queensland university of technology school of nursing institute...

321
Queensland University of Technology School of Nursing Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy in Taiwanese Outpatients with Cancer Pain Shu-Yuan Liang RN, BSN, MSN Submitted for the Award of Doctor of Philosophy September 2007

Upload: others

Post on 26-Jun-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Queensland University of Technology

    School of Nursing

    Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation

    Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy in Taiwanese Outpatients with Cancer Pain

    Shu-Yuan Liang RN, BSN, MSN

    Submitted for the Award of Doctor of Philosophy

    September 2007

  • i

    Keywords

    Decision making

    Analgesic-taking self-management

    Self-efficacy

    Confidence

    Cancer pain

    Pain management

  • ii

    Abstract

    Despite the fact that as many as 80-90% of patients with cancer pain can be

    effectively treated using pharmacological therapies and other advanced approaches,

    31% to 85% of cancer patients in Taiwan still experience varying levels of pain. Pain

    is one of the symptoms that patients fear most; it overwhelms all aspects of patients’

    lives and creates a sense of uncertainly and hopelessness. Pain control is, therefore, a

    high priority in the treatment of cancer patients. Pharmacological therapy is the

    cornerstone of cancer pain management. With the current trend toward outpatient

    care, many patients are being required to assume greater responsibility for self-

    management of prescribed analgesics at home to deal with the variable and complex

    nature of cancer pain and side effects of opioids. Patients however, have

    misconceptions regarding analgesics and a series of difficulties when attempting to

    put a pain management regimen into practice.

    This research addressed the hypothesis that self-efficacy beliefs might play an

    important role in analgesic adherence and pain experience in Taiwanese outpatients

    with cancer. The purpose of this study was to develop a scale to measure the self-

    efficacy expectations relating to opioid-taking in Taiwanese outpatients with cancer.

    Another purpose was to explore how opioid-taking self-efficacy and beliefs about

    opioid analgesics contribute to patients’ analgesic adherence and pain experience in

    Taiwanese outpatients with cancer.

    In the first stage semi-structured interviews were conducted to collect data from a

    purposeful sample (n=10) of oncology outpatients from two teaching hospitals in the

    Taipei area of Taiwan. The purpose of this phase was to identify behaviours and

  • iii

    situational impediments associated with analgesic taking. Findings from this phase

    were used to develop a scale to measure opioid-taking self-efficacy. In the second

    stage a pilot test with a convenience sample (n=30) was conducted to test the validity

    and reliability of the new scale and to identify the feasibility of using the scale in a

    cross-sectional survey. In the third stage a cross-sectional survey was undertaken

    (n=92) to describe pain experiences, analgesic adherence, beliefs about opioid

    analgesics, and opioid-taking self-efficacy in Taiwanese outpatients with cancer and

    to explore how opioid-taking self-efficacy and beliefs about opioid analgesics

    contributed to analgesic adherence and pain experience.

    Results of this study highlight an important issue – under-treatment of cancer pain in

    this group of Taiwanese outpatients. As well, low adherence rates to opioid

    analgesics in cancer outpatients arose as an important issue in this study. A range of

    misconceptions about using opioids for pain was also common amongst the sample.

    Despite these misconceptions, patients reported being moderately confident in their

    ability to perform self-management behaviours related to their prescribed opioid-

    taking. Results of this research supported the notion that patients’ self-efficacy in

    relation to taking their prescribed opioid regimen was a significant independent

    predictor of patients’ adherence behaviour and pain relief, but not of pain severity.

    Beliefs about opioid analgesics were also an independent predictor of patients’

    adherence, but not of pain relief or pain severity. In addition, findings from this study

    provided support for the validity and reliability of the opioid-taking self-efficacy

    scale.

  • iv

    Results suggested there is a need for systematic assessment of beliefs affecting

    patients’ opioid adherence behaviours for cancer pain control, including perceived

    personal self-efficacy and beliefs about opioid analgesics. Educational programs that

    focus on overcoming patients’ misconceptions (beliefs) about taking opioid

    analgesics may be particularly beneficial. In addition, this study advocates that

    conducting self-efficacy-enhancing interventions may improve medication adherence

    for patients and therefore pain relief. More research is needed to demonstrate the

    construct validity of the self-efficacy scale and to evaluate self-efficacy enhancing

    interventions in cancer pain management.

  • v

    Table of Contents

    Keywords ............................................................................................................ i

    Abstract.............................................................................................................. ii

    Table of Contents .............................................................................................. v

    List of Appendices ............................................................................................ ix

    List of Tables ..................................................................................................... x

    List of Figures.................................................................................................. xii

    Statement of Original Authorship ................................................................xiii

    Acknowledgements......................................................................................... xiv

    Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................................... 1 1.0 Introduction................................................................................................... 1 1.1 Background and Significance of the Study................................................... 1

    1.1.1 The Prevalence of Cancer Pain ............................................................ 1 1.1.2 Impact of Cancer Pain.......................................................................... 2 1.1.3 Cancer Pain in Taiwan ......................................................................... 3

    1.2 Problem Statement and Significance of this Study....................................... 4 1.3 Aims .............................................................................................................. 8

    1.3.1 Aims of the Research ........................................................................... 8 1.3.2 Research Questions .............................................................................. 8

    1.4 Research Design and Methods ...................................................................... 9 1.5 Overview of the Thesis ............................................................................... 11 Chapter 2: Conceptual Model of Pain and Pain Management................... 14 2.0 Introduction................................................................................................. 14 2.1 Definition of Pain........................................................................................ 14 2.2 Multidimensional Components of Pain....................................................... 15

    2.2.1 Physiological Component of Pain...................................................... 16 2.2.2 Sensory Component of Pain............................................................... 17 2.2.3 Affective Component of Pain............................................................. 18 2.2.4 Cognitive Component of Pain............................................................ 18 2.2.5 Behavioural Component of Pain ........................................................ 22 2.2.6 Sociocultural Component of Pain ...................................................... 24

    2.3 Multidimensional Approaches to Pain Management .................................. 26 2.4 Evidence Review on Difficulties with Patients’ Adherence to

    Pharmacological Therapies ......................................................................... 30 2.4.1 The Concepts of Adherence ............................................................... 30 2.4.2 Practical Problems with Adherence to Prescribed Medications ........ 32

    2.4.2.1 Obtaining the Prescribed Medications .................................. 32 2.4.2.2 Accessing Information .......................................................... 33 2.4.2.3 Tailoring Prescribed Regimens to Meet Individual Needs .... 34 2.4.2.4 Managing Side Effects ........................................................... 37

  • vi

    2.4.2.5 Cognitively Processing Information ..................................... 38 2.4.2.6 Managing Multiple Symptoms Simultaneously..................... 38

    2.5 Summary of Literature ............................................................................... 39 Chapter 3: Theoretical and Empirical Approaches to Understanding Self-

    Efficacy........................................................................................... 41 3.0 Introduction ................................................................................................ 41 3.1 Self-Efficacy Construct............................................................................... 41 3.2 Theoretical Concepts................................................................................... 44

    3.2.1 Self-Efficacy Models ......................................................................... 44 3.2.2 Efficacy Expectations and Outcome Expectations ............................ 45 3.2.3 Information Sources of Efficacy Expectations ................................. 48 3.2.4 Cultural and Personal Factors Influencing Efficacy Expectations..... 49

    3.3 Relationships between Concepts in Self-Efficacy Model........................... 51 3.3.1 Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Expectations ........................................ 52 3.3.2 Efficacy Expectations and Adherence Behaviour.............................. 53 3.3.3 Efficacy Expectations and Pain Outcomes ........................................ 54 3.3.4 Personal and Cultural Factors Influencing Efficacy Expectations..... 55

    3.3.4.1 Personal Factors Influencing Efficacy Expectations.............. 55 3.3.4.2 Efficacy Expectations amongst the Taiwanese Population.... 56

    3.4 Measuring Efficacy Expectations .............................................................. 58 3.5 Critique of Self-Efficacy Theory ............................................................... 61 3.6 Summary .................................................................................................... 64 Chapter 4: Methodology................................................................................ 65 4.0 Introduction................................................................................................ 65 4.1 Research Design......................................................................................... 65 4.2 Stage One: Semi Structured Interviews ..................................................... 65

    4.2.1 Aim.................................................................................................... 65 4.2.2 Research Question............................................................................. 66 4.2.3 Research Design................................................................................ 66 4.2.4 Interview Guide................................................................................. 66 4.2.5 Sample, Procedure, and Setting ........................................................ 68 4.2.6 Data Analysis .................................................................................... 69 4.2.7 Rigour of the Interviews ................................................................... 72 4.2.8 Scale Development ........................................................................... 74

    4.3 Stage Two: Pilot Testing of the Scale ........................................................ 75 4.3.1 Aims .................................................................................................. 75 4.3.2 Research Questions ........................................................................... 75 4.3.3 Research Design................................................................................ 75 4.3.4 Research Population.......................................................................... 76 4.3.5 Sampling Strategy, Sample Size, and Procedure .............................. 76

    4.3.5.1 Assessing Face Validity and Reliability of the Scale............. 76 4.3.5.2 Assessing Content Validity of the Scale ............................... 78

    4.3.6 Data Analysis .................................................................................... 80 4.4 Stage Three: Cross-Sectional Survey......................................................... 81

    4.4.1Aims ................................................................................................... 81 4.4.2 Research Questions ........................................................................... 81 4.4.3 Research Design................................................................................ 82 4.4.4 Sample Size and Power Analysis...................................................... 82

  • vii

    4.4.5 Sampling Strategy and Procedure ..................................................... 83 4.5 Definitions of Study Variables................................................................... 85 4.6 Instrument/ Measurement Strategy ............................................................ 86

    4.6.1 Instrument Translation ...................................................................... 86 4.6.2 Measures/ Assessment tools.............................................................. 87

    4.6.2.1 Sociodemographic Variables ................................................ 87 4.6.2.2 Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy................................................. 87 4.6.2.3 Pain and Opioid Analgesic Beliefs ....................................... 88 4.6.2.4 Analgesic Adherence ............................................................ 88 4.6.2.5 Pain Experience..................................................................... 91

    4.7 Data Analysis Plan ..................................................................................... 92 4.7.1 Preanalysis Phase .............................................................................. 92 4.7.2 Analysis Phase .................................................................................. 93

    4.8 Research Ethics / Statement ....................................................................... 94 4.9 Summary .................................................................................................... 95 Chapter 5: Interview Findings and Pilot Testing........................................ 97 5.0 Introduction................................................................................................ 97 5.1 Stage One: Development of the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale.......... 97

    5.1.1 Framework for the Semi Structured Interview................................... 97 5.1.2 Sample Characteristics of the Semi-Structured Interview ................. 99 5.1.3 Findings from the Semi-Structured Interviews ................................ 100

    5.1.3.1 Communicating about Pain and Analgesic-Taking ............. 102 5.1.3.2 Tailoring Medication Regimens........................................... 106 5.1.3.3 Taking Analgesics According to Schedule .......................... 111 5.1.3.4 Acquiring Help..................................................................... 113 5.1.3.5 Managing Treatment Related Concerns............................... 116

    5.2 Item Development for the Opioid Self-Efficacy Scale ............................. 120 5.2.1 Integrating Items from Literature Review into the Generated Scale 120 5.2.2 Scaling Format and Scoring Procedures .......................................... 124

    5.3 Expert Review of the Scale ....................................................................... 124 5.3.1 Native English Speaking Experts Review of the Scale.................... 124 5.3.2 Scale Translation.............................................................................. 127 5.3.3 Native Chinese Speaking Experts Review of the Scale ................... 128

    5.4 Stage Two: Pilot Test ................................................................................ 131 5.4.1 Sample Characteristics of Pilot Test ................................................ 131 5.4.2 Feasibility......................................................................................... 134 5.4.3 Internal Consistency of the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale...... 134 5.4.4 Test-Retest Reliability of the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale... 135

    5.5 Summary ................................................................................................... 137 Chapter 6: Results of Cross-Sectional Survey............................................ 138 6.0 Introduction............................................................................................... 138 6.1 Stage Three: Cross-Sectional Survey........................................................ 138

    6.1.1 Sample Characteristics for the Cross-Sectional Survey................... 138 6.2 Evaluating Construct Validity of the Self-Efficacy Scale......................... 142

    6.2.1 Sample Size...................................................................................... 142 6.2.2 Factor Analysis for the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Items ............ 145 6.2.3 Coefficient Alpha for the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale......... 158

  • viii

    6.3 Coefficient Alpha for the Brief Pain Inventory and the Pain Opioid Analgesics Beliefs Scale ........................................................................... 160

    6.4 Analysis of Normality ............................................................................... 160 6.5 Results of Research Questions .................................................................. 161 6.6 Summary ................................................................................................... 183 Chapter 7: Discussion ................................................................................... 185 7.0 Introduction............................................................................................... 185 7.1 Major Findings of the Study: Pain Experience amongst Taiwanese

    Outpatients with Cancer............................................................................ 185 7.2 Major Findings of the Study: Predictors of Pain Outcomes in Taiwanese

    Outpatients with Cancer............................................................................ 190 7.2.1 Adherence to Analgesics amongst Taiwanese Outpatients with

    Cancer .............................................................................................. 191 7.2.2 Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics in Taiwanese Outpatients with

    Cancer .............................................................................................. 192 7.2.2.1 Describing Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics in Taiwanese

    Outpatients with Cancer....................................................... 192 7.2.2.2 Relationships between Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics,

    Analgesic Adherence, and Pain Experience in Taiwanese Outpatients with Cancer....................................................... 195

    7.2.3 Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy in Taiwanese Outpatients with Cancer .......................................................................................................... 197 7.2.3.1 Levels of Self-Efficacy in Taiwanese Outpatients with

    Cancer .................................................................................. 197 7.2.3.2 Relationships between Self-Efficacy, Analgesic Adherence,

    and Pain Experience in Taiwanese Outpatients with Cancer .............................................................................................. 202

    7.3 Strengths and Limitations of the Study..................................................... 207 7.4 Implications of the Study and Recommendations for Future Research.... 210 7.5 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 217 Appendices ..................................................................................................... 219 References ...................................................................................................... 265

  • ix

    List of Appendices

    Appendix A: Study Information Sheet for Interview (Stage 1) ...................... 219

    Appendix B: Study Information Sheet for Survey (Stage 2 & Stage 3) ......... 220

    Appendix C: Study Consent Form (Stage1, 2 & 3) ........................................ 221

    Appendix D: Consent Form for Transcription................................................ 222

    Appendix E: Instruction Sheet ........................................................................ 223

    Appendix F: Demographic Form .................................................................... 224

    Appendix G: Medical Characteristic Form..................................................... 225

    Appendix H: Brief Pain Inventory (BPI- Short Form) Questionnaire ............ 226

    Appendix I: Pain Opioid Analgesics Beliefs Scale-Cancer (POABS-CA)..... 228

    Appendix J: Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale- Cancer (OTSES-CA) ........ 229

    Appendix K: Form for Collecting Prescribed Opioid Analgesics .................. 232

    Appendix L: Permission for Using Brief Pain Inventory (BPI-Short Form)

    Questionnaire ............................................................................. 233

    Appendix M: Permission for Using Pain Opioid Analgesics Beliefs

    Scale-Cancer (POABS-CA) ....................................................... 234

    Appendix N: Interview Questions................................................................... 235

    Appendix O: Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale for Expert Panellists

    Review ....................................................................................... 236

    Appendix P: Consent Form for Agreement to Participate on Expert Panel.... 243

    Appendix Q: Consent Form for Agreement to Participate on Expert Panel ... 244

    Appendix R: Consent Form for Agreement to Participate on Expert Panel ... 245

    Appendix S: Consent Form for Agreement to Participate on Expert Panel.... 246

    Appendix T: Consent Form for Agreement to Participate on Expert Panel ... 247

    Appendix U: CVI of each item in the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale

    (English Version) ...................................................................... 248

    Appendix V: CVI of each item in the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale

    (Chinese Version)...................................................................... 252

    Appendix W: Structured Interview Guides for Pilot Test............................... 254

    Appendix X: Permission to Tape Interviews .................................................. 255

    Appendix Y: Ethics Committee Approval ...................................................... 256

    Appendix Z: The Chinese Version of Questionnaires in Present Research.... 257

  • x

    List of Tables

    Table 4.1 Variable, Conceptual Definition, and Operational Definition .......... 85

    Table 5.1 Characteristics of Interview Participants for Stage 1 (N = 10) ....... 100

    Table 5.2 Categories of Behaviours and Situations Associated with

    Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy............................................................ 101

    Table 5.3 Preliminary Items Developed from Stage One Interviews

    and Literature Review...................................................................... 121

    Table 5.4 Number of Items Generated from the Interview Findings

    and the Literature Review ................................................................ 123

    Table 5.5 Suggestions of Native English Speaking Experts on the

    Preliminary 36-Item Self-Efficacy Scale ........................................ 126

    Table 5.6 Summary of Changes Made to Items as a Result of Translation into

    Chinese Version of the Self-Efficacy Scale ..................................... 128

    Table 5.7 Suggestions of Native Chinese Speaking Experts on the 35-Item

    Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale ................................................. 130

    Table 5.8 Demographic Variables of the Sample for Stage 2 (N = 30) .......... 131

    Table 5.9 Medical Characteristics of the Sample for Stage 2 (N = 30) .......... 133

    Table 5.10 Cronbach’s Alpha Values of the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy

    Total Scale and Subscales (Original Version) (N=30)..................... 135

    Table 5.11 Test-Retest Reliability of Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy

    Total Scale and Subscales (Original Version) (N=30)..................... 136

    Table 6.1 Demographic Data of the Sample for Stage 3 (N = 92).................. 139

    Table 6.2 Medical Characteristics of the Sample for Stage 3 (N = 92) .......... 141

    Table 6.3 Components of Factors and Communality for the 35-Item

    Self-Efficacy Scale (N=92) (Initial Factor Analysis) ...................... 146

    Table 6.4 Summary of Items Loading on the Factors and Item Inclusion

    for the 35-Item Self-Efficacy Scale (Initial Factor Analysis) .......... 151

    Table 6.5 Components of Factors and Communalities for the 31-Item

    Self-Efficacy Scale (N=92) (Final Factor Analysis)....................... 153

    able 6.6 Correlations between Factor4, 5, and 6 of the 31-Item Self-

    Efficacy Scale (N=92)...................................................................... 155

  • xi

    Table 6.7 Items Loading on the Factors along with Initial Conceptual

    Description for the 31-Item Self-Efficacy Scale.............................. 156

    Table 6.8 Subscale Names and Item Descriptions for the 30-Item

    Self-Efficacy Scale........................................................................... 157

    Table 6.9 Reliability of Subscales and Total Scale for the 30-Item

    Self-Efficacy Scale (N=92) .............................................................. 159

    Table 6.10 Correlations of the Four Subscales of the 30-Item

    Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy Scale (N=92) ..................................... 160

    Table 6.11 Cronbach’s Alpha Values of the Brief Pain Inventory

    and the Pain Opioid Analgesics Beliefs Scale (N=92) .................... 160

    Table 6.12 Analysis of Skewness and Kurtosis for Variables ........................ 161

    Table 6.13 Participants’ Score on the Brief Pain Inventory

    (BPI, Short Form) (N=92)................................................................ 162

    Table 6.14 Participants’ Pain Relief (N=92)................................................... 163

    Table 6.15 Participants’ Score on the Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy

    Scale (N=92) .................................................................................... 164

    Table 6.16 Participants’ Score on the Pain Opioid Analgesics

    Beliefs Scale (N=92)........................................................................ 167

    Table 6.17 Total Daily Oral Morphine Equivalent Conversion Table............ 169

    Table 6.18 Adherence Rates for Opioid Analgesics ....................................... 169

    Table 6.19 Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy, Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics,

    Analgesic Adherence, and Pain Experience by Gender................... 171

    Table 6.20 Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy, Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics,

    Analgesic Adherence, and Pain Experience by Age and Education

    .......................................................................................................... 173

    Table 6.21 Correlations between Beliefs about Opioid Analgesics,

    Opioid-Taking Self-Efficacy, Analgesic Adherence, and

    Pain Experience (Pearson’s r)......................................................... 175

    Table 6.22 Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Variables

    Predicting Pain Experience .............................................................. 180

    Table 6.23 Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Variable

    Predicting Opioid Adherence on an ATC Basis .............................. 183

  • xii

    List of Figures

    Figure 3.1 Self-Efficacy Model, Adapted from McDowell, Courtney, Edwards, &

    Shortridge-Baggett, 2003 ................................................................ 45

    Figure 5.1 Bland-Altman Plots (n=30) to Establish the Agreement between

    Test and Retest for Self-Efficacy. The Mean Difference (Solid Line)

    and the Limit of Agreement (Broken Lines- 95% Confidence Interval)

    Are Indicated................................................................................. 136

    Figure 6.1 Scree Plot ....................................................................................... 148

  • xiii

    Statement of Original Authorship

    The work contained in this thesis has not been previously submitted to meet

    requirements for an award at this or any other higher education institution. To the

    best of my knowledge and belief, the thesis contains no material previously

    published or written by another person except where due reference is made.

    Signature: ____________________

    Date: _____________________

  • xiv

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to thank my Principal Supervisor Professor Patsy Yates for her advice

    and patient guidance during my doctoral studies and, in particular, for her

    organizational insight and editorial review. I would also like to thank my Associate

    Supervisor Professor Helen Edwards for her insightful comments and criticisms and

    substantive opinions throughout this process. I am grateful for the time they devoted

    to this project and for providing me with numerous learning opportunities over the

    past several years.

    Special thanks are extended to Dr. Zandy Clavarino for reviewing this document and

    providing her invaluable comments and Professor Mary Courtney for being my panel

    member. Appreciation is extended to Professor Christine Miaskowski for the

    research measurement advice. Professor Lillie Shortridge-Baggett’s and Dr. Jan

    McDowell’s invaluable suggestions and counsel related to instrument development

    are deeply appreciated. Special thanks are also given to language and learning

    adviser Martin Reese for advice about my academic writing.

    I would like to express my thanks to my family for their unceasing support and

    encouragement during all my academic endeavours. Their support, encouragement

    and love have always been, and will remain, my motivation in life.

    Finally, I would like to thank the directors of the oncology department in the two

    hospitals for permitting their clinicians to refer their patients for the study. I also

    would like to thank all of the subjects who participated in this study. Their

  • xv

    willingness to contribute to improving the care of those experiencing cancer is a

    constant source of inspiration for me.

  • 1

    Chapter 1: Introduction

    1.0 Introduction Pain is a frequent and disturbing symptom of cancer. For many patients it is the most

    feared consequence of cancer. Unrelieved pain causes unnecessary suffering.

    Effective pain control is associated with an overall increase in quality of life and

    should allow patients to have higher levels of function. The purpose of this chapter is

    to discuss the background and significance of the study, and to present the aims of

    the research. The chapter also briefly describes the research design and methods.

    1.1 Background and Significance of the Study 1.1.1 The Prevalence of Cancer Pain

    In an advanced stage of cancer, pain is a major symptom for many patients. Surveys

    demonstrate that about 30% to 50% of cancer patients, and 70% to 90% of those in

    an advanced stage, suffer from chronic pain during active cancer therapy (Foley,

    2000; Higginson & Hearn, 1997; Jun, Shujun, & Lijun, 2000; Portenoy & Lesage,

    1999; Svendsen et al., 2005; Wells, 2000). A study of 9,105 terminally ill patients

    revealed that 50% of conscious patients who died in hospital experienced moderate

    to severe pain at least half the time (Connors, 1995). Experts estimate that 80-90% of

    cancer pain can be relieved with available methods of pain relief (U. S. Department

    of Health and Human Services, 1994; American Pain Society, 2003; Reder, 2001;

    World Health Organization, 1996). Several studies have demonstrated that,

    unfortunately, these methods are not always used for these patients, leading to

    inadequate pain relief (Jun et al., 2000; Moriwaki et al., 2000; Wit, Dam, Vielvoye-

    Kerkmeer, Mattern, & Abu-Saad, 1999) and diverse effects on many aspects of

    patients’ lives (Jun et al., 2000).

  • 2

    1.1.2 Impact of Cancer Pain

    Pain is a significant and obvious cause of suffering. Cancer pain not only affects

    patients’ physical well-being, but also affects their psychological, social, and

    spiritual well-being and thus their quality of life (QOL) (Holtan et al., 2007; Jun et

    al., 2000). It can impede functional capability (Thomason et al., 1998), and can result

    in disturbed sleep (Avemark, Ericsson, & Ljunggren, 2003; Jun et al., 2000),

    decreased activity (Jun et al., 2000), loss of appetite (Avemark et al., 2003),

    increased depression, and anxiety (Chen, Chang, & Yeh, 2000; Ferrell, Grant, Funk,

    Otis-Green, & Garcia, 1998; Liang & Lin, 1999), and loss of control (Ferrell et al.,

    1998). Pain interferes with relationships and roles and with the ability to interact with

    loved ones (Ferrell et al., 1998; Keefe et al., 2003). Pain can also create a sense of

    uncertainty and hopelessness (Avemark et al., 2003; Ferrell et al., 1998), with

    emotional distress being the most consistent psychological variable associated with

    pain reports in the cancer population. Furthermore, some researchers (Faller,

    Buelzebruck, Drings, & Lang, 1999; Herndon et al., 1999; Mantyh, 2006) have

    demonstrated that such emotional distress is significantly associated with shorter

    survival times among lung and breast cancer patients, independent of biomedical

    prognostic factors. Even when the underlying disease process is stable, uncontrolled

    pain prevents patients from working productively, enjoying recreation, or taking

    pleasure in their usual roles in the family and society (Jun et al., 2000).

    Pain has also been identified as one of the consequences most feared by cancer

    patients (Ghooi & Ghooi, 2003; Holtan et al., 2007). Marchettini, Formaglio, and

    Lacerenza (2001) have shown that cancer is a very or extremely painful disease, and

    that pain is also associated with its treatment. Studies revealed that patients agreed

  • 3

    that cancer pain could be a reason for stopping life-prolonging treatment or for

    committing suicide (Filiberti et al., 2001; Morita, Sakaguchi, Hirai, Tsuneto, &

    Shima, 2004). Moreover, they thought that cancer patients usually die a painful death

    (Ghooi & Ghooi, 2003). Unfortunately, poor control of cancer pain appears to be a

    problem all over the world (Foley, 2000; Reyes-Gibby et al., 2006; Wells, 2000).

    Pain control, therefore, merits a high priority.

    1.1.3 Cancer Pain in Taiwan

    Cancer has been the leading cause of death in Taiwan since 1982 (Department of

    Health, 1983-2005). In the year 2005, cancer amounted to 26.8% of all causes of

    death, and the mortality rate from cancer in Taiwan was 163.8 per 100,000 of the

    population (Department of Health, 1983-2005). Studies estimate that 31% to 85% of

    cancer patients in Taiwan suffer from pain (Chiu, 1997; Ger et al., 1998). For

    example, Ger et al. (1998) reported that 38% of cancer patients in Taiwan had

    cancer-related pain at the time of admission. Of the patients experiencing pain, 92%

    had cancer-related pain, 5% had treatment-related pain, and 3% had both cancer-

    related and treatment-related pain. Moreover, 65% to 77% of patients in pain had

    “significant worst pain” (worst pain levels at or above five on a ten-point scale) (Ger

    et al., 2004; Ger et al., 1998), while 31% had “significant average pain” (average

    pain levels at or above five most of the time). Importantly, 54% to 69% of patients in

    pain received inadequate medication (not “by the ladder”) (Ger et al., 2004; Ger et al.,

    1998) and 23% of patients had pain medication that was not administered at a fixed

    interval (not “by the clock”) (Ger et al., 1998). Most patients (69%) in this study,

    however, were newly diagnosed (within 14 days) and had just started receiving

  • 4

    anticancer therapy. Nevertheless, significant pain can be present for long periods of

    time, especially with the development of treatments that prolong life.

    1.2 Problem Statement and Significance of this Study Despite the fact that 80-90% of patients could be effectively treated with present

    pharmacological therapies and advanced techniques (U. S. Department of Health and

    Human Services, 1994; American Pain Society, 2003; Reder, 2001; World Health

    Organization, 1996), pain still remains a major source of suffering experienced by

    cancer patients (Higginson & Hearn, 1997; Holtan et al., 2007; Jun et al., 2000).

    Many deficiencies exist in the treatment of cancer pain (Anderson et al., 2000;

    Fazeny et al., 2000; Gunnarsdottir, Donovan, Serlin, Voge, & Ward, 2002; Liu et al.,

    2001; Pargeon & Hailey, 1999). The sources of these deficits are multidimensional,

    including healthcare provider and patient factors (Anderson et al., 2000; Fazeny et al.,

    2000; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002; Pargeon & Hailey, 1999). The major reasons (35%-

    64.4%) for poor management of pain are reported to be patient-related factors

    (Ferrell, Eberts, McCaffery, & Grant, 1991; Liu et al., 2001).

    Because pharmacological agents are considered to be a cornerstone of cancer pain

    management, patients’ adherence to prescribed analgesics is a key to successful

    cancer pain control. Consequently, optimal management is dependent on the

    patient’s compliance with pain therapies. Evidence suggests that patients, however,

    are often reluctant to use pain medication; many patients have misconceptions

    regarding analgesics, and do not have enough knowledge about cancer pain and pain

    management (Chang, Chang, Chiou, Tsou, & Lin, 2002; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002;

    Yates et al., 2002). Furthermore, many patients have practical difficulties with

    adherence to prescribed medications, such as managing constipation in relation to

  • 5

    opioid analgesics or accessing medication (Miaskowski et al., 2001; Schumacher,

    Koresawa et al., 2002). These factors are likely to effect treatment adherence and to

    contribute to inadequate pain relief (Chang et al., 2002; Ersek, Miller Kraybill, & Du

    Pen, 1999; Lai et al., 2002; Lin, Chou, Wu, Chang, & Lai, 2006; Thomason et al.,

    1998).

    Social and behavioural scientists have developed a number of theories and models in

    an effort to explain health behaviours such as treatment adherence. These models

    include the Health Belief Model (HBM) (Becker, 1974), Theory of Reasoned Action

    (TRA) (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975), and Social-Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 1986).

    Such models propose that a person’s belief system crucially influences his or her

    behaviour. In the context of pain management these models therefore suggest that

    beliefs are one of the key factors contributing to adherence to medication. Moreover

    self-efficacy expectations have been proposed in Social Cognitive Theory as a key

    factor in understanding health behaviours such as pain management behaviours.

    Self-efficacy expectation is defined as a person’s confidence in being able to perform

    relevant behaviours in a particular situation (Bandura, 1997). Efficacy beliefs are the

    perceived ability to regulate the behaviour effectively and consistently, especially

    under difficult circumstances (Bandura, 1997). According to Social Cognitive

    Theory (Bandura, 1986), individuals who regard themselves as highly efficacious

    with respect to adherence will set higher goals, be more firmly committed to them,

    and so exercise greater control over behaviours that foster adherence. Several recent

    studies have addressed the role of self-efficacy in the adherence of patients to

    exercise (Sweeney, Taylor, & Calin, 2002), diet (Burke, Dunbar-Jacob, Sereika, &

  • 6

    Ewart, 2003), and medication (Berg, Dunbar-Jacob, & Sereika, 1997; Gifford et al.,

    2000; Johnson et al., 2006; Simoni, Frick, & Huang, 2006). These studies reveal that

    self-efficacy beliefs predict adoption and maintenance of exercise behaviours and

    weight reduction. Self-efficacy has been shown to correlate positively with

    compliance with medications in persons with asthma and HIV/AIDS (Berg et al.,

    1997; Gifford et al., 2000; Simoni et al., 2006).

    Numerous investigators have begun to examine the relationship of perceived self-

    efficacy with the perception of chronic non-malignant pain, and have consistently

    shown that perceived self-efficacy is an important determinant of the perception of

    chronic pain. For example, perceived self-efficacy is associated with degree of

    disability (Barry, Guo, Kerns, Duong, & Reid, 2003), psychological functioning

    (Arnstein, Caudill, Mandle, Norris, & Beasley, 1999; Lefebvre et al., 1999; Rahman,

    2004), coping behaviour (Lin, 1998a), and medication adherence (Brus, van de Laar,

    Taal, Rasker, & Wiegman, 1999; Johnson et al., 2006; Simoni et al., 2006). The

    results of these studies suggest that self-efficacy theory may lead to refinements in

    understanding of behavioural interventions that may improve both treatment

    outcomes and the maintenance of treatment gains (Barry et al., 2003; Brus et al.,

    1999; Lefebvre et al., 1999).

    Nevertheless, few studies have specifically explored self-efficacy relating to

    management of cancer pain. Syrjala and Chapko (1995) focused on a

    biopsychosocial model of cancer-treatment-related pain. Their results suggested that

    biomedical variables did not fully reflect the sources of cancer-treatment-related pain,

    with self-efficacy being a significant predictor of pain for patients. These results

  • 7

    emphasise the value of self-efficacy as a construct worthy of evaluation with not only

    non-malignant pain, but also cancer pain.

    With an increase in outpatient care, patient self-care has become a necessary

    component of the cancer experience. Patients are assuming greater responsibility for

    cancer pain management at home, so many will have to undertake self-care activities

    to control their pain. Many outpatients with cancer will independently regulate their

    use of pain medications in response to the complex nature of cancer pain and

    incidence of breakthrough pain at home. Because pharmacological agents are

    considered to be a cornerstone of cancer pain management, patients’ adherence to

    prescribed analgesics has been recognised as the key to successful cancer pain

    control. Due to various difficulties (Beck, 1998; Elliott, Elliott, Murray, Braun, &

    Johnson, 1996; Miaskowski et al., 2001; Riddell & Fitch, 1997; Schumacher,

    Koresawa et al., 2002), however, medication adherence rates amongst patients who

    have prescribed analgesics for their cancer pain are lower than what is needed to

    achieve optimal pain control (Chang et al., 2002; Du Pen et al., 1999; Lai et al., 2002;

    Miaskowski et al., 2001; Zeppetella, 1999).

    Active participation in health behaviours can influence outcomes (Punamaki &

    Aschan, 1994). Availability of a psychometrically robust instrument to study active

    participation as characterised by self-efficacy could assist in efforts to understand the

    psychological determinants of health outcomes such as pain. Because self-efficacy is

    task-specific, every domain of interest, such as health-related self-care behaviour,

    demands a different self-efficacy measure (Berg et al., 1997; Burke et al., 2003;

    Simoni et al., 2006; Sweeney et al., 2002). A search of the literature yielded no

  • 8

    instrument that measured perceived opioid-taking self-efficacy in cancer patients

    with pain. A standardised measure of self-efficacy among cancer patients with pain is

    needed.

    Studying self-efficacy in cancer patients with pain is important to help health care

    professionals to understand the obstacles to cancer pain management. This may

    enable the development of more effective strategies for overcoming barriers to

    effective treatment of cancer pain.

    1.3 Aims 1.3.1 Aims of the Research

    The aims of this research were as follows:

    1. To develop a scale to measure the self-efficacy expectations in opioid-taking in

    Taiwanese outpatients with cancer.

    2. To explore how opioid-taking self-efficacy and beliefs about opioid analgesics

    contribute to patients’ analgesic adherence and pain experience in Taiwanese

    outpatients with cancer.

    1.3.2 Research Questions

    There were three stages in this study. The research questions for these three stages

    were as follows:

    Stage One

    1. What are the behavioural tasks and situational impediments involved in opioid-

    taking self-efficacy in Taiwanese outpatients with cancer pain?

  • 9

    Stage Two

    1. What is the validity and reliability of the newly developed opioid-taking self-

    efficacy scale in Taiwanese outpatients with cancer?

    2. Is the self-efficacy scale feasible for use in a cross-sectional survey of Taiwanese

    outpatients with cancer?

    Stage Three

    1. What is the pain experience amongst Taiwanese outpatients with cancer?

    2. What is the level of opioid-taking self-efficacy in Taiwanese outpatients with

    cancer?

    3. What beliefs about opioid analgesics do Taiwanese outpatients with cancer have?

    4. What is the adherence rate for analgesics amongst Taiwanese outpatients with

    cancer?

    5. What is the relationship between opioid-taking self-efficacy, beliefs about opioid

    analgesics, adherence behaviours, and pain experience amongst Taiwanese

    outpatients with cancer?

    6. How much of the variance in pain experience amongst Taiwanese outpatients with

    cancer could be accounted for by opioid-taking self-efficacy and beliefs about

    opioid analgesics?

    7. How much of the variance in analgesic adherence amongst Taiwanese outpatients

    with cancer could be accounted for by opioid-taking self-efficacy and beliefs

    about opioid analgesics?

    1.4 Research Design and Methods This study was divided into three stages. Stage One involved semi-structured

    interviews, comprising a series of open-ended questions, to describe the self-efficacy

    expectations relating to the behaviours and situational impediments or challenges

  • 10

    involved in prescribed opioid-taking amongst Taiwanese outpatients with cancer pain.

    In Stage Two, a preliminary test of the validity, reliability, and feasibility of the

    opioid-taking self-efficacy scale for this study was undertaken. Stage Three involved

    a cross-sectional survey to investigate pain experience, analgesic adherence, beliefs

    about opioid analgesics, and opioid-taking self-efficacy, and to determine how self-

    efficacy and beliefs about opioid analgesics are related to analgesic adherence and

    pain experience. The sampling frame for these three stages was defined as all those

    cancer patients with pain who were admitted to the outpatient oncology units of two

    teaching hospitals in the Taipei area of Taiwan. Patients were eligible for enrolment

    in the study if they met the following inclusion criteria: (1) had a cancer diagnosis; (2)

    had an average pain intensity score of ≥ 3 on a 0-10 scale in the past 24 hours; (3)

    had been prescribed opioid analgesics for cancer related pain on an around the clock

    (ATC) ± as needed (PRN) basis, and had taken them for at least the past week; (4)

    was over 18 years of age; (5) was conscious and able to sign the consent form.

    For Stage One, data were collected using purposeful sampling. Sampling was ceased

    when the researcher failed to obtain new information on the categories/themes from

    the participants. This stage began with a pilot study to familiarise the researcher with

    the process of conducting an interview and to assess the usefulness of a set of

    questions that had been developed for the semi-structured interviews. Scale items

    were written based on the themes emerging from the interview data and the literature.

    For Stage Two, a convenience sample with 30 eligible subjects was recruited to

    examine the face validity and reliability of the initial version of the opioid-taking

    self-efficacy scale and to assess the feasibility of the opioid-taking self-efficacy scale

  • 11

    for this study. This was done by measuring the time the subjects took to complete

    these scales, and asking them their views on the clarity of these questions, and

    whether they were easy to answer. The survey was administered to the same 30

    cancer patients to calculate test-retest reliability of the self-efficacy questionnaire

    after a two-week interval. For Stage Three a cross-sectional survey was administered

    to a convenience sample of 92 subjects.

    The researcher approached potential subjects who met the inclusion criteria and gave

    a brief explanation regarding the purposes and procedures of the study. Consent was

    obtained from those who met the criteria and agreed to participate. Depending on

    their physical condition and personal preference, the patients responded to the

    questions by themselves in writing, or orally to questions read by the researcher. The

    researcher collected demographic, disease, and treatment-related data from the

    patients’ medical records. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 13.0)

    was used for the quantitative data analysis.

    1.5 Overview of the Thesis The thesis consists of seven chapters. Chapter One describes the purpose and

    rationale for the study, research questions, background to the problem and its

    significance for health outcomes.

    Chapter Two presents a conceptual model of pain and multidimensional approaches

    to pain management. Pain is characterised as a multidimensional experience

    consisting of physiological, sensory, affective, cognitive, behavioural, and

    sociocultural components. This chapter discusses the mainstay of cancer pain

    control — pharmacological management. This requires a very individual, dynamic,

  • 12

    and ongoing approach. In addition, this chapter includes a review of evidence about

    the difficulties patients experience with adherence to analgesics. These practical

    difficulties may influence patients’ adherence to their prescribed regimen.

    Chapter Three provides a review of theoretical and empirical literature on self-

    efficacy beliefs. This chapter presents self-efficacy as the beliefs regarded as a

    requirement for behaviour change that have an important role in self-regulation. Self-

    efficacy beliefs affect the amount of effort devoted to a task, and the length of

    persistence when difficulties are encountered. This chapter presents evidence from

    various studies that indicate that self-efficacy beliefs have a critical function in health

    behaviour and health outcomes, including adherence to medications.

    Chapter Four outlines the methodology used in the current study. This chapter

    provides a detailed description and justification of the research design and methods

    of data collection, which included both quantitative and qualitative components. The

    chapter provides details of the sample, the procedures, the measures, and the

    processes of data collection and data analysis.

    Chapter Five presents findings of the interviews conducted with patients to identify

    key aspects of opioid-taking self-efficacy, presenting the major category and themes

    that emerged in the context of the patients’ beliefs about their abilities to successfully

    perform behaviours or tasks in relation to prescribed opioid taking. This Chapter also

    describes the development of the self-efficacy scale and the results of a pilot test to

    assess the validity and reliability of the scale and the feasibility of using the self-

    efficacy scale.

  • 13

    Chapter Six presents the findings from analysis of a cross-sectional survey in

    relation to the key research questions in this study. This results chapter includes the

    findings from an additional exploratory factor analysis to assess the construct validity

    of the new self-efficacy scale.

    Chapter Seven is a discussion of the major findings of this study. The results of this

    study are reviewed in the light of those from previous studies. The chapter includes

    an analysis of important conclusions and implications that can be drawn from the

    findings, and suggests how these may be applied to develop improvements in nursing

    care and nursing research.

  • 14

    Chapter 2: Conceptual Model of Pain and Pain Management

    2.0 Introduction Cancer-related pain is a multidimensional experience, consisting of physiological,

    sensory, affective, cognitive, behavioural, and sociocultural components. Pain is also

    highly subjective and is unique to the individual experiencing it. In this Chapter,

    definitions and models of pain and the various factors that influence an individual’s

    perception of and response to pain and its pharmacological and non-pharmacological

    management are presented.

    2.1 Definition of Pain Although a number of authors have proposed definitions of pain, a widely accepted

    definition is that of the International Association for the Study of Pain [IASP] (1986),

    which states that pain is “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated

    with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage” (p.217).

    This definition encompasses not only pain of pathophysiological origin but also the

    psychological dimension of the pain experience. McCaffery’s often-cited definition

    emphasises the subjective dimension of pain, when she writes that pain is “whatever

    the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever he or she says it does” (p.2)

    (McCaffery, 2000). This subjective nature of the pain experience presents some

    important challenges for its assessment, measurement, and management. In particular,

    these definitions reveal that the type and extent of disease may not be the only factor

    influencing an individual’s report of pain.

  • 15

    2.2 Multidimensional Components of Pain The concepts of the Gate Control Theory of Pain were the first to emphasise the

    different aspects of pain perception and integrate the physiological, psychological,

    and cognitive components involved in the experience of pain (Melzack & Wall, 1965;

    Melzack & Wall, 1996). The theory emphasised that pain has multidimensional

    components. The neuromatrix theory of pain was proposed as an expansion of the

    Gate Control Theory (Melzack & Wall, 1996) to understand brain functions as a

    neural network and provide an understanding of the multidimensional phenomenon

    of pain (Melzack, 1999; Melzack & Wall, 1996). The expanded theory of Melzack

    (1999) integrated pain and other stressors and identified the relationship between

    pain sensitivity and homeostatic physiological activities. A body self neuromatrix

    that can influence pain sensitivity to maintain homeostatic equilibrium was proposed.

    He suggested that this neurosignature pattern is modulated by three dimensions of

    pain experience and behaviour. Ahles, Blanchad and Rucksdeschel (1983) similarly

    built on the Gate Control Theory to describe a multidimensional conceptualisation of

    cancer-related pain, dividing the pain experiences of patients into five components:

    physiologic, sensory, affective, cognitive, and behavioural. McGuire (1995) added

    the sociocultural dimension to the personal experience of pain. This

    conceptualisation of cancer-related pain provides a useful framework to understand

    the factors that may affect an individual’s perception and interpretation of pain.

    Common to all contemporary models of pain is acknowledgement of the

    multidimensional nature of this experience. In the following section, these various

    components of the pain experience will be described; while they are presented

    separately for the purpose of description the complex interrelationships between the

    components need to be acknowledged.

  • 16

    2.2.1 Physiological Component of Pain

    The physiological component of cancer-related pain encompasses the underlying

    pathological basis of the pain. The pathophysiology of pain in cancer is complex.

    Cancer pain can be classified according to aetiology. Several studies have grouped

    cancer pain into three categories: (1) pain associated with cancer as a direct result of

    tumour growth (invasion of bone or neural structures is the most common cause)

    (Banning, Sjogren, & Henriksen, 1991a; Portenoy & Hagen, 1990); (2) pain as a

    consequence of antineoplastic therapy (Banning et al., 1991a); and (3) pain in cancer

    patients that is unrelated to their cancer (Banning et al., 1991a). Patients often

    present with multiple pains, many of which can have a combination of different

    causes (McGuire, 1991; Portenoy & Lesage, 1999).

    Cancer pain can also be divided into three classes: somatic, visceral, and

    deafferentation pain (American Cancer Society, 2001; Portenoy & Hagen, 1990).

    Somatic pain results from activation of nociceptors in cutaneous and deep tissues,

    such as bone or muscle (Kanner, 1993; Kelly & Payne, 1991). This pain is typically

    well localized and is usually constant (Kanner, 1993). Visceral pains are related to a

    lesion in a hollow or solid viscus, resulting from stretching or distending (Procacci &

    Maresca, 1990). It is typically poorly localized and commonly referred to

    subcutaneous sites (Patt, 1993). Deafferentation pain refers to a lesion of the

    peripheral and/or central nervous system, from tumour compression or infiltration of

    peripheral nerves or the spinal cord (Cherny & Foley, 1994). Also it can be caused

    by injury to peripheral nerves as a consequence of surgery, chemotherapy, or

    radiation therapy for cancer. Most patients with advanced cancer have multiple pains

  • 17

    due to different pathologies (Banning et al., 1991a; Dicks, 1990; Twycross, Harcourt,

    & Bergl, 1996).

    2.2.2 Sensory Component of Pain

    The sensory component of cancer pain is experienced in terms of its perceived

    quality and intensity. The quality of pain may vary according to the underlying cause

    or aetiology. For example, somatic pains are frequently described as aching, or

    gnawing (Kanner, 1993). Visceral pains result from stretching or distending

    (Procacci & Maresca, 1990); their character is described as deep, squeezing, and as

    pressure. Deafferentation pains are typically described as constant, burning, tight, or

    lancinating (Kanner, 1993).

    Pain intensity is the most commonly assessed aspect of pain. Intensity is an

    extremely subjective phenomenon that may or not be related to the underlying

    pathophysiology. As suggested by the multidimensional model of pain, pain intensity

    can be affected by many factors including medication, as well as emotional and

    cognitive factors (Ahles et al., 1983; Arnstein et al., 1999; Lefebvre et al., 1999).

    In addition to constant background pain, many cancer patients frequently experience

    breakthrough pain. Breakthrough pain has been defined as a transitory increase in

    pain intensity on a baseline of moderate intensity in patients on regularly

    administered analgesic treatment (Mercadante, Radbruch et al., 2002). It is

    characterised as spontaneous, occurring either without an identifiable precipitating

    event, or precipitated. In various surveys, 40-90% of cancer patients with pain have

    been reported to experience breakthrough pain (Banning, Sjogren, & Henriksen,

  • 18

    1991b; Davis, Walsh, Lagman, & LeGrand, 2005; Fine & Busch, 1998; Portenoy &

    Hagen, 1990). Breakthrough pain has been recognised as a challenging pain

    phenomenon in cancer.

    2.2.3 Affective Component of Pain

    The affective component of the pain experience encompasses the subjective

    unpleasant feeling of pain and the emotional disturbances or negative moods

    associated with pain, for example, anxiety, depression, and anger (Glover, Dibble,

    Dodd, & Miaskowski, 1995; Portenoy, Payne, & Jacobsen, 1999; Speigel, Sands, &

    Koopman, 1994; Zimmerman, Story, Gaston-Johansson, & Rowles, 1996).

    According to Chapman (1995), emotion is a fundamental part of the pain experience.

    The International Association for Study of Pain similarly states that pain “is

    unquestionably a sensation in a part or parts of the body, but it is also always

    unpleasant and therefore also an emotional experience” (p.250) (Merskey, Albe-

    Fessard, & Bonica, 1979). Conceptually, unpleasant emotional symptoms often occur

    concurrently with the pain sensation, which may trigger further emotional distress.

    As suggested by this multidimensional conceptualisation of pain, these emotional

    responses to pain may be affected not only by pain intensity, but also by cognitive

    processes. For example, studies reveal that patients reporting higher levels of self-

    efficacy have lower levels of pain as well as lower levels of psychological distress

    (Arnstein et al., 1999; Lefebvre et al., 1999).

    2.2.4 Cognitive Component of Pain

    The cognitive component of the pain experience relates to the processes by which

    pain influences the individual’s thoughts and the way in which they perceive,

  • 19

    interpret, and relate to their pain, for example, attitudes toward pain, reporting pain,

    pain medication, and specific self-efficacy beliefs (Wit, Dam, Litjens, & Abu-Saad,

    2001). Beliefs about pain (Lai et al., 2003; Sherwood, Adams-McNeill, Starck, Nieto,

    & Thompson, 2000), pain medication (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et al.,

    2002), and beliefs about ability to control pain (Asghari & Nicholas, 2001) are

    suggested also to be related to other key behavioural constructs, such as compliance

    with treatment.

    Patients’ knowledge and beliefs about pain are critical components in pain

    management (Beck, 1998; Lai et al., 2003). Lack of knowledge or misconceptions

    regarding pain may lead to poor symptom management. Patients frequently possess

    negative beliefs regarding cancer pain. For example, the idea that cancer pain is an

    inevitable part of the disease process (Levin, Cleeland, & Dar, 1985; Taylor &

    Crisler, 1988), pain can not be relieved (Levin et al., 1985; Riddell & Fitch, 1997),

    pain indicates a deteriorating condition (Ahles et al., 1983; Cleeland, 1989), when

    cancer pain persists and worsens it is an indicator of progressive disease (Lin &

    Ward, 1995; Moore, 1999; Ward, Goldberg, Miller-McCauley, & Mueller, 1993;

    Ward & Hernandez, 1994) and an adult should endure as much pain as possible (Lai

    et al., 2003), are common misconceptions. In addition, some patients believe the pain

    serves as a warning to protect the part that hurts and they might overdo it if they were

    too comfortable (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002). These beliefs

    may affect patients’ expectation about cancer pain management and the effect of

    analgesics, and therefore, effect patients’ willingness to take analgesics for managing

    their pain (Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002; Lai et al., 2002).

  • 20

    Many patients are reluctant to report pain due to various thoughts; however, without

    the patients’ detailed reporting, accurate pain assessment is impossible. Concerns of

    patients that may influence their beliefs about reporting pain include their concern

    about not being a “good” patient, if they complain about their pain to clinicians. In

    addition, patients may be concerned about distracting their physician from treating of

    underlying disease (Lin & Ward, 1995; Ward et al., 1993; Ward & Hernandez, 1994).

    Furthermore they may also fear a negative response from clinicians when they

    request narcotic analgesics (Tucker, 1990) and are concerned about clinician’s

    hesitancy to give medications because of fears of addiction (Ferrell, Rhiner, Cohen,

    & Grant, 1991). These considerations may limit their willingness to report pain and

    access to analgesics, thus resulting in unrelieved pain.

    Other studies have demonstrated that many patients are concerned about the use of

    pain medication. The concerns include concern about addiction, tolerance, and side

    effects (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002; Paice, Toy, & Shott,

    1998; Riddell & Fitch, 1997). The Word Health Organization (1996) has consistently

    reported that the likelihood of addiction occurring from use of opioids for pain relief

    is very small,

  • 21

    (Paice et al., 1998). Experts have suggested that concern about tolerance should

    never prevent the early use of analgesics for patients with cancer pain (Portenoy,

    1993).

    Concern about side effects deserves particular attention. Previous or ongoing

    experiences with analgesic side effects can limit patients’ willingness to follow

    prescribed treatment for pain management (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et

    al., 2002). Many participants tended to hold back on medication, because they were

    concerned about side effects such as nausea, constipation, and potential liver damage

    (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002). In addition, the unpleasant

    experiences of drowsiness and mental confusion may cause patients to be unwilling

    to take pain medications (Coward & Wilkie, 2000; Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002). This

    is despite the fact that many analgesic side effects can be prevented from occurring

    and others can be effectively managed with existing strategies (McNicol et al., 2003).

    Patients still hold the belief that side effects of analgesics are inevitable and

    unmanageable (Gunnarsdottir et al., 2002).

    Other beliefs about pain medications such as that pills may do more harm than

    suffering some pain (Coward & Wilkie, 2000) and pain medications were “bad” for

    their body (Riddell & Fitch, 1997) may influence patients to be reluctant to take pain

    medications regularly or to take sufficient doses for adequate pain relief.

    Patients’ negative beliefs regarding cancer pain and pain management, reporting their

    pain, and their concerns about use of analgesics are likely to influence patients’

    analgesic-taking behaviour. However, for reasons that are not fully understood, some

  • 22

    patients continue to have difficulty adhering to pain management regimens even after

    educational interventions in relation to knowledge and beliefs about pain

    management (Wells, Hepworth, Murphy, Wujcik, & Johnson, 2003; Yates, Edwards

    et al., 2004).

    Although pain management education results in improved pain control for some

    patients, it does not enable all patients to change their behaviour (Schumacher,

    Koresawa et al., 2002; Ward, Donovan, Owen, Grosen, & Serlin, 2000) or achieve

    reduction in pain (Ward et al., 2000; Yates, Edwards et al., 2004). In turn,

    improvements in patients’ knowledge and attitudes do not necessarily result in

    behaviour change. This may reflect the difficulties of applying this knowledge to a

    practical situation (Schumacher, Koresawa et al., 2002) and underscores the

    importance of understanding the multiple dimensions of pain. Clearly a full

    understanding of the complex and multidimensional nature of pain and pain

    management and the relationship between pain dimensions such as belief and

    behaviour is a priority (Du Pen et al., 1999).

    2.2.5 Behavioural Component of Pain

    The behavioural component of pain includes a variety of behaviours related to

    responses to pain, such as self-reporting of pain, or the abilities and behaviours

    related to use of pharmacological methods to treat pain (Kimberlin, Brushwood,

    Allen, Radson, & Wilson, 2004; Lin, 1998b). When patients make conscious

    decisions to engage in some of these behaviours, they make cognitive decisions. But

    the actual use of these techniques lies within the behavioural dimension (McGuire,

    1995). The notion highlights the importance of cognitive and behavioural

  • 23

    dimensions of cancer pain management. For example, the concept of self-efficacy

    beliefs has been used to explain many of the behaviours and disabilities of people in

    chronic non-cancer pain (Barry et al., 2003; Brus et al., 1999; Lefebvre et al., 1999),

    but has received less attention in the cancer pain literature.

    Around 80-90% of cancer patients could be kept free from cancer pain by following

    the pharmacological guidelines of the Agency of Health Care Policy and Research

    (AHCPR)(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1994; American Pain

    Society, 2003; Reder, 2001; World Health Organization, 1996). Patients’ adherence

    behaviour is therefore important in effective pain control.

    There is however, wide variation in adherence to prescribed analgesics. Studies

    report adherence levels ranging from 16% to 90% in patients with cancer-related

    pain (Chang et al., 2002; Du Pen et al., 1999; Lai et al., 2002; Miaskowski et al.,

    2001; Zeppetella, 1999). Overall adherence rates are reported to be around 22% to

    27% when patients use the analgesics prescribed on an as-needed basis (Miaskowski

    et al., 2001).

    Another important behavioural response to pain is communication about pain and

    pain management. Studies suggest patients’ negative beliefs about analgesics affects

    their reluctance to report pain and to use analgesics (Kimberlin et al., 2004; Lai et al.,

    2002).

    Studies also suggest that patients have difficulties overcoming various practical

    impediments to engaging in effective behavioural responses to pain, such as

  • 24

    accessing information, managing side effects, or tailoring prescribed regimens to

    meet individual needs (Schumacher, Koresawa et al., 2002). Bandura asserted that

    self-efficacy beliefs are a critical element that contribute to health behaviours such as

    medication adherence. Self-efficacy reflects the belief in one’s confidence in his or

    her ability to overcome various difficult situations in order to perform a specific

    behaviour. For example, according to Bandura, one’s confidence in his or her ability

    to overcome various difficult situations in performing a behaviour is an important

    bridge between knowing what to do and actually doing it. Besides knowing what to

    do, he or she must know how to do it and want to do it (Bandura, 1977a; Lawrance &

    McLeroy, 1986). These notions emphasize the crucial link between practical

    difficulties of taking analgesics and beliefs related to one’s confidence in his or her

    ability to overcome these difficulties. These associations further underscore the

    multidimensional nature of pain experiences, and the complex association between

    cognitive and behavioural aspects of pain. Few studies however, focus on self-

    efficacy beliefs associated with cancer pain management.

    2.2.6 Sociocultural Component of Pain

    The sociocultural component of pain includes demographic, ethnic, cultural and

    other factors, such as social support, that may influence perception of and response to

    pain (Cleeland, Gonin, Baez, Loehrer, & Pandya, 1997; Fillingim & Maixner, 1995;

    Thomason et al., 1998; Unruh, 1996). For example, sociocultural factors can

    influence an individual’s beliefs about pain and adjustment to the total pain

    experience. As Morris (1999) stated “no one experiences pain free from human

    mediation, including the mediation of our own experiences” (p. 118). This notion

    implies that an individual’s pain experience bears the imprint of social structures,

  • 25

    such as family or hospital, and of cultural environments such as religion, ethnic

    group, or nationality that necessarily shape and influence pain.

    Based on the neuromatrix theory of pain, cognitive processes involving emotional,

    sociocultural, and experiential factors influence the pain process. That is, when a

    noxious event occurs, the individual’s past memories and cultural beliefs may

    influence whether pain impulses reach the levels of awareness, and may significantly

    influence the perception of pain and the response to pain (Barkwell, 2005; Cohen,

    Musgrave, Munsell, Mendoza, & Gips, 2005). For example, cultural patterning and

    interpersonal relationships among people in the same culture may teach a person

    whether pain is to be ignored or accepted, or whether or not certain reactions to pain

    will receive approval. Culture similarly has an important influence on illness beliefs

    and health care utilization, help-seeking activities, and receptivity to medical care

    interventions (Juarez, Ferrell, & Borneman, 1999; Lasch et al., 2000; Moore &

    Spiegel, 2000).

    Although research has shown no racial or ethnic differences in sensory threshold

    (Ramer, Richardson, Cohen, & Bedney, 1999), some studies (Cleeland et al., 1997;

    Vallerand, Hasenau, Templin, & Collins-Bohler, 2005) indicate that pain tolerance

    reflects attitudinal and behavioural aspects of pain that may be culturally determined.

    These studies suggest culture has a significant impact on pain perception and pain

    responses, including the use of specific coping strategies (Cleeland et al., 1997;

    Juarez, Ferrell, & Booneman, 1998; Lin, 1998a). For example, Juarez et al. (1998)

    found that in Hispanic cancer patients, the suppressed verbal or behavioural

  • 26

    expression of pain does not indicate less pain, but rather the influence of culture,

    family beliefs, and religion on pain responses.

    Based on a Chinese cultural attribute, the teaching of Taoism, Chinese patients

    perceive cancer as an incurable disease; they accept pain as their fate, part of their

    life, and a reminder of life (Chung, Wong, & Yang, 2000). In addition, due to the

    abuse of opium in China in the nineteenth century, the Chinese government has a

    very stringent policy toward any opioids, thus giving the impression that opioids are

    bad. Beliefs that it is wrong to take opioids and that one can easily become addicted

    to opioids are deeply rooted in Chinese people’s minds (Ger, Ho, & Wang, 2000).

    Many cancer patients in Taiwan are, like Western patients, dealing with and

    suffering from pain (Chang et al., 2002; Lai et al., 2003; Lai et al., 2002). These

    problems may be highly influenced by the social and cultural context.

    2.3 Multidimensional Approaches to Pain Management The pain experience is multidimensional and must be viewed as an interrelated and

    interactive whole. For example, as illustrated by the multidimensional model of pain,

    pain (sensory component) may lead to psychological distress (affective component),

    which in turn may exacerbate the pain (sensory component) (Liang & Lin, 1999). In

    addition, patients’ perceptions of their capability to control their pain (cognitive

    component) may influence their adherence behaviour relevant to their

    pharmacological regimen (behavioural component) and therefore influence their pain

    intensity (sensory component) or psychological distress (affective component) or

    other outcomes of pain control.

  • 27

    To modify responses to multidimensional phenomenon of pain, a range of strategies

    has been considered for managing cancer pain, including pharmacological and non-

    pharmacological methods (U. S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1994).

    Non-pharmacological methods such as relaxation can enable patients to gain a sense

    of control and improve their coping skills. Many non-medicinal complementary

    therapies have been shown to help the patient with cancer. Strategies such as

    relaxation/imagery (Anderson et al., 2004; Kwekkeboom, Kneip, & Pearson, 2003;

    Wallace, 1997), distraction (Anderson et al., 2004), transcutaneous electrical nerve

    stimulation (TENS) (Chiarini et al., 1997; Hidderley & Weinel, 1997), acupuncture

    (Alimi et al., 2003; He, Friedrich, & Ertan, 1999; Mao-Ying et al., 2006), hypnosis

    (Richardson, Smith, McCall, & Pilkington, 2006), massage (Calenda, 2006; Wilkie,

    2000), or herbal medicine (Kalant, 2001; Martin & Willey, 2004) have been shown

    to be effective in improving pain outcomes. Nonpharmacological methods are

    especially targeted at the cognitive, affective and behavioural components of pain.

    For example, cognitive behavioural pain relief strategies such as guided imagery may

    help patients cope with the meaning of their pain (cognitive component) and

    decrease feelings of distress and hopelessness (affective component), thereby

    resulting in pain relief (sensory component). In cancer pain control such

    nonpharmacological measures are thus an important adjunct to pharmacological

    approaches for cancer pain management (U.S. Department of Health and Human

    Services, 1994).

    Pharmacological therapy is the cornerstone of cancer pain management. For effective

    pharmacological treatment of cancer pain, agreement exists on the principles of

    pharmacological management by several institutes, such as the World Health

  • 28

    Organization [WHO] (1996), American Pain Society (2003), and Agency for

    Healthcare Policy and Research [AHCPR] (U.S. Department of Health and Human

    Services, 1994). The appropriate application of pain management regimens relies on

    initiating therapy, titrating dose, scheduling around the clock (ATC), and

    preventing/minimizing side effects. These standards guide the appropriate

    application of pharmacological management of cancer pain.

    Essential components of analgesic therapy include administering it following the

    WHO ‘ladder’ regimen (World Health Organization, 1996). The WHO 3-step ladder

    of analgesic therapy is as follows: Step 1: non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs

    (NSAIDs); Step 2: weak opioid analgesics; Step 3: strong opioids, with (a) ± co-

    analgesics (adjuvants) at every step, (b) titrating drug dose to effect, (c) dosing

    around the clock (ATC), (d) using the oral route when possible, and (e) preventing

    and aggressively treating analgesic side effects. These pharmacological principles are

    also endorsed by the American Pain Society (American Pain Society, 2003) and the

    Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (U.S. Department of Health & Human

    Services, 1994).

    The goal of chronic cancer pain management is not only pain relief but also pain

    prevention (Mercadante, Villari, Ferrera, & Casuccio, 2004). Therefore, prescribing

    guidelines emphasise titrating the dose to effectiveness and then taking around the

    clock doses (ATC) at regularly scheduled intervals, rather than on the more common

    “as needed” basis (World Health Organization, 1996). The advantages of ATC

    therapy and oral medication are some of the motivations for the development of

    controlled-release, oral formulations of opioids (Reder, 2001). The reduced dosing

  • 29

    frequency makes the oral medication more convenient for patients, making it easier

    for them to comply with the dosing regimen, assuming analgesic use will become a

    routine behaviour. The exception to this is the recommended practice of having

    “breakthrough” medication available. Cancer patients with pain often routinely take

    one analgesic ATC and then learn to take another medication as needed (PRN) for

    extra pain. Guidelines also emphasise how to treat a number of side effects, including

    constipation, nausea, vomiting, sedation, mental clouding, respiratory depression, dry

    mouth, and sleep disturbances (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,

    1994).

    Studies show that skilled practitioners can adequately relieve upwards of 80-90% of

    patients from suffering cancer pain when treatment follows the recommended

    guidelines for pain management (Levy, 1994; Portenoy, 1993; Schug, Zech, & Dorr,

    1990; World Health Organization, 1996). Nevertheless, the variable and complex

    nature of cancer pain, the incidence of breakthrough pain and the side effects of

    analgesics, mean that there are many factors that can reduce the likelihood of

    adequate pain control (Mercadante, Fulfaro, & Casuccio, 2002; Mercadante et al.,

    2004; Portenoy et al., 1999; Zeppetella, O'Doherty, & Collins, 2000). In particular,

    the complex, variable and subjective nature of pain mean that patients need to tailor

    their medication regimens by problem-solving around individual responses to the

    medications and progressively modifying their regimen as needs change, for example

    in response to breakthrough pain at home. Tailoring a prescribed regimen to meet

    individual needs is a complex, dynamic, and ongoing effort (Schumacher, Koresawa

    et al., 2002). While such prescribing decisions are clinically based, patients are

    involved in decision-making for this tailoring process, as they need to be able to

  • 30

    interpret pain experiences, communicate these and respond appropriately; so their

    adherence to prescribed pain therapies may be affected (Chang et al., 2002; Du Pen

    et al., 1999; Lai et al., 2002; Miaskowski et al., 2001; Zeppetella, 1999). Such

    findings again underscore the importance of multidimensional models of pain, and

    developing a better understanding of the nature of the relationships between the

    various dimensions.

    2.4 Evidence Review on Difficulties with Patients’ Adherence to Pharmacological Therapies

    2.4.1 The Concepts of Adherence

    A number of terms are commonly used with respect to medication-taking, including

    “compliance” and “adherence” (Steiner & Earnest, 2000; Vlasnik, Aliotta, & DeLor,

    2005). Some researchers suggest, however, that these terms subtly overstate the

    importance of the clinician and may not accurately represent patients’ motivations

    for choosing to take their medications in a certain way (Bissell, May, & Noyce, 2004;

    Steiner & Earnest, 2000). Other researchers have thus described models such as the

    “self-medication hypothesis” (Mitchell, 2007) or the “concordance model” (Bissell et

    al., 2004) to reflect patient perspectives in the context of prescribed medication.

    More specifically, some writers argue that “compliance” implies a submissive

    behaviour of complying with a provider’s “will and order” (Vlasnik et al., 2005).

    Others prefer the contrasting term “adherence” (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005;

    Vlasnik et al., 2005), as this is seen to be a more patient-centred term. It is argued

    that such terms better reflect reality, that patients carry out and maintain certain

    behaviours, such as taking medications, after making an informed choice in a

  • 31

    supportive environment (Vlasnik et al., 2005). Adherence thus implies the patient has

    autonomy in choosing to follow a health care regimen (Evangelista, 1999). The term

    implies that patients are active participants in their own health care, with control over

    health care decisions. The concept of adherence further implies that the patient does

    not passively conform to health care advice (Evang