perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … university sargent college of health and...

62
Boston University OpenBU http://open.bu.edu Theses & Dissertations Boston University Theses & Dissertations 2015 Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible nasal emission in cleft palate speakers Downing, Kerri http://hdl.handle.net/2144/13658 Boston University

Upload: trandan

Post on 26-Mar-2018

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

Boston University

OpenBU http://open.bu.edu

Theses & Dissertations Boston University Theses & Dissertations

2015

Perceptual judgment of

hypernasality and audible nasal

emission in cleft palate speakers

Downing, Kerri

http://hdl.handle.net/2144/13658

Boston University

Page 2: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

   

BOSTON UNIVERSITY

SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES

Thesis

PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND

AUDIBLE NASAL EMISSION IN CLEFT PALATE SPEAKERS

by

KERRI DOWNING

B.A., University of Connecticut, 2010

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the

requirements for the degree of

Master of Science

2015

Page 3: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

   

© 2015 by KERRI DOWNING All rights reserved

Page 4: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

   

Approved by First Reader _________________________________________________________ Cara E. Stepp, Ph.D.

Assistant Professor of Speech, Language & Hearing Sciences Assistant Professor of Biomedical Engineering Second Reader _________________________________________________________ Barbara B. Oppenheimer, M.A., CCC-SLP Clinical Associate Professor Third Reader _________________________________________________________ Melanie L. Matthies, Ph.D.

Senior Associate Dean Associate Professor of Speech, Language & Hearing Sciences

Page 5: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

iv  

PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND

AUDIBLE NASAL EMISSION IN CLEFT PALATE SPEAKERS

KERRI DOWNING

ABSTRACT  

Objective: The purpose of this study is to determine whether a novel, user-

friendly rating system, visual sort and rate (VSR) provides comparable ratings to the

currently used direct magnitude estimation (DME) rating system for rating perceptions of

audible nasal emission (ANE) and hypernasality in cleft palate speakers.

Methods: Twelve naïve listeners rated 152 speech samples of speakers with cleft

palate across four conditions: rating hypernasality and ANE using either a VSR or DME

rating scale. Raters were provided with a short training session, prior to rating each day.

Inter- and intra-rater reliabilities, as well the line of best fit between scores using VSR

and scores using DME was calculated to determine usability of VSR as a novel rating

system.

Results: Direct magnitude estimation resulted in the highest levels of inter-rater

reliability, when rating hypernasality (DME r= .48; VSR r=.14), as well as ANE (DME

r= .27; VSR r=.15). Most raters demonstrated high intra-rater reliabilities across

conditions. A curvilinear line of best fit most accurately captured the relationship

between DME and VSR scores when rating hypernasality (r=.64) and ANE (r=.66).

Conclusions: A curvilinear relationship between ratings suggests that both

variables are prothetic, and therefore, best captured using a DME rating scale (Eadie &

Doyle, 2002). The use of DME is supported for continued use rating hypernasality, even

Page 6: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

v  

amongst naïve listeners given a training session. Rating ANE was difficult, as ratings

yielded low inter-rater reliabilities, regardless of the scale used. Further research

regarding perceptions of audible nasal emission is warranted.

Page 7: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

vi  

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ....................................................................................................................... iv  

TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................... vi  

LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix  

LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ x  

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS ............................................................................................ xi  

BACKGROUND ................................................................................................................ 1  

Prevalence and Characteristics of Cleft Palate ............................................................... 1  

Resonance Patterns in Individuals with Palatal Cleft Repair .......................................... 2  

Management of Cleft Palate ............................................................................................ 2  

Nasal Air Emission ......................................................................................................... 4  

Nasometry as an Objective Measure of Nasal Resonance .............................................. 5  

Perceptual Judgments of Nasal Resonance ..................................................................... 7  

Inter- and Intra- Rater Reliability ................................................................................... 9  

How Experience Affects Perceptual Judgments ........................................................... 10

Hypernasality and Nasal Emission as a Coninuum? .................................................... 11

Methodologies Used to Collect Perceptual Ratings ...................................................... 12  

Aligning Perceptual Judgments with Objective Measurements ................................... 15  

STIMULI .......................................................................................................................... 16

Sample Collection ......................................................................................................... 16  

Page 8: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

vii  

Sample Selection ........................................................................................................... 18  

Participants .................................................................................................................... 19  

PROCEDURES ................................................................................................................ 20

Training ......................................................................................................................... 20  

Perceptual Ratings ........................................................................................................ 21  

STATISTICS .................................................................................................................... 24

Inter- and Intra-rater Reliability of DME vs. VSR ....................................................... 24  

Does VSR Supply Valid Esimates of ANE and Hypernasality .................................... 25  

RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 26

Intra-rater Reliability .................................................................................................... 26  

Inter-rater Reliability .................................................................................................... 27  

Does VSR Supply Valid Esimates of ANE and Hypernasality .................................... 30  

DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 31

Validity and Reliability of Scores ................................................................................. 31  

Benefits of a Training Session ...................................................................................... 33  

ANE and Hypernasality as Prothetic Variables ............................................................ 34  

Use of VSR as a Novel Scale ........................................................................................ 35  

Limitations .................................................................................................................... 36  

CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................. 36

APPENDIX ....................................................................................................................... 38

Appendix A: Training Materials ................................................................................... 38  

Page 9: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

viii  

BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................. 40

VITA…………………………………………………………………………………….. 50

 

 

Page 10: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

ix  

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1. Speaker Characteristics ....................................................................................... 17

Table 2. Inter-rater Reliability. ......................................................................................... 28

Table 3. Mean Differences in VSR Ratings ...................................................................... 29  

 

 

Page 11: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

x  

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Training Condition ............................................................................................ 21  

Figure 2. DME Condition ................................................................................................. 22  

Figure 3. VSR Condition .................................................................................................. 23

Figure 4. Assigning Numerical Values in VSR Condition ............................................... 24

Figure 5. Pearson Correlations .......................................................................................... 27

Figure 6. Linear and Polnomial Models ........................................................................... 31  

Page 12: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

   

xi  

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

ANE ................................................................................................ Audible Nasal Emission

DME ........................................................................................ Direct Magnitude Estimation

VSR ...................................................................................................... Visual Sort and Rate

Page 13: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

1  

 

Background:

Prevalence and Characteristics of Cleft Palate:

Craniofacial defects including cleft lip and palate, are one of the most common

birth defects (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2013). Estimates from

30 countries around the world, indicate that cleft palate is a prevalent, worldwide

occurrence (“Prevalence at Birth of Cleft Lip With or Without Cleft Palate,” 2011).

Therefore, classifying speech abnormalities in this population is of importance.

According to Kummer (2008), a cleft is described as, “an abnormal opening of a fissure

in an anatomical structure that is normally closed” (p.37). These abnormal openings can

affect the complete formation of the lip or palate resulting in cleft lip and/or palate,

resulting in velopharyngeal insufficiency.

Velopharyngeal insufficiency is defined as a structural or functional abnormality

that prevents velopharyngeal closure (Kummer, 2008). Individuals usually opt for

corrective surgery; however, even after surgery, individuals often continue to experience

velopharyngeal insufficiency (Timmons, Wyatt, & Murphy, 2001), incompetence

(neuromotor or physiological disorder that prevents the proper velopharyngeal

movement), or mislearning (incorrect velopharyngeal movement caused by articulation

problems). Velopharyngeal dysfunction is an umbrella term which includes insufficiency,

incompetency, and mislearning. Velopharyngeal dysfunction (VPD) is defined as, “a

condition where the velopharyngeal valve does not close consistently and completely

during the production of oral sounds” (Kummer, 2008, p. 178). Velopharyngeal

dysfunction can cause: hypernasal speech, hyponasal speech, audible nasal emission

Page 14: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

2  

 

(ANE), cul-de-sac resonance, and/or compensatory articulations (Kummer, 2008).

Resonance Patterns in Individuals with Palatal Cleft Repair:

Resonance, as described by Kuehn and Moller (2000), is described as, “…the

direct effect of coupling of the nasal space with the oral-pharyngeal space during vowel

and vocalic productions” (p.348–4). There are several common resonance patterns

amongst individuals with velopharyngeal dysfunction. According to Henningsson et al.

(2008), the five most important dimensions of cleft speech ratings are hypernasality,

hyponasality, nasal emission, compensatory articulations, and voice disorder.

Hypernasality, or the presence of increased nasal resonance during non-nasal sounds, is a

common result of velopharyngeal insufficiency. After cleft palate repair surgery,

children’s voices are still often perceived as hypernasal (Persson, Lohmander, & Elander,

2006). This becomes problematic as resonance problems can affect speech intelligibility,

thereby warranting further therapy (Kuehn & Moller, 2000; Persson et al., 2006). Even

five years after surgery, hypernasality remained significant when compared to controls

(Paliobei, Psifidis, & Anagnostopoulos, 2005).

Management of Cleft palate:

Velopharyngeal Insufficiency (VPI) can be corrected through prosthetic use,

surgery, and/or speech therapy (Kummer, 2008). Kummer states that while the use of

prosthetics previously was the predominant treatment for cleft-palate, surgery has

become more prominent (2008). Surgeries that correct deformities in the palate are called

palatoplasty, while any surgical procedure of the pharynx aimed to treat VPI is called

Page 15: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

3  

 

pharyngoplasty. Kummer suggests that repair of the palate typically requires two

surgeries to account for the growth of the palate through adulthood. The first of the two

surgeries typically occurs between 6 and 15 months of age.

Early repair is conducted to reduce velopharyngeal mislearning and compensatory

articulations (Kummer, 2008). Several types of surgeries exist for cleft palate repair

including: Von Langenbeck surgery, Wardill-Kilner surgery, intraveloplasty, and furlow

palatoplasty (Hopper, Tse, Smartt, Swanson, & Kinter, 2014; Woo, 2012). Secondary

surgeries, or pharyngeal wall augmentation, has been known to improve VPI (Kummer,

2008; Seagle, Mazaheri, Dixon-Wood, & Williams, 2002). Two types of pharyngoplasty,

pharyngeal flap and sphincter pharyngoplasty are used. Currently, the pharyngeal flap

palatoplasty is the most common surgical technique known to improve VPI (Armour,

Fischbach, Klaiman, & Fisher, 2005; Cable, Canady, Karnell, Karnell, & Malick, 2004).

Prosthetics have become less necessary as surgeries are typically completed at

younger ages (Gardner & Parr, 1996; Kummer, 2008). However, for those individuals

who do not qualify for surgery, prosthetics can be important for treating velopharyngeal

disorders. Three types of prosthetics can be used: palatal lift, palatal obturator, and a

speech bulb obturator. The palatal lift is useful for treating velopharyngeal incompetence,

as it aids the palate to lift during the production of oral sounds (Kummer, 2008). A palatal

obturator covers any openings in the palate to stop airflow through the open palate, and

through the nasal cavity. Lastly, a speech bulb obturator aids velopharyngeal

insufficiency, as it can fill the empty space between the velum and posterior pharyngeal

wall during velar closure.

Page 16: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

4  

 

As noted by Kummer (2008), speech therapy is useful once prosthetic or surgical

techniques have been completed to aid patients in managing the newly acquired oral

pressure. Similarly, compensatory articulations (e.g. pharyngeal fricatives, glottal stops,

and nasal shunting) may develop in children with VPI; speech therapists can resolve

compensatory articulations. Hardin-Jones and Jones (2005) found that sixty-seven

percent of preschoolers with repaired cleft-palate are currently enrolled or were

previously enrolled in speech therapy. Their findings indicated aberrant articulations

persisted after palatal repair including glottal stops, pharyngeal fricatives, middorsum

palatal stops, and nasal substitutions.

Nasal Air Emission:

Velopharyngeal insufficiency often results in nasal emission, or air leaking

through the nose during high pressure oral consonants such as /b/, /p/, and /d/ (Wyatt et

al., 1996). The most severe type of audible nasal emission (ANE) is typically called nasal

turbulence or rustle (Kummer, Curtis, Wiggs, Lee, & Strife, 1992). While resonance is

amongst one of the most studied voice variables in perceptual assessments of individuals

with cleft palate, nasal air emission is less studied, as fewer than half of recent studies

reviewed, assessed nasal emission (Baylis, Munson, & Moller, 2011; Lohmander &

Olsson, 2004). Nasal emission is more subtle than other measures of nasality and occurs

from a different velopharyngeal mechanism than hypernasality; therefore, further

research on nasal emission is warranted to better understand its effect on speech

intelligibility.

Page 17: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

5  

 

Hypernasality and nasal emission occur through different velopharyngeal

mechanisms. As stated by Sell, Harding, and Grunwell (1999), “hypernasality modifies

vocal tone, whereas nasal emission reflects nasal airflow which is unrelated to voice…

nasal emission is most readily perceived on the production of voiceless consonants” (p.

19). Nasal emission and nasality are also associated with different sizes of the opening of

the nasal port. Using videofluoroscopy, Kummer et al. (1992) found that a large opening

of the velopharyngeal gap is often associated with hypernasality whereas a small opening

is typically associated with nasal rustle. It is believed that the small opening causes air to

whistle through, creating a high frequency nasal emission. Meanwhile, the large opening

of the port can result in an overabundance of nasalization during typically non-nasal

sounds.

Nasometry as an Objective Measure of Nasal Resonance:

Currently, perceptual judgments are the gold standard for classifying speech

differences amongst individuals with cleft palate (Kuehn & Moller, 2000; Lee, Whitehill,

& Ciocca, 2009). However, there is conflicting evidence regarding the effects of

experience on perception, and a lack of consistent inter- and intra-rater reliabilities.

Therefore, the nasometer was designed as an objective measure to identify speech

differences. A nasometer is a non-invasive headset with a plate separating the nasal and

oral areas. Two microphones, one placed on each side of the plate make simultaneous

recordings and create a ratio, comparing the acoustic energy coming from the oral and

nasal areas, which is provided as a measure of nasalance (KayPentax, 2011). According

to Dalston, Warren, and Dalston (1991b), a high nasalance score results when there is

Page 18: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

6  

 

increased nasal resonance.

However, the nasometer cannot be used in isolation, without perceptual

judgments because it is not reliably able to distinguish between normal nasalance during

typical nasal consonants and hypernasality (Watterson, Hinton, & McFarlane, 1996). As

found by Sweeney and Sell (2008), the presence of high nasalance readings from a

nasometer did not always reflect high perceptual ratings of hypernasality. In addition, the

presence of nasal emission changes the accuracy of the nasometer. When detecting

hypernasality including samples of individuals with nasal emission, sensitivity was 0.3;

excluding samples with nasal emission increased the sensitivity of the nasometer to 0.67

(Dalston, Warren, & Dalston, 1991a). In addition, total nasalance scores increase when

audio samples include nasal emission (Karnell, 1995). The nasometer cannot differentiate

between audible nasal emission and hypernasality, as both are perceived as an excess of

nasalance. Discrimination between hypernasality and ANEs is therefore impossible

without the use of an additional measure, such as perceptual judgment.

Considering that hypernasality is a result of a largely open velopharyngeal port,

and ANEs are due to a slightly open funneling of air, the inability for the nasometer to

detect these differences can lead to problems in creating a treatment plan for these

children post-surgery. Although an individual with hypernasality and an individual with

ANE may both receive the same nasometer scores, they would each require different

treatment plans, as an individual with a large opening may require additional surgeries,

while a small opening may be considered a successful surgery and necessitate speech

therapy for resolution (Kummer et al., 1992). Therefore, perceptual judgments that

Page 19: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

7  

 

differentiate between hypernasality and the presence of ANEs would provide crucial

treatment information about children with cleft palate.

Perceptual Judgments of Nasal Resonance:

Several measurement systems have been employed to assess perceptual judgment.

Four main rating scales are typically used in rating voice, Equal-appearing interval (EAI),

Visual Analog scale (VAS), Direct Magnitude Estimation (DME), and Paired

comparison. In EAI, individuals are asked to rate a voice sample from 1 to n (depending

on the scale) with equal intervals between each rating. Typically these scales are seven

point scales (with some nine or five point scales) and end points of the scales which

represent the range of dysfunction of the samples. So for instance, a one may quantify the

most typical rating, with the other endpoint, a seven, representing the most severe.

Direct Magnitude Estimation asks individuals to provide a rating for the speech

sample that is typically unrestricted, and is usually accompanied by a modulus to provide

a marker of the average of the perceptual continua for the quality being assessed (Eadie

& Doyle, 2002). The modulus provided is given a rating of 100, and raters can assign any

rating they desire to capture the stimulus by comparison. This rating system provides

magnitudes of how stimuli differ from one another. Another rating system, paired

comparison ratings, asks raters to compare the two samples on an aspect of voice

(Kreiman, Gerratt, Kempster, Erman, & Berke, 1993). This creates a forced choice

between which sample sounds more dysfunctional. Although this rating provides

information regarding which sample is more impaired along a certain dimension, it does

not provide information about how much more deviant the sample is.

Page 20: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

8  

 

Visual Analog Scaling provides raters with a line in which ratings are made by

placing a mark along the continuum. The line acts as a visual cue for raters to place their

score along the continuum; numerical ratings are typically gathered by measuring the

location of the tick mark relative to the length of the line. A variant of VAS scaling,

visual sort and rate (VSR), asks individuals to sort samples based on severity, then assign

values to them using a VAS interface. The VSR provides raters with the opportunity to

place stimuli in order from most disordered to least disordered, as well as to place each

sample along a visual axis to capture severity (as in a visual analog scale), providing a

numerical rating. Sound samples placed closer to one another represent more similar

samples, while samples further from one another represent samples different from one

another.

Qualities of voice and resonance can be considered either a prothetic or metathetic

quality. A prothetic quality is one that varies in magnitude, and is representative of a

quantitative change, such as loudness. A metathetic quality varies in quality, such as pitch

(Eadie & Doyle, 2002). Certain qualities, depending on whether they are prothetic or

metathetic, can be best captured using different rating systems. As stated by Eadie and

Doyle (2002), “a prothetic continuum… is best scaled with DME because observers

cannot subdivide a prothetic continuum into equal intervals (p. 3015).” Therefore,

prothetic variables are typically best investigated using DME, as EAI scaling are not a

valid way of ascertaining ratings of hypernasality (Whitehill, Lee, & Chun, 2002; Zraick

& Liss, 2000). Therefore, careful consideration of the type of scale used to collect ratings

is necessary for valid perceptual ratings.

Page 21: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

9  

 

Inter- and Intra- Rater Reliability

Inter- and Intra- rater reliabilities are used to determine agreement of perceptual

ratings. Intra-rater reliability measures how stable a given judges’ score is. Additionally,

inter-rater reliability describes how well a group of listeners agrees on scoring. Past

research has shown poor inter- and intra- rater reliability on ratings of nasality and ANE

(Brunnegård, Lohmander, & van Doorn, 2012). Amongst experienced speech language

pathologists, there was low inter-rater reliability on ratings of hypernasality during oral

sentences, ranging from 42% to 62%. Ratings of hyponasality had even larger ranges,

with percentages of inter-rater reliability ranging from 38% to 96%. Ratings of ANE

revealed inter-rater reliability ranging from 7% to 99%.

Ratings of ANEs are often not reported even when they are collected (Baylis et

al., 2011). Several studies have asked listeners to provide opinions regarding ANEs but

few have reported inter and intra-rater reliabilities (Kummer, Briggs, & Lee, 2003;

Kummer et al., 1992; McWilliams et al., 1996; Randall et al., 2000; Witt, Berry, Marsh,

Grames, & Pilgram, 1996). Amongst the studies that have reported inter- and intra- rater

reliabilities, inter-rater reliabilities are moderate at best (ranging from .58–.70) and intra-

rater reliabilities ranging from .42–.94 (Baylis et al., 2011; John, Sell, Sweeney, Harding-

Bell, & Williams, 2006; Keuning, Wieneke, van Wijngaarden, & Dejonckere, 2002;

Persson et al., 2006; Pinborough-Zimmerman, Canady, Yamashiro, & Morales, 1998).

Given the large range of rater reliabilities, studies have focused on qualitative differences

between raters, such as experience amongst listeners, to explain variance in ratings.

Page 22: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

10  

 

How Experience Affects Perceptual Judgments:

An individual’s level of expertise may affect perceptual judgments. Previous

studies have found inconsistent results regarding the effects of experience. Some studies

state that experienced listeners are more likely to use an internalized scale built from

client’s voices to produce judgments (Kummer, Clark, Redle, Thomsen, & Billmire,

2012). These subjective measurements are at risk of being colored by professional

experiences over many years of clinical practice. Professionals may use previous patients

as an example of the most severe type of voice dysfunction when making perceptual

judgments. Studies have shown that experience leads to greater inter-rater reliabilities

compared to naïve listeners (Brunnegård et al., 2012). For example, when rating nasality,

inter-rater reliability for trained speech language pathologists was between 48–60% and

was 30–62% for non-expert SLPs, illustrating significant differences in ratings between

experienced SLPs and naïve raters. However, contrasting these results, one study found

lower inter-rater reliabilities amongst expert listeners (r=.27–.73) compared to naïve

listeners (r=.42–.66) when rating pathological voices (Kreiman, Gerratt, Precoda, &

Berke, 1992). The expert listeners showed a larger range in reliabilities, with some

speech pathologists agreeing more than others.

Often, inexperienced listeners are more likely to rate severity as more severe than

experienced listeners (Helou et al., 2010; Laczi, Sussman, Stathopoulos, & Huber, 2005;

Lewis, Watterson, & Houghton, 2003). However, Kreiman and Gerratt (2000) suggested

that raters are not reliably able to isolate different aspects of speech. Therefore, perhaps

they are using other voice qualities to create their ratings while experienced listeners are

Page 23: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

11  

 

better at isolating and scoring hypernasality independently. In order to account for these

differences, it has been suggested that naïve listeners should receive training sessions to

improve reliability (Eadie & Baylor, 2006; Lee et al., 2009).

Hypernasality and Nasal Emission as a Continuum?:

Studies have found conflicting evidence whether measures of nasality should be

rated on a continuum or whether they represent discrete mechanisms. For example, are

hyper and hyponasality two ends of the same scale of nasalance (Fletcher, 1976;

Henningsson et al., 2008)? Other studies have suggested these to be discrete disorders

and support separate scoring criteria (Keuning, Wieneke, & Dejonckere, 1999; Sell et al.,

1999). Scoring nasalance can be difficult as raters often group hyper and hyponasality

together, suggesting that perceived increases in hypernasality often increase reports of

increased hyponasality (Brunnegård et al., 2012; Nellis, Neiman, & Lehman, 1992).

Typically, as nasalance scores increase, perception of hypernasality increases and

therefore, perception of hyponasality should decrease.1

Similar problems have been cited when rating nasal emission. This subtle

emission can be categorized as a nasal rustle, nasal turbulence, and auditory nasal

emission. While some studies have suggested that varying amounts of nasal emission

affect the label of nasal turbulence or rustle (Baylis et al., 2011; Kummer et al., 1992),

other studies have suggested that nasal rustle and turbulence are completely distinct

perceptions and should be measured on their own scales (Sell et al., 1999). However,

                                                                                                                         1 For the proposed study, hypernasality will be assessed in isolation, without asking raters to assess hyponasality.

Page 24: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

12  

 

most studies do not differentiate the types of nasal emission (Keuning et al., 2002;

Keuning et al., 1999; Persson et al., 2006; Randall et al., 2000; Witt et al., 1996).2

Methodologies Used to Collect Perceptual Ratings:

Research by Zraick and Liss (2000) concluded that nasality is a prothetic variable,

and therefore, would be best measured using a Direct Magnitude Estimation. As stated by

Stevens (1975), “Whenever given a prothetic continuum, the matching procedure

employed is such that the observer is forced out of ratioing and into differencing, so to

speak, there results a distorted scale” (p.135). Therefore, the use of a DME scale allows

users to use a scale that befits the stimuli, while decreasing the possibility that the

extreme ends of the spectrum will be poorly represented. Similarly, Whitehill et al.

(2002), found that EAI ratings had a curvilinear relationship to DME ratings suggesting

DME as the most appropriate rating system. A novel study by Baylis et al. (2011)

examined nasal emission as prothetic or metathetic quality. Their results support for nasal

emission as a prothetic quality. They concluded that nasal emission is a continuum of

impairment, and therefore, best captured using DME measurements. Additionally, using

DME can increase intra-rater reliability (Baylis et al., 2011). In a later study by Baylis,

Chapman, Whitehill, and Group (2014), further support was provided for DME and ANE

as prothetic variables. Findings revealed a curvilinear relationship when comparing

ratings using DME compared to EAI. From this evidence, it seems as if DME is the best

measurement for nasality.

                                                                                                                         2 Listeners will not be asked to differentiate among the different types of ANE and will provide one unique rating for ANEs as a group.

Page 25: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

13  

 

Overall, most perceptual continua are prothetic, and therefore, best measured

using DME (Stevens, 1975). In addition, “magnitude estimation permits the free

assignment of numbers so that magnitudes, as well as ratios among magnitudes, can be

reflected in the numbers that the observer emits to depict his subjective impression”

(Stevens, 1975, p. 135). In addition, Eadie and Doyle (2002) assessed perceptual ratings

of a prothetic variable, voice severity, and a metathetic variable, voice pleasantness. They

found that EAI or DME could be accurately used to provide ratings for the metathetic

variable, while only DME was appropriate for the prothetic variable. Therefore, the use

of DME may be superior to using an equal interval scale, as it can adequately measure

both prothetic and metathetic values.

Visual sort and rate (VSR), has not been investigated in as many studies

evaluating resonance. Higher correlation coefficients were found using VSR than when

using VAS ratings to rate synthetic voices on the presence of high frequencies, and

natural voices on breathiness, roughness, and hyperfunctionality. (Granqvist, 2003).

Since VSR allows listeners to compare each sample to a previous sample, there is no

need for a modulus. This VSR system can decrease the effects of professional experience,

as naïve listeners have direct comparison of each of the stimuli to one another. Trained

professionals will be less likely to compare these samples by using internal standards of a

disordered voice, since they are asked to rank the samples in severity in relation to one

another. Although EAI is the most common rating scale when assessing nasality

(Kreiman et al., 1993), recent findings suggest nasality is better captured using DME

(Whitehill et al., 2002; Zraick & Liss, 2000). However, other studies have found that

Page 26: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

14  

 

DME is inferior to more user friendly rating systems (Brancamp, Lewis, & Watterson,

2010). Therefore, VSR could provide a user friendly alternative to DME.

When using an EAI scale to rate roughness of pathological voices, raters tended to

increase severity ratings as raters heard more samples. Kreiman et al. (1993) posited that

raters were using previous samples as comparisons, resulting in higher overall severity

for later trials than earlier ones. These methodological differences were not true when

raters were using a VAS scale (Kreiman et al., 1993). Furthermore, a study by Keuning,

et al. (1999), found that visual analog scales often resulted in raters differing in their use

of the scale with ratings skewed to the extremes of the scale. Therefore, the use of VSR,

in which voices are ordered by virtue of dysfunction, should reduce methodological

concerns about the use of endpoints.

In addition, individuals often differ in how they weigh various aspects of speech.

For instance, some raters find fundamental frequency as the most important indicator of a

pathological voice, while others note perceptions of shimmer and roughness (Kreiman,

Gerratt, & Precoda, 1990). Since individuals tend to use idiosyncratic anchor referents

and compare it against their own mental representations of atypical voices, the authors

suggested the use of an anchor stimulus after every few stimulus presentations to lessen

the effects of internal mental representations (Kreiman et al., 1992). Therefore, it is

advisable to provide a modulus for raters when using the DME rating system.3

                                                                                                                         3 A modulus will be provided for raters when rating samples using DME.

Page 27: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

15  

 

Aligning Perceptual Judgments with Objective Measurements:

Studies investigating how well perceptual judgments align with objective

measurements of nasalance have found conflicting results. One study found that

experience mattered, with expert speech-language pathologists having higher correlations

with objective measures than naïve listeners. Expert speech-language pathologists had

strong correlations (.67–.74) between their perceptual ratings of nasality and the

nasalance scores, while untrained speech-language pathologists and untrained listeners

had moderate correlations ranging from .55–.76 and .42 –.48 respectively (Brunnegård et

al., 2012). Other studies have found good agreement between perceptual judgments and

nasometer scores across naive listeners. In one study, when asking naïve listeners to

assign ratings for how much nasality is present, listener judgment ratings correlated with

nasalance scores with a correlation of .91 (Fletcher, 1976).

Additionally, comparing ratings of the Temple Street scale, an EAI scale used to

rate nasality, to nasalance scores from a nasometer, the nasometer had a sensitivity of

.83–.88 and specificity ranging from .78–.95 when using perceptual ratings as diagnostic

judgment (Sweeney & Sell, 2008). Other studies have also found similar sensitivity and

specificity, ranging from .70 to .79 (Dalston, Neiman, & Gonzalezlanda, 1993;

Watterson, McFarlane, & Wright, 1993). These moderately high sensitivities and

specificities of nasometry suggest that it could be an effective non-invasive useful tool in

objectively identifying speech differences. However the nasometer is not sufficient to

diagnose voice perceptions on its own. As stated by Kuehn and Moller (2000), “suffice it

to say that at the present time, there is no technique that demonstrates a sufficient

Page 28: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

16  

 

relationship to perceived nasality to eliminate the use of perceptual judgments in

satisfactory description of speech” as objective measurements require further research

regarding efficacy.

Overall, there has been a wide range of research on nasality and how these scores

should be measured. However, studies on nasal emission have been underrepresented in

the literature, especially in-depth studies on natural speech and how they are best

categorized. Direct magnitude estimation has been used to provide ratings for both

metathetic and prothetic variables. However, few studies have studied using visual sort

and rate for providing ratings of nasality. An initial study investigating voice quality

found high correlations and highlighted it as a user friendly interface (Granqvist, 2003).

Given the literature summarized above, the goals of the current study are:

1) To determine which methodology results in the highest inter and intra-rater reliability

for hypernasality and ANE.

2) To investigate the validity of VSR as a novel rating system of hypernasality and ANE

perceptions relative to DME.

Stimuli: Sample Collection

Speech Samples were collected from children with cleft palate at the University of

Wisconsin Madison, at the American Family Children’s Hospital Pediatric Cleft and

Craniofacial Anomalies Clinic through a jointly-approved human studies protocol.

Samples were recorded from 28 native English-speaking children (aged 4–16, 8 females)

with one of the following disorders: repaired sub-mucous cleft palate (SMCP), repaired

Page 29: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

17  

 

unilateral cleft lip and palate, or repaired bilateral cleft lip and palate (CL+P). Some

children had syndromic comorbidities commonly associated with cleft palate (e.g.

hemifacial microsomia, Kabuki syndrome, Robin sequence). Some children had a

secondary surgery, including pharyngeal flap, complete revisions, and fistula repair. See

Table 1 for more details regarding speaker characteristics.

Table 1: Speaker characteristics

Speaker Age Gender Diagnosis Secondary repair 1 7 M Unilateral CL+P 2 5 M SMCP 3 6 M SMCP 4 7 M CL+P; Hemifacial microsomia Pharyngeal Flap 5 4 F Bilateral CL+P 6 7 M Bilateral CL+P Complete revision 7 14 M CP Sphincter pharyngoplasty 8 8 M CL+P Complete revision 9 11 M Unilateral CL+P 10 10 F Unilateral CL+P 12 13 F CL+P 13 9 F SMCP; 22Q deletion 14 16 F CP; Kabuki Syndrome; DD Pharyngeal Flap

15 12 M Unilateral CL+P Fistula Repair and Palatal Revision

16 10 M SMCP; Kabuki Syndrome 17 7 M CP; Robin Sequence 19 16 M CL+P Sphincter Pharyngoplasty 20 5 M CL+P 21 11 M CL+P

22 5 M CL+P Facial Artery Musculomucosal Flap

23 9 F CP Palatal revision and Pharyngeal flap

24 4 F CP 25 9 M CP Furlow Palatoplasty 26 5 M CL+P Palatal repair 27 9 F CL+P Pharyngeal flap 28 5 M SMCP

Page 30: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

18  

 

One of two different sets of instrumentation was used to obtain speech samples.

The first set-up used a Sennheiser PC131 headset and the second set-up utilized a WH20

XLR microphone. Samples were collected in quiet clinic rooms. Speakers completed the

MacKay-Kummer SNAP Test-R (SNAP test-R). The SNAP Test-R elicits consonant

vowel (CV) syllable repetitions with /a/ and /i/ vowels (e.g. /pa/ or /pi/), eliciting

sentences loaded with high pressure plosives, nasals, and sibilants, as well as a short

paragraph reading loaded with plosives and sibilants (Kummer, 2005).

Sample Selection:

Since the main goal of this study was to determine the most accurate paradigm to

rate hypernasality and ANE, isolated CV syllable repetitions were chosen as stimuli to

reduce the risk of elevated scores due to mis-articulations during connected speech.

Furthermore, two of the 28 children were excluded from stimuli selection, as their

samples had a markedly lower signal to noise ratio than other samples, which could

reduce the accuracy of rating the percepts in question. It was also found that amplifying

samples resulted in distortion of the samples; ultimately, a total of 26 speakers were

utilized. Their samples were peak-amplitude normalized. Fourteen phoneme sets were

available (e.g. /pi/, /ti/, /ʃi/, etc.). The five CV syllables with the highest number of usable

samples across speakers were selected (/pi/, /ki/, /ʃi/, /ti/, /si/). Phonemes with /i/ were

selected, as an independent speech-language pathologist noted instances of hypernasality

and audible nasal emission during the production of some of those phoneme productions.

Since raters were rating nasality, nasal phonemes were not rated.

Page 31: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

19  

 

The use of both sibilants and plosives provided opportunities to capture

hypernasality and nasal emission. Any phoneme set in which the speaker did not

complete the full six repetitions of the phoneme, or there was measureable background

noise were excluded. Ultimately, 9/133 or 6.2% of phoneme sets were excluded from the

ratings. Lastly, 20% of samples were repeated, in order to determine intra-rater reliability

of ratings for both rating scales. A total of 152 phoneme sets per condition, or a total of

608 stimuli samples across four conditions were rated: DME-hypernasality, DME-ANE,

VSR-hypernasality, and VSR-ANE. Since the DME paradigm requires a standard to

compare the audio samples against, a modulus had to be selected. In order to determine

the modulus, four pilot participants rated samples, via each of the four conditions. The

four pilot subject’s scores on the VSR paradigms were averaged to create an average

hypernasality and average ANE score for each sample. The mid-point of the range was

selected and the audio sample with an average score in the middle of the range was

selected as the modulus.

Participants:

Twelve participants were recruited (aged 20–28; 3 males). Since this study

required careful review of audio samples, participants passed a pure-tone hearing

screening at 25 dB hearing level (HL) for pure tones at 500 Hz, 1000 Hz, 2000 Hz, and

4000 Hz bilaterally. As the purpose of this study was to assess hypernasality and

perceptual judgments amongst naïve listeners, any graduate students studying speech-

language pathology were excluded from participation to ensure limited exposure to the

Page 32: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

20  

 

percepts. Participants were compensated for their participation and attended two, two

hour rating sessions, scheduled within a two week period.

Procedures: Training:

Every participant completed a brief, ten-minute training session at the start of

each day. To begin, participants read definitions of hypernasality, and audible nasal

emission (see Appendix A) adapted from Kummer (2008). As hyponasality is a common

resonance disorder accompanying cleft-palate, participants were provided with the

definition of hyponasality and asked to disregard instances in their ratings.

Participants also participated in a brief listening training on the computer in which

they listened to samples containing varying severity of hypernasality and ANE (see

Figure 1). Unused samples from the cleft speakers (e.g. /kɑ/, /tɑ/) were chosen as training

stimuli to prevent exposure to the rating stimuli. Nine samples were chosen with varying

resonance and ANE severity to represent a range of typical resonance, mild, moderate,

and severe instances of hypernasality and ANE. An experienced speech language

pathologist confirmed the presence of each percept in the audio samples and determined

its severity. Severity of hypernasality or ANE was indicated with each sample

presentation for the listener to reference. Listeners were instructed to listen to each

sample as many times as they desire, and select the next sample when they felt they

understood the provided percept.

Page 33: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

21  

 

Figure 1: Training condition: Listeners were asked to listen to a range of samples

including typical, mild, moderate, and severe hypernasality and ANE.

Perceptual Ratings:

Participants rated each audio sample using two different rating systems, on two

different voice percepts: hypernasality and nasal emission. Therefore, each participant

participated in four conditions: 1: Rating hypernasality using VSR, 2: Rating

hypernasality using DME, 3: rating ANE using VSR, 4: rating ANE using DME. To

avoid rater confusion between hypernasality and ANE, all participants completed

hypernasality ratings on day 1 and ANE on day 2. The order of rating system (VSR and

DME) was counterbalanced across participants. Listeners rated the same audio samples

across all conditions to determine whether either rating system provided different ratings.

Participants listened to audio samples on a computer using Sennheiser HD 280

Page 34: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

22  

 

pro headphones and provided ratings. For the DME condition (see Figure 2), listeners

were instructed to listen to the modulus stimulus first. They were told that the modulus

represented 100, and that it represented the middle of the range of ANE or hypernasality.

Next, participants listened to each auditory stimulus and assigned a rating based on its

comparison with the modulus. This rating was unrestricted so that they could assign any

number to a sample to capture their percept of the sample. For example, if they perceived

a speech sample as twice that of the modulus, they should have assigned the sample a

200. If they believed it to be half as nasal or contain half as severe ANE, they should

have assigned the stimulus a 50. Raters listened to the modulus and sample as many times

as they wished.

Figure 2: DME condition: Listeners were asked to rate a sample audio sample in

comparison to a modulus set at 100. The DME score was unrestricted.

For the VSR rating system, participants were provided with a set of eight samples

Page 35: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

23  

 

per trial. Sets were created based on pilot ratings using DME for each percept and were

established to include typical, mild, moderate, and severe samples. Given a set of 8 sound

samples, listeners were instructed to play the audio sample as many times as needed to

provide their rating. They were asked to rank the samples from least to most severe (see

Figure 3). They moved the audio samples around the screen so that they sorted samples

from least severe to most severe cases imaginable for the given percept. Once the

samples are placed in order of severity, they moved the audio stimulus along a numbered

axis to assign a numeric value on a visual analog scale from 0–100 (see Figure 4). Raters

progressed through 19 sets, providing ratings for all 152 samples. The set order (1–19), as

well as the order of samples (1–8) within each set was randomized across participants.

Figure 3: The VSR condition: Participants moved each sample, represented by the black dot, around the screen. They were instructed to order the samples from most (top of axis) to least severe (bottom of axis). Participants assigned a numerical value to each sample by its location on the axis.

Page 36: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

24  

 

Figure 4: As listeners moved the samples around the screen, a numerical value populated on the y-axis. Raters were asked to ensure that the numerical value most accurately captured the stimulus.

Statistics:

Inter- and Intra-rater Reliability of DME vs. VSR:

Each rater randomly scored 20% of samples (30 samples) twice and participants

were not alerted to when a sample was repeated. Pearson correlations were calculated to

compare their rating on the first iteration of the sample, to the second iteration of the

sample for each condition (Baylis et al., 2011; Eadie & Doyle, 2002). The intra-rater

reliabilities for each condition were compared to determine which rating scale yielded the

highest intra-rater reliabilities for each percept. In order to determine inter-rater

reliability, type 2 intra-class correlations were calculated for each condition (Baylis et al.,

2011; Whitehill et al., 2002). This resulted in a unique inter-rater reliability measure for

Page 37: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

25  

 

each of the four conditions (e.g. hypernasality using DME). The inter- and intra-rater

reliabilities across all four conditions were compared to determine which rating system

provided the highest levels of inter- and intra- rater reliability for the two percepts. In

order to assess whether raters experienced a ceiling effect for interval rating, the mean

difference was calculated for each participant for the VSR scale comparative to the group

mean (see Table 3) for both hypernasality and ANE. It was not necessary to assess DME

ratings, as there is no ceiling effect because ratings are completely unrestricted.

Does VSR Supply Valid Estimates of ANE and Hypernasality?

To compare ratings across rating systems, a single average score across listeners

was created for each sample. This overall impression rating was calculated for the DME

scale by taking the geometric mean of each listener’s rating for each sample. The

geometric mean has historically been used as DME scores are completely unrestricted

and occasionally result in large scores (Baylis et al., 2011; Schiavetti, Martin, Haroldson,

& Metz, 1994; Stevens, 1975; Whitehill et al., 2002). Since scores from the visual sort

and rate were out of 100, the arithmetic mean scores of each sample across raters were

taken to provide one overall impression rating of hypernasality or ANE per phoneme set

in the VSR rating system. This overall impression rating was calculated for each

condition and was used to compare scores from each rating system.

To compare the ratings between the DME and VSR scales for each percept, the

sample’s geometric means from DME was plotted against the arithmetic mean ratings in

VSR. The line of best fit was calculated. If there was a linear relationship with a positive

Page 38: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

26  

 

slope between DME ratings and VSR ratings, then VSR was most likely acting in the

same fashion as DME and could be used as a suitable replacement. This relationship

indicates that the variables are metathetic. As stated by Stevens (1975), metathetic

qualities are thought to be measurements that are representative of changes in quality.

These changes can be partitioned into equal intervals. However, if the line of best fit is

best represented by a non-linear relationship, then differences are thought to be

considered additive effects, or variation in magnitude. These variables are said to be

prothetic variables. When scores are best captured by curvilinear relationships, it is said

that interval scaling (e.g. VSR) is not appropriate for use.

Results: Intra-rater reliability: Most raters demonstrated strong intra-rater reliabilities (grater than .40) across all

four conditions (Figure 5). Some listeners had poor intra-rater reliabilities for one

condition, but did not demonstrate this across all four conditions. For example, as noted

in Figure 5, participant 10 (blue triangle) had weak inter-rater reliability for rating

hypernasality with DME but a strong intra-rater reliability for detecting hypernasality

with VSR. Thus, despite having strong intra-rater consistency for hypernasality with one

scale, his/her consistency was not strong across both rating scales. Ratings of

hypernasality using DME reflected intra-rater reliabilities ranging between r (120) =.10–

.79. When rating hypernasality using VSR, intra-rater reliabilities were comparable

ranging between r (120) =.14 – .79. Similarly, when rating ANE using DME, Pearson

correlations ranged between r (120) = .31–.89. When rating ANE using VSR, Pearson

Page 39: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

27  

 

correlations ranged between r (120) =.21 – .83.

Figure 5: Comparison of intra-rater Pearson correlation values for each listener across conditions.

Inter-rater reliability:

To compute inter-rater reliability, ratings for each stimulus across listeners were

compared using an intraclass correlation ICC(2,k). Overall, inter-rater agreement was

moderate to poor across conditions ranging from .14–.48. See Table 2 for inter-class

correlations by condition. The greatest inter-rater agreement existed between raters when

assessing hypernasality using DME (r=.48). Overall, the DME conditions yielded higher

intraclass correlations (.48 and .27) than VSR conditions (.14 and .15). The lowest ICC

Page 40: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

28  

 

resulted when rating hypernasality using VSR, indicating that it would not be a suitable

rating system for hypernasality. Poor inter-rater reliability was found rating ANE using

VSR with an ICC of .15. Rating ANE using DME resulted in a higher ICC of .27.

Table 2: Inter-rater reliability across conditions using ICC (2) DME Hyper VSR Hyper ANE DME ANE VSR Inter-rater reliability .48 .14 .27 .15

To determine whether inter-rater reliability was impacted by ratings from those

with weak intra-rater reliability on multiple paradigms, inter-rater reliability was re-run

removing subjects. Subjects 3, 7, 8, and 12 were observed to have low intra-rater

reliability from visual inspection of Figure 5. When removing their ratings from analyses,

inter-rater reliability improved for the DME conditions (ICC values of .65 when rating

hypernasality and .38 for ANE). However, ICC values remained the same or decreased

for the VSR conditions (ICC values of .07 when rating hypernasality and .18 for ANE).

Mean differences, standard deviations, and minimum and maximum ratings were

calculated for the VSR scale to investigate whether a ceiling effect restricted results. It

was not necessary to assess these values for DME ratings, as ratings are completely

unrestricted. As depicted from the mean differences, standard deviations, and minimum

and maximum values, listeners used the VSR scale in a similar fashion. Most raters were

within 10 points of average ratings (9 of 12 raters). Minimum and maximum ratings

indicate that most users utilized the entire range to capture their ratings. Standard

deviations listed below represent the variation in the ratings used by each rater. Higher

Page 41: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

29  

 

standard deviations represent increased use of the scale, which would be consistent with

capturing scores from mild to severe ratings.

Table 3: The tables below represent the mean differences of ratings across samples for hypernasality (top table) and audible nasal emission (bottom table) for the VSR scale.

Hypernasality Mean

Difference Standard Deviation

Minimum Rating

Maximum Rating

Participant 1 6.5 24.1 5.4 95.6 Participant 2 11.5 19.7 16.0 99.4 Participant 3 11.2 17.5 10.6 100 Participant 4 -2.0 28.5 0 100 Participant 5 -1.2 18.8 8.4 96.6 Participant 6 -9.0 25.8 0 94.7 Participant 7 -2.5 31.3 0 100 Participant 8 -22.7 30.1 0 100 Participant 9 6.4 25.3 2.2 96.9 Participant 10 -6.4 23.2 3.1 97.0 Participant 11 2.8 16.0 17.5 92.8 Participant 12 5.5 19.6 4.2 95.5

ANE Mean

Difference Standard Deviation

Minimum Rating

Maximum Rating

Participant 1 -1.1 28.2 0 99.2 Participant 2 7.6 18.1 15.2 91.0 Participant 3 3.0 6.1 22.7 70.7 Participant 4 -3.5 25.2 3.4 100 Participant 5 -2.7 17.9 10.4 95.1 Participant 6 2.6 24.9 0 95.4 Participant 7 -2.8 23.8 3.7 94.9 Participant 8 -8.4 26.0 0 89.9 Participant 9 3.1 18.1 5.6 92.9 Participant 10 -25.0 24.2 0 85.9 Participant 11 10.0 17.6 19.8 90.4 Participant 12 17.3 12.8 31.1 90.5

Page 42: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

30  

 

Does VSR Supply Valid Estimates of ANE and Hypernasality?

Average ratings of each sound sample were calculated by averaging all listeners’

ratings of each sample to produce a single perceptual average of each sample. This

average rating was computed for all four conditions. To assess whether there was a linear

or non-linear relationship between the values, each sample’s average rating using the

DME scale was plotted against its average rating in VSR. A line of best fit was calculated

to assess the relationship. Figure 6A shows the linear line of best fit of DME rating

compared to VSR ratings of hypernasality. The ratings were correlated with an r2=.62.

Figure 6B shows the same data using a curvilinear, polynomial model reflecting an

r2=.64. Figure 6C and 6D represents the ratings for ANE, plotting DME ratings against

VSR ratings. Results show that the curvilinear line (polynomial) of best fit, with an r2=

.68, more efficiently captures the data than the linear best fit line, r2=.64. Both percepts

were more accurately captured using a polynomial model.

Page 43: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

31  

 

Figure 6: Figures A, B represent average hypernasality VSR scores plotted against average DME scores. Figure A is the linear relationship between these values. Figure B is the polynomial (curvilinear) line of best fit. Figure C and D represent average ANE scores for DME and VSR scales. Figure C is the linear relationship between those values, while figure D is the polynomial (curvilinear) line of best fit.

Discussion:

Validity and Reliability of Scores:

Intra-rater reliabilities ranged between r=.10–.89, indicating a large range of

reliability within participants across conditions. As seen in Figure 5, Pearson correlations

were variable across listener and condition, with no participant demonstrating poor

correlations on all four conditions. Two participants showed negligible Pearson

correlations for both scales when rating hypernasality. However, their intra-rater

R²  =  0.61709  

0  

20  

40  

60  

80  

100  

0   100   200   300   400  

VSR  Ra

&ngs  

DME  Ra&ngs  

Comparison  of  DME  and  VSR  ra&ngs  of  Hypernasality  

A  

R²  =  0.63908  

0  

20  

40  

60  

80  

100  

0   100   200   300   400  

VSR  Ra

&ngs  

DME  Ra&ngs  

Comparison  of  DME  and  VSR  scores  of  Hypernasality  

B  

R²  =  0.61784  

0  

20  

40  

60  

80  

100  

0   100   200   300   400  

VSR  Ra

&ngs  

DME  Ra&ngs  

Comparison  of  DME  and  VSR  scores  of  ANE  

C  

R²  =  0.66197  

0  

20  

40  

60  

80  

100  

0   100   200   300   400  

VSR  Ra

&ngs  

DME  Ra&ngs  

Comparison  of  DME  and  VSR  scores  of  ANE  

D

Page 44: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

32  

 

reliabilities were moderate to strong when rating ANE. Thirty-eight of 48 intra-rater

reliabilities were strong (correlations greater than .4). Strong intra-rater reliability

suggests that individuals were attentive when rating speech samples, as they were likely

to score repeated samples with high consistency. Overall, though intra-rater reliabilities

varied, most participants demonstrated strong intra-rater reliabilities.

Regarding inter-rater reliability, listeners demonstrated moderate reliability in the

DME hypernasality condition (r=.48), but negligible to weak reliability across other

conditions (r=.14–.27), indicating little agreement between listeners. Variability in inter-

rater reliability is common amongst perceptual listening studies when rating nasality and

nasal emission (John et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2009). In a study by Lee et al. (2009),

speech-pathology students had weak to moderate inter-judge reliability, even with

training (ranging from .29–.42). In regards to audible nasal emission, inter-rater

reliabilities (Table 2) were negligible to weak regardless of the rating system used. This

percept was difficult for raters to discern, as evidenced by low inter-rater reliability.

Poor inter-rater reliability suggests that VSR would not be a suitable replacement for

DME ratings. Poorest inter-rater reliabilities were found when raters were using the VSR

scale.

In order to further investigate the VSR scale, average ratings were compared to

determine whether raters utilized the same portions of the scale. As depicted by Table 3,

most of the raters utilized the VSR scale similarly, providing ratings within 10 points of

the average. Ratings across samples and across listeners were around 50, which were

expected considering representations of samples of various severities (53.0 capturing

Page 45: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

33  

 

hypernasality, and 50.2 capturing ANE). Standard deviations in this case, represent the

distance of ratings from the mean. So in this case, higher standard deviations, represent

that the listener used a large range surrounding the mean ratings to capture extreme

ratings. In the two cases in which the mean difference was greater than 20, the two raters

had negative mean differences, indicating that their average ratings were lower than the

remainder of the cohort. These findings support that raters were not constricted in their

rating by a ceiling effect.

Strong intra-rater reliabilities suggest strong internal consistency. However, weak

inter-rater values suggest inconsistency between raters. Perhaps internal representations

of these perceptions differed across raters. Raters may have utilized other aspects of the

speech samples than hypernasality and ANE. Or, perhaps raters were using other aspects

of recording (e.g. voice quality or misarticulations) to determine severity. This suggests

that further training may raise inter-rater agreement.

Benefits of a Training Session:

Previous studies have supported the use of naïve listeners, lending support to

using naïve listeners in the current study. Findings by Brunnegard, Lohmander, and van

Doorn (2009) suggested that untrained listeners rated hypernasality as well as trained

speech-language pathologists, using a five point rating scale. However, naïve listeners

were less sensitive in detecting nasal emission with expert listeners yielding higher inter-

rater reliability of ratings. Therefore, perhaps raters in the current study were not as adept

when rating ANE. However, naïve listeners in the current study achieved mostly strong

Page 46: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

34  

 

intra-rater reliabilities, suggesting success of the training. As all listeners received a

training session, exact benefits of training are unknown.

The current training was a passive listening task. Previous research by Lee, et al.

(2009), did not find significant differences between the use of a training session with and

without contingent feedback. Training resulted in improved reliability of hypernasality

ratings regardless of the type of training. However, considering the low inter-rater

reliabilities, perhaps participants were not rating percepts in the same manner. A training

with contingent feedback of severity of training stimuli may help increase inter-rater

performance.

ANE and Hypernasality as Prothetic Variables:

Plotting DME ratings against VSR ratings resulted in a curvilinear relationship,

which may indicate that nasality and nasal emission are prothetic variables. This finding

is consistent with previous findings of nasality as a prothetic variable (Baylis et al., 2011;

Zraick & Liss, 2000). Caution is warranted, as the polynomial model only improved the

model slightly (from an r2 of .61 to .64). Additional samples on the severe end should be

added to provide further information regarding the classification of these percepts.

Nasal emission was best represented by a curvilinear relationship, consistent with

findings by Baylis et al. (2011). This suggests these variables may be best captured using

magnitude estimation, rather than interval scaling, as magnitude scaling is more

appropriate when scoring prothetic variables (Stevens, 1975).

Page 47: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

35  

 

Use of VSR as a Novel Scale:

Direct magnitude estimation yielded higher inter-rater reliability than VSR in both

conditions. Visual sort and rate conditions yielded the lowest inter-rater reliabilities,

regardless of the percept being scored. Therefore, VSR may not be a suitable scale to

measure these percepts. A curvilinear relationship was found between ratings on DME

and VRS, which suggests they are best captured using ratio scaling (such as DME).

Direct Magnitude Estimation has historically been used to measure prothetic variables.

Therefore, DME should continue to be used when rating hypernasality.

In regards to ANE, low inter-rater reliability regardless of rating scale used

suggests difficulty providing ANE ratings. Perhaps rating nasal emission on a numerical

scale proved too challenging. Clinically, nasal emission is typically rated as present or

absent, described by the frequency of nasal emission (consistent, intermittent), and

whether they are audible, visible, or turbulent in nature (McWilliams & Philips, 1979).

John et al. (2006) rated audible nasal emission and nasal turbulence as absent, occasional

or frequent, rather then on a numerical scale. In the John et al. study, seven expert speech

language pathologists had difficulty providing reliable ratings of ANE, as evidenced by

low inter-rater reliability (inter-rater reliability range 16–64%) when rating nasal

emission and turbulence using an EAI measure, the Cleft Audit Protocol for Speech-

Augmented (CAPS-A). As expert speech-pathologists had difficulty providing reliable

ratings with one another, it is not surprising that naïve listeners in the current study had

such low reliabilities. Therefore, perhaps rating ANE perceptions in a continuous manner

is not ideal. More research is needed regarding a more appropriate scale for rating ANE.

Page 48: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

36  

 

Limitations:

It was assumed that rating hypernasality in isolated syllables would be more

effective, perhaps more naturalistic speech is needed to provide accurate nasality ratings.

Findings by Brunnegård et al. (2012) indicated higher inter-rater reliabilities when rating

hypernasality across naïve listeners (46–57%) than the current study (48%). Their stimuli

consisted of connected speech rather than isolated syllable strings. Therefore, future

studies should investigate the use of connected speech over syllable phoneme repetitions

as stimuli samples.

In regards to stimuli, samples were collected from children as young as four.

Therefore, the quality of samples was not consistent. Although samples with overt noise

in the background were excluded, children may not have delivered the syllable sets

reliably. Similarly, children with articulatory or phonological processes were not

excluded. As shown in Paliobei et al. (2005), the presence of articulation errors in

samples resulted in higher severity ratings of hypernasality. Therefore, further research is

warranted to separate perceptions of disordered speech production from nasality and

nasal air emission.

Conclusions:

Overall, intra-rater reliability was moderate to strong amongst most participants

across conditions, with negligible/weak inter-rater reliabilities when using VSR. This

indicates that raters were reliable at rating these percepts but may have different internal

representations of them. Rating hypernasality using the DME scale resulted in the highest

Page 49: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

37  

 

inter-rater reliability, which supports current literature in the field. Therefore, the use of

VSR as a novel rating scale for these percepts is not supported by current findings, as

VSR yielded lower inter-rater reliability. A curvilinear relationship was observed

between ratings of DME and VSR for both hypernasality and audible nasal emission.

This suggests that these variables may be prothetic, and better captured by magnitude

estimation (Stevens, 1975). Further research is needed with additional samples in the

severe range. Overall, ANE was difficult to characterize and additional work is needed to

investigate the best way to categorize nasal emission.

Page 50: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

38  

 

Appendix A: Training Materials

For the purposes of this study, you will be rating hypernasality and audible nasal emission. Both of these sound qualities can be very subtle and hard to hear. Some of the perceptions you will rate today have been described as “static” noise in the background. However, all of the samples are high quality, and you should assume any deviations may be potential instances of nasal emission, or hypernasality.

Hypernasality

Hypernasality is a speech disorder that occurs when sounds that are supposed to be produced only in the mouth/oral cavity are also transmitted through the nose/nasal cavity. In this listening task, you will hear speech sounds that are all typically produced orally, without any sound from the nose. Therefore, if you hear a sound that appears to be coming from the nasal cavity, please consider it hypernasal. Then, please rate how much of that quality you hear in the sample using the rating scale/task provided. Audible Nasal Emission:

Nasal emission (ANE) can be described as the perception of ‘‘extra noise’’ in the speech signal. The noise associated with ANE is air escaping through the nasal passageway. Audible nasal emission are typically perceived in these speakers during the production of consonants that require high air pressure in the mouth (such as /pa/,/sha/) and often results in a high pitched whistle or large amount of air, due to air escaping out of the nose. Nasal emission can be very loud and distracting, or very soft and barely audible. For the purposes of this study, please do not consider de-nasalized speech (also called hyponasality). Hyponasality can often be confused with hypernasality, as both often have been described as “muffled speech” and both often results in less intelligible speech.

For your reference the definition is included below.

PLEASE IGNORE ANY HYPONASALITY THAT YOU HEAR. DO YOUR BEST TO ONLY JUDGE THE ATTRIBUTE (HYPERNASALITY OR ANE) THAT YOU ARE ASKED TO JUDGE.

Hyponasality:

Hyponasality is a resonance disorder when there is a reduction in the amount of sound coming from the nose when typically needed. Sounds such as /m/ /n/ /ng/- as in “ring” always include sounds from the nose to produce those sounds. Place your finger on your nostril, and say the word “man.” Notice how you can feel your nose vibrating when you

Page 51: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

39  

 

say it. Those individuals who have hyponasality have a reduction in sounds from the nose. Hyponasality is often described as if they person sounds as if “they are stuffed up” or “that they have a cold.”

Kummer, A. W. (2008). Cleft palate and craniofacial anomalies: effects on speech and resonance. Clifton Park, N.Y.: Thomson Delmar Learning.

Page 52: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

40  

 

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Armour, A., Fischbach, S., Klaiman, P., & Fisher, D. M. (2005). Does Velopharyngeal

Closure Pattern Affect the Success of Pharyngeal Flap Pharyngoplasty? Plastic

and Reconstructive Surgery, 115(1), 45–52.

Baylis, A. L., Munson, B., & Moller, K. T. (2011). Perceptions of Audible Nasal

Emission in Speakers with Cleft Palate: a Comparative Study of Listener

Judgments. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 48(4), 399–411.

doi:10.1597/09-201

Baylis, A., Chapman, K., Whitehill, T. L., & Group, T. A. S. (2014). Validity and

Reliability of Visual Analog Scaling for Assessment of Hypernasality and

Audible Nasal Emission in Children with Repaired Cleft Palate. The Cleft Palate-

Craniofacial Journal [Epub ahead of print].

Brunnegård, K., Lohmander, A., & van Doorn, J. (2012). Comparison between

Perceptual Assessments of Nasality and Nasalance Scores. International Journal

of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(5), 556–566.

Brancamp, T. U., Lewis, K. E., & Watterson, T. (2010). The Relationship between

Nasalance Scores and Nasality Ratings Obtained with Equal Appearing Interval

and Direct Magnitude Estimation Scaling Methods. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial

Journal, 47(6), 631–637. doi:10.1597/09-106

Brunnegård, K., Lohmander, A., & van Doorn, J. (2009). Untrained Listeners’ Ratings of

Speech Disorders in a Group with Cleft Palate: a Comparison with Speech and

Language Pathologists’ Ratings. International Journal of Language &

Page 53: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

41  

 

Communication Disorders, 44(5), 656–674. doi:10.1080/13682820802295203

Brunnegård, K., Lohmander, A., & van Doorn, J. (2012). Comparison between

Perceptual Assessments of Nasality and Nasalance Scores. International Journal

of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(5), 556–566. doi:10.1111/j.1460-

6984.2012.00165.x

Cable, B. B., Canady, J. W., Karnell, M. P., Karnell, L. H., & Malick, D. N. (2004).

Pharyngeal Flap Surgery: Long-Term Outcomes at the University of Iowa: Plastic

and Reconstructive Surgery, 113(2), 475–478.

doi:10.1097/01.PRS.0000100806.45065.35

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC]. (2013). Facts about Cleft Lip and

Cleft Palate. Retrieved October 1, 2013, from

http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/birthdefects/cleftlip.html

Dalston, R. M., Neiman, G. S., & Gonzalezlanda, G. (1993). Nasometric Sensitivity and

Specificity - a Cross-Dialect and Cross-Culture Study. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial

Journal, 30(3), 285–291. doi:10.1597/1545-

1569(1993)030<0285:NSASAC>2.3.CO;2

Dalston, R. M., Warren, D. W., & Dalston, E. T. (1991a). A Preliminary Investigation

Concerning the Use of Nasometry in Identifying Patients with Hyponasality

and/or Nasal Airway Impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,

34(1), 11–18.

Dalston, R. M., Warren, D. W., & Dalston, E. T. (1991b). Use of Nasometry as a

Diagnostic Tool for Identifying Patients with Velopharyngeal Impairment. The

Page 54: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

42  

 

Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 28(2), 184–188; discussion 188–189.

doi:10.1597/1545-1569(1991)028<0184:UONAAD>2.3.CO;2

Eadie, T. L., & Baylor, C. R. (2006). The Effect of Perceptual Training on Inexperienced

Listeners’ Judgments of Dysphonic Voice. Journal of Voice, 20(4), 527–544.

doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2005.08.007

Eadie, T. L., & Doyle, P. C. (2002). Direct Magnitude Estimation and Interval Scaling of

Pleasantness and Severity in Dysphonic and Normal Speakers. Journal of the

Acoustical Society of America, 112(6), 3014–3021.

Fletcher, S. G. (1976). “Nasalance” vs. Listener Judgments of Nasality. Cleft Palate

Journal, 13, 31–44.

Gardner, L. K., & Parr, G. R. (1996). Prosthetic Rehabilitation of the Cleft Palate Patient.

Seminars in Orthodontics, 2(3), 215–219. doi:10.1016/S1073-8746(96)80017-X

Granqvist, S. (2003). The Visual Sort and Rate Method for Perceptual Evaluation in

Listening Tests. Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology, 28(3), 109–116.

Hardin-Jones, M. A., & Jones, D. L. (2005). Speech Production of Preschoolers with

Cleft Palate. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 42(1), 7–13. doi:  10.1597/03-

134.1

Helou, L. B., Solomon, N. P., Henry, L. R., Coppit, G. L., Howard, R. S., & Stojadinovic,

A. (2010). The Role of Listener Experience on Consensus Auditory-Perceptual

Evaluation of Voice (CAPE-V) Ratings of Postthyroidectomy Voice. American

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(3), 248–258. doi: 10.1044/1058-

0360(2010/09-0012)

Page 55: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

43  

 

Henningsson, G., Kuehn, D. P., Sell, D., Sweeney, T., Trost-Cardamone, J. E., &

Whitehill, T. L. (2008). Universal Parameters for Reporting Speech Outcomes in

Individuals with Cleft Palate. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 45(1), 1–17. doi:  

10.1597/06-086.1

Hopper, R. A., Tse, R., Smartt, J., Swanson, J., & Kinter, S. (2014). Cleft Palate Repair

and Velopharyngeal Dysfunction. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 133(6),

852e–64e. doi:10.1097/PRS.0000000000000184

John, A., Sell, D., Sweeney, T., Harding-Bell, A., & Williams, A. (2006). The Cleft

Audit Protocol for Speech-Augmented: A Validated and Reliable Measure for

Auditing Cleft Speech. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 43(3), 272–288.

doi:10.1597/04-141.1

Karnell, M. P. (1995). Nasometric Discrimination of Hypernasality and Turbulent Nasal

Airflow. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 32(2), 145–148. doi:10.1597/1545-

1569(1995)032<0145:NDOHAT>2.3.CO;2

KayPentax. (2011). KayPentax Products. Retrieved from

http://www.kayelemetrics.com/index.php?option=com_product&view=product&I

temid=3&controller=product&cid[]=78&task=pro_details

Keuning, K. H. D. M., Wieneke, G. H., van Wijngaarden, H. A., & Dejonckere, P. H.

(2002). The Correlation between Nasalance and a Differentiated Perceptual

Rating of Speech in Dutch Patients with Velopharyngeal Insufficiency. Cleft

Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 39(3), 277–284. doi:10.1597/1545-

1569(2002)039<0277:TCBNAA>2.0.CO;2

Page 56: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

44  

 

Keuning, K. H. D., Wieneke, G. H., & Dejonckere, P. H. (1999). The Intrajudge

Reliability of the Perceptual Rating of Cleft Palate Speech before and After

Pharyngeal Flap Surgery: The Effect of Judges and Speech Samples. The Cleft

Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 36(4), 328–333. doi:10.1597/1545-

1569(1999)036<0328:TIROTP>2.3.CO;2

Kreiman, J., Gerratt, B., Kempster, G., Erman, A., & Berke, G. (1993). Perceptual

Evaluation of Voice Quality - Review, Tutorial, and a Framework for Future-

Research. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(1), 21–40.

Kreiman, J., Gerratt, B., & Precoda, K. (1990). Listener Experience and Perception of

Voice Quality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33(1), 103–115.

Kreiman, J., Gerratt, B., Precoda, K., & Berke, G. (1992). Individual-Differences in

Voice Quality Perception. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(3), 512–

520.

Kreiman, J., & Gerratt, B. R. (2000). Sources of Listener Disagreement in Voice Quality

Assessment. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108(4), 1867–1876.

Kuehn, D. P., & Moller, K. T. (2000). Speech and Language Issues in the Cleft Palate

Population: The State of the Art. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 37(4), 348–

348. doi:10.1597/1545-1569(2000)037<0348:SALIIT>2.3.CO;2

Kummer, A. W. (2005). The MacKay-Kummer Snap Test-R Simplified Nasometric

Assessment Procedures. Retrieved from

http://www.kaypentax.com/index.php?option=com_product&view=product&Item

id=3&controller=product_innerpage&rec_id=50&no_id=2

Page 57: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

45  

 

Kummer, A. W. (2008). Cleft Palate and Craniofacial Anomalies: Effects on Speech and

Resonance. Clifton Park, N.Y.: Thomson Delmar Learning.

Kummer, A. W., Briggs, M., & Lee, L. (2003). The Relationship between the

Characteristics of Speech and Velopharyngeal Gap Size. Cleft Palate-

Craniofacial Journal, 40(6), 590–596.

Kummer, A. W., Clark, S. L., Redle, E. E., Thomsen, L. L., & Billmire, D. A. (2012).

Current Practice in Assessing and Reporting Speech Outcomes of Cleft Palate and

Velopharyngeal Surgery: a Survey of Cleft Palate/craniofacial Professionals. Cleft

Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 49(2), 146–152.

Kummer, A. W., Curtis, C., Wiggs, M., Lee, L., & Strife, J. L. (1992). Comparison of

Velopharyngeal Gap Size in Patients with Hypernasality, Hypernasality and Nasal

Emission, or Nasal Turbulence (rustle) as the Primary Speech Characteristic.

Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 29(2), 152–156.

Laczi, E., Sussman, J. E., Stathopoulos, E. T., & Huber, J. (2005). Perceptual Evaluation

of Hypernasality Compared to HONC Measures: The Role of Experience. Cleft

Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 42(2), 202–211. doi:  10.1597/03-011.1

Lee, A., Whitehill, T. L., & Ciocca, V. (2009). Effect of Listener Training on Perceptual

Judgement of Hypernasality. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 23(5), 319–334.

doi:  10.1080/02699200802688596

Lewis, K. E., Watterson, T. L., & Houghton, S. M. (2003). The Influence of Listener

Experience and Academic Training on Ratings of Nasality. Journal of

Page 58: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

46  

 

Communication Disorders, 36(1), 49–58. doi: 10.1597/1545-

1569(2003)040<0040:CONSOF>2.0.CO;2

Lohmander, A., & Olsson, M. (2004). Methodology for Perceptual Assessment of Speech

in Patients with Cleft Palate: a Critical Review of the Literature. Cleft Palate-

Craniofacial Journal, 41(1), 64–70. doi:10.1597/02-136

McWilliams, B. J., & Philips, B. J. (1979). Velopharyngeal Incompetence. W.B.

Saunders, Philadelphia.

McWilliams, B. J., Randall, P., LaRossa, D., Cohen, S., Yu, J., Cohen, M., & Solot, C.

(1996). Speech Characteristics Associated with the Furlow Palatoplasty as

Compared with Other Surgical Techniques. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery,

98(4), 610–619; discussion 620–621.

Nellis, J. L., Neiman, G. S., & Lehman, J. A. (1992). Camparison of Nasometere and

Listener Judgments of Nasality in the Assessment of Velopahryngeal Function

after Pharyngeal Flap Surgery. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 29(2), 157–

163.

Paliobei, V., Psifidis, A., & Anagnostopoulos, D. (2005). Hearing and Speech

Assessment of Cleft Palate Patients after Palatal Closure Long-term Results.

International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 69, 1373–1381.

Persson, C., Lohmander, A., & Elander, A. (2006). Speech in Children with an Isolated

Cleft Palate: a Longitudinal Perspective. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 43(3),

295–309.

Page 59: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

47  

 

Pinborough-Zimmerman, J., Canady, C., Yamashiro, D. K., & Morales, L., Jr. (1998).

Articulation and Nasality Changes Resulting from Sustained Palatal Fistula

Obturation. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 35(1), 81–87.

doi:10.1597/1545-1569(1998)035<0081:AANCRF>2.3.CO;2

Prevalence at Birth of Cleft Lip With or Without Cleft Palate: Data From the

International Perinatal Database of Typical Oral Clefts (IPDTOC). (2011). The

Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 48(1), 66–81. doi:10.1597/09-217

Randall, P., LaRossa, D., McWilliams, B. J., Cohen, M., Solot, C., & Jawad, A. F.

(2000). Palatal Length in Cleft Palate as a Predictor of Speech Outcome. Plastic

and Reconstructive Surgery, 106(6), 1254–1259; discussion 1260–1261.

Schiavetti, N., Martin, R. R., Haroldson, S. K., & Metz, D. E. (1994). Psychophysical

Analysis of Audiovisual Judgments of Speech Naturalness of Nonstutterers and

Stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(1), 46–52.

Seagle, M. B., Mazaheri, M. K., Dixon-Wood, V. L., & Williams, W. N. (2002).

Evaluation and Treatment of Velopharyngeal Insufficiency: the University of

Florida Experience. Annals of Plastic Surgery, 48(5), 464–470.

Sell, D., Harding, A., & Grunwell, P. (1999). GOS.SP.ASS.’98: An Assessment for

Speech Disorders Associated with Cleft Palate and/or Velopharyngeal

Dysfunction (revised). International Journal of Language & Communication

Disorders, 34(1), 17–33. doi: 10.1080/136828299247595

Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to its Perceptual, Neural, and Social

Prospects. New York: A Wiley-Interscience Publication.

Page 60: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

48  

 

Sweeney, T., & Sell, D. (2008). Relationship between Perceptual Ratings of Nasality and

Nasometry in Children/Adolescents with Cleft Palate and/or Velopharyngeal

Dysfunction. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,

43(3), 265–282.

Timmons, M. J., Wyatt, R. A., & Murphy, T. (2001). Speech after Repair of Isolated

Cleft Palate and Cleft Lip and Palate. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 54, 377–

384.

Watterson, T., Hinton, J., & McFarlane, S. (1996). Novel Stimuli for Obtaining

Nasalance Measures from Young Children. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal,

33(1), 67–73. doi:10.1597/1545-1569(1996)033<0067:NSFONM>2.3.CO;2

Watterson, T., McFarlane, S. C., & Wright, D. S. (1993). The Relationship between

Nasalance and Nasality in Children with Cleft Palate. Journal of Communication

Disorders, 26(1), 13–28.

Whitehill, T. L., Lee, A. S. Y., & Chun, J. C. (2002). Direct Magnitude Estimation and

Interval Scaling of Hypernasality. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing

Research, 45(1), 80.

Witt, P. D., Berry, L. A., Marsh, J. L., Grames, L. M., & Pilgram, T. K. (1996). Speech

Outcome Following Palatoplasty in Primary School Children: Do Lay Peer

Observers Agree with Speech Pathologists? Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery,

98(6), 958–965; discussion 966–970.

Woo, A. S. (2012). Velopharyngeal Dysfunction. Seminars in Plastic Surgery, 26(4),

170–177. doi:10.1055/s-0033-1333882

Page 61: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

49  

 

Wyatt, R., Sell, D., Russell, J., Harding, A., Harland, K., & Albery, L. (1996). Cleft

Palate Speech Dissected: a Review of Current Knowledge and Analysis. British

Journal of Plastic Surgery, 49(3), 143–149.

Zraick, R. I., & Liss, J. M. (2000). A Comparison of Equal-Appearing Interval Scaling

and Direct Magnitude Estimation of Nasal Voice Quality. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 979–88.

Page 62: Perceptual judgment of hypernasality and audible … UNIVERSITY SARGENT COLLEGE OF HEALTH AND REHABILITATION SCIENCES Thesis PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT OF HYPERNASALITY AND AUDIBLE NASAL

50  

 

VITA

Kerri Downing was born in 1988, in Long Beach, NY. She graduated from the University

of Connecticut (UCONN) in May of 2010 with a Bachelor of the Arts (B.A) in

Psychology. She subsequently worked on a collaborative longitudinal research study at

Boston University and Boston Children’s Hospital, investigating the development of

infants with a sibling with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and specific Language

Impairment (SLI). She is expected to graduate from Boston University with a Master of

Science (M.S) in Speech Language Pathology in 2015.