research original research under embargo until …

10
RESEARCH Original Research Slower Eating Speed Lowers Energy Intake in Normal-Weight but not Overweight/Obese Subjects Meena Shah, PhD; Jennifer Copeland, MS; Lyn Dart, PhD; Beverley Adams-Huet, MS; Ashlei James, MS; Debbie Rhea, EdD ARTICLE INFORMATION Article history: Accepted 28 October 2013 Keywords: Eating speed Energy intake Appetite ratings Normal weight Overweight Supplementary materials: Podcast available at www.andjrnl.org/content/ podcast Copyright ª 2013 by the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2212-2672/$36.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.002 ABSTRACT Background The effect of eating speed on energy intake by weight status is unclear. Objective To examine whether the effect of eating speed on energy intake is the same in normal-weight and overweight/obese subjects. Design The effect of slow and fast eating speed on meal energy intake was assessed in a randomized crossover design. Participants/setting Thirty-ve normal-weight (aged 33.312.5 years; 14 women and 21 men) subjects and 35 overweight/obese (44.113.0 years; 22 women and 13 men) subjects were studied on 2 days during lunch in a metabolic kitchen. Intervention The subjects consumed the same meal, ad libitum, but at different speeds during the two eating conditions. The weight and energy content of the food consumed was assessed. Perceived hunger and fullness were assessed at specic times using visual analog scales. Statistical analyses Effect of eating speed on ad libitum energy intake, eating rate (energy intake/meal duration), energy density (energy intake per gram of food and water consumed), and satiety were assessed by mixed-model repeated measures analysis. Results Meal energy intake was signicantly lower in the normal-weight (804.5438.9 vs 892.6330.2 kcal; P¼0.04) but not the overweight/obese (667.3304.1 vs 724.8355.5 kcal; P¼0.18) subjects during the slow vs the fast eating condition. Both groups had lower meal energy density (P¼0.005 and P¼0.001, respectively) and eating rate (P<0.0001 in both groups) during the slow vs the fast eating condition. Both groups reported less hunger (P¼0.01 and P¼0.03, respectively), and the normal-weight subjects reported more fullness (P¼0.02) at 60 minutes after the meal began during the slow compared with the fast eating condition. There was no eating speed by weight status interaction for any of the variables. Conclusions Eating slowly signicantly lowered meal energy intake in the normal- weight but not in the overweight/obese group. It lowered eating rate and energy density in both groups. Eating slowly led to lower hunger ratings in both groups and increased fullness ratings in the normal-weight group at 60 minutes from when the meal began. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;-:---. O BESITY, DEFINED AS BODY MASS INDEX (BMI) OF 30, is a major problem in the United States. Ac- cording to national surveys, the prevalence of obesity in US adults has increased from 14.5% in the years 1971-1974 to 35.9% in the years 2009-2010. 1,2 The rapid rise in the prevalence of obesity may be partly due to a 12% increase in reported energy intake in the United States from 1971-1975 to 1999-2002. 3 One possible behavioral strategy to control energy intake and body weight may be to reduce the speed of eating. According to de Graaf and Kok 4 and de Graaf, 5 a factor that weakens the ability to control energy intake is a high eating rate. The researchers suggest that a high eating rate impairs the congruent relationship between the sensory signals and the metabolic processes that determine how much we eat. Findings from a number of epidemiologic studies show that eating speed is positively related to body weight. 6-12 However, most of the studies were conducted in subjects with normal or mostly normal weight (BMI 18.5 to 24.9) and from largely Asian populations. These limitations make it difcult to generalize the ndings to the US population. In addition, the information on eating rate was self-reported. Melanson and colleagues 13 found that self-reported eating rate may not be correlated with measured free-living eating rate. Methodologic issues with assessment of eating rate render the epidemiologic results more difcult to evaluate. A number of investigators have conducted intervention studies on the effect on food intake of eating speed, modied by changing the bite size, bite rate, number of chews per bite, and/or eating duration, or by giving foods with different textures, and the results are controversial. Several studies ª 2013 by the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 1 FLA 5.2.0 DTD ĸ JAND672_proof ĸ 24 December 2013 ĸ 2:36 am ĸ ce CORRECTED PROOF UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL DECEMBER 30, 2013, 12:01 AM ET

Upload: others

Post on 01-Dec-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

U

ª 2013 by the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics.

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof � 2

R

R

RESEARCH

Original Research

NDER EMBARGO UNTIL DECEMBE

30, 2013, 12:01 AM ET

Slower Eating Speed Lowers Energy Intake inNormal-Weight but not Overweight/Obese SubjectsMeena Shah, PhD; Jennifer Copeland, MS; Lyn Dart, PhD; Beverley Adams-Huet, MS; Ashlei James, MS; Debbie Rhea, EdD

ARTICLE INFORMATION

Article history:Accepted 28 October 2013

Keywords:Eating speedEnergy intakeAppetite ratingsNormal weightOverweight

Supplementary materials:Podcast available at www.andjrnl.org/content/podcast

Copyright ª 2013 by the Academy of Nutritionand Dietetics.2212-2672/$36.00http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.002

ABSTRACTBackground The effect of eating speed on energy intake by weight status is unclear.Objective To examine whether the effect of eating speed on energy intake is the samein normal-weight and overweight/obese subjects.Design The effect of slow and fast eating speed on meal energy intake was assessed in arandomized crossover design.Participants/setting Thirty-five normal-weight (aged 33.3�12.5 years; 14 women and21 men) subjects and 35 overweight/obese (44.1�13.0 years; 22 women and 13 men)subjects were studied on 2 days during lunch in a metabolic kitchen.Intervention The subjects consumed the same meal, ad libitum, but at different speedsduring the two eating conditions. The weight and energy content of the food consumedwas assessed. Perceived hunger and fullness were assessed at specific times using visualanalog scales.Statistical analyses Effect of eating speed on ad libitum energy intake, eating rate(energy intake/meal duration), energy density (energy intake per gram of food andwater consumed), and satiety were assessed by mixed-model repeated measuresanalysis.Results Meal energy intake was significantly lower in the normal-weight (804.5�438.9vs 892.6�330.2 kcal; P¼0.04) but not the overweight/obese (667.3�304.1 vs724.8�355.5 kcal; P¼0.18) subjects during the slow vs the fast eating condition. Bothgroups had lower meal energy density (P¼0.005 and P¼0.001, respectively) and eatingrate (P<0.0001 in both groups) during the slow vs the fast eating condition. Both groupsreported less hunger (P¼0.01 and P¼0.03, respectively), and the normal-weight subjectsreported more fullness (P¼0.02) at 60 minutes after the meal began during the slowcompared with the fast eating condition. There was no eating speed by weight statusinteraction for any of the variables.Conclusions Eating slowly significantly lowered meal energy intake in the normal-weight but not in the overweight/obese group. It lowered eating rate and energydensity in both groups. Eating slowly led to lower hunger ratings in both groups andincreased fullness ratings in the normal-weight group at 60 minutes from when themeal began.J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;-:---.

ECTED PROOF

O

O

BESITY, DEFINED AS BODY MASS INDEX (BMI) OF�30, is a major problem in the United States. Ac-cording to national surveys, the prevalence ofobesity in US adults has increased from 14.5% in the

years 1971-1974 to 35.9% in the years 2009-2010.1,2 The rapidrise in the prevalence of obesity may be partly due to a 12%increase in reported energy intake in the United States from1971-1975 to 1999-2002.3

One possible behavioral strategy to control energy intakeand body weight may be to reduce the speed of eating.According to de Graaf and Kok4 and de Graaf,5 a factor thatweakens the ability to control energy intake is a higheating rate. The researchers suggest that a high eating rateimpairs the congruent relationship between the sensorysignals and the metabolic processes that determine howmuch we eat.

CR

Findings from a number of epidemiologic studies showthat eating speed is positively related to body weight.6-12

However, most of the studies were conducted in subjectswith normal or mostly normal weight (BMI 18.5 to 24.9) andfrom largely Asian populations. These limitations make itdifficult to generalize the findings to the US population. Inaddition, the information on eating rate was self-reported.Melanson and colleagues13 found that self-reported eatingrate may not be correlated with measured free-living eatingrate. Methodologic issues with assessment of eating raterender the epidemiologic results more difficult to evaluate.A number of investigators have conducted intervention

studies on the effect on food intake of eating speed, modifiedby changing the bite size, bite rate, number of chews per bite,and/or eating duration, or by giving foods with differenttextures, and the results are controversial. Several studies

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 1

4 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 2: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

have found a decrease in energy or food intake with areduction in eating rate,14-17 some studies observed lowerfood intake in response to slower eating in men but notwomen18 or in linear (typically eating at a constant rate) butnot decelerated eaters,19 three studies found no relationshipbetween eating rate and energy intake,20-22 and one study23

observed a higher energy intake with more pauses withinmeals. In addition, Forde and colleagues24 reported a lowereating rate and ad libitum intake when subjects were given asolid-savory meal vs a mashed-savory meal, and Zijlstra andcolleagues25 reported higher ad libitum intake in the highestquartile vs the lowest quartile of eating rate. Moreover, onlythree of these studies15,20,21 have examined how eating speedinfluences energy intake in obese/overweight vs normal-weight subjects. One of these three studies15 found a reduc-tion in energy intake with increased chewing activity per bitein both the normal-weight and overweight/obese groups,whereas the other two studies20,21 found no significant effectof ingestion rate on energy intake in either the normal-weight or overweight/obese groups. The controversial re-sults by weight status may be due to the fact that the threestudies15,20,21 had a limited number of subjects (six to 16normal-weight and six to 14 obese subjects) and did notmention controlling for potential confounding factors such asenergy intake and/or physical activity before the study. Inaddition, two15,21 of the three studies did not randomlyassign the meals. Lastly, Laessle and colleagues26 reportedhigher eating rate, bite size, and ad libitum energy intake inoverweight compared with normal-weight subjects. Laessleand colleagues26 did not assess the effect of different eatingspeeds on energy intake.The above issues, as well as the fact that some professional

organizations27 and weight loss programs advise obese in-dividuals to reduce eating speed to control energy intake,stress the importance of further examining the relationshipbetween eating speed and energy intake in a well-designedstudy with a larger group of normal-weight and over-weight/obese subjects. We investigated whether eating ameal slowly would lead to lower meal energy intake and lesshunger and desire to eat and higher levels of fullness after themeal compared with eating the same meal more quickly innormal-weight and overweight/obese subjects. Subjects weregiven general instructions on how to change their eatingspeed. The two eating conditions were implemented in eachsubject in a randomized crossover design, and physical ac-tivity and energy intake before the study were controlled.

MATERIALS AND METHODSSubjects and RecruitmentNormal-weight (n¼35) and overweight/obese (n¼35) menand women between the ages of 19 and 65 years wererecruited for the study (Table 1). To be classified as over-weight, a subject had to meet two of the following threecriteria: waist circumference >88 cm for women or >102 cmfor men, BMI �25.0 for both men and women, and percentbody fat >25% for men and >32% for women.28

Exclusion criteria included being severely obese (BMI �40),dieting, taking medications that affect appetite, participatingin >150 minutes per week of vigorous physical activity,smoking, or drinking heavily (men: >14 alcoholic drinks perweek; women: >7 alcoholic drinks per week). Other

CORRECT

2 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof

exclusion criteria were self-reported disordered eating,depression, type 1 or 2 diabetes, adrenal disease, or untreatedthyroid disease. Severely obese subjects were excludedbecause binge eating is related to the degree of obesity29 andthe presence of a binge eating disorder may affect the results.The subjects were recruited from Texas Christian Univer-

sity (TCU) and the surrounding community. The study wasconducted in a metabolic kitchen at TCU. Before data collec-tion, each subject signed a consent document approved bythe TCU Institutional Review Board. The TCU InstitutionalReview Board has a Federalwide Assurance with the US Officeof Human Research Protection and has committed to complywith the requirements of the US Department of Health andHuman Services Protection of Human Subjects regulations.

Experiment DesignThe effect of eating condition (slow or fast eating speed) onmeal energy intake was assessed in normal-weight andoverweight/obese subjects using a randomized crossoverdesign. Following screening, each subject reported to themetabolic kitchen on 2 study days for lunch. The 2 study dayswere separated by a washout period of at least 4 days(13.8�12.6 days). On the first study day, subjects wererandomly assigned to consume a meal at a fast or slow eatingspeed. On the subsequent study day, subjects consumed thesame meal but at the alternative eating speed.

Test MealThe test meal during each study day was a mixed meal ofvegetable pasta. The mixed meal was made with ditalinipasta, tomatoes, olive oil, green onions, garlic, parsley, basil,sugar, salt, pepper, and Parmesan cheese. Women wereserved 900 g and men were served 1,200 g mixed meal cor-responding to 1,300 kcal and 1,734 kcal, respectively, duringeach study day. The meal contained 50.5% energy from car-bohydrate, 38.6% energy from fat, 12.2% energy from protein,and 330 mg sodium per 100 g. The subjects were also given355 mL (12 fl oz) water with each meal and allowed morewater, ad libitum, if they wished.

Study ProtocolEach subject consumed the meal at the same time on the 2study days. For menstruating women, each study day wasscheduled in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle.Subjects were instructed to keep their food/drink intake

and physical activity levels the same on the day before thestudy days and the morning of the study days. During thestudy days, subjects were instructed to consume the samebreakfast at least 4 hours before lunch and not to consumeany food or drink other than water between breakfast andlunch and any water an hour before lunch. To verify that theabove conditions were met, subjects were asked to keep afood record the day before the study days and the morning ofthe study days as well as recall their physical activity levelsduring that time.During the fast eating condition the subjects were asked to

consume their meal as quickly as possible without feelinguncomfortable. To speed their eating they were instructed toeat as if they had a time constraint, take large bites, chewquickly, and refrain from pausing and putting the spoondown between bites. During the slow eating condition, they

ED PROOF

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

� 24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 3: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

Table 1. Demographic and behavioral characteristics of participants in a study to compare the effect of eating speed on energyintake in normal-weight and overweight/obese subjects

VariableNormal weight(n[35)

Overweight/obese(n[35) P valuea

Body mass index 23.9�2.6 31.3�4.6 <0.0001

Body fat (%)

Male 16.5�4.5 24.4�3.5 <0.0001

Female 23.5�5.4 34.5�5.9 <0.0001

Waist circumference (cm)

Male 88.1�6.8 109.1�7.1 <0.0001

Female 79.2�5.5 100.1�14.8 <0.0001

Age (y) 33.3�12.5 44.1�13.0 0.001

Sex (%) 0.06

Female 40 63

Male 60 37

Ethnicity (%) 0.39

Hispanic or Latino 6 11

Non-Hispanic 94 89

Race (%) 0.71

Black 6 11

Asian 14 9

White 71 74

Mixed race 9 6

Self-reported eating rate (%) 0.54

Very slow 0 0

Relatively slow 11 9

Medium 46 34

Fast 37 54

Very fast 6 3

Eating attitudesb 4.3�3.1 4.8�2.4 0.49

Depressionc 27.4�7.0 30.2�5.8 0.07

Three Factor Eating Questionnaired

Dietary restraint score 10.7�3.0 9.6�3.5 0.16

Disinhibition score 6.9�2.6 7.4�2.6 0.40

Hunger score 5.9�3.1 6.2�2.5 0.70

aThe differences in subject characteristics by weight status were analyzed by independent t test (2-tailed) for continuous variables and by c2 test for categorical variables.bDisordered eating was assessed using the eating attitudes test by Garner and colleagues32,33 and a score >20 on the test may indicate disordered eating.cDepression was assessed by the self-rating depression scale by Zung.31 A higher score indicates a greater level of depression and the maximum possible score is 80. Most individuals withdepression score between 50 and 69.dDietary restraint, disinhibition, and hunger were assessed using the Three-Factor Eating questionnaire by Stunkard and Messick.30

RESEARCH

CORRECTED PROOF

were instructed to eat as though they had no time constraint,take small bites, chew each bite thoroughly, and pause andput the spoon down between bites. Subjects were not givenspecific instructions during the two eating conditions such asthe precise size of each bite, the bite rate, or the number ofchews per bite to keep the eating experience as natural aspossible. During both eating conditions, subjects were asked

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof �

to eat as much food as they wished until comfortablysatisfied.To minimize the effect of the environment on food

intake, subjects were served the pasta in the same sizewhite bowls and were given the same spoon during eacheating condition. They were also seated in their own areaaway from the other subjects and not facing anyone.

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 3

24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 4: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

Subjects were also not allowed to communicate with eachother while eating.Subjects were inconspicuously observed by the in-

vestigators and encouraged if necessary to ensure that thestudy conditions were being followed. The time spent eatingwas assessed using a stopwatch and the amount of food andwater consumed was measured to the nearest 0.5 g using adigital scale (FD Series, OHAUS Corporation).Many participants said they enjoyed the meals and a few

requested the recipe for the meal. One participant, how-ever, would have preferred a spicier meal. Three partici-pants expressed feeling slightly uncomfortable after thefast meal condition despite being instructed to eat untilthey were comfortably full. The participants did not havequestions regarding the two conditions, possibly becausethey were given detailed instructions by the investigators.In addition, many participants were aware of the two mealconditions from previous participants before the study wasexplained to them by the study investigators. One partici-pant asked whether reading or telephone use was allowedduring the slow eating condition and was instructed tofocus only on the eating. The eating condition may haveinfluenced eating behavior beyond changing the speed ofeating. Eating condition affected water intake (see theResults section).

MeasuresEnergy Intake, Rate of Energy Intake, and EnergyDensity during the Eating Conditions. Ad libitum energyintake during the slow and fast eating conditions wascalculated from the weight (in grams) of the mixed mealconsumed during the respective eating conditions and theenergy content of the meal per 100 g. The rate of energyintake was assessed by dividing the energy intake in ki-localories during the two eating conditions by therespective eating duration in minutes. Energy density wascalculated by dividing energy intake during the two eatingconditions by the respective total grams of food and waterconsumed.

Health Questionnaires. All subjects completed a de-mographic, lifestyle, and health questionnaire that includedquestions on age, sex, ethnicity, race, and self-reportedeating rate. The question on the self-eating rate questionwas adopted from the study by Sasaki and colleagues.7

Subjects also completed three questionnaires, includingthe three-factor eating questionnaire by Stunkard andMessick30 to measure dietary restraint, hunger, and disin-hibition; the self-rating depression scale by Zung31; and theeating attitudes test to assess disordered eating by Garnerand colleagues.32,33 A higher score on the self-ratingdepression scale indicates a higher level of depressionwith a maximum possible score of 80. Most individuals withdepression score between 50 and 69 on the self-ratingdepression scale. A score >20 on the eating attitudes testmay indicate disordered eating.

Anthropometric Measurements. Height was measured tothe nearest millimeter without shoes using a Seca wallmounted stadiometer (model 222). Body weight wasmeasured in light clothing and without shoes to the nearest

CORRECT

4 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof

0.1 kg using Befour scales (model FS-0961). Measured heightand weight were used to calculate BMI. Waist and hip cir-cumferences were measured in duplicate to the nearest 0.25cm using Sammons Preston Gulick cloth tape measure withspring loading (model 5193). Skinfold thickness wasmeasured in duplicate at the abdominal, triceps, chest, mid-axillary, subscapular, suprailiac, and thigh sites to the nearest1 mm using the Lafayette Skinfold II Calipers (model 01128).Body density was calculated from the sum of seven skinfoldmeasurements,34 and percent body fat was calculated frombody density.35

Assessment of Food Intake and Physical Activity Priorto the Meals. Subjects were instructed to record all thefood and drink consumed on the day before the study daysand the morning of the study days. The food records wereanalyzed for nutrient content using the Food Processor SQLsoftware program (ESHA Research Inc). Physical activitylevel was also assessed during the same time period using amodified version of the validated 7-day Physical ActivityRecall.36

Assessment of Hunger, Fullness, Desire to Eat, Thirst,and Palatability. During both eating conditions, perceivedhunger, fullness, desire to eat, and thirst were assessed usinga validated 100 mm visual analog scale (VAS).37 The mea-surements were taken immediately before the meal wasserved and periodically after the meal began in both eatingconditions. The measurements from when the meal beganwere taken at 5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, 45, and 60 minutes. Thepalatability of the meal was also assessed, using VAS, at 1minute after the meal began and at meal completion. Eachscale was labeled with opposite evaluative labels (“Not at all”and “Extremely”) at either end of the scale.

Statistical Analyses and Power CalculationA mixed-model repeated measures analysis was used toexamine the effect of eating condition (slow and fast), weightstatus (normal-weight and overweight/obese), and eatingcondition by weight status interaction on meal duration,weight of food consumed, energy intake, water intake,combined weight of food and water consumed, energy den-sity, rate of energy intake, and palatability rating of the meal(average of the rating at 1 minute and at meal completion).The differences in these variables by eating condition andweight status were assessed by least squares means. Theabove analyses were also performed with age and sexadjustment. In addition, the above analyses were performedby age tertile.A mixed-model repeated measures analysis was also used

to assess the effect of the eating condition, weight status, andmeal stage (before the meal and 5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, 45, and60 minutes after the meal began) and the interaction be-tween these factors on hunger, fullness, desire to eat, andthirst. The differences in these variables by meal stage, eatingcondition, and weight status were analyzed by least squaresmeans. The data were analyzed using SAS software, version9.2 (2008, SAS Institute Inc).The sample size of 35 normal-weight and 35 overweight/

obese subjects had 95% power to detect an expected differ-ence of 70 kcal14 between the slow and fast eating condition

ED PROOF

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

� 24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 5: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

E

within each group while assuming a<.05 and a standarddeviation of the difference for repeated measures analysis of110 kcal.14,38

RESULTSSubject CharacteristicsTable 1 shows the subject characteristics. BMI, percent bodyfat, waist circumference, and age were significantly higher inthe overweight/obese compared with the normal-weightsubjects. There were more men in the normal-weight groupthan in the overweight/obese group but this difference wasnot statistically significant. In both groups, about 90% of thesubjects were of non-Hispanic origin and nearly 30% wereminorities. There was no difference in ethnicity and race byweight status. Most subjects in both groups reported theireating speed to be either “medium” or “fast” and there wasno difference by weight status. The scores for eating attitudesand depression did not indicate disordered eating ordepression and were not different by weight status. Dietaryrestraint, disinhibition, and hunger levels were also similar byweight status.

Energy Intake and Physical Activity Levels Beforethe Study DaysEnergy intake and physical activity levels on the day beforethe study days and in the morning of the study days were notdifferent by eating condition. The data are not shown here.

Palatability of the MealThe average palatability rating (there was no difference be-tween the rating at 1 minute and at meal completion) washigh and not different between the slow and fast eatingconditions in the normal-weight subjects (80.8�14.0 mm vs80.6�13.9 mm, respectively; P¼0.92) or overweight/obesesubjects (83.1�14.6 mm vs 82.8�14.6 mm, respectively;CT

Table 2. The effect of eating speed on meal duration; food, energin normal-weight and overweight/obese subjects

Normal Weigh

Eating Cond

Slow F

�������������

Meal duration (min) 21.9�7.0Weight of food consumed (g) 556.9�303.8 6

Energy intake (kcal) 804.5�438.9 8

Weight of water intake (g) 324.0�199.5 2

Weight of food and water intake (g) 880.9�403.4 8

Energy density (kcal/g of food and water) 0.91�0.21Rate of energy intake (kcal/min) 38.7�22.1 1

aA mixed-design repeated measures analysis was used to examine the effect of eating conditiondensity; and rate of energy intake.*P<0.05 for the difference in the outcome variables between the slow and fast eating conditi**P<0.01 between the slow and fast eating condition within the normal-weight and overweig***P<0.0001 for the difference in the outcome variables between the slow and fast eating cond

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof �

CORRE

P¼0.86). There was no eating condition by weight statusinteraction effect on the mean palatability rating of the meal(P¼0.96).

Meal Duration, Food, Energy, and Water Intake,Energy Density, and Rate of Energy IntakeData on meal duration, food, energy, and water intake, energydensity, and rate of energy intake are presented in Table 2.There was no eating condition by weight status interactioneffect for any of the above variables. Food and energy intakewere lower during the slow compared with the fast eatingcondition in both the groups, but the differences were onlystatistically significant in the normal-weight subjects(P¼0.04) and not the overweight/obese subjects (P¼0.18).Both groups had significantly lower energy density (P¼0.005and P¼0.001, respectively) and rate of energy intake(P<0.0001 in both groups), and higher meal duration(P<0.0001 in both groups) and water intake (P¼0.02 andP¼0.003, respectively), during the slow compared with thefast eating condition. The combined weight of the food andwater consumed was not different by eating condition ineither the normal weight (P¼0.85) or overweight/obese(P¼0.21) group. None of the above trends by eating speed inthe different outcome variables changed after adjustment forage. The trends were also the same after adjustment for sex.Only the unadjusted data have been shown.In addition, the trends in the above outcome variables were

the same by age tertile (19 to 29 years, 30 to 48 years, and 49to 65 years). Mean eating duration was significantly higher(P<0.0001) during the slow compared with the fast eatingcondition across the different age groups (effect size: 12.6,13.1, and 12.5 minutes, respectively). Food and energy intakewere lower during the slow vs the fast eating condition in thethree age groups (food intake: 58.9, 55.9, and 36.2 g,respectively; energy intake: 84.9, 80.5, and 52.1 kcal,respectively), but the differences were significant (P¼0.02)

D PROOF

y, and water intake; energy density; and rate of energy intake

t (n[35) Overweight/Obese (n[35)

P valueaition Eating Condition

ast Slow Fast

�mean�standard deviation

��������������!9.4�4.1*** 21.9�10.1 9.0�4.5*** 0.80

17.9�228.6* 461.9�210.5 501.7�246.1 0.62

92.6�330.2* 667.3�304.1 724.8�355.5 0.62

55.8�121.7* 358.1�188.4 270.1�128.0** 0.63

73.7�302.2 820.0�319.8 771.8�294.8 0.45

1.02�0.16** 0.81�0.21 0.93�0.25** 0.72

02.2�39.4*** 34.0�17.7 91.8�49.3*** 0.55

by weight status interaction effect on meal duration; food, energy, and water intake; energy

on within the normal-weight and overweight groups, assessed by least squares means.ht/obese groups.ition within the normal-weight and overweight groups, assessed by least squares means.

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 5

24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 6: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

only in the middle tertile for age. Water intake was higherin the slow compared with the fast eating condition acrossthe three age groups (102.4, 72.0, and 60.2 g, respectively),but the difference was significant only in the first (P¼0.02)and second (P¼0.04) age tertiles. The total amount of foodand water consumed was not different (P�0.41) betweenthe slow and fast eating conditions across the three agegroups. Both energy density (P<0.05) and rate of energyintake (P<0.0001) were significantly lower in the slowcompared with the fast eating condition in the three agegroups (energy density: 0.13, 0.09, and 0.12 kcal/g food andwater consumed, respectively; rate of energy intake: 72.9,64.9, and 43.9 kcal/min, respectively). Some of the subgroupdifferences above by eating condition did not reach statisticalsignificance possibly because the analyses by age tertile werenot adequately powered.

A

C

Figure. Changes in postprandial hunger (A), fullness (B), desire toconditions in 35 normal-weight (NW) and 35 overweight/obese (OThere was no eating condition by weight status interaction effect fmodel repeated measures analysis. In both the NW and OW/O g(P¼0.001 or less) for hunger, fullness, desire to eat, and thirst. Hunge(P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15 (P<0.0001), and 60 (P¼0.01) minu(P¼0.0001), 20 (P¼0.003), and 60 (P¼0.03) minutes in the OW/O greating condition were different at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15 (Pthe NW group and at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15 (P¼0.0001)ratings (C) by eating condition were different at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P(P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15 (P<0.0001), 20 (P¼0.0003), and 25 (Pcondition were different at 10 (P¼0.02), 45 (P¼0.03), and 60 (P¼0OW/O group.

6 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof

CORRECT

Hunger, Fullness, Desire to Eat, and ThirstData on hunger, fullness, desire to eat, and thirst are pre-sented in the Figure. There was no eating condition by weightstatus interaction effect for hunger (P¼0.13), fullness(P¼0.11), desire to eat (P¼0.13), and thirst (P¼0.77). In boththe normal-weight and overweight/obese groups, there was asignificant eating condition by time effect (P¼0.001 or less)for hunger, fullness, and desire to eat, and thirst; that is, theVAS measures were influenced by the interaction betweeneating condition and time.The normal-weight group reported significantly higher

ratings of hunger at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), and 15(P<0.0001) minutes but lower ratings at 60 minutes (P¼0.01)after the start of the meal in the slow compared with the fasteating condition. The overweight/obese group reportedsignificantly higher ratings of hunger at 5 (P<0.0001), 10

B

D

eat (C), and thirst (D) ratings during the slow and fast eatingW/O) subjects. The values are means and standard deviations.or any of the appetite or thirst ratings as evaluated by a mixed-roups, there was a significant eating condition by time effectr ratings (A) by eating condition were significantly different at 5tes in the NW group and at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15oup as assessed by least squares means. Fullness ratings (B) by<0.0001), 30 (P¼0.04), 45 (P¼0.01), and 60 (P¼0.02) minutes in, and 20 (P¼0.003) minutes in the OW/O group. Desire to eat<0.0001), and 15 (P<0.0001) minutes in the NW group and at 5¼0.04) minutes in the OW/O group. Thirst ratings (D) by eating.04) minutes in the NW group at 10 minutes (P¼0.008) in the

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

� 24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

ED PROOF

Page 7: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

(P<0.0001), 15 (P¼0.0001), and 20 (P¼0.003) minutes andlower ratings at 60 minutes (P¼0.03) in the slow comparedwith the fast condition. Ratings of fullness were significantlylower at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), and 15 (P<0.0001)minutes and higher at 30 (P¼0.04), 45 (P¼0.01), and 60(P¼0.02) minutes in the normal-weight group in the slowcompared with the fast eating condition. The overweight/obese group reported significantly lower ratings of fullness at5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), 15 (P¼0.0001), and 20(P¼0.003) minutes during the slow compared with the fasteating condition. Desire to eat ratings were significantlyhigher in the slow compared with the fast eating condition at5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001), and 15 (P<0.0001) minutes inthe normal-weight group and at 5 (P<0.0001), 10 (P<0.0001),15 (P<0.0001), 20 (P¼0.0003), and 25 (P¼0.04) minutes inthe overweight/obese group. Thirst ratings were significantlyhigher at 10 minutes (P¼0.02) and lower at 45 (P¼0.03) and60 (P¼0.04) minutes in the slow compared with the fastcondition in the normal-weight group and higher at 10 mi-nutes (P¼0.008) during the slow condition in the overweight/obese group.

E

DISCUSSIONCompared with the fast eating condition, the slow eatingcondition led to a reduction in energy intake by 10% (88 kcal)in the normal-weight and 8% (58 kcal) in the overweight/obese subjects. The difference in the overweight/obese groupwas smaller and not statistically significant. Among thestudies that examined results by weight status, Li and col-leagues15 reported lower energy intake (11.9%) during theslow compared with the fast eating condition in both thenormal-weight and obese groups, whereas Kaplan20 andSpeigel and colleagues21 found no difference in energy intake(amounts not given) by eating rate in either group. Amongthe studies that did not compare the results by weight status,Andrade and colleagues14 reported a reduction of 67 kcal inenergy intake during the slow compared with the fast eatingcondition and Scisco and colleagues16 observed a reduction of71 kcal when participants were asked to slow their bite rateby 50%. Martin and colleagues,18 who asked subjects toreduce their eating rate by 50%, observed a reduction of 102kcal in men but only 3 kcal in women during the slowcompared with the fast eating condition. Zijlstra and col-leagues25 reported significantly lower food intake (52%) insubjects in the lowest quartile of eating rate compared withsubjects in the highest quartile of eating rate. Smit and col-leagues17 examined the effect of different chewing counts (35and 10 chews per mouthful) on ad libitum energy intake andfound that the higher chewing count increased the durationof eating but reduced energy intake by 12.6% compared withthe lower chewing count. In addition, Forde and colleagues24

examined the effect of texture (mashed vs whole) and taste(savory vs standard) on ad libitum food intake at a meal andreported that eating rate was lower in the whole comparedwith the mashed meal condition irrespective of taste and thewhole condition led to less food intake (91 g) than themashed condition only during the savory meal.Slowing the speed of eating in our study led to a significant

reduction in energy intake in the normal-weight but not theoverweight/obese group, probably because the latter groupconsumed less food during both the eating conditions

CORRECT

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof �

compared with the normal-weight subjects. It is possible thatthe overweight/obese subjects were more self-conscious and,thus, ate less during the study. In a study of social influenceson meal size, overweight adults purchased less food, whereasnormal-weight adults purchased more food for lunch whenaccompanied by others compared with when alone.39 Salvyand colleagues40 reported similar findings in children. Over-weight children consumed less energy, whereas normal-weight children consumed more energy when they werewith their peers compared with when they were alone.The rate of energy intake in our study was lowered by the

same amount (62% to 63%) during the slow compared withthe fast eating condition in both groups, implying thatreducing the speed of eating has the same effect on energyconsumed per minute irrespective of weight status. Andradeand colleagues14 reported a reduction of 75% in the rate ofenergy intake during the slow compared with the fast eatingcondition. Smit and colleagues17 reported a 53% reduction iningestion rate during the prolonged chewing conditioncompared with the less prolonged chewing condition.Water consumption was higher during the slow compared

with the fast eating condition by 27% (68 g) in the normalweight and 33% (88 g) in the overweight/obese group. Waterintake was higher during the slow eating condition probablybecause the subjects were asked to slow their speed ofeating during that phase. The total weight of food and waterconsumed was not different between the two eating condi-tions in either the normal weight or overweight/obese group.Bell and colleagues41 have shown that subjects consume thesame weight of food irrespective of the energy density of themeal. Among the studies on eating speed and energy intakein which the data were examined by weight status, waterintake was either not reported15,20 or found to be notdifferent by eating speed.21 Among studies that did notexamine the results by weight status, two studies16,18 foundno difference in water intake by eating rate, threestudies17,24,25 controlled water intake across the differenteating conditions, and Andrade and colleagues14 reported ahigher water intake (120 g) during the slow compared withthe fast condition.Energy density was lower in the slow compared with the

fast eating condition by 0.11 kcal/g in the normal-weight and0.12 kcal/g in the overweight/obese group. These changes canbe attributed to the greater amount of water and lesseramount of food and energy consumed in the slow eatingcondition in both groups. Andrade and colleagues14 found adecrease in energy density of 0.2 kcal/g during the slowcompared with the fast condition. Andrade and colleagues14

observed a higher difference than that observed in ourstudy, possibly because the difference in water intake byeating conditions was higher in their study than in our study.Energy density may be related to energy intake. Bell andcolleagues41 have reported that ad libitum intake in normal-weight subjects was higher when given high-energy densefoods compared with lower-energy dense foods of similarpalatability. In another study, Bell and Rolls42 have shownthat lowering energy density during a meal lowers ad libitumenergy intake in both normal weight and obese subjects.In both the groups, ratings of hunger were lower at 60

minutes from the beginning of the meal during the slowcompared with the fast eating condition. The normal-weightsubjects also reported feeling more full at 60 minutes during

D PROOF

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 7

24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 8: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

the slow condition. These results indicate that greater satietycould be expected from a meal that is consumed more slowlyacross both groups. Andrade and colleagues14 reported thatupon meal completion, despite lower energy intake, satietyratings were significantly higher under the slow eating con-dition compared with the fast eating condition. Zanadian andcolleagues19 also reported a higher level of satiety wheneating rate was decreased but only in subjects who typicallyate at a constant rate. Other investigators, including re-searchers who evaluated the data by weight status,15 foundno difference in fullness or satiety ratings by eating orchewing rate15-18 or observed lower levels of satiety withpauses within a meal compared with no pauses within thesame meal.23 If the increased satiety observed in our studyduring the slow eating condition could be maintained beyond60 minutes and prevent a compensatory increase in energyintake at the subsequent meal is unknown and remains to beevaluated.The lower energy intake during the slow eating condition

in our study may be due to the greater consumption of waterduring the slow meal. This may have caused more stomachdistention during the slow meal. Statistically adjusting forwater intake did not change the effect of eating speed onenergy intake in either group, suggesting that other mecha-nisms may be more important. The slow eating conditionmay also have allowed subjects to eat more mindfully andbetter sense their feelings of hunger suppression andsatiety.43 Another mechanism may be that foods that areeaten slowly remain in the oral cavity for a longer period oftime and lead to increased orosensory exposure that may berelated to lower food intake.4,5 A high eating rate, on theother hand, impairs the congruent relationship between thesensory signals and the metabolic processes that determinehow much we eat.4,5 Wijlens and colleagues44 examined theeffect of oral (1 or 8 minutes modified sham feeding) andgastric (100 or 800 mL intragastrically infused liquid) stim-ulation on ad libitum energy intake and found that the longer(8 minutes) but not the shorter (1 minute) oral exposure witheither the 100 or 800 mL gastric stimulation led to a signif-icant decrease in energy intake compared with the controlcondition without any oral or gastric stimulation.Eating speed may be associated with different responses in

the anorexigenic (peptide YY, glucagon-like peptide-1, andcholecystokinin) and orexigenic (ghrelin) gut hormones.Kokkinos and colleagues45 reported higher peptide YY andglucagon-like peptide-1 response following consumption of atest meal in 30 minutes vs 5 minutes. Li and colleagues15

reported lower ghrelin and higher glucagon-like peptide-1and cholecystokinin concentrations, and Zhu and col-leagues46 reported a higher concentration of cholecystokininfollowing consumption of a meal with more vs less chews perbite. In all the above studies, the hormone differences byeating speed, however, occurred at 30, 60, or 90 minutes afterthe meal began and could not have played a role in theamount of food consumed during that meal. Zhu and col-leagues46 also examined future food consumption and foundthat the changes in the gut hormones did not affect foodintake at the subsequent meal.A limitation of this study is that the meals were consumed

in a metabolic kitchen rather than in a natural setting. Tomake the metabolic laboratory more appealing, we laid eachtable with attractive place settings and flowers and played

CORRECT

8 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof

classical music. Nevertheless, even an aesthetic laboratorysetting cannot substitute for a natural setting. In addition, theslow and fast eating conditions were engineered; that is,subjects were instructed on how to slow or speed their eatingrate. A more natural approach would be to manipulate theeating speed by choosing foods based on their oral processingcharacteristics. Forde and colleagues47 and Viskaal-VanDongen and colleagues48 have determined the eating rateof a variety of foods and have found that it varies widelyacross foods. Based on this, Forde and colleagues24 designedmeals with different eating rates and demonstrated adecrease in both eating rate and energy intake. Another issuewith conducting this research in a laboratory setting is thateating rates in a non-natural setting may not be the same aseating rates in a free-living situation. Melanson and col-leagues13 have reported that ad libitum lunches in a labora-tory setting tend to be consumed at a faster rate than homebreakfasts or free-living dinners.Our study also did not assess prospective food intake, so it

is not known whether eating less food or feeling less hungryat 60 minutes during the slow eating condition would affectfood consumption later on. The study also did not include aneating condition with the usual eating rate, so it is notpossible to assess how changing a subject’s usual eating ratewould change energy intake. Another limitation is that bitesize and chew rate were not assessed and the subjects werenot screened for having normal dentition and eating andchewing abilities. All of these factors could potentially affectthe findings. The crossover design may mitigate the effect ofthese potential confounders on energy intake. Our study alsocannot distinguish the roles of taking smaller bites, chewingmore thoroughly, and pausing between bites on energyintake because the subjects were asked to engage in all threeactivities simultaneously to reduce their eating speed. Fordeand colleagues47 studied the relationship between oral pro-cessing characteristics of solid savory meal components andexpected satiation and found that foods that were consumedin smaller bites were chewed more and for longer and ex-pected to result in higher satiation. The overweight/obesegroup in our study was older than the normal-weight group.Adjusting the data for age did not change the results. Inaddition, similar trends in energy intake by eating conditionwere observed in the different age groups.Strengths of this study include the fact that it was a ran-

domized, crossover study. Our study also compared the samenumber of normal-weight and overweight/obese subjects. Inaddition, our subjects included approximately the samenumber ofmen andwomen andnearly 30%minorities,makingthe results more generalizable than most of the previousstudies. We also minimized the effect of eating with others onenergy intake49,50 by seating each subject in a private area. Inaddition, physical activity levels and energy intake before thestudy were controlled, minimizing any confounding effect ofthe prior energy intake and expenditure.Whether the results from this study, conducted in a labo-

ratory setting, can be reproduced in a more natural settingremains to be tested. Some research has already been con-ducted on how to reduce the speed of eating and food intakein a more natural way using different foods with differenttextures, as mentioned above.24,47,48 It is also important toexamine whether slowing eating speed over the long termwill lead to weight loss. Ford and colleagues51 used a

ED PROOF

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

� 24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 9: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

E

computerized device, Mandometer (AB Mando), to providereal-time feedback to obese adolescents to slow down thespeed of eating during meals. They reported that using theMandometer as well as lifestyle counseling led to a lower BMIat 12 months compared with just lifestyle counseling. Using aMandometer, however, creates an unnatural environmentand it is not clear whether the device use will be maintained.Further studies are needed to examine if reducing the speedof eating more naturally leads to weight loss in the long term.

CONCLUSIONSConsuming a meal at a slower vs faster speed resulted in asignificant reduction in food and energy intake in normal-weight but not in overweight/obese subjects. Energy den-sity and rate of energy intake were significantly lower andwater intake was significantly higher in the slow comparedwith fast eating condition in both groups. At 60 minutes afterthe start of the meal, normal-weight subjects reported higherratings of fullness and less hunger and overweight/obesesubjects reported less hunger in the slow compared with thefast eating condition. There was no eating speed by weightstatus interaction for any of the variables. Further research isneeded to examine the influence of eating slowly on energyintake by weight status in a more natural setting.

References1. Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Ogden CL, Johnson CL. Prevalence and

trends in obesity among US adults, 1999-2000. JAMA. 2002;288(14):1723-1727.

2. Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Ogden CL. Prevalence of obesity andtrends in the distribution of body mass index among US adults,1999-2010. JAMA. 2012;307(5):491-497.

3. Kant AK, Graubard BI. Secular trends in patterns of self-reported foodconsumption of adult Americans: NHANES 1971-1975 to NHANES1999-2002. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84(5):1215-1223.

4. de Graaf C, Kok FJ. Slow food, fast food and the control of food intake.Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2010;6:290-293.

5. de Graaf C. Balancing intake and output: food v. exercise. Why liquidenergy results in overconsumption. Proc Nutr Soc. 2011;70:162-170.

6. Takayama S, Akamine Y, Okabe T, et al. Rate of eating and bodyweight in patients with type 2 diabetes or hyperlipidaemia. J Int MedRes. 2002;30(4):442-444.

7. Sasaki S, Katagiri A, Tsuji T, Shimoda T, Amano K. Self-reported rateof eating correlates with body mass index in 18-y-old Japanesewomen. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2003;27(11):1405-1410.

8. Otsuka R, Tamakoshi K, Yatsuya H, et al. Eating fast leads to obesity:Findings based on self-administered questionnaires among middle-aged Japanese men and women. J Epidemiol. 2006;16(3):117-124.

9. Maruyama K, Sato S, Ohira T, et al. The joint impact on being over-weight of self-reported behaviours of eating quickly and eating untilfull: Cross-sectional survey. BMJ. 2008;337:a2002.

10. Otsuka R, Tamakoshi K, Yatsuya H, et al. Eating fast leads to insulinresistance: Findings in middle-aged Japanese men and women. PrevMed. 2008;46(2):154-159.

11. Xiong LH, Wang CL, Chen ZQ, et al. Study on food preference anddietary behavior to overweight/obesity in school children and ado-lescents in Guangzhou: A case-control study. Zhonghua Liu Xing BingXue Za Zhi. 2008;29(10):965-969.

12. Tanihara S, Imatoh T, Miyazaki M, et al. Retrospective longitudinalstudy on the relationship between 8-year weight change and currenteating speed. Appetite. 2011;57(1):179-183.

13. Melanson KJ, Greene GW, Petty AJ. Laboratory and free-living eatingrates in young adults reporting to be slow, medium, or fast pacedeaters. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2011;19(suppl):S101.

14. Andrade AM, Greene GW, Melanson KJ. Eating slowly led to de-creases in energy intake within meals in healthy women. J Am DietAssoc. 2008;108(7):1186-1191.

CORRECT

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof �

15. Li J, Zhang N, Hu L, et al. Improvement in chewing activity reducesenergy intake in one meal and modulates plasma gut hormoneconcentrations in obese and lean young Chinese men. Am J Clin Nutr.2011;94(3):709-716.

16. Scisco JL, Muth ER, Dong Y, Hoover AW. Slowing bite-rate reducesenergy intake: An application of the bite counter device. J Am DietAssoc. 2011;111(8):1231-1235.

17. Smit HJ, Kemsley EK, Tapp HS, Henry CJK. Does prolonged chewingreduce food intake? Fletcherism revisited. Appetite. 2011;57:295-298.

18. Martin CK, Anton SD, Walden H, Arnett C, Greenway FL,Williamson DA. Slower eating rate reduces the food intake of men,but not women: Implications for behavioral weight control. BehavRes Ther. 2007;45(10):2349-2359.

19. Zandian M, Ioakimidis I, Bergh C, Brodin U, Sodersten P. Deceleratedand linear eaters: Effect of eating rate on food intake and satiety.Physiol Behav. 2009;96(2):270-275.

20. Kaplan DL. Eating style of obese and nonobese males. PsychosomMed. 1980;42(6):529-538.

21. Spiegel TA, Kaplan JM, Tomassini A, Stellar E. Bite size, ingestionrate, and meal size in lean and obese women. Appetite. 1993;21(2):131-145.

22. Ebbeling CB, Garcia-Lago E, Leidig MM, Seger-Shippee LG,Feldman HA, Ludwig DS. Altering portion sizes and eating rate toattenuate gorging during a fast food meal: Effects on energy intake.Pediatrics. 2007;119(5):869-875.

23. Yeomans MR, Gray RW, Mitchell CJ, True S. Independent effects ofpalatability and within-meal pauses on intake and appetite ratings inhuman volunteers. Appetite. 1997;29(1):61-76.

24. Forde CG, van Kuijk N, Thaler T, de Graaf C, Martin N. Texture andsavoury taste influences on food intake in a realistic hot lunch timemeal. Appetite. 2013;60(1):180-186.

25. Zijlstra N, Mars M, Stafleu A, de Graaf C. The effect of texturedifferences on satiation in 3 pairs of solid foods. Appetite.2010;55(3):490-497.

26. Laessle RG, Lehrke S, Duckers S. Laboratory eating behavior inobesity. Appetite. 2007;49(2):399-404.

27. Mechanick JI, Kushner RF, Sugerman HJ, et al. American Associationof Clinical Endocrinologists, The Obesity Society, and American So-ciety for Metabolic & Bariatric Surgery medical guidelines for clinicalpractice for the perioperative nutritional, metabolic, and nonsurgicalsupport of the bariatric surgery patient. Obesity (Silver Spring).2009;17(suppl 1):S1-S70, v.

28. American College of Sports Medicine Guidelines for Exercise Testing andPrescription. 8th ed. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins;2010.

29. Zwaan MD. Binge eating disorder and obesity. Int J Obes.2001;25(suppl):S51-S55.

30. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire tomeasure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res.1985;29(1):71-83.

31. Zung WW. A Self-Rating Depression Scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry.1965;12:63-70.

32. Garner DM, Garfinkel PE. The Eating Attitudes Test: An index of thesymptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 1979;9(2):273-279.

33. Garner DM, Olmsted MP, Bohr Y, Garfinkel PE. The eating attitudestest: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychol Med.1982;12(4):871-878.

34. Jackson AS, Pollock ML. Practical assessment of body composition.Physician Sport Med. 1985;13(5):76-90.

35. Heyward VH, Stolarczyk LM. Applied Body Composition Assessment.Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 1996.

36. Blair SN, Haskell WL, Ho P, et al. Assessment of habitualphysical activity by a seven-day recall in a community surveyand controlled experiments. Am J Epidemiol. 1985;122(5):794-804.

37. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A. Reproducibility, power andvalidity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensa-tions in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord.2000;24(1):38-48.

38. Shah M, Schroeder R, Winn W, Adams-Huet B. A pilot study toinvestigate the effect of plate size on meal energy intake in normal

D PROOF

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS 9

24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce

Page 10: RESEARCH Original Research UNDER EMBARGO UNTIL …

RESEARCH

weight and overweight/obese women. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2011;24(6):612-615.

39. Krantz DS. A naturalistic study of social influences on meal sizeamong moderately obese and nonobese subjects. Psychosom Med.1979;41(1):19-27.

40. Salvy SJ, Coelho JS, Kieffer E, Epstein LH. Effects of social contexts onoverweight and normal-weight children’s food intake. Physiol Behav.2007;92(5):840-846.

41. Bell EA, Castellanos VH, Pelkman CL, Thorwart ML, Rolls BJ. Energydensity of foods affects energy intake in normal-weight women. Am JClin Nutr. 1998;67(3):412-420.

42. Bell EA, Rolls BJ. Energy density of foods affects energy intake acrossmultiple levels of fat content in lean and obese women. Am J ClinNutr. 2001;73(6):1010-1018.

43. Silverman J. The joy of eating mindfully: Chew, chew, chew. ConnMed. 2009;73(4):235.

44. Wijlens AG, Erkner A, Alexander E, Mars M, Smeets PA, de Graaf C.Effects of oral and gastric stimulation on appetite and energy intake.Obesity (Silver Spring). 2012;20(11):2226-2232.

45. Kokkinos A, le Roux CW, Alexiadou K, et al. Eating slowly increasesthe postprandial response of the anorexigenic gut hormones,

10 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF NUTRITION AND DIETETICS

FLA 5.2.0 DTD � JAND672_proof

COR

peptide YY and glucagon-like peptide-1. J Clin Endocrinol Metab.2010;95(1):333-337.

46. Zhu Y, Hsu WH, Hollis JH. Increasing the number of masticatorycycles is associated with reduced appetite and altered postprandialplasma concentrations of gut hormones, insulin and glucose. Br JNutr. 2013;110(2):384-390.

47. Forde CG, van Kuijk N, Thaler T, de Graaf C, Martin N. Oral processingcharacteristics of solid savory meal components, and relationshipwith food composition, sensory attributes and expected satiation.Appetite. 2013;60(1):208-219.

48. Viskaal-van Dongen M, Kok FJ, de Graaf C. Eating rate of commonlyconsumed foods promotes food and energy intake. Appetite.2011;56(1):25-31.

49. Hetherington MM, Anderson AS, Norton GN, Newson L. Situationaleffects on meal intake: A comparison of eating alone and eating withothers. Physiol Behav. 2006;88(4-5):498-505.

50. Koh J, Pliner P. The effects of degree of acquaintance, plate size, andsharing on food intake. Appetite. 2009;52(3):595-602.

51. Ford AL, Bergh C, Sodersten P, et al. Treatment of childhood obesityby retraining eating behaviour: Randomised controlled trial. BMJ.2010;340:b5388.

F

OO

AUTHOR INFORMATIONM. Shah and D. Rhea are professors, Department of Kinesiology, and L. Dart is an associate professor, Department of Nutritional Sciences, all atTexas Christian University, Fort Worth. J. Copeland is a nursing student, Texas A&M University, College Station; at the time of the study, she was agraduate student, Department of Kinesiology, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth. B. Adams-Huet is an assistant professor, Department ofClinical Sciences, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas. A. James is an exercise physiologist, Cardiology and InterventionalVascular Associates, Dallas, TX; at the time of the study, she was a graduate student, Department of Kinesiology, Texas Christian University, FortWorth.

Address correspondence to: Meena Shah, PhD, Department of Kinesiology, Texas Christian University, TCU Box 297730, Fort Worth, TX 76129.E-mail: [email protected]

STATEMENT OF POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTERESTNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

FUNDING/SUPPORTThis study was funded by Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTSThe authors thank Kate Brown, Brittany Chatley, Jennifer Crane, Samar Mashni, and Kristin Pelzel for helping with recruitment, data collection,and food preparation, as well as Liz Pettijohn for helping to recruit some of the subjects.

ECTED PR

R

-- 2013 Volume - Number -

� 24 December 2013 � 2:36 am � ce