.

    Inthis case,

    the

    degree

    of

    completeness

    is

    defined

    not

    only

    by

    musicalstabilitybutalsoby the

    listener's

    knowledgeof thecon-

    tinuation of the melody. As a consequence, the participants (no-

    tably

    the

    nonmusicians) tended

    to

    give higher

    ratings of

    stability

    formusicalfragments

    of

    increasing duration. This suggested that

    for

    them,

    regardless of the

    musicalfunction

    of the

    last tone,

    the

    longer

    the fragment, the

    higher

    its

    degree

    of

    completeness. This

    linear tendencywas notobserved whenthe fragmentswere pre-

    sented

    in a

    sequential order (Bigand, 1993, Experiment

    2).

    Results

    Participantagreement.

    To

    assess

    interparticipant

    agree-

    ment,

    the Kendall

    coefficient

    ofconcordancewas computed

    separately

    for the

    musicians

    and the nonmusicians in

    each

    experimental situation.Theobserved coefficients were all

    significant (p

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    6/15

    PERCEIVING

    MUSICAL

    STABILITY

    813

    indicating

    the

    reliability

    of the

    experimental method.

    The

    lowest coefficientwasobservedfor themusiciansinT1R1,

    r(21)

    = .77,p

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    7/15

    814

    BIGAND

    Table2

    Correlation

    (Pearson r) ComparingTl and T2Ratings

    WithKrumhansl s Key Profiles

    r for.

    Participants

    Musicians

    Nonmusicians

    Key

    A

    minor

    Gmajor

    Aminor

    G

    major

    Tl

    .69**

    -.45*

    .53**

    -.28

    T2

    -.54**

    .58**

    -.42*

    .40

    *p

  • 7/24/2019 Perceiving Musical Stability the Effect of Tonal Structure, Rhythm, And Musical Expertise

    8/15

    PERCEIVING MUSICAL STABILITY

    815

    Musicians Nonmusicians

    Strong

    beat

    oWeak

    beat Strong beat

    oWeak

    beat

    notes

    Z 3 4 5 6 9 1 11 12 13 16 17 18 19 2 3 4 S 6 9 1 11 12 13 16 17 18 19

    2

    Musicians

    Nonmusicians

    Quarter

    note oEighth note Qyarternote

    o

    Eighth note

    14

    5

    21

    Figure

    3.

    Effects ofmetric(a) and

    duration

    (b)on

    musical stability.

    average ratings. However, additional analysis revealed a

    significant interaction between tonal weights and the

    extent

    of

    musical expertise,

    t

    = 3.22,

    p