rahn jay african rhythm european mt

Upload: username519

Post on 02-Jun-2018

228 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    1/20

    Turning the Analysis around: Africa-Derived Rhythms and Europe-Derived Music TheoryAuthor(s): Jay RahnReviewed work(s):Source: Black Music Research Journal, Vol. 16, No. 1 (Spring, 1996), pp. 71-89Published by: Center for Black Music Research - Columbia College Chicagoand University of Illinois Press

    Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/779378.Accessed: 11/01/2012 00:21

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    University of Illinois Pressand Center for Black Music Research - Columbia College Chicagoare

    collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access toBlack Music Research Journal.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=cbmrhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=illinoishttp://www.jstor.org/stable/779378?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/779378?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=illinoishttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=cbmr
  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    2/20

    TURNING

    THE

    ANALYSIS AROUND:

    AFRICA-DERIVED RHYTHMS AND

    EUROPE-DERIVED MUSIC THEORY

    JAY

    RAHN

    As

    Samuel

    A.

    Floyd

    Jr.

    (1993,

    1)

    has

    emphasized,

    analysis

    is an

    activity

    that

    was

    developed

    as

    a

    way

    of

    examining chiefly Europe-derived

    works

    of music.

    Susan

    McClary

    and Robert Walser

    (1994,

    77)

    have stressed fur-

    ther that the

    Europe-derived discipline

    of music

    theory

    has

    notoriously

    neglected rhythm

    in

    favor of

    abstract

    patterns

    of

    pitch

    and

    form.

    I

    feel

    that

    Europe-derived theory

    and

    analysis

    have not handled well the

    fol-

    lowing

    topics, long

    and

    widely

    acknowledged

    to

    be

    important

    for

    Africa-

    derived

    traditions: constant

    syncopation,

    off-beat

    phrasing, turning

    the

    beat

    around,

    backbeat,

    cross-rhythm,

    anticipation

    (as

    contrasted with

    re-

    tardation),

    constant

    repetition

    of

    rhythmic

    and

    melodic

    figures,

    the relat-

    ed

    phenomena

    of

    call-and-response

    phrasing,

    riffs,

    vamps,

    time-lines,

    particular

    additive

    rhythms,

    metronomic

    pulse

    (or

    approximately,

    what

    ethnomusicologists

    have

    referred to

    as

    the metronome

    sense)-all

    this

    grounded

    in

    the

    unique

    forward momentum

    of

    unflagging

    rhythms

    highlighted by Floyd (1991, 268, 279;

    cf.

    Floyd 1993, 1, 4).

    Attempted

    below is a

    technical account

    of Africa-derived

    syncopation

    that

    might

    serve as an

    alternative to

    orthodox,

    Europe-derived

    accounts.

    Rather than

    depicting

    syncopated rhythms merely

    as deviations from a

    four-square

    metrical

    hierarchy,

    I

    try

    to

    show

    how

    they

    can be

    portrayed

    as

    highly integrated

    wholes

    in

    their

    own

    right.

    Such wholes favor a

    dif-

    ferent

    aesthetic

    imagery

    than

    has been usual

    in

    Europe-derived analyses

    of

    rhythm: specifically,

    (1)

    an

    imagery

    of

    complementation, long

    estab-

    JAY

    RAHN

    is associate

    professor

    and music

    coordinator,

    Fine

    Arts

    Department,

    Atkinson

    College, YorkUniversity. His writings include A TheoryforAll Music: Problemsand Solutions

    in the

    Analysis

    of

    Non-Western

    Forms

    (University

    of

    Toronto

    Press,

    1983)

    and,

    with Edith

    Fowke,

    A

    Family

    Heritage:

    The

    Story

    and

    Songs of

    LaRenaClark

    (University

    of

    Calgary

    Press,

    1993).

    71

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    3/20

    BMR

    Journal

    lished in

    Europe-derived

    discourse on

    pitch

    structure

    in

    post-tonal

    music

    (e.g., John

    Rahn

    1980)

    but little

    developed

    in orthodox

    rhythmic analysis;

    (2)

    metaphors

    of

    braiding

    and

    circularity,

    only

    recently

    developed by

    the

    technical

    apparatus

    of

    post-tonal theory;

    and

    (3)

    pendularity

    and

    cyclism,

    long recognized

    as central

    values in the

    aesthetic of

    Africa-derived

    music

    that

    Floyd

    has identified

    by

    the

    expression

    "Call-Response"

    but

    largely

    neglected

    in

    Europe-derived analyses.

    Such

    images

    contrast

    with

    con-

    ceptions

    that

    presume

    as

    central

    such

    metaphors

    and

    concepts

    as

    the

    fol-

    lowing:

    relentless

    linearity;

    the

    arrow

    of

    time;

    immediate

    expectancy;

    constantly

    frustrated,

    teleological, goal-directed

    processes

    (e.g.,

    of

    desire

    in Schenkerian

    analysis); segmentation;

    and

    asymmetric hierarchy.

    These

    two

    conceptual

    groupings

    greatly

    differ in

    their

    possible

    analytic

    conse-

    quences

    and

    can

    thrive

    independently.

    Nonetheless,

    I

    try

    to

    show

    that

    one can

    shift from

    one

    to

    the

    other,

    in

    whole

    or

    part,

    or

    even

    hold to

    both

    at

    once,

    without

    contradiction.

    There

    are at

    least

    two

    reasons for

    seeking

    such

    multiplicity

    in

    analysis.

    First,

    as

    Floyd

    has

    outlined,

    interactions

    between

    audience

    and

    perform-

    ers,

    and

    among

    performers

    themselves,

    are

    important

    components

    of

    the

    Call-Response

    aesthetic.

    Second,

    reception

    and

    appropriation

    of

    Africa-

    derived

    idioms and

    genres

    have

    been

    persistently multiple

    in

    their

    sensi-

    bilities for

    at least a

    century.

    Thus,

    Gary

    Tomlinson

    (1991,

    240),

    for

    exam-

    ple,

    advocates

    decentered

    analysis

    of

    jazz

    to admit

    a

    variety

    of

    vantage

    points

    rather than

    legislating

    a

    singular,

    authoritative

    perception.

    By

    contrast,

    Europe-derived

    music

    theory

    has

    tended

    to

    eschew inter-

    pretative

    diversity

    in

    favor

    of

    readings

    that

    are

    convergent, singular,

    un-

    ambiguous,

    exclusive-indeed,

    it

    could

    be

    said,

    canonic,

    authoritative,

    correct,

    or to

    use a

    long

    discredited

    term from

    the

    early

    decades

    of

    eth-

    nomusicology,

    authentic. In

    intercultural

    settings,

    intracultural

    ensemble,

    self-delectative solo

    performance,

    even

    reading

    or

    listening

    to

    perfor-

    mance that

    is

    mediated

    (e.g.,

    by

    recording

    or

    notation),

    one can

    find

    com-

    munity

    where

    one

    might

    have

    been led to

    expect,

    as

    a

    musico-social

    value,

    conformity

    (cf.

    McClary

    and

    Walser

    1994,

    79).

    Decentered

    analysis

    would

    reserve a

    spot

    for

    the

    interlocative

    self

    that can

    change

    places

    with

    another

    (Holquist

    and

    Liapunov

    1990,

    xxvi;

    cf.

    Bakhtin

    1990,

    22-23),

    even

    if

    the

    acts of

    others,

    which

    can

    comprise

    invention,

    sounding,

    moving,

    and,

    of

    course,

    listening,

    are

    merely

    imagined

    or

    recalled

    on

    the basis

    of

    sounds or

    other

    signifiers.

    Quite

    early

    in

    the

    history

    of

    African-American music

    analysis,

    Winthrop Sargeant

    (1938,

    58-64)

    identified the

    rhythm

    of

    Figure

    la as

    es-

    pecially typical

    of

    jazz

    and

    other

    Africa-derived

    styles.

    Jazz

    analysts

    long

    have

    recognized

    that this

    basic 3+3+2

    rhythm

    also

    can take

    such

    forms as

    those

    notated in

    Figure

    lb.

    Ernst

    Bornemann

    (1946),

    Gunther

    Schuller

    72

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    4/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    (1968,

    24),

    and Marshall Steams

    (1956, 142)

    traced

    such 3+3+2

    patterns

    to

    African

    musical traditions. Don

    Knowlton

    (1926,

    581), quoting

    an

    unidentified

    African-American

    guitarist,

    had used the

    expression

    "sec-

    ondary rag"

    for

    rhythms

    of this sort.

    Shortly

    thereafter,

    Aaron

    Copland

    (1927, 10-12)

    noted that the

    temporal organization

    of these

    "foxtrot"

    pat-

    terns contrasted with

    Europe-derived

    practice,

    suggesting

    unsyncopated

    measures of

    I,

    ],

    and

    A

    as

    notationally

    preferable

    to

    syncopated

    measures

    of t.

    Referring

    to

    the basic

    pattern

    of three

    syncopated

    tones within four

    quarter-note

    beats,

    Sargeant

    designated

    these

    rhythms

    "three-over-four,"

    and like later

    analysts

    traced

    them to

    Africa,

    crediting

    this

    important hy-

    pothesis

    to

    the much

    earlier,

    prodigious

    investigations

    of

    Nicholas

    J.

    G.

    Ballanta-Taylor

    (1922).1

    Figure

    1.

    3+3+2 and

    close

    variants

    a.

    Basic

    3+3+2

    rhythm

    J.

    J. J

    b. Close variants

    J.

    J

    J J

    bJ.

    JJ

    m;

    J

    As in

    such

    parallel

    cases as

    the

    so-called

    Scotch

    snap

    of

    strathspeys

    and

    flings

    and the canzona

    rhythm

    of

    Renaissance

    song

    and

    closely

    related in-

    strumental

    music,

    one can ask

    whether

    the

    3+3+2

    figure

    is

    structurally

    special

    or

    privileged.

    Or does

    this

    figure

    stand out

    stylistically

    only

    be-

    cause of its

    disproportionately

    frequent

    occurrence

    in

    traditions of

    Africa

    and

    the

    African

    diaspora?

    Is

    this

    rhythm,

    and

    others

    closely

    related

    to

    it,

    to be

    understood in

    its

    own

    terms,

    or

    only

    through

    the

    filter or

    lens

    of Eu-

    rope-derived

    common-practice

    rhythmic

    theory?

    I

    present

    two

    groups

    of

    interpretations

    of

    this

    and

    other,

    cognate

    patterns:

    one

    group

    set in

    a Eu-

    rope-derived

    framework

    of

    common-practice

    concepts,

    the

    other in

    an

    emerging

    paradigm

    of

    recent

    music

    theory.

    In a

    Europe-derived,

    common-practice

    conception

    of

    meter,

    the

    basic

    3+3+2

    rhythm

    can

    be

    considered

    both

    dotted and

    syncopated.

    In

    nota-

    tional

    terms,

    the

    beginning

    of

    the

    second dotted

    quarter

    is

    unprepared

    because no

    note

    appears

    on

    the

    second

    quarter

    and

    unresolved

    since

    there is

    no

    note on

    the

    third

    quarter.

    Extending

    this

    conception

    slightly,

    1.

    Ballanta-Taylor,

    who had

    just

    arrived in

    the

    United

    States from

    Sierra

    Leone via

    a Mus.

    Bac.

    degree

    at

    Durham

    University

    and

    studies of

    harmony

    with Sir

    John

    Stainer

    (on

    which

    see

    Cuney-Hare

    1936,

    347-348),

    published

    his

    pathbreaking

    technical

    account of

    African

    and

    closely

    related

    jazz

    rhythms

    partly

    in

    response

    to

    critical

    opinions

    that

    had

    been

    voiced

    in

    both

    the Boston

    Transcript

    and the

    Negro

    Musician.

    73

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    5/20

    BMR

    Journal

    the

    attack of the second dotted

    quarter

    can be

    considered to be both

    pre-

    pared

    and resolved

    texturally

    if

    the

    part containing

    the

    3+3+2

    rhythm

    is

    accompanied

    by

    another

    portion

    of the texture

    that has notes

    starting

    on

    the second and third

    quarters-as

    in a

    walking

    bass line in

    swing

    and

    later

    styles,

    a stride bass

    in

    piano rags

    and other

    march-related

    forms,

    or

    steady strumming

    in

    quarters

    by banjo

    or

    guitar

    in New

    Orleans,

    Chica-

    go,

    and other

    early jazz

    idioms

    (see

    Fig.

    2).

    Figure

    2.

    Preparation

    and

    resolution

    of syncopation

    of

    the

    basic 3+3+2

    rhythm

    Basic 3+3+2

    rhythm

    J.

    J. J

    Quarter-note

    pulsation

    J

    J

    J J

    Combined

    rhythm

    J J

    In

    a

    monophonic

    break or

    unaccompanied

    introduction,

    the

    second

    dotted

    quarter

    is not

    prepared

    or

    resolved

    explicitly-neither

    within

    the

    3+3+2 line

    itself,

    nor

    in

    another,

    simultaneous

    portion

    of the

    texture. If

    an

    immediately

    earlier or later

    passage

    has

    established a

    quarter-note

    pulsa-

    tion,

    the

    second

    dotted

    quarter

    could

    be

    considered

    prepared

    and

    re-

    solved in

    an

    extraordinary

    sense;

    that

    is,

    implicitly,

    or

    to use a

    logical

    term,

    modally,

    i.e.,

    by

    virtue

    of

    notes that

    could

    have

    begun

    on

    the

    sec-

    ond and

    third beats

    and

    thus could

    have

    formed

    time-interval

    matching

    relations with earlier

    or later

    pairs

    of

    notes. Even

    without a

    preceding

    or

    following

    passage

    of

    pulsating quarters,

    one can

    understand

    the

    second

    and

    third

    quarters

    as

    being modally

    implied by

    the end

    of

    the 3+3+2

    rhythm,

    providing

    an

    instance of

    what

    Sargeant

    appears

    to

    have

    meant

    by

    "internal"

    syncopation;

    that

    is,

    syncopation

    relative

    to

    a

    metrical

    hier-

    archy

    which can

    be

    inferred on

    the

    basis of

    the

    rhythm's

    own

    notes,

    irre-

    spective

    of

    other

    passages

    or

    parts

    of

    the

    texture.2

    Sargeant's

    observation

    that

    anticipations

    are

    more

    frequent

    in

    the

    early

    jazz

    he

    surveyed

    than

    are

    retardations

    can be

    understood

    in

    at

    least

    two

    ways.

    Disregarding

    such

    pitch

    structures as chord

    progressions,

    one can

    take

    his

    generalization

    to mean

    that

    an

    anticipatory

    transformation

    of a

    measure of four even

    quarters

    into an

    eighth,

    two

    quarters,

    and a dotted

    2.

    Such

    inferrable

    pulses,

    which

    can

    be

    "generated"

    in

    theory

    via a

    modal

    axiom,

    e.g.,

    along

    the

    lines of

    Jay

    Rahn

    (1978),

    could be

    considered to

    correspond

    to the

    metronome

    sense in

    ethnomusicological

    accounts.

    74

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    6/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    quarter,

    for

    example,

    is more usual than its retardative

    counterpart

    (actu-

    ally,

    its

    temporal

    inversion or

    retrograde)

    consisting

    of a dotted

    quarter,

    two

    quarters,

    and an

    eighth

    (see

    Fig.

    3).

    Figure

    3. Variants

    of

    basic

    quarter-notepulsation

    Basic

    quarter-note pulsation

    J

    J

    J

    Anticipatory

    variant

    b

    J

    J

    Retardative

    variant

    J

    J J

    b

    Widely

    affirmed since

    1938,

    Sargeant's

    hypothesis

    also can

    be under-

    stood

    in terms of both

    pitch

    and

    rhythm.

    If

    so,

    it

    would

    predict

    that har-

    monic

    or

    contrapuntal

    anticipations

    are

    more

    frequent

    than

    suspensions.

    The second dotted

    quarter

    of a 3+3+2

    rhythm might

    anticipate

    a

    change

    of chord on the fifth

    eighth

    from a chord on the first

    eighth

    or

    might

    re-

    solve,

    in

    retardative

    fashion,

    a

    suspended

    dissonance heard

    on the third

    eighth

    that had been

    prepared

    on

    the first.

    In

    anticipations,

    melody

    notes

    can be heard

    as

    leading

    or

    directing

    chord

    changes

    or

    progressions;

    metaphorically,

    d incites or

    provokes

    Y

    (see

    Fig.

    4a).

    In

    suspensions,

    chord

    progressions

    can be

    heard as

    leading

    or

    directing

    changes

    of

    melody

    notes or

    melodic

    progression;

    Y

    incites or

    provokes

    c

    (see

    Fig.

    4b).

    In this

    way,

    Sargeant's

    hypothesis

    would

    predict

    that

    melody

    leads

    chords,

    not vice versa.

    The

    preceding

    analyses

    of 3+3+2 and

    other,

    closely

    related

    syncopated

    rhythms

    presume

    four-square

    meter as a

    framework and

    understand

    3+3+2

    not

    in,

    or

    on,

    its own

    terms but

    rather in

    terms of a

    putatively

    more

    privileged,

    hierarchical

    metric

    structure

    consisting

    of

    stronger

    and weak-

    er beats and

    subdivisions.

    Recent work in

    theory

    and

    analysis

    yields,

    to

    extend

    Gunther

    Schuller's

    suggestive

    idea

    (1968,

    6-10),

    a

    more

    democra-

    tic, or,

    one

    might

    say,

    non-metrocentric,

    construal.

    This

    understanding

    is

    clearly

    connected

    to

    concepts

    of

    metronome

    sense,

    smallest

    rhythmic

    units,

    basic

    pulses,

    nominal

    values,

    and the

    density

    referent in

    ethnomu-

    sicology

    (cf.,

    e.g.,

    Nketia 1974, 125-138). Also related to Leonard B.

    Meyer's

    notion of a

    flat

    hierarchy

    (1973,

    90),

    the

    technical

    elaboration of

    these

    essentially

    non-hierarchical

    conceptions

    has

    not been

    realized with-

    in

    any

    of

    the

    analytic

    traditions

    just

    cited.

    Instead,

    technical

    resources for

    75

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    7/20

    BMR

    Journal

    Figure

    4.

    The 3+3+2

    rhythm

    with

    anticipation

    and

    suspension

    a.

    Anticipation

    b.

    Suspension

    Consonance/dissonance

    -.------

    c-

    c------..

    c

    d--c--

    ----.

    .----

    Melodic

    rhythm

    J.

    J

    .

    J

    .

    J.

    J

    Chords

    x

    Y

    x

    Y

    z

    Chord

    rhythm

    J

    J

    J

    J

    J J

    comprehending

    non-metrocentrism

    have resided at

    the

    edges

    of

    what

    might

    seem the least

    relevant of

    formulations,

    namely,

    the

    large body

    of

    post-tonal

    theory

    developed

    mostly

    to deal

    with

    pitch

    structures in

    twen-

    tieth-century Europe-derived avant-garde music.

    A

    key

    component

    of

    this shift

    of

    paradigm

    for

    rhythmic

    analysis

    is

    Mil-

    ton

    Babbitt's

    theory

    of

    time-points

    (1962).

    Published more than

    three

    decades

    ago,

    Babbitt's

    formulation of

    time-points

    was

    developed

    not

    from

    existing

    theories

    of

    rhythm

    and

    meter-nor even

    from

    traditions de-

    veloped

    in

    common-practice

    theory

    to

    describe

    intervals,

    scales,

    and

    chords

    (which

    remain

    influential

    throughout jazz

    theory

    and

    analysis).

    Instead,

    Babbitt's

    time-points

    emerged

    as an

    extension of

    classic,

    twelve-

    tone

    serialism.

    Babbitt's

    insight

    was that

    time-intervals between

    the

    at-

    tacks or

    beginnings of notes can produce temporal structures parallel to

    those

    formed

    by

    pitch-intervals.

    A

    second,

    heterodox

    stage

    of

    paradigm

    development

    involved

    the em-

    pirical

    finding

    that

    there are

    structural

    parallels

    between

    syncopated per-

    cussion

    ostinatos or

    time-lines,

    in

    several

    African

    traditions,

    and the

    dia-

    tonic

    scale

    (Pressing

    1983;

    Jay

    Rahn

    1983;

    Jay

    Rahn

    1987).

    The

    diatonic

    scale or

    collection is

    labeled

    7-35 in

    Allen

    Forte's standard

    listing

    of

    sub-

    sets

    of the

    12-semitone

    aggregate

    (Forte

    1973,

    Appendix

    I;

    John

    Rahn

    1980,

    140-143).

    The

    collection 7-35

    comprises

    not

    only major

    or

    Ionian-

    024579E

    (cf. CDEFGAB,

    or

    TTSTTTS,

    or in

    semitones, 2212221,

    or with

    eighths

    corresponding

    to

    1

    and

    quarters

    to

    2,

    the

    rhythm

    J J

    h

    J J J

    )-but

    also the

    remaining

    modes

    (Dorian,

    Phrygian,

    etc.)

    and

    their

    rhythmic

    counterparts (e.g.,

    2122212

    or

    J JJ J J

    'J for

    Dorian).

    Such

    diatonic

    time-

    lines have

    long

    been

    well

    documented for

    music in

    such African

    cultures

    76

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    8/20

    Rahn *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    as

    Ewe,

    Ghana

    Jones

    1959, 93, 112,

    121, 138, 170;

    Koetting

    1970, 129;

    Ladzekpo

    and

    Pantaleoni

    1970,

    21;

    Ladzekpo

    1971,

    14;

    Pantaleoni

    1972,

    21, 59a;

    Chemoff

    1979, 85, 86, 119,

    120);

    Ashanti,

    Ghana

    (Koetting

    1970,

    136);

    Yoruba,

    Nigeria (King

    1960, 53;

    King

    1961,

    i-xxxviii;

    Koetting

    1970,

    135);

    and

    Venda,

    South Africa

    (Blacking

    1967,

    151-153; 1970,

    33,

    43, 46, 47,

    50).

    Time-lines of this kind

    appear

    also in

    Mwenda

    Jean

    Bosco's

    guitar

    pieces,

    which stand at the

    beginning

    of the

    important

    modem

    guitar

    tra-

    dition of Zaire

    (Rycroft

    1962,

    100)

    and have

    been transcribed for

    music

    of

    otherwise

    unidentified

    cultures

    in

    Ghana

    (Jones

    1954,

    59;

    Nketia

    1963a,

    89),

    Benin

    (Kolinski

    1967,

    16,

    20),

    and,

    more

    generally,

    West

    Africa

    (Ekwueme

    1975-1976,

    30).

    Understood

    in

    terms

    of

    pitch

    and

    time,

    number-theory

    proofs

    have

    shown how far

    certain

    features of

    this diatonic

    structure

    extend into

    cog-

    nate

    rhythms

    and

    micro-tonal,

    non-12-semitone scales.

    The most

    impor-

    tant of

    these

    theorems

    have

    appeared

    in the

    already

    epochal

    study

    of

    maximally

    even

    sets

    by

    John

    Clough

    and

    Jack

    Douthett

    (1991)

    and

    in

    Clough's

    recent

    treatment

    of diatonic

    interval

    cycles

    (1994).

    Increasingly,

    this

    body

    of

    theoretical lore

    has

    shown close

    structural

    connections with-

    in,

    between,

    and

    among

    the

    7-tone/12-pulse rhythms

    just

    discussed,

    as

    well

    as

    diatonically

    structured

    rhythms comprising

    5

    tones

    among

    12

    pulses,

    5

    or 3

    among

    8,

    and 9 or

    7

    among

    16

    (see

    Fig.

    5).

    These

    rhythms

    underlie

    and

    even

    form

    explicit

    components

    of

    African-

    American

    and

    African-Hispanic

    traditions of

    the Western

    hemisphere.

    As

    time-lines,

    they

    have

    permeated

    traditional

    music of

    West,

    Central,

    Southern,

    and

    East

    Africa

    as well

    as

    pan-African

    idioms of

    recent

    decades. In

    addition

    to the

    traditions

    and

    7-note/12-pulse

    time-lines

    just

    cited,

    music

    using

    time-lines of the

    more

    general

    sort

    illustrated

    in

    Figure

    5

    has been

    transcribed in

    studies of

    the

    following

    cultures:

    Ga,

    Ghana

    (Nketia

    1958,

    22);

    Akan,

    Ghana

    (Nketia

    1963b,

    102,

    106, 110, 114,

    118, 122,

    124, 126,

    128, 130,

    132, 136,

    138);

    Luba,

    Zaire

    (Rycroft

    1962,

    100);

    Ngbaka-

    Maibo,

    Central

    African

    Republic

    (Arom

    1976,

    509);

    Hausa,

    Nigeria

    (Raab

    1970, 100;

    Besmer

    1974,

    58);

    Tonga,

    Zimbabwe

    (Jones

    1964,

    11-12);

    and

    Shananga-Tsonga,

    Mozambique

    (Johnston

    1971,

    69).

    Theorems

    concern-

    ing

    this

    more

    general

    sort of

    diatonic structure

    draw

    attention

    to several

    affinities

    among

    these

    traditional

    time-lines and

    show

    how

    they

    are both

    distinct

    from and

    yet

    highly

    compatible

    with

    other,

    four-square,

    hierar-

    chical

    patterns.

    Some

    mathematical

    results

    bearing

    on

    these

    rhythms

    are

    quite

    prob-

    lematic,

    for

    they

    resist concrete

    interpretation.

    For

    instance,

    arresting

    for-

    mulations

    by

    John

    Clough

    and

    Gerald

    Myerson

    (1985)

    prove

    that in dia-

    tonic

    structures of

    the sort

    just

    illustrated,

    for

    each

    kind of two-note

    set

    (i.e.,

    each

    kind of

    interval or

    dyad,

    namely,

    second,

    third,

    fourth,

    fifth,

    77

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    9/20

    BMR

    Journal

    Figure

    5.

    Diatonic

    rhythmsfor

    which

    theoremsand

    proofs

    appear

    n

    Clough

    and

    Douthett

    (1991)

    and

    Clough

    (1994)

    7

    tones/12

    pulses

    JJDJJJJ

    JbJJJJJ

    DJJJdJJ

    JJJDJJJ

    JJDJJDJ

    5

    tones/12

    pulses

    JJJ.JJ. JJ.JJ.J .JJ.JJ JJ.JJ. .JJJ.J

    5

    tones/8

    pulses

    .Jf.J.t.J

    W.

    JJ

    J

    JJ.J

    JJ.J.

    3

    tones/8

    pulses

    J..J

    J.JJ.

    JJ.J.

    9

    tones/16

    pulses

    JJJJ.JJJJ

    JJJ.JJJJJ

    JJ~JJJJJJ

    J.JJJJJJJ

    .JJJ

    JJJJ

    JJJbJJJJ^

    JJDJJJJfJ

    JJJJJJJ

    JJJJJJJ

    7

    tones/16

    pulses

    JJJJ.JJJ. J.JJJ.J JJ.JJJ.JJ J.JJJ.J JJJ.JJJJ.

    JJ.JJJJ.J

    J.IJJJ

    J

    etc.)

    there are

    two

    types.

    For

    example,

    the

    types

    are

    minor

    and

    major

    for

    seconds,

    thirds, sixths,

    and

    sevenths;

    perfect

    and

    augmented

    for

    fourths;

    perfect

    and

    diminished for fifths.

    Similarly,

    for

    each kind

    of

    trichord

    or

    three-note set in a

    diatonic

    structure

    (whether

    of

    pitch

    or

    time),

    the

    num-

    ber of

    types

    is

    three;

    for

    example,

    the

    types

    of

    triads

    in a

    diatonic

    scale

    are

    diminished,

    minor,

    and

    major.

    The

    types

    of

    trichords

    that

    comprise

    con-

    secutive

    steps

    or scale

    degrees

    are

    similarly

    threefold: from bottom to

    top,

    major-second-plus-major-second

    (e.g.,

    C-D-E,

    F-G-A,

    and

    G-A-B),

    major-

    second-plus-minor-second

    (e.g.,

    D-E-F

    and

    A-B-C),

    and

    minor-second-

    plus-major-second

    (e.g.,

    E-F-G

    and

    B-C-D);

    and

    so

    forth,

    for all

    the

    possi-

    78

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    10/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    bilities.

    Translated

    into

    time,

    this

    highly

    abstract

    set of features

    holds

    for

    all

    the

    8-, 12-,

    and

    16-pulse rhythms

    listed above. What

    such

    precise

    nu-

    merical relations

    among

    types

    or kinds of

    things

    actually might

    sound

    like has not been

    as clear as the

    proofs

    themselves. Other

    aspects

    of

    these

    rhythms,

    formulated

    just

    as

    mathematically,

    seem more

    directly

    connect-

    ed

    to both

    perception

    and

    performance.

    These features can

    be

    translated

    into concrete

    terms

    that

    involve

    neither

    abstract kinds or

    types

    of

    things,

    nor

    precise

    numbers

    (see

    Jay

    Rahn 1995

    on the

    general

    translatability

    of

    abstractions into

    concrete

    things

    for

    music).

    Concepts

    of

    evenness,

    coherence,

    and

    proportionacy

    developed

    in

    these

    formulations

    arguably

    correspond

    to the

    smoothness shared

    by

    the

    time-lines listed

    above.

    These

    concepts

    also

    define

    analogies

    between the

    time-lines and

    much

    plainer

    successions

    of,

    for

    example,

    even

    quarters

    or

    dotted

    quarters,

    or

    quarter-eighth

    pairs.

    The

    concept

    of

    maximum

    match-

    ing

    among

    time-intervals

    helps

    define a

    way

    in

    which

    these

    rhythms

    can

    be

    heard

    as

    integral,

    unified

    wholes or

    temporal

    Gestalten and

    distin-

    guishes

    them from

    maximally

    redundant

    (and

    maximally

    even)

    succes-

    sions

    of

    indefinitely repeated

    eighths,

    or

    quarters,

    or

    dotted

    quarters,

    etc.,

    and from such

    minimally

    redundant

    (but

    nonetheless

    maximally

    even)

    successions as

    eighth-quarter

    and

    quarter-half

    pairs.

    Among

    maximally

    even

    rhythms,

    another

    concept,

    namely

    individua-

    tion

    (closely

    analogous

    to

    the

    notion of

    affinities

    in

    medieval

    modal the-

    ory), expresses

    a

    further

    contrast

    between

    diatonic

    rhythms

    and

    those

    in

    which

    the

    most or

    fewest

    time-intervals

    match

    each other in

    size.

    Diaton-

    ic

    rhythms

    are

    maximally

    individuated in the

    sense that

    each

    of their

    notes

    bears

    a

    unique

    constellation

    of

    relations to

    every

    other note.

    For in-

    stance,

    in

    the

    5-tone/8-eighth-pulse

    diatonic

    rhythm

    J

    hJ

    J the

    third

    note

    appears

    1

    eighth

    and

    3

    eighths

    after but

    2

    and 3

    eighths

    before

    other

    notes

    in

    the

    rhythm,

    whereas

    the fifth

    note

    appears

    1

    and 3

    eighths

    after

    and 2

    and 4

    eighths

    before other notes.

    Irrespective

    of

    how

    often the

    rhythm

    might

    be

    repeated,

    this

    individuation is

    sustained

    for

    all

    five

    notes

    in

    the

    pattern,

    which

    thus

    constantly

    renews

    itself

    through

    note-to-note

    diver-

    sity. By

    contrast,

    every

    note

    in

    a

    (nonetheless

    maximally

    even)

    succession

    of

    straight

    eighths

    is like

    every

    other

    note,

    as is

    every

    other

    note

    in an

    in-

    definitely

    repeated

    (and

    again,

    maximally

    even)

    rhythm

    of

    quarter-

    eighth pairs.

    A

    rhythm

    need

    not

    be

    even,

    coherent,

    proportionate,

    etc. in

    order to

    be

    maximally

    individuated.

    Any rhythm

    that

    cannot

    be

    fully

    subdivided or

    partitioned into

    repeated

    segments

    will, on

    subsequent

    repetition,

    be

    maximally

    individuated in

    this

    technical

    sense;

    for

    example,

    J

    J

    D.J

    s

    con-

    trasted

    with,

    for

    instance,

    J .bJ

    J,

    which

    subdivides into

    two

    statements

    of J .b.

    Whereas

    particular

    notes

    within

    a

    repeated

    four-square rhythm

    79

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    11/20

    BMR

    Journal

    stand out

    as the clearest candidates

    for

    being

    heard as

    strongest,

    second

    strongest,

    etc.

    beats or

    parts

    of

    beats,3

    the

    diatonic

    rhythms

    considered

    here elude such linear

    ordering.

    Instead,

    they

    can be

    understood as

    com-

    prising

    cycles

    of

    (approximate)

    half-measures

    analogous

    to

    half-octave

    cycles

    that have

    been discerned for

    scales

    (see

    Jay

    Rahn

    1977).

    Within

    such

    cycling processes,

    these

    rhythms

    can be

    understood

    as

    twisting

    or

    tunneling

    through

    time,

    forming

    a

    special,

    braided

    structure.

    As

    recently

    formulated

    by Clough

    (1994),

    the

    braided

    structures char-

    acteristic of

    diatonic

    rhythms

    can

    be

    contrasted

    clearly

    with

    four-square

    rhythms.

    For

    example,

    in

    the 7-tone

    rhythms

    discussed

    above,

    every

    tone

    is

    immediately

    inside a

    pair

    of

    tones, i.e.,

    a

    single step away

    from

    each.

    Each tone of

    this

    pair

    is the same

    number

    (i.e., two)

    of

    steps away

    from

    the other and

    yet

    another. In

    turn,

    each

    of these is four

    steps

    from

    the

    other and another

    still;

    and so on.

    The entire

    pattern

    comes full

    circle. Be-

    cause of the

    clumsiness of

    Europe-derived

    rhythmic

    notation,

    this

    point

    Figure

    6.

    Braided,

    circular

    structures in

    7-tone/12-pulse

    rhythm

    C

    D

    E

    F G

    A

    B

    C

    D

    E F

    G

    A

    B C

    D

    E F

    G A

    B...

    J

    J

    D J J

    J

    D

    J

    J

    J

    J

    J

    J

    J J

    JJ

    4

    ,...

    a.

    Each

    C is one

    step

    earlier than

    a

    D;

    each

    D

    is

    one

    step

    earlier

    than an

    E

    Each

    C is two

    steps

    earlier than

    an

    E;

    each

    E

    is

    two

    steps

    earlier

    than a

    G

    Each

    C

    is

    four

    steps

    earlier

    than a

    G;

    each

    G is four

    steps

    earlier than

    a

    D

    Each C is eight

    steps

    earlier than a D; each D is

    eight

    steps

    earlier than

    an E

    (cf.

    first line in

    (a):

    "Each C

    is one

    step

    earlier

    ...")

    b.

    C's are one

    step

    later than

    B's;

    B's are

    one

    step

    later

    than

    A's

    C's are

    two

    steps

    later than

    A's;

    A's are

    two

    steps

    later

    than

    F's

    C's

    are four

    steps

    later

    than

    F's;

    F's are

    four

    steps

    later than

    B's

    C's are

    eight

    steps

    later

    (i.e.,

    one

    step

    later)

    than

    B's;

    B's are

    eight

    steps

    later than A's; i.e., C's are one step later than B's, and B's are one step

    later

    than

    A's

    (cf.

    first

    line

    in

    (b):

    "C's are

    one

    step

    later

    ...")

    3. For

    example,

    in

    Jay

    Rahn

    (1978).

    80

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    12/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    is more

    easily

    illustrated

    by

    pitch

    letters,

    as

    in

    Figure

    6a. In a

    fully

    circu-

    lar

    approach

    to

    time,

    one

    step

    earlier has the same

    sense as six

    steps

    later;

    two

    steps

    earlier,

    the same

    sense as five

    later;

    . . .

    and

    eight steps

    earlier

    has the

    same

    sense as one

    step

    earlier or six

    steps

    later.

    Reversing

    direc-

    tions

    yields Figure

    6b. Each tone

    (e.g.,

    C)

    is related to

    each other tone

    (e.g.

    D,

    E,

    G,

    and

    B,

    A,

    F)

    in

    a

    distinct

    way.

    Each

    tone shares with all other

    tones all the distinct

    ways.

    Just

    as the

    center of

    a

    circle is not on

    the circle

    itself,

    each tone

    stands outside the

    tones with

    which it is related in

    this

    manner. In

    Figure

    6,

    C is

    distinct from

    D,

    E, G,

    and

    B, A,

    F;

    D

    is distinct

    from

    E, F,

    A,

    and

    C, B, G; E,

    from

    F, G, B,

    and

    D,

    C, A;

    and

    so

    forth,

    for all

    tones

    in

    the

    rhythm.

    Proofs

    in

    number

    theory

    show that such

    completely

    braided,

    truly

    cir-

    cular

    structures can arise

    only

    in

    3-, 5-, 7-, 9-,

    etc.

    tone

    successions like

    those listed

    in

    Figure

    5.

    By

    contrast,

    in

    the 4-tone

    pattern

    of

    Figure

    7a,

    no

    circularity

    arises.

    Each such

    4-step process

    converges

    on a

    single

    beat.

    This is

    arguably

    a model of

    four-square

    time,

    which

    seems not to

    renew

    itself but instead

    quickly

    reaches a

    clear limit where it

    remains.

    Indeed,

    if

    one

    step

    later

    corresponds

    to three

    steps

    earlier,

    this model

    comprises

    a

    technical basis for the

    Europe-derived

    norm

    of 4-measure

    phrasing.

    In

    re-

    verse,

    this

    patterning corresponds

    to

    Figure

    7b.4

    The last technical

    points

    to be considered here involve

    variability

    and

    complementarity.

    The

    eighths

    that

    are not

    sounded

    in

    a

    diatonic

    7-

    note/12-eighth rhythm

    (stems

    upward)

    form a

    5-note/12-eighth rhythm

    (stems

    downward)

    which is

    also

    diatonic

    (in

    the

    special

    sense

    employed

    here):5

    Each

    diatonic

    rhythm

    listed

    above

    has

    a

    unique complementary

    partner

    which

    is itself also a

    diatonic

    rhythm.

    Not

    only

    is

    this

    relationship

    be-

    tween paired diatonic rhythms mutual or reciprocal, but such pairs over-

    lap

    maximally. By way

    of a

    single

    illustration,

    if

    the

    stems-downward,

    5-

    note/12-eighth rhythm just

    presented

    begins

    an

    eighth

    earlier,

    each

    of its

    notes

    coincides

    with

    its

    stems-upward,

    7-note/12-eighth

    partner

    (see

    Fig.

    8).

    Another

    possibility

    for

    tacit motor mediation

    of

    sounding

    notes is for

    the

    unheard

    portion

    of

    the

    gesture

    to

    peak

    at the

    temporal

    mid-point

    be-

    tween

    sounds,

    so that the

    major-mode

    time-line

    4.

    If 0 is

    the

    first,

    3 the

    fourth,

    6

    the

    seventh,

    and 12 the

    thirteenth

    measure,

    this

    process

    could be

    considered to model standard

    12-bar

    blues, highlighting

    its

    cyclical, pendular,

    tonic-permeated,

    harmonic

    structures,

    in

    contrast with the

    teleological

    delay

    of tonic

    reso-

    lution

    (via

    counterpoint)

    in

    Europe-derived

    Schenkerian

    analysis.

    5. The

    orderly

    notations

    of

    7-note time-lines

    by

    Kolinski

    in

    Blesh

    (1958,

    Ex.

    42;

    cf.

    34,

    384)

    suggest

    his

    awareness of their

    connection with diatonic modes and

    complementary

    rela-

    tionship

    with anhemitonic

    pentatonic

    rhythms.

    81

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    13/20

    BMR

    Journal

    JJ)fJJJ

    would produce the following as its shadow.6

    rP' '

    r

    P' '

    Although complementarity

    and

    overlap might

    seem

    highly

    abstract as-

    pects

    of relations

    between

    rhythms,

    read

    into them after the

    fact,

    as it

    were,

    I believe

    they

    can be understood

    quite concretely

    in

    connection

    with Erich von Hornbostel's reflections on African

    drumming:

    Figure

    7.

    Convergent

    4-tone

    cycle

    a.

    J J

    J J

    J

    J J J J...

    w x

    Y

    Z

    W

    X

    Y

    Z W...

    W's

    are one

    step

    earlier than

    X's;

    X's are

    one

    step

    earlier than

    Y's

    W's are two steps earlier than Y's; Y's are two steps earlier than W's

    W's are four

    steps

    earlier than

    W's,

    and

    (trivially)

    W's are four

    steps

    earlier

    than

    W's

    W's are

    eight steps

    earlier than

    W's,

    and

    (trivially, again)

    W's are

    eight

    steps

    earlier than

    W's

    b.

    Reading

    underlined letters from

    right

    to

    left, and,

    for the

    parenthetical

    statements, italicized letters from left to right

    J J J J

    J

    J J J J

    J

    J J J

    w

    X Y Z

    W

    X

    Y

    Z W X

    Y Z

    W

    W

    is one

    step

    later

    than Z

    (cf.

    W

    is three

    steps

    earlier than

    Z)

    and Z

    is

    one

    step

    later than

    Y

    (cf.

    Z

    is

    three

    steps

    earlier than

    Y)

    W

    is

    two

    steps

    later than Y

    (cf. W is six steps earlier than Y) and Y is

    two

    steps

    later than W

    (cf.

    Y

    is

    six

    steps

    earlier than

    W)

    W

    is four

    steps

    later than

    W

    (cf.

    W

    is twelve

    steps

    earlier than

    W)

    6.

    Compare

    the

    structure of

    rast

    in

    North

    African

    maqamat.

    82

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    14/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    Figure

    8.

    Overlap

    of complementary

    diatonic

    rhythms

    7-tone/12-pulse

    J

    J

    J J J

    5-tone/12-pulse

    r

    r r

    r

    r

    African

    rhythm

    s

    ultimately

    founded

    on

    drumming.Drumming

    can be

    re-

    placed

    by

    hand-clapping

    or

    by

    the

    xylophone;

    what

    really

    matters

    s

    the

    act

    of

    beating;

    and

    only

    from this

    point

    can African

    rhythms

    be

    understood.

    Each

    singlebeating

    movements...

    two-fold:

    he

    muscles re

    trained

    nd

    released,

    the hand

    s

    lifted

    and

    dropped.Only

    the second

    phase

    is stressed

    acoustically;

    but

    the

    first,

    inaudible one

    has the motor

    accent,

    as it

    were,

    which

    consists

    in

    the

    straining

    of

    the muscles. This

    implies

    an essential contrast between

    our

    rhythmic

    conception

    and the

    Africans';

    we

    proceed

    from

    hearing, they

    from

    motion;

    we

    separate

    the two

    phases by

    a

    bar-line

    and commence the

    metrical

    unity,

    the

    bar,

    with the

    acoustically

    tressed

    time-unit;

    o

    them,

    the

    beginning

    of

    the

    movement,

    the

    arsis,

    is

    at the same time the

    beginning

    of

    the

    rhythmical

    figure;

    up-beats

    are

    unknown

    to them.

    (Hornbostel

    1928,

    52-53;

    emphasis

    added)

    Despite

    the

    presumptuous,

    arguably

    essentialist,

    us/them

    contrast

    of

    Hombostel's

    account,

    a

    number of

    his

    points

    remain of

    value. For African

    traditions,

    John

    Blacking

    (1955a;

    1955b;

    1961)

    stressed-as has

    John

    Baily

    (1985;

    1992)

    for

    other,

    non-Africa-derived

    music-that not

    only

    drum-

    ming,

    hand-clapping,

    and

    mallet

    performance,

    but

    performing

    in

    gener-

    al

    can

    be

    understood as

    highly

    patterned

    motor

    activity.

    To

    extend Horn-

    bostel's

    approach,

    the

    complement

    of the

    3-note/8-eighth rhythm

    heard

    as J. J. J

    need

    not

    be

    understood as

    mere

    sounds

    nor

    as a mere

    abstraction

    from

    sounds.

    Rather,

    such a

    rhythm

    can

    be

    considered to

    comprise

    a

    fully

    concrete

    motor

    rhythm

    that

    results not

    just

    from

    motions

    corresponding

    to

    the

    sounds

    actually

    heard

    but

    also from

    motions,

    unheard

    but felt

    by

    performers,

    which

    occur

    between

    these

    sounds and which

    complete

    or

    complement

    the

    rhythm

    actually

    heard

    (see

    Fig.

    9).

    In

    this

    manner,

    motor,

    and

    plausibly

    social,

    aspects

    of

    such

    rhythms

    can be

    drawn into

    other-

    wise

    purely

    sonic

    analysis.7

    Incorporating

    something

    like

    the

    Effort values used in

    Labanotation for

    dance

    (Bartenieff

    and

    Lewis

    1980),

    one

    might gain

    a

    more

    comprehensive

    understanding

    of

    such

    a

    rhythmic

    practice.

    In this

    regard,

    Hombostel's

    dichotomy

    between strained-stressed-lifted and

    released-inaudible-

    dropped portions

    of

    a

    movement

    cycle

    might

    not

    be

    fully

    appropriate.

    7.

    Conversely,

    Baily's

    motor

    grammar

    of

    instrumental

    performances

    (1985;

    1992)

    omits

    a

    detailed account

    of

    time-interval

    relations.

    83

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    15/20

    BMR

    Journal

    Figure

    9.

    Complementary

    diatonicmotor

    rhythms

    Totalrhythm

    b

    f f

    Sounding portion

    J

    J

    Silent

    portion

    r

    r

    r

    English

    was not his first

    language,

    and

    for all we

    know he

    never

    under-

    took traditional African

    drumming.8

    And the

    maximum felt

    contrast

    of

    the

    unheard,

    medial

    portions

    of

    movement with

    heard

    parts

    that

    result

    in

    sounds

    might

    not

    always

    correspond precisely

    to the

    attacks of the

    stems-

    down

    eighths

    in the

    above

    notations.

    Nonetheless,

    a

    technical

    account of

    the remarkable

    time-interval

    structures involved

    merely

    in

    the

    sounding

    portions

    of these

    rhythms

    provides

    a

    framework within

    which

    one

    might

    enhance

    sensitivity

    to

    the

    larger

    motor

    processes

    within

    which

    they

    occur. In

    fact,

    though

    often

    responded

    to as

    if

    they

    were

    legislative

    and

    unquestionable,

    mathematical

    forms of

    analysis,

    including

    the

    framing

    of

    proofs,

    can be

    understood as

    discovery

    procedures-ways

    of

    imagina-

    tively exploring regions

    where

    common

    sense,

    which in

    music

    theory

    often seems

    to have consisted of

    insufficiently analyzed

    Europe-derived

    concepts,

    has failed to secure

    satisfactory

    models or

    representations

    of ex-

    perience.

    Despite

    the enhanced

    understanding

    that

    results from

    taking

    motor

    ac-

    tivity

    into

    account,

    analysts

    are

    unlikely

    to

    adopt

    a

    view of

    music

    exactly

    parallel

    to the

    following

    iconoclastic

    perspective

    on

    speech

    advocated

    by

    pioneer

    motor

    phonologist

    R.

    H.

    Stetson:

    "Speech

    is rather a

    set of

    move-

    ments

    made

    audible

    than a

    set of

    sounds

    produced

    by

    movements"

    (quoted

    in

    Kelso and Munhall

    1988,

    58;

    see

    also Kubik

    1979,

    228 for a

    ten-

    tative

    musical

    rendering

    of

    this

    doctrine).

    An

    important

    difference is

    that

    all

    spoken

    languages,

    as

    such,

    are

    founded on

    structures that

    are

    signifi-

    cantly

    articulative

    in

    ways

    not

    shared

    by

    such

    arguably

    musical

    activities

    as one

    encounters in the

    production

    of

    chance and

    electronic

    forms.

    Nonetheless,

    there is

    sufficient

    basis to

    understand

    the

    notes of

    music as

    symbols,

    not

    merely

    of

    sounds heard in

    certain

    ways

    but

    also of

    sounds

    8. In

    seeming

    contrast to

    Hornbostel's

    account of

    drumming,

    Blacking

    (1955b,

    51-52)

    as-

    sociates a

    feeling

    of release with the

    portion

    of a hand

    movement in which a

    flute

    player's

    hand

    is

    away

    from,

    rather than

    on,

    the instrument.

    84

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    16/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    produced

    in certain

    ways;

    and to hear such sounds with a motor

    imagery

    that

    can,

    in

    principle,

    be shared

    with

    others

    (see,

    e.g.,

    Kubik

    1972,

    29).

    A

    plausible

    case

    in

    point

    is

    off-beat

    clapping,

    which has

    accompanied

    the

    spread

    of

    African

    musical

    practices

    throughout

    the world.

    Often

    in

    Africa-derived

    music,

    constantly

    syncopated playing

    and/or

    singing

    is

    accompanied

    solely by

    off-beat

    clapping.

    Problematic for

    a

    merely

    sonic

    analysis

    of

    music,9

    this effect is

    thoroughly

    straightforward

    if

    one

    accepts

    the axiom

    that

    every

    act of

    performance implies

    a

    counter-act;

    every

    clap

    of

    hands

    that

    is

    heard

    implies

    a

    pair

    of hands

    unclapped

    and

    unheard;

    every key pressed,

    a

    key

    released;

    etc.

    Such

    alternations

    are

    cyclic

    and

    highly compatible

    with diatonic

    braiding.

    Diatonic

    braiding

    involves

    processes

    comprising

    powers

    of

    two,

    which

    sequence

    as 2n

    =

    1, 2, 4,

    8,

    16,

    ...,

    and moduli

    that

    comprise multiples

    of 4

    units,

    e.g.,

    4, 8, 12,

    16

    ...

    (see

    discussion

    above).

    Off-beat

    clapping

    is

    pendular,

    well

    modeled

    by

    the

    sequence

    -ln

    =

    -1,

    +1, -1, +1,

    ...

    Each

    of

    its

    cycles

    has two

    parts,

    each

    the

    opposite

    of

    the

    other,

    neither

    being necessarily

    first nor second.

    Capable

    of

    forming

    epi-

    cycles

    within

    larger

    cycles

    (like

    dancers

    turning

    alternately

    to left

    and

    right,

    within

    a

    larger,

    counter-clockwise

    circling),

    off-

    (or,

    for

    that

    matter,

    on-)

    beat

    clapping

    provides

    orientation

    enough

    to

    specify

    that the

    beat

    has been turned

    around,

    helically

    or

    toroidally,

    as

    it

    were,

    within each

    cycle

    of

    such

    an

    odd-numbered

    figure

    (i.e.,

    of

    3,

    5,

    7,

    etc.

    notes),

    as

    in

    Fig-

    ure

    10,

    which

    constantly

    renews

    itself

    by

    assimilating

    cycles

    that

    cross-

    cut,

    intensify,

    subdivide,

    or

    multiply

    each other.

    Figure

    10.

    Cross-cutting

    of

    diatonic

    7-note time-line

    by

    pendular

    clapping

    7-note

    ime-line

    r

    r r

    rJ

    Pendularclapping

    r

    J

    r

    r

    r

    r

    Although

    much

    hearing-as

    can

    be

    shared,

    much

    can be

    divergent.

    The

    diatonic

    rhythms

    discussed here can

    be varied

    considerably

    without los-

    ing

    their

    relevance. As

    Figure

    11

    illustrates,

    diatonic

    rhythms

    can sustain

    extra

    or

    omitted notes

    without

    losing

    the

    characteristics of

    evenness,

    etc.

    outlined

    above,

    if

    the variant

    forms

    are

    heard

    as versions of

    a

    diatonic

    rhythm

    rather than

    independently

    as

    rhythms

    in their own

    right

    (see

    Jay

    Rahn

    1991,

    44-49).

    Such

    variants

    of

    diatonic

    rhythms

    can also

    be heard in

    a

    four-square

    manner and in

    many

    instances reward

    substantially

    such

    9.

    For

    example,

    Arom's elaborate

    theory

    founders on this

    point

    (1991, 202-205).

    85

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    17/20

    BMR

    Journal

    an

    approach.

    For

    example, adding

    a note at the fourth

    eighth

    of the

    basic

    5-pulse/8-eighth rhythm

    of

    Figure

    3 results in

    J

    J

    .

    f

    J which

    can

    be

    heard not

    only

    as

    a

    "chromatic" version of

    the diatonic

    rhythm,

    but

    also

    as an

    unproblematic

    realization

    of

    four-square

    t

    meter.

    Figure

    11. Diatonic

    5-note/8-pulse

    rhythm

    and variants

    involving

    extra

    and

    omitted notes

    Diatonic

    5-note/8-pulse

    rhythm

    J

    J

    6-note variant JJ b . b

    4-note variant

    J

    b

    J

    J

    In

    fact,

    each

    group

    of diatonic

    rhythms

    includes at

    least

    two

    (mutually

    retrograde) patterns

    which,

    even

    without

    being

    varied,

    involve

    little

    or

    no

    syncopation

    relative to a

    particular

    four-square

    Europe-derived

    framework.

    8-eighths:

    cf.

    J:

    b

    J

    J

    and

    J

    J.J

    12-eighths:

    cf.

    4 +4

    (or

    ?):

    J J

    and

    JJ

    JJJ

    16-eighths:

    cf.

    +

    :

    J J

    J

    JJJ

    and

    JJJ

    JJJJ

    Europe-derived analysis,

    even in

    ethnomusicology,

    has

    tended

    to

    insist

    that

    only

    one

    interpretation

    is

    possible

    on

    any

    particular

    occasion.

    For in-

    stance,

    Mieczyslaw

    Kolinski

    (1973,

    502)

    asserted

    that

    "one

    is

    absolutely

    unable to

    perceive

    [a

    particular

    African

    piece]

    at the same time in

    g

    or i."

    Nevertheless,

    within

    the

    doctrine

    of

    Gestalt

    psychology,

    which

    analysts

    like

    Kolinski

    have taken

    as a

    basis

    for such

    skepticism,

    one

    finds

    Wolf-

    gang

    Kohler

    saying,

    in

    connection with

    an

    ambiguous,

    "Rubin's"

    draw-

    ing

    (i.e.,

    of

    the

    duck-rabbit

    variety,

    which

    provides

    a

    visual

    parallel

    to

    the

    question

    of

    hearing

    or

    feeling

    a

    particular

    passage

    in

    I

    and/or

    in

    i-or

    for

    that

    matter,

    according

    to

    a

    four-square

    and/or

    diatonic

    framework):

    "Under

    certain

    unusual

    conditions both

    objects

    may

    be

    seen

    at the

    same

    time"

    (Kohler

    1947,

    183).10

    Just as

    there is

    no

    contradiction in

    seeing,

    on

    10.

    Blesh

    (1958,

    39-40)

    interprets

    psychologically

    what

    he calls

    "time-shifts"

    that

    result

    from

    "off-beat

    ostinatos"

    (cf.

    off-beat

    clapping,

    above)

    as

    follows:

    "the

    placing

    of

    the mea-

    sure

    division

    tends to

    shift,

    the

    counting

    is

    advanced

    and

    the

    off-beat

    becomes

    the

    princi-

    pal

    beat,"

    noting

    further

    the

    similarity

    of

    this

    phenomenon

    to

    "various

    types

    of

    optical

    il-

    lusion

    and

    the

    seeing

    of

    complementary

    hues

    during

    color

    fatigue."

    86

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    18/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    any

    particular

    occasion,

    a

    given drawing

    both

    as

    a

    drawing

    of

    a

    duck and

    as

    a

    drawing

    of a

    rabbit,

    there is no contradiction in

    hearing

    or

    feeling

    a

    passage

    as

    simultaneously

    both

    in I

    and

    1,

    nor

    as,

    at

    the

    same

    time,

    both

    four-square

    and diatonic in

    rhythm.

    It

    would

    be

    contradictory

    to claim

    to

    have

    perceived

    a

    passage,

    at a

    particular

    time,

    both as

    four-square

    and

    not

    as

    four-square.

    Generally,

    however,

    such

    baldly contradictory

    claims

    do not arise

    in

    analysis, although

    other,

    non-contradictory

    claims

    (e.g.,

    of

    the

    duck-and-rabbit

    sort)

    are

    cited to

    justify singular, convergent

    under-

    standing.

    If

    diatonic

    rhythms

    can be

    experienced divergently,

    even within

    a

    sin-

    gle experience,

    how

    can

    one enhance skill

    in

    their

    production

    and

    per-

    ception?

    Although

    the

    topic

    is

    vast,

    a few

    (admittedly

    anecdotal)

    obser-

    vations seem called for.

    Much of

    the

    material

    developed

    for

    curricula in

    rhythm reading

    and dictation

    appears

    to

    presume

    that

    unsyncopated

    rhythms

    are

    substantially

    easier

    than

    syncopated rhythms.

    To be

    sure,

    syncopated rhythms appear

    more

    complicated

    on

    the

    page, especially

    if

    conveyed

    with a multitude of ties.

    However,

    in

    my

    own

    experience,

    stu-

    dents who read and take down both diatonic and

    unsyncopated rhythms

    from the

    outset encounter less

    difficulty

    later on.

    Whereas a retardative

    approach

    to

    syncopation

    encourages

    a

    sudden

    grasping

    at a note

    just

    after the beat it follows and an

    anticipative

    strate-

    gy

    favors

    staying

    a

    beat ahead of the

    music

    (e.g.,

    in

    one's

    imagination),

    diatonic

    rhythms

    reward

    sustaining relatively

    large

    wholes

    (e.g.,

    of

    8,

    12,

    or 16

    units)

    that

    could

    continue,

    with or

    without

    variation,

    indefinitely

    far in the

    future. As in so

    many

    other

    regions

    of

    pedagogy,

    here it seems

    best

    to

    begin

    not at the

    beginning

    (i.e.,

    atomistically,

    in

    bottom-up

    fash-

    ion,

    from

    beat

    and

    subdivision

    to

    measure,

    phrase,

    and

    piece)

    nor at

    the

    end

    (i.e.,

    from

    piece

    to

    part,

    in

    the

    top-down

    manner of

    obscurantist

    cog-

    nitive

    theories)

    but

    rather

    in

    the

    middle

    (i.e.,

    inside-out,

    from

    substantial,

    significant

    units toward both the

    parts

    that

    constitute

    them

    and

    the

    larg-

    er

    wholes

    they

    form

    with one

    another).

    REFERENCES

    Arom,

    Simha. 1976.

    The use of

    play-back

    techniques

    in

    the

    study

    of oral

    polyphonies.

    Eth-

    nomusicology

    20,

    no.

    3:483-519.

    .

    1991.

    African polyphony

    and

    polyrhythm:

    Musical

    structure and

    methodology.

    Cam-

    bridgeshire,

    U.K.:

    Cambridge University

    Press.

    Babbitt,

    Milton.

    1962.

    Twelve-tone

    rhythmic

    structure and

    the electronic

    medium.

    Perspec-

    tives

    of

    New

    Music 1:49-79.

    Baily,

    John.

    1985. Music

    structure and

    human

    movement.

    In

    Musical structure and

    human

    cog-

    87

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    19/20

    BMR

    Journal

    nition,

    edited

    by

    Peter

    Howell,

    Ian

    Cross,

    and Robert

    West,

    237-258.London:

    Academic

    Press.

    .1992. Musicperformance,motorstructure, nd cognitivemodels.In Europeantud-

    ies in

    ethnomusicology:

    istorical

    evelopments

    ndrecent

    rends,

    dited

    by

    MaxPeter

    Bau-

    mann,

    Artur

    Simon,

    and Ulrich

    Wegener,

    142-158.

    Wilhelmshaven,

    Germany:

    Florian

    Noetzel

    Verlag.

    Bakhtin,

    Mikhail.1990.

    Author and hero in

    aesthetic

    activity.

    n Art and

    answerability:arly

    philosophical

    ssays

    by

    M. M.

    Bakhtin,

    dited

    by

    Michael

    Holquist

    and Vadim

    Liapunov,

    4-256. Austin:

    University

    of

    TexasPress.

    Ballanta-Taylor,

    icholas

    J.

    G.

    1922.

    Jazz

    music and its

    relation

    to

    African

    music. Musical

    Courier

    4,

    no.

    22

    (June

    1):7.

    Bartenieff,

    rmgard,

    nd

    Dori

    Lewis. 1980.

    Body

    movement:

    oping

    withthe

    environment. ew

    York:

    Gordon

    and

    Breach

    SciencePub.

    Besmer,Fremont.1974. KidanDaranSalla.Bloomington:AfricanStudiesProgram, ndiana

    University.

    Blacking,

    ohn.

    1955a.Some notes

    on

    a

    theory

    of

    rhythm

    advanced

    by

    Erichvon

    Hornbostel.

    African

    Music

    1,

    no. 2:12-20.

    .

    1955b.

    Eight

    flute tunes

    from

    Butembo,

    East

    Belgian

    Congo. African

    Music

    1,

    no.

    2:24-52.

    1961.

    Patternsof

    Nsenga

    kalimba

    music.

    African

    Music

    2,

    no.

    4:26-43.

    1967.

    Venda

    hildren's

    ongs.

    Johannesburg:

    Witwatersrand

    University

    Press.

    . 1970.

    Tonal

    organization

    n

    the

    music

    of

    two

    Venda nitiation

    chools.

    Ethnomusicol-

    ogy

    14,

    no. 1:1-56.

    Blesh,

    Rudi.

    1958.

    Shining rumpets:

    history

    fjazz.

    2nd

    ed. New York:

    AlfredA.

    Knopf.

    Bornemann,

    Ernst.1946.A

    critic ooks t

    jazz.

    London:

    Jazz

    Music Books.

    Chernoff,JohnMiller. 1979.African

    hythm

    ndAfrican

    ensibility.

    Chicago:

    University

    of

    Chicago

    Press.

    Clough,

    John.

    1994.

    Diatonic interval

    cycles

    and

    hierarchical

    tructure.

    Perspectives

    f

    New

    Music

    32,

    no.

    1:228-253.

    Clough,

    John,

    and

    Jack

    Douthett. 1991.

    Maximally

    even sets.

    Journal

    of

    Music

    Theory

    35:93-173.

    Clough,

    John,

    and Gerald

    Myerson.

    1985.

    Variety

    and

    multiplicity

    n

    diatonic

    systems.

    Jour-

    nal

    of

    Music

    Theory

    9:249-270.

    Copland,

    Aaron. 1927.

    Jazz

    structureand

    influence.

    ModernMusic

    4,

    no. 2:9-14.

    Cuney-Hare,

    Maud.

    1936.

    Negro

    musicians nd

    their

    music.

    Washington:

    Associated

    Publish-

    ers.

    Ekwueme,LazarusE. N. 1975-1976.Structuralevels of rhythmsandform nAfricanmusic.

    African

    Music

    5,

    no.

    4:27-35.

    Floyd,

    Samuel

    A.,

    Jr.

    1991.

    Ring

    shout

    Literary

    tudies,

    historical

    tudies,

    and

    black

    music

    inquiry.

    BlackMusic

    Research

    ournal

    1,

    no.

    2:265-287.

    .1993. On

    integrative

    nquiry:

    Towarda

    common

    scholarship.

    CBMR

    Digest

    6,

    no.

    1:1-6.

    Forte,

    Allen.

    1973.The tructure

    of

    atonal

    music.

    New

    Haven,

    Conn.:

    Yale

    University

    Press.

    Holquist,

    Michael,

    and Vadim

    Liapunov.

    1990.

    Introduction.

    Art

    and

    answerability: arly

    philosophicalssaysby

    M. M.

    Bakhtin.

    ustin:

    University

    of Texas

    Press.

    Hornbostel,

    ErichMaria

    von.

    1928.

    African

    Negro

    music.

    Africa

    :30-61.

    Johnston,

    Thomas

    F.

    1971.

    Shananga-Tsonga

    rum and bow

    rhythms.African

    Music

    5,

    no.

    1:59-72.

    Jones.

    A. M. 1954.Eastand

    west,

    northandsouth.

    African

    Music

    1,

    no. 1:59-62.

    .1959.

    Studies n

    African

    music.

    2

    vols.

    London:

    Oxford

    University

    Press.

    .

    1964.

    African

    metrical

    lyrics.African

    Music

    3,

    no.

    3:6-14.

    Kelso,

    J.A.S.,

    and

    K.

    G.

    Munhall.

    1988.

    R.

    H.

    Stetson's

    "Motor

    honetics":

    retrospective

    di-

    tion.

    Boston:

    Little,

    Brown.

    88

  • 8/11/2019 Rahn Jay African Rhythm European MT

    20/20

    Rahn

    *

    Turning

    the

    Analysis

    Around

    King,

    Anthony.

    1960.

    Employments

    of

    the

    "standard

    pattern"

    n Yoruba

    music.

    African

    Music

    2,

    no.

    3:51-54.

    1961. YorubaacredmusicromEkiti. badan,Nigeria:IbadanUniversityPress.

    Knowlton,

    Don.

    1926.The

    anatomy

    of

    jazz. Harper'sMonthly

    Magazine

    April):578-585.

    Koetting,

    James.

    1970.

    Analysis

    and

    notationof WestAfricandrum

    ensemble music.

    Select-

    ed

    Reports

    ,

    no.

    3:115-146.

    Kohler,

    Wolfgang.

    1947. Gestalt

    psychology.

    nd ed. New

    York:

    Liveright.

    Kolinski,

    Mieczyslaw.

    1967.

    Recent trends in

    ethnomusicology.

    Ethnomusicology

    1,

    no.

    1:1-24.

    1973.A

    cross-cultural

    approach

    o

    metro-rhythmic

    atterns.

    Ethnomusicology

    7,

    no.

    3:494-506.

    Kubik,

    Gerhard.

    1972.

    Transcription

    f African

    music from silent

    film.

    African

    Music

    5,

    no.

    2:28-39.

    1979.Patternperceptionand recognition n Africanmusic. In Theperformingrts:

    Musicand

    dance,

    dited

    by

    John

    Blacking

    and

    Joann

    W.

    Kealiinohomoku,

    21-249.

    The

    Hague:

    Mouton.

    Ladzekpo,

    Kobla. 1971.

    The

    social

    mechanicsof

    good

    music:

    A

    description

    of

    dance

    clubs

    among

    the

    Anlo-speaking

    people

    of

    Ghana.

    African

    Music

    5,

    no.

    1:6-22.

    Ladzekpo,

    Kobla,

    and

    Hewitt

    Pantaleoni.

    1970. Takada

    drumming.

    African

    Music

    4,

    no.

    4:6-31.

    McClary,

    usan,

    and

    Robert

    Walser. 994.

    Theorizing

    he

    body

    in

    African-American

    music.

    Black

    MusicResearch

    ournal

    4,

    no. 1:75-84.

    Meyer,

    Leonard

    B.

    1973.

    Explaining

    music.

    Berkeley:

    University

    of

    California

    Press.

    Nketia,

    J.

    H.

    Kwabena.

    1958.

    Traditional

    music of the Ga

    people.

    African

    Music

    2,

    no.

    1:21-30.

    . 1963a.

    African

    music n Ghana.

    Evanston,

    ll.:Northwestern

    University

    Press.

    .

    1963b.Folk

    songsof

    Ghana.

    Legon:University

    of

    Ghana.

    .

    1974.

    Themusic

    of Africa.

    New York:W.

    W.

    Norton.

    Pantaleoni,

    Hewitt. 1972.

    Toward

    understanding

    he

    play

    of

    sogo

    in

    atsia.

    Ethnomusicology

    16,

    no. 1:1-37.

    Pressing,

    Jeff.

    1983.

    Cognitive

    isomorphisms

    n

    pitch

    and

    rhythm

    in world

    musics:

    West

    Africa,

    he Balkans

    and

    Western

    tonality.

    Studies

    n

    Music 17:38-61.

    Raab,

    Claus. 1970.

    Trommelmusik

    erHausa n

    Nord-West-Nigeria.

    unich:

    Kommissionsver-

    lag

    Klaus Renner.

    Rahn,

    Jay.

    1977.

    Some

    recurrent eaturesof

    scales. In

    TheoryOnly

    2,

    no.

    11/12:43-52.

    .1978.

    Evaluating

    metrical

    interpretations.

    erspectives

    f

    New Music

    16,

    no. 2:35-49.

    .1983. A theoryorallmusic:Problemsndsolutions n theanalysis fnon-Westernforms.

    Toronto:

    University

    of Toronto

    Press.

    1987.

    Asymmetrical

    ostinatos n

    sub-Saharan

    music:

    Time,

    pitch,

    and

    cycles

    recon-

    sidered. In

    Theory

    Only

    9,

    no. 7:23-38.

    .1991.

    Coordination

    f

    intervalsizes in

    seven-tone

    collections.

    Journal

    f

    Music

    Theo-

    ry

    35:33-60.

    Rahn,

    Jay.

    1995.

    A

    non-numerical

    predicate

    of wide

    applicability

    or

    perceived

    ntervallic

    e-

    lations.

    Muzica

    6,

    no.

    2:23-33.

    Rahn,

    John.

    1980.

    Basicatonal

    heory.

    New

    York:

    Longman.

    Rycroft,

    David.

    1962. The

    guitar

    improvisation

    of

    Mwenda

    Jean

    Bosco

    (Part

    II).

    African

    Music

    3,

    no.

    1:86-101.

    Sargeant,Winthrop.

    1938.

    Jazz:

    Hotand

    hybrid.

    New York:Arrow

    Editions.

    Schuller,

    Gunther.1968.

    Early azz:

    Its

    rootsandmusical

    evelopment.

    ew York:

    OxfordUni-

    versity

    Press.

    Stearns,

    Marshall.1956.The

    tory

    of

    jazz.

    New York:

    Oxford

    University

    Press.

    Tomlinson,

    Gary.

    1991.

    Cultural

    dialogics

    and

    jazz:

    A

    white historian

    signifies.

    Black

    Music

    Research

    ournal

    1,

    no.

    2:229-264.

    89