running head: bifactor-esem and motivation 1similar esem representation of doctoral students’...

59
Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1 Using Bifactor-Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Test for a Continuum Structure of Motivation Joshua L. Howard and Marylène Gagné University of Western Australia Alexandre J.S. Morin Institute for Positive Psychology and Education, Australian Catholic University Jacques Forest Université du Québec à Montréal This is a prepublication version of a manuscript accepted on 23 March 2016 for publication in the Journal of Management. Citation: Howard, J., Gagné, M., Morin, A.J.S., Wang, Z.N., & Forest, J. (Accepted, 23 March 2016). Using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. Journal of Management. Acknowledgements: Preparation of this article was facilitated through funds from the Australian Postgraduate Award scholarship awarded to the first author, as well as grants from the Fonds Québécois de la Recherche sur la Société et la Culture and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada awarded to the second and fourth author, a grant from the Australian Research Council (LP140100100) awarded to the third author, and a grant from the Fondation de l’Ordre des CRHA awarded to the fourth author. Corresponding author: Joshua L. Howard, School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Hwy, Crawley 6009 WA, Australia. Email: [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 02-Oct-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1

Using Bifactor-Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Test for a Continuum

Structure of Motivation

Joshua L. Howard and Marylène Gagné

University of Western Australia

Alexandre J.S. Morin

Institute for Positive Psychology and Education, Australian Catholic University

Jacques Forest

Université du Québec à Montréal

This is a prepublication version of a manuscript accepted on 23 March 2016 for publication in the Journal of Management.

Citation: Howard, J., Gagné, M., Morin, A.J.S., Wang, Z.N., & Forest, J. (Accepted, 23 March 2016). Using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. Journal of Management.

Acknowledgements: Preparation of this article was facilitated through funds from the

Australian Postgraduate Award scholarship awarded to the first author, as well as grants

from the Fonds Québécois de la Recherche sur la Société et la Culture and the Social

Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada awarded to the second and fourth

author, a grant from the Australian Research Council (LP140100100) awarded to the third

author, and a grant from the Fondation de l’Ordre des CRHA awarded to the fourth author.

Corresponding author: Joshua L. Howard, School of Psychology, University of Western

Australia, 35 Stirling Hwy, Crawley 6009 WA, Australia.

Email: [email protected]

Page 2: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 2

Abstract

This paper explores the nature of workplace motivation by testing the continuum structure of

motivation proposed by self-determination theory (SDT) through the application of relatively

new and advanced methodological techniques. Specifically we demonstrate the usefulness of

the overarching bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling (B-ESEM) framework in

organizational psychology and discuss implications of such models over more traditional

confirmatory factor analyses. This framework is applied to responses obtained from 1124

Canadian employees who completed a multidimensional measure of workplace motivation.

The results support a continuum of self-regulation but furthermore indicate the importance of

accounting for both quality of motivation in addition to its global quantity. Indeed, the results

showed that specific types of motivation explained variance in covariates over and above the

variance already explained by the global quantity of self-determination. The current study

further demonstrates the limitation of the commonly used relative autonomy index and offers

alternate conceptualizations of human motivation.

Keywords: work motivation, self-determination theory, continuum, bifactor exploratory

structural equation modeling (B-ESEM)

Page 3: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 3

USING BIFACTOR-EXPLORATORY STRUCTURAL EQUATION

MODELING TO TEST FOR A CONTINUUM STRUCTURE OF MOTIVATION

Modern psychological science is progressing to a level of theoretical complexity which

necessitates the use of equally sophisticated methodological and statistical tools. This

observation calls for substantive-methodological synergy (Marsh & Hau, 2007). Substantive-

methodological synergies are joint ventures in which methodological advances are applied to

substantively important areas of research in order to help provide more precise, or refined,

answers to complex research questions. The current paper is anchored into such a

substantive-methodological synergy framework and aims to: (1) Test the continuum structure

of motivation proposed by self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985), and (2)

demonstrate the usefulness of the overarching bifactor exploratory structural equation

modeling (B-ESEM) framework in organizational psychology. We start this paper by

reviewing key substantive issues related to the SDT continuum hypothesis of motivation, and

then present in greater details the bifactor-ESEM psychometric framework.

SUBSTANTIVE ISSUES: THE CONTINUUM STRUCTURE OF MOTIVATION

Self-Determination Theory of Human Motivation

SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000) proposes that individuals

experience autonomous motivation when their reasons for engaging in behaviors are

volitional, and experience controlled motivation when their reasons for engaging in the

behaviors are pressured either internally or externally. Each of these two forms of motivation

can be characterized by different types of motivation, expected to form a continuum. At one

extreme, the most autonomous form of motivation is intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation

occurs when individuals derive a sense of enjoyment and satisfaction from the enactment of

the behavior itself. At the other extreme, the most controlled form of motivation is external

regulation, which occurs when individuals engage in an activity for purely instrumental

Page 4: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 4

reasons, such as to obtain rewards or avoid punishment. Between these extremes, introjected

regulation happens when a person engages in behavior to reduce negative self-related feelings

(e.g., shame, guilt), or to experience positive self-related feelings (e.g., pride), and identified

regulation occurs when the outcome of the behavior is personally meaningful. Introjected

regulation can be exemplified by employees who work late to maximize their performance

and feel better about themselves, whereas identified regulation would be exemplified by

employees who stay late to finish work that they perceive to be important to the organization.

In the work domain, external regulation can be further subdivided according to whether

the source of the external pressure to enact the target behavior is material or social (Gagné et

al., 2015). External material regulation stems from tangible rewards and punishments, such as

monetary benefits and job security. In contrast, external social regulation is related to social

rewards and punishments, such as approval and criticism from others. Although not initially

covered in SDT, others have noted that it is also important to assess amotivation (Pelletier,

Fortier, Vallerand, Tuson, Brière, & Blais, 1995; Vallerand, Pelletier, Blais, Brière, Senécal,

& Vallières, 1993), referring to an absence of willingness to exert effort, in order to cover

scenarios where people have no reason or willingness to put any effort into an activity.

Indeed, early SDT-based motivation instruments were limited in that they implicitly assumed

that everyone would have some reason to embark on a targeted course of action, failing to

explicitly assess lack of motivation. In the current study, we rely on the Multidimensional

Work Motivation Scale (MWMS; Gagné et al., 2015), a newly developed scale designed to

measure these distinct types of motivation in the work domain. Previous CFA has supported

the multidimensional structure of the MWMS in seven different languages (Gagné et al.,

2015). Here, we use a subsample from Gagné et al. (2015) and use bifactor-ESEM to test

SDT’s hypothesis that motivation types follow an underlying continuum.

Motivation Types on a Continuum

Page 5: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 5

A key aspect of SDT’s conceptualization of motivation is that motivation types are

ordered along a continuum depicting the degree of relative autonomy, or self-determination

(Ryan & Deci, 2000). SDT suggests that the qualitatively different motivation types also

differ quantitatively along a single continuum of self-determination. The continuum

hypothesis has typically been examined through an inspection of the correlations between the

motivation types (Ryan & Connell, 1989; see Guay, Morin, Litalien, Valois, & Vallerand,

2015 for a review in the education domain). For instance, Ryan and Connell (1989) tested

whether the correlations between motivation types followed a simplex structure. The simplex

structure refers to a correlation pattern showing that adjacent motivation types correlate more

strongly and positively with one another than more distal motivation types (which should

correlate negatively). For example, intrinsic motivation should correlate more strongly with

identified regulation than with introjected or external regulations. Although amotivation has

not traditionally been taken into account in tests of the SDT continuum, a case has been made

for this factor to fall at the lowest point of the continuum (Cox, Ullrich-French, Madonia,

&Witty, 2011; Guay, Ratelle, Roy, & Litalien, 2010; Stevenson & Lochbaum, 2008).

Some past research has supported the continuum hypothesis (e.g., Li, 1999; Li &

Harmer, 1996; Ryan & Connell, 1989), while other research has not, especially when using

more advanced statistical techniques (e.g., Chemolli & Gagné, 2014; Guay et al., 2015;

Wininger, 2007). For example, Chemolli and Gagné (2014) argued that if the continuum

hypothesis truly represented the structure of motivation, CFA should support the adequacy of

a single factor model, and loadings on this single factor would range from negative for the

least self-determined forms of motivation to positive for the most self-determined forms of

motivations. They used Rasch analysis, a stringent statistical test specifically developed to

evaluate continuum structures (Rasch, 1960), to test whether there was a continuum structure

underlying the items of the MWMS and the Academic Motivation Scale (Vallerand et al.,

Page 6: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 6

1992), and found no support for it. These results concur with past research that has

consistently supported multidimensional models over unidimensional ones (e.g., Gagné et al.,

2010, 2015; Li, 1999; Mallett, Kawabata, Newcombe, Otero-Forero, & Jackson, 2007).

A different test of this hypothesis was conducted by Guay et al. (2015), who

investigated the continuum assumption of SDT using exploratory structural equation

modeling (ESEM). Guay et al. (2015) further noted that in ESEM, the SDT continuum could

be expressed in two distinct and complementary manners. In line with previous studies, the

continuum hypothesis would be supported by the observation of the expected simplex pattern

at the level of factor correlations. Because ESEM tends to result in more exact estimates of

these factor correlations (see the methodological section below), the simplex pattern could be

expected to be clearer using ESEM than CFA. Moreover, support for the continuum

hypothesis could also come from the observation of larger cross-loadings between adjacent

subscales than between more theoretically distal subscales. Testing these propositions with

the Academic Motivation Scale (Vallerand et al., 1992), Guay et al. (2015) found that the

data fit the ESEM representation better than the CFA model, and that factor correlations were

more in line with the expected SDT continuum with ESEM than with CFA. However, even

though the simplex pattern was cleaner with ESEM, the results still showed many digressions,

and did not fully replicate across samples. Results also revealed cross-loadings somewhat in

line with the SDT continuum (i.e., stronger between adjacent factors), though they remained

generally small. Overall, these results partially supported the continuum hypothesis (for a

similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay,

& Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend these previous studies by combining into a

single bifactor-ESEM framework the tests conducted separately by Chemolli and Gagné

(2014) and Guay et al. (2015) to a new measure of work motivation (Gagné et al., 2015).

METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES: INTRODUCTION TO BIFACTOR-ESEM

Page 7: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 7

CFA has become the ubiquitous test of factor structures in psychological measurement.

Measures which fail to meet designated goodness-of-fit standards are deemed of little worth

in the eyes of researchers and reviewers alike. Despite this, psychological scales which

consistently meet these rather arbitrary benchmarks are few. This has caused many to

question the necessity of the Independent Cluster Model (ICM) constraints inherent in CFA,

in which cross-loadings between items and non-target factors are assumed to be exactly zero.

Undoubtedly, CFA has had a positive influence on psychological measurement by

encouraging researchers to develop more a priori, precise, and parsimonious models (e.g.,

Morin, Marsh, & Nagengast, 2013). Through its integration into the Structural Equation

Modeling (SEM) framework, CFA has provided ways to test how the data fits with a priori

expectations, to systematically investigate the degree to which a measurement or predictive

model is invariant across meaningful subgroups of participants, and to assess relations

between constructs corrected for measurement errors. These developments have been so

substantial that CFA has completely superseded traditional Exploratory Factor Analyses

(EFA) for all but the most preliminary tests of factor structure.

Over and above the intuitive appeal of clearly defined concepts, measured by a small

number of items perfectly designed to assess a single construct, has come a recent recognition

that the ideals pursued through a CFA approach are often impossible to achieve in applied

research (e.g., Marsh et al., 2009, 2010). Nowadays, many researchers recognize that the

ICM constraints inherent in CFA are oftentimes not appropriate given the nature of the data

(for a review, see Marsh, Morin, Parker, & Kaur, 2014). Specifically, the fallible and

imperfect nature of typical psychometric indicators which typically can be expected to tap

into more than one source of true score variance calls into question the usefulness of CFA

(e.g., Morin, Arens, & Marsh, 2016a). As noted by Morin et al. (2016a), indicators are rarely,

if ever, perfectly and uniquely related to a single construct, and will almost always display

Page 8: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 8

some degree of construct-relevant association with non-target factors assessing conceptually-

related (such as interrelated motivation types, see Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al., 2015) or

hierarchically-ordered constructs (such as when motivation types are expected to assess an

overarching motivation continuum, see Chemolli & Gagné, 2014).

CFA versus ESEM

Relying on an EFA measurement model allowing for the estimation of cross-loadings is

typically required as a test of construct-relevant multidimensionality related to the assessment

of conceptually related constructs (Morin et al., 2016a; Morin, Arens, Tran, & Caci, 2016b).

However, EFA has often been criticized for being data driven and “exploratory” in nature

(e.g., Kahn, 2006; Preacher & MacCallum, 2003). This implies an approach in which

multiple models are compared and the model producing the best correspondence to the data

(based on a variety of criteria) is retained for further use. In contrast, CFA is generally

assumed to be theory-driven and models are assessed in and of themselves using a variety of

goodness-of-fit indices. This view has led to the erroneous assumption that EFA is a data-

driven procedure unsuited to confirmatory studies. According to Morin et al. (2013: 396):

“This perception is reinforced by the erroneous semantically-based assumption that

EFA is strictly an exploratory method that should only be used when the researcher has no a

priori assumption regarding factor structure and that confirmatory methods are better in

studies based on a priori hypotheses regarding factor structure. This assumption still serves

to camouflage the fact that the critical difference between EFA and CFA is that all cross-

loadings are freely estimated in EFA. Due to this free estimation of all cross-loadings, EFA is

clearly more naturally suited to exploration than CFA. However, statistically, nothing

precludes the use of EFA for confirmatory purposes, except perhaps the fact that most of the

advances associated with CFA/SEM were not, until recently, available with EFA.”

The recent development of ESEM (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009; Marsh et al., 2014;

Page 9: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 9

Morin et al., 2013) provides a promising way to circumvent restrictive ICM assumptions.

ESEM provides an overarching framework allowing for the combination of CFA, EFA, and

SEM into a single model. ESEM thus incorporates the benefits from each technique into a

single analytic framework where factors defined according to ICM assumptions can

cohabitate with EFA factors incorporating cross-loadings.

A frequent misunderstanding about EFA/ESEM is that the inclusion of cross-loadings

is likely to change, or taint, the meaning of the latent factors that are estimated. This flawed

criticism neglects the fact that EFA/ESEM corresponds to a reflective measurement model

where the factors are assumed to influence the items, rather than the opposite. A perhaps

more critical issue is whether the factor itself is adequately captured, from a psychometric

perspective, as being primarily reflected in its a priori indicators. Indeed, whenever results

show large and hard to explain cross-loadings suggesting that some specific factors are

mainly reflected in unexpected items rather than in their a priori items, then alternatives

models should be explored. As noted by Morin et al. (2016a: 135-136):

“Small cross-loadings should be seen as reflecting the influence of the factor on the

construct-relevant part of the indicators, rather than the indicators having an impact on the

nature of the factor itself. It should be kept in mind that this interpretation applies to

relatively small cross-loadings that are in line with theoretical expectations, whereas any

model showing large and unexplainable cross-loadings or cross-loadings larger than target

loadings should be re-examined.”

Forcing cross-loadings to be exactly zero involves ignoring some potentially true

influence of a factor (such as stress) on indicators presenting some residual association with

these factors (such as insomnia) over and above their association with their main a priori

factor (such as burnout). In fact, even when large cross-loadings suggest a problem in the

model, forcing them to be zero simply hides sources of misspecification that will in turn be

Page 10: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 10

expressed as model misfit – leading to an examination of model modification indices to

locate the source of misfit. An advantage of EFA/ESEM is that it allows for the simultaneous

consideration of all cross-loadings in a single step, whereas modification indices are

calculated based on the inclusion of a single cross-loading at a time (Morin & Maïano, 2011).

It could be argued that ignoring these associations between items and non-target

constructs simply results in reduced goodness-of-fit indices and that typical interpretation

guidelines for goodness-of-fit indices are just too stringent for complex measurement models

(e.g., Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). However, a clear demonstration that cross-loadings do not

taint the meaning of the latent factors comes from simulation studies showing that

EFA/ESEM tends to provide more exact estimates of true population values for factor

correlations when cross-loadings (even small ones) are present in the population model, and

to remain unbiased when the population model corresponds to ICM-CFA (Asparouhov &

Muthén, 2009; Marsh, Lüdtke, Nagengast, Morin, & Von Davier, 2013; Morin et al., 2016a;

Sass & Schmitt, 2010; Schmitt & Sass, 2011; for a more extensive discussion, see

Asparouhov, Muthén, & Morin, 2015). In turn, biased CFA estimates of factor correlations

likely affect the discriminant validity of the factors by creating artificial multicollinearity in

subsequent analyses where these factors are used in prediction.

Another legitimate concern about EFA/ESEM is the issue of rotational indeterminacy,

meaning that the parameter estimates from any EFA/ESEM model will vary as a function of

the rotation procedure that is retained (e.g., Morin & Maïano, 2011; Sass & Schmitt, 2010;

Schmitt & Sass, 2011). More precisely, EFA/ESEM models based on different rotations

procedures – designed to reduce either cross-loadings or factor correlations to various degrees

– will converge on alternative solutions that have equivalent covariance implications (i.e.,

data fitting different models equivalently). In practice, alternative forms of oblique rotations

tend to provide nearly identical, or at least equivalent in terms of substantive interpretations,

Page 11: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 11

solutions as demonstrated in simulation studies by Sass and Schmitt (2010); and Schmitt and

Sass (2011), and real data examples by Morin and Maïano (2011) and Morin et al. (2013).

However, users should remain aware of this issue and, whenever they decide to use empirical

(atheoretical) rotation procedures, should always conduct at least a preliminary exploration of

alternative rotations to verify the stability of results. Recent recommendations suggest that

this issue can be solved in confirmatory applications of EFA/ESEM relying on target rotation

whereby the rotation is guided by a priori expectations regarding the expected factor structure

(Marsh et al., 2014; Morin et al., 2016a). The development of target rotation (in which all

cross-loadings are freely estimated but “targeted” to be as close to zero as possible) makes it

possible to use a fully confirmatory approach to the specification of EFA/ESEM factors

(Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009; Browne, 2001). Indeed, the most common use of ESEM so far

has been the testing of theoretically established models in which the number and content of

specified latent factors was a priori defined (Marsh et al., 2014).

Bifactor-ESEM

As noted above, a second source of construct relevant multidimensionality is related to

the assessment of hierarchically-ordered constructs (such as an overarching motivation

continuum). This possibility has typically been investigated using higher-order factor models,

which directly test the hypothesis that the various factors can combine into one or many

higher-order factors. However, higher-order factor models rely on highly restrictive implicit

assumptions that may not hold in practice and may explain why they often fail to meet

minimal requirements of adequate fit (Gignac, 2008; Morin et al., 2016a; Reise, 2012). More

precisely, higher-order models assume that the association between items and the higher-

order factor is fully mediated by the first-order factors (McAbee, Oswald, & Connelly, 2014),

so that the higher-order factor does not in itself explain any unique variance over and above

that already explained by the first-order factors. For this reason, the first-order factors in a

Page 12: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 12

higher-order model reflect a combination of the variance explained by the higher-order factor

and of the variance uniquely attributable to each first-order factor (Morin et al., 2016b). More

importantly, because the relation between the higher-order factor and the item is mediated

through the first-order factor, this relation is captured by the product of the loading of the

item on a first-order factor, and the loading of this first-order factor on the higher-order factor:

A constant for all items associated with a single first-order factor. Similarly, the relations

between the items and the disturbances of the first-order factors (reflecting the variance

uniquely attributable to the first-order factor) is also indirect, and reflected by the product of

the loadings of the items on their first-order factor with a constant for all items associated

with a single first-order factor. Because of this characteristic, the ratio of variance attributed

to the global factor versus uniquely attributed to the first-order factor is constant for all items

associated with a single first-order factor (Gignac, 2008; Morin et al., 2016b; Reise, 2012).

An alternative, and far more flexible, way to examine whether the presence of a single

global SDT factor underlying answers to motivation questionnaires involves the use of a

bifactor representation, in which all items are used to define their respective motivation

subscales while also being used to directly define a global SDT motivation factor that

represents the continuum (Reise, 2012). Bifactor models have existed for decades (Holzinger

& Swineford, 1937), and are well known in research on intelligence (Gignac, 2008) or

personality (McAbee, Oswald, & Connelly, 2014). In comparison to higher-order models,

bifactor models present none of these redundancies or restrictions, and provide a way to

explicitly separate the variance attributable to specific factors from the variance attributable

to the global general factor, while allowing for the estimation of direct relations between the

items and both the specific and global factors. More precisely, bifactor models assume that

the covariance among a set of n items can be explained by a set of f orthogonal factors

including one Global (G) factor and f-1 Specific (S) factors. In bifactor-CFA models, each

Page 13: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 13

item is used to define the G-factor and one of the S-factors. Bifactor models thus partition

covariance into a G-factor underlying all items, and f-1 S-factors corresponding to the

covariance not explained by the G-factor. This clean partitioning is made possible by the

orthogonality of the factors, which forces all of the variance shared among all items to be

absorbed into the G-factor, and the S-factors to represent what is shared among a specific

subset of items but not the others.

Interestingly, higher-order models form restricted nested versions of bifactor models.

While the above mentioned proportionality constraints implicit in higher-order models

introduce some parsimony to the model, they are unlikely to hold in most research settings

(Reise, 2012; Yung, Thissen, & McLeod, 1999) or to make sense theoretically (Gignac,

2016), thus positioning bifactor models as the more robust modeling procedure. Jennrich and

Bentler (2011) showed that while bifactor models were able to properly recover true higher-

order factor structures, higher-order factor models could not always properly recover true

bifactor structures. Bifactor models should thus be preferred over higher-order models unless

strong theoretical reasons are present to support the need to model the relations between the

indicators and the global factors as indirect, and the presence of the implicit proportionality

constraints (for a more extensive discussion of these issues, see Gignac, 2016).

Whenever a single instrument is expected to incorporate both conceptually-adjacent

constructs and hierarchically-ordered constructs, it becomes important to rely on a model that

allows for the incorporation of both cross-loadings (i.e., EFA) and global factors (i.e.

bifactor). Indeed, research has shown that unmodeled cross-loadings tend to result in inflated

estimates of the global factor in bifactor-CFA, and that an unmodeled global factor tends to

result in inflated cross-loadings in EFA (e.g., Morin et al., 2016a; Murray & Johnson, 2013).

Recent development of bifactor target rotation for EFA makes it possible to incorporate

bifactor modeling to the ESEM framework (Reise, 2012; Reise, Moore, & Maydeu-Olivares,

Page 14: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 14

2011). The resulting bifactor-ESEM method offers the most detailed and flexible models

possible, more so than either EFA or CFA/SEM alone, and can now be implemented while

relying on a confirmatory bifactor target rotation approach (Morin et al., 2016a, 2016b).

The Present Study

In the present study, we conducted an integrated test of SDT’s continuum hypothesis of

motivation combining the perspectives of: (1) Guay et al. (2015; also see Litalien et al., 2015),

which showed that taking into account sources of construct-relevant psychometric

multidimensionality related to the assessment of conceptually-related constructs was

necessary to obtain a clearer representation of the continuum structure of motivation; and (2)

Chemolli and Gagné (2014), who argued that the strongest evidence in favor of the SDT

continuum hypothesis should come from the demonstration that all motivation items

contribute to the assessment of a single overarching self-determination factor (i.e., from the

observation of another source of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality related

to the assessment of hierarchically-ordered constructs). To do so, we rely on the bifactor-

ESEM framework proposed by Morin et al. (2016a, 2016b), which allows for the

simultaneous consideration of these two perspectives into a single model.

The bifactor component of this framework directly tests whether the items measuring

the different types of motivation load onto a single factor with loadings ranging from

negative to positive according to the expected position of the items along the SDT continuum

(aligned with Chemolli and Gagné’s perspective), while allowing the estimation of specific

factors for each motivation type. Essentially, should this hypothesis be supported, this global

factor is expected to provide a global estimate of the overall quantity of self-determined

motivation characterizing individual employees, whereas the resulting specific factors would

reflect the more specific quality, or flavor, of employees’ motivational profiles. More

precisely, because of the inherent orthogonality of bifactor models, employees’ overall

Page 15: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 15

amount (quantity) of self-determined motivation will be reflected in the G-factor, whereas the

specific features (quality) of employees’ motivational profiles left unexplained by this global

amount of self-determination will be reflected in the S-factors (e.g., pleasure, guilt, pressure).

The ESEM component of this framework allows us to incorporate the presence of a second

layer of continuity in motivation ratings expressed through the estimation of cross-loadings

between motivation factors (e.g., Morin et al., 2016a), as advocated by Guay et al. (2015).

Thus, based on current theory suggesting the existence of an overarching continuum of

motivation (Chemolli & Gagné, 2014) and research suggesting the importance of controlling

for cross-loadings between motivation factors in order to obtain a proper depiction of the

underlying structure of motivation measures (Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al., 2015), we

expect the bifactor-ESEM model to provide the most adequate representation of employees’

answers to the MWMS (Gagné et al., 2015). However, following Morin et al. (2016a, 2016b)

recommendations regarding the application of the bifactor-ESEM framework for the

identification of the sources of construct-relevant multidimensionality present in complex

psychometric measures, as well as basic principles of model testing (e.g., Bollen, 1989), we

contrast this a priori bifactor-ESEM representation with more parsimonious alternative

models including either none (CFA) or only-one (ESEM, Bifactor-CFA) of these likely

sources of multidimensionality. These four alternative models are presented in Figure 1.

------------------------------ Insert Figure 1 about here ------------------------------

To establish the criterion-related validity of the resulting global (G) and specific (S)

motivation factors, we also test the extent to which they are related to a series of covariates

that occupy a core position in SDT theorization both generically (i.e., the satisfaction of the

needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness) and in the work context (i.e., employees

levels of affective and continuance commitment to the organization). SDT proposes that the

Page 16: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 16

satisfaction of basic psychological needs for autonomy (a sense of volition), competence (the

experience of mastery) and relatedness (feeling connected to others) should promote

autonomous over controlled types of motivation (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Gagné & Deci, 2005).

Research has found strong support for this proposition both in general and organizational

psychology (Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2008; Gagné et al., 2015).

Past research has also found rather robust relations between different types of

motivation and distinct commitment mindsets (Gagné, Chemollli, Forest, & Koestner, 2008;

Gagné et al., 2015). In particular, affective organizational commitment (emotional attachment

to the organization; Meyer, Allen, & Smith, 1997) has been positively related to autonomous

(identified and intrinsic) motivation (Gagné et al., 2008), while continuance commitment

(staying in the organization because of the perceived cost of leaving and lack of alternatives;

Meyer et al., 1997) has been positively related to external regulation and negatively related to

more internalized regulations (Battistelli, Galletta, Portoghese, & Vandenberghe, 2013;

Gagné, Chemolli, Forest, & Koestner, 2008; Vandenberghe & Panaccio, 2012).

In order to more precisely assess the criterion-related validity of the motivational

factors, we also systematically contrast models in which only the G-factor (i.e., reflecting the

overall quantity of self-determined motivation) is allowed to predict the covariates, with

models in which the S-factors (i.e., reflecting the specific quality of motivation) are also

allowed to predict the covariates. These comparisons systematically test the added value (in

terms of percentage of explained variance in the covariates) that is afforded by the

simultaneous consideration of both motivation quantity and quality.

METHOD

Participants and Procedure

This study used archival data collected between 2008 and 2012 that has been previously

used to validate the MWMS (Gagné et al., 2015). The current sample includes 1124 full time

Page 17: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 17

Canadian employees from a range of organizations and industries. Content of the surveys

varied within data sets in terms of covariates and demographics, but all participants

completed the same 19 items forming the MWMS. Employees completed confidential

surveys voluntarily on an online platform or in paper format on their work premises.

Additional details are provided in Gagné et al. (2015).

Measures

The MWMS (Gagné et al., 2015) includes 19 items assessing six distinct motivation

types. Each item is a response to the stem, “Why do you or would you put efforts into your

current job?” along a 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely) point Likert scale. Example items

include, “I don’t know why I’m doing this job, it’s pointless work” (Amotivation; α = .78 in

the current study), “To get others’ approval (e.g., supervisor, colleagues, family, clients...)”

(External regulation social; α = .77), “Because others will reward me financially only if I put

enough effort in my job (e.g., employer, supervisor...)” (External regulation material; α = .63),

“Because otherwise I will feel ashamed of myself” (Introjected regulation; α = .71), “Because

putting efforts in this job aligns with my personal values” (Identified regulation; α = .80), and

“Because the work I do is interesting” (Intrinsic motivation; α = .90). Validation evidence for

the MWMS based on the current data set has already demonstrated adequate fit for a six-

factor ICM-CFA structure (invariant across French and English languages), acceptable scale

score reliability (Cronbach’s α ranged from .70-.90 for all subscales), and supported the

convergent and discriminant validity of the scales (Gagné et al., 2015).

Need satisfaction was measured using an early version of the work-related Basic Needs

Satisfaction Scale (Van den Broeck, Vansteenkiste, Witte, Soenens, & Lens, 2010). This

instrument assesses the satisfaction of the three basic needs for: (a) autonomy (3 items, α

= .81, e.g., “I feel like I can be myself at my job”); (b) competence (3 items, α = .85, e.g., “I

Page 18: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 18

really master my tasks at my job”), and (c) relatedness (4 items, α = .82,e.g., “At work I feel

part of a group”) on a 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree) Likert scale.

Affective commitment to the organization was measured using Meyer, Allen, and

Smith’s (1993) organizational commitment measure (6 items, α = .84, e.g., “This

organization has a great deal of personal meaning to me”). Continuance commitment to the

organization was measured by Stinglhamber, Bentein, and Vandenberghe’s (2002) French

adaptation of Meyer et al.’s (1993) measure to ensure a complete coverage of both high-

sacrifice (i.e., cost of leaving) and low-alternative (i.e., Lack of alternatives) facets (6 items, α

= .70, e.g., “I consider my job opportunities as too limited to consider leaving the

organization”). All items were rated on a 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree) Likert scale.

Estimation and Specification

All models were estimated using Mplus 7.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2014) robust

Maximum Likelihood (MLR) estimator. CFA models were specified according to ICM

assumptions, with items allowed to load onto their a priori motivation factor, and all cross-

loadings constrained to be exactly zero. ESEM was specified using target rotation: Item

loadings on their a priori motivation factors were freely estimated, and all cross-loadings

were also freely estimated but “targeted” to be as close to 0 as possible. Bifactor-CFA (B-

CFA) models were specified as orthogonal, with each item specified as loading on the SDT

G-factor as well as on their a priori S-factors corresponding to the six distinct motivation

types. Finally, bifactor-ESEM (B-ESEM) was estimated using bifactor target rotation: All

items were used to define the SDT G-factor, while the 6 S-factors were defined using the

same pattern of target and non-target loadings and cross-loadings as in the ESEM solution.

The current models correspond to typical bifactor specifications where all items are used to

define the G-factor, and one S-factor in line with theoretical expectations that all items reflect

motivation types organized according to the expected continuum structure of motivation

Page 19: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 19

reflected in the G-factor.1 We note however that hybrid models, such as models including

more than one G-factor (e.g., Caci, Morin, & Tran, 2015), or models where only a subset of

items are used to define the G-factor (e.g., Brunner, Lüdtke, & Trautwein, 2008), are also

possible when theoretical expectations suggest that these might be more appropriate.

Covariates were then integrated to the final retained measurement model, allowing

estimation of relations between the motivation factors and the covariates. In a first model,

only the G-factor was allowed to covary using the ESEM-within-CFA method described by

Morin et al. (2013, 2016a) which allows for the estimation of relations between only a subset

of B-ESEM factors (i.e., here only the G-factor) and covariates. This model simulates the

common approach used in SDT of using a single motivation score (quantity; i.e., the relative

autonomy index; Fernet, Gagné, & Austin, 2010; Grolnick & Ryan, 1987; Markland &

Ingledew, 2007; Pelletier, Seguin-Levesque, & Legault, 2002), which ignores the relative

impact of different types (or qualities) of motivation. In a second model (relying on a regular

B-ESEM representation), both the G-factor and the S-factors were allowed to predict scores

on all covariates. These two models were contrasted to one another on the basis of goodness

of fit information, but also based on standardized regression coefficients and model-based

estimates of the percentage of explained variance (R2) in the covariates afforded by the model.

Model Comparisons

Because of the known oversensitivity of the chi-square test of exact fit to sample size and

minor model misspecifications (e.g., Marsh, Hau, & Grayson, 2005), model fit was assessed

using commonly used goodness-of-fit indices and information criteria: the comparative fit

index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation

(RMSEA) with its confidence interval, the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC), the Constant

AIC (CAIC), the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), and the sample-size adjusted BIC

(ABIC). According to typical interpretation guidelines (e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh, Hau,

Page 20: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 20

& Wen, 2004; Marsh et al., 2005), values greater than .90 and .95 for the CFI and TLI

respectively support adequate and excellent fit of the data to the model while values smaller

than .08 or .06 for the RMSEA support acceptable and excellent fit. When comparing models,

changes in RMSEA, CFI and TLI greater than .01 were deemed significant as established by

Cheung and Rensyold (2002), and Chen (2007). Although they cannot be used to assess the

global fit of a single model, the information criteria (AIC, CAIC, BIC, ABIC) are particularly

useful in the comparison of alternative models, with lower values supporting a better fitting

model. These guidelines have so far been established for CFA, and have also been used in

previous applications of ESEM (e.g., Marsh et al., 2009, 2014; Morin et al., 2013, 2016a).

However, because ESEM includes many more parameters than ICM-CFA, due to the free

estimation of cross-loadings, it has been suggested that indicators including a correction for

parsimony (i.e., TLI, RMSEA, AIC, CAIC, BIC, ABIC) will be critical to the assessment of

model fit in an ESEM context (Marsh et al., 2009, 2010, 2014; Morin et al., 2013, 2016a).

It is important to keep in mind that these remain rough guidelines for descriptive model

evaluation, which also needs to take into account the even more important information

coming from parameters estimates, statistical conformity and theoretical meaningfulness

(Marsh et al., 2004, 2005). Indeed, each of these models is able to absorb unmodelled sources

of construct-relevant multidimensionality (e.g., Asparouhov et al., 2015; Morin et al., 2016a;

Murray, & Johnson, 2013). For this reason, a close examination of parameter estimates and

theoretical conformity is necessary to select the best alternative among a series of models as

simple goodness-of-fit-assessment is often insufficient to differentiate among models that

often provide similar levels of fit to the data (Marsh et al., 2011; Morin et al., 2016a). Morin

et al. (2016a, 2016b) suggest to start with a comparison of CFA and ESEM solutions. In this

comparison, as long as the factors remain well-defined by strong target factor loadings, the

key issue is related to the factor correlations. Statistical evidence that ESEM tends to provide

Page 21: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 21

more exact estimates of true factor correlations (Asparouhov et al., 2015) suggests that

ESEM should be retained whenever the results show a discrepant pattern of factor

correlations. Otherwise, the CFA model should be preferred based on parsimony. Then, the

second comparison involves contrasting the retained model with its bifactor counterpart (B-

CFA or B-ESEM). Here, the key elements favoring a bifactor representation are the

observation of a G-factor that is well-defined by strong factor loadings, and the observation

of reduced cross-loadings in B-ESEM compared to ESEM.

RESULTS

Measurement Models

Table 1 presents the goodness-of-fit indices and information criteria associated with

each of the estimated models. The ICM-CFA demonstrated marginally adequate fit, whereas

the B-CFA did not. In contrast, the ESEM solution provided an excellent representation to the

data according to all indices, and provided a better representation than the ICM-CFA solution

based on lower scores on the information criteria and substantial improvement on the

goodness-of-fit indices (ΔCFI = +.05; ΔTLI = +.06; ΔRMSEA = -.02). Furthermore, the 90%

confidence intervals for the RMSEA showed no overlap between the CFA and ESEM

solutions, indicating a high degree of differentiation between competing models. Finally,

although the B-ESEM solution provided an excellent representation of the data, it displayed

only marginal improvement relative to the ESEM solution according to goodness-of-fit

indices (ΔCFI < .01; ΔTLI = +.01; ΔRMSEA = -.01), overlapping confidence intervals for

the RMSEA, almost identical values for the information criteria, but resulted in a non-

significant chi square value suggesting that it is the only model providing exact fit to the data.

However, no analysis should be conducted in disconnection from theory, expectations, and a

detailed examination of parameter estimates (Marsh et al., 2014; Morin et al., 2016a).

Insert Table 1 about here

Page 22: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 22

Because the data did not fit the B-CFA model to even a minimally acceptable standard,

and following Morin et al. (2016a, 2016b) recommendations, we first turn to a comparison of

ICM-CFA and ESEM, before moving on to the B-ESEM solution. Before doing so however,

it is worth nothing that on the strict basis of goodness-of-fit assessment showing the

superiority of the ESEM and B-ESEM solutions when compared to the ICM-CFA and B-

CFA solutions, cross-loadings are clearly to be expected in the solution. Because of this,

factor correlations are expected to be higher in the ICM-CFA model compared to the ESEM

models as this is the only way through which these cross-loadings can be expressed. Because

the B-CFA model is orthogonal however, the only way for these omitted cross-loadings to be

expressed is through an inflated estimate of the factor loadings of items on the G-factor

(Morin et al., 2016a), which is unlikely to be a sufficient to compensate for this source of

misfit should the cross-loadings reflect another source of multidimensionality than the

presence of an underlying global construct. This likely explains why the fit of the B-CFA

model was lower and less adequate than that of the ICM-CFA.

Parameter estimates for ICM-CFA and ESEM are reported in Tables 2 (correlations)

and 3 (factor loadings, cross-loadings and uniquenesses). Looking first at the loadings and

cross-loadings, the overall size of the factor loadings of the items on their target factors

remained similar in the ICM-CFA (λ = .50 to .90; M = .69) and ESEM (λ = .37 to .93; M

= .65) solutions, showing well-defined factors corresponding to a priori expectations. In the

ESEM solution, target factor loadings systematically remained higher than cross-loadings,

which generally remained very small (|λ| = 0 to .37 M = .02). In fact, only two cross-loadings

were higher than .30: Item 1 of identified regulation (“Because I personally consider it

important to put effort into this job”) cross-loaded on the introjected regulation factor at .37,

and introjected item 2 (“Because it makes me feel proud of myself”) on the identified factor

at .31. Closer inspection suggested no pattern of larger cross-loadings between adjacent

Page 23: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 23

factors and smaller or more negative cross-loadings between more distant factors, providing

weak support for the continuum hypothesis at the cross-loadings level.

Insert Tables 2 and 3 about here

As expected, factor correlations proved to be slightly lower in ESEM (r = -.47 to .77,

|M| = .31) than ICM-CFA (r = -.48 to .81, |M| = .37).2 The overall pattern of those

correlations, however, was not changed by the decision to rely on an ICM-CFA or ESEM

solution. A closer examination of these correlations reveals that they match the continuum

hypothesis relatively well, being stronger between conceptually closer factors than between

conceptually distant factors. Additionally, the amotivation factor appears to represent one end

of the hypothesized continuum, showing generally negative correlations with more

autonomous forms of motivations (intrinsic and identified), a smaller correlation with

introjected regulation, and positive correlations with the social and material forms of external

regulation (with a slightly larger correlation for the material factor than for the social factor).

Looking at the B-ESEM solution, we already noted that it represented the data quite

well, and provided an exact fit to the data. It is interesting to note that typical (i.e. orthogonal)

representations of bifactor models attempt to synthetize the covariance (i.e., correlations)

among factors through the estimation of a single G-factor, and to keep in mind that the ESEM

correlations generally supported the continuum hypothesis. As such, a key advantage of the

bifactor-ESEM model in comparison to the ESEM model, in addition to its exact fit to the

data, is that it provides a single directly interpretable self-determination G-factor.

Interestingly, results from the bifactor-ESEM solution (see Table 4) revealed a well-defined

G-factor representing general self-determination. This G-factor follows the idea of a

continuum underlying motivation: The loadings on the G-factor were high and positive for

the items associated with the autonomous motivation S-factors (λ = .71 to .75 for intrinsic

motivation, and .56 to .79 for identified regulation), moderate for the items associated with

Page 24: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 24

introjected regulation (λ = .26 to .61), lower for the items associated with external regulation-

social (λ = .02 to .21), small or negative for the items associated with the external regulation-

material S-factor (λ = -.07 to .25), and negative for the items associated with amotivation (λ =

-.35 to -.30).

Insert Table 4 about here

Further examination of this solution reveals reasonably low cross-loadings, remaining

lower than target loadings (|λ| < .01 to .33; M = .08), and reasonably well-defined S-factors (λ

= .26 to .78; M = .50), with very few noteworthy exceptions. Importantly, the cross loadings

tended to be smaller in the bifactor-ESEM solution than in the ESEM solution, suggesting

that part of the ESEM cross-loadings reflected the presence of the unmodelled G-factor. It is

also interesting to note that the S-factors located at the ends of the continuum (amotivation,

external-social regulation, external-material regulation, and intrinsic motivation; λ = .41

to .78; M = .57) retained substantial specificity once the covariance attributed to the self-

determination G-factor was taken into account. Conversely, the S-factors located toward the

midpoint of the continuum (identified and introjected regulation; λ = .26 to .55; M = .38)

retained less specificity once the general self-determination factor is taken into account.

Based on the evidence presented thus far, both in terms of exact fit to the data but most

importantly theoretical conformity of the parameter estimates, the final retained model is the

bifactor-ESEM model. In practical terms, this model also provides a way to simultaneously

take into account all motivation factors (motivation quality), together with a global estimate

of the quantity of self-determined motivation into a single predictive model, and to do so

without having to rely on a psychometrically suboptimal RAI.

Predictive Models

From the final retained bifactor-ESEM solution, SEM analyses were used to assess the

criterion-related validity of the various motivation factors. More precisely, these models were

Page 25: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 25

used to compare the added value of the specific motivation facets (representing the specific

quality of employees’ motivational profiles) over and above the G-factor (representing

overall quantity of self-determined motivation) in terms of percentages of explained variance

in the various covariates considered. This comparison was achieved by contrasting a model in

which only the G-factor was allowed to predict scores on the covariates, with a model in

which both the G- and S-factors were allowed to predict scores on the covariates. As shown

in Table 5, when considered as the sole predictor of covariates, the self-determination G-

factor was significantly associated, as expected, with higher scores on the affective

commitment mindset (explaining 38% of its variance), as well as on the satisfaction of the

needs for autonomy (R2 = 15%), competence (R2 = 1%), and relatedness (R2 = 15%). It was

not significantly associated with continuance commitment.

These relations were all maintained in the next model where the S-factors were also

allowed to relate to the covariates. This more complete model resulted in visible increases in

explained variance in the various covariates: (a) from 38% to 42% for affective commitment;

(b) from 0% to 14% for continuance commitment; (c) from 15% to 26% for the satisfaction

of the need for autonomy; (d) from 1% to 5% for the satisfaction of the need for competence;

(e) from 15% to 25% for the satisfaction of the need for relatedness. These increases in

percentages of explained variance are also accompanied by increases in goodness-of-fit

(complete model: CFI = .99; TLI = .98; RMSEA = .02; restricted model where only the G-

factor relates to the covariates; CFI = .98; TLI = .98; RMSEA = .03).

Interestingly, relations observed between the S-factors and the covariates appear to be

partly in line with our expectations, showing that continuance commitment was mainly, and

positively, associated with levels of amotivation and external-social regulation. Contrary to

our expectations, continuance commitment and external-material regulation were not

significantly related. In contrast, and fully in line with our expectations, affective

Page 26: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 26

commitment was significantly, and negatively, associated with levels of amotivation,

external-material regulation, and introjected regulation, but positively associated with

identified and intrinsic motivation, as well as with the G-factor. However, we also noted an

unexpected positive relation between affective commitment and external-social regulation.

With regards to basic need satisfaction, the results are also essentially in line with our

expectations that all three needs would be positively related to more autonomous forms of

motivation. Indeed, significant positive relations between the satisfaction of the needs for

autonomy and relatedness and motivation factors were limited to intrinsic motivation and to

the G-factor, whereas they were strictly limited to the G-factor for the needs for competence.

Moreover, relations between satisfaction of the needs for autonomy and relatedness were

significant and negative with amotivation, introjection, and even identified regulation.

Insert Table 5 about here

DISCUSSION

The present study had the dual objective of testing SDT’s continuum hypothesis of

motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1985) using ratings obtained on the Multidimensional Work

Motivation Scale (Gagné et al., 2015), while demonstrating the usefulness of the bifactor-

ESEM framework for management research. Support for the continuum hypothesis of

motivation has been at best inconsistent in previous research (Chemolli & Gagné, 2014;

Gagné et al., 2015; Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al., 2015; Mallett et al., 2007). In particular,

recent studies relying on more systematic tests of this hypothesis using ESEM (Guay et al.,

2015; Litalien et al., 2015) and Rasch analysis (Chemolli & Gagné, 2014) respectively

showed weak or no support for the continuum hypothesis. In the current study, we relied on a

newly developed overarching bifactor-ESEM framework, which combines the logic of

previous analyses conducted by Guay et al. (2015; Litalien et al., 2015) and Chemolli and

Gagné (2014) to conduct a more comprehensive test of the SDT continuum hypothesis.

Page 27: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 27

Using this framework, the SDT continuum could be expressed in three different

manners. First, SDT’s continuum can be evidenced by the observation of ICM-CFA or

ESEM factor correlations corresponding to the expected simplex pattern, showing larger

correlations between conceptually adjacent motivation factors, and smaller or negative

correlations between conceptually distant factors (Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al., 2015). In

the current study, the ICM-CFA/ESEM correlations provided support for SDT’s continuum

hypothesis, showing stronger correlations between conceptually adjacent factors, and smaller

or negative between more conceptually distal factors. Second, the SDT’s continuum can be

evidenced by the observation of ESEM or B-ESEM cross-loadings that are larger and

positive between conceptually adjacent motivation factors, and smaller or negative between

conceptually distant factors (Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al., 2015). In the current study, the

results did not support this proposition as cross-loadings were uniformly small and evidenced

no clear pattern of loading more highly on theoretically closer specific factors. Third, the

strongest evidence in favor of SDT’s continuum hypothesis could come from the observation

that the G-factor estimated as part of the B-CFA or B-ESEM solutions is characterized by a

pattern of target loadings from the items associated with the motivation types corresponding

to the continuum assumptions of SDT (e.g., Chemolli & Gagné, 2014). In the current study,

the G-factor loadings were largely in line with the presence of an underlying continuum of

motivation. Given that the bifactor-ESEM model was retained for final interpretation, these

results support the notion that motivation types follow an underlying continuum.

However, although our results provide strong evidence that motivation types follow a

continuum structure globally aligned with SDT hypothesis, the observed continuum structure

is not completely in agreement with SDT assumptions that that external and introjected

regulations should load negatively on a continuum factor. Such a factor structure would

represent what has been described in SDT as “relative self-determination” (Grolnick & Ryan,

Page 28: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 28

1987). Instead, the introjection subscale loaded positively on the G-factor and the external

regulation subscales loaded weakly but positively. As such, the factor loadings on the G-

factor rather seem to represent a general quantity of self-determination (rather than relative

self-determination), as they ranged from strongly positive for autonomous motivation items,

slightly positive for introjected items, non-significant for external regulation items, to

moderately negative for amotivation items. The pattern of factor loadings on the S-factors

also suggested that while the subscales may be ordered in a predictable fashion, each still

provided relevant unique information. As such, although our results support the presence of a

continuum structure of motivation as proposed by SDT, they also suggest a need to revise the

exact nature of this theoretical continuum, pending replication of the present results.

Turning now to the methodological contribution of this research, the results revealed

that the data fit the ESEM representation better than the ICM-CFA model. This suggests that

even the small cross-loadings present in the current data were enough to cause significant

model misspecification. These cross-loadings are not surprising given the conceptually fine

distinction between motivation types. Accounting for conceptual relatedness between the

motivation types resulted in a significantly better fitting model, but also in more precise

estimates of the factor correlations (Marsh et al., 2013; Morin et al., 2016a, 2016b; Sass &

Schmitt, 2010; Schmitt & Sass, 2011).

The estimated factor correlations were slightly lower in the ESEM solution than in the

ICM-CFA solution. While not directly relevant to this study where the final retained model

was a bifactor-ESEM model, this discrepancy in correlation estimates could have significant

implications if these latent factors were used in prediction. Indeed, relying on ICM-CFA

would introduce unnecessary multicollinearity (Asparouhov et al., 2015), which may explain

why there are few published studies that use all of the separate motivation subscales in

predictive regression or SEM models. Instead, many SDT studies typically rely on a single

Page 29: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 29

relative autonomy index (RAI; Grolnick & Ryan, 1987) or on two higher-order factors of

autonomous and controlled motivation (e.g., Gillet, Gagné, Sauvagère, & Fouquereau, 2013).

The data did not fit the bifactor-CFA model as well as the other models. This was most

likely the result of suppressing cross-loadings, which has been shown in past research to be

problematic (Morin et al., 2016a, 2016b), especially for measures of conceptually close

constructs such as motivation types (Guay et al., 2015). Given the orthogonality of this

solution, these cross-loadings could only be expressed through an inflation of the loadings on

the G-factor. Thus, the relatively poor fit of the B-CFA solution, and the superior fit of the B-

ESEM solution, supports the idea that these cross-loadings are needed to reflect the presence

of conceptually related constructs that could not entirely be captured by an overarching

global factor. Indeed, the bifactor-ESEM displayed excellent fit, and revealed a pattern of

factor loadings on the G-factor that supports the presence of a continuum structure.

Importantly, a key practical and theoretical advantage of the B-ESEM model is that it

provided an explicit expression of the expected self-determination continuum (rather than

implicitly assuming its existence through an eyeballing of factor correlations). More precisely,

the B-ESEM solution has the advantage of providing a directly interpretable latent estimate

of overall self-determined motivation, and of allowing explicit tests of whether the S-factors

(reflecting the residual variance attributable to qualitatively different motivation types over

and above the global self-determined motivation factor) contribute to the prediction of

meaningful outcomes over and above this global self-determined motivation factor. Pending

replication of the current results, this advantage clearly suggests that this method should be

given careful attention in future research in which the objective is to assess relations between

self-determined motivation and various predictors, covariates, and outcomes.

Through the incorporation of covariates into the final retained B-ESEM model, the

current study has uniquely been able to test the criterion-related validity of the G- and S-

Page 30: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 30

factors, and examine the degree to which the global quantity of self-determined motivation

and the more specific qualities of motivation over and above this global factor explained

variability in the covariates. Our results clearly showed the added value of considering these

specific motivation facets over and above the global quantity of self-determined motivation.

Specifically, across all covariates, the results showed that the complete model consistently

resulted in a higher proportion of explained variance in the covariates when compared to the

model in which only the G-factor was allowed to associate with covariates.

Furthermore, the simplified quantity-only model failed to recognize key directional

differences between the various forms of regulations. When examining the relations between

motivation and affective commitment for example, amotivation, external-material, and

introjected regulations all displayed negative relations with affective commitment, whereas

external-social regulation did not. This result in important as it suggests that the regulations

proposed by SDT are not always associated with covariates in a manner that directly and

linearly follows their expected position on the continuum but rather (once the global quantity

of self-determination is taken into account), are qualitatively different from one another to the

extent of presenting differentiated patterns of relations with covariates. Similarly, when

examining continuance commitment, which was not related to the G-factor, the quantity only

model contributed to hide valuable information, such as a positive relation with external-

social regulation and a negative association with amotivation. It appeared that quantity of

self-determined motivation had essentially no association with continuance commitment

whereas qualities specific to external-social and amotivation were significantly associated

with this covariate. These examples demonstrate the importance of recognizing and modeling

both quantity and qualities of motivation in not only explaining more variance in covariates,

but also in creating a more detailed picture of the relations between covariates and motivation.

The results, when considered together, suggest that though there is evidence for a

Page 31: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 31

continuum structure underlying the types of motivation, important information would be lost

if we were to assume that all motivation types can be summarized within a single (latent or

manifest) score reflecting a self-determination continuum, such as the RAI. More precisely, it

is critical to note that although the current results support “a” continuum of self-regulation,

they do not represent “the” classical representation of the SDT continuum hypothesis (see

above discussion), and clearly do not support the way this hypothesis has been used to justify

the use of difference scores to combine all motivation types into a single RAI (Grolnick &

Ryan, 1987, see Chemolli and Gagné, 2014, for an in-depth discussion of the RAI). More

precisely, the RAI is typically calculated by subtracting scores on the external and introjected

regulation subscales from the scores on the identified and intrinsic motivation subscales to

obtain a single indicator of self-regulation (Grolnick & Ryan, 1987). When amotivation is

included, it is also given a negative weight. Results from this study clearly show that this

mode of calculation is flawed given that very few loadings on the G-factors are negative, with

the sole exception of those involving amotivation. Instead, it appears that in order to fully

utilize the richness of information inherent within SDT, it is important to take into account

both the quantity of self-determination and the specific effects of individual regulations.

The resulting bifactor-ESEM structure provides an alternative approach that allows for

the simultaneous consideration of the global quantity of self-determined motivation, together

with all qualitative variations along the SDT continuum in a single model not tainted by

multicollinearity. These findings have important implications for self-determination theory in

explicitly showing that individual regulations do provide valuable information both in terms

of increasing the amount of variance accounted for by the models, but also in providing more

theoretical precision regarding the nature of the observed relations with key covariates. Two

recommendations emerge out of these results. First, the continuum hypothesis could be

revised to focus on the global “quantity of self-determined motivation” rather than on

Page 32: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 32

“relative autonomy”. Second, researchers using self-determination theory should not ignore

quality over quantity in motivation research, as both aspects were shown to have

complementary predictive power and are themselves meaningful factors. Bifactor-ESEM

models provide researchers with the means to take into account both quality and quantity of

self-determined motivation. In the current study, both the general and specific factors were

used as both were theoretically pertinent to the hypotheses under examination. The decision

to contrast predictive models including only the G-factor to predictive models including both

the G- and S- factors aimed to illustrate the loss of information related to the reliance on a

simplified “quantity-only” representation of human motivation. Still, it should be kept in

mind that bifactor models are essentially designed to represent theoretically meaningful G-

and S-factors estimated from the same set of items whenever there are reasons to expect the

presence of construct-relevant multidimensionality due to the presence of hierarchically-

ordered constructs. As such, it is part of the inherent theoretical logic of bifactor models that

all factors need to be incorporated in further predictive models. In contrast, alternative

models are available whenever there is a need to control for theoretically meaningless, or

construct irrelevant, sources of multidimensionality in a measure, such as models

incorporating correlated method factors (Eid, 2000), or models incorporating a global factor

aiming to control for shared responses tendencies in the estimation of meaningful correlated

factors (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, 2003).

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

The present study is not without limitations. First, though our sample was large, it was

limited to Canadian employees, and to a handful of different work settings. As such, future

research should aim to replicate the current study to samples including more job types, work

conditions, and cultures. Chemolli and Gagné (2014) noted through a semi-systematic review

of the literature covering different life domains (including work, sport, and education) that

Page 33: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 33

the pattern of correlations between the motivation subscales appears to be more variable

across studies than self-determination theory predicts. This variability may be due to different

scales being used to assess motivation, but also possibly moderated by contextual factors,

such as life domain (e.g., work, sport, education) and work conditions. Meta-analytic

examination of these correlations across domains would help elucidate this issue. Similarly,

although not directly relevant to organizational research, it is also critical, from the

perspective of SDT, to see whether the present results replicate across different levels of

analyses (e.g., state versus domain; Vallerand, 1997), life periods, contexts, and activities

(Guay et al., 2015; Pelletier Rocchi, Vallerand, Deci, & Ryan, 2013; Vallerand et al., 1992).

Another potential direction for future research will be the introduction of Bayesian

models in which prior knowledge of cross-loadings could be directly specified and

incorporated to the estimated models (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009). This method provides a

way to achieve a balance between accounting for the most influential cross-loadings while at

the same time retaining greater parsimony in areas where knowledge has advanced enough to

afford a priori predictions regarding the nature of these most significant cross-loadings. In

these contexts, it is important to note that a key advantage of Bayesian methods is that the use

of model priors does not completely constrain the estimation of the model, thus allowing for

unexpected cross-loadings to be incorporated (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009). While

promising, this approach will require more research into identifying expectable cross-

loadings and is not without limitations (e.g., no clear goodness-of-fit information,

approximate invariance constraints). For a comprehensive coverage of ESEM versus

Bayesian SEM models, we refer readers to Gucciardi and Zyphur (2016). However it is

important to reinforce that, irrespective of parsimony, current evidence suggests that there is

no risk to adopting an ESEM parameterization of the data even when no cross-loadings are

present in the population model (Asparouhov, Muthen & Morin, 2015).

Page 34: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 34

Conclusion

Methodologically, this study demonstrated the use of the relatively new bifactor-ESEM

framework for organizational researchers by showing how it can help to provide a more

precise test of SDT’s continuum hypothesis. Substantively, our results inform the value of

postulating a continuum structure underlying the motivation types on both theoretical and

practical grounds. There has been mixed support for a continuum structure, as past research

has mostly used insensitive tests of the continuum structure (see Chemolli and Gagné, 2014).

Recent research, including the current study, have used more stringent techniques to test this

assumption with mixed results (Chemolli & Gagné, 2014; Guay et al., 2015; Litalien et al.,

2015). The current study provided clearer support for a continuum structure of motivation,

though this continuum is not completely in line with the way it is postulated in SDT.

However, criterion-related tests revealed that relying solely on a single latent motivation

construct results in the loss of critical information specific to each motivation type.

In practice, the continuum hypothesis has led researchers to use the RAI. Not only is

this formulaic motivational construct not supported by the factor structure obtained in the

current study, nor by results of previous research (e.g., Chemolli & Gagné, 2014), but the

results show that relying on a single construct representing quantity of self-determined

motivation is insufficient to fully explain motivational covariates. Rather, our results

demonstrate the added value of considering quality of motivation through the motivation

subscales even when accounting for quantity of self-determined motivation. Such omission

could potentially have grave consequences for evidence-based decisions taken in workplaces

to motivate the workforce. In this context, a key advantage of the bifactor model comes from

its orthogonality, providing a way to simultaneously consider all motivation facets without

encountering potentially severe problems of multicollinearity. In SDT research conducted so

far, it has typically been impossible to simultaneously consider all types of motivations in a

Page 35: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 35

single model, possibly due to the presence of substantial multicollinearity among motivation

subscales that is likely to remain even when using an ESEM approach due to the lack of

control for the global quantity of self-determination underlying ratings to all motivation items.

The bifactor-ESEM approach thus provides a more comprehensive approach to testing the

critical assumptions of SDT regarding the role of self-determination and motivation types

than has been available so far in the literature.

ENDNOTE

1 Higher-order CFA and ESEM models were also estimated for comparison purposes, but

excluded from further analysis due to a lack of theoretical and empirical support. In

conformity with our expectations, fit statistics for these models proved to be significantly

lower than for bifactor alternatives: (a) higher-order CFA: χ2 = 1003, df = 146, p ≤ .01;

RMSEA = .07; CFI = .86; TLI = .83; AIC = 74522; BIC = 74839; CAIC = 74902; ABIC =

74639; ∆χ2 relative to B-CFA: ∆χ2 = 159, ∆df = 13, p ≤ .01; (b) higher-order ESEM: χ2 =

320, df = 81; p ≤ .01; RMSEA = .05; CFI = .96; TLI = .92; AIC = 73817; BIC = 74460;

CAIC = 74588; ABIC = 74053; ∆χ2 relative to B-ESEM: ∆χ2 = 236, ∆df = 22, p ≤ .01.

2 Correlations between the external-material and external-social factors (r =.74 in ICM-CFA

and .68 in ESEM), and between the identified regulation and intrinsic motivation factors (r

= .78 in ICM-CFA, and .77 in ESEM) were high and not substantially deflated in ESEM.

However, alternative models in which these factors were collapsed into a single factor did

systematically result in a substantial decrease in model fit. Thus, when the external social and

material factors were collapsed into a single factor, the goodness-of-fit showed a substantial

decrease for both ICM-CFA (ΔTLI = -.02, ΔRMSEA = -.01) and ESEM (ΔTLI = -.02,

ΔRMSEA = -.01). Likewise, when identified and intrinsic regulations were merged into a

single factor, the goodness-of-fit again showed a substantial decrease for ICM-CFA (ΔTLI =

-.07, ΔRMSEA = -.02) and ESEM (ΔTLI = -.02, ΔRMSEA = -.01).

Page 36: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 36

REFERENCES

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. 2009. Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural

Equation Modeling, 16: 397-438.

Asparouhov, T., Muthén, B., & Morin, A. J. S. 2015. Bayesian Structural equation modeling

with cross-loadings and residual covariances: Comments on Stromeyer et al. Journal of

Management, 41: 1561-1577.

Battistelli, A., Galletta, M., Portoghese, I., & Vandenberghe, C. 2013. Mindsets of

Commitment and Motivation: Interrelationships and Contribution to Work Outcomes.

The Journal of Psychology, 147: 17–48.

Bollen, K. A. 1989. Structural Equations with latent variables. New York, NY: Wiley.

Browne, M. 2001. An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis.

Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36: 111-150.

Brunner, M., Lüdtke, O., & Trautwein, U. 2008. The internal/external frame of reference

model revisited: Incorporating general cognitive ability and general academic self-

concept. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 43: 137-172.

Caci, H., Morin, A. J. S., & Tran, A. 2015. Investigation of a bifactor model of the Strengths

and Difficulties Questionnaire. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24: 1291-1301

Cox, A., Ullrich-French, S., Madonia, J., & Witty, K. 2011. Social physique anxiety in

physical education: Social contextual factors and links to motivation and behavior.

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12: 555-562.

Chemolli, E., & Gagné, M. 2014. Evidence against the continuum structure underlying

motivation measures derived from self-determination theory. Psychological Assessment.

26: 575–585.

Chen, F. F. 2007. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance.

Structural Equation Modeling, 14: 464–504.

Page 37: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 37

Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. 2002. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing

measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9: 233–255.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 1985. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

behavior. New York: Plenum.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 2000. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and

the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11: 227–268.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 2008. Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-

being across life’s domains. Canadian Psychology, 49: 14–23.

Eid, M. 2000. Multitrait-multimethod model with minimal assumptions. Psychometrika, 65:

241-261.

Gagné, M., Chemolli, E., Forest, J., & Koestner, R. 2008. A temporal analysis of the

relationship between organisational commitment and work motivation. Psychologica

Belgica, 48: 219-241.

Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. 2005. Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 26: 331-362.

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Gilbert, M., Aubé, C., Morin, E., & Malorni, A. 2010. The motivation

at work scale: Validation evidence in two languages. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 70: 628–646.

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Crevier-Braud, L., Van den Broeck, A., Kristin-

Aspeli, A., … Westbye, C. 2015. The multidimensional work motivation scale:

Validation evidence in seven languages and nine countries. Journal of Work and

Organizational Psychology, 24: 178-196.

Gignac, G. E. 2008. Higher-order models versus direct hierarchical models: G as

superordinate or breadth factor. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50: 21– 43.

Gignac, G. E. 2016. The higher-order model imposes a proportionality constraint: That is why the

Page 38: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 38

bifactor model tends to fit better. Intelligence, 55: 57-68.

Gillet, N., Gagné, M, Sauvagère, S., & Fouquereau, E. 2013. Predicting employees’

satisfaction and turnover intentions using self-determination theory. European Journal of

Work and Organizational Psychology, 22: 450-460.

Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. 1987. Autonomy in children's learning: experimental and

individual investigation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52: 890-898.

Guay, F., Morin, A., Litalien, D., Valois, P., & Vallerand, R.J. 2015. Application of

exploratory structural equation modeling to evaluate the academic motivation scale. The

Journal of Experimental Education, 83: 51-82.

Guay, F., Ratelle, C., Roy, A., & Litalien, D. 2010. Academic self-concept, autonomous

academic motivation, and academic achievement: Mediating and additive effects.

Learning and Individual Differences, 20: 644–653.

Gucciardi, D. F., & Zyphur, M. J. 2016. Exploratory structural equation modelling and

Bayesian estimation. In N. Ntoumanis & N.D. Myers (Eds.). An introduction to

intermediate and advanced statistical analyses for sport and exercise scientists (pp. 172-

194). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Holzinger, K.J., & Swineford, F. 1937. The bi-factor model. Psychometrika, 2: 1-17.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6: 1-55.

Jennrich, R. I., & Bentler, P. M. 2011. Exploratory bi-factor analysis. Psychometrika, 76:

537–549.

Kahn, J. 2006. Factor analysis in counselling psychology research, training, and practice:

Principles, advances, and applications. The Counseling Psychologist, 34: 684-718.

Li, F. 1999. The exercise motivation scale: Its multifaceted structure and construct validity.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11: 97-115.

Page 39: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 39

Li, F., & Harmer, P. 1996. Testing the simplex assumption underlying the sport motivation

scale: A structural equation modeling analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and

Sport, 67: 396-405.

Litalien, D., Guay, F., & Morin, A.J.S. 2015. Motivation for Ph.D. studies: Scale

development and validation. Learning and Individual Differences, 41: 1-13.

Mallett, C., Kawabata, M., Newcombe, P., Otero-Forero, A., & Jackson, S. 2007. Sport

motivation scale-6 (SMS-6): A revised six-factor sport motivation scale. Psychology of

Sport and Exercise, 8: 600–614.

Markland, D., & Ingledew, D.K. 2007. The relationships between body mass and body image

and relative autonomy for exercise among adolescent males and females. Psychology of

Sport and Exercise, 8, 836–853.

Marsh, H., & Hau, K.-T. 2007. Applications of latent-variable models in educational

psychology: The need for methodological-substantive synergies. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 32: 151–171.

Marsh, H., Hau, K.-T., & Grayson, D. 2005. Goodness of fit evaluation in structural equation

modeling. In A. Maydeu-Olivares & J. McArdle (Eds.), Contemporary psychometrics. A

Festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald. Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum.

Marsh, H., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. 2004. In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis

testing approaches to cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu &

Bentler’s (1999). Structural Equation Modeling, 11: 320-341.

Marsh, H., Liem, G., Martin, J., Morin, A., & Nagengast, B. 2011. Methodological

measurement fruitfulness of exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM): New

approaches to key substantive issues in motivation and engagement. Journal of

Psychoeducational Assessment, 29: 322–346.

Marsh, H., Lüdtke, O., Muthén, B., Asparouhov, T., Morin, A., Trautwein, U., & Nagengast,

Page 40: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 40

B. 2010. A new look at the big five factor structure through exploratory structural

equation modeling, Psychological Assessment, 22: 471–491.

Marsh, H., Lüdtke, O., Nagengast, B., Morin, A., & Von Davier, M. 2013. Why item parcels

are (almost) never appropriate: Two wrongs do not make a right-camouflaging

misspecification with item parcels in CFA. Psychological Methods, 18: 257-284.

Marsh, H., Morin, A., Parker, P., & Kaur, G. 2014. Exploratory structural equation modeling:

An integration of the best features of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis.

Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10: 85–110.

Marsh, H., Muthén, B., Asparouhov, T., Lüdtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Morin, A. J. S., &

Trautwein, U. 2009. Exploratory structural equation modeling, integrating CFA and EFA:

Application to students’ evaluations of university teaching. Structural Equation

Modeling, 16: 439–476.

McAbee, S., Oswald, F., & Connelly, B. 2014. Bifactor models of personality and college

student performance: A broad versus narrow view. European Journal of Personality, 26,

604-619.

Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. 1997. Commitment in the workplace: Theory, research, and

application. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Meyer, J. P., Allan, N., & Smith, C, A. 1993. Commitment to organizations and occupations:

Extension and test of a three component conceptualization. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 78: 538-551.

Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A., & Marsh, H. 2016a. A bifactor exploratory structural equation

modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant

psychometric multidimensionality. Structural Equation Modeling, 23: 116-139.

Morin, A. J. S., Arens, K., Tran, A., & Caci, H. 2016b. Exploring sources of construct-

relevant multidimensionality in psychiatric measurement: a tutorial and illustration using

Page 41: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 41

the Composite Scale of Morningness. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric

Research. Early View: DOI: 10.1002/mpr.1485

Morin, A. J. S., & Maïano, C. 2011. Cross-validation of the short form of the physical self-

inventory (PSI-S) using exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM). Psychology of

Sport & Exercise, 12: 540-554.

Morin, A. J. S., Marsh, H., Nagengast, B. 2013. Exploratory structural equation modeling: an

introduction. In GR Hancock & RO Mueller (Eds.), Structural Equation Modeling: A

Second Course, 2nd Edition (pp. 395–436). Greenwich, CT: IAP.

Murray, A., & Johnson, W. 2013. The limitations of model fit in comparing bifactor versus

higher-order models of human cognitive ability structure. Intelligence, 41: 407–422.

Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Tuson, K. M., Briere, N. M., & Blais, M. R.

1995. Toward a new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and

amotivation in sports: The Sports Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal of Sport and

Exercise Psychology, 17: 35–53.

Pelletier, L. G., Rocchi, M. A., Vallerand, R. J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 2013. Validation

of the revised sport motivation scale. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14: 329-341.

Pelletier, L. G., Seguin-Levesque, C., & Legault, L. 2002. Pressure from above and pressure

from below as determinants of teachers’ motivation and teaching behaviors. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 94: 186–196.

Podsakoff, P., MacKenzie, S., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. 2003. Common method biases in

behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 88: 879–903.

Preacher, K., & MacCallum, R. 2003. Repairing Tom Swift’s electric factor analysis machine.

Understanding Statistics, 2: 13-43.

Rasch, G. 1960. Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests.

Page 42: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 42

Copenahagen, Denmark: Danmarks Paedagogiske Institut.

Reise, S. 2012. The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behavioral

Research, 47: 667-696.

Reise, S. P., Moore, T. M. & Maydeu-Olivares, A. 2011. Targeted bifactor rotations and

assessing the impact of model violations on the parameters of unidimensional and

bifactor models. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 71: 684–711.

Ryan, R., & Connell, P. 1989. Perceived locus of causality and internalization. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 57: 749–761.

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. L. 2000. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic

motivation, social development, and wellbeing. American Psychologist, 55: 68–78.

Sass, D., & Schmitt, T. 2010. A comparative investigation of rotation criteria within

exploratory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45: 1-33.

Schmitt, T., & Sass, D. 2011. Rotation criteria and hypothesis testing for exploratory factor

analysis: implications for factor pattern loadings and interfactor correlations. Educational

& Psychological Measurement, 71: 95-113.

Stevenson, S., & Lochbaum, M. 2008. Understanding exercise motivation: examining a

revised model of achievement motivation. Journal of Sport Behavior, 31: 389-412.

Stinglhamber, F., Bentein, K., & Vandenberghe, C. 2002. Extension of the three-component

model of commitment to five foci: Development of measures and substantive test.

European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 18: 123-138.

Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Witte, H., Soenens, B., & Lens, W. 2010. Capturing

autonomy, competence, and relatedness at work: Construction and initial validation of

the work‐related basic need satisfaction scale. Journal of Occupational and

Organizational Psychology, 83: 981-1002.

Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., Senecal, C., & Vallieres, E. F.

Page 43: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 43

1992. The academic motivation scale: A measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and

amotivationin education. Education and Psychological Measurement, 52: 1003-1017.

Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., Senécal, C., & Vallieres, E. F.

1993. On the assessment of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education: Evidence

on the concurrent and construct validity of the Academic Motivation Scale. Educational

and Psychological Measurement, 53: 159–172.

Vallerand, R. J. 1997. Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In M.

P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. 29, 271–360. San Diego,

CA: Academic Press.

Vandenberghe, C., & Panaccio, A. 2012. Perceived sacrifice and few alternatives

commitments: The motivational underpinnings of continuance commitment's

subdimensions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81: 59–72.

Wininger, S. 2007. Self-determination theory and exercise behavior: An examination of the

psychometric properties of the exercise motivation scale. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 19: 471-486.

Yung, Y, -F., Thissen, D., McLeod, L. D. 1999. On the relationship between the higher order

factor model and the hierarchical factor model. Psychometrika, 64: 113-128.

Page 44: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 44

TABLE 1

GOODNESS OF FIT STATISTICS AND INFORMATION CRITERIA

χ2 df RMSEA (90% CI) CFI TLI AIC BIC CAIC ABIC ICM-CFA 513* 137 .05 (.05 - .06) .94 .92 73958 74320 74392 74091 ESEM 128* 72 .03 (.02 - .03) .99 .98 73608 74296 74433 73861 Bifactor-CFA 834* 133 .07 (.06 - .7) .88 .85 74332 74714 74790 74472 Bifactor-ESEM 75 59 .02 (.00 - .03) 1.00 .99 73574 74328 74478 73851 Note. ICM = Independent cluster model; CFA = Confirmatory factor analysis; ESEM = Exploratory structural equation modeling; df = Degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CI = confidence interval; AIC = Akaike information criterion; CAIC = Constant AIC; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; ABIC = Sample size adjusted BIC; * p < .01.

TABLE 2

STANDARDIZED FACTOR CORRELATIONS FOR THE ICM-CFA (ABOVE THE DIAGONAL) AND ESEM (BELOW THE

DIAGONAL) SOLUTIONS

1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Intrinsic .81** .46** .05 .06 -.48**

2. Identified .77** .66** .20** .15** -.42**

3. Introjected .25** .44** .45** .53** -.22**

4. External-social .05 .12** .41** .74** .18**

5. External-material -.05 .18** .36** .68** .16**

6. Amotivation -.47** -.41** -.17** .14** .15** * p < .05 ** p < .01

Page 45: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 45

TABLE 3

STANDARDIZED FACTOR LOADINGS (λ) AND UNIQUENESSES (δ) FOR ICM-CFA AND ESEM

ICM-CFA solution ESEM solution Items λ δ Factor 1 (λ) Factor 2 (λ) Factor 3 (λ) Factor 4 (λ) Factor 5 (λ) Factor 6 (λ) δ 1. Intrinsic

Item 1 .84 .29 .79 .06 .02 -.04 <.01 -.01 .29 Item 2 .88 .23 .93 -.08 .06 <.01 .02 .02 .21 Item 3 .90 .19 .86 .06 -.03 .04 -.03 -.01 .19

2. Identified Item 1 .63 .60 .03 .37 .37 -.10 .06 -.12 .52 Item 2 .81 .34 .09 .81 -.11 .03 .02 <.01 .30 Item 3 .79 .38 -.01 .81 .02 -.01 -.01 .02 .36

3. Introjected Item 1 .58 .67 .04 .06 .39 .24 .01 -.01 .66 Item 2 .69 .53 .09 .31 .38 .05 <.01 -.01 .52 Item 3 .59 .66 <.01 .01 .62 .04 .06 .08 .55 Item 4 .50 .75 .04 -.09 .65 -.05 -.01 <.01 .63

4. External-social Item 1 .53 .72 .08 -.01 -.03 .68 .03 -.01 .51 Item 2 .72 .49 -.01 .04 .04 .61 .01 .01 .58 Item 3 .62 .62 -.06 -.08 .11 .56 .09 <.01 .55

5. External-material Item 1 .67 .55 .10 -.02 -.12 -.01 .61 <.01 .67 Item 2 .65 .58 <.01 .04 .01 <.01 .70 .02 .48 Item 3 .67 .55 -.13 -.01 .09 .08 .53 -.01 .62

6. Amotivation Item 1 .73 .47 <.01 -.03 <.01 -.02 <.01 .71 .48 Item 2 .65 .58 .04 <.01 <.01 -.09 .08 .68 .56 Item 3 .71 .50 -.04 .05 .05 .08 -.08 .72 .48

Page 46: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 46

TABLE 4

STANDARDIZED FACTOR LOADINGS (λ) AND UNIQUENESSES (δ) FOR BIFACTOR-CFA AND BIFACTOR-ESEM

Bifactor-CFA Bifactor-ESEM

Items G-Factor

(λ) S-Factor

(λ) δ G-Factor

(λ) S-Factor 1

(λ) S-Factor 2

(λ) S-Factor 3

(λ) S-Factor 4

(λ) S-Factor 5

(λ) S-Factor 6

(λ) δ

1. Intrinsic Item 1 .70 .46 .29 .73 .41 -.01 -.05 -.07 -.05 -.04 .29 Item 2 .70 .55 .21 .71 .54 .02 -.01 -.02 -.03 -.05 .21 Item 3 .73 .53 .19 .75 .48 .04 -.09 -.03 -.17 -.05 .19

2. Identified Item 1 .67 -.12 .54 .56 .04 .27 .31 -.02 .02 -.10 .51 Item 2 .81 .58 .01 .79 .04 .26 -.10 -.04 <.01 .05 .30 Item 3 .75 .10 .43 .73 .01 .34 .01 -.04 -.01 .05 .35

3. Introjected Item 1 .32 .47 .68 .33 -.03 .03 .38 .28 .10 .03 .66 Item 2 .62 .32 .51 .61 -.02 .08 .33 .06 .04 -.05 .50 Item 3 .28 .61 .55 .28 -.05 .06 .55 .18 .12 .09 .56 Item 4 .25 .54 .65 .26 -.05 -.02 .55 .05 .03 <.01 .62

4. External-social Item 1 .16 .67 .53 .21 -.03 -.09 .08 .61 .22 .09 .51 Item 2 .15 .64 .57 .18 -.05 -.01 .13 .59 .17 .09 .57 Item 3 .03 .66 .56 .02 -.03 .06 .21 .59 .25 .07 .53

5. External-material Item 1 .12 .54 .69 .25 -.13 -.32 -.07 .06 .78 .11 .19 Item 2 .18 .71 .46 .18 -.03 .09 .13 .33 .47 .11 .61 Item 3 .04 .58 .66 -.07 .07 .35 .22 .32 .59 <.01 .38

6. Amotivation Item 1 -.37 .62 .48 -.35 -.05 -.04 .01 .07 .08 .62 .48 Item 2 -.27 .60 .56 -.30 <.01 .03 <.01 .04 .09 .59 .56 Item 3 -.31 .64 .49 -.31 -.06 .02 .06 .12 .06 .62 .49

Page 47: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 47

Table 5

RELATIONS WITH COVARIATES: STANDARDIZED COEFFICIENTS

Covariates Quantity only Quantity and Quality G-Factor R2 Amotivation External-M. External-S. Introjection Identified Intrinsic G-Factor R2

Affective Commitment

.61 (<.01) .38 -.11 (.01) -.12 (<.01) .10 (.02) -.14 (.03) .20 (.04) .17 (.02) .55 (<.01) .42

Continuance Commitment

-.02 (.64) .00 .15 (.01) -.02 (.63) .23 (<.01) .09 (.33) .24 (.08) -.09 (.37) -.02 (.80) .14

Autonomy .39 (<.01) .15 -.21 (<.01) -.01 (.93) -.06 (.21) -.14 (.02) -.15 (.04) .16 (<.01) .38 (<.01) .26 Competence .09 (.03) .01 -.07 (.21) -.10 (.06) -.09 (.14) .06 (.43) -.11 (.18) .02 (.76) .12 (.01) .05 Relatedness .39 (<.01) .15 -.19 (<.01) .01 (.83) -.04 (.51) -.14 (.02) -.15 (.04) .17 (<.01) .37 (<.01) .25

Note. p values are in brackets; G-factor: Global factor representing the global quantity of self-determined motivation; M: Material; S: social.

Page 48: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 48

Figure 1

SIMPLIFIED REPRESENTATIONS OF SPECIFIED MODELS

Note. ICM-CFA: Independent cluster model- confirmatory factor analysis; ESEM: Exploratory structural equation modelling.

S1

X1

X2

X3

X4

X5

X6

X7

X8

X9

S2 S3

ICM-CFA

S1

X1

X2

X3

X4

X5

X6

X7

X8

X9

S2 S3

ESEM

S1

X1

X2

X3

X4

X5

X6

X7

X8

X9

S2 S3

Bifactor-CFA

G

S1

X1

X2

X3

X4

X5

X6

X7

X8

X9

S2 S3

Bifactor-ESEM

G

Page 49: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 49

Online Supplements for: Using Bifactor-Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Test for a Continuum

Structure of Motivation (Manuscript ID JOM-15-0440)

Authors’ note: These online technical appendices are to be posted on the journal website and hot-linked to the manuscript. If the journal does not offer this possibility, these materials can alternatively be posted on one of our personal websites (we will adjust the in-text reference upon acceptance). We would also be happy to have some of these materials brought back into the main manuscript as Appendices if you deem it useful. We developed these materials mostly to provide additional technical information and to keep the main manuscript from becoming needlessly long. 1. ICM-CFA Page 2 2. Bifactor CFA Page 3 3. ESEM Page 4 4. Bifactor ESEM Page 5 5. Bifactor ESEM with covariates Page 7 5. Bifactor ESEM with ESEM-within-CFA Page 8

Page 50: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 50

Title: 6 Factor CFA; ! In all input files, statements preceded by ! are annotations. ! Use the following statement to identify the data set. Here, the data set is labelled BESEM.dat. ! If the data set is not in the same folder as the input file, include the complete path to the data set. Data: File is BESEM.dat; ! The variables names function identifies all variables in the data set, in order of appearance, ! whereas the usevar command identifies the variables used in the analysis. Variable: Names are Am1 Am5 Am6 Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Ident1 Ident3 Ident4 Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6; Usevariable are Am1 Am5 Am6 Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6; ! The next section defines the analysis. Here the Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) estimator is used. Analysis: ESTIMATOR = MLR; ! The next statement defines the model. Here, a simple CFA model with no cross loading is specified ! with 6 factors (amotiv - intrin) defined respectively with items from the usevariable list. ! The name of the factors is selected by the user and comes before the “By” command. ! The “By” command indicates with items serve to define which factor. Model:

Amotiv by Am1 Am5 Am6; ExtMat by Emap1 Emap4 Emav4; ExtSoc by Esap2 Esap4 Esav1; Introj by Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2; Ident by Ident2 Ident3 Ident4; Intrin by Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6; ! Specific sections of output are requested Output: sampstat stdyx mod res svalues;

Page 51: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 51

Title: Bifactor CFA; ! Common sections of inputs are skipped to focus only on changes in the MODEL section ! The next statement defines the model. Here, a bifactor CFA model is ! specified with 6 specific factors (amotiv - intrin) defined as in the CFA model. ! All items are also used to define global factor called G. Model:

Amotiv by Am1 Am5 Am6; ExtMat by Emap1 Emap4 Emav4; ExtSoc by Esap2 Esap4 Esav1; Introj by Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2; Ident by Ident2 Ident3 Ident4; Intrin by Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6; G by

Am1 Am5 Am6 Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6; ! All factors are specified as orthogonal, with their correlations (WITH) constrained to be 0 (@0). Extsoc with amotiv@0; ExtSoc with Extmat@0; ExtSoc with Introj@0; ExtSoc with Ident@0; ExtSoc with Intrin@0; Extmat with amotiv@0; ExtMat with Introj@0; ExtMat with Ident@0; ExtMat with Intrin@0; Introj with amotiv@0; Introj with Ident@0; Introj with Intrin@0; ident with amotiv@0; Ident with Intrin@0; Intrin with amotiv@0; g with amotiv@0; g with ExtSoc@0; g with ExtMat@0; g with Introj@0; g with Ident@0; g with Intrin@0; Output: sampstat stdyx mod res svalues;

Page 52: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 52

TITLE: 6 factor ESEM ! Common sections of inputs are skipped to focus only on the ANALYSIS and MODEL sections. ! The Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) estimator is used together with the oblique target rotation. ANALYSIS: ESTIMATOR = MLR; ROTATION = target; ! The next statement defines the model. Here, an ESEM model is specified with target rotation. ! The 6 factors (amotiv - intrin) are defined respectively with main loadings from their respective items. ! In addition to these main loadings, all other cross-loadings are estimated but targeted ! to be as close to 0 as possible (~0). Factors forming a single set of ESEM factors (with cross- ! loadings between factors, are indicated by using the same label in parenthesis after * (*1). MODEL: Amotiv BY Am1 Am5 Am6 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ext_mat by Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ext_soc by Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Introj by Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ident by Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Intrin by Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0

Page 53: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 53

Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 (*1); Output: sampstat stdyx mod res svalues; TITLE: Bifactor-ESEM (measurement model) ! Common sections of inputs are skipped to focus only on the ANALYSIS and MODEL sections. ! The next section defines the analysis. Here the Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) estimator is used ! together with orthogonal bifactor target rotation (making all factors orthogonal as in Bifactor-CFA). ANALYSIS: ESTIMATOR = MLR; ROTATION = target (orthogonal); ! The next statement defines the model. Here, an ESEM model is specified with target rotation. ! The 6 factors (amotiv - intrin) are defined respectively with main loadings from their respective items. ! In addition to these main loadings, all other cross-loadings are estimated but targeted ! to be as close to 0 as possible (~0). All items are also used to define one global factor (called G). ! Factors forming a single set of ESEM factors (with cross- loadings between factors, ! are indicated by using the same label in parenthesis after * (*1). MODEL: G by Am1 Am5 Am6 Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6 (*1); Amotiv BY Am1 Am5 Am6 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ext_mat by Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ext_soc by Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Introj by Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0

Page 54: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 54

Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Ident by Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Intrin2~0 Intrin4~0 Intrin6~0 (*1); Intrin by Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6 Am1~0 Am5~0 Am6~0 Emap1~0 Emap4~0 Emav4~0 Esap2~0 Esap4~0 Esav1~0 Inap1~0 Inap2~0 Inav1~0 Inav2~0 Ident2~0 Ident3~0 Ident4~0 (*1); Output: sampstat stdyx mod res svalues;

Page 55: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 55

TITLE: Bifactor ESEM with covariates relations with all factors freely estimated ! Common sections of inputs are skipped to focus only on the USEVARIABLE, ANALYSIS and MODEL ! sections. Variables autonomy, competence, relatedness, affective commitment (AC), and continuance ! commitment (CC) have been added to this analysis in the use variable section. Usevariable are Am1 Am5 Am6 Emap1 Emap4 Emav4 Esap2 Esap4 Esav1 Inap1 Inap2 Inav1 Inav2 Ident2 Ident3 Ident4 Intrin2 Intrin4 Intrin6 Autonomy Competence Relatedness AC CC ; ! The next section defines the analysis. Here the Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) estimator is used ! together with the orthogonal bifactor target rotation. Analysis: ESTIMATOR = MLR; ROTATION = target (orthogonal); Model: ![…] The first section of the model is identical to the previous example. ! The following ON statement specifies estimation of regressions between S-factors (amotiv – to intrin) ! and G-factor and covariates (Autonomy, Competence, Relatedness, AC, and CC). AC CC Autonomy Competence Relatedness ON Amotiv ExtMat ExtSoc Introj Ident Intrin G ;

Page 56: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 56

TITLE: Bifactor ESEM with ESEM-within-CFA with covariates relations with the S-factors constrained to be zero ! ANALYSIS and MODEL sections only Analysis: estimator is MLR; ! Start values in the following section are obtained through estimation of the bifactor-ESEM measurement ! model requesting the “svalues” section of the output. ! Using these values is required to relax some constraints of ESEM and Bifactor-ESEM. For instance ! in ESEM and Bifactor-ESEM, all factors need to present the same patterns of freely estimated relatiosn ! to covariates. Here, the ESEM-within-CFA approach is required to constrain the relations between the ! S-factors (but not the G-factor) and the covariates to be zero. ! In ESEM-within-CFA, one referent indicator is selected for each factor (including the G-factor) and all ! cross loadings for this indicators are constrained (@) to take exactly the value it had in the freely ! estimated model. All other loadings and cross loadings are given a start value corresponding to ! their values (*) from the freely estimated model. All factor variances are constrained to be 1 (@1). ! For a bifactor model, all factor correlations are also constrained to be 0 (@0). Model: g BY [email protected]; g BY am5*-0.33615; g BY am6*-0.33080; g BY [email protected]; g BY esap4*0.33666; g BY esav1*0.03294; g BY [email protected]; g BY emap4*0.31362; g BY emav4*-0.14727; g BY inap1*0.63006; g BY inap2*0.90622; g BY [email protected]; g BY inav2*0.48894; g BY ident2*0.77973; g BY ident3*1.31872; g BY [email protected]; g BY intrin2*1.14160; g BY [email protected]; g BY intrin6*1.12897; Amotiv BY am1*0.68981; amotiv BY am5*0.65837; amotiv BY am6*0.65592; amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY emap4*0.19998; amotiv BY emav4*0.00300;

Page 57: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 57

amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY esap4*0.17431; amotiv BY esav1*0.11986; amotiv BY inap1*0.05677; amotiv BY inap2*-0.07635; amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY inav2*0.01314; amotiv BY ident2*-0.14293; amotiv BY ident3*0.07663; amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY [email protected]; amotiv BY intrin6*-0.07964; extmat BY emap1*1.53774; extmat BY emap4*0.84067; extmat BY emav4*1.13499; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY am5*0.10347; extmat BY am6*0.06301; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY esap4*0.32382; extmat BY esav1*0.45489; extmat BY inap1*0.18847; extmat BY inap2*0.05461; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY inav2*0.06022; extmat BY ident2*0.02634; extmat BY ident3*0.00048; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY [email protected]; extmat BY intrin6*-0.10479; extsoc BY esap2*1.13210; extsoc BY esap4*1.09945; extsoc BY esav1*1.08788; extsoc BY [email protected]; extsoc BY am5*0.04667; extsoc BY am6*0.12917; extsoc BY [email protected]; extsoc BY emap4*0.58741; extsoc BY emav4*0.61396; extsoc BY inap1*0.54434; extsoc BY inap2*0.08778; extsoc BY [email protected]; extsoc BY inav2*0.10321; extsoc BY ident2*-0.02858; extsoc BY ident3*-0.06371; extsoc BY [email protected];

Page 58: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 58

extsoc BY [email protected]; extsoc BY [email protected]; extsoc BY intrin6*-0.04847; introj BY inap1*0.73127; introj BY inap2*0.48896; introj BY inav1*1.12825; introj BY inav2*1.06249; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY am5*0.00518; introj BY am6*0.06226; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY emap4*0.23147; introj BY emav4*0.41822; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY esap4*0.25184; introj BY esav1*0.38897; introj BY ident2*0.43978; introj BY ident3*-0.16744; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY [email protected]; introj BY intrin6*-0.13357; ident BY ident2*0.37232; ident BY ident3*0.42740; ident BY ident4*0.59507; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY am5*0.03338; ident BY am6*0.01853; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY emap4*0.15928; ident BY emav4*0.68637; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY esap4*-0.02558; ident BY esav1*0.10667; ident BY inap1*0.06167; ident BY inap2*0.12079; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY inav2*-0.03615; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY [email protected]; ident BY intrin6*0.05258; intrin BY [email protected]; intrin BY intrin4*0.86685; intrin BY intrin6*0.72335; intrin BY [email protected]; intrin BY am5*-0.00221; intrin BY am6*-0.06204;

Page 59: Running head: BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 1similar ESEM representation of doctoral students’ academic motivation, see Litalien, Guay, & Morin, 2015). In the present study, we extend

BIFACTOR-ESEM AND MOTIVATION 59

intrin BY [email protected]; intrin BY emap4*-0.04727; intrin BY emav4*0.14359; intrin BY [email protected]; intrin BY esap4*-0.08515; intrin BY esav1*-0.04757; intrin BY inap1*-0.05376; intrin BY inap2*-0.02423; intrin BY [email protected]; intrin BY inav2*-0.09012; intrin BY ident2*0.06175; intrin BY ident3*0.06970; intrin BY [email protected]; G-Intrin@1; extmat WITH [email protected]; extsoc WITH [email protected]; extsoc WITH [email protected]; introj WITH [email protected]; introj WITH [email protected]; introj WITH [email protected]; ident WITH [email protected]; ident WITH [email protected]; ident WITH [email protected]; ident WITH [email protected]; intrin WITH [email protected]; intrin WITH [email protected]; intrin WITH [email protected]; intrin WITH [email protected]; intrin WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; g WITH [email protected]; ! The following inputs specify G to be freely estimated in relation to covariates while constraining ! S-factors (amotv-intrin) to be unrelated to covariates. AC CC Autonomy Competence Relatedness ON Amotiv@0 ExtMat@0 ExtSoc@0 Introj@0 Ident@0 Intrin@0; AC CC Autonomy Competence Relatedness ON G; AC CC Autonomy Competence Relatedness;