research in autism spectrum disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · the...

8
The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder Jina Jang *, Johnny L. Matson, Paige E. Cervantes, Matthew J. Konst Louisiana State University, United States Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a common, neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by qualitative deficits in social communication as well as the presence of restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests or activities (Fodstad, Matson, Hess, & Neal, 2009; Horovitz & Matson, 2010; Matson, Boisjoli, Hess, & Wilkins, 2010; Matson, Kozlowski, Hattier, Horovitz, & Sipes, 2012; Matson, Mahan, Hess, Fodstad, & Neal, 2010). In addition to core deficits, individuals with ASD often evince a range of associated symptoms such as cognitive impairment, deficits in adaptive functioning, and feeding and sleep difficulties (Kozlowski, Matson, Belva, & Rieske, 2012; Krakowiak, Goodlin-Jones, Hertz-Picciotto, Croen, & Hansen, 2012; Matson, Hattier, & Belva, 2012; Matson & Shoemaker, 2009). Additionally, challenging behaviors commonly co-occur and are problematic (Matson, Hess, & Boisjoli, 2010; Matson & Rivet, 2008). Because individuals with ASD are affected across the lifespan and often need continued services into adulthood (Howlin, Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000; Matson, Dempsey, & Fodstad, 2009; Matson, Rivet, Fodstad, Dempsey, & Boisjoli, 2009), ASD can be a very emotionally and economically taxing condition for those afflicted and their caregivers. ASD was once considered a relatively rare condition with original prevalence rates estimated at one in 2500 individuals (Matson & Kozlowski, 2011). Conversely, epidemiological studies from the early 2000s indicated that ASD prevalence estimates were closer to one in 150 individuals (Center for Disease Control [CDC], 2007; Inglese & Elder, 2009). Today, estimated prevalence rates are higher than ever; ASD affects approximately one in 88 children in the United States (CDC, 2012). Further, ASD has previously been thought to predominantly affect non-Hispanic white children (CDC, 2007); however, the rate of ASD in all ethnicity/race has increased dramatically since earlier studies. Compared to prevalence rates published in 2007, there has been a 70% increase in ASD among non-Hispanic white children, a 91% increase among non- Hispanic black children, and a 110% increase among Hispanic children (CDC, 2012). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932 A R T I C L E I N F O Article history: Received 31 January 2014 Received in revised form 16 March 2014 Accepted 9 April 2014 Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder First concern Ethnicity/race A B S T R A C T The current study examined the relationship between ethnicity and the age at which parents first become concerned about their children’s development in 1478 toddlers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and atypical development. Based on the current findings, there were no racial/ethnic differences in age of parent’s first concerns. Caregivers of toddlers with ASD first developed concerns around the same time independent of which ethnic groups they belong to. In addition, the age of caregivers’ first concerns was significantly younger for those with ASD compared to those who are atypically developing without an ASD diagnosis. Implications regarding these findings are discussed. ß 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. * Corresponding author at: Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, United States. Tel.: +1 9098150565. E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Jang). Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders Jo ur n al h o mep ag e: ht tp ://ees.els evier.c o m/RA SD/d efau lt.asp http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.04.003 1750-9467/ß 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Upload: others

Post on 22-Jun-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Jo ur n al h o mep ag e: ht tp : / /ees .e ls evier .c o m/RA SD/d efau l t .asp

The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in

toddlers with autism spectrum disorder

Jina Jang *, Johnny L. Matson, Paige E. Cervantes, Matthew J. Konst

Louisiana State University, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O

Article history:

Received 31 January 2014

Received in revised form 16 March 2014

Accepted 9 April 2014

Keywords:

Autism spectrum disorder

First concern

Ethnicity/race

A B S T R A C T

The current study examined the relationship between ethnicity and the age at which

parents first become concerned about their children’s development in 1478 toddlers with

autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and atypical development. Based on the current findings,

there were no racial/ethnic differences in age of parent’s first concerns. Caregivers of

toddlers with ASD first developed concerns around the same time independent of which

ethnic groups they belong to. In addition, the age of caregivers’ first concerns was

significantly younger for those with ASD compared to those who are atypically developing

without an ASD diagnosis. Implications regarding these findings are discussed.

� 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a common, neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by qualitative deficits in socialcommunication as well as the presence of restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests or activities(Fodstad, Matson, Hess, & Neal, 2009; Horovitz & Matson, 2010; Matson, Boisjoli, Hess, & Wilkins, 2010; Matson, Kozlowski,Hattier, Horovitz, & Sipes, 2012; Matson, Mahan, Hess, Fodstad, & Neal, 2010). In addition to core deficits, individuals withASD often evince a range of associated symptoms such as cognitive impairment, deficits in adaptive functioning, and feedingand sleep difficulties (Kozlowski, Matson, Belva, & Rieske, 2012; Krakowiak, Goodlin-Jones, Hertz-Picciotto, Croen, & Hansen,2012; Matson, Hattier, & Belva, 2012; Matson & Shoemaker, 2009). Additionally, challenging behaviors commonly co-occurand are problematic (Matson, Hess, & Boisjoli, 2010; Matson & Rivet, 2008). Because individuals with ASD are affected acrossthe lifespan and often need continued services into adulthood (Howlin, Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000; Matson, Dempsey, &Fodstad, 2009; Matson, Rivet, Fodstad, Dempsey, & Boisjoli, 2009), ASD can be a very emotionally and economically taxingcondition for those afflicted and their caregivers.

ASD was once considered a relatively rare condition with original prevalence rates estimated at one in 2500 individuals(Matson & Kozlowski, 2011). Conversely, epidemiological studies from the early 2000s indicated that ASD prevalenceestimates were closer to one in 150 individuals (Center for Disease Control [CDC], 2007; Inglese & Elder, 2009). Today,estimated prevalence rates are higher than ever; ASD affects approximately one in 88 children in the United States (CDC,2012). Further, ASD has previously been thought to predominantly affect non-Hispanic white children (CDC, 2007);however, the rate of ASD in all ethnicity/race has increased dramatically since earlier studies. Compared to prevalence ratespublished in 2007, there has been a 70% increase in ASD among non-Hispanic white children, a 91% increase among non-Hispanic black children, and a 110% increase among Hispanic children (CDC, 2012).

* Corresponding author at: Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, United States.

Tel.: +1 9098150565.

E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Jang).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.04.003

1750-9467/� 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Page 2: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932926

One of many factors that may be contributing to the growing prevalence of ASD is increased awareness of the conditionin the general and professional populations. Increased awareness of the symptoms of ASD may lead to earlier and improveddetection in young children (Matson & Kozlowski, 2011). Although the onset of symptoms primarily occurs in affectedindividuals before 30 months of age, typical diagnosis of ASD is made at 3 or 4 years old (Matson, Wilkins, & Gonzalez,2008). However, researchers suggest reliable diagnosis of ASD can be made as early as 2 years old (Horovitz, Matson,Turygin, & Beighley, 2012; Matson, Gonzalez, Wilkins, & Rivet, 2008). With growing prevalence rates as well as thewidespread and lifelong impairments associated with ASD, early evaluation and diagnosis is imperative (Matson, Gonzales,& Wilkins, 2008).

Caregiver detection of symptoms at a young age is imperative in ensuring early diagnosis and early treatment.Researchers indicate that parents of children with ASD report first having concerns around 13 months of age (Kozlowski,Matson, Horovitz, Worley & Neal, 2011). Because earlier detection of symptoms is correlated with earlier age of whichparents’ seek evaluation (Kozlowski et al., 2011), research into the factors related to parental symptom recognition is ofimportance. Child gender has been associated with earlier parental detection of delays; parents of female children reportearlier concerns (Horovitz et al., 2012). However, little is known about the influence of child ethnicity on parent’s firstdetection of symptoms. Therefore, the current study seeks to explore the relationship of child ethnicity to age of first concern.Previous researchers looked at the relationship between child ethnicity and child age at diagnosis and found that AfricanAmerican and Hispanic children received an autism diagnosis at older ages than Caucasian children (Mandell, Listerud, Levy,& Pinto-Martin, 2002; Mandell et al., 2009). Based on results of these studies, it is hypothesized that child’s age of parent’sfirst concern and ethnicity will be significantly different. More specifically, it is hypothesized that the mean age of Caucasianparents’ first concerns will be earlier than those of non-Caucasian parents.

1. Methods

1.1. Participants

A total of 1478 toddlers (1034 male and 444 female) were included in the current study. Participants ranged in agefrom 17 to 37 months (M = 25.79, SD = 4.72) at the time of assessment. Based upon informant report, the ethnicity ofthe participants included Caucasian (n = 799), African-American (n = 552), and other ethnicity including Hispanic andAsian (n = 127). Because the number of participants who were not Caucasians or African-Americans was small, thoseof Hispanic, Asian, and other ethnicity were collapsed into one group; however, uneven distribution of race of oursample is representative of the Louisiana population. Participants were assessed as a part of their inclusion in thestate of Louisiana’s Early Steps, an early intervention program provided by the Individuals with Disabilities EducationAct, Part C.

Following assessment, participants were separated into one of two diagnostic groups: ASD groups who met diagnosticcriteria (n = 255) and Atypical development (n = 1223). Participants in the Atypical development group were not diagnosedwith an ASD but were identified as being developmentally delayed or were reported to have a disorder such as DownSyndrome, seizure disorder, sickle cell anemia, premature birth, and microcephaly. Diagnostic decisions were made basedupon results of the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins, Fein, Barton, & Green, 2001), the Battelle

Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2; Newborg, 2005), diagnostic criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual

of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and clinical judgment. Clinical diagnosiswas made by a licensed clinical psychologist with over three decades of experience in working with individuals withdevelopmental disabilities. Complete demographic information can be found in Table 1.

1.2. Measures

Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins et al., 2001). The M-CHAT is a brief assessment designed for useby specialists and pediatricians when ASD is suspected. A total of 23 items are administered to parents and caregivers toassess the individual’s behavior, social skills, and communication. Additional evaluation is recommended if a parentendorses ‘‘no’’ for three or more total items or two critical items. Diagnostic specificity (.99) and specificity (.87) are reported

Table 1

Demographic characteristics per race and diagnostic group (n = 1478).

ASD Non ASD

African-American (n = 112) Caucasian (n = 121) Other (n = 22) African-American (n = 440) Caucasian (n = 678) Other (n = 105)

Age

M (SD) 26.45 (4.5) 25.88 (4.47) 27.32 (5.41) 26.03 (4.74) 25.53 (4.73) 25.33 (4.7)

Gender %

Male 70.50% 71.90% 68.20% 67.50% 70.20% 76.20%

Female 29.50% 28.10% 31.80% 32.50% 29.80% 23.80%

Note: Age is denoted in months.

Page 3: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

Table 2

First age of parental concern means (standard deviations) by race.

African-American (n = 840) Caucasian (n = 1174) Other (n = 183)

Age

Mean (SD) 12.78(8.88) 13.21(8.56) 12.31(9.47)

Table 3

First age of parental concern means (standard deviations) by diagnostic group.

ASD (n = 255) Atypically developing (n = 1223)

Age

Mean (SD) 12.05 (8.48)a 13.16 (8.81)a

a Denotes significant difference found between groups.

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932 927

to be high. Overall, the M-CHAT demonstrates good internal reliability (.85) and internal reliability for the critical items isreported to be .83 (Robins et al., 2001; Tables 2 and 3).

Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2; Newborg, 2005). The BDI-2 is designed to assess children frombirth to 7 years 11 months for delay in five domains of childhood development (i.e., personal/social, adaptive, motor,communication, and cognitive). The BDI-2 consists of 450 items that are scored based upon direct observation and informantresponse. For most items, the examiner may choose from one of three examination methods to best allow item completion.Item endorsement includes a 3-point scale ranging from ‘‘no ability in this skill’’ (0), to ‘‘ability in this skill’’ (2). Adevelopmental quotient (DQ) based upon item endorsement is calculated for each individual domain and for the Total Score.Content and criterion validity of the BDI-2 has been observed in multiple populations (e.g., developmental delays, motordelays, speech and/or language delays, premature birth, and ASD). The BDI-2 was observed to be moderately correlated withthe Vineland Adaptive Behaviors Scale-2nd edition (Bliss, 2007). Internal consistency for the computed total score is reportedto be between .98 and .99 (Newborg, 2005). Test–retest reliability for the BDI-2 was observed to be above .80 for theindividual domains and total scores (Newborg, 2005).

Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT) Demographic Form (Matson, Boisjoli, & Wilkins, 2007). TheBISCUIT is a battery of assessments designed to assess for ASD symptoms, comorbid symptoms, and problem behaviors ininfants and toddlers. The larger assessment battery also includes a Demographic Form in order to gain relevant informationsuch as age, race, ethnicity, weight, and height (Matson et al., 2007). In addition to demographic information the formincludes additional questions to be read aloud to the informant by a qualified professional. This section includes backgroundinformation such as age of first concern and the specific concern, relevant medical history, child’s age for each developmentalmilestone, and additional family information.

1.3. Procedure

The present study was approved by the institutional review board of a state university in the southern United States andthe Office for Citizens with Developmental Disabilities for Louisiana. All parents or caregivers provided informed consentprior to their inclusion in the current study. Parents or caregivers were administered a battery of assessments as part of theirparticipation in the EarlySteps program. Diagnostic measures (i.e., BISCUIT, BDI-2, and M-CHAT) were administered bycertified or licensed practitioners in fields such as early childhood development, speech/language pathology, specialeducation, social work, and psychology. Raters held a minimum of a bachelor level degree and received additional training inassessment administration.

1.4. Statistical analyses

First, a priori analyses were conducted to determine if the groups vary significantly on any of the diagnostic variables (i.e.,age, gender). Chi square tests were conducted to determine potential differences among groups in gender of the participants.A priori Chi Square test indicated that groups did not significantly differ for gender, x2 (2) = 2.41 p > .05. An Analysis ofVariance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine potential group differences in age. The assumption of homogeneity ofvariances was not violated, as assessed by Levene’s Test of Homogeneity of Variance (p = .47). An ANOVA did not reveal anydifferences between groups on age, F (2, 1475) = 2.12, p > .05. Following analyses of demographic data, a two-way, betweensubjects Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to investigate differences in age of first concern among differentethnic groups. Child’s ethnicity and diagnostic group were entered as independent variables, age of parents’ first concernswas entered as the dependent variable, and age at assessment was entered as a covariate. Age at assessment was entered as acovariate because previous literature showed significant relationship between age of parents’ first concerns and age atassessment (e.g., Horovitz et al., 2012; Kozlowski et al., 2011). Our sample also confirmed this significant relationship, r = .24,p < .001.

Page 4: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

Fig. 1. Assumption of linearity for all groups of the independent variable, race.

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932928

2. Results

An ANCOVA was run to determine the effect of different ethnicity on child’s age of parents’ first reported concern aftercontrolling for age at assessment. There was a linear relationship between age at assessment and child’s age of parent’s firstconcern for each level of independent variables, as assessed by visual inspection of a scatterplot (see Figs. 1 and 2). There washomogeneity of regression slopes as the interaction term was not statistically significant, F (2, 1471) = 1.04, p = .35; F (1,1471) = 1.97, p = .16. There was homoscedasticity and homogeneity of variances, as assessed by visual inspection of ascatterplot and Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance (p = .153), respectively (see Fig. 3). After adjustment for age atassessment, there was no statistically significant difference in the child’s age of parent’s first concern between differentethnicity, F (2, 1471) = .57, p = .57, partial h2 = 001. The mean age (months) of parent’s first concerns for African-Americanchildren, Caucasian children, and other ethnicity including Hispanic and Asian were 12.8, 13.2, 12.3, respectively.

A significant main effect was found for diagnostic group, F (1, 1471) = 4.05, p < .05, partial h2 = .003, such that parents’ firstconcerns for those with ASD were younger (M = 12.05, SD = 8.48) than for those without an ASD diagnosis (M = 13.16,SD = 8.81).

The covariate, age at assessment, was also significant, F (1, 1471) = 90.03 p < .001. The interaction of race and diagnosticgroup was not significant, F (2, 1471) = .41, p = .66, partial h2 = .058.

3. Discussion

The core symptoms of ASD and its associated conditions such as challenging behavior and comorbid disorders may affectquality of life of those afflicted with ASD; they may experience lifelong deficits in social, cognitive, and adaptive skills(Matson, Dempsey, & Fodstad, 2009a, 2009b; Matson, Mayville, Lott, Bielecki, & Logan, 2003). Given the lifelong nature ofASD, it is critical to identify the condition as early as possible in order to effectively treat it (LoVullo & Matson, 2009; Matson,Rieske, & Tureck, 2011; Poon, 2012; Smith & Matson, 2010a, 2010b, 2010c). EIBI is considered the most well-establishedtreatment for ASD; EIBI involves the use of operant conditioning techniques (i.e., applied behavior analysis) to targetbehavioral deficits and excesses in children with ASD. EIBI has been shown to improve the core symptoms of ASD and provesmost effective when treatment is intensive (20–40 h/week) and administered directly to the child over a long period of time(Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, & Stanislaw, 2005; Matson, Tureck, Turygin, Beighley, & Rieske, 2012; Matson, Gonzalez,Wilkins, & Rivet, 2008; Matson, Gonzales, & Wilkins, 2008). Previous researchers have found that EIBI is effective inimproving cognitive, social, and adaptive skills and decreasing autism severity and challenging behaviors in children withASD (Dawson et al., 2010; Eikeseth, Klintwall, Jahr, & Karlsson, 2012; Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, 2007; Fava et al., 2011;

Page 5: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

Fig. 2. Assumption of linearity for all groups of the independent variable, diagnosis.

Fig. 3. Assumption of homoscedasticity: Scatterplot of the standardized residuals against the predicted values.

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932 929

Page 6: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932930

Fernell et al., 2011; Granpeesheh, Tarbox, & Dixon, 2009; Howard et al., 2005; Makrygianni & Reed, 2010; Matson & Wilkins,2009; Peters-Scheffer, Didden, Korzilius, & Matson, 2012; Virues-Ortega, 2010).

Further highlighting the importance of EIBI, a number of researchers have demonstrated that the best treatmentoutcomes result from earlier intervention (Howard et al., 2005; Horovitz et al., 2012). Many researchers examined age atintervention and treatment outcome and found that they were positively related. For example, Fenske, Zalenski, Krantz, andMcClannahan (1985) found that children’s age at intervention entry was strongly related to positive treatment outcome,which was measured by continued residence with family and attendance at public school. Harris and Handleman (2000)found that younger children with higher IQ at time of admission to a behavioral treatment had better educational placementthan older children with lower IQ. Granpeesheh, Dixon, Tarbox, Kaplan, and Wilke (2009) also found that younger childrenwith ASD showed greater treatment outcome compared to older children and discussed that it becomes more challenging toaddress deficits as the gap between chronological and developmental age increases (Howard et al., 2005). Results of thesestudies emphasize the importance of early detection and intervention in autism.

Due to inconsistent methodology of most of the EIBI studies, researchers can come to different conclusions about factorsthat affect the effectiveness of EIBI (Matson & Smith, 2008). Without a doubt, additional research with more consistentmeasurement methods is needed to learn more about the true value of EIBI; however, despite the shortcomings of EIBIstudies, EIBI still is one of the few evidence-based treatment for individuals with ASD and is the pre-eminent intervention foryoung children. Then, early detection of autism symptom is essential for faster ASD diagnoses to ensure treatment as early aspossible (Fernell & Gillberg, 2010; Giannoni & Kass, 2010).

In the current study, we found that parents of children with ASD developed their first concerns earlier compared toparents of atypically developing children without an ASD. Caregivers of this sample reported that they first recognized theirchildren’s developmental problems around 12–13 months of age. This finding is in agreement with previous studies(Chawarska et al., 2007; Kozlowski et al., 2011). Early recognition of autism was also detected in home videotapes of 1-yearold children; those with ASD evinced significantly fewer social and joint attention behaviors (e.g., pointing, showing objects,looking at others, responding to name) than the control group (Osterling & Dawson, 1994). The earlier recognition than otherstudies (Baghdadli, Picot, Pascal, Pry, & Aussilloux, 2003; De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998) may be explained by increasedawareness and better understanding of ASD.

More importantly, no racial differences were found in age of parent’s first concerns. In other words, caregivers reportedtheir first concerns of their children’s development around the same time independent of their races. The finding of thecurrent study is of importance because racial/ethnic disparities exist in receipt of autism diagnoses despite the fact that race/ethnicity was not a factor of symptom recognition. For example, researchers found significant racial/ethnic disparities inautism diagnosis; African American and Hispanic children received an autism diagnosis at older ages than Caucasianchildren (CDC, 2006; Mandell et al., 2002, 2009). Mandell et al. (2002) found that on average, Caucasian children receivedautism diagnoses at 6.3 years of age, African American children at 7.9 years of age, and Hispanic children at 8.8 years of age.Furthermore, researchers found racial disparities in autism treatment. Feinberg, Silverstein, Donahue, and Bliss (2011) foundthat African American children at 24 months were five times less likely to receive early intervention than Caucasian children.Magana and colleagues also found racial disparities in quality of care; African American and Latino children with autismfaced greater difficulties in receiving high-quality health care than Caucasian children did (Magana, Parish, Rose, Timberlake,& Swaine, 2012).

Limitation of the current study is unequal group sizes. Also, participants of Hispanic and Asian ethnicity were groupedtogether due to the small Asian and Hispanic population in Louisiana. Uneven group sizes are representative of the currentpopulation of Louisiana. However, with unequal group sizes, caution must be applied, as the findings might not generalizewell to the population. Asian groups and Hispanic groups may have different views so the fact that they were grouped in onegroup may not reflect their true views. Future research should include more equal sample groups to ensure that results of thecurrent study are meaningful.

Generally, there is an overall delay in diagnosis. A 13-month delay between evaluation and autism diagnosis has beenreported (Wiggins, Baio, & Rice, 2006); they reported that their sample children were initially evaluated at a mean age of 48months but were not diagnosed with ASD until a mean age of 61 months. Many studies including the current study showedthat autism symptoms arise as early as 12 months. Clinicians need to pay particular attention when evaluating infants andtoddlers and look for subtle behaviors such as attentiveness and responsiveness (Matson, Gonzalez, & Wilkins, 2009).Furthermore, more research is needed to examine racial/ethnic disparities in autism diagnosis and treatment (Matson &LoVullo, 2009). Researchers and clinicians should better understand culture differences on health issues (i.e., differentperspectives on disability/disorder, help-seeking, and support/advocacy). More research, support, and education are neededto eliminate racial/ethnic/gender differences in autism diagnosis and treatment in order to maximize positive outcomes forall affected children (Rivet & Matson, 2011).

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association DSM 5.Baghdadli, A., Picot, M. C., Pascal, C., Pry, R., & Aussilloux, C. (2003). Relationship between age of recognition of first disturbances and severity in young children

with autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 122–127.Bliss, S. L. (2007). Test review: Newborg, J. (2005). Battelle developmental inventory-second edition. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 409–415.

Page 7: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932 931

Chawarska, K., Paul, R., Klin, A., Hannigen, S., Dichtel, L. E., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers with autism spectrumdisorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 62–72.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). Mental health in the United States: Parental report of diagnosed autism in children aged 4–17 years—UnitedStates, 2003–2004. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 55, 481–486.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2007). Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs) Retrieved January 15, 2014, from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss5601a2.htm.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs) Retrieved January 15, 2014, from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6103a1.htm.

Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., et al. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: Theearly start Denver model. Pediatrics, 125, 17–23.

De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 131–136.

Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Jahr, E., & Karlsson, P. (2012). Outcome for children with autism receiving early and intensive behavioral intervention in mainstreampreschool and kindergarten settings. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 829–835.

Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2007). Outcome for children with autism who began intensive behavioral treatment between ages 4 and 7: Acomparison controlled study. Behavior Modification, 31, 264–278.

Fava, L., Strauss, K., Valeri, G., D’Elia, L., Arima, S., & Vicari, S. (2011). The effectiveness of a cross-setting complementary staff- and parent-mediated early intensivebehavioral intervention for young children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1479–1492.

Feinberg, E., Silverstein, M., Donahue, S., & Bliss, R. (2011). The impact of race on participation in Part C early intervention Services. Journal of Developmental andBehavioral Pediatrics, 32, 284–291.

Fenske, E. C., Zalenski, S., Krantz, P., & McClannahan, L. E. (1985). Age at intervention and treatment outcome for autistic children in a comprehensive interventionprogram. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5, 49–58.

Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Autism spectrum disorder diagnoses in Stockholm preschoolers. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 680–685.Fernell, E., Hedvall, A., Westerlund, J., Carlsoon, L. H., Eriksson, M., Olsson, M. B., et al. (2011). Early intervention in 208 Swedish preschoolers with autism

spectrum disorder. A prospective naturalistic study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2092–2101.Fodstad, J. C., Matson, J. L., Hess, J., & Neal, D. (2009). Social and communication behaviors in infants and toddlers with autism and pervasive developmental

disorder-not otherwise specified. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 152–157.Giannoni, P. P., & Kass, P. H. (2010). Risk factors of children who exited from an early intervention program without an identified disability and returned with a

developmental disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 848–856.Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., & Wilke, A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treatment intensity on behavioral intervention outcomes for

children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 1014–1022.Granpeesheh, D., Tarbox, J., & Dixon, D. R. (2009). Applied behavior analytic interventions for children with autism: A description and review of treatment

research. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 21, 162–173.Harris, S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (2000). Age and IQ at intake as predictors of placement for young children with autism: A four to six year follow-up. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 137–142.Horovitz, M., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Communication deficits in babies and infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified

(PDD-NOS). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 390–398.Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., Turygin, N., & Beighley, J. S. (2012). The relationship between gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum

disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 466–471.Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G., & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young

children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 359–383.Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social,

behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 561–578.Inglese, M. D., & Elder, J. H. (2009). Caring for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, part I: Prevalence, etiology, and core features. Journal of Pediatric Nursing,

24, 41–48.Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., Belva, B., & Rieske, R. (2012). Feeding and sleep difficulties in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 6, 385–390.Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., Worley, J. A., & Neal, D. (2011). Parents’ first concerns of their child’s development in toddlers with autism spectrum

disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 72–78.Krakowiak, P., Goodlin-Jones, B., Hertz-Picciotto, I., Croen, L. A., & Hansen, R. L. (2012). Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental

delays, and typical development: A population-based study. Journal of Sleep Research, 21, 231.LoVullo, S. V., & Matson, J. L. (2009). Comorbid psychopathology in adults with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 30, 1288–1296.Magana, S., Parish, S. L., Rose, R. A., Timberlake, M., & Swaine, J. G. (2012). Racial and ethnic disparities in quality of health care among children with autism and

other developmental disabilities. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50, 287–299.Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). A meta-analytic review of the effectiveness of behavioural early intervention programs for children with autism spectrum

disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 577–593.Mandell, D. S., Listerud, J., Levy, S. E., & Pinto-Martin, J. A. (2002). Race differences in the age at diagnosis among Medicaid-eligible children with autism. Journal of

the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4, 1447–1453.Mandell, D. S., Wiggins, L. D., Carpenter, L. A., Daniels, J., DiGuiseppi, C., Durkin, M. S., et al. (2009). Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of children with

autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Public Health, 99, 493–498.Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., Hess, J. A., & Wilkins, J. (2010). Factor structure and diagnostic fidelity of the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtism Traits Part1

(BISCUIT-Part 1). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 72–79.Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., & Wilkins, J. (2007). The baby and infant screen for children with aUtIsm traits (BISCUIT). Baton Rouge, LA: Disability Consultants LLC.Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009a). Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviors in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder.

Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 122–127.Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009b). The effect of autism spectrum disorders on adaptive independent living skills in autism with severe intellectual

disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 1203–1211.Matson, J. L., Gonzalez, M. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Validity study of the Autism Spectrum Disorders-Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC). Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 3, 196–206.Matson, J. L., Gonzalez, M. L., Wilkins, J., & Rivet, T. T. (2008). Reliability of the Autism Spectrum Disorder-Diagnostic for children (ASD-DC). Research in Autism

Spectrum Disorders, 2, 696–706.Matson, J. L., Hattier, M. A., & Belva, B. (2012). Treating adaptive living skills with autism using applied behavior analysis. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6,

271–276.Matson, J. L., Hess, J. A., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2010). Comorbid psychopathology in infants and toddlers with autism and pervasive developmental disorders-not

otherwise specified (PDD-NOS). Research in Autism Disorders, 4, 300–304.Matson, J. L., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 418–425.Matson, J. L., Kozlowski, A. M., Hattier, M. A., Horovitz, M., & Sipes, M. (2012). DSM IV versus DSM V diagnostic criteria for infants and toddlers with autism.

Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 15, 185–190.Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2009). Trends and topics in autism spectrum disorders research. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3. 252, 257.

Page 8: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorderssites01.lsu.edu/faculty/matson/wp-content/uploads/... · The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism

J. Jang et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8 (2014) 925–932932

Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., & Neal, D. (2010). Convergent validity of the Autism Spectrum Disorder-Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC) andChildhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 633–638.

Matson, J. L., Mayville, E. A., Lott, J. D., Bielecki, J., & Logan, R. (2003). A comparison of social and adaptive functioning in person with psychosis, autism, and severeor profound mental retardation. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 15, 57–65.

Matson, J. L., Rieske, R. D., & Tureck, K. (2011). Additional considerations for the early detection and diagnosis of autism: Review of available instruments. Researchin Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1319–1326.

Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008). Characteristics of challenging behaviors in adults with autistic disorder, PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal ofIntellectual and Developmental Disability, 33, 313–329.

Matson, J. L., Rivet, T. T., Fodstad, J. C., Dempsey, T., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2009). Examination of adaptive behavior differences in adults with autism spectrum disordersand intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 1317–1325.

Matson, J. L., & Shoemaker, M. (2009). Intellectual disability and its relationship to autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 1107–1114.

Matson, J. L., & Smith, K. R. M. (2008). Current status of intensive behavioral interventions for young children with autism and PDD-NOS. Research in AutismSpectrum Disorders, 2, 60–74.

Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., Turygin, N., Beighley, J., & Rieske, R. (2012). Trends and topics in early intensive behavioral intervention for toddlers with autism. Researchin Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1412–1417.

Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Psychometric testing methods for children’s social skills. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 249–274.Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Gonzalez, M. L. (2008). Early identification and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: How early is too

early? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 75–84.Newborg, J. (2005). Battelle developmental inventory (2nd ed.). Itasca IL: Riverside.Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 24, 247–257.Peters-Scheffer, N., Didden, R., Korzilius, H., & Matson, J. (2012). Cost comparison of early intensive behavioral intervention and treatment as usual for children

with autism spectrum disorder in the Netherlands. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1763–1772.Poon, K. K. (2012). Challenging behaviors among children with autism spectrum disorders and multiple disabilities attending special schools in Singapore.

Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 578–582.Rivet, T. T., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Review of gender differences in core symptomology in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,

957–976.Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism

and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144.Smith, K. R. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010a). Behavior problems: differences among intellectually disabled adults with co-morbid autism spectrum disorders and

epilepsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1062–1069.Smith, K. R. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010b). Psychopathology: Differences among adults with intellectual disability and comorbid autism spectrum disorders and

epilepsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 743–749.Smith, K. R. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010c). Social skills: Differences among intellectually disabled adults with co-morbid autism spectrum disorder and epilepsy.

Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1366–1372.Wiggins, L. D., Baio, J., & Rice, C. (2006). Examination of the time between first evaluation and first autism spectrum diagnosis in a population-based sample.

Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, S79–S87.Virues-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic intervention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis, meta-regression and dose-response meta-analysis

of multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 387–399.